+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Attention deficit disorders: Are we barking up the wrong tree?

Attention deficit disorders: Are we barking up the wrong tree?

Date post: 04-Sep-2016
Category:
Upload: pedro-cabral
View: 214 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
12
Review article Attention deficit disorders: Are we barking up the wrong tree? Pedro Cabral * Pediatric Neurology Unit, CHLO, Estr. do Forte Alto do Duque, 1400 Lisboa, Portugal ARTICLE INFO Article history: Received 15 July 2005 Received in revised form 16 January 2006 Accepted 19 February 2006 Keywords: Attention Hyperactivity Sleep/wake Neurotransmission Prefrontal cortex Co-morbidities ABSTRACT Attention deficit disorder (AAD) and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are very frequent and protean developmental disorders without a definite biologic marker. This review proposes a framework to understand the enlarged spectrum of its manifestations based on current knowledge of the mechanisms underlying arousal and attention variations during sleep/wake cycle. The neuro-modulation’s pivotal role in this process as well as in the fine tuning of synaptic architecture during development must be taken into account when trying to understand the marked fuzziness of the symptoms and the very high prevalence of reported co-morbidities. The series of related interactions includes a cyclic deactivation of the dorso-lateral portion of the prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) during sleep, suspending executive functions, co-occurring with rhythmic periods of decreased noradrenergic tonus. A protracted unbalance in modulation, with catecholaminergic relative deficiency, could explain less-than-optimum waking DLPFC activation and the most important manifestations of ADD. Beside the well documented dopaminergic effects of stimulant medication used in ADD and ADHD, a more important role must be assigned to noradrenaline (NA). At this light hyperactivity and impulsivity are less important dimensions. Rather, an attention deficit spectrum disorder should probably be regarded as a complication of a core defect in prefrontal cortex dependent inhibitory control, underlying inattention. Q 2006 European Paediatric Neurology Society. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction ADHD is a very common 1,2 heterogeneous developmental condition which could probably encompass several different entities. It is, like other developmental disorders, a behaviou- rally defined syndrome, which integrates three dimensions: inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity. There is no definite biologic marker and, genetically, the evidence is still conflict- ing. 2–10 It is a well-documented cause of learning disabilities at school age, of behavioural problems in infants and preschoo- lers, and of social and professional inadequacy in adolescents and adults, with a growing demand for stimulant medication in these groups. 11–14 Claims of increasing incidence of ADD/ ADHD in recent years, perhaps due to changing of speed and technology of information 15 could more probably represent an effect of growing pressure to academic achievement, as identical prevalence has been shown to occur in deeply varied social, economic and cultural sets. 16 Most ADD patients present co-morbidities: 11,17–28 hyper- activity, tics, anxiety, depression, oppositional and conduct disorders, developmental motor coordination and perception disabilities, autistic features and several other specific developmental troubles. The prevalence of co-pathology, so Official Journal of the European Paediatric Neurology Society 1090-3798/$ - see front matter Q 2006 European Paediatric Neurology Society. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.ejpn.2006.02.004 E-mail address: [email protected] EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PAEDIATRIC NEUROLOGY 10 (2006) 66–77
Transcript

E U R O P E A N J O U R N A L O F P A E D I A T R I C N E U R O L O G Y 1 0 ( 2 0 0 6 ) 6 6 – 7 7

Review article

Attention deficit disorders: Are we barking up the wrongtree?

Pedro Cabral*

Pediatric Neurology Unit, CHLO, Estr. do Forte Alto do Duque, 1400 Lisboa, Portugal

A R T I C L E I N F O

Article history:

Received 15 July 2005

Received in revised form

16 January 2006

Accepted 19 February 2006

Keywords:

Attention

Hyperactivity

Sleep/wake

Neurotransmission

Prefrontal cortex

Co-morbidities

1090-3798/$ - see front matter Q 2006 Europedoi:10.1016/j.ejpn.2006.02.004

E-mail address: [email protected]

A B S T R A C T

Attention deficit disorder (AAD) and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are very

frequent and protean developmental disorders without a definite biologic marker. This

review proposes a framework to understand the enlarged spectrum of its manifestations

based on current knowledge of the mechanisms underlying arousal and attention

variations during sleep/wake cycle. The neuro-modulation’s pivotal role in this process as

well as in the fine tuning of synaptic architecture during development must be taken into

account when trying to understand the marked fuzziness of the symptoms and the very

high prevalence of reported co-morbidities.

The series of related interactions includes a cyclic deactivation of the dorso-lateral portion

of the prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) during sleep, suspending executive functions, co-occurring

with rhythmic periods of decreased noradrenergic tonus. A protracted unbalance in

modulation, with catecholaminergic relative deficiency, could explain less-than-optimum

waking DLPFC activation and the most important manifestations of ADD. Beside the well

documented dopaminergic effects of stimulant medication used in ADD and ADHD, a more

important role must be assigned to noradrenaline (NA). At this light hyperactivity and

impulsivity are less important dimensions. Rather, an attention deficit spectrum disorder

should probably be regarded as a complication of a core defect in prefrontal cortex

dependent inhibitory control, underlying inattention.

Q 2006 European Paediatric Neurology Society. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

ADHD is a very common1,2 heterogeneous developmental

condition which could probably encompass several different

entities. It is, like other developmental disorders, a behaviou-

rally defined syndrome, which integrates three dimensions:

inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity. There is no definite

biologic marker and, genetically, the evidence is still conflict-

ing.2–10 It is awell-documented cause of learning disabilities at

school age, of behavioural problems in infants and preschoo-

lers, and of social and professional inadequacy in adolescents

an Paediatric Neurology

and adults, with a growing demand for stimulant medication

in these groups.11–14 Claims of increasing incidence of ADD/

ADHD in recent years, perhaps due to changing of speed and

technology of information15 couldmore probably represent an

effect of growing pressure to academic achievement, as

identical prevalence has been shown to occur in deeply varied

social, economic and cultural sets.16

Most ADD patients present co-morbidities:11,17–28 hyper-

activity, tics, anxiety, depression, oppositional and conduct

disorders, developmental motor coordination and perception

disabilities, autistic features and several other specific

developmental troubles. The prevalence of co-pathology, so

Official Journal of the European Paediatric Neurology Society

Society. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

E U R O P E A N J O U R N A L O F P A E D I A T R I C N E U R O L O G Y 1 0 ( 2 0 0 6 ) 6 6 – 7 7 67

high as to raise the doubt on the existence of the so-called

‘pure’ form of ADD and ADHD, complicates the task of

delineating basic features and does not help the searching

for genetic markers.

Specific developmental disorders present no clear

border separating patients from the normal population,

as opposed to acquired deficits. This was shown by Sally

Shaywitz in dyslexia,29 and even in the so called ‘pervasive

developmental’ (autistic spectrum) disorders, a continuum

with normal performance can be shown in the empathy

ability.30 Moreover, in developmental ADD, three features

should be underlined: symptoms present marked intra-

individual variation during daytime, a different behavioural

expression is to be expected in growth until adulthood,

and there can even be peculiar aspects of genetic

transmission.4

2. Physiopathologic aspects

Multiple hypothesis1,2,26,27,31–35 have been proposed to

explain the enlarged spectrum of symptoms pertaining to

ADD and ADHD, similarly recognized in many monogenic

syndromes, and also as a sequel of trauma, hypoxic–

ischemic encephalopathy, static and progressive neurologi-

cal conditions, and adverse effects of pregnancy and

premature birth.36,37

Until recently, the most widespread hypothesis were

taking into account the well documented dopaminergic

effects of stimulant medication35,38–45 and the role of

dopamine (DA) in prefrontal cognitive processes46 and in

reward mechanisms.27,47 However, some pitfalls in accept-

ing DA as the sole or even the most important neurotrans-

mitter implicated in ADD/ADHD should be pointed. In the

last few years48 a shift has been challenging previous

believes on these topics.47–59 Heterogeneity at the gen-

etic,10,48,53,54,60 physiopathologic31,35,49,55,56,61 and clinical

levels11,49,54,55,62,63 has been commonly accepted. Most

noticeably, very important interactions exist among the

main neurotransmitters involved in diffuse cortical neuro-

modulation.64–68 These dynamic interactions play a docu-

mented role in ongoing cognitive processing, and play a role

in shaping patterns during development. Also, there is most

probably no such thing as a perfectly targeted drug, exerting

its effects on only one system or neurotransmitter.34,69–74 DA

plays a unique role in cognitive functions.32,46,75–77 Its

deficiency in Parkinson disease (a pathologic model perhaps

overused for the study of dopamine cognitive effects) has its

counterpoint in psychotic symptoms in peaks of DA dose.

There is strong correlation of hyperdopaminergic states with

psychosis. As a ‘salience detector’ DA can probably act

either as an ‘attractor’ or as a ‘distractor’.32,78–82 This should

be an important issue, as the role of a neurotransmitter can

change substantially depending on circumstances and stage

of development.64,83,84 It could even be that the arousal state

could modulate the DA circuitry activation, with co-variation

of attraction. The DA absolute role as a motivational

salience detector and reward mechanisms mediator could

probably not justify the whole set of manifestations that

encompass ADD/ADHD.27,47,48,50,55 Also the emergence of

new drugs with more marked noradrenergic effect or of

drugs with less stimulant or non-stimulant effect85–95 can

turn more difficult to accommodate a ‘hypodopaminergic’

model as a sole explanation of ADD/ADHD.47,52,96–100

As new technologies of brain imaging allow functional

and metabolite exams for the purpose of studying circuitry

instead of isolated structures,1,35,101–103 complex dynamic

relations between neurotransmitters,104 the rate of acti-

vation of specific circuits and the way it can be modified by

therapy105–109 need to be addressed. Appearing to work

independently of pre or postsynaptic sensibility modification

mechanisms (as shown by their immediate effect and

absence of tolerance110–114), these stimulants have well-

known cathecolaminergic effects, both DA and NA being

involved. The issue of whether one of them, or both, play a

role in attention mechanisms appears to be an important

one.32–34,97–99

Dopamine, acetylcholine, serotonine and norepinephrine

are the major neurotransmitters that diffusely modulate the

cerebral cortex. From the brainstem, they modify cortical

excitability in different areas depending on rhythms of sub-

cortical pacing. Specifically, serotonine (SE) and norepi-

nephrine (NE) have been shown to present daily rate

variations, regulating sleep/wake cycles. Should not the role

of these neurotransmitters in regulating vigilance and

attention be considered when approaching the spectrum of

developmental disorders where an attention deficit is

documented?

3. Hyperactivity or disorganized activity?

Gillberg115 and other authors have pointed the variability of

the ‘activity’ axis in approaching attention disorders, and

have included hypo-activity and disorganized activity as

very frequent symptoms. Indeed there are well known

gender differences on ADD children’s behaviour, and

declining externalizing motor manifestations is the rule in

adolescence in both sexes.116,117 Perhaps more importantly,

as mentioned, marked intra-individual variations in activity

throughout the day are apparent. Could better knowledge

about pathogenic mechanisms underlying impulsivity con-

tribute to disentangle its nuclear defects? On biological

grounds, should impulsivity be separated from hyperactiv-

ity?.9 Pure ‘phenomenological’ descriptions of the syndrome,

emphasising behaviour, could complicate the search for

‘endophenotypes’35,53,54 and their biological markers. Pyra-

midal tract-dependent hyperactivity as depicted in DSM IV

and Connor’s118–120 list is only part of the symptoms. These

lists ignore, for example, deficits in inhibition of saccadic

ocular movements so often found in attention deficit

children in clinical practice.121–123

Also the search for cognitive differences in neuropsycho-

logical tests in ADHD known subgroups124–127 have been

criticized on methodological grounds.128 Long lasting rel-

evance on impulsivity/hyperactivity as a nuclear deficit does

not appear to have contributed significantly when considering

attention deficit disorders.57,129,130

E U R O P E A N J O U R N A L O F P A E D I A T R I C N E U R O L O G Y 1 0 ( 2 0 0 6 ) 6 6 – 7 768

4. Attention and the search for core defects

Attention is a complex function, served by segregated

neuronal systems, object of several publications not detailed

in this review.131–134 Sustained attention, mostly related to

vigilance, is dependent on integrity of the (right) prefrontal

cortex. Focused activity (capacity for ruling out irrelevant

stimuli) and executive functions (including inhibitory control,

planning,mental representationworkingmemory, capacity to

postpone an answer to a stimuli)32,121,122,135–141 are supported

mainly or exclusively by anterior attention networks, and

these could possibly be genetically determined.142 Partial

functions integrating this ‘anterior’ attention could be

represented in a bell-shaped curve (author, unpublished

data). Its quantitative evaluation is strongly age-dependent,

in accordance with what is known about developmental

maturation of myelination and synaptic pruning in prefrontal

cortex.127,143,144

Attention deficit is pervasive in the known ADHD sub-

types, and its persistence in adulthood, when impulse and

activity control deficit slow down,make it a likely candidate in

the search of core defects.59 Could inhibitory control deficit

explain most deficiencies in executive function and working

memory currently described? There is evidence that it could

be so.35,47,49,50,121,132,135–138,140

A quite often overlooked feature of attention is its labile

nature. Many children under incomplete arousal circum-

stances can behave in good performance in attention-

requiring tasks, and even in formal evaluation tests (for

example, the ‘towers’) when their responses are dependent of

attention ‘peaks’, normally recruited by salience. Problems

arise usually inmonotonous play or work. This is indeedwhat

is regularly reported by parents and care-givers, when

testifying quiet behaviour in front of TV sets or computer

game screens, mistakenly attributing attention capacities to

this kind of play that, obviously, is not an executive function

hard-demanding task. Recognized difficulty in designing

laboratory tests for ‘anterior’ attention is indeed based on its

marked temporal, even minute to minute variability. The

search for measuring average attention (and behaviour) as

depicted by DSM-IV and Connor’s scales is based on this.

Attention is, evolutionarily, a fragile construct, supposed to be

constantly available. There are well known limits to selective

attention imposed by wake-dependent working memory.145

The need to engage sub-cortical structures (thalami) when the

waking state is not strong enough to maintain a desired level

of attention is documented.146 Limited and vulnerable as it is,

recruiting top–down mechanisms to keep better achievement

should be proof of these unique features.

5. Sleep in ADD children

Among the first models proposed to explain attention deficit

was indeed an arousal deficiency. There is extensive literature

about it.147–152 And there is also some evidence of sleep

behaviour particularities in such patients.153–160 A complaint

of bad sleep during the first 2 years of life is very frequent in

children with attention disorders, and colic behaviour has

been considered a manifestation of sleep consolidation

trouble during the first months,161,162 and is reported to be

more frequent in children who later become attention-

impaired.163,164 Vast and contradictory documentation about

sleep differences in ADD children exists,165,166 and better

understanding of sleep biologic mechanisms is certainly

needed to integrate all this variability as, most probably,

commonly used sleep stage markers are not accurate enough

to measure sleep efficiency.158–160,167–174

Cognitive troubles induced by sleep disturbance have long

been studied175–180 and different memory consolidation

problems in sleep deficits, both in laboratory animals and

humans, have been paving the way for a theorization of sleep

function and sleep ontogeny.

6. Sleep/wake cycle neurochemistry

Hobson’s long-standing work on a model relating neurotrans-

mitter activation and deactivation to sleep/wake cycle and

conscious states181–189 should be considered in this context,

even if much of it has yet to become consensual. For example,

dreams should not be considered as merely a secondary

phenomena, as they are emotionally loaded and emotions

appear to have a pivotal role in driving cognitive contents of

REM sleep (in accordance with what is known about definite

maturity of emotion processing circuits in the human new-

born190). Dopaminergic circuit activity appears essential in

this REM stage, as in other conscious states, either in normal

and pathologic conditions, and there is still a lot to be known

concerning the way it produces its effects.188,189,191–195 But

dopaminergic circuit activity does not presentmajor circadian

variation.196 Its role in mediating waking, vigilance and

attention should not be the primary one.

Cyclic variation of noradrenergic and serotoninergic neuro-

transmitter circuits is, on the contrary, well established.197–203

NE and SEmodulate extensive areas of the brain duringwaking

periods, and are seen to gradually decrease their presence

during periods of decreasingwakefulness and vigilance. During

non-REM sleep NE activity is halved, compared to wakeful

hours, and during REM sleep it is virtually zeroed. Noticeably

there is parallel deactivationof certain brain structures, namely

the DLPFC.204,205 REM sleep, it is well known, presents a special

dream activity, which appears to be activated only after

suspension of DLPFC dependent executive functions. But this

progressive decrease of NE-dependent DLPFC activation co-

occurs with peculiar behaviour of anterior cingulated cortex

(ACC) activation, which is seen to decrease during non-REM

sleep, parallelingDLPFC, butmarkedly reactivatingduringREM-

sleep, to values close to the waking state, instead of deactivat-

ing.184 It looks difficult not to relate this dissociation, with DA-

dependent enhancing of ACC structures during the short

periods of REM-sleep, to what is known of different functions

of these two structures. ACC is said to process options among

differentmotivational values and conflict detection206–213 while

DLPFC is said to play a decision role on material presented by

ACC, with strong interaction.214–218 Some of the features of

dream content, namely the peculiar passivity, could be

E U R O P E A N J O U R N A L O F P A E D I A T R I C N E U R O L O G Y 1 0 ( 2 0 0 6 ) 6 6 – 7 7 69

explainedbythisparticularpatternofACCactivationandDLPFC

deactivation.

7. Sleep, dream and memory

REM sleep’s unique vulnerability features of skeletal muscle

decreased tonus, enhanced threshold for sensory stimuli and

loosening of body thermal control must represent a challenge

from an evolutionary point of view and presuppose ontogen-

etically some fundamental yet largely ignored role. Stable

constant temperature is needed in developed brained organ-

isms. Decreased REM sleep latency in sleep deficit physiologic

and pathologic conditions points to its importance. Memory

efficacy could be at least one of these main objectives, and

there is some evidence of experience-dependent and

emotionally oriented REM sleep synaptic reshaping.219–222

Noticeably, autistic spectrum disorder patients, with their

well known difficulty in dealing with changing sets, present

decreased REM sleep.184 Evolved brain structures relying on

very narrow thermal limits for synaptic efficiency in adapting

to different environments seem to depend on cyclic

depression of waking behaviour, even at their own risk, for

what appears to be a moment of central nervous system re-

mapping. This should be accomplished by loosening of stable

synaptic links, and obviously needs inhibitory control and

executive functions suspension.179,181–184,200,219,220

8. Attention and arousal deficiency

Arousal and sleep should not be seen as discrete, ‘all or

nothing’ entities, but integrating a continuum with more or

less extensive transitory states, with marked variation from

person to person and even in the same person. Prolonged

transitory states, and state 1 sleep, present an altered pattern

of response, which is often magnified, to sensory stimuli.

Inhibition deficit and passivity to attractors can then easily be

noticed, and this is a common feature of ADD/ADHD patients,

regardless of motor output.223 Easy habituation to stimuli and

difficulty to automatize are also to be expected under

incomplete arousal situations, and this is indeed what is

seen in clinical ADD practice.115 Habituation can establish

itself very fast in repetitive exposure settings, and be detected

in a few seconds. But even with perfect functional ima-

ging108,224 and with as yet unknown systems allowing

enormous temporal resolution, measuring the state of

vigilance would be useless, as comparisons should not be

made to absolute or median values but intra-individually, and

specific tasks should even be considered.

‘Anterior’ attention225 seems to be particularly sensitive to

the effects of sleep deprivation, and theories about attention

deficit children have stressed for long the relation of

symptoms with poor vigilance.226–230 Only the amount

(duration) and timing of the different sleep states has been

considered when measuring sleep efficiency in cognitive

functions. There are reasons to believe that other markers

should be used in the future in assessing sleep efficacy.231 We

should presumably wait for better knowledge of sleep biology

and sleep repair mechanisms.

9. A link between sleep/wake cycle andattention deficit: prefrontal cortexdependent inhibition

Should ADD/ADHD still be regarded solely as a set of

heterogeneous developmental disorders with very frequent

co-morbidity and fuzzy borders? So far, considering the

previously defined three-dimensional ADD spectrum, the

search for core defects, ‘endophenotypes’ or genetic markers

has been disappointing. Has the phenomenological descrip-

tion of the disorder impaired our insight on its mechanisms?

Among DLPFC executive sub-functions, inhibitory control

serves a very important role of curtailing unnecessary circuitry

activation during a task. This circuitry can potentially relate

widespread sensory, association and motor maps to ongoing

task-related activity. ADD/ADHD being due to aminergic

deficiency, a dysfunction of prefrontal cortex tonus during

waking periods (akin to sleep deficit conditions) should cause a

decrease in inhibition power, and allow irruption of apparently

task-unrelated contents. Inattention, with or without externa-

lizing hyperactive or impulsive behaviour, would be just a by-

product of a more fundamental inhibition core deficit.

10. Implications for brain development:comorbidities?

If an actual deficit in attention and activity control can exert so

deep effects in global performance in children, what is to be

expected if the same causal, genetically determined mechan-

isms are present in foetal, neonatal and growth periods of life?

An early, protracted catecholaminergic, mainly noradren-

ergic, deficit should be profoundly operative in neural systems

sub-serving different functions and proved to be sensitive

during these periods. It looks as if this is indeed the

case.32,224,232–245 Neurotransmitters have very important, yet

largely unknown functions in modulating synaptic contacts.

Even sustained emotions experienced by pregnant females or

infants should have amine-mediated effects on synaptic fine

tuning during foetal life and early infancy.246,247

Functions like perception andmotor control, dependent as

they are on perfect circuitry, should be deeply sensitive to

persistent unbalance in neurotransmitters. This could

explain, at least in part, very frequent motor coordination

and perceptual problems in ADD patients.

On the other hand, relative deficiency in serotonine

activity, that normally parallels rhythmic activity of NE

circuits52,180–183 could, on other way, up-regulate DA circuits66

with hyperdopaminergic effects,248 including detail hypersa-

lience and psychosis. SE is a very ancient and ubiquitous

molecule in nature.64,84,243,249 It was first described as a

neurotransmitter in 1963 by Gerschenfeld and Stefani250,251

and has multiple functions recognized in the nervous system.

In psychiatric clinic there is widespread use of the SE

E U R O P E A N J O U R N A L O F P A E D I A T R I C N E U R O L O G Y 1 0 ( 2 0 0 6 ) 6 6 – 7 770

re-uptake inhibitors in depression, anxiety, obsessive/com-

pulsive and mood control disorders. It seems that most

psychiatric co-morbidities frequently described in attention

deficit disorders, a putative catecholamine deficit disorder,

could be at least partially related to homologous SE deficit.

11. Improvement in areas other than attention

In clinical practice most strategies that are supposed to

enhance vigilance and DLPFC dependent executive functions

can have positive effects, not only on ‘anterior’ attention

deficits but also, at least in part, on motor control impair-

ments, perception deficits, hyperactivity, impulsive aggres-

sion, opposition and defiant conduct disorders, depression

and anxiety.252,253 Specific learning impairments can be also

helped by stimulant therapy. A significant proportion of

autistic spectrum and Asperger patients can show improve-

ment in several areas, including sociability, under stimulant

medication, perhaps started in much lower dosages (author,

unpublished data). This could also mean that vigilance is an

important dimension in personal, eye-to-eye communication.

It looks as if up-grading vigilance could, in a certain sense,

promote better performance on apparently unrelated areas,

but served by circuits at least partially catecholamine

dependent for optimal functioning.254

12. Routine, creativity andattention/inattention cycling

A question should be addressed about what creativity could

mean in this framework. There is widespread confusion

between talent and genius255,256 and the question has rarely

been addressed, although it is suspected that a strong link

exists between creativity and psychopathology. ‘Savant skills’

have long been recognized in autistic spectrum patients, and

there is undoubtedly unusual performance in half of these

patients in certain areas (namely memory and calculation,

etc.). This appears to represent something different from

creativity, as it is seen to occur in patients to the detriment of

other areas, mostly empathy and socialization, which are

clearly deficient in most of theses cases.257 It is well-known

that in several developmental disorders and autism a defect in

plasticity can be suspected as a major pathologic mechan-

ism.258 It looks as if a distinction should be made between

early, unrelenting, repetitive performance of certain tasks

(mainly memory tasks) which can be noticed in certain

children and herald autistic symptoms, and unusual, unex-

pected ideas which can make their appearance in school

routines or in artistic or scientific domains in some people

that are often considered ‘eccentric’, mainly because they are

inattentive. ADD patients can indeed surprise others by ‘peak’

reply and by bright, unexpected ideas that emerge in a context

of poor response to regular tasks. If in the everyday up-and-

down recurring curve of attention, re-mapping tasks for

updating experience imply cyclic periods of inhibitory control

attenuation, then this can be the opportunity for unexpected,

’creative’ links to emerge. From this viewpoint, attention

deficit patients and artistic personalities would be expected to

spendmore time in ‘less than optimal’ arousal andwould thus

be more prone to unusual associations, such as a deficit in

inhibition state can allow. That is probably why they cannot

easily cope with regular, routine-demanding tasks.

13. Conclusions

This review reconsiders existing hypothesis on pathogenic

mechanisms of ADD, based on current knowledge on the

subject. It recognizes its strong genetic dependency and

stresses its implications in infancy and the search for basic

defects taking into account its protean manifestations.

Attention is a complex function, which implicates sub-

cortical and brain stem circuitry, and pervades several areas

of central nervous system functioning. Its defects can have

deep implications in multiple dimensions, and do not fit well

in an ‘all or nothing’ paradigm. There is evidence of a strong

link between arousal, attention, and sleep/wake cycle neuro-

chemical pacing and of genetic, sub-cortical ‘bottom–up’

mechanisms underlying the efficacy of this pacing.

Attention deficit disorders are specific developmental

impairments that present unique features of marked tem-

poral, circadian variability. Their pharmacological treatment

includes stimulant medication that has strong known ami-

nergic (NA and DA) effects. NA and SE, but not DA, circuits

present important variations during the 24-h cycle, with

decreased activity during sleep, and absent activity during

REM sleep, which parallel congruent deactivation of DLPFC.

‘Anterior’ attention, recruited for learning school tasks, is

strongly dependent on DLPFC executive functions.

An important feature of ADD and ADHD complex is the

frequency of co-morbidities, which can be at least partially

explained by enduring unbalance of these neurotransmitters

during pre and postneonatal life. Positive effects of stimulant

medications on some of these co-morbidities manifestations

should support evidence for sharing part of the same

mechanisms.

Functional imaging intending to study ADD deficits should

have much higher temporal resolution and be directed to

intra-individual comparison. Better knowledge about rest and

sleep biology and ‘sleep genes’ expression is needed to

understand its cognitive effects and efficiency.

Previous views emphasizing impulsivity and hyperactivity

and disregarding inattention should be reconsidered. Instead,

inhibitory control deficit should probably be viewed as the

main defect.

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Isabel Pavao Martins,

Carlos Nunes Filipe and Jose Carlos Ferreira for very

helpful discussions, and Ana Gerschenfeld, Margarida

Menezes Ferreira and Ana Isabel Dias for reading of the

manuscript.

E U R O P E A N J O U R N A L O F P A E D I A T R I C N E U R O L O G Y 1 0 ( 2 0 0 6 ) 6 6 – 7 7 71

R E F E R E N C E S

1. Zametkin AJ, Liotta W. The neurobiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Psychiatry1998;59(S7):17–23.

2. Tannock R. Neuropsychology of attention disorders. In:Segalowitz SJ, Rapin I, editors. Child neuropshychology.Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2003. p. 753–84.

3. Rutter M, Silberg J, O’Connor T, Simonoff E. Genetics andchild psychiatry: II empirical research findings. J ChildPsychol Psychiatry 1999;40(1):19–55.

4. Faraone SV. Genetics of adult attention deficit/hyperactivitydisorder. Psychiatr Clin N Am 2004;27:303–21.

5. Retz W, Thome J, Blocher D, Baader M, Roster M.Association of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder-related psychopathology and personality traits with theserotonin transporter promoter region polymorphism.Neurosci Lett 2002;319(3):133–6.

6. Gerra G, Avanzini P, Zaimovic A, et al. Neurotransmitters,neuroendocrine correlates of sensation-seekingtemperaments in normal humans. Neuropsychobiology1999;39(4):207–13.

7. Fisher SE, Francks C, McCraken JT, et al. A genomewidescan for loci involved in attention deficit/hyperactivitydisorder. Am J Hum Genet 2002;70:1183–96.

8. Zoroglu SS, Erdal ME, Erdal N, Ozen S, Alasehirli B, Sivasli.No evidence for an association between the T102C and 1438G/A polymorphisms of the serotonin 2A receptor gene inattention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a Turkishpopulation. Neuropsychobiology 2003;47:17–20.

9. Wigg K, Zai G, Schachar, et al. Attention deficithyperactivity disorder and the gene for dopamine-betahydroxylase. Am J Psychiatry 2002;159:1046–8.

10. Bobb AJ, Castelanos FX, Addington AM, Rapoport JL.Molecular genetics studies of ADHD: 1991–2004. Am J MedGenet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2005;132(1):109–25.

11. Rasmussen P, Gillberg C. Natural outcome of ADHD withdevelopmental coordination disorder at age 22 years: acontrolled, longitudinal, community-based study. Am AcadChild Adolesc Psychiatry 2000;39(11):1424–31.

12. Howell DC, Huessy HR, Hassuk B. Fifteen-year follow-up ofa behevioral history of attention deficit disorder. Pediatrics1985;76(2):185–90.

13. Eichlseder W. Tem years of experience with 1000hyperactive children in a private practice. Pediatrics1985;76(2):176–84.

14. Lahey BB, Pelham WE, Loney J, et al. Three-year predictivevalidity of DSMIV attention deficit hyperactivity disorder inchildren diagnosed at 4–6 years of age. Am J Psychiatry2004;161:2014–20.

15. Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ, DiGiusepe DL, McCarthy CA.Early television exposure and subsequent attentionalproblems in children. Pediatrics 2004;113:708–13.

16. Anderson J. Is childhood hyperactivity the product ofwestern culture? Lancet 1996;348:73–4.

17. Kadesjo B, Gillberg C. The comorbidity of ADHD in thegeneral population of Swedish school-age children. J ChildPsychol Psychiatry 2001;42(4):487–92.

18. Spencer TJ, Biederman J, Faraone S, et al. Impact of tiedisorders on ADHD outcome across the life-cycle: findingsfrom a large group of adults with and without ADHD. AmJ Psychiatry 2001;158(4):611–7.

19. Geller B, Zimerman BB, Williams M, et al. DSM-IV maniasymptoms in a prepubertal and early adolescent bipolardisorder phenotype compared to attention deficithyperactivity and normal controls. J Child AdolescPsychopharmacol 2002;12(1):11–25.

20. Sachs GS, Baldassano CF, Truman CJ, Guille C. Comorbidityof attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with early analate-onset bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry2000;157(3):466–8.

21. Pliszka SR. Comorbidity of attention deficit/hyperactivitydisorder with psychiatry disorder: an overview. J ClinPsychiatry 1998;59(S7):50–8.

22. Burke JD, Loeber R, Birmaher B. Oppositional defiantdisorder and conduct disorder: a review of the past 10years, part II. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry2002;41(11):1275–93.

23. Ramus F. Neurobiology of dyslexia: a reinterpretation ofdata. Trends Neurosci 2004;27(12):720–6.

24. Neville HJ, Coffey SA, Hocolomb PJ, Tallal P. Theneurobiology of sensory and language processing inlanguage-impaired children. J Cognitive Neurosci1993;5(2):235–53.

25. Both R, Charlton R, Hughes C, Happe F. Disentangling weakcoherence and executive dysfunction: planning drawing inautism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. PhilosTrans R Soc Lond B 2003;358:387–92.

26. Brown TE. Attention-deficit disorders and comorbidities inchildren, adolescents and adults. Washington, DC: AmericanPsychiatric Publishing; 2000.

27. Sonuga-Barke EJ. Psychological heterogenecity in ADHD; adual pathways model of motivation and cognition. BehavBrain Res 2002;130:29–36.

28. Tseng MH, Henderson A, Chow SM, Yao G. Relationshipbetween motor proficiency, attention, impulse, and activityin children with ADHD. Dev Med Child Neurol 2004;46:381–8.

29. Shaywitz S, Escobar M, Shaywitz B, Fletcher JM, Makuch R.Evidence that dyslexia may represent the lower tail of anormal distribuition of reading hability. N Engl J Med1992;236:145–50.

30. Baron-Cohen S, Richler, Bisarya D, Gurunathan N,Wheelwright S. The systemizing quotient: an investigationof adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioningautism, and normal sex differences. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B2003;358:361–74.

31. Faraone SV, Biederman J. Neurobiology of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry1998;44(10):951–8.

32. Solanto MV. Neuropsychopharmacological mechanisms ofstimulant drug action in attention deficit hyperactivitydisorder: a review and integration. Behav Brain Res1998;94:127–52.

33. Todd RD, Botteron KN. Is attention deficit/hyperactivitydisorder an energy deficiency syndrome? Biol Psychiatry2001;50:151–8.

34. Biederman J, Spence T. Attention deficit/hyperactivitydisorder (ADHD) as a nor-adrenergic disorder. Biol Psychiatry1999;46:1234–42.

35. Castellanos FX, Tannock R. Neuroscience of attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder: the search forendophenotypes. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3:1–12.

36. Jambaque I, Lassonde M, Dulac O, editors. Neuropsychologyof childhood epilepsy. New York: Kluwer/Plenum Press; 2001.

37. Elgen I, Lundervold AJ, Sommerfelt K. Aspects ofinattention in low birth weigth children. Pediatr Neuro2004;30:92–8.

38. Castellanos FX, Elia J, Kruesi MJ, et al. CSF Homovanillieacid predicts behavioural response to stimulants in 45 boyswith attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder.Neuropsychopharmacology 1996;14(2):125–37.

39. Krause KH, Dresel SH, Krause J, la Fougere C, Ackenheil M.The dopamine transporter and neuroimaging in attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev2003;27(7):605–13.

E U R O P E A N J O U R N A L O F P A E D I A T R I C N E U R O L O G Y 1 0 ( 2 0 0 6 ) 6 6 – 7 772

40. Volkow ND, Swanson JM. Variables that affect the clinicaluse of methylphenidate in the treatment of ADHD. AmJ Psychiatry 2003;160:1909–18.

41. Dougherty DD, Bonab AA, Spencer TJ, Raich SL, Madras BK,Fischman AJ. Dopamine transporter density in patientswith deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet 1999;354:2132–3.

42. Carrey N, MacMaster FP, Fogel J, et al. Metabolite changesresulting from treatment in children with ADHD: a IH-MRSstudy. Clin Neuropharmacol 2003;26(4):218–21.

43. Krause KD, Dresel SH, Krause J, Kung HF, Tatsch K.Increased srtiatal dopamine transporter in adult patientswith attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: effects ofmethylphenidate as measured by single photon emissioncomputed tomography. Neurosci Lett 2000;285:107–10.

44. Vles JS, Feron FJ, Hendriksen JG, Jolles J, vanKroonenburgh MJ, Weber W. Methylphenidate down-regulates the dopamine receptor and transporter system inchildren with attention deficit hyperkinetic disorder(ADHD). Neuropediatrics 2003;34:77–80.

45. Lou H, Rosa P, Pryds O, et al. ADHD: increased dopaminereceptor availability linked to attention deficit and lowneonatal cerebral blood flow. Dev Med Child Neurol2004;46:179–83.

46. Roberts AC, Robbins TW, Weiskrantz L, editors. Theprefrontal cortex—executive and cognitive functions. Oxford:Oxford University Press; 1998.

47. Sagvolden T, Aase H, Johansen EB, Russell VA. A dynamicdevelopmental theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder (ADHD) predominantly hyperactive/impulsive andcombined subtypes. Behav Brain Sci 2005;28(3):397–419.

48. Biederman J. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: aselective overview. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:1220–51.

49. Xavier Castellanos F, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Scheres A, DiMartino A, Hyde C, Wlater JR. Varieties of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-related intra-individualvariability. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:1416–23.

50. Sonuga-Barke EJS, Castellanos FX. A common coredysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivty disorder: ascientific red herring? Behav Brain Sci 2005;28(3):443–4.

51. Banaschewski T, Himpel S, Rothenberger A. Unitary ormultiple pathways: the trap of radical behaviourism. BehavBrain Res 2005;28:425–6.

52. Oades RD, Christiansen H. ADHD theories still need to takemore on board: serotonin and pre-executive variability.Behav Brain Res 2005;28(3):438.

53. Doyle AE, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ, et al. Areendophenotypes based on measures of executive functionsusefull for molecular genetic studies of ADHD? J ChildPsychol Psychiatry 2005;46(7):774–803.

54. Doyle AE, Willcutt EG, Seidman LJ, et al. Attention-deficithyperactivity disorder endophenotypes. Biol Psychiatry2005;57:1324–35.

55. Sonuga-Barke EJS. Causal models of attention-deficithyperactivity disorder: from common simple deficits tomultiple developmental pathways. Biol Psychiatry2005;57:123–1238.

56. Sergeant JA. Modeling attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder: a critical appraisal of the cognitive-energeticmodel. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:1248–55.

57. Geurts H, Verte S, Oosterlan J, Roeyers H, Sergeant JA.ADHD subtypes: do they differ in their executivefunctioning profile? Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2005;20:457–77.

58. Nigg JT, Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Sonuga-Barke EJ. Causalheterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: dowe need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? BiolPsychiatry 2005;57:1224–30.

59. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF.Validity of the executive function theory of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. BiolPsychiatry 2005;57:1336–46.

60. Smoller JW, Biederman J, Arbeitman L, et al. Associationbetween the 5HT1B receptor gene (HTR1B) and theinattentive subtype of ADHD. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57.

61. Mulas F, Capilla A, Fernandez S, et al. Shifting-related brainmagnetic activity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.Biol Psychiatry 2006;59(4):373–9.

62. Mick E, Spencer T, Wozniak J, Biederman J. Heterogeneity ofirritability in attention-deficit hyperactivity disordersubjects with and without mood disorders. Biol Psychiatry2005;58:576–82.

63. Kessler RC, Adler LA, Barkley R, et al. Patterns andpredictors of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorderpersistence into adulthood: results from the nationalcomorbidity survey replication. Biol Psychiatry2005;57:1442–51.

64. Cravchik A, Goldman D. Neurochemical individuality.Genetic diversity among dopamine and serotoninereceptors and transporters. Arch Gen Psychiatry2000;57:1105–14.

65. Okado N, Narita M, Narita N. A biogenic amine-synapsemechanism for mental retardation and developmentaldisabilities. Brain Dev 2001;23:S11–S15.

66. Kapur S, Remington G. Serotonin–dopamine interaction andits relevance to schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry1996;153:466–76.

67. Huether G. Acute regulation and long-term modulation ofpresynaptic serotonin output. Adv Exp Med Biol1999;467:1–10.

68. Wang QP, Guan JL, Shioda S. Synaptic contacts betweenserotoninergic and cholinergic neurons in the rat dorsalraphe nucleus and laterodorsal tegmental nucleus.Neuroscience 2000;97(3):553–63.

69. Brown WD, Taylor MD, Roberts AD, et al. FluoroDOPA PETshows the nondopaminergic as well as the dopaminergicdestinations of levodopa. Neurology 1999;53:1212–8.

70. Stahl SM. Neurotransmission of cognition, part 3.Mechanism of action of selective NRIs: both dopamine andnorepinephrine increase in prefrontal cortex. J ClinPsychiatry 2003;64(3):230–1.

71. Kapur S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: aframework linking biology, phenomenology, andpharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry2003;160:13–23.

72. Lanau F, Zener M-T, Civelli O, Hartman D. Epineprineand norepinephrine act as potent agonists at therecombinant human dopamine D4 receptor. J Neurochem1997;68:804–12.

73. Bymaster F, Katner J, Nelson D, et al. Atomoxetineincreases extracelular levels of norepinephrine anddopamine in prefrontal cortex of rat: a potentialmechanism for efficacy in attention deficit/hyperactivitydisorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 2002;27:699–711.

74. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, et al. Evidence thatmatylphenidate enhances the saliency of a mathematicaltask by increasing dopamine in the human brain. AmJ Psychiatry 2004;161(7):1173–80.

75. Remy P, Smson Y. The role of dopamine in cognition:evidence from functional imaging studies. Curr Opin Neurol2003;16(S2):37–42.

76. Pillon B, Czernecki V, Dubois B. Dopamine and cognitivefunction. Curr Opin Neurol 2003;16(S2):17–22.

77. Kimberg DY, D’Esposito M. Cognitive effects of thedopamine receptor agonist pergolide. Neuropsychology2003;41(8):1020–7.

78. Metha MA, Swainson R, Ogilvie AD, Sahakian J,Robbins TW. Improved short-term spatial memory but

E U R O P E A N J O U R N A L O F P A E D I A T R I C N E U R O L O G Y 1 0 ( 2 0 0 6 ) 6 6 – 7 7 73

impaired reversal learning following the dopamine D2agonist bromocriptine in human volunteers.Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 2001;159(1):10–20.

79. Berridge KC, Robinson TE. What is the role of dopamine inreward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentivesalience? Brain Res Rev 1998;28:309–69.

80. Winterer G, Weinberger DR. Genes, dopamine and corticalsignal-to-noise ratio in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci2004;27(11):683–90.

81. McClure SM, Daw ND, Montague PR. A computationalsubstrate for incentive salience. Trends Neurosci2003;26(8):423–8.

82. Ungless MA. Dopamine: the salience issuc. Trends Neurosci2004;27(12):702–6.

83. Herlunius E, Lagercrantz H. Neurotransmitters andneuromodulators during early human development. EarlyHum Dev 2001;65(1):21–37.

84. Azmitia EC. Neuronal instability: implications for Rett’ssyndrome. Brain Dev 2001;23(S1–S10).

85. Dunn DW, Kronenberger WG. Attention deficithyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. NeurolClin N Am 2003;21:933–40.

86. Taylor FB, Russo J. Efficacy of modafinil compared todextroamphetamine for the treatment of attention deficithyperactivity disorder in adults. J Child AdolescPsychopharmacol 2000;10(4):311–20.

87. Swanson JM. Role of executive function in ADHD. J ClinPsychiatry 2003;64(S14):35–9.

88. Pliszka SR. Non-stimulant treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. CNS Spectr 2003;8(4):253–8.

89. Rugino TA, Copley TC. Effects of modafinil in childrenwith attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an open-label study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry2001;40(2):230–5.

90. Brown TE. Atomoxetine and stimulants in combination fortreatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: fourcase reports. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol2004;14(1):131–8.

91. Eggermann E, Serafin M, Bayer I, et al. Orexins/hypocretinsexite basal forebrain cholinergic neurones. Neuroscience2001;108(2):177–81.

92. Marino J, Cudero J. Nitric oxide-mediated corticalactivation: a diffuse wake-up system. J Neurosci2003;23(10):4299–307.

93. Stahl SM. Awakening to the psychopharmacology of sleepand arousal: novel neurotransmitters and wake-promotingdrugs. J Clin Psychiatry 2002;63(6):467–8.

94. Rugino TA, Samsock MS. Modafinil in children withattention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr Neurol2003;29:136–42.

95. Chapotot F, Pigeau R, Canini F, et al. Distinctive effects ofmodafinil and damphetamine on the homeostatic andcircadian modulation of the human waking EEG.Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 2003;166(2):127–38.

96. Viggiano D, Vallone D, Ruocco LA, Sadile AG. Behavioural,pharmacological, morpho-functional molecular studiesreveal a hyperfunctioning mesocortical dopamine systemin an animal model of attention deficit and hyperactivitydisorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2003;27:683–9.

97. Solanto MV. Dopamine dysfunction in AD/HD: integratingclinical and basic neuroscience research. Behav Brain Res2002;130:65–71.

98. Sadile AG, Viggiano D. Is the hypodopaminergic hypothesisplausible as neural bases of ADHD? Behav Brain Res2005;28(3):440–1.

99. Pliszka SR. The neuropsychopharmacology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry2005;57:1385–90.

100. Spencer TJ, Biederman J, Madras BK, et al. In vivoneuroreceptor imaging in attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder: a focus on the dopamine transporter. BiolPsychiatry 2005;57:1293–300.

101. Castellanos FX, Giedd SN, Berquin P, et al. Quantitativebrain magnetic resonance imaging in girls with ADHD. ArchGen Psychiatry 2001;58:289–95.

102. Castellanos FX, Lee PP, Sharp W, et al. Developmenttrajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children andadolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder.J Am Med Assoc 2002;14:1740–8.

103. Berquin PC, Gied JN, Jacobsen LK, et al. Cerebellum inattention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a morphometricMRI study. Neurology 1998;50:1087–93.

104. Singer W. The brain as a self-organizing system. Eur ArchPsychiatry Neurosci 1986;236(1):4–9.

105. Hesslinger B, Tebartz van Elst L, Thiel T, Haegele K,Henning J, Ebert D. Frontoorbital reductions in adultpatients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.Neurosci Lett 2002;328(3):319–21.

106. Anderson CM, Ann Polcari RN, Lowen SB, Renshaw PF,Teicher MH. Effects of methylphenidate on functionalmagnetic resonance relaxometry of the cerebellar vermis inboys with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry 2002;159:1322–8.

107. Zametkin AJ, Nordahl TE, Gross M, et al. Cerebral glucosemetabolism in adults with hyperactivity of childhoodonset. N Engl J Med 1990;323:1361–6.

108. Castellanos FX. Proceed with caution: SPECT cerebral bloodflow study of children and adolescents with attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder. J Nucl Med 2002;43(12):1630–3.

109. Fan J, Fossella J, Sommer T, Posner MI. Mapping the geneticvariation of the executive attention into brain activity. ProcNatl Acad Sci USA 2003;100(12):7406–11.

110. Findling RL, Dogin JW. Psychopharmacology of ADHD:children and adolescents. J Clin Psychiatry 1998;59(S7):42–7.

111. Elia J, Ambrosini P, Rapoport J. Treatment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. N Engl J Med1999;340(10):780–8.

112. Biederman J. Pratical considerations in stimulant drugselection for the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.Patient-efficacy, potency and titration. Todays Ther Trends2002;20(4):311–28.

113. Chabot RJ, Orgill AA, Crawford G, Harris MJ, Serfontein G.Behavioral and electrophysiologic predictors of treatmentresponse to stimulants in children with attention disorders.J Child Neurol 1999;14:343–51.

114. Shafritz KM, Marchione KE, Gore JC, Shaywitz SE,Shaywitz BA. The effects of methylphenidate on neuralsystems of attention in attention deficit hyperactivitydisorder. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:1990–7.

115. Gillberg C. Clinical child neuropsychiatry. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press; 1995 [Chapter 8].

116. Biederman J. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a life-span perspective. J Clin Psychiatry 1998;59(S7):4–16.

117. Biederman J, Mick E, Faraone SV. Age-dependent decline ofsymptoms of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder:impact of remission definition and symptom type. AmJ Psychiatry 2000;157(5):816–8.

118. Conners CK. Conners rating scales-revised: technical manual.North Tonowanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems, Inc.; 1998.

119. Conners CK. A teacher rating scale for use in drug studieswith children. Am J Psychiatry 1969;126:884–8.

120. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statisticalmanual for the classification of mental disorders. 4th ed.Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1994.

121. Condy C, Rivaud-P’choux S, Ostendorf F, Ploner CJ,Gaymard B. Neural sustrate of antisaccades. Role ofsubcortical structures. Neurology 2004;63:1571–8.

E U R O P E A N J O U R N A L O F P A E D I A T R I C N E U R O L O G Y 1 0 ( 2 0 0 6 ) 6 6 – 7 774

122. Mostofsky SH, Lasker AG, Cutting LE, Denckla MB, Zee DS.Oculomotor abnormalities in attention deficit hyperactivitydisorder: a preliminary study. Neurology 2001;57:423–30.

123. Gould TD, Bastain TM, Israel ME, Hommer DW,Castellanos FX. Altered performance on an ocular fixationtask in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. BiolPsychiatry 2001;50(8):633–5.

124. Houghton S, Douglas G, West J, et al. Differential patternsof executive function in children with attention-deficithyperactivity disorder according to gender and subtype.J Child Neurol 1999;14:801–5.

125. Klorman R, Hazel-Fernandez LA, Shaywitz SE, et al.Executive functioning deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder are independent ofoppositional defiant or reading disorder. J Am Acad AdolescPsychiatry 1999;38(9):1148–55.

126. Hesslinger B, Thiel T, Tebartz van Elst L, Hennig J, Ebert D.Attention-deficit disorder in adults with or withouthyperactivity: where is the difference? A study in humansusing short echo (1) H-magnetic resonanse spectroscopy.Neurosci Lett 2001;304(1–2):117–9.

127. Gansler DA, Fucetola R, Krengel M, Stetson S, Zimering R,Makary C. Are there cognitive subtypes in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? J Nerv Ment Dis1998;186(12):776–81.

128. Sergeant JA, Geurts H, Oosterlaan J. How specific is a deficitof executive function for attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder? Behav Brain Res 2002;130:3–28.

129. Chabbildas N, Pennington BF, Willeutt EG. A comparison ofthe neuropsychological profiles of the DSM-IV subtypes ofADHD. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2001;6:529–40.

130. Huang-Pollock CL, Nigg JT, Carr CH. Deficient attention ishard to find: applying the perceptual load model ofselective attention to attention deficit hyperactivitydisorder subtypes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry2005;46(11):1211–8.

131. Posner MI, Peterson SE. The attention system of the humanbrain. Annu Rev Neurosci 1990;13:25–42.

132. Barkley RA. ADHD and the nature of self-control. New York:Guilford; 1997.

133. Bly BM, Rumelhart DE, editors. Cognitive science. San Diego:Academic Press; 1999. p. 43–97 [LaBerge D. Attention].

134. Crick F. The astonishing hypothesis. The scientific search for thesool. New York: Macmillan; 1994.

135. Nigg JT, Butler KM, Huang-Pollock CL, Henderson JM.Inhibitory processes in adults with persistent childhoodonset ADHD. J Consult Clin Psychol 2001;70(1):153–7.

136. Perchet C, Revol O, Fourneret P, Maugniere F, Garcia-Larrea C. Attention shifts and antecipatory mechanisms inhyperactive children: an ERP study using the Posnerparadigm. Biol Psychiatry 2001;50(1):44–57.

137. Rapport LJ, Van Voorhis A, Tzelpis A, Friedman SR.Executive functioning in adult attention deficithyperactivity disorder. Clin Neuropsychol 2001;15(4):479–91.

138. Benneto I, Pnnington B. Executive function in normal andabnormal development. In: Segalowitz SJ, Rapin I, editors.Child neuropsychol. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2003.

139. D’Esposito M, Grossman M. The psychological basis ofexecutive function and working memory. Neuroscientist1996;2(6):345–52.

140. Crosbie J, Schachar R. Deficient inhibition as a markerfamilial ADHD. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158:1884–90.

141. Roberts AC, Robbins TW, Weiskrantz I, editors. Theprefrontal cortex — executive and cognitive functions. Oxford:Oxford University Press; 1998.

142. Fan J, Wu Y, Fossella JA, Posner MI. Assessing theheritability of attencional networks. BMC Neurosci2001;2(19):14.

143. Luna B, Thulborn K, Munoz D, et al. Maturation of widelydistributed brain function subserves cognitivedevelopment. Neuroimage 2001;13:786–93.

144. Bunge S, Dudukovic N, Thomason M, Vaidya CJ, Gabrieli JD.Immature frntal lobe contributions to cognitive control inchildren: evidence from MRI. Neuron 2002;33:301–11.

145. De Fockert JW, Rees G, Larie N. The role of working memoryin visual selective attention. Science 2001;291:1803–5.

146. Portas CM, Rees G, Howseman AM, Josephs O, Turner R,Frith CD. A specific role for the thalamus in mediating theinteraction of attention and arousal in humans. J Neurosci1998;18(21):8979–89.

147. Corkum P, Tannock R, Moldofsky H. Sleep disturbances inchildren with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J AmAcad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1998;37(6):637–46.

148. Lecendreux M, Konofal E, Bouvard M, Talissard B, Mouren-Simeoni MC. Sleep and alertness in children with ADHD.J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000;41(6):803–12.

149. Tirosh E, Sadeh A, Munvez R, Lauie P. Effects ofmethylphenidate on sleep in children with attention deficithyperactivity disorder. An activity monitor study. AJDC1993;147(12):1313–5.

150. Mick E, Biederman J, Jetton J, Faraone SV. Sleepdisturbances associated with attention deficit hyperactivitydisorder: the impact of psychiatric comorbidity andpharmacholotherapy. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol2000;10(3):223–31.

151. Ring A, Stein D, Barak Y, et al. Sleep disturbances inchildren with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: acomparative study with healthy siblings. J Learn Disabil1998;31(6):572–8.

152. Thunstrom M. Severe sleep problems in infancy associatedwith subsequent development of attention deficithyperactivity disorder at 5.5 years of age. Acta Pediatr2002;91(5):584–92.

153. Kooij JJ, Middelkoop HA, van Gils, Buitelaar JK. The effect ofstimulants on nocturnal motor activity and sleep quality inadults with ADHD: an open-label case–control study. J ClinPsychiatry 2001;62(12):952–6.

154. Leitner Y, Bloch AM, Sadch A, et al. Sleep–wake patterns inchildren with intrauterine growth retardation. J Clin Neurol2002;17(12):872–6.

155. Owens JA, Maxim R, Nobile C, McGuinn M, Msall M.Parental and self-report of sleep in children with attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med2000;154(6):549–55.

156. Dagan Y, Zeevi-Luria S, Sever Y, et al. Sleep quality inchildren with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: anactigraphic study. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1997;51(6):383–6.

157. O’Brien LM, Hotbrook CR, Mervis CB, et al. Sleep andneurobehavioral characteristics of 5-to-7 years old childrenvs parentelly reported symptoms of attention deficithyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics 2003;111(3):554–63.

158. Chervin RD, Archbold KH. Hyperactivity andpolysomnographic findings in chindren evaluated fordisordered breathing. Sleep 2001;24(3):313–20.

159. Gruber R, Sadeh A, Raviv A. Instability of sleep patterns inchildren with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J AmAcad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000;39(4):409–501.

160. Konofal E, Lecendreux M, Bouvard MP, Mouren-Simeoni MC. High levels of nocturnal activity in childrenwith attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a videoanalysis. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2001;55(2):97–103.

161. In Christian Guilleminault, editor. Sleep and its disorders inchildren. New York: Raven Press; 1987. Marc WeissbluthSleep and the colicky infant.

162. Thomas AJ, Erokwu BO, Yamamoto BK, Ernsberger P,Bishara O, Strohl KP. Alterations in respiratory behevior,

E U R O P E A N J O U R N A L O F P A E D I A T R I C N E U R O L O G Y 1 0 ( 2 0 0 6 ) 6 6 – 7 7 75

brain neurochemistry and receptor density induced bypharmacologic suppression of sleep in the neonatal period.Brain Res Dev Brain Res 2000;120(2):181–9.

163. Wolke D, Rizzo P, Woods S. Persistent infant crying andhyperactivity problems in middle childhood. Pediatrics2002;109(6):1054–60.

164. Rao MR, Brenner RA, Schisterman EF, Vik T. Long termcognitive development in children with prolonged crying.Arch Dis Child 2004;89(11):989–92.

165. Fallone G, Arnedt JT, Seifer R, Carskadon MA. Effects ofacute sleep restriction on behaviour, sustained attentionand response inhibition in children. Percept Mot Skills2001;93(1):213–29.

166. Fulda S, Schulz H. Cognitive dysfunction in sleep disorders.Sleep Med Rev 2001;5(6):423–45.

167. Foster RG, Kreitzman L. Rythms of life. The biological clocksthat control the daily lives of every living thing. London: ProfileBooks; 2004.

168. Czeisler CA, Duffy JF, Shanahan TL, et al. Stability,precision and near-24-hour period of the human circadianpace-maker. Science 1999;284(5423):2177–81.

169. Turek FW, Dugovic C, Zee PC. Current understanding of thecircadian clock and the clinical implications forneurological disorders. Arch Neurol 2001;58:1781–7.

170. Barry RJ, Clarke AR, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M, Rushby JA,Ploskova E. EEG differences in children as a function ofresting-state arousal level. Clin Neurophysiol 2004;115:402–8.

171. Barry RJ, Clarke AR, Johnstone SJ. A review ofelectrophysiology in attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder: qualitative and quantitativeelectroencephalography. Clin Neurophysiol 2003;114:171–83.

172. Crabtree VM, Ivanenko A, O’Brien LM, Gozal D. Periodiclimb movement disorder of sleep in children. J Sleep Res2003;12:73–81.

173. Kirov R, Kinkelbur J, Heipke S, et al. Is there a specificpolysomnographic sleep pattern in children with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder? J Sleep Res 2004;13:87–93.

174. Brown TE, McMullen Jr WJ. Attention deficit disorders andsleep/arousal disturbance. Ann NY Acad Sci 2001;931:271–86.

175. Frank MG, Heller HC. The ontogeny of mammalian sleep: areappraisal of alternative hypothesis. J Sleep Res2003;12(19):25–34.

176. Stickgold R. Toward a cognitive neuroscience of sleep. SleepMed Rev 2001;5(6):417–21.

177. Ambrosini MV. Learning and sleep: the sequentialhypotesis. Sleep Med Rev 2001;5(6):477–99.

178. Smith C. Sleep states and memory processes in humans:procedural versus declarative memory systems. Sleep MedRev 2001;5(6):491–506.

179. Kavanau JL. Memory, sleep and the evolution ofmechanisms of synaptic efficacy maintenance. Neuroscience1997;79(1):7–44.

180. Garcia-Rill E. Mechanisms of sleep and wakefulness. Lee-Chong T, Sateia M, Carskadon M, editors. Sleep Med.Philadelphia: Hanley & Belfus; 2002.

181. Hobson JA. Consciousness. New York: Sci Am Libraru; 1999.182. Hobson JA. The dream drugstore—chemically altered states of

consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press;2001.

183. Hobson JA. The chemistry of conscious states—how the brainchanges its mind. Boston Little: Brown and Co.; 1994.

184. Pace-schott EF. Recent findings in the neurobiology of sleep anddreaming. Sleep and dreaming—scientific advances andreconsiderations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press;2001. p. 335–50.

185. Buchsbaum MS, Hazlett EA, et al. Positron emissiontomography with deoxiglucose-F18 imaging of sleep.Neuropsychopharmacology 2001;25(S5):S50–S6.

186. Python A, Steimer T, de Saint Hilaire Z, Mikolajewsk R,Nicoladis S. Extracellular serotonin variations duringvigilance states in the preoptic area of rats: a microdyalisisstudy. Brain Res 2001;910(1–2):49–54.

187. Ursin R. Serotonin and sleep. Sleep Med Rev 2002;6(1):55–69.188. Wisor JP, Nishino S, Sora I, Uhl G, Mignot E, Edgar DM.

Dopaminergic role instimulant-induced wakefulness.J Neurosci 2001;21(5):1787–94.

189. Gottesmann C. The neurochemistry of waking and sleepingmental activity: the disinhibition-dopamine hypothesis.Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002;56(4):345–54.

190. LeDoux J, et al. The emotional brain. New York: Phoenix;1999.

191. Kobayashi T, Homma Y, Good C, Skinner RD, Garcia-Rill E.Developmental changes in the effects of serotonin onneurons in the region of the pedunculopontine nucleus.Brain Res Dev Brain Res 2003;140(1):57–66.

192. Nieoullon A, Coquere A. Dopamine: a key regulator to adaptaction, emotion, motivation and cognition. Curr OpinNeurol 16(S2):3–10.

193. Van der Weer NJ, Stevens H, Haderman JA, et al. Enhanceddopamine transporter density in psychotropic-nativepatients with obsessive–compulsive disorder showed by(123 I) beta-Cit spect. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:2201–6.

194. Kimberg DY, Aguirre GK, Lease J, D’Esposito M. Corticaleffects of bromocriptine, a D-2 dopamine receptor agonist,in human subjects, revealed by MRI. Hum Brain Mapping2001;12(4):246–57.

195. Rothenberger A, Kirov R. Changes in sleep–wake behaviourmay be more than just an epiphenomenon of ADHD. BehavBrain Sci 2005;28(3):439.

196. Miller JD, Farber J, Gatz P, Roffwarg H, German DC. Activityof mesencephalic dopamine and non-dopamine neuronsacross stages of sleep and waking in the rat. Brain Res1983;273:133–41.

197. Pace-Schott EF, Hobson JA. The neurobiology of sleep:genetics, cellular physiology and subcortical networks. NatRev Neurosci 2002;3:591–605.

198. Muzur A, Pace-Schott EF, Hobson JA. The prefrontal cortexin sleep. Trends Cogn Sci 2002;6(11):475–81.

199. Mignot E, Taheri S, Nishino S. Sleeping with thehypothalamus: emerging therapeutic targets for sleepdisorders. Nat Neurosci 2002;5(Suppl. 1 Nov):1071–5.

200. Hobson JA, Pace-Schott EF, Stickgold R. Dreaming and thebrain: toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Sleepand dreaming—scientific advances and reconsiderations.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2003.

201. Kahn D, Pace-Schott EF, Hobson JA. Consciousness inwaking and dreaming: the roles of neuronal oscillation andneuromodulation in determining similarities anddifferences. Neuroscience 1997;78(1):13–38.

202. Solms M. Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled bydifferent brain mechanisms. Behav Brain Sci2000;23(6):843–50.

203. Nomura Y. Neurophysiology of Reet syndrome. Brain Dev2001;23:S50–S7.

204. Schwartz, Maquet P. Sleep imaging and the neuropsychology assessment of dreams. Trends Cogn Sci2002;6(1):23–30.

205. Buchsbaum MS, Hazlett EA, Bunney E. Positron emissiontomography with deoxyglucose-F18 imaging of sleep.Neuropsychopharmacology 2001;25:S50–S6.

206. Paus T. Primate anterior cingulated cortex: where motorcontrol, drive and cognition interface. Nat Rev Neurosci2001;2:417–24.

207. Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald III AW, Cho RY, Stenger A,Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring andadjustments in control. Science 2004;303:1023–6.

E U R O P E A N J O U R N A L O F P A E D I A T R I C N E U R O L O G Y 1 0 ( 2 0 0 6 ) 6 6 – 7 776

208. Cohen JD, Botvinick M, Carter C. Anterior cingulate andprefrontal cortex: who‘s in control? Nat Neurosci2000;3(5):421–3.

209. Damasio A. The feeling of what happens. New York: HarcourtBrace & Co.; 1999.

210. Stuss DT, Floden D, Alexander MP, Levine B, Katz D. Stoopperformance in focal lesion patients: dissociation ofprocesses and frontal lobe lesion location. Neuropsychology2001;39:771–86.

211. Sergeant J. The cognitive-energetic model: an empiricalapproach to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2000;24:7–12.

212. Fitzgerald KD, Welsh RC, Zbrozek JC, Taylor SF.Oculomotor response inhibition: a probe of anteriorcingulated function in children and adolescents.Presented at AACAP, Washington DC; October 19–24,2004.

213. Cazalis F, Valabregue R, Pelegrini-Issac M, Asloun S,Robbins TW, Granon S. Individual differnces in prefrontalcortical activation on the Tower of London planning task:implication for effortful processing. Eur J Neurosci2003;17(10):2219–25.

214. Bush G, Frasier J, Rauch S, et al. Anterior cingulated cortexdysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorderrevealed by fMRI and the Counting Stroop. Biol Psychiatry1999;45:1542–52.

215. Bush G, Shin LM, Holmes J, Rosen BR, Vougt BA. Themulti-Source Interference Task: validation study withfMRI in individual subjects. Mol Psychiatry 2003;8:60–70.

216. Bush G, Luu Phan, Posner M. Cognitive and emotionalinfluences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci2000;4(6):215–22.

217. Williams Z, Bush G, Rauch S, Crosgrove G, Eskandar E.Human anterior cingulated neurons and the integration ofmonetary reward with motor responses. Nat Neurosci2004;7(12):1370–5.

218. Bush G, Vogt B, Holmes J, et al. Dorsal anterior cingulatedcortex: a role in reward-based decision making. PNAS2002;99(1):523–8.

219. Ruskin DN, Liu C, Dunn KE, Bazan NG, LaHoste GJ. Sleepdeprivation impairs hippocampus-mediated contextuallearning but not amygdala-mediated cued learning in rats.Eur J Neurosci 2004;19(11):3121–4.

220. Stickgold R, Hobson JA, Fosse M. Sleep, learning anddreams: off-line memory reprocessing. Science2001;294(2):1052–7.

221. Siegel JM. The Rem sleep-memory consolidationhypothesis. Science 2001;294(2):1058–63.

222. Tononi G, Cirelli. Modulation of brain gene expressionduring sleep and wakefulness: a review of recentfindings. Neuropsychopharmacology 2001;25(S5):S28–S35.

223. Groenewegen HJ, van den Heuvel AO, Cath DC, Voorn P,Veltman DJ. Does an imbalance between the dorsal andventral striatopallidal systems play a role in Tourette’ssyndrome? A neuronal circuit approach. Brain Dev2003;25(Suppl. 1):S3–S14.

224. Novotny Jr EJ, Fulbright RK, Pearl PL, Gibson KM,Rothman DL. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy ofneurotransmitters in human brain. Ann Neurol2003;54(S6):S25–S31.

225. Posner M, Marcus ER. Images of mind. New York: ScientificAmerican Library; 1994 [Chapter 7 Networks of Attention.p. 153–180].

226. Mefford IN, Potter WZ. A neuroanatomical and biochemicalbasis for attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity inchildren: a defect in tonic adrenaline mediated inhibition oflocus coeruleus stimulation. Med Hypotheses1989;29(1):33–42.

227. Kariyawasam SH, Zaw F, Handley SL. Reduced salivarycortisol in children with co-morbid attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and oppositional defiantdisorder. Neuroendocrinol Lett 2002;1:45–8.

228. Berridge CW, Waterhouse BD. The locus coeruleus-noradrenergic system: modulation of behavioural state andstate-dependent cognitive processes. Brain Res Rev2003;42(1):33–84.

229. Jones BE. Arousal systems. Front Biosci 2003;8:S438–S51.230. Courvoisie H, Hooper S, Fine C, Kuyock L, Castillo M.

Neurometabolic functioning and neuropsychologicalcorrelates in children with ADHD-H: preliminaryfindings. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2004;16(1):63–9.

231. Jones K, Harrison Y. Frontal lobe function, sleep loss andfragmented sleep. Sleep Med Rev 2001;5(6):463–75.

232. Verhage M, Maia AS, Plomp JJ, et al. Synaptic assembly ofthe brain in the absence of neurotransmitter secretion.Science 2000;287(5454):864–9.

233. Laifenfeld D, Klein E, Ben-Shachar D. Norepinephrine altersthe expression of genes involved in neuronal sprouting anddifferentiation: relevance for major depression andantidepressant mechanisms. J Neurochem2002;83(5):1054–64.

234. Levitt P, Harvey JA, Friedman E, Simansky K, Murphy EH.New evidence for neurotransmitter influences on braindevelopmente. Trends Neurosci 1997;20:269–74.

235. Ernest M, Zametkin A, Matochik J, Jons P, Cohen R.DOPA decarboxylase activity in attention deficithyperactivity disorder adults. A (Fluorine-18) Fluorodopapositrion emission tomographic study. J Neurosci1998;18(15):5901–7.

236. Hutsler J, Galuske RAW. Hemispheric asymmetries incerebral cortical networks. Trends Neurosci2003;26(8):429–35.

237. Yang HW, Lin YW, Yen CD, Min MY. Change in bi-directional plasticity at CA1 synapses in hyppocampalslices taken from 6-hydroxydopamine-treated rats: the roleof endogenous norepinephrine. Eur J Neurosci2002;16(6):117–28.

238. Gu Q. Neuromodulatory transmmiter systems in the cortexand their role in cortical plasticity. Neuroscience2002;111(4):815–35.

239. Sakuragawa N, Elwan MA, Fujii T, Kawashima K. Possibledynamic neurotransmitter metabolism surrounding fetus.J Child Neurol 1999;14(4):265–6.

240. Sporns O, Alexander WH. Neuromodulation and plasticityin an autonomous robot. Neural Networks 2002;15(4–6):761–74.

241. Kulkarni VA, Jha S, Vaidya VA. Depletion ofneuropinephrine decreases the poliferation, but does notinfluence the survival and differentiation, of granule cellprogenitors in the adult rat hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci2002;16(10):2008–12.

242. Alvarez C, Vitalis T, Fon EA, et al. Effects of geneticdepletion of monoamines on somatosensory corticaldevelopment. Neuroscience 2002;115(3):753–64.

243. Chugani DC, Muzik O, behen M, et al. Developmentalchanges in brain serotonin synthesis capacity in autisticand nonautistic children. Ann Neurol 1999;45:S218–S95.

244. Segawa M. Discussant—pathophysiologies of Rettsyndrome. Brain Dev 2001;23:S218–S23.

245. Cohen-Cory S. The developing synapse: construction andmodulation of synaptic structures and circuits. Science2002;298:770–6.

246. Dettling ACC, Feldon J, Pryce CR. Repeated parentaldeprivation in the infant common marmoset (Callithrixjacchus, primates) and analysis of its effects on earlydevelopment. Biol Psychiatry 2002;52(11):1037–46.

E U R O P E A N J O U R N A L O F P A E D I A T R I C N E U R O L O G Y 1 0 ( 2 0 0 6 ) 6 6 – 7 7 77

247. Kanitz E, Otten W, Tuchscherer M, Manteuffel G. Effects ofprenatal stress on corticosteroid receptors and monamineconcentrations in limbic areas of suckling piglets (susScrofa) at different ages. J Vet Med A Physiol Pathol Clin Med2003;50(3):132–9.

248. Van der Wee NJ, Stevens H, Hardman JA, et al. Enhanceddopamine trnsporter density in psychotropic-nativepatients with obsessive-compulsive disorder shown by(123 I) beta-CIT SPECT. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:2201–6.

249. Snock H, Van Goozen SH, Matthys W, et al. Serotoninergicfunctioning in children with oppositional defiant disorder:a sumatriptan challenge study. Biol Psychiatry2002;51(4):319–25.

250. Gerschenfeld HM, Stefani E. 5-Hydroxytryptamine receptorsand synaptic transmission in molluscan neurons. Nature1965;205:1216–8.

251. Gerschenfeld HM, Stefani E. An electrophysiologicalstudy of 5 hydroxytriptamine receptors of neurons inthe molluscan nervous system. J Physiol1966;185:684–700.

252. The Tourrette’s syndrome study group. Treatment of ADHDin children with tics—a randomized controlled trial.Neurology 2002;58:527–36.

253. Dorrego MF, Canevaro L, Starkstein SE. A randomizeddouble-blind, cross-over study of methyliphenidate andlithium in adults with ADHD: preliminary findings.Neuroscience 2002;14(3):289–95.

254. Hansen AL, Johnson BH. Vagal influence on workingmemory and attention. Int J Psychophysiol 2003;48(3):263–74.

255. John Popp A. Music, musicians and the brain: anexploration of musical genius. The 2004 presidentialaddress. J Neurosurg 2004;895–903.

256. Keynes M. Creativity and psychopathology. Lancet1995;345:138–9.

257. DeLong GR. Disorders of memory in childhood with a focuson temporal lobe disease and autism. In: Segalowitz SJ,Rapin I, editors. Child neuropshychology. Amsterdam:Elsevier; 2003. p. 731–51.

258. Johnston MV. Brain plasticity in pediatric neurology. EurJ Paed Neurol 2003;7:105–13.


Recommended