+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached...

Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached...

Date post: 11-Mar-2021
Category:
Upload: others
View: 4 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
21
Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language Impairment: A New Look at an Old Hypothesis Purpose: To explore the sensitivity of children with specific language impairment (SLI) to amplitude-modulated and durational cues that are important for perceiving suprasegmental speech rhythm and stress patterns. Method: Sixty-three children between 7 and 11 years of age were tested, 21 of whom had a diagnosis of SLI, 21 of whom were matched for chronological age to the SLI sample, and 21 of whom were matched for language age to the SLI sample. All children received a battery of nonspeech auditory processing tasks along with standardized measures of phonology and language. Results: As many as 70%80% of children diagnosed with SLI were found to perform below the 5th percentile of age-matched controls in auditory processing tasks measuring sensitivity to amplitude envelope rise time and sound duration. Furthermore, individual differences in sensitivity to these cues predicted unique variance in language and literacy attainment, even when age, nonverbal IQ, and task-related (attentional) factors were controlled. Conclusion: Many children with SLI have auditory processing difficulties, but for most children, these are not specific to brief, rapidly successive acoustic cues. Instead, sensitivity to durational and amplitude envelope cues appear to predict language and literacy outcomes more strongly. This finding now requires replication and exploration in languages other than English. KEY WORDS: phonology, auditory processing, speech and language C hildren with specific language impairment (SLI) have expressive and receptive oral language deficits that interfere with their edu- cational achievements and their communicative abilities. These difficulties are exhibited in the presence of normal nonverbal intelligence and hearing ability, along with an apparent absence of neurological dys- function. Although the general profile of children with SLI is well es- tablished, the underlying cause or causes of the disorder have been the subject of much debate. A prominent low-level causal theory is that chil- dren with SLI have difficulties in processing brief, rapidly successive acoustic stimuli and that these difficulties lead directly to their language problems (Tallal & Piercy, 1973a). Higher level theories fall into two broad categories. One category includes theories that assume specific deficits in language knowledgefor example, knowledge of implicit rules for marking tense, number, and person (e.g., Gopnik & Crago, 1991), or an extended period during which children believe that finiteness-marking is optional (Rice & Wexler, 1996). The second category includes theories that assume deficits in language processingfor example, the surface deficit hypothesis proposed by Leonard (1995). According to this hypothesis, Kathleen Corriveau Elizabeth Pasquini Usha Goswami Centre for Neuroscience in Education, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol. 50 647666 June 2007 D American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 1092-4388/07/5003-0647 647
Transcript
Page 1: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

Basic Auditory Processing Skillsand Specific Language Impairment:A New Look at an Old Hypothesis

Purpose: To explore the sensitivity of children with specific language impairment (SLI)to amplitude-modulated and durational cues that are important for perceivingsuprasegmental speech rhythm and stress patterns.Method: Sixty-three children between 7 and 11 years of age were tested, 21 of whomhad a diagnosis of SLI, 21 of whom were matched for chronological age to theSLI sample, and 21 of whom were matched for language age to the SLI sample.All children received a battery of nonspeech auditory processing tasks along withstandardized measures of phonology and language.Results: As many as 70%–80% of children diagnosed with SLI were found to performbelow the 5th percentile of age-matched controls in auditory processing tasksmeasuring sensitivity to amplitude envelope rise time and sound duration. Furthermore,individual differences in sensitivity to these cues predicted unique variance in languageand literacy attainment, even when age, nonverbal IQ, and task-related (attentional)factors were controlled.Conclusion: Many children with SLI have auditory processing difficulties, but formost children, these are not specific to brief, rapidly successive acoustic cues. Instead,sensitivity to durational and amplitude envelope cues appear to predict languageand literacy outcomes more strongly. This finding now requires replication andexploration in languages other than English.

KEY WORDS: phonology, auditory processing, speech and language

C hildren with specific language impairment (SLI) have expressiveand receptive oral language deficits that interfere with their edu-cational achievements and their communicative abilities. These

difficulties are exhibited in the presence of normal nonverbal intelligenceand hearing ability, along with an apparent absence of neurological dys-function. Although the general profile of children with SLI is well es-tablished, the underlying cause or causes of the disorder have been thesubject of much debate. A prominent low-level causal theory is that chil-dren with SLI have difficulties in processing brief, rapidly successiveacoustic stimuli and that these difficulties lead directly to their languageproblems (Tallal & Piercy, 1973a). Higher level theories fall into two broadcategories. One category includes theories that assume specific deficitsin language knowledge—for example, knowledge of implicit rules formarking tense, number, and person (e.g., Gopnik & Crago, 1991), or anextended period during which children believe that finiteness-markingis optional (Rice &Wexler, 1996). The second category includes theoriesthat assume deficits in language processing—for example, the surfacedeficit hypothesis proposed by Leonard (1995). According to this hypothesis,

Kathleen CorriveauElizabeth Pasquini

Usha GoswamiCentre for Neuroscience in Education,

University of Cambridge,Cambridge, England

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 50 • 647–666 • June 2007 • D American Speech-Language-Hearing Association1092-4388/07/5003-0647

647

Page 2: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

childrenwithSLImayhave difficulties in acquiring gram-maticalmorphemeswith lowphonetic substance (i.e., mor-phemeswith short-duration, low-intensityacoustics).Bothlanguage knowledge and language processing accountsexpect that the hypothesized deficits will characterizechildren with SLI across the world’s languages.

The potential role of auditory processing difficultiesin explaining SLI has been explored in depth by Tallaland her colleagues (Benasich & Tallal, 2002; Spitz,Tallal, Flax, & Benasich, 1997; Tallal & Piercy, 1973a,1973b, 1974, 1975). They have proposed a rapid tem-poral processing deficit account of SLI. Difficulties inrapid temporal processing are thought to explain lan-guage problems “as speech occurs at roughly 80 ms perphoneme” (Tallal & Piercy, 1973a, p. 397). The originalrapid temporal processing hypothesis was based on alandmark series of studies by Tallal and Piercy (1973a,1973b, 1974, 1975; see alsoEfron, 1963). Tallal andPiercyadministered a temporal order judgment (TOJ) task totwelve 8- to 12-year-old children with language impair-ments and 12 control participants matched for age andnonverbal IQ. In the TOJ task, the children had to learnto associate a button press with a particular tone (high orlow). They were then asked to listen to two tones and torespond by pressing the correct buttons in the appropri-ate order. Childrenwith SLIwere found to be impaired inthis task when stimuli were brief (75 ms) and were sep-aratedbyshort interstimulus intervals (ISIs). The childrenwith SLI did not differ from the control participants whenISIs were longer (>150 ms or >305 ms, depending on thestudy). Although difficulties in rapid auditory processinghave subsequently been reported in some studies of chil-drenwithSLI (Alexander&Frost,1982; Frumkin&Rapin,1980), theyhavenot been found in others (Bishop,Carlyon,Deeks,&Bishop, 1999;Helzer,Champlin,&Gillam,1996;Norrelgen, Lacerda, & Forssberg, 2002). Some now arguethat although children with SLI may show auditory pro-cessing deficits, these deficits are not characterized by therapidity of the stimuli (seeMcArthur&Bishop, 2001, andRosen, 2003, for reviews). Others have argued thatwhenchildren with SLI show difficulties in perceptual tasks,thismay arise from auditory immaturity (Bishop, Adams,Nation, & Rosen, 2005) or from task artifacts (Coady,Kluender,&Evans, 2005).The role of auditoryperceptualdeficits in explaining the etiology of SLI is thus stronglydebated.

Auditory processing of cues related to speech pros-ody has not been widely investigated in children withSLI. This is surprising because recent work in infant lan-guage acquisition has shown that prosody plays an im-portant role in word learning. For example, prosodic cues(in particular, changes in duration and stress) carry im-portant information about how sounds are ordered intowords when the words are multisyllabic. It is estimatedthat 90% of English bisyllabic content words follow a

strong–weak syllable pattern, with the stress on thefirst syllable (e.g.,monkey, bottle, doctor, sister). Jusczyk,Houston, and Newsome (1999) were able to show that7.5-month-old infants can learn that word onsets arealigned with strong (stressed) syllables and that thisguides them in picking out words in speech. The infantstended tomis-segmentwordswithanatypicalweak–strongsyllable pattern, such as guitar and surprise. More re-cently, Curtin, Mintz, and Christiansen (2005) demon-strated that stresswas an integral part of the phonologicalrepresentations developed by 7-month-old infants. Theyfirst analyzeda corpus of phonologically transcribed speechdirected to British infants between 6 and 16 weeks oldto see whether a connectionist model would be able tolearn word representations better when stress providedan additional cue. The addition of stress to the syllablerepresentations led to better segmentation performanceby the model. Curtin et al. suggested that lexical stressmakes it easier to distinguish transitional probabilitiesin the speech stream. To test this idea, they familiarized7-month-old infants with novel words presented in realEnglish sentences. The novel words either had the lex-ical stress typical of English (DObita) or atypical stress(doBIta). The question was whether the two types ofword, which contained the same phonemes and transi-tional probabilities, would be represented as distinct bythe infants on the basis of whether they contained initialor medial stress. The results showed that the infantspreferred the sentences that contained the words withinitial stress. Curtin et al. concluded that lexical stressis retained in the protolexical representation. Indeed,in natural language, lexical forms with identical pho-nemes but differential stress patterns may be differentwords (as inCONtent and conTENT; Fry, 1954). Experi-ments such as these suggest that an early insensitivityto auditory prosodic cues to speech rhythm and stresscould have profound effects on the development of thelanguage system.

In this study, we therefore set out to explore thepossibility that children with SLI might have basic au-ditory processing impairments to suprasegmental cuesto speech rhythm and syllable stress. Recent theoriesof stress perception give central importance to the cuesof amplitude and duration (e.g., Greenberg, 1999, 2006;Greenberg, Carvey, Hitchcock, &Chang, 2003; Kochanski,Grabe,Coleman,&Rosner, 2005).For example,Greenberg(1999) described an automatic prosodic algorithm devel-oped to label stressed and unstressed syllables in a cor-pus of spontaneous speech. The algorithm depends onthree separate parameters of the acoustic signal: (a) du-ration, (b) amplitude, and (c) fundamental frequency. Incontrast to classic accounts, Greenberg (1999) reportedthat “fundamental frequency turns out to be relativelyunimportant for distinguishingbetween thepresence andabsence of prosodic prominence . . . the results indicate

648 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 50 • 647–666 • June 2007

Page 3: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

that the product of amplitude and duration . . . yields theperformance closest to . . . linguistic transcribers” (p. 172).Kochanski et al. (2005) reached similar conclusions in aninvestigation of a large corpus of natural speech coveringseven English dialects. They concluded that “Contrary totextbooks and common assumption, fundamental fre-quency played a minor role in distinguishing prominentsyllables from the rest of the utterance. . .speakers pri-marilymarkedprominencewith patterns of loudness andduration” (p. 1038). Choi, Hasegawa-Johnson, and Cole(2005) also demonstrateda greater role for amplitudeandduration cues in detecting stressed syllables in compar-ison to pitch cues in their study of machine detection ofprosodicboundaries in theBostonUniversityRadioSpeechCorpus. Amplitude and duration also played a key rolein detecting intonational boundaries, the detection ofwhich is particularly likely to be related to grammaticalacquisition by children. It is very notable that grammat-ical deficits in SLI tend to vary across languages (e.g.,Bedore & Leonard, 2000; Bortolini & Leonard, 2000;Leonard & Bortolini, 1998; Roberts & Leonard, 1997).The reasonmay be that different languages use differentprosodic cues to highlight different aspects of syntax. Inthe present study, we focused on children’s sensitivity toamplitude envelope rise time and duration.

The hypothesis underlying the present studies—that grammatical deficits in SLI may be linked to theperception of amplitude and duration cues that signalstressed and unstressed parts of words and sentences—is similar in principle to the position long advocated byLeonard (e.g., Leonard, Eyer, Bedore, & Grela, 1997;McGregor & Leonard, 1994). Leonard has also proposedthat difficulties with prosody may underlie many of theimpairments noted in the grammatical morphology ofchildren with SLI. However, Leonard’s perceptual hy-pothesis is framed in terms of phonetic substance. It ishypothesized that syllables that are shorter, of loweramplitude, and of lower pitch cause particular difficul-ties. For example, childrenwith SLI showproblemswithnonfinal weak syllables across languages (Bedore &Leonard, 2000; Bortolini & Leonard, 2000; Leonard &Bortolini, 1998; Roberts & Leonard, 1997). The hypoth-esis is that processing limitations are the key to thispattern of data. Processing speed in children with SLI ishypothesized to be slow, and consequently it is arguedthat processing limitations are exacerbated when mor-phemes are brief. In contrast, we propose that childrenwith SLI do not have a problem with processing speed,per se; instead, they are expected to have difficultieswhenacoustic cues are extended over time. Hence, it is longerduration rather than shorter duration that should be prob-lematic, particularly if amplitude changes with duration.Extended amplitude envelope onsets or cues with lon-ger durations should be particularly difficult to distin-guish. This alternative prosodic hypothesis argues that

the variation in children’s grammatical errors across lan-guages and contexts is related to the temporal integrationof changes in amplitude across duration—that is, ampli-tude envelope onset cues—rather than to slower process-ing of briefer cues.

The purpose of this studywas, therefore, to examinebasic auditory processing abilities related to perceivingstress and syllable prominence in a sample of childrendiagnosed ashavingSLI.The rise timeandduration tasksused were drawn from prior studies of children with devel-opmental dyslexia. These studies have reported impairedsensitivity to rise time and duration in such samples(Goswami et al., 2002; Muneaux, Ziegler, Truc, Thomson,&Goswami,2004;Richardson,Thomson,Scott,&Goswami,2004). Dyslexia is a developmental language disorder thatis sometimes comorbid with SLI (Catts, Adlof, Hogan, &Weismer, 2005). In the present study, comparisons weremade between a sample of children diagnosed with anSLIand samples of typically developing childrenmatchedfor age and language abilities. If a specific deficit in sen-sitivity to rise time and duration were to be found inchildren diagnosed with SLI, this would be a first stepin investigating whether developmental speech and lan-guage deficits arise, in part, from a relative insensitivityto stress cues to syllable prominence thatmay carry gram-matical information.

MethodParticipants

Sixty-three children aged between 7 and 11 yearsparticipated in this study. Only children who had no di-agnosed additional learning difficulties (e.g., dyspraxia,attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, autistic spec-trum disorder, dyslexia1), a nonverbal IQ above 80, andEnglish as the first language spoken at home were in-cluded. All participants received a short hearing screenvia an audiometer. Sounds were presented in both theleft and right ear at a range of frequencies (250, 500,1000, 2000, 4000, and 8000Hz), and all participants weresensitive to sounds at the 20 dB HL level.

Twenty-one of the children (13 boys and 8 girls;M = 10;2 [years;months], SD = 0;11) had a statement ofSLI from their local education authority andwere drawnfrom school language support units or referred by localspeech-language therapists (SLI group). They were as-sessed experimentally using two expressive and two re-ceptive subtests of the Clinical Evaluation of LanguageFundamentals—3 (CELF–3; Semel,Wiig,&Secord, 1995)

1No children in the sample had a diagnosis of dyslexia. However, whentested for the study, 5 children in the SLI sample were found to havestandardized single-word reading scores that were more than 1.5 SDsbelow the population mean.

Corriveau et al.: Basic Auditory Processing in SLI 649

Page 4: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

and were included in the study if they scored below1.5 SDs on two or more of these subtests. Individualstandard scores of the children in the SLI group for thefour CELF subtests administered—the expressive andreceptive vocabularymeasures, nonverbal IQ, and single-word reading scores—are shown in Table 1. Note that inour prior studies of dyslexia, only children with a clinicaldiagnosis of dyslexia and no history of speech or languageimpairments were studied. Here, we studied childrenwith a clinical diagnosis of SLI and no diagnosis of read-ing impairments; hence, the overlap between this pop-ulation and the children in our prior studies is small(although note that 5 of the 21 children in the samplewith SLI did score poorly on the test of single-word read-ing that we administered experimentally—namely, Par-ticipants 5, 6, 7, 16, and 18).

Forty-two control children from a local school wereincluded. Of these, 21 were chronological-age-matchedcontrols (CA group; 9 boys, 12 girls; M = 9;9, SD = 2;4),and21were language-ability-matched controls (LAgroup;11 boys, 10 girls; M = 7;8, SD = 8 months). The LA groupwasmatched to the childrenwith SLI through use of rawscores on the tests of expressive vocabulary (WechslerIntelligence Scale for Children, Vocabulary subtest;

Wechsler,1974) and receptive vocabulary (BritishPictureVocabularyScale [BPVS];Dunn,Dunn,Whetton,&Pintilie,1982). Raw scoreswerematched towithin 5 points (± 2SE).Participant characteristics are shown in Table 2.

TasksPsychometric Tests

The children received psychometric tests of IQ, lan-guage, reading, rapid naming ability, andworkingmem-ory. Language abilities in the SLI group were checkedthrough the use of two receptive subtests (Concepts andDirections, Semantic Relations) and two expressive sub-tests (Formulating Sentences, Sentence Assembly) ofthe CELF–3. For all children, receptive vocabulary wasmeasured through use of the BPVS. All children werealso given standardized tests of single-word and non-word reading (Test of WordReadingEfficiency [TOWRE];Torgesen,Wagner,&Rashotte, 1999), reading comprehen-sion (Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions [WORD],Comprehension subtest; Rust, Golombok, & Trickey,1992), spelling (British Ability Scales; Elliott, Smith, &McCulloch, 1996), rapid color naming (CELF–3 RapidColor Naming subtest; Semel et al., 1995), word recall

Table 1. Individual scores for the language measures in the group of children with specific language impairment (SLI).

ParticipantReceptive

vocabularyaExpressivevocabularyb

CELFExpressivec CELF ReceptiveNonverbal

IQdSingle-wordreadingeFS SA SR CD

1 88 8 3 3 3 3 82 1162 69 3 3 3 3 3 92 943 88 9 3 4 3 4 88 1174 95 7 3 6 4 3 85 1125 81 7 3 7 5 3 107 716 90 5 3 6 5 6 85 637 90 7 3 4 3 4 85 718 92 10 3 3 4 4 125 839 81 8 3 7 5 9 95 91

10 75 7 3 3 3 3 85 9611 78 6 3 3 4 3 103 8712 74 7 3 3 3 3 99 9613 73 5 5 3 4 3 80 8914 77 6 3 3 3 3 101 9115 84 4 3 4 5 3 85 9616 83 6 3 3 3 3 80 6717 76 6 5 4 3 3 88 9018 85 5 3 3 5 3 85 6419 90 6 3 5 5 3 110 9220 82 8 3 3 4 3 95 8221 79 6 3 6 4 6 80 97

Note. FS = Formulating Sentences; SA = Sentence Assembly; SR = Semantic Relationships; CD = Concepts and Directions.aBritish Picture Vocabulary Scale standard score (M = 100, SD = 15). bWechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) Vocabulary standard score(M = 10, SD = 3). cClinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF) Expressive and Receptive subtests (M = 10, SD = 3). dWISC Performance IQ(M = 100, SD = 15). eTest of Word Reading Efficiency Sight Word subtest (M = 100, SD = 15).

650 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 50 • 647–666 • June 2007

Page 5: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

(Working Memory Test Battery for Children; Pickering& Gathercole, 2001), and nonword repetition (Children’sTest of NonwordRepetition;Gathercole&Baddeley, 1996).Finally, all children received four subtests of the WechslerIntelligence Scale for Children (WISC–III; Wechsler,1974): (a) Block Design, (b) Picture Arrangement, (c) Sim-ilarities, and (d) Vocabulary. IQ scores were then proratedfor each child from these subtests following the procedureadopted by Sattler (1982).

Experimental Phonological TasksPhoneme deletion task. This task was a shortened

form of a similar task described by McDougall, Hulme,Ellis, and Monk (1994). Children heard 18 nonwords(15 test words and 3 practice words) presented orally bythe experimenter and were asked to delete a particularphoneme, or phoneme cluster and to repeat the wordswithout that phoneme or phoneme cluster; for example,“Say bice without the /b/.” “Say splow without the /p/.”Phonemes and phoneme blends were deleted at variouspoints throughout the word (initial, medial, final). Themaximum score possible on this task was 15. After pi-loting, a livevoice taskwasselectedovera synthetic speechtaskbecause thechildrenweremoreengagedby the former.

Rime oddity task. In this task, children heard 24 trip-lets of words (20 test triplets and 4 practice triplets) pre-sented on the computer using digitized speech createdfrom a native female speaker of British English. Ten ofthe experimental word triplets came from dense rimeneighborhoods (mean number of rime neighbors = 24.33,

SD = 3.13), and the remaining 10 triplets came fromsparse rimeneighborhoods (meannumber of rime neigh-bors = 8.63, SD = 3.20). All 60 experimental words werematched for frequency, and triplets werematched for thedifference in rimeneighborhood density between the oddword out and the two rhyming words. The maximumscore possible in this task was 20. The stimuli were pre-sented in a random order through headphones at 73 dBSPLusing E-Prime 1.0 (Psychology Software Tools, Pitts-burgh, PA). Three different orders of trial presentationwere used, counterbalanced across children. After eachtriplet, the child was asked to say the word that did notsound the same as the other two, and his or her responsewas recorded on the keyboard by the experimenter.

Psychoacoustic TasksAll psychoacoustic stimuli were presented binaurally

through headphones at 73 dB SPL. Earphone sensitivitywas calculated using a Zwislocki coupler in one ear of aKEMARmanikin (Burkhard & Sachs, 1975). Children’sresponses were recorded on the keyboard by the exper-imenter. Many of the psychoacoustic measures usedthe “dinosaur” threshold estimation program created byDorothy Bishop (Oxford University), which used a two-interval forced-choice paradigmwith a 500-ms ISI. In alltasks using the dinosaur program, the child heard eachdinosaur make a sound and was asked to choose whichdinosaur produced the target sound. Feedback was givenonline throughout the course of the experiment. The di-nosaur program used the more virulent form of Param-eter Settings by Sequential Estimation (PEST; Findlay,1978) to staircase adaptively through the stimulus set onthe basis of the participant’s previous answer. Therefore,the number of trials completed by individual participantsvaried slightly (maximum number of trials = 40). Thethreshold score achieved was based on the 75% correctpoint for the last four reversals. In ongoing work with chil-dren with dyslexia, we are investigating the effects of at-tentional lapses in thePESTprocedure usedhere and arefinding that it is robust in terms of thresholds achieved.

Intensity discrimination. This dinosaur task wasmodeled after the loudness perception task as describedby Ivry and Keele (1989) and was intended as a controltask for the attentional demands of the psychoacousticprocedures.2 A continuum of 31 stimuli was createdusing half of the stimuli used by Ivry and Keele. Thestimuli ranged in loudness from 73 to 81.1 dB SPL, with0.27 dB SPL between each step. Only half of the originalIvry and Keele stimuli were used because this task wasalso presented in the dinosaur format and, therefore,could take only a single adaptive staircase procedure.Each pure tone was presented at 1000 Hz for 50 ms. The

2To date, participants in our studies of developmental dyslexia have nothad any difficulties in intensity discrimination.

Table 2. Mean (standard deviation) participant characteristics for thestandardized tasks.

Group SLI CA match LA match F(2, 60)

N 21 21 21

Agea,b 10;2 9;9 7;8 66.69***SD 0.94 2.38 0.67

Nonverbal IQc 92.14 97.29 104.09 1.37SD 11.75 10.08 8.67

BPVS rawb,d,e 78.43 104.19 79.19 50.76***SD 7.48 8.89 11.39

WISC Vocab. rawb,d,f 20.15 28.7 20.67 23.10***SD 2.64 6.33 4.15

aSLI > language-ability-matched (LA) participants, p < .001. bchronological-age-matched (CA) participants > LA, p < .001. cNonverbal IQwas estimatedfrom the WISC Block Design and Picture Arrangement subtests (M = 100,SD = 15). dCA > SLI, p < .001. eRaw score was calculated using standardceiling-to-floor guidelines of the British Picture Vocabulary Scale (BPVS;maximum = 144). fRaw score was calculated using the WISC vocabulary(Vocab.) procedures (maximum = 40).

***p < .001.

Corriveau et al.: Basic Auditory Processing in SLI 651

Page 6: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

level of the standard tone was 73 dB SPL. Children wereasked to choose the dinosaur thatmade the loudest sound.

Amplitude envelope onset rise time (one-ramp rise timetask). For this dinosaur task, a continuum of 40 stimuliwas created from a 500-Hz sinusoid. The linear rise timeenvelope varied logarithmically from15ms to 300ms.Theshortest rise time was set at 15 ms to avoid spectral splat-ter. The overall duration of the stimuli remained constantat 800 ms, and the duration of the linear fall time wasfixed at 50 ms. Children heard the stimulus with thelongest rise time (300 ms) as the standard sound andwere asked to choose the dinosaur that made the soundthat was sharpest at the beginning. Examples of thestimuli are shown in Figure 1.

Rise time from a carrier (two-ramp rise time task). Inthis dinosaur task, a continuum of 40 stimuli was cre-ated using a sinusoidal carrier at 500 Hz amplitude-modulated at the rate of 0.7 Hz (depth of 50%). Eachstimulus was 3,750 ms long (2.5 cycles). Rise time was

again varied logarithmically from 15 ms to 300 ms, andthe fall time was fixed at 350ms. Children always heardthe longest rise time sound as the standard sound andwere asked to choose the dinosaur that made the soundthat had the sharpest beat. Examples of the stimuli areshown in Figure 2.

Temporal order judgment (TOJ) task. This task wasmodeled after similar tasks used by Tallal and col-leagues (Tallal & Piercy, 1973a, 1973b) but avoided theadditional cognitive demands introduced by their button-press procedure and used more ecologically valid stim-uli. It was intended to examine children’s sensitivity tobrief consecutive sounds presented rapidly in time. Twosounds readily identifiable as a dog bark and a car hornwith a fundamental frequency of 400 Hz and a durationof 115 ms were used as the stimuli in this task. A con-tinuum of 40 soundswas createdwith variable stimulusonset asynchrony (SOA) fromj405ms (dog first, SOA =405 ms) to 405 ms (car first, SOA = 405 ms) with a step

Figure 1. Schematic depiction of the stimulus wave form for theone-ramp task with (a) 15-ms rise time and (b) 300-ms rise time. Figure 2. Schematic depiction of the stimulus wave form for the two-

ramp task with (a) 15-ms rise time and (b) 300-ms rise time.

652 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 50 • 647–666 • June 2007

Page 7: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

size of about 20 ms. Stimuli were allowed to overlap tothe degree necessary to present the specified SOA. Thestimuli were presented using a Speech Pattern Audi-ometer II, a C++ derivative of a categorical perceptionprocedure created by Stuart Rosen (UCL, London, En-gland). Before the test block, participants were given sixpractice stimuli, which represented the most extremedog–car and car–dog SOA (405 ms), a middle SOA (237 ms),and a very difficult SOA (74ms). Before beginning the testtrials, each participant was required to give the correctanswer for all 6 practice trials. A maximum of 40 exper-imental trials were then presented to each participant.Each subsequent trial was determined using a modifica-tion of Levitt’s (1971) procedure created by Stuart Rosen.Summary statistics for the categorization slope and cate-gory boundarywere derivedusing probit analysis (Finney,1971). For all trials, children were asked to choose thesound (dog bark or car horn) that was perceived first.Note that because environmental noises were used, thistask is not directly comparable to the TOJ task of Tallaland Piercy. However, the auditory characteristics of ourstimuli probablymake themmoreappropriate tousewhenexamining the influence of auditory perception on speechperception.

Duration Discrimination Task 1 (briefer sounds du-ration task). This dinosaur task was modeled after thetime perception task described by Ivry and Keele (1989).A continuum of 31 stimuli was created using pure tones.The stimuli ranged in duration from 400 ms to 640 ms,with 8 ms between each step. Each pure tone was pre-sented at 1000 Hz. The duration of the standard tonewas 400ms. Children were asked to choose the dinosaurthat made the longest sound.

Duration Discrimination Task 2 (longer sounds du-ration task). This task used the exact parameters of thetime estimation task used by Ramus, Pidgeon, and Frith(2003), which was modeled after Nicolson, Fawcett, andDean (1995). Twenty-three pure-tone stimuli with a fre-quency of 800Hzwere createdwith respective durationsof 400, 700, 800, 900, 950, 1000, 1050, 1100, 1140, 1160,1180, 1200, 1220, 1240, 1260, 1300, 1350, 1400, 1450,1500, 1600, 1700, and 2000 ms. The ISI between the twotoneswas 1,000ms. Each trial was repeated three times,amounting to 66 test trials, presented randomly. Thetest block was preceded by a practice block of the eightmost extreme stimulus pairs, presented randomly. Feed-back was given in the practice block but not in the testblock. The stimuli were presented in E-Prime 1.0 using anonadaptive paradigm. The duration of the standardtone was 1,200 ms. The child was asked to determine ifthe second tone was longer or shorter than the first tone.Although this procedure differs from that used in Dura-tion Discrimination Task 1, it was followed in order to beconsistent with prior studies of durational processing oftones by children with dyslexia.

ProcedureThe children were assessed individually in a quiet

room within their school. Children first received theWISC–III, BPVS, CELF–III (if applicable), and the audi-ometer screening tasks. The remaining tasks were pre-sented in the following fixed order: one-ramp rise time,rapid color naming,DurationDiscriminationTask 1, TOJ,rime oddity, workingmemory, phoneme deletion, nonwordrepetition, two-ramp rise time, Duration DiscriminationTask 2, single-word reading, nonword reading, intensitydiscrimination, spelling, reading comprehension. Sessionlengths varied on the basis of children’s attentiveness;sessions were terminated as soon as children exhibitedattentional lapses. On average, children completed thetasks in four sessions of approximately 40min.However,session lengths for childrenwithSLIwereusually shorterthan for children in the two control groups, and somechildren with SLI were seen for five sessions.

To ensure that all children understood the directionsfor the computer tasks, a rigorous practice procedure wasapplied before the presentation of the experimental tasks.For the dinosaur tasks, children were required to answerfour of five practice trials correctly. For all other computertasks, children were required to answer all practice trialscorrectly. Practice trialswere repeated until childrenwereable to complete the requisite number of trials correctly.Both verbal and nonverbal (pointing) responses were ac-cepted;many children inall threeparticipant groups choseto point to the pictures on the computer screen instead ofgiving a verbal response.

ResultsChildren’s performance in the reading, memory,

and phonological awareness measures is displayed inTables 3 and 4. One-way between-subjects analyses ofvariance (ANOVAs) by group (SLI, CA, LA) were con-ducted for all of the tasks given. The ANOVAs revealedsignificant group differences for all psychometric mea-sures, as shown in Table 3. Post hoc Bonferroni testsrevealed that children with SLI were significantly im-paired comparedwith their CA andLA control groups onevery measure except for CELF Rapid Color Naming.For this task, the children with SLI were only impairedcompared with the CA control participants.

Children’s mean performance in the experimentalmeasures is displayed in Table 4. For the intensity andamplitude envelope tasks, children’s performance wasmeasured in terms of the threshold at which they wereable to detect reliably the difference between the twosounds (75%of the time). For example, an intensity thresh-old of 76 dB would indicate that the person can detect adifference between the standard stimulus (73 dB SPL)

Corriveau et al.: Basic Auditory Processing in SLI 653

Page 8: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

and a test stimulus with 75% accuracy as long as thesetwo stimuli differ by 3 dB. An amplitude envelope thresh-old of 190 ms would indicate that the person can detect adifference between the standard stimulus (300 ms) and atest stimulus with 75% accuracy when the test stimulushas a rise time of 110 ms. An amplitude envelope thresh-old of 265 ms would indicate that the person can detect adifference between the standard stimulus (300 ms) and atest stimulus with 75% accuracy when the test stimulushas a rise time of 35ms. Children’s 75% correct thresholdwasalso calculated in the twoduration tasks; these scoreswere then log-transformed following customary practice.For the TOJ task, performance was measured in termsof slope, boundary, and threshold. The slope is ameasureof the child’s consistency in categorizing the sounds aseither “dog first” or “car first,” regardless of accuracy. Amore negative slope indicates more consistent catego-rization than a less negative slope. The boundary is ameasure of the difference between the point at which the

stimuli shifted from “dog first” to “car first” and the pointat which children judged this shift to occur. Childrenwith lower boundary scores were more accurate in judg-ing this shift than were children with higher boundaryscores. The mean TOJ threshold is the average SOA atwhich children correctly judged the temporal order of thestimuli with 75% accuracy. Recall that the length of thedog/car sounds was 115 ms. Thus, a child with a thresh-old of 130mswould accurately judge the order of the twosounds with 75% accuracy as long as they were separatedby an interval of at least 15 ms (130 ms minus 115 ms).

One-way between-subjects ANOVAs by group (SLI,CA, LA) were conducted for all experimental measures.Significant group differences were revealed on all tasksexcept for the intensity task, TOJ slope, TOJ boundary,and TOJ threshold. Post hoc Bonferroni tests indicatedthat the SLI group was significantly impaired with re-spect to the CA control participants in the one-ramp risetime, two-ramp rise time, and Duration Discrimination

Table 3. Mean performance (standard deviation) in the reading, memory, and rapid naming tasks by group (SLI, CA, LA).

Group SLI SD CA match SD LA match SD F(2, 60) Effect size (h2)

Working memorya,b 77.43 9.52 94.24 13.37 103.00 13.56 14.23*** .44Nonword repetition (max = 30)a,b 18.28 3.30 25.42 4.11 22.62 4.54 16.81*** .36TOWRE worda,b 88.81 15.51 107.91 11.37 114.95 9.45 25.09*** .46TOWRE nonworda,b 85.09 11.23 104.47 13.65 109.85 12.34 30.31*** .50BAS spellinga,b 84.09 14.04 113.43 11.10 118.05 12.14 39.76*** .56Reading comprehensiona,b 76.52 8.51 96.95 11.02 105.57 9.78 48.36*** .61CELF rapid color naming (s)a 32.15 11.92 22.19 3.77 26.25 6.07 8.24*** .22

Note. TOWRE = Test of Word Reading Efficiency; BAS = British Ability Scales; s = seconds.aCA > SLI, p < .001. bLA > SLI, p < .001.

***p < .001.

Table 4. Mean performance (standard deviation) in the experimental phonological and psychoacoustic measures by group (SLI, CA, LA).

Group SLI SD CA match SD LA match SD F(2, 60)Effect

size (h2)Standard

tone Range of tones

Phoneme deletion (max = 15)a,c 4.90 2.09 10.67 2.37 8.95 2.87 30.19*** .50 — —Rime oddity (max = 20)a,b,c 10.43 3.09 15.95 2.01 13.71 2.95 21.79*** .42 — —Intensity threshold (dB) 76.14 1.80 75.48 1.41 76.02 2.12 0.78 .02 73 dB SPL 73.0–81.1 dB SPLOne-ramp threshold (ms)a,b 264.19 154.27 191.78 139.92 254.28 130.93 11.39*** .27 300 ms 15–300 msTwo-ramp threshold (ms)a 267.51 147.1 239.89 153.94 242.30 142.42 3.93* .12 300 ms 15–300 msLn Duration 1 threshold (ms)a,b 2.50 0.49 1.90 0.38 2.38 0.52 9.16*** .25 400 ms 400–640 msLn Duration 2 threshold (ms)a,c 5.58 0.51 4.94 0.19 5.19 0.34 15.47*** .34 1,200 ms 400–2,000 msTOJ slope j.136 .104 j.237 .159 j.189 .138 2.85 .08 — —TOJ boundaryd (ms) 19.48 4.99 17.89 5.32 20.08 3.81 1.20 .04 — —TOJ thresholde (ms) 127.00 95.2 82.4 58.2 130.2 118.8 1.70 .04 — 0–405 ms

Note. Ln = log; TOJ = temporal order judgment. Em dashes indicate that data are not applicable.aCA > SLI, p < .001. bCA > LA, p < .001. cLA > SLI, p < .001. d True boundary = 0 ms. eDog/car tone = 115 ms.

*p < .05. ***p < .001.

654 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 50 • 647–666 • June 2007

Page 9: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

Task 1 (briefer sounds) tasks and that the group withSLI was impaired relative to both the CA and the LAcontrol participants in theDurationDiscriminationTask 2(longer sounds) task and the phoneme deletion and rimeoddity tasks. Note that administration of the phonemedeletion task used a live voice procedure, and so the pos-sibility of inadvertent cueing by the experimenter cannotbe ruled out. Nevertheless, results by group were iden-tical in the two different phonological awareness tasks.

To determine whether the group-level auditory pro-cessing deficits found for rise time and duration were, infact, due to a subset of the children with SLI, the perfor-mance of the CA control group was used to calculate per-formance levels falling below the 5th percentile. This wascomputed following the criteria suggested byRamus et al.(2003) and used by Richardson et al. (2004). Childrenfalling below the 5th percentile typical for their age canbe considered to have a processing deficit. The percent-age of SLI children with performance levels below the5th percentile was then examined for all the auditorytasks. To examine the full extent of individual differencesin duration discrimination, the untransformed version ofthese tasks was examined (a scatter plot is also providedfor the log data of Duration Discrimination Task 2 forcomparison). For the rise time tasks, 15 children in theSLI group fell below the 5th percentile of the CA controlparticipants in the one-ramp rise time task (71.4%), butno child fell below this criterion in the two-ramp task.However, themajority of the childrenwith SLI (52%) fellbelow the 25th percentile of control performance in thetwo-ramp task, and 76% fell below the 50th percentile.For the durationmeasures, 10 of 21 (47.6%) childrenwithSLI fell below the 5th percentile of the CA control partici-pants using the untransformed Duration DiscriminationTask 1 (briefer sounds) measure, 17 of 20 (85%) fell belowcriterion using the untransformed Duration Discrimina-tionTask 2 (longer sounds)measure, and9 of 20 (45%) fellbelow criterion in the log Duration Discrimination Task 2(longer sounds) measure. For the rapid auditory process-ingmeasure, 9 of 21 (38.1%) childrenwithSLIwere belowcriterion (using the TOJ threshold measure). Figure 3shows the performance of individual children with SLIcompared with CA control participants for the one-ramprise time, untransformedDurationDiscriminationTask1,untransformedDurationDiscrimination Task 2, log Dura-tion Discrimination Task 2, and TOJ threshold measures.

Of the 17 children with SLI who fell below the 5thpercentile in the untransformed Duration Discrimina-tion Task 2 (longer sounds), 15 also fell below the 5thpercentile in the amplitude envelope onset (one-ramprise time) task, 10 also fell below the 5th percentile injudging sounds with briefer durations (untransformedDurationDiscriminationTask 1), and 8 also fell below the5th percentile in their TOJ thresholds. Thus, the childrenexhibiting difficulty with the duration discrimination

tasks were the same children who had difficulty withtheamplitude envelope rise time task, consistentwith thedevelopmental dyslexia literature. Furthermore, 8 of the9 children who could be said to have a rapid processingdeficit for environmental sounds also had difficulties inprocessing auditory cues of longer durations. To date,this overlap has not been found in children with devel-opmental dyslexia (see Richardson et al., 2004).

Exploration of simple correlations controlling for ageshowed that nonverbal IQ was not related to any of themeasures of interest (see Table 5). To explore the rela-tionships between the psychoacoustic tasks and the vo-cabulary, phonological, working memory, and readingmeasures, a series of fixed-order multiple regressionswere computed. For each regression, the Cook’s distancemetric was calculated. No data points had a Cook’s dis-tance score of above 1.0, and thus no participants wereexcluded from the regressions (Tabachnik&Fidell, 2001).Because the groups did not differ in the intensity mea-sure, and because this measure used the same psycho-acoustic procedure as most of our other auditory tasks,the intensity measure was used as a control measure forthe attentional demands of the psychoacoustic proce-dures. However, it should be noted that this measure canprovide only an estimate of children’s attention to task.

Four composite variables were created for the re-gressions: (a) vocabulary (including WISC Vocabularyand BPVS), (b) phonological awareness (including pho-neme deletion and rime oddity), (c) reading and spelling(including TOWRE Sight Word Efficiency, TOWREPho-nemicDecoding Efficiency,WORDReadingComprehen-sion, and British Ability Scales Spelling), and (d) workingmemory (Nonword Repetition andWorkingMemory Testfor Children, Word subtest). These four composite var-iables were used as the dependent variables along withrapid color naming. The independent variables in eachregression were (in a fixed order): (a) age, (b) WISC Per-formance IQ,3 (c) intensity discrimination (attentionalcontrol), and (d) an additional psychoacoustic measure(two-ramp rise time, one-ramp rise time, log DurationDiscrimination Task 1 (shorter sounds), log Duration Dis-crimination Task 2 (longer sounds), TOJ threshold. Theresulting parameter estimates are displayed in Tables 6through 10 along with the unique variance accounted forby each variable (showingDR2). Only unique variance thatwas significant after Bonferroni corrections were appliedis indicated; however, before applying Bonferroni correc-tions, changes in R2 of 6% or greater were significant.

Inspection of Tables 6–10 reveals that the durationmeasures and the single-ramp amplitude rise timemea-surewere consistent predictors of unique variance in the

3Because a language deficit is the defining criterion of having a speech orlanguage impairment, we did not control for verbal IQ in these regressions.

Corriveau et al.: Basic Auditory Processing in SLI 655

Page 10: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

Figure 3. Scatter plots showing deviance as compared to CA controls for the (a) one-ramp rise time, (b) Duration Discrimination Task 2 (longersounds), (c) Duration Discrimination Task 1 (shorter sounds), (d) ln duration 2 (longer sounds), (e) TOJ threshold, and (f) two-rampmeasures. ln = log.

656 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 50 • 647–666 • June 2007

Page 11: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

Table5.

Partial

correlations

(rs)of

intellige

nce,

lang

uage

,rea

ding

,and

psycho

acou

sticmea

sures,controlling

forag

e(m

onths).

12

34

56

78

910

1112

1314

15

1.Non

verbal

IQa

—.134

.183

.041

.105

.076

.073

.051

.098

.124

.028

j.030

.001

.047

j.108

2.Ve

rbal

IQb

—.719

***

.593

***

.553

***

.351

**.388

**.453

***

.631

***

j.050

j.336

*j.377

**j.370

**j.110

j.265

*3.

Receptivevocabu

lary

c—

.605

***

.637

***

.393

**.364

**.485

***

.639

***

j.040

j.461

***

j.402

**j.427

**j.211

j.165

4.Ph

onem

ede

letio

n—

.701

***

.631

***

.638

***

.691

***

.622

***

j.148

j.440

**j.560

***

j.527

***

j.308

*j.399

**5.

Rimeod

dity

—.477

***

.400

**.527

***

.637

***

j.074

j.455

***

j.498

***

j.517

***

j.264

*j.322

*6.

Wordread

ingd

—.844

***

.827

***

.530

***

j.182

j.404

**j.373

**j.401

**j.198

j.272

*7.

Non

wordread

inge

—.825

***

.481

***

j.188

j.312

*j.363

**j.396

**j.239

Èj.275

*8.

Spellingf

—.494

***

j.235

Èj.311

*j.426

**j.503

***

j.319

*j.351

**9.

Read

ingcomprehensio

ng—

j.013

j.441

**j.372

**j.440

**j.175

j.148

10.Intensitydiscrim

ination

—.160

.279

*.349

**.423

**.101

11.D

uration1discrim

ination

—.338

**.439

**.362

**.164

12.D

uration2discrim

ination

—.551

***

.401

**.282

*13

.One-ram

pdiscrim

ination

—.586

***

.233

È14

.Two-rampdiscrim

ination

—.211

15.T

OJthreshold

a Non

verbal

IQwas

estim

ated

from

theBlockDesignan

dPictureArran

gementsub

tests

(M=10

0,SD

=15

).bVe

rbal

IQwas

estim

ated

from

theSimila

ritiesan

dVocab

ularysubtests(M

=10

0,SD

=15).

c British

PictureVo

cabu

lary

Test(M

=100

,SD=15

).dTestof

WordRe

adingEfficiency,

Sigh

tWordsubtest(M

=10

0,SD

=15

).eTestof

WordRe

adingEfficiency,

Non

-Wordsubtest(M

=10

0,SD

=15

).f British

AbilityScales,S

pelling

subtest(M

=10

0,SD

=15

).gW

ORD

Read

ingCom

prehensio

nsubtest(M

=10

0,SD

=15

).

*p<.05.

**p<.01.

***p

<.001

.

Corriveau et al.: Basic Auditory Processing in SLI 657

Page 12: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

language, reading, and phonology measures. For the vo-cabulary composite (seeTable6), asmuchas19%ofuniquevariation was explained by these auditory processingmeasures. For the phonological awareness composite (seeTable 7), up to 31% of unique variance was accounted forby individual differences in rise time and duration pro-cessing. For theworkingmemory composite (see Table 8),rise time and Duration Discrimination Task 2 both con-tributed 17% additional variance. All three measureswere able to account for significant additional variance inthe reading and spelling composite variable (see Table 9).TheDurationDiscriminationTask1 (briefer sounds) taskaccounted for the most additional variance, explaining21% of the variance in the reading composite variable.

The results of the regression equations show thatindividual differences in auditory processing of rise timeand duration explained between 10.7% and 31.2% ofunique variance in vocabulary attainment, phonologicalawareness, and reading. It is important to recall that IQ

and an estimate of children’s attention to task were con-trolled in these analyses. In contrast to the results ob-tained for the amplitude envelope onset and durationmeasures, the measures of rapid auditory processingand intensity discrimination that we included showedno significant relationships. This mirrors the develop-mental dyslexia literature. Intensity discrimination hasnot been found to be impaired in groups of children withdyslexia. Even when group differences have been foundin the TOJ task used here, individual differences in TOJrarely predict unique variance in phonological aware-ness and literacy (Goswami et al., 2002; Muneaux et al.,2004; Ramus et al., 2003; Richardson et al., 2004).

As an additional measure of whether the poor per-formance of childrenwith SLI in the psychoacoustic taskswas due to a subgroup of children with especially severelanguage,workingmemory, or literacydifficulties, a seriesof good–poor splits were applied to the sample. ChildrenwithSLIweredivided into “good”and “poor”groups for each

Table 6. Stepwise regressions of the unique variance in vocabulary ability accounted for by thepsychoacoustic variables.

Step

Vocabulary ability

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5

1. Ageb .532a .370a .431a .513a .493a

DR2 .243 .243 .243 .251 .243

2. Performance IQb .122 .114 .132 .151 .106DR2 .014 .014 .014 .019 .014

3. Intensityb .082 .134 .053 .061 .005DR2 .000 .000 .000 .001 .000

4. Two-ramp rise timeb j.212DR2 .024

4. One-ramp rise timeb j.465a

DR2 .169

4. Duration 1b j.456a

DR2 .195

4. Duration 2b j.341**DR2 .107

4. TOJ thresholdb j.157DR2 .024

Note. n = 63. The composite vocabulary score includes the WISC Vocabulary subscale and the BPVS.aChange in R2 significant using a Bonferroni-corrected alpha (a/25, p < .002).

658 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 50 • 647–666 • June 2007

Page 13: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

of the vocabulary, working memory, reading, and spellingtasks, making a total of 8 good–poor splits. “Poor” perfor-mance was judged to be below 1 SD of the standardizedmean (below85) or the standardmean (below7), dependingon the test. Each of the pairs of groups yielded by the vo-cabulary, workingmemory, spelling, and reading good–poorsplits was entered into a series of one-way between-groupANOVAs, with each of the psychoacoustic measures as thedependent variable. This resulted in 56 one-way ANOVAs.No significant main effect of group was found for any of thepsychoacousticmeasures.Hence, in no case did the “poor”group appear to be carrying the auditory processing def-icits found in thewhole-sample analyses, evenwhenwork-ing memory was the subgrouping variable.

DiscussionTheongoingdebate concerningwhether childrenwith

SLI can be characterized by a rapid auditory processing

deficit (e.g., Rosen, 2003; Tallal, 2004) has obscured thepossibility that these children could have a range of audi-tory processingdifficulties. Ours is the first study of whichwe are aware that examines the amplitude rise time andduration processing abilities of children with SLI. Thedeficits that we have documented are remarkably consis-tent across children with SLI. The majority (70%–80%)of children in our sample performed below the 5th per-centile of performance achieved by control participantsfor detecting both amplitude envelope rise time and theduration of simple tones. A minority also had a rapid au-ditory processing deficit. Individual differences in therise time and duration processing measures accounted fora significant amount of variability in standardized mea-sures of language and reading and in experimental mea-sures of phonological awareness; individual differencesin rapid auditory processing did not. The data indicatethat the auditory processing difficulties that are moststrongly predictive of languageandphonology in children

Table 7. Stepwise regressions of the unique variance in phonological awareness ability accounted for bythe psychoacoustic variables.

Step

Phonological awareness ability

Model 6 Model 7 Model 8 Model 9 Model 10

1. Ageb j.050 j.272 j.155 j.082 j.126DR2 .006 .006 .004 .006 .006

2. Performance IQb .083 .084 j.117 .054 .044DR2 .005 .005 .006 .006 .005

3. Intensityb .016 .052 j.071 .041 j.093DR2 .017 .017 .016 .018 .017

4. Two-ramp rise timeb j.335DR2 .091

4. One-ramp rise timeb j.615a

DR2 .296

4. Duration 1b j.482a

DR2 .218

4. Duration 2b j.586a

DR2 .312

4. TOJ thresholdb j.387DR2 .145

Note. n = 63. The composite phonological awareness score includes Phoneme Deletion and Rime Oddity.aChange in R2 significant using a Bonferroni-corrected alpha (a/25, p < .002).

Corriveau et al.: Basic Auditory Processing in SLI 659

Page 14: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

with SLI are found in tasks requiring the integrationof temporal information over relatively long temporalwindows.

As noted earlier, there was no overlap in terms ofsampling between the children included in this studyand the children tested in our studies of developmen-tal dyslexia (Goswami et al., 2002;Muneaux et al., 2004;Richardson et al., 2004; see also Rocheron, Lorenzi,Fullgrabe, &Dumont, 2002). The children in the presentstudy were referred by speech-language therapists andhad a statement of SLI from their local education au-thorities. No child had a diagnosis of dyslexia. The chil-drenwith developmental dyslexia in our previous studieshad no diagnosis of speech and language impairmentsand no history of developmental language problems, butthey did have a specific impairment in reading. Never-theless, it is of interest to compare the pattern of resultsfound here with those found in prior studies of children

diagnosed with developmental dyslexia. The most com-parable study is that reported byRichardson et al. (2004).In that study, the two-ramp rise time task used hereaccounted for 13% of unique variance in a rime odditytask after controlling for age, verbal and nonverbal IQ,and vocabulary (p < .01), and for 8% and 11% of uniquevariance in reading and spelling, respectively (ps < .05).In the current study, the two-ramp rise time task ac-counted for 9% of unique variance in the phonologicalcomposite, 7% of unique variance in the reading/spellingcomposite, and 7% of variance in the rapid naming task(all nonsignificant). The stimuli used here for the one-ramp rise time task were used in an AXB task formatwith children with developmental dyslexia (Richardsonet al.). Individual differences in this AXB task accountedfor 9% of unique variance in rime oddity after control-ling for age, IQ, and vocabulary (p < .05) and accountedfor 8% of unique variance in reading and 13% of unique

Table 8. Stepwise regressions of the unique variance in reading and spelling awareness ability accountedfor by the psychoacoustic variables.

Step

Reading and spelling ability

Model 11 Model 12 Model 13 Model 14 Model 15

1. Ageb .207 .017 .117 .197 .141DR2 .039 .039 .044 .047 .039

2. Performance IQb .051 .052 .073 .078 .017DR2 .001 .001 .001 .006 .001

3. Intensityb j.039 j.013 j.106 j.088 j.136DR2 .028 .028 .024 .045 .028

4. Two-ramp rise timeb j.297DR2 .071

4. One-ramp rise timeb .523a

DR2 j.214

4. Duration 1b j.420a

DR2 .166

4. Duration 2b j.421a

DR2 .161

4. TOJ thresholdb j.332DR2 .100

Note. n = 63. The composite reading score includes TOWRE Sight Word Efficiency, TOWRE PhonemeDecoding Efficiency, WORD Reading Comprehension subtest, and British Ability Scales Spelling subtest.aChange in R2 significant using a Bonferroni-corrected alpha (a/25, p < .002).

660 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 50 • 647–666 • June 2007

Page 15: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

variance in nonword reading (ps < .01). In the currentstudy, the one-ramp rise time task accounted for 30% ofunique variance in the phonological awareness compos-ite and 21% of unique variance in the reading composite(ps < .002 using Bonferroni corrections). The durationtask used by Richardson et al. used speechlike VCV syl-lables (“ata,” “atta”), and the childrenwithdevelopmentaldyslexia had to decide which syllable was longer. Individ-ual differences in duration detection accounted for 10%of unique variance in real word reading after controllingfor age, IQ, and vocabulary (p < .01), for 8% of uniquevariance in spelling (p < .05), and for 12% of unique var-iance in nonword reading (p < .01). In the current study,the tone duration tasks accounted for 22% and 31% ofunique variance in the phonological awareness compos-ite (shorter vs. longer sounds, respectively),17% and 16%of unique variance in the reading/spelling composite, and13% and 18% of unique variance in rapid naming (all

ps < .002). The TOJ threshold measure did not accountfor any significant unique variance in phonological aware-ness or literacy in the current sample. This replicates thefindings for children with developmental dyslexia usingthe same dog/car TOJ task reported by Richardson et al.(2004). Overall, therefore, the relationships among au-ditory processing, phonological awareness, and literacy arevery similar in both samples of children with a develop-mental language disorder. However, the relationships ap-pear stronger in terms of absolute variance accounted forin the sample of children with SLI.

One explanation for the apparent severity of thedifficulties experienced by the current sample of chil-dren with SLI is that task complexity and attentionalfactors might be especially problematic for such children.To attempt to control for attentional factors,we tested oursample on auditory variables that we either did or did notexpect to show a deficit, using the same psychoacoustic

Table 9. Stepwise regressions of the unique variance in working memory ability accounted for by thepsychoacoustic variables.

Step

Working memory ability

Model 16 Model 17 Model 18 Model 19 Model 20

1. Ageb j.065 j.224 j.136 j.075 j.117DR2 .003 .003 .002 .002 .003

2. Performance IQb .076 .076 .092 .069 .040DR2 .003 .003 .003 .005 .003

3. Intensityb j.152 j.081 j.195 j.107 j.186DR2 .048 .048 .053 .054 .048

4. Two-ramp rise timeb j.160DR2 .021

4. One-ramp rise timeb j.469a

DR2 .172

4. Duration 1b j.337DR2 .107

4. Duration 2b j.429a

DR2 .167

4. TOJ thresholdb j.344DR2 .115

Note. n = 63. The composite working memory score includes Nonword Repetition and Working MemoryScales for Children Word subtest.aChange in R2 significant using a Bonferroni-corrected alpha (a/25, p < .002).

Corriveau et al.: Basic Auditory Processing in SLI 661

Page 16: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

paradigm. As expected on the basis of prior work withchildren with dyslexia (Richardson et al., 2004), no im-pairments were found in the two-interval forced-choicepsychoacoustic procedure for intensity discrimination, butsignificant impairments were found using the same pro-cedure for rise time and duration. It could, nevertheless,be argued that the PEST procedure used was vulnerableto lapses in attention, as indeed all such procedures are.However, this should have affected all tasks equally,rather than selected tasks only. As a further control, whenexploring relationships between the auditory variablesand the outcomemeasures usingmultiple regression tech-niques, we used performance in the intensity task as anestimate of the children’s attention to task before inves-tigating specific relations between auditory processing,language, reading, and phonology. The relations reportedwere those remaining after controlling for age, nonverbalIQ, and this estimate of attention to task. The same logicapplies to alternative objections that our findings may

reflect working memory deficits in the sample of childrenwith SLI. If problems with working memory in the chil-dren with SLI explain our auditory findings, then thechildren should have been impaired in all of the psycho-acoustic tasks. They were not. Also, it could be arguedthat the Duration Discrimination 2 task should havebeen significantly more difficult than the Duration Dis-crimination 1 task, because the stimuli were longer (cre-ating a larger memory load). It was not. Finally, it couldbe that we are demonstrating differences in learningrather than underlying differences in basic auditory pro-cessing abilities. Because each auditory task was givenonly once, it is impossible to separate learning of the taskfrom underlying auditory capabilities. Nevertheless, be-cause performance remained poor in tasks received later(e.g., Duration Discrimination Task 2), learning effectsacross auditory tasks appears unlikely.

We submit that these auditory data are interestingfor a number of reasons. First, they provide converging

Table 10. Stepwise regressions of the unique variance in rapid color naming ability accounted for by thepsychoacoustic variables.

Step

Rapid color naming ability

Model 21 Model 22 Model 23 Model 24 Model 25

1. Ageb j.040 .094 .041 j.103 .021DR2 .000 .000 .001 .000 .000

2. Performance IQb j.166 j.168 j.200 j.153 j.140DR2 .022 .022 .025 .124 .022

3. Intensityb .071 .105 j.156 .080 .177DR2 .041 .041 .039 .045 .041

4. Two-ramp rise timeb .302DR2 .073

4. 1-ramp rise timeb .333DR2 .087

4. Duration 1b .382a

DR2 .138

4. Duration 2b .447a

DR2 .181

4. TOJ thresholdb .269DR2 .070

Note. n = 63.aChange in R2 significant using a Bonferroni-corrected alpha (a/25, p < .002).

662 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 50 • 647–666 • June 2007

Page 17: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

evidence for the view that quite low-level processesmightbe important in explaining the etiology of developmen-tal language disorders. Certainly, lower level processesare an important determinant of language acquisition.Recent studies show that abilities as diverse as track-ing the conditional probabilities in auditory sequences at8 months of age, discriminating between two synthesizedvowels at 6 months of age, and distinguishing syllablestress at 5 months of age, are related to language devel-opment (Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996; Tsao, Liu, &Kuhl, 2004;Weber,Hahne, Friedrich,&Friederici, 2004).Electrophysiological studies also suggest that childrenwith SLI differ from CA peers in peripheral and centralneurological processing of nonspeech acoustic signals (e.g.,Marler & Champlin, 2005; Marler, Champlin, & Gillam,2002;McArthur&Bishop, 2005). Thedata presentedheresupport the possibility that SLI could be caused by lowerlevel processing difficulties in the auditory domain.Withrespect to our focus on the auditory cues of amplituderise time and duration, it seems plausible that early dif-ficulties, present from infancy, in accurately processingthese cues to prosody could impair the acquisition of lan-guage via impaired word segmentation and the develop-ment of degraded phonological representations (Curtinet al., 2005; Juscyzk et al., 1999). An early insensitivity toauditory cues to rhythm and stress could have profoundand lasting consequences on word segmentation and thedevelopment of the language system.

At the biological level, the recent work of Heil andcolleagues is relevant. Biermann and Heil (2000) re-ported neurons in the auditory cortex of the cat thatare specialized to detect the slopes of amplitude envel-ope onsets. The neurons respond selectively at differ-ent points of the amplitude onset slopes. Comparableresponse patterns were reported for humans in a parallelstudy using magnetoencephalography. This postulatedneuronal mechanism should be activated by the rise-time-varying stimuli being used here. Neubauer andHeil (2004) considered the ability of the human auditorysystem to integrate sound over time and pointed out thatclassic accounts assume that the quantity ultimately in-tegrated is sound intensity (e.g., Plomp&Bouman, 1959).Heil and Neubauer (2001, 2003) argued that their ex-periments suggested a different model of temporal inte-gration, based on the integration of the sound’s pressureenvelope rather than its intensity. When stimuli of dif-ferent durations and of different envelopes were used tocharacterize thresholds independent of envelope shape,temporal summation was found to depend on the pres-sure envelope and not on intensity. In terms of patientswith hearing loss, Heil andNeubauer suggested that theeffective portion of the stimulus changes as a conse-quence of hearing loss. There is an elevation in the base-line above which sound pressure is effective in drivingthe auditory system. A similar mechanism could be at

work in the children being studied here. Auditory thresh-olds could be elevated in these children because of in-efficient processing at the neuronal level, which couldaffect the effective portion of the stimulus.

The data also speak to the importance of cross-language studies when trying to understand etiology. Forexample, testing the phonological deficit hypothesis ofdevelopmental dyslexia across languages has been ex-tremely fruitful in terms of refining our understandingof the developmental links between phonological repre-sentation and literacy (Ziegler&Goswami, 2005). Studiesof basic auditory processing in dyslexia across languagesare also finding consistent lower level deficits, notably insensitivity to rise time and durational cues (Hamalainen,Leppanen, Torppa, Muller, & Lyytinen, 2005; Muneauxet al., 2004; Richardson, Leppänen, Leiwo, & Lyytinen,2003). One possibility is that the different grammaticaldeficits found in samples of childrenwith SLI across lan-guages could reflect the ways in which stress is used tomark grammatical constructions in the languages that theyare learning. To understand possible developmental linksamong syntax, rhythm, and stress, a good starting pointwould be a systematic analysis in different languages ofthe types of grammatical construction that should be im-paired if the processing of stress cues were compromised.ChildrenwithSLI in languages other thanEnglish couldthen be studied to seewhether they also lack sensitivity torise time and durational cues and whether this insensi-tivity is linked inpredictableways to their ability to acquireparticular grammatical distinctions in their language.

Finally, the data are more consistent with the viewthat there is a continuum of developmental languagedisorder (Catts, 1996) than with the view that SLI anddevelopmental dyslexia are distinct syndromes (Bishop& Snowling, 2004). Although some studies have put thecomorbidity for dyslexia and SLI as high as 50% (e.g.,McArthur, Hogben, Edwards, Heath, & Mengler, 2000),other estimates of comorbidity are as lowas 10% (Bishop& Snowling, 2004). Recently, Catts et al. (2005) reportedonly limited (but significant) overlap between dyslexiaand SLI in a population-based sample of 527 school-agechildren. They concluded that the two disorders weredistinct but were comorbid in some children. They alsonoted that a problem in phonological processing did notappear to be a major factor in SLI when it occurred inisolation from dyslexia. Nevertheless, the auditory pro-cessing deficits found in the children with SLI studiedhere were the same deficits found in previous studiesof children with developmental dyslexia, only more se-vere (see Goswami et al., 2002; Muneaux et al., 2004;Richardson et al., 2004). Although it does not necessar-ily follow that similar performance is evidence of a sharedroot cause, it makes biological sense that language devel-opment and audition should be linked. An intriguing pos-sibility is that themajority of childrenwith developmental

Corriveau et al.: Basic Auditory Processing in SLI 663

Page 18: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

language disorders have auditory processing deficits thatare brain based. However, the developmental trajectoryof a core auditory deficit may vary depending on the se-verity of the deficit; the types of auditory processingmostaffected; reciprocal developmental interactions with pho-nology, semantics, and syntax; the language being learnedby the child; and social cognition and environmental fac-tors that lie outside the core language domain. Pinpoint-ing etiology in developmental disorders is a very difficulttask. Nevertheless, studies across languages that mea-sure the lowest level deficits that can be discovered andthen track developmental trajectories offer one of the bestways of uncovering the causal basis of speech and lan-guage disorders (Goswami, 2003; Tsao et al., 2004).

AcknowledgmentsThe first author was supported by a Cambridge Gates

Scholarship, and the second author was supported by aNational Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship.We thank the following individuals and groups for taking partin this study: the head teacher(s) and the children of Arburyand Stapleford Primary Schools, Cambridge, England; RoundDiamond JMI, Stevenage, England; Southfields Junior School,Peterborough,England; andSt.Helen’s, St. Francis, St. Angela’s,Star, and Sandringham Primary Schools, London, England.

ReferencesAlexander, D., & Frost, B. (1982). Decelerated synthesizedspeech as a means of shaping speed of auditory processing ofchildren with delayed language. Perceptual & Motor Skills,55, 783–792.

Bedore, L. M., & Leonard, L. B. (2000). The effects of in-flectional variation on fast mapping of verbs in English andSpanish. Journal of Speech, Language, andHearingResearch,43, 21–23.

Benasich, A. A., & Tallal, P. (2002). Infant discriminationof rapid auditory cues predicts later language impairment.Behavioral Brain Research, 136, 31–49.

Biermann, S., & Heil, P. (2000). Parallels between timingof onset responses of single neurons in cat and of evokedmagnetic fields in human auditory cortex. Journal ofNeurophysiology, 84, 2426–2439.

Bishop, D. V. M., Adams, C. V., Nation, K., & Rosen, S.(2005). Reception of transient nonspeech stimuli is normalin specific language impairment: Evidence from glidediscrimination. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 175–194.

Bishop, D. V. M., Carlyon, R. P., Deeks, J. M., & Bishop,S. J. (1999). Auditory temporal processing impairment:Neither necessary nor sufficient for causing language im-pairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, andHearing Research, 42, 1295–1310.

Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmentaldyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different?Psychological Bulletin, 130, 858–886.

Bortolini, U., & Leonard, L. B. (2000). Phonology and chil-dren with specific language impairment: Status of structuralconstraints in two languages. Journal of CommunicationsDisorders, 33, 131–150.

Burkhard, M. D., & Sachs, R. M. (1975). Anthropometricmanikin for acoustic research. Journal of the AcousticalSociety of America, 58, 214–222.

Catts, H. W. (1996). Defining dyslexia as a developmentallanguage disorder: An expanded view. Topics in LanguageDisorders, 16, 14–29.

Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., Hogan, T. P., & Weismer, S. E.(2005). Are specific language impairment and dyslexiadistinct disorders? Journal of Speech, Language, andHearing Research, 48, 1378–1396.

Choi, J.-Y., Hasegawa-Johnson, M., & Cole, J. (2005).Finding intonational boundaries using acoustic cues relatedto the voice source. Journal of the Acoustical Society ofAmerica, 118, 2579–2587.

Coady, J. A., Kluender, K. R., & Evans, J. L. (2005). Cate-gorical perception of speech by children with specific lan-guage impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, andHearing Research, 48, 944–959.

Curtin, S., Mintz, T. H., & Christiansen, M. H. (2005).Stress changes the representational landscape: Evidencefrom word segmentation. Cognition, 96, 233–262.

Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Pintilie, D.(1982). British Picture Vocabulary Scale. Windsor, England:NferNelson.

Efron, R. (1963). Temporal perception, aphasia and déjà vu.Brain, 86, 403–424.

Elliott, C. D., Smith, P., & McCulloch, K. (1996). BritishAbility Scales Second Edition—Spelling subtest. Windsor,England: NferNelson.

Findlay, J. M. (1978). Estimates on probability functions:A more virulent PEST. Perception & Psychophysics, 23,181–185.

Finney, D. J. (1971). Probit analysis (3rd ed.). Cambridge,England: Cambridge University Press.

Frumkin, B., & Rapin, I. (1980). Perception of vowels andconsonant–vowels of varying duration in language impairedchildren. Neuropsychologia, 18, 443–454.

Fry, D. B. (1954). Duration and intensity as physical cor-relates of linguistic stress. The Journal of the AcousticalSociety of America, 26, 138.

Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1996). The Chil-dren’s Test of Nonword Repetition. London: PsychologicalCorporation.

Gopnik, M., & Crago, M. B. (1991). Familial aggregation ofa developmental language disorder. Cognition, 39, 1–50.

Goswami, U. (2003). Why theories about developmentaldyslexia require developmental designs. Trends in CognitiveSciences, 7, 534–554.

Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp,R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S., & Scott, S. K. (2002). Ampli-tude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A newhypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,USA, 99, 10911–10916.

664 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 50 • 647–666 • June 2007

Page 19: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

Greenberg, S. (1999). Speaking in shorthand—A syllable-centric perspective for understanding pronunciation variation.Speech Communication, 29, 159–176.

Greenberg, S. (2006). A multi-tier framework for understand-ing spoken language. In S. Greenberg & W. Ainsworth (Eds.),Listening to speech: An auditory perspective (pp. 411–433).Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Greenberg, S., Carvey,H.,Hitchcock,L.,&Chang, S. (2003).Temporal properties of spontaneous speech—A syllable-centric perspective. Journal of Phonetics, 31, 465–485.

Hamalainen, J., Leppanen, P. H. T., Torppa, M., Muller,K., & Lyytinen, H. (2005). Detection of sound rise timeby adults with dyslexia. Brain & Language, 94, 32–42.

Heil, P., & Neubauer, H. (2001). Temporal integration ofsound pressure determines thresholds of auditory nervefibres. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 7404–7415.

Heil, P., & Neubauer, H. (2003). A unifying basis of auditorythresholds based on temporal summation. Proceedings of theNational Academy of Sciences, USA, 100, 6151–6156.

Helzer, J. R., Champlin, C. A., & Gillam, R. B. (1996).Auditory temporal resolution in specifically language-impaired and age-matched children. Perceptual & MotorSkills, 3, 1171–1181.

Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1989). Timing functions and thecerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 136–152.

Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D. M., & Newsome, M. (1999).The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learninginfants. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 159–207.

Kochanski, G., Grabe, E., Coleman, J., & Rosner, B.(2005). Loudness predicts prominence: Fundamental fre-quency adds little. The Journal of the Acoustical Societyof America, 118, 1038–1054.

Leonard, L. B. (1995). Functional categories in grammarsof children with specific language impairment. Journal ofSpeech and Hearing Research, 38, 1270–1283.

Leonard, L. B., & Bortolini, U. (1998). Grammatical mor-phology and the role of weak syllables in the speech ofItalian-speaking children with specific language impairment.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41,1363–1374.

Leonard, L. B., Eyer, J. A., Bedore, L. M., & Grela, B. G.(1997). Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme dif-ficulties of English-speaking children with specific languageimpairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and HearingResearch, 40, 741–753.

Levitt, H. (1971). Transformed up-down methods in psycho-acoustics. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,49, 467–477.

Marler, J. A., & Champlin, C. A. (2005). Sensory processingof backward-masking signals in children with language-learning impairment as assessed with the auditory brain-stem response. Journal of Speech, Language, and HearingResearch, 48, 189–203.

Marler, J. A., Champlin, C. A., & Gillam, R. B. (2002).Auditory memory for backward masking signals in childrenwith language impairment. Psychophysiology, 39, 767–780.

McArthur, G. M., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2001). Auditoryperceptual processing in people with reading and oral lan-guage impairments: Current issues and recommendations.Dyslexia, 7, 150–170.

McArthur, G. M., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2005). Speech andnon-speech processing in people with specific languageimpairment: A behavioural and electrophysiological study.Brain and Language, 94, 260–273.

McArthur, G. M., Hogben, J. J., Edwards, V. T., Heath,S. M., & Mengler, E. D. (2000). On the “specifics” of specificreading disability and specific language impairment. Jour-nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Dis-ciplines, 41, 869–874.

McDougall, S., Hulme, C., Ellis, A., & Monk, A. (1994).Learning to read: The role of short-term memory and pho-nological skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,58, 112–133.

McGregor, K. K., & Leonard, L. B. (1994). Subject pronounand article omissions in the speech of children with specificlanguage impairment: A phonological interpretation.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 171–181.

Muneaux, M., Ziegler, J. C., Truc, C., Thomson, J., &Goswami, U. (2004). Deficits in beat perception and dys-lexia: Evidence from French. NeuroReport, 15, 1255–1259.

Neubauer, H., & Heil, P. (2004). Towards a unifying basis ofauditory thresholds: The effects of hearing loss on temporalintegration reconsidered. Journal for the Association ofResearch in Otolaryngology, 5, 436–458.

Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (1995). Timeestimation deficits in developmental dyslexia: Evidence ofcerebellar involvement. Proceedings of the Royal Societyof London B, 259, 43–47.

Norrelgen, F., Lacerda, F., & Forssberg, H. (2002). Tem-poral resolution of auditory perception and verbal workingmemory in 15 children with language impairment. Journalof Learning Disabilities, 35, 539–543.

Pickering, S., & Gathercole, S. (2001).Working Memory TestBattery for Children. London: The Psychological Corporation.

Plomp, R., & Bouman, M. A. (1959). Relation betweenhearing and duration for tone pulses. The Journal of theAcoustical Society of America, 31, 749–758.

Ramus, F., Pidgeon, E., & Frith, U. (2003). The relationshipbetween motor control and phonology in dyslexic children.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 712–722.

Rice, M., & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinicalmarker of specific language impairment in English-speakingchildren. Journal of Speech, Language, and HearingResearch, 39, 1239–1257.

Richardson, U., Leppänen, P. H. T., Leiwo, M., &Lyytinen, H. (2003). Speech perception of infants with highfamilial risk for dyslexia differ at the age of six months.Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 385–397.

Richardson, U., Thomson, J., Scott, S. K., & Goswami, U.(2004). Supra-segmental auditory processing skills andphonological representation in dyslexic children. Dyslexia,10, 215–233.

Roberts, B., & Leonard, L. B. (1997). Grammatical deficitsin German and English: A cross-linguistic study of childrenwith SLI. First Language, 17, 131–150.

Rocheron, I., Lorenzi, C., Fullgrabe, C., & Dumont, A.(2002). Temporal envelope perception in dyslexic children.NeuroReport, 13, 1683–1687.

Rosen, S. (2003). Auditory processing in dyslexia andspecific language impairment: Is there a deficit? What is

Corriveau et al.: Basic Auditory Processing in SLI 665

Page 20: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

its nature? Does it explain anything? Journal of Phonetics,3–4, 509–527.

Rust, J., Golombok, S., & Trickey, G. (1992). WechslerObjective Reading Dimensions—Comprehension subtest.London: The Psychological Corporation.

Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996,December 13). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants.Science, 274, 1926–1928.

Sattler, J. M. (1982). Assessment of Children’s Intelligenceand Special Abilities. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Semel, D., Wiig, W. H., & Secord, W. (1995). Clinical Eval-uation of Language Fundamentals—Third Edition.San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Spitz, R. V., Tallal, P., Flax, J., & Benasich, A. A. (1997).Look who’s talking: A prospective study of familial trans-mission of language impairments. Journal of Speech,Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 990–1001.

Tabachnik, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariatestatistics (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Tallal, P. (2004). Improving language and literacy is a matterof time. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 721–728.

Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1973a). Developmental aphasia:Impaired rate of non-verbal processing as a function ofsensory modality. Neuropsychologia, 11, 389–398.

Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1973b, February 16). Defects of non-verbal auditory perception in children with developmentalaphasia. Nature, 241, 468–469.

Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1974). Developmental aphasia:Rate of auditory processing and selective impairment ofconsonant perception. Neuropsychologia, 12, 83–93.

Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1975). Developmental aphasia:The perception of brief vowels and extended stop consonants.Neuropsychologia, 13, 69–74.

Torgesen, J., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. (1999). Testof Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Tsao, F. M., Liu, H. M., & Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Speech per-ception in infancy predicts language development in thesecond year of life: A longitudinal study. Child Development,75, 1067–1084.

Weber, C., Hahne, A., Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A. D.(2004). Discrimination of word stress in early infant per-ception: Electrophysiological evidence. Cognitive BrainResearch, 18, 149–161.

Wechsler, D. (1974).Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.

Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition,developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across lan-guages: A psycholiguistic grain size theory. PsychologicalBulletin, 131, 3–29.

Received June 14, 2005

Revision received November 29, 2005

Accepted August 7, 2006

DOI: 10.1044 /1092-4388(2007/046)

Contact author: Usha Goswami, Centre for Neuroscience,Faculty of Education, 184 Hills Road, Cambridge CB2 8PQ,United Kingdom. E-mail: [email protected].

Kathleen Corriveau and Elizabeth Pasquini are now affiliatedwith the Graduate School of Education, HarvardUniversity.

666 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 50 • 647–666 • June 2007

Page 21: Basic Auditory Processing Skills and Specific Language ... · Kochanski etal. (2005)reached similarconclusionsin an investigationof a largecorpusof naturalspeechcovering sevenEnglishdialects.

Recommended