+ All Categories
Home > Documents > digital.library.unt.edu/67531/metadc130615/m2/1/high... · associative learning represents one of...

digital.library.unt.edu/67531/metadc130615/m2/1/high... · associative learning represents one of...

Date post: 15-May-2018
Category:
Upload: dinhnhan
View: 214 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
44
THE PUNCTIONIHO OF IMMEDIATE VERBAL FEEDBACK IK PAIRED ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING WITH JFORMALS AMD RETARDATES AMOPIPT (/ / / N. Minor bromsor IJAXT (V-o^-v—, Direetap tin* beptrtaent or fsytnology Dean bf the'Graduate Sehool
Transcript

THE PUNCTIONIHO OF IMMEDIATE VERBAL FEEDBACK IK

PAIRED ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING WITH

JFORMALS AMD RETARDATES

AMOPIPT

(/ / / N .

Minor bromsor

IJAXT (V-o^-v—, Direetap o£ tin* bep t r taen t o r f sy tno logy

Dean bf the'Graduate Sehool

¥HB FUNCTIONING OF IMMEDIATE VERBAL FEEDBACK IK S mmm m W4W1F m V#r J*#** mmm • m SmJfm-mrwmvVk0*1» imww

mmm mswmrm mmtm® WITH

mnmt& Mm

THESIS

Presented to the Graduate Council of the

North Texas State University la Partial

Fulfillment of the Requirements

For the Degree of

MASTER OF SCXEKCE

Joseph Vllllaa Ferrer*, B. ft.

Benton, Texas

AUGUST,, 196$

TABLE OF C0UTENTS

LIST OF * s m m . . . . . . . . . . . f i f ®

C h a p t e r

I . I N T R O D U C T I O N 3,

i i » m L A T w s t u d i e s 1*

I I I . M e t h o d . 2 1

S u b j a o t a

Experimental Design Selection T a s k P r o c e d u r e

IV , RESULTS AMD DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . 26

B e e u l t a

D i s c u s s i o n

1 , SUKMAH* A m m c m m m a t i o u i . 3 1

Summary

Recoraaiendat lone

APPENDIX A 34 4k 4MNk ifftft iMiriiiriM-ttirrtiaif i Tn uri n ^

APPIUDIX B 36

s m i o s R A P u r 3@

i l l

LIST W TABLES

Tabl* Page

X. Summary of Analysis of Yarlanee with formal® and Retardates Taatad Under Conditions ©f Reinforcement and ITon-llelnfor«eiient ©a a Paired Associativa Learning Tack . . , . 26

II. ltocn» and Standard Deviations of Intelligent# Laval Evaluated Under Conditions of Relnforoeaant and Xon-Beinforeeskent . . . . 27

lv

CHAPTER I

INTRODUGflOM

In psychological research the underlying Male purpose

is to question, discover phenomena, variables or condition®

which affect then and find lawfulness or orderliness of their

effects. The manipulation of verbal feedback In paired

associative learning represents one of the many methods

enabling scientists to explore natural events, the variables

affecting such eonditlons, and the lawfulness of each effect.

Considerable experimental evidence lndleates that a person

who has knowledge of his performance will learn more than a

subject of comparable ability who is working win the dark" (1)*

Knowledge of performance may be gained In either of two weyst

(a) the subject may be Informed concerning the correctness of

each response as it is made, or (b) the individual may be

Informed of his learning after .he has completed the entire

task. Zn general# research concentrated on knowledge of

performance involves the giving or withholding of Information

during and for varying amounts of time after the performance.

The central purpose of this study is to ascertain the

function of immediate verbal feedback after each response on

learning a paired associative task with normal and retarded

subjects.

2

Based on various learning theories reporting that the

level of learning is related to reinforcement, it was felt

that a theoretical background could be formulated whloh would

be the basis for the present study.

^fhe basic concepts of modern assQciatlonisra were foww-

lated by Edward L. Thorndlke (12), His interpretations were

based upon experimental observations of the problem-solving

behavior of eats in pussle boxes. Impressed by the charae-

terlstle trial-and-error behavior which his eats displayed

in eecaplng from pussle boxes, Thorndlke ooneluded that the

basis process Involved in learning was one of "stamping In"

correct responses and of "stamping out" incorrect responses.

This stamping in and stamping out of a response tendency

by its satisfaction and pleasantness or discomfort and annoy-

ance constitutes Thorndlke*s original "law of effect", which

he formulated as followss

Any act which in a given situation produces satis-faction becomes associated with the situation, so that when the situation recurs the act is more likely than before to recur alao. Conversely, any act which in a given situation produces discomfort become disassociated from that situation, so that when the situation recurs the act Is less likely than before to recur (IS, p. 203).

Some thirty years after the original animal experiments

which led to the formulation of the law of effect, Thorndlke

re-examined the law by experlmenta on human subjects (10, 13).

One such experiment involved the administering of an examina-

tion consisting of Spanish words. Bach Spanish word had five

alternative Sngllsh words, only one of whloh was correct. The

subjects were to underline the word which they thought to be

correct and listen for the examiner to announce, "flightw

(rewarded response) or "Wrong" (punished response}# As a

result of the experiment involving Spanish terminology with

human subjects, Thorndlke (11) concluded that reward leads

to repetition of the rewarded eonneetioni but punishment does

not lead to a weakening of the punished connection. Thorodike

maintained that the announcement of wrong did not weaken con-

nective bonds. These results demonstrated a strong positive

effect of reward but no comparable negative effect of punish-

ment. The effeet of punishment* Thorndlke concluded, was not

exactly to "dissociate a response from a situation" (11), but

rather to cause the learner to try something else until a

response was rewarded, thus becoming positively associated

with the situation.

Thorndlke'* law of effect is a law of association* The

organism forms associative reinforced "bonds'1 or "connections8 b e t w , e n * n a )

There were several investigations performed by Ivan Pavlov (9)

that demonstrated the establishment of an association. In a

series of animal experiments, Pavlov applied the term uncondi-

tional stimulus (UCS) to the mat powder, which elicited the

unconditioned response (OCR). Pavlov demonstrated that after

repeated ©cessions on which a bell was sounded immediately

before the meat powder was placed in the dog(s mouth, the bell

alone came to produce an increased flow of saliva. Pavlov

called this change in the animate behavior a conditioned

response (CH)j the previously "neutral" stimuli!© (the bell)

had become a conditioned stimulus (C8)» /

(As a result of Thorodike•s formulation of the law of

effect and Favlov's discovery of the conditioned response,

a common observation of their studies is evident. On© such

observation Is that fhorndito's "reward* and Pavlov*a "rein-

forcement" appear to have a eoosoon function. Both contribute

positively to establishing an assoeiation.

'ihe theoretical basis for Guthrie's system is formulated

on the principle of conditioning. 3ueh a theory has as its

cornerstone the contiguity of stimulus and response. Quthrie

stated his position of learning as being! "a combination of

stimuli which has accompanied a movement will on its recur*

reuse tend to be followed by that movement* (15# p. 23)$ or

learning depends upon contiguity of stimulus and response

pattern alone.

Such a framework is best represented by an experiment

introduced by Quthrie and liorton (6). Unlike Thorndike'e

pusele boxes, Quthrie*s design included two separate and dis-

tinct features. The front of Quthrie*s pussele box was wade

of glass allowing freedom for not only observation in the

usual sense, but photographic recordings. The second feature

that differentiated Guthrie*s pussle box from that of

ThrondIke's was the addition of a special release mechanism.

Such an apparatus consisted of a post suspended from the top

or protudlng from the floor of the box. The slightest

pressure by the animal from any direction would open the door

and also activate a camera which recorded the animal's move-

mint at that moment.

After the cats were introduced into the puzsle box from

a starting compartment in the rear, observations of the ani-

mal's behavior were recorded. The first awl. foremost obser-

vation was a tendency for the animal to repeat the precise

movements leading up to and including the escape movement.

The importance of successful repetitive patterning of behavior

provided evidence for continuity learning. The pattern of

escape movements was repeated because it removed the animal

from the pussle box. Still further, the learning of new

responses over old supported the principle that associations

reach their full strength upon the first pairing* or learning

is complete in one trial.

Unlike Thorndlke, Outhrie does not subscribe to rein-

forcement as a. factor necessary for learning, The eat learns

the correct Movements by stimulus response contiguity.

Our position is that what is associated is a stimulus and a response. By calling them assoel&ted# we mean that the stimulation M s become the occasion for the response because of a past association of the two (*, p. 23).

Reward or reinforcement, according to Guthrie, functions In

preventing the animal from breaking up previously learned pat*

terns. The eat engaged in consuramatory responses is kept not

only from moving about, but from establishing new stimulus

response connections. The ultimate condition of reward is to

prevent the animal from unlearning nuwessful movements.

6

TnomxUfce maintained that reward strengthens a connec-

tion, favlov imported that a conditioned atimulua paired

with an unconditioned stimulus produces a conditioned

response, and Guthrie stated that reinforcement prevents

the unlearning of successful aoveainti, but Clark I#. Hull

demonstrated that reward leads to "drive reduction, which

functions aa the essentlsl factor in order for learning to

occur .j Thle la evident in the following quotation of pos-

tulate row aa originally formulated by Hull.

When an effector activity (r~R) and a receptor activity (S-s) occur in close temporal contiguity (sflr), and this (sOr) l» closely associated with the diminution of a need (0) or with a stimulus which has been closely and consistently associated with the diminution of a need (0), there will result an increment to a tendency ( elr) for that afferent impulse on later occasions to evoke that reaction, fhe increments for successive reinforcement summate in a manner which yields a combined habit strength (sKr) which is a simple positive growth function of the number of reinforcements (R). The upper limit (N) of this curve of learning is the product of (1) a positive growth function of the magnitude of need reduction which is involved in primary, or which Is associated with secondary reinforcement* (2) a neg-ative function of the delay (t) in reinforcementi and (3) (a) a negative growth junction of the degree of aaynchronism (t') of S and ft when both are of brief duration, or (b) in case the action of S is prolonged so as to overlap the beginning of R, a negative growth function of the duration (t1) of the continuous action of 8 on the receptor when 1 begins (8, p. 178).

In the preceding quotation of Hull's postulate number . / * j -f '1

four, there are two major parts 4^ there is the factor which

deals with temporal contiguity as an important condition of

learning and the later portion of the postulate concerning the functioning of reinforcement.

The first factor which deals with temporal contiguity

i« where the conditioned stimulus gust be closely associated

with the unconditioned response before conditioning occurs,

Nor* precisely, habit strength (sXr), which represents the

Hulllan intervening variable for learning, depends upon olose

temporal contiguity of the receptor and effector activities

involved. It la the tendency to respond under conditions of

need reduction which become associated with the stimulus*

The second portion of Hull's fourth postulate concerns

the concepts of primary and secondary reinforcement. Primary

reinforcement Is similar to the food in Pavlov's condition-

lng experiment, the food is a primary reinforoer because it

reduces the animal's need for food whereas secondary relti~

forcemtnt results when stimuli closely associated with primary

reinforcement become effective in reducing needs. Itill

further, secondary reinforcement is a function of stimuli

rather than of needs. Bull (7) later defines primary rein-

forceaent as drlve-stlnulus, being those stimuli arising

froa the viscera, such as hunger pangs. fhe critical condi-

tion of reinfercement then is not the reduction of a need

for food, but the reduction of the drive stimuli, as in

satisfying a craving rather than a need. 4

As indicated in the revised portion of Bull's fourth

postulate {?), habit strength is built as a result of repe-

titions of successive reinforcement. Thus, learning or habit

strength (sHr) is related to the nuafeer of reinforced trials.

a

H. H. Elliott (3)< in a place learning experiment,

utilised a T-mase in which two groups of hungry rats wrt

to find rewards of bran sash and sunflower teed. The brut

mash group was the experimental group while the rats receiv-

ing sunflower seed represented the oontrol group. After a

ten day period# the reward of the experimental group ©f rats

was changed from bran sash to sunflower seed. At this point,

learning was disrupted, as demonstrated by an increase in

errors. This disruption of learning resulted because the

animals had built up an expectancy for the bran nash. Their

expectancy was not confirmed when they were changed to sun-

flower seed, as the bran mash was the more desirable of the

two. What was established was not primarily a motor response

but an expectancy of finding food. If the food was found,

the expectancy was confirmed and strengthened. As a result

of Elliott's experiment, a brief summary of Thorndlke's

position on reinforcement is In order.

The core of Thorod lice's theory was that learning

occurred 1» the course of a stimulus-response-reward

sequence. The reward or reinforcement acted as a factor

to strengthen the stimulus response connection. The reward

Itself was not learned. In the following Quotation# Tolman

rejects Thorodike's reinforcement concept, stating that the

whole sequence of stlmulus-response-reinforcement was

learned.

Hr objection to Thorndike's LAW of Sffeot was not to the importance of motivation as a faetor in learning, tout rather to his wholly asohanleal notion as to it» operation toy way of effect. Aooordlng to Thorndlke, an animal learned, not because it achieved a waited goal toy a certain aariaa of responses, tout nerely toeeauae a quite irrelevant "pleasantness" or "unpleasantness* was, s© to speak, shot at it, as from a squirt gun, after it tadI resetted the given goal-box or gone into the given eul de sae. And it la this mm quite mechanical and irrelevant notion as to tlit operation of the modern successor of 8ffe«t*~ wReinforcementtt--which underlies, t believe, m o&ln objection to it (2, p. 329-30).

folwan ( I k ) proposed a cognitive theory of learning in

place of the Law of Iffeet. His theory suggests that the

learner toullda up a comprehensive aequaintanoe or field

nap with his envlronnent, so that he la atole to go from one

pleoe to another without toeing restrleted toy a series of

toodily Movements. An experiment was eoastrueted toy Tolman

and Bonslk (15) to test the proposed eognltlve theory. A

naze in the for* of a eroee with two poasltole starting

positions and two alternative goals was designed. One

group of rats was trained so that they always found food

at the same place, irrespective of their starting position.

Another group of rats was required to m m the sane response

of turning right regardless of their starting position,

the results supported the cognitive theory in that the

place learners were significantly toetter in their perform-

ance than the response learners. Thus, learning a plaee

requires a eognltlve map of that plaee as opposed to a

specific set of motor responses.

10

tti® principle in Tolaan1* theory that Is the counterpart

or replacement for reinforcement is the principle of csenflr®a-

tion. If en expectancy is confirmed, it is strengthened and

its probability value inoreases. However, if an expectancy

is not eonf iraed, it is weakened i and its probability value

decreases ami leads to extinction.

At this point, one Bight ask what is it that is rein*

foroed or strengthened? Is it the stimulus response bond*

the assoeiation between the aetronoae and the salivary

response# the eontiguity of stlaulus and response, the

reduction of drive, or the confirming of an expectancy? A

possibility that has been suggested is that feedback or

knowledge of results is the strengthening condition* and

it contributes to the subject by giving hi» important

information about his task or environment. Troland (16)

recognized the fact that behavior does not end with the

response to a stimulus, but includes adjustment of that

response on the basis of further Information about the

situation.

These empirical studies and statements represent the

basis from which the following hypotheses were fonaulated.

1. Subjects having immediate knowledge of their per-

formance will reach a higher level of proficiency on a

paired associative learning task than those subjects who

are deprived of reinforcement.

11

2. normal subtests with immediate knowledge of their

performance will perform at «. higher level of profieleney

than retardate® who have imraedlate knowledge of their per-

formme.

3. The performance level of normal eubjaota deprived

of immediate verbal reinforceaent will be algnifloently

hi^ier than retarded subjects who are deprived of immediate

verbal reinf oroe**nt.

CKAPTIR i m M m m m f w

1. Amnons, R. B*» "Effects of Knowledge of Performance," il of Oatwral fayohology, LIV (April. 1956).

2. Boring, I. 0., editor, A History of Psychology 1 Autobiography, VolT IfTWroHsl ;er,' Massachuse11s, Clark University Press, 1952.

3. Billott# N. it., "The Bffeet of Change of Reward on the Maze Performance of Rata,® gnlverslty of California Publication* Psychology. IV (May, 192tJJ, 19-^0.

k. Quthrie, Edwin Rv, Candlfcloningt A theory; of learning 1 Aaaoe latlonT tonal '¥o®lety

for the Study of Education, 19%2.

5. •vSE ¥mPm* r York, Harper, 1952.

6. and 0. P. Morton, Cats In a ?uzzle fork, Rlnehart Company, "*

7- Hull, Clark L., A Behavior Syateau lew Haven, Yale University fress, 1^52.

8. , Principles of Behavior. Mew York, Appletori-Century-Croftc, 1943.

Pavlov, Ivan P., Condition Reflexes; A of the Cortex Oxford University Press, 1927.

10. Thomdlke, Edward L., |m giffp|immi £M$Z 2t M®mttb Mew York, Bureau of Publications, Columbia University,, 1933.

11. . "Reward and Punlshaent," Journal - J " 1 "L -ur'Jmimi Psychology, I {April* 1932),' K , ""

12. , The Elements of PsyehGlofor* Mew woman; win wnteJiMM* ii<agfe»wji)tsi»wwfe »w»i' * *mtrnrnm m m iiij>>fliwiiiiii»i«B>!i rwi i m r n d m m *- *

YoipIC$ Jfc i> 0 V 2|0 XHI } X *

12

13

13- Thorndlke, Edward L., Ttie ffundaiaentalB of Learning. Hew York, Bureau of imiMiiew, coraola ffiSwBwity» 193a,

1%. Tolaan, Bdward C., "Cognitive Naps In Bats and Ifen." Psychological Review, LV (July, 19*8}» 189-208.

15- , ., _. . ._ „ and C. H. Honxik, "Degree* of Hunger Reward and fon-Reward* and Maze Learning in

16. Troland, L. T.# The Fundamental® of Huaan Motivation, Hew York, I>7TanT&oS*rtt^^

cHArra® xi

RELATED STUDIES

Subjects who are effectively provided with immediate

knowledge of their performance reach a higher level of

learning proficiency than subjects who are deprived, of

reinforcement. Studies exploring the conditions of knowl-

edge of performance span a wide range of different tasks

and subject variable*. However, research appears lacking

when comparing normal subjects with retarded subjects In

connection with immediate knowledge of performance. As

a result, investigations in the general area concerning

knowledge of performance will formulate the basis of

related studies*

Thorndlke (11) in teaan refers to the former

acceptance of the view that the mere repetition of a situation

in and of itself produces learning, and then states that this

is not true. In an experiment involving 3#0OO attempts to

draw with eyes closed lines of given lengths, the lines drawn

in the last two of the twelve sittings were not drawn any bet-

ter than those in the first and second sittings. 4s reported

by Thorndlke*s investigation, the learner had a task and a

goal but no way of knowing how far he fell short of it each

time and no way of knowing in what manner to change his per-

formance in order to function at a higher level. The necessity

14

15

for some type of check or appraisal of performance lit terms

of tiie desired outcome was indicated by the results of his

Investigation.

Kingsley (?) informed M s subjects after each trial as

to the degree of over or underestimation of their line draw-

ings. Three groups with seven subjects each were utilized.

They were asked to close their eyes and draw four inch lines.

This was repeated until eaoh subject had drawn 400 lines

without opening his eyes, looking at his product, or receiv-

ing any information regarding the suocess of their attempts.

They were then given twenty-five more trialsi and after each

trial, they opened their eyes and placed a four inch strip

of cardboard along the line they had Just drawn. It showed

at once how Much their line was too short or too long. 0nder

this procedure, errors were reduced and improvement was rapid

throughout the twenty-five trials.

Trowbridge and Cason (12) duplicated Thorndlke's study

with the additional observation that the degree of improve-

ment was directly related to amount of information provided.

The more specific the knowledge concerning the nature of

performance, the greater the improvement.

Brown (3) obtained results which demonstrated the sig-

nificance of providing knowledge of performance. Arithmetic

problems were given dally to 138 subjects for twenty day® in

fifth and seventh grade classes. Plowman and Strowd (9)

demonstrated the significance of providing knowledge of

16

performance with 850 tenth and eleventh grade «las®#« also

utilising arlthmrti* proble«s. In both of these Iwveatlga*

tiooii eaah group served as Its own working under

both conditions of waiving results and being without

taowledge of their performance.

Johsnsea (5) «t«tfi that sttbjeets effeetively given

teowledge of performance eoaaeming their annual motion

tiae with a telegraph icey significantly Improved their

speed, whereas those who received no knowledge of their

perfenttnee demonstrate?! little iaproveaent to their manual

reaction tim.

Working with difficult Xagllsh and Russian vocabulary

items and fasts in the field of psychology, Pressey (10)

demonstrated the signifioaase of knowledge of performance.

Subjeeta who repeated examination® with an immediate self-

sowing arrangement demonstrated a m m h higher level of

learning than did those to whoa the teat was repeatedly

given without the self-sserlng arrangement* Congruent

with Pressey's study, Angell <1) and Morgan and Horgan (8)

also discovered signifieant gains with subjects who lass-

diately received correctness of examination aaawers and

results of test seores.

army (2) reported an experiment on training gunners

to traek targets with a *0-aa gun. the taak in traeking

involved continuous sighting of the gun on a moving object.

Tracking with the %0-wa gun required two men, one to follow

IT

the horizontal movements of the target, the other to follow

the vertical movements. The standard Army method of train-

ing in thia skill was to give verbal inatruetions and

coaching before, during, and after each session or trial

of gunnery praotlce.

Three experimental oonditions were utilised to validate

the effectiveness of the training procedure. In group A

the standard Army method was utilised) in Group B each man

coached his partner! and in Oroup € a special telescopic

sight mounted on the gun was used to check the accuracy of

aim, so that the instructor eould sound a bucser whenever

the gun was off target, giving the men Immediate knowledge

of results. The men in Oroup C were also told what scores

they had at the end of each course. As Indicated by the

results of Bray*® investigation, subjects receiving imme-

diate knowledge of performance reached a higher level of

accuracy or proficiency than did those in the other groups..

Angell (1) experimented with immediate chemistry test

results. Students receiving information about right and

wrong answers performed at a higher level than those stu-

dents who were deprived of Immediate test results. Keller (6)

utilised a Morse code reception apparatus t© validate imme-

diate knowledge of performance. Subjects, who were Informed

after each word whether or not they were right, learned wore

than those subjects which received reinforcement after a

group of words. Still further, Houston (4) designed a study

18

of firing with the Pedestal Sight Manipulation fest so that

it was possible to compare trainees who were Informed Imme-

diately about their performance toy seeing a red filter drop

over the pieture of the target plane with trainees who were

given their results after larger units of trials, those

trainees who were given isnaediate reinforcement fey means of

the red filter were superior in performance.

In the preoedlng investigations, it was pointed out

that conditions toeing similar* the sooner a subject knows

of his performance, the more effect the given information

has in allowing suitable corrections of responses from one

trial to the next.

CHAPTER II BIBLIOGRAPHY

1.

2.

4.

5-

T.

8.

9.

10,

11.

Angell, 0. W,, "The Sffects of Immediate Knowledge of Quiz Results on Final Examination Soorea in Freshnan Chemistry." Journal of Educational Resea^gh, XLXI (May,

Bray, C. M., Psychology and Military Proficiency, Princeton, Princeton ttaiwrafty Press,

Brown, F., "Knowledge of Basulta as an Incentive in Schoolroom Practice." Journal of Sduoational ggyoholofor, m i l (Octolkr, "l935r)7

Houaton, R. €*, "The Function of Knowledge of Results in Learning A Coaplex Motor Skill,/1 unpublished master's thesis, Bepartmeat of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, 19*7.

Johanaon, A. N., "Influence of Inaantiva and Puniahmant

? S U r S £ e ^ 7 1 1 1

Keller, F. 8«, "Studiea in International Korea Coda," l ^ l ^ f A p p U ^ Fwoholomr. XXVII (Ootober,

Kings ley, 8* L., Mature an Hew Jersey, frenHce-

and. Conditio* » •**©»*

of Learning

Morgan, C. L. and I#. "V. Morgan, "Effects of Immediate Awareness of Suooeee a M Failure Upon Objective Examination Scores," Journal of gxyrlaanfcal

I? (Bepteal •35T,^r 55"

Plowman, Letha and J. Stroud, "Effecta of Informing Pupils of the Correctness of Their Response® to Objective Teat Questions," Journal Research. XXXVI (September, lpl)7 X6-SG.

Preaaey, 8. L., "Development and Appraisal of Devioes Providing mediate Automatic Scoring of Objective Teata and Coneoaitant Self-Instruction," Journal 2£ Paxohology. XXIX (January, 1950), 417-447.

Thorndlke, Sdward L., Human Learning. New York, Appleton, 1931•

19

20

12. Trowbridge, N. H. awl H. Cason, "An Experimental Study of Thorndilce'R Theory of Learning,"

a a t s 6 2 i f i t t ' T O ( 0 o t o b * r '

cmmm in

METHOD

Subjects

Twenty-two normal and twenty-two retarded students

were utilized a® subject® for the present study. Th@

normal subjects w w from an elementary school in Denton,

Texasi and the mentally retarded subjeots were fro® the

Benton State School, Only those students with mental ages

(MA) In the range fro® seven to ten years were included.

There were three subjects at the seven, eight, and nine

year mental age levels and two subjects at the ten year

mental age level for both normal and retarded groups. For

the normal group, IQ's ranged fro* 90-110, while chrono-

logical age (CA) was seven to ten years. In the mentally

retarded group, IQ's ranged fro® 50-70, whereas their

chronological age (CA) ranged fro® nine to twenty-one

years.

Experimental Design

The design was a two by two analysis of variance in

which groups of normal and retarded subjects were tested !

under conditions of reinforcement and non-reinforcement.

Reinforcement was Immediate verbal knowledge of performance

21

22

on a paired associative learning task recording the total

number correct for each subject.

Selection

The twenty-two normal subjects were selected on the

basis of the California Short-Form Test of Hental Maturity (1),

and an equal number of retarded subjects were selected by

the Stanford-Binet Test of Intelligence. I960 revision (2).

Test results dating back to 1963 through 1965 for each nor-

mal and retarded subject were ascertained from the master

file® of the elementary school and Denton State School

respectively. All retardates with apparent sensory or motor

Impairment were exoluded, as were subjects with moderate or

gross brain damage. The criterion for evaluation of brain

damage was the Bender Qestalt examination and records desig-

nating the frequency of seizures since time of admission to

the Denton State School. Retarded subjects with significant

impairment due to medication were also excluded.

Task

The coding A subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale

for Children (3) was adapted for the present investigation.

Each subject was shown a star, circle, triangle, cross, and

square having corresponding alphabetical letters A, 1# c , D,

and X. For the reinforced subjects, each geometric symbol

was shown manually on a silver screen for a ten second dura-

tion. The non-reinforced subjects were shown each geometric

23

aynbol electronically on the same aliver m m m for a ten

aecond Swot ion*

Each aubject waa given a aheet of white, unruled paper

that was eight and one-half itwshea wide ami eleven inches

long to be uaed for tha recording of their reapontes (aee

appendix A). Acroaa tha top of tha reaponae aheet waa tha

word "Oroup", which designated tha aubgroup for each sub-

Jeot. For the convenience of tha exaatlner, a acoring key

waa deviaed on whiah the correct alphabetical letter® for

each reaponae appear (aee appendix B)»

Procedure

Fro* their reapectiv* groups, tha noraal and mentally

retarded eubjecta were randomly divided into four subgroups

containing eleven meitibera aaoh. Upon entering tha dimly

lighted experimental classroom, subjects were seated facing

a aliver acreen in pairs of two to each table, and placed

before them were a reaponae sheet and a red lead pencil

without an eraaer. After each subject waa aeated In hi®

respective place, attention was focused ©w hia reaponae

aheet. Each aubject waa inatructed to notice that hia

reaponae aheet conaiated of two reaponae columna, and that

each of these reaponae columna contained forty-five blank

apace® except for lespona® Colmm I which had five addi-

tional blank apacea. At thia point, subjects were inatructed

that the five additional blank spaces of Reaponae Column I

were to be uaed aa sample learning trlala in order to

2#

facilitate complete understanding of how, when, and where to

record their response®. The initial instructions were then

administered!

fou are going to tee shown a set of geometric symbols that have corresponding alphabetical letter®. You are to place the letter you think corresponds with the exposed symbol on your answer sheet.

Each geometric symbol was exposed separately with Its co-

responding alphabetical letter accompanied by the following

instructionst

When you see a star, you are to record on your answer sheet the alphabetical letter A. When you see a circle, you are to record on your answer sheet the alphabetical letter B, etc.

Sample learning trials V-Z were then given without rein-

forcement to all subgroups followed by the initial numerical

trials 1-90 with subgroups A and 1 receiving immediate ver-

bal reinforcement, Reinforcement, as used in the present

experiment, consisted of the instructor giving the oorrect

alphabetical letter verbally after each geometric syiasbol

was exposed for a ten second duration.

cmnrn m bibliooraph*

1. Sullivan, 1. W. W. Clark, and 1. W. fi«ga, California Short-Form ftp. of Ifiilal iffipl&h

2, Taraan, L. II. and X. A. Marrill, Stanford-Btnet

Waehslar, D., jtoasl. M m IPI.

25

CHAPTER I?

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Results

In an attempt to measure immediate knowledge of per-

formance, twenty-two normal subjects and an equal number of

mentally retarded subjects were administered a paired asso-

ciative learning task under conditions of reinforcement and

non-reinforcement.

TABLE I

mWlhM Of ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE WITH NORMALS AND RETARDATES TESTED UNDER CONDITIONS OF REINFORCEMENT AND NON-REINFORCHHENT

ON A PAIRED ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING TASK

Source of Variation Sun SO, DP Mean SQ f P

Reinforcement Intelligence Level Reinforcement x

Intelligence Within

23578.1290 2339-1*72

16.3980 7393.oOll

1. 1.

1. 39*

23378.1290 2339*1472

16.3980 189-5795

124,3706 12.3386

.0864

• 001 .01

Total 42

A summary of the analysis of variance concerning immediate

knowledge of performance with reinforced groups compared to

non-relnforoed groups and normals contrasted with retardates

on a paired associative learning task appear in Table I.

26

27

The analysis reveals significant performance levels between

the reinforced, non-reinforced groups and the normals versus

retarded groups at the .001 and *01 levels respectively.

However, the interaction was not significant at the .05

level.

TABLE I I

MEANS AW M M M M V M U F I O M OF INTELLIGENCE LEVEL m&WMm UNDER cmamom w mimmmmm

tm mn~miMmcwmm

Reinforcement Non-He Inforceaent

Mean SD Mean 8D

Normal8 22 83.7272 9.3817 38,0909 10.6553

Retardates HH 70.2000 13.2*23 22.0909 17.6401

'Total J~ M""

Means and standard deviations appear in Table IX for

normal and retarded groups under conditions of reinforcement

and non-reinforcement.

Discussion

The results of this study support the following predic-

tions! (1) notwals and retardates having immediate verbal

knowledge of their performance reached a higher level of

proficiency than those subjects who were deprived of rein-

forcement . (2) normal subjects given Immediate knowledge of

their performance were superior to retarded subject® who also

28

received immediate verbal reinforcement, (3} The perform-

ance level of normal subjects deprived of immediate verbal

reinforeeraent will be significantly higher than retarded

subjects who are deprived of immediate verbal reinforcement.

As concluded in the present study, normals and retardates

having immediate knowledge of their performance and those

being deprived of such reinforcement corroborates the findings

reported by Thomdike (4), Trowbridge and Cason (5)#

Johanson (l), and Lorge and Thorndike (2). Also, results

Indicate that superior performance on the part of normal

subjects compared to retarded subjects suggest a limited

range of mental ability of the retardates In terms of intel-

ligence. Still further, this study supports the view that

a decrement in performance is directly related to those

subjects whose performance trial® were non-reinforced, The

two different measuring scales utilized in selecting each

subject for his respective group might account for further

differences in their performance. This Is represented by

the fact that the California Short-Form Test of Mental

Maturity, which has a correlation of .88 with the Stanford-

Binet, Is the less sophisticated measuring scale of the two.

Still further, higher chronological ages of the retarded sub-

jects might have indirect bearing on the results.

Hentlon is made of an investigation by Macpherson,

Bees, and Grindley (3) concerning adaptation to the experi-

mental task. Their finding® indicate that those subjects

29

receiving knowledge of their performance were better

orientated to the experimental arrangement® and perforuied

at a higher level as compared to those subjects who were

deprived of relnforoement. A© in Kacphereon, Dees, and

Orindley'a study and for the present investigation, those

subjects receiving ljamedlate verbal reinforcement adapted

to the experimental arrangements of the learning task at

a higher level of performance than those subjects who were

deprived of this reinforcing condition.

CHAPTER IV BIBLIOOHAPHV

1. Johanaon, A. If** "Influanoa of I imei i t iw and Punlahaant on Reaction f lue.11 Arohivee of Psychology, VIIX ((torch, 1982), 5*.

2* Lorge, 1. and MvtiHt L. fhorndika, "Tha Influence of Delay i n tha Aftsar-Iffaat of a Connection,* Journal of Exiseriiaental Psychology, XVI I I ( f a l r w T T t f f t e h 186-19*.

3. Mscphereon, I , J . , V. Dees, and 0. C. Grindley, "The Bffacta of Knowledge of Raaulta on Learning and Perfortyanoo," Quarterly Journal of BxpOT^iynta} Psychology, I (February, X P f ) , Bf-W*

4. Thoradilce, Sdward L . , "The Law of E f fec t , " American Journal ojf Fayeholo&y, XXXXX (Apr i l , i w ) » w t -&mm- *

5. Trowbridge, N. H. and H. Caaon, "An Experimental Study of Thorndike'a Theory of Learning," Journal of General Psychology. TO (October, 19WTS%^588.

30

CHAPTER V

StnOfABY ASD

Suiamary

The purpose of tliis study was to weasure the functioning

of Immediate verbal reinforcement with normals and retardates.

To explore the variables or conditions of immediate verbal

reinforcement* a paired associative learning task Has admin-

istered to two separate groups of subjects. Adapted for

tills study was the coding A subtest of the Wechsler

Intelligence Soale for Children* Each subject was shown a

star, circle* triangle, cross, and square having correspond-

ing alphabetical letters A, B, 0, D, and X. the first group

was composed of twenty-two noraal students from an elensn-

tary school in Denton, Texas. This normal group of subjects

was further divided into two subgroups, A and 9, consisting

of eleven weavers each. Subgroup A of the normals received

immediate verbal reinforcement while subgroup B of the nor-

mals was deprived of this variable* The second group con-

sisted of twenty-two mentally retarded students fro* the

Denton State School, this mentally retarded group of sub-

jects was also divided into two subgroups, Y and %, having

eleven members each. Subgroup ¥ of the mentally retarded

received liasiedlate verbal reinforcement) while subgroup Z

of the retardatea was effectively deprived of this condition.

31

38

Only those students with mental ages In the range from

seven to ten years were included. For the normal ©c*oup, I^'s

ranged from 90-llOj wtill# chronological age was sewn to ten

years. For the mentally retarded group, IQ*s ranged from

50~70; whereas their range of chronological age was from nine

to twenty-one years. Retarded subjects with apparent sensory

or motor impairment were excluded as were any subjects with

moderate or gross brain damage. The criterion for evaluating

brain damage was the Bender Qestalt examination and records

designating the frequency of seizures since tine of admission

to the Denton State School. Retarded subjects with a signifi-

cant impairment due to medication were also excluded.

Analysis of the data indicated that subjects receiving

immediate verbal reinforcement reached a higher level of

proficiency than those subjects who were effectively deprived

of this reinforcing condition. Also, superior performance of

the normals compared to retardates suggested a limited range

of mental ability of the retardates In terms of XQ's.

Recommendations

Based upon the results and conclusions of this investi-

gation, several additional related variables or conditions

require further experimentation and exploration.

1. directly related to the present study, future inves-

tigations should modify the time sequence of each exposed

geometric symbol after twenty response trials from ten seconds

33

to five seconds for the remaining seventy or more trials*

Such a modification will tend to stabilise the level of

motivation throughout the later portion of the taste.

a. Future research effort® should contrast rate of

learning with norwlc and retardates under conditions of

immediate verbal reinforcement.

3. The influence and function of distraction or an

increase in the subject's attention utilising nonsense syl-

lables after each response or larger units of responses

should be explored as possible reinforcers with normals and

retardates.

#. Pasilial retardates could be contrasted to brain

injured subjects on a learning task involving rate of learn-

ing and distraction or attention.

5, An experiment in which delay of knowledge of per-

formance is conpared with immediate knowledge of performance

utilising familial retardates and brain damaged subjects

represents another area to explore,

APPENDIX A

Bwmcf »s xssimm SHEET

Group

Response Column

Trial 1 V W ~ " * ~ 3T^

t r 2* "

?-

10 11" 12"

15 iS* IT :

18""""""""""

ML aor 21" 22*" a1~ 2 2

2 2 29** 30" 31" 32"

i:

Kespona© Column

Trial 2

m

m

34

35

APPEKDIX A"Continued

Response Column

1

Response ColUBff)

2

86 8'

$9" 90'

*8

Ji-ts :

ktmmm b

EXAMINER SCCRIHQ SOOT

Oroup

trial

Response Column

1 ? b V" e 'X'"" , rj„ - a

Y' ' c z " 1Jl a &" 3 e: 4 c 5 d 6 T""'" T d a e 9 S"

10 I — 11 ¥ 12" " a 1? 0 14 e 15 ¥ 16 a 1? ¥ " *' 18 e 10 ' a to'"" <© 21 22""'" 0 23 '""IfJ,lJU""" 24 S | Z »

H I e 28 29..-.:: 30 i si e

. a , 33 a 34

36

37

APFEHDIX B~Contlnugd

R«»pon*« Column

1 35 Q 3o e 37 a _ 3*

I 0 8 41 0 42 r r r

Hi *£

-1

85

Response Colusa*

a

o? jfife

89" 9or

0

t

it iT

BIBLIOOMF®

Bootes

Boring, S, a., •dltor, A

1952":

s u s * *

™ U 8 d

» W York, SlaSSr? c : ^ 0 ^ * 4 * * * * *

Hull, ClM* L., A Behavlog System. Km Ha von, ?»le v l&j*ir&31*11 X tjjr *3P45jBSf Xyf/21*

— 3 1 asas2iaE'"*" *ork'Appleton-

Kln8*J^i."; P r i n l & f f : 8 C T W * iSS£SiaS'

Pavlov

translate vereity Press, 1927

Thorodiko, Edward L., An New York, Burmu W 1933*

.„, „.,* l a m learning, New York, Appltton, 1931 *

o n r o r T ^ * * F'y"wlo'g' »- t o r k'

1938.

T r o l ag^-d t • » « • ? § § § ^ ' K e w

38

39

Articles

Simoom, ft. ®,, "Kffsots of icnowledge of Perforsi&nce.'' Journal of general Psychology, LI? (April, 1956),

Angell, Q. W*, "The Effects of Ximaedlate Knowledge of Quls Results on Final Examination Stores in Freshman Chemlstrjr^ Journal^of Educational Beeearch, XiXf II

Brown, F», "Knowledge of Results a* an Incentive in Schoolroom Practice," Journal of Educational Psychology. XXXII (Ootoftir? 193?)# $3S-55z.

Elliott, N« H., "The Effect of Change of Reward on the Mas* Performance of Rata," University of California Publications Psychology. IV (May, i<^3d.

Johanson, A. N., * Influence of Ineentive and Punishment on Reaction Time." Archives of Psychology. VIII (Marob, 1982), 5$. ** **•

Keller, F. S., "Studies in International Norse Code," |g|g^| $t tmrnm* (@t*ober, 1943),

Lorge, I* and Edward X*. Thorndlke, '*The Influence of Delay in the After-Effect of a Connection,* Journal of^lxgerlscntel Fsyeholofgy, XVIII (February, 19l5)V

MaePherson, S. J., V. Dees, and Q* C. Grindley, "The Effects of Knowledge of Results on Learning and Performance,"

%£rSary E f f i M M * *

Morgan, C« t»* and L. V. Morgan, "Effects of Tnwsdlate Awareness of Success and Failure Upon Objective Examination Scores, * J o w m I of oerliaental Education. IV (September,19357* ©3*®&.

Plowman, Letha and J. Stroud, "Effecte of Informing Pupils of the Correetness of Their Responses to Objective

Si ssssassii. XXXVI | I J # p f $ 15r*«I# io^ZQ*

Pressey, S. L., "Development and Appraisal of Devices Providing Immediate Automatic Scoring of Objective Tests and Concomitant Self-Inetruotion," Journal of Psychology, XXIX (January, 1950), HT-W?T 11

40

Thorndlke, Edward L., "Reward and Punishment." Journal of Experimental Psychology. X {April, 1932), 10, i»-S$T

, "The tow of Effect," AltriM. ImliStf1 of liyS Qlofgsr, XXXXX (April, 1927); *12-222.

Tolman, Edward C., "Cognitive Mapa in Hate and Men," Psychological Review, LV (July, 19*8), 189-200.

. and C. H. Hoiueik, "Degrees of Hunger. Reward and Non-Reward, and Maze Learning in Rats,

W W ! w m S B ,

Trowbridge, H. H. and H. Cason, "An Experimental Study of Thorndike's Theory of Learning." Journal of General Psychology, VII (Ootober, 1932), 245-288.

Publications of Learned Organizations

Quthrle, Sdwln Terns of vmen Study of Education, 1942,

Unpublished Materials

Houston, R« C., "The Function of Knowledge of Results in Learning a Complex Motor Skill, unpublished master*® thesis, Departwent of Psychology, Morthwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, 1947.

Standardised Tests

fearlook

Sullivan

Teraan, L. M. and M. A. Merrill, Stanford-Blnet Scalet Iftnual for the Third Bevlsioni Ejri BSugKtonMi ?f lTnC oSpaiyTT9^tT

st on

Veehsler, B., Wechsler Manual, Hew Tori 19*9 •


Recommended