+ All Categories
Home > Documents > a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

Date post: 02-Mar-2018
Category:
Upload: meylinchoy
View: 217 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 14

Transcript
  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    1/14

    A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Ethical Orientations and Willingness to Sacrifice EthicalStandards: China versus Peru

    Author(s): Christopher J. Robertson, Bradley J. Olson, K. Matthew Gilley and Yongjian

    Bao

    Source: Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 81, No. 2 (Aug., 2008), pp. 413-425

    Published by: Springer

    Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/25482223

    Accessed: 29-05-2016 22:18 UTC

    R F R N S

    Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:

    http://www.jstor.org/stable/25482223?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents

    You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at

    http://about.jstor.org/terms

    JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted

    digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about

    JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    Springeris collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of BusinessEthics

    This content downloaded from 190.216.182.133 on Sun, 29 May 2016 22:18:29 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    2/14

    Journal ofBusiness Ethics (2008) 81:413-425 ? Springer 2007

    DOI 10.1007A10551-007-9504-3

    A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Ethical

    Orientations and Willingness to Sacrifice

    Ethical Standards: China Versus Peru

    Christopher J. Robertson

    Bradley J. Olson

    K. Matthew Gilley

    Yongjian Bao

    ABSTP^ACT. Despite an increase in international busi

    ness ethics research in recent years, the number of studies

    focused on Latin America and China has been deficient.

    As trade among Pacific Rim nations increases, an

    understanding of the ethical beliefs of the people in this

    region of the world will become increasingly important.

    In the current study 208 respondents from Peru and

    China are queried about their ethical ideologies, firm

    practices, and commitment to organizational perfor

    mance. The empirical results reveal that Chinese workers

    are more relativistic and less idealistic than their Peruvian

    counterparts. One explanation for the disparity between

    these two groups is likely the variation in collectivism that

    can be traced to different levels of importance across in

    groups and outgroups. In addition to a summary of the

    results, future research directions and managerial impli

    cations are discussed.

    KEY WORDS: culture, ethics, Peru, China

    The power, prowess, and potential of the Chinese

    economy have been an undeniably hot topic in the

    cross-cultural management literature. And for good

    reason: China is the most populous nation in the

    world with over 1.3 billion people, and the Chinese

    economy, now one of the world's largest, has grown

    at a rate of over 8% for 3 years in a row. The

    Chinese presence is truly global and one of the

    more recent focal areas of Chinese trade and

    investment has been Latin America. Between 1999

    and 2004 overall trade between China and Latin

    America has increased by over 900% (Lloso, 2005).

    In 2004 alone approximately $20 billion, or nearly

    half of all overseas Chinese investment, was destined

    for Latin America. Chinese officials have also

    discussed the possibility of investing an impressive

    $100 billion in building up the infrastructure in

    Latin America (Lloso, 2005). The already impressive

    relations between China and Latin America are

    indeed poised for phenomenal growth in the coming

    decades.

    Peru in particular has cultivated a strong rela

    tionship with China. Formal trade relations between

    Peru and China go back 30 years: trade spiked in

    2004 when Peru formally recognized China as a

    market economy. The November 2003 signing of an

    agreement between Peru and China to boost tour

    ism and trade, resulting in China's designation of

    Peru as an official destination for Chinese tourism,

    has also fostered economic growth. In 2004 the

    value of trade between these two nations was

    $1.75 billion. This level is expected to increase more

    than 12% in 2005 to over $2 billion (Latin American

    News Digest, 2004). Trade between China and Peru

    is also a two-way street, as signified by the fact that

    China is the second most important destination for

    Peruvian exports, behind the United States, and

    China could potentially pass the U.S. in the near

    future as the top Peruvian export market.

    Scholarly research of Chinese and Peruvian cul

    tural values has been varied with China receiving the

    lion's share of research attention since joining the

    World Trade Organization in 2001. Researchers

    have focused on a number of Chinese management

    topics such as guanxi, foreign direct investment

    strategy, work values, and ethical beliefs (Luo, 2002;

    Redfern, 2005; Ralston et al., 1997). The number

    of empirical studies in which Chinese managers and

    employees are compared to Western counterparts

    has also been growing at an impressive rate. Yet

    despite China's trade presence and influence in Latin

    America, cross-cultural studies focused on China and

    This content downloaded from 190.216.182.133 on Sun, 29 May 2016 22:18:29 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    3/14

    414 Christopher J. Robertson et al.

    trade partners such as Peru have been virtually

    nonexistent (Arruda, 1997). This has been the case

    with business ethics research, an area that is still

    deficient in empiricism and in cross-national studies

    (Arruda, 1997; Robertson and Crittenden, 2003).

    Thus we have elected to assess differences in

    employee ethical orientations between China and

    Peru. Peru was selected due to its membership with

    China in the Asia-Pacific Economic Cooperation

    (APEC) and its significant level of trade with China

    (http://www.apec.org). Also, based on prior studies

    of Peruvian culture, it appears that Peruvian values

    tend to be somewhat representative of the cultural

    values of the Andean region of South America

    (Lenartowicz and Johnson, 2002). The overall

    objective of this study is therefore to examine and

    compare the beliefs of Chinese and Peruvian

    employees related to ethical orientations and will

    ingness to sacrifice ethical standards.

    In the next section of this article an analysis of

    Chinese and Peruvian cultural values, with an eye

    toward ethical orientations, will be presented. This is

    followed by the development of formal hypotheses,

    a description of the methods, and a discussion of the

    statistical results. The article concludes with an

    assessment of future research directions and mana

    gerial implications.

    Literature review and hypotheses

    The burgeoning body of cross-cultural research has

    yielded strong evidence that documents the exis

    tence of cultural differences of managers from dif

    ferent nations (Hofstede, 1997; Husted, 1999;

    Lenartowicz and Johnson 2002; Robertson et al.,

    2003; Trompenaars 1994). While notable differences

    have been found in such aspects of culture as per

    ceptions of power, concern for time, ability to cope

    with uncertainty, regard for material goods, display

    of emotions, change, and group orientation (i.e.,

    Hofstede and Bond, 1988; Rawwas et al., 1994),

    very little empirical work has focused on cross

    national ethical beliefs or ideologies (Robertson et

    al., 2003). This dearth of empirical business ethics

    research has been more punctuated in the develop

    ing world, especially areas that have been tradition

    ally underrepresented in management research such

    as Latin America (Arruda, 1997; Lenartowicz and

    Johnson, 2002; Marshall 2003). In fact, in a recent

    review by O'Fallon and Butterfield (2005), of the 19

    studies from 1996-2003 that examine cross-cultural

    differences in ethical decision-making, only three

    incorporate Latin American samples.

    Despite the geographical gaps in empirical studies

    of business ethics, in the past two decades significant

    progress has been made toward a deeper under

    standing of the ethical decision-making process

    (Hosmer, 2000; Trevino, 1986). Business ethics

    scholars have examined a multitude of influential

    factors including, but not limited to, individual

    values, moral intensity, situational factors, moral

    philosophies, cultural values, political inclination,

    and economic variables (Husted, 1999; Jones, 1991;

    Ralston et al., 1997; Rest, 1986).

    The concept of ethical ideology has been one area

    of business ethics research that has received minimal

    attention in cross-cultural research. Empirical studies

    in the management and marketing fields, mostly

    performed with purely American samples, have found

    ethical judgments are mediated by the individual's

    ethical ideology. In one study, Vitell et al. (1993)

    found that more idealistic members showed higher

    levels of honesty and integrity than their less idealistic

    and more relativistic counterparts. A more recent

    study of Russian and American managers found that

    Russian managers were both more idealistic and more

    relativistic than their American colleagues, and that

    both idealism and relativism affect the individual's

    willingness to sacrifice ethics (Robertson et al., 2003).

    The ethical ideologies of idealism and relativism

    have received considerable attention in the realm of

    business ethics (Ferrell et al., 2000; Robertson et al.,

    2003). Forsyth (1980) developed a measurement tool

    for idealism and relativism that set the foundation for

    future empirical studies. According to Rawwas et al.

    (1994, p. 228), a high score on Forsyth's idealism

    scale signifies that an individual or group believes

    more in the moral absolute of specific actions or

    behaviors in determining what course of action one

    should follow. Thus, idealism refers to the degree

    to which an individual believes that the right

    decision can be made in an ethically tenuous situa

    tion. This is essentially the deontological perspective

    that embodies concern for others' welfare when

    evaluating alternatives.

    The notion of relativism, conversely, refers to

    the rejection of universal rules in making ethical

    This content downloaded from 190.216.182.133 on Sun, 29 May 2016 22:18:29 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    4/14

    A Cross-Cultural Comparison 415

    judgments. People who espouse relativism make

    decisions on a situationalfy specific basis. That is,

    high-relativists evaluate the current situation and use

    this as the basis for making a judgment (Robertson

    et al., 2003). High relativists place little emphasis on

    laws and regulations, and low relativists, on the other

    hand, believe that standard rules can be applied

    regardless of the issue at hand (Rawwas et al., 1994).

    Relativism, however, does not create a sort of cha

    otic or anarchic front: with relativism, there is still

    order and rules, just not universality, and thus, many

    ways to solve situations (Warren et al., 2004).

    Although very little research has focused on

    Chinese ethical judgments, one recent study found

    that the Chinese appear to score high in relativism

    (Redfern, 2005). Based on the vertical collectivist

    culture in China (Bhagat et al., 2002), these pre

    hminary results are not surprising. A collectivist

    culture refers to a society that values unity and

    cohesion, encourages loyalty within groups, and

    discourages disharmony (Hofstede, 1993), and ver

    tically collective societies stress top?down authority.

    This does not imply, however, that those immersed

    in collectivism will not engage in actions of dishar

    mony or deceit in particular situations. In fact,

    Triandis (2004) has found that cultures steeped in

    vertical collectivism are more apt to be deceptive if

    this dishonesty benefits the ingroup. A vertical col

    lectivistic society emphasizes the ingroup while still

    recognizing the importance of submitting to

    authority (Tjosvold et al., 2001). Those in the lower

    ranks may be asked to comply with unethical actions

    based on this respected structure, while those in

    outgroups may receive unwarranted actions from

    those in the ingroup based on status of hierarchy. In

    addition, guanxi, which is culturally specific to the

    Chinese society, can create disharmony or deceit

    within certain groups. Studies have shown that

    although guanxi can help those within the ingroup

    to succeed professionally, those within the outgroups

    normally receive the greatest harm from this cul

    turally derived relationship (Warren et al., 2004).

    Although Latin America traditionally has been

    classified as a collectivistic culture (Hofstede, 1997;

    Trompenaars, 1994), the degree to which Peruvian

    collectivism compares to Chinese collectivism is not

    clear. Although cross-cultural research has gained

    invaluable insight from Hofstede (1980) and others,

    such as Triandis (1995), we must be cautious in

    assuming that the behaviors of those in a specified

    collectivist country will mirror the actions of those

    in different collectivistic nations. For example, the

    added dimension of guanxi appears to accelerate and

    strengthen the relativistic qualities of the Chinese

    culture, whereas, Latin societies may not have such

    influential networks. Lenartowicz and Johnson

    (2002) provide strong support that examining

    regional/country differences is essential even when

    cultural similarities have been noted by past research.

    These researchers found that among managers from

    12 Latin American nations, the Peruvian group

    ranked highest, relative to its peers, along an indi

    vidualistic-oriented dimension titled self-direction.

    Moreover, in a recent qualitative study of Chinese

    managers it was concluded that some respondents

    justified their actions, such as bribery to government

    officials to receive some sort of preferential treat

    ment, which is against formal laws, based on their

    desire to maintain a guanxi relationship (Warren

    et al., 2004). Thus, the guanxi relationship, which is

    prominent in China and unknown in Peru, requires

    those within this network to adjust their ethical

    judgments based on the situation at hand, which is a

    classic form of relativism. Based on this analysis:

    Hypothesis 1 Chinese employees will be more

    Relativistic than their Peruvian counterparts

    when faced with ethical dilemmas.

    Idealism, which refers to the degree of concern

    for morals ideals and a concern for others' welfare, is

    less consistent with collectivistic values such as those

    linked with relativism (Robertson et al., 2003). The

    Chinese culture incorporates Confucian teachings,

    which certainly aligns with idealistic beliefs (Red

    fern, 2005). Yet, our previous discussions of vertical

    collectivism and guanxi and its double-edge nature

    creates both cooperative peace (ingroups) and con

    tentious needs (outgroups) depending on the spec

    trum of the relationship. Thus, with such

    relationship networks, would there be as much

    concern for another's welfare as there would be in

    collectivistic societies?

    For those immersed in this complex network,

    there may be a paradox within the Chinese culture

    based their collectivistic culture and guanxi

    relationships. There is a recent study that found

    that Chinese managers who were less influenced by

    This content downloaded from 190.216.182.133 on Sun, 29 May 2016 22:18:29 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    5/14

    416 Christopher J. Robertson et al.

    the outside world were less idealistic than those

    with greater external influences (Redfern, 2005). In

    addition, one study found that Asian decision-makers

    have lower concern in ethical decision-making for

    external stakeholders (Jackson et al., 2000). There is

    also support that when Chinese managers were

    confronted with conflict from outside groups, their

    strategy was to embarrass the external group in order

    to influence the Chinese manager's position. Thus,

    there may be a dilemma within the Chinese society

    between the collectivist and Confucist teachings of

    total harmony and peace and the pull of guanxi

    relationships; they may accept the paradox as a Yin

    and Yang. Having the latter perspective would

    bring assurance that their ethical decision-making

    was appropriately justified.

    Consistent with idealism is the notion of integrity,

    which emphasizes overall honesty in decision-mak

    ing, and has been closely associated with Chinese and

    Peruvian societies (Holt et al., 1994; Lenartowicz and

    Johnson 2002). Individualistic values may also play a

    role in the worldview of idealists. Individualism tends

    to be more prevalent in a free-market economy, and

    Peru may be experiencing a cultural shift due to a

    number of capitalist policies implemented in recent

    years. Moreover, idealistic individuals believe that

    there is a morally correct alternative that will not harm

    others. Thus less idealistic individuals may make

    decisions without reference to its impact on others. In

    Lenartowicz and Johnson's (2002) study of Latin

    American values, Peru scored highest along the

    integrity dimension, compared to civility, self-direc

    tion and drive. Further, Peru's ranking on Transpar

    ency International's Corruption Perceptions Index

    (CPI) for 2005 was 65th, while China ranked 78th,

    which suggests a more idealistic culture relative to

    daily business corruption as measured by the CPI

    (Transparency International, 2005). Thus, based on

    the above linkages, the following hypothesis has been

    developed:

    Hypothesis 2 Peruvian employees will be more

    Idealistic than their Chinese counterparts when

    faced with ethical dilemmas.

    An additional area of business ethics research that

    has gradually received attention is the analysis of

    common business practices (Robertson and Fadil,

    1999). In the United States, many firms now have

    formal codes of ethics. While different countries tend

    to have their own distinct moral paradigms, the

    characteristics of individual managers and workers

    help shape a nation's overall worldview. Idealism and

    relativism both possess intuitive links to the willing

    ness to sacrifice ethical standards for individual and

    firm financial rewards. Collectivistic individuals, like

    those who are highly idealistic, typically believe that

    any means are justified by end results (Rawwas et al.,

    1994). Relativists, with their situational ethics, ap

    pear to be more focused on the current situation

    rather than end results. Relativists also tend to be

    more inclined to sacrifice personal moral standards

    for the perceived norms of the group. Specific to the

    U.S. versus Peru situation is the idea of a tradeoff

    between profitability and ethicality. Robertson et al.

    (2003), in their study of ethical beliefs between U.S.

    and Russian managers, found that in both samples,

    relativism and idealism each had a positive effect on

    the willingness to sacrifice ethical standards for

    individual and firm financial rewards. Unfortunately,

    research on ethical ideologies has been neglected in

    developing countries, although such settings could

    provide rich data concerning the problems of sub

    stantial growth and ethical matters. Ethical dilemmas

    would be inherent for those immersed in collectiv

    istic societies when collectivist-promoted harmony

    clashed with attractive profits gleaned from a flowing

    economic society. Although prior empirical research

    of ethical ideologies and ethical business practices is

    extremely limited, we have found no evidence that

    contradicts the findings of Robertson et al. (2003)

    and we therefore find it consistent to theorize that the

    relationship between ideology and sacrificing of

    moral standards will hold up in the Peruvian and

    Chinese samples. Thus,

    Hypothesis 3 In Peru and China, relativism will

    have a positive effect on the willingness to sacri

    fice ethical standards for individual and firm

    financial rewards.

    Hypothesis 4 In Peru and China, idealism will

    have a positive effect on the willingness to sacri

    fice ethical standards for individual and firm

    financial rewards.

    Moreover, due to the proximity in corruption

    scores on the CPI scale and a lack of evidence that

    This content downloaded from 190.216.182.133 on Sun, 29 May 2016 22:18:29 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    6/14

    A Cross-Cultural Comparison 417

    suggests a significant gap in the relationship between

    the sacrificing of standards, we anticipate no differ

    ence between the Chinese and Peruvian groups:

    Hypothesis 5 There will be no significant differ

    ence between Chinese and Peruvian employees

    when faced with sacrificing ethical standards for

    individual and firm financial rewards.

    Research method

    Sample and sampling procedure

    In order to empirically test each hypothesis, primary

    data was collected from Peruvian and Chinese

    employees using a survey-based methodology. The

    Chinese employees were from a major urban area in

    Mainland China, while the Peruvian employees

    were from the largest city in the country. The aim

    was to gather data from Mainland China, since

    Mainland Chinese employees would likely have

    been reared with more traditional cultural values

    than those residing in places with long-standing

    exposure to Western influences, such as Hong Kong

    or Taiwan (Olson et al., 2007). Surveys were ini

    tially written in English and then translated into

    Spanish and Chinese by native speakers. To ensure

    appropriate translation, the surveys were then back

    translated from Spanish and Chinese to English by a

    separate translator, and appropriate changes were

    then made before final distribution to the partici

    pants (Alreck and Settle, 1995).

    Respondents came from a number of firms in

    various industries. Surveys were distributed to

    respondents attending executive education or

    continuing education classes in Peru and China. In

    each case, one of the researchers met the employees

    in a group setting and distributed the surveys.

    Respondents were told that their participation was

    voluntary and that their responses were completely

    anonymous. Any potential foreign (not Chinese or

    Peruvian) respondents were dismissed from partici

    pation. Respondents were allowed unlimited time to

    complete the surveys. When all respondents had

    finished filling out the surveys the on-site researcher

    then collected the complete set for the participating

    group. During the data collection phase, we targeted

    a sample size of 60 respondents from each country,

    which is consistent with the sampling protocol set

    forth by Malhotra et al. (1996). After data collection

    was complete, the sample included responses from

    64 Peruvian and 144 Chinese employees. While

    conducting our statistical analyses, the usable

    responses ranged from 49 to 64 for the Peruvian

    group and 127-144 for the Chinese contingent.

    About 63% of the Peruvian and 73% of the Chinese

    respondents were married, just over one-half occu

    pied management positions, and the modal age

    (measured with a categorical variable) was in the 30

    39 year-old range.

    M easures

    We elected to follow the research design employed

    by Robertson et al. (2003) in their cross-cultural

    study of ethical orientations between the United

    States and Russia. Replication of a previously

    employed research method provided us with two

    distinct advantages: (1) we utilized a valid and

    established scale and (2) we fostered comparability of

    results between the prior and current studies. Idealism

    and relativism were measured using the scale origi

    nally published by Forsyth (1980). Idealism was

    measured using 10 items, and relativism was mea

    sured using nine items. Idealism questions included

    items such as One should never psychologically or

    physically harm another and The dignity and

    welfare of people should be the most important

    concern in any society. Examples of relativism

    questions are What is ethical varies from one situ

    ation and society to another and What is ethical

    for everyone can never be resolved since what is

    moral or immoral is up to the individual. The

    reliability for idealism was .74 for both Peru and

    China, while the reliability for relativism was .69 for

    Peru and .56 for China (Cronbach, 1951), and sta

    tistical results suggest no concerns with abnormal

    distributions.

    Employees' Willingness to Sacrifice Ethical Standards

    for Individual and Firm Financial Rewards was measured

    using four items developed specifically for this study.

    Respondents were asked to indicate the extent to

    which: (1) making money is more important than

    running an honest business; (2) investors should be

    more interested in their ROI than in creating social

    This content downloaded from 190.216.182.133 on Sun, 29 May 2016 22:18:29 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    7/14

    418 Christopher J. Robertson et al.

    good; (3) firms are successful based on financial gains

    instead of moral accomphshments; and (4) top

    management should be concerned with increasing

    shareholder wealth instead of business ethics. Re

    sponses were coded on a five-point scale, where

    1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree. The

    reliability of this measure was .76 for Peru and .68 for

    China, and statistical results suggest no concerns with

    abnormal distributions.

    We controlled for the extent to which the

    respondents' organizations had a formal system of ethics

    in place. Respondents were asked to indicate their

    level of agreement (1 = strongly disagree to

    5 = strongly agree) with each of the following: (1)

    My firm has standards and procedures in place, such

    as codes of ethics; (2) my firm has high-level per

    sonnel responsible for ethics compliance programs;

    (3) my firm has ethics training programs in place; (4)

    my firm has in place systems that monitor, audit, and

    report misconduct; (5) my firm is consistent with the

    enforcement of ethics standards; and (6) my firm

    focuses on ethics training programs. The reliability

    of this measure was .79 for Peru and .81 for China,

    and statistical results suggest no concerns with

    abnormal distributions.

    We also incorporated a variety of other control

    variables into our regression models to enhance

    confidence in our findings. To control for firm

    level effects when predicting employees' feelings

    regarding the profit/ethics tradeoff, we included a

    categorical measure of firm size based on number of

    employees. In larger firms, employees may be less

    likely to be intimately involved with and aware of

    key ethical issues. Conversely, in smaller firms,

    employees may be more likely to see the effects of

    the firm on key stakeholders, thus enhancing their

    awareness of potential profit-ethics tradeoffs.

    Therefore, we feel that controlling for firm size is

    important. A similar argument may be made with

    regard to another of our controls, hierarchical level

    within the organization. Higher-level managers are

    more likely to have information that crosses internal

    departmental boundaries, as well as the boundary

    between the company and its external stakeholders.

    Thus, an individual's hierarchical level may affect

    his or her awareness of the profit-ethics tradeoff,

    necessitating its inclusion as a control variable. Level

    within the organizational hierarchy was measured

    by asking respondents to indicate whether their

    position was in senior executive, middle manage

    ment, junior management, non-management, or

    staff. Several additional individual-level factors were

    included as controls in our regression models,

    including education level, marital status, gender, and

    age. With regard to education level, those with

    greater educational attainment may be more likely

    to have been exposed to ethics curricula and

    training that may have addressed and enhanced their

    ethical awareness. Respondents were asked to

    indicate their highest level of education based on

    the following possible values: high school, some

    college, college graduate, some graduate school, and

    graduate degree. Prior studies have also included

    marital status and age in cross-national ethical

    decision-making analyses (i.e., Robertson et al.,

    2003). Marital status was measured simply with an

    indication of married versus non-married status.

    Age was a categorical variable where ages were

    grouped into 10-year ranges from 1 = 20-29 years

    of age through 5 = 60 and older.

    Results

    Means, standard deviations, and zero-order correla

    tions are found in Table I.

    To test hypotheses 1, 2, and 5, f-tests were per

    formed in an attempt to determine whether Peru

    vian and Chinese employees differed significantly

    with regard to relativism, idealism, and a willingness

    to sacrifice ethical standards for financial gain (the

    profit/ethics tradeoff'). As noted in Figure 1, sig

    nificant differences were present between Peruvian

    and Chinese employees on all three variables. This

    provides support for Hypotheses 1 and 2, but refutes

    Hypothesis 5. Chinese employees appear to be sig

    nificantly more relativistic than their Peruvian

    counterparts. Thus, it appears that the Chinese

    group places more emphasis on the specific situation

    and the relationships among the individuals in

    volved, when confronted with an ethical dilemma.

    Hypothesis 2 suggested that Peruvian employees will

    be more idealistic than Chinese employees when

    faced with ethical dilemmas. Our results indicate that

    this is, indeed, the case, and it can be deduced that

    Peruvian employees believe more in moral absolutes

    and have stricter, more cogent, definitions of

    morality. Finally, Hypothesis 5 theorized that there

    This content downloaded from 190.216.182.133 on Sun, 29 May 2016 22:18:29 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    8/14

    A Cross-Cultural Comparison 419

    TABLE I

    Descriptive statistics and correlations

    ManSD12345 6 7 89

    Peruvian respondents

    1. Profit/ethics tradeoff 7.98 4.16

    2. Relativism 25.86 6.72 .31

    3. Idealism 41.70 5.52 -.04 .22

    4. Co. has ethics procedures 22.65 4.16 -.06 .10 .11

    5. Marital status 0.63 0.49 -.07 .10 -.03 -.11

    6. Gender 0.36 0.48 .04 -.04 .03 -.02 -.20

    7. Hierarchical level 2.40 1.26 .00 .06 .06 -.00 -.18 .07

    8. Age 2.98 0.96 -.13 .06 -.03 .19 .41*** -.31* .01

    9. Education 3.38 0.71 .18 -.13 .08 .06 -.03 .23 -.17 .17

    10. Company Size 4.23 1.38 .09 .04 .20 -.04 -.12 .05 .44 .22 .17

    Chinese respondents

    1. Profit/ethics tradeoff 9.96 3.75

    2. Relativism 27.99 5.33 .04

    3. Idealism 36.57 6.37 -.19 .07

    4. Co. has ethics procedures 21.24 5.17 -.07 .06 .17*

    5. Marital status 0.73 0.45 -.09 -.05 -.15 -.01

    6. Gender 0.67 0.47 .17 .06 -.17 -.12 .02

    7. Hierarchical level 2.08 0.94 -.08 .07 .04 .00 .37 -.18

    8. Age 2.06 0.93 .06 -.00 .15 .04 -.50*** .08 -.30***

    9. Education 2.34 0.91 .02 .06 -.00 -.14 .18 .07 -.03 -.07

    10. Company Size 2.26 1.28 .21* -.05 -.03 .10 -.22* .14 -.57*** .22** .09

    *j?

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    9/14

    420 Christopher J. Robertson et al.

    41.70

    36.57

    Relativism Idealism Willingness to

    Sacrifice Ethical

    Standards for

    Financial Gain

    llll Mean for Chinese Sub-Sam ple, n =144

    Mean for Peruvian Sub-Sample, n = 64

    Figure 1. Means of relativism, idealism, and willingness

    to sacrifice ethical standards for financial gain for Peru

    vian and Chinese sub-samples, Note: The means of all

    three variables for the Peruvian and Chinese sub-sam

    ples are significantly different at the p < .05 level.

    backgrounds has perplexed many managers (Hu

    sted, 1999; Robertson et al., 2003). This moral

    dimension of managerial decision-making, as op

    posed to the economic dimension, is likely to be

    come more of a concern for corporate leaders,

    organizational researchers and society as a whole in

    the future (Luo, 2002). Globalization has indeed

    forced more managers to confront ethical percep

    tions and beliefs that are inconsistent with their

    own moral codes (Velasquez 2000). Great care

    must be employed to fully understand the processes

    and procedures of firms with different cultural

    backgrounds. Thus, managers must be somewhat

    flexible with members of different cultures in eth

    ical situations, yet they must remain true to the

    underlying ethical principles of their organization.

    The present study's emphasis on Chinese versus

    Peruvian ethical orientations sheds light on two

    nations that have recently entered the global mar

    ketplace and have forged strong economic and

    political relationships.

    Our results have revealed some interesting find

    ings about employees in China and Peru. First, as

    predicted, it appears that relativism is more prevalent

    in China than in Peru. It is perhaps the Confucian

    collectivism combined with a deeply seeded tradi

    tion of guanxi that has stimulated this moral para

    digm. When compared to the earlier study by

    Robertson et al. (2003) China's mean relativism

    score, 27.99, is actually a tick below that of Russia

    (28.75). Perhaps it is the moral acceptance of

    behaviors such as price fixing and data manipulation

    in Russia that bolsters the Russian relativism (Puffer

    and McCarthy, 1995). The rationale and significance

    of these scores could be a fruitful area for future

    research. Second, the Peruvian sample scored sig

    nificantly higher along the idealism dimension when

    compared to their Chinese counterparts. This

    underlying value of idealism appears to be particu

    larly strong when compared to the Russian and

    American mean scores from Robertson et al.'s

    (2003) previous analysis with a Peruvian mean score

    of 41.70, close to three points above the Russian and

    American mean scores. The lack of full support for

    Hypotheses 3 and 4 supports this distinction. Indeed

    it appears that there may be multiple shades of ide

    alism and relativism. Moreover, the relationship

    between in-group versus out-group collectivism and

    relativism may have played a role in the perception

    of ethicality in our study. Future researchers may

    want to probe deeper into these constructs in the

    quest for sub-cultural differences within Peru and

    Ch ina.

    Guanxi clearly plays a role in the overall Chinese

    value system. Guanxi relationships are predicated

    upon loyalty to those within the ingroup; thus,

    when individuals are faced with ethically tenuous

    situations, decisions may be based more on those

    who are affected (such as ingroup or outgroup) than

    the laws at hand. It is guanxi that creates the envi

    ronment for ingroups and outgroups. There are

    privileges to those within the ingroups, such as

    favoritism, personal benefits, access to resources and

    cooperation, and discrimination toward outgroups

    (Ashforth and Mael, 1989). Those within the

    ingroup strive for harmony, and respect the feelings

    of others within this group when engaging in deci

    sion-making. Thus, there is a double-edge sword

    component to this social network (Warren et al.,

    2004). In addition, those in guanxi relationships are

    guided by the norms and rules within that system

    that may supersede formal laws. While guanxi is

    generally thought of as a strictly Chinese construct,

    the extent to which it exists, in form, in other cul

    This content downloaded from 190.216.182.133 on Sun, 29 May 2016 22:18:29 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    10/14

    TABLE II

    e l a t i v i s m a n d i d e a l i s m o n t h e w i l l i n g n e s s t o s a c r i f i c e e t h i c a l s t a n d a r d s f o r f i n a n c i a l g a i n a

    P e r u v i a n m a n a g e r s C h i n e s e m a n a g e r s

    P r e d ic t o r s a n d c o n t r o l s C o lu m n C o n t r o ls o n l y C o l u m n 2 F u ll m o d e l C o l u m n 3 C o n t r o l s o n l y C o l u m n 4 F u l l m o d e l

    C o m p a n y h a s e t h i c s p o l i c i e s / p r o c e d u r e s . 1 0 . 0 4 ? . 1 0 . 0 4

    M r u 1 1 02 0 0 0 4

    Gender .10 .09 -.10 -.07

    Hierarchical level .01 - .01 .04 .07 Q

    Age 24 24 09 12

    E d u c a t i o n . 2 3 . 2 8 . 0 4 . 0 6 q

    C o m p a n y s i z e .1 0 . 0 6 . 2 5 . 2 3 I r

    Rela

    I d e a l i s m - . 0 8 - . 2 6 * q

    F full model ) .68 1.33 1.70 2.11* |

    35 330

    R2 .10 .23 .11 .16 1

    9407100

    a T h e d e p e n d e n t v a r i a b l e i s T h e W i l l i n g n e s s t o S a c r i f i c e t h i c a l S t a n d a r d s f o r F i n a n c i a l G a i n

    S t a n d a r d i z e d r e g r e s s i o n c o e f f i c i e n t s a r e r e p o r t e d

    * p < . 0 5 , * * p < . 0 1 P e r u v i a n n = 4 9 , C h i n e s e n = 1 0 7

    to

    This content downloaded from 190.216.182.133 on Sun, 29 May 2016 22:18:29 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    11/14

    422 Christopher J. Robertson et al.

    tures is relatively unknown yet worthy of future

    investigation.

    In addition, further research should examine the

    Western influence on China versus the traditional

    effects of guanxi relative to ethical decisions. As

    mentioned previously, there are many positive

    aspects of guanxi, which are in harmony with the

    underlying principles of collectivism, yet such rela

    tionships can also promote unethical actions. The

    controls that Western firms have incorporated in

    their regular business practices to curb unethical

    decisions such as bribery may help to curtail unethical

    guanxi practices. Moreover, national or local Chi

    nese governments may feel pressured by international

    forces to minimize the negative effects so common

    with these exclusive networks (Lee et al., 2001).

    Finally, Triandis (2004) argues that Chinese managers

    who are more educated and exposed to international

    cultures and social settings will be less affected by the

    stringent protocols of guanxi.

    The negative relationship between idealism and

    willingness to sacrifice ethical standards for financial

    gains of Chinese employees provides support that

    those with high idealism will not allow the negative

    aspects of guanxi to counter the positive aspects of

    Confucius collectivism. Idealism within the Chinese

    culture, then, may be an important mitigating factor

    in the double-edged dilemma of guanxi. In addition,

    research shows that in certain instances, Chinese

    employees who engaged in unethical decisions did

    so not for personal financial gain, but out of respect

    for those who were entitled to favors based on the

    guanxi relationship (Warren et al., 2004). In other

    words, there may be many other underlying reasons

    where guanxi will require individuals to act uneth

    ically.

    There are limitations in any study, and one lim

    itation of this study is that the generalizability of our

    results may be somewhat hmited with regard to the

    Peruvian sample because the number of Peruvian

    respondents was less than half the number of Chi

    nese respondents. Thus, generalizing our findings to

    individuals with different functional backgrounds

    may not be appropriate, because of the lack in dis

    parity of our Peruvian group. In addition, given that

    our study is cross-sectional and correlational in nat

    ure, one must view our causal hypotheses with

    caution. Even though there is compelling theoretical

    justification for our assertion that idealism will affect

    the profit/ethics tradeoff, there is still the possibility

    that causation is not occurring. Nevertheless, given

    the theoretical justifications provided in the

    hypothesis development section, we are reasonably

    confident in the causal relationships hypothesized.

    A general criticism of survey research is common

    method variance, because the independent and

    dependent constructs are often measured entirely

    with self-reported data (as was the case here). One

    cannot dismiss Podsakoff and Organ's (1986)

    admonition to avoid measuring the independent and

    dependent variables using the same source, since

    there is a potential for questionable results. Yet,

    common method variance may not be as much of a

    limitation as once thought, because, although com

    mon method variance inflates zero-order correla

    tions, it also increases the shared variance among the

    independent variables (Shaffer et al., 1999). This

    makes it more difficult to find unique, significant

    regression coefficients (Florey and Harrison, 2000)

    and reduces the chance that common method vari

    ance had a major effect on the conclusions of this

    study. Nevertheless, one cannot absolutely rule out

    the possibility that respondents artificially answered

    in a consistent fashion, yielding possibly invalid re

    sults. Another limitation is our use of the number of

    employees as a measure of firms size rather than

    annual total firm revenues. We elected to use the

    former measure due to the potential problem of

    comparing revenue amounts with different levels of

    purchasing power parity.

    A number of regional implications may exist

    based on the findings of this study. In the study

    conducted by Lenartowicz and Johnson (2002), the

    scores of the Peruvian managers along all four

    dimensions studied clustered well with Ecuador and

    Chile, signifying commonalities across the Andean

    region. Therefore it may be plausible to make some

    tentative generalizations about moral value systems

    in Andean South America. While Chile has more of

    a European history with respect to immigration

    patterns, Peru and Ecuador do share a strong com

    mon thread with historical ties to the Inca empire.

    Regarding managerial implications, top manage

    ment must understand that the ethical climate of a

    multinational organization can be extremely fragile.

    The current results impact the communications,

    strategic objectives, and control systems that may be

    appropriate across multinational settings, having

    This content downloaded from 190.216.182.133 on Sun, 29 May 2016 22:18:29 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    12/14

    A Cross-Cultural Comparison 423

    substantial potential importance for the development

    of a country's corporate governance system. Our

    findings also have revealed patterns that could be

    helpful to managers in multinational firms that are in

    the process of developing corporate codes of ethics

    in Peru or China (Robertson and Fadil, 1999).

    Specific, culturally contingent guidelines could be

    created while considering some of the diversity in

    values and beliefs across borders. Any adaptations to

    policies should be reviewed by managers from both

    nations in an attempt to dispel any deep discrepan

    cies in firm policies and practices. Further, actions

    and corrective steps taken by managers will likely

    become more important in establishing, maintain

    ing, or destabilizing a multinational's ethical envi

    ronment. Perhaps the establishment of a more formal

    code will facilitate stronger governance over indi

    vidual behavior in both nations (Garten, 2002).

    Moreover, studies of ethical beliefs can offer a

    window into the core values of a culture. Ethical and

    cultural values tend to be intertwined, and managers

    can incorporate beliefs about idealism and relativism

    into decisions related to human-resource manage

    ment policies such as performance, training, and

    reward systems.

    We believe that the results of this study have

    added breadth and depth to the inadequate empirical

    database of studies that have focused on cross

    cultural ethics between Peru and China. Moreover,

    this study has identified a number of salient future

    research endeavors for ethics scholars. For example,

    future research should examine whether codes of

    ethical conduct and ethics training have the same

    effects on Peruvian and Chinese employees. Scholars

    may also elect to examine the cross-cultural nature of

    behavioral traits such as guanxi in various national

    settings. Depth will also be added to this research

    stream through the replication of studies of ethical

    antecedents such as collectivism and other value

    dim ensions .

    In addition, the current research design could

    be employed in other countries in Latin America

    that have significant levels of trade with China,

    such as Argentina or Brazil. A deeper analysis into

    specific ethical dilemmas may add to our under

    standing of the shape, form, and rhythm of the

    relativism in China and idealism in Peru. Other

    influential factors that may play a causal role in the

    formation of moral ideology, such as religion and

    form of government, could also be examined in

    the future.

    Note

    APEC, founded in 1989. is a trade association that has

    21 member nations which account for one-third of the

    world's population and over 60% of global GDP. Peru and

    Chile are the only South American members of APEC.

    References

    Alreck, P. and R. Settle: 1995, The Survey Research

    Handbook, 2nd Edition (Irwin, Homewood, IL).

    Arruda, M.: 1997, 'Business Ethics in Latin America',

    Journal of Business Ethics 16, 1597-1603.

    Ashforth, B. E. and F. Mael: 1989, 'Social Identity

    Theory and the Organization', Academy of Management

    Review 14(1), 20-39.

    Bhagat, R. S., B. L. Kedia, P. D. Harveston and H. C.

    Triandis: 2002, 'Cultural Variations in the Cross

    Border Transfer of Organizational Knowledge: An

    Integrative Framework', Academy of Management

    Review 27'(2), 204-221.

    Cronbach, L. J.: 1951, 'Coefficient Alpha and the Internal

    Structure of Tests', Psychometrika 16, 297-334.

    Ferrell, O., J Fraedrich and L. Ferrell: 2000, Business

    Ethics: Ethical Decision Making and Cases (Houghton

    Mifflin, Boston).

    Florey, A. and D. Harrison: 2000, 'Responses to Informal

    Accommodation Requests from Employees with Dis

    abilities: Multistudy Evidence on Willingness to Com

    ply', Academy of Management Journal 43, 224-233.

    Forsyth, D.: 1980, 'A Taxonomy of Ethical Ideologies',

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39(1), 175

    184.

    Garten, J.: 2002, 'Corporate Standards: Raise the Bar

    Around the World', Business Week, May 13, 30.

    Hofstede, G: 1980, Culture's Consequences: International

    Differences in Work-Related Values (Beverly Hills, CA,

    Sage).

    Hofstede, G: 1993, 'Cultural Constraints in Management

    Theories', Academy of Management Executive 7, 81-94.

    Hofstede, G: 1997, Cultures and Organizations: Software of

    the Mind (New York, McGraw-Hill).

    Hofstede, G and M. H. Bond: 1988, 'The Confucius

    Connection: From Cultural Roots to Economic

    Growth', Organizational Dynamics 16(4), 5-21.

    Holt, D., D. Ralston and R. Terpstra: 1994, 'Constraints

    on Capitalism in Russia: The Managerial Psyche.

    This content downloaded from 190.216.182.133 on Sun, 29 May 2016 22:18:29 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    13/14

    424 Christopher J. Robertson et al.

    Social Infrastructure and Ideology', California Manage

    ment Review 36(3), 124-141.

    Hosmer, L.: 2000, 'It's Time for Empirical Research in

    Business Ethics', Business Ethics Quarterly 10(1), 233

    242.

    Husted, B.: 1999, 'Wealth, Culture, and Corruption',

    Journal of International Business Studies 30(2), 339

    360 .

    Jackson, T., C. David, S. Deshpande, J. Jones, J. Jo

    seph, K. Lau, K. Matsuno, K. Nakano, H. Park, J.

    Kokoszko, Taka and I. H. Yoshihara: 2000, 'Making

    Ethical Judgements: A Cross-Cultural Management

    Study', Asia Pacific Journal of Management 17, 443

    472.

    Jones, T.: 1991, 'Ethical Decision Making by Individuals

    in Organizations: An Issue-Contingent Model', Acad

    emy of Management Review 16(2), 366-395.

    Latin American News Digest, Peru, China Trade seen at

    $1.75 billion in 2004, December 13, 2004.

    Lee, D., J. Pae and Y. Wong: 2001, 'A Model of Close

    Business Relationships in China (guanxi)', European

    Journal of Marketing 35, 51-65.

    Lenartowicz, T. and J. Johnson: 2002, 'Comparing

    Managerial Values in Twelve Latin American Coun

    tries: An Exploratory Study', Management International

    Review 42(3), 279-307.

    Lloso, A.: 2005, 'Growing Chinese Presence in Latin

    America no Threat: Economic Growth, not Ideology,

    Motivates the Move across the Pacific Ocean', Ed

    monton Journal, October 15, A19.

    Luo, Y.: 2002, 'Corruption and Organization in Asian

    Management Systems', Asia Pacific Journal of Manage

    ment 19, 405-422.

    Malhotra, N. K., J. Agarwal and M. Peterson: 1996,

    'Methodological Issues in Cross-Cultural Marketing

    Research: A State-of-the-Art Review', International

    Marketing Review 13(5), 7-43.

    Marshall, R.: 2003, 'Building Trust Early: The Influence

    of First and Second Order Expectations on Trust in

    International Channels of Distribution', International

    Business Review 12(4), 421-443.

    O'Fallon, M. and K. Butterfield: 2005, 'A Review of the

    Empirical Ethical Decision-Making Literature: 1996

    2003', Journal ofBusiness Ethics 59, 375-413.

    Olson, B., Y. Bao and S. Parayitam: 2007, 'Strategic

    Decision Making within Chinese Firms: The Effects of

    Cognitive Diversity and Trust on Decision Out

    comes', Journal of World Business 42(1), 35-46.

    Podsakoff, P. M. and D. W. Organ: 1986, 'Self-Reports

    in Organization Research: Problems and Prospects',

    Journal of Management 12, 531-544.

    Puffer, S. and D. McCarthy: 1995, 'Finding the Common

    Ground in Russian and American Business Ethics',

    California Management Review 37(2), 29-46.

    Ralston, D. A., D. H. Holt, R. H. Terpstra and Y.

    Kai-Cheng: 1997, 'The Impact of National Culture

    and Economic Ideology on Managerial Work Values:

    A Study of the United States, Russia, Japan, and

    China', Journal of International Business Studies 28(1),

    177-207 .

    Rawwas, M., S. Vitell and J. Al-Khatib: 1994, 'Consumer

    Ethics: The Possible Effects of Terrorism and Civil

    Unrest on the Ethical Values of Consumers', Journal of

    Business Ethics 13, 223-231.

    Redfern, K.: 2005, 'The Influence of Industrialization of

    Ethical Ideology of Managers in the People's Republic

    of China', Cross-Cultural Management 12(2), 30-50.

    Rest, J.: 1986, Moral Development: Advances in Research and

    Theory (Praeger, New York).

    Robertson, C. and P. Fadil: 1999, 'Ethical Decision

    Making in Multinational Organizations: A Culture

    Based Model', Journal of Business Ethics 19, 385-392.

    Robertson, C, K. Gilley and M. Street: 2003, 'The

    Relationship Between Ethics and Firm Practices in

    Russia and The United States', Journal of World Business

    38 , 375-384 .

    Robertson, C. and W. Crittenden: 2003, 'Mapping Moral

    Philosophies: Strategic Implications for Multinational

    Firms', Strategic Management Journal 24, 385-392.

    Shaffer, M. A., D. A. Harrison and K. M. Gilley: 1999,

    'Dimensions, Determinants, and Differences in Expa

    triate Adjustment', Journal of International Business

    Studies 30, 557-581.

    Tjosvold, D., C. Hui and K. S. Law: 2001, 'Constructive

    Conflict in China: Cooperative Conflict as a Bridge

    Between East and West', Journal of World Business

    36(2), 166-183.

    Transparency International: 2005, http://www.transpar

    ency.org.

    Trevino, L.: 1986, 'Ethical Decision Making in Organi

    zations: A Person-Situation Interactionist Model',

    Academy of Management Review 11, 601-617.

    Triandis, H. C: 1995, Individualism and Collectivism

    (Boulder, CO, Westview).

    Triandis, H. C: 2004, 'The Many Dimensions of

    Culture', Academy of Management Executive 18, 88-93.

    Trompenaars, F.: 1994, Riding the Waves of Culture (Irwin,

    New York, NY).

    Vitell, S., S. Nwachukwu and J. Barnes: 1993, 'The

    Effects of Culture on Ethical Decision-Making: An

    Application of Hofstede's Typology', Journal of Business

    Ethics 12, 753-760.

    This content downloaded from 190.216.182.133 on Sun, 29 May 2016 22:18:29 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 7/26/2019 a cross cutlural comparison of ethical ortientations.pdf

    14/14

    A Cross-Cultural Comparison 425

    Velazquez, M.: 2000, 'Globalization and the Failure of

    Ethics', Business Ethics Quarterly 10, 343-352.

    Warren, D. E., T. W. Dunfree and N. Li: 2004, 'Social

    Exchange in China: The Double-Edged Sword of

    Guanxi', Journal of Business Ethics 55, 355-372.

    Christopher f. Robertson

    International Business,

    College of Business Administration,

    Northeastern University,

    313 Hayden Hall, Boston, MA, 02115, U.S.A.

    E-mail: [email protected]

    Bradley J. Olson and Yongjian Bao

    University of Lethbridge,

    Lethbridge, AB, Canada

    K. Matthew Gilley

    Department of Management, Bill Greehey School

    ofBusiness, St. Mary's University,

    One Camino Santa Maria, San Antonio,

    TX, 78228, U.S.A., (210) 436-3705

    E-mail: [email protected]


Recommended