+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Behavior Study

Behavior Study

Date post: 06-Apr-2018
Category:
Upload: samudrala-jayaprakash-narayana
View: 215 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 24

Transcript
  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    1/24

    Parental Assessment of Behavior in Chinese Adoptees During Early Childhood

    Jay W. Rojewski, Ph.D.

    Department of Occupational Studies

    University of Georgia

    Michael S. Shapiro, Ph.D.

    Michael S. Shapiro and Associates, PC

    Piedmont College

    Athens, Georgia

    Mary Shapiro, Ph.D.

    Michael S. Shapiro and Associates, PC

    Piedmont College

    Athens, Georgia

    All correspondence concerning this manuscript should be addressed to Dr. Jay W. Rojewski, Department

    of Occupational Studies, University of Georgia, 210 Rivers Crossing, Athens, GA 30602 (706.542.4461;

    e-mail: [email protected]).

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    2/24

    Abstract

    The Parent Rating Scale of the Behavior Assessment Scale for Children (PRS-BASC) was used to

    examine the behavior of 45 Chinese adoptees. In all but one case, results from the 9 BASC-PRS scales

    ranged in the average, or normal, range. Thus, as a group, no deviations from normal behavior were

    revealed. However, the variability of ratings for several scalesHyperactivity, Aggression, Conduct

    Problems, and Attention Problemsindicated a greater potential for at-risk behavior. Age of adoption

    from China was not a significant influence on parents perceptions of adoptees behavior. However, older

    adoptees were more likely to be rated hyperactive or aggressive than younger children, while younger

    adoptees were more likely to exhibit withdrawal.

    KEY WORDS: Intercountry Adoption, Chinese Adoptees, Child Behavior.

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    3/24

    Parental Assessment of Behavior in Chinese Adoptees During Early Childhood

    Today, adoption is viewed as a viable alternative for building a family. However, it has only been since

    the end of World War II that the negative stigma associated with adoption has diminished.1

    Ironically, as

    the interest in adoption increased, the availability of healthy adoptable babies and young children in the

    United States (and other industrialized Western countries) decreased due to falling birth rates and growing

    social acceptance of single parenthood. As a result, many adoptive couples began to look to other

    countries and cultures for adoptive children.2, 3

    International adoption, also referred to as intercountry adoption (ICA), was popularized after the

    Korean War when extensive efforts were initiated to bring orphaned and abandoned South Korean

    children to the United States.4

    Around this time, international adoption was largely motivated by the

    adoptive parents desire to rescue abandoned children (p. 84).2

    While many couples still cite religious and

    humanitarian reasons for adopting internationally, more recent interest has increased among couples

    primarily motivated by infertility. Regardless of adoptive couples motives, international adoption has

    steadily increased over the past decade to where now the practice is a permanent part of American

    culture.3, 5, 6

    For example, in 1989, a total of 7,948 intercountry adoptions were recorded by the U.S.

    Immigration and Naturalization Service.7

    This figure almost doubled to 15,774 in the 1998 calendar year.

    While many countries are represented in U.S. intercountry adoptions, a majority of adoptees come from

    Asia, Central America, and South America.8

    One country where international adoption has received increased attention over the past several years

    is China. Given Chinas one childpolicy to control population growth and an historical preference for sons

    to support and care for parents in old age, many female children are abandoned in hopes of giving birth to

    a male child.9,10

    As a result, the number of girls in state-run orphanages has swelled dramatically during

    the past decade. In an effort to relieve some of the pressures associated with this extremely complex and

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    4/24

    difficult situation, the Chinese government allows the international adoption of abandoned girls.6

    Adoption

    of Chinese children by U.S. families has grown from 201 in 1992 to 4,206 in 1998.7

    According to INS

    immigration visa records, a total of 14,749 Chinese children have been adopted by U.S. parents from 1985

    through 1998.11

    Research on international adoption by U.S. families is limited, although a fair number of studies have

    been carried out in Europe [Unfortunately,] most are not available in English and many are not

    published (p. 747).3

    Available research has tended to focus on the initial and long-term adjustment of

    adopted children and their familiesKorean adoptees in the 1970s and a more diverse population including

    Vietnamese and Colombian adoptees in the 1980s. Although much of the early research reports generally

    positive outcomes, empirical findings on the adjustment of international adoptees is still inconclusive and, at

    times, contradictory.4

    Some studies have shown that functioning levels of intercountry adoptees do not significantly differ

    from nonadopted or domestically adopted children. A nationwide study by Kim12

    examined the long-term

    adjustment of Korean adoptees in the U.S. focusing on identity and socialization patterns. Korean

    adoptees were found to progress well in all areas of life, report a positive self-concept, and did not

    experience any serious mental health problems. Bagley8

    reported that Korean adoptees exhibit very good

    patterns of adjustment and high academic achievement. Recently, Levy-Shiff, Zoran, and Shulman13

    found

    no significant differences in the school adjustment, psychological adjustment at home (e.g., anxiety,

    depression, hyperactivity, and self-concept), or ability to cope with adoption issues between international

    and domestic adoptees in a sample of 100 Israeli families. Tizard3

    provided an extensive summary of

    additional studies that have reported positive findings about the behavioral-emotional characteristics of

    international adopteeschildren adopted from South Korea, Latin America, Thailand, Vietnam, India, and

    other Asian countries (e.g., Hoksbergen,14

    Kuhl15

    ).

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    5/24

    Other studies have reported less positive findings. Kim et al.1

    reported that common behavioral

    problems of young Korean adoptees included temper tantrums and excessive or frequent crying, especially

    in children adopted before the age of 3 years. In children adopted after the age of 3, learning difficulties

    and shyness-withdrawing behavior were the most common problems. Jenista and Chapman16

    compared

    adopted and nonadopted children referred for mental health counseling and found that behavioral and

    emotional problems increased for adoptees as the childs age of placement increased. Even then, however,

    reported problems usually fell within the normal ranges of all children clinically referred.

    Verhulst, Althaus, and Versluis-Den Bieman17

    compared the prevalence of behavioral-emotional

    problems and competencies of internationally adopted children living in the Netherlands with nonadopted

    Dutch children and found that adopted boys, ages 12-15 years, exhibited higher rates of externalized

    behavior problemshyperactivity and delinquency. In a follow-up study, Verhulst and Versluis-Den

    Bieman18

    continued to see a significant increase in maladaptive behaviorsparticularly withdrawn and

    delinquent behaviorand a decrease in competencies of ICA adolescents. The authors were not able to

    attribute these problem behaviors to the age or medical condition of the child at placement, early abuse or

    neglect, or racial antagonism. Hoksbergen19 also reported increased behavior problems in his study of

    international adopteeshigher rates of internalized behavior problems for girls and externalized behavior

    problems for boys. In general, problem behaviors were most likely to be found in adolescents rather than

    younger children.3

    It is important to remember that for a majority of international adoptees outcomes are positive; they

    have close and mutually satisfying relationships with their parents and are as successful as other adoptive

    families.5

    When difficulties do occur they are often attributable to children who were adopted at a

    relatively late age or as a consequence of early traumatic experiences.3

    Despite the increasing numbers of ICA adoptions, including those of Chinese children, by U.S.

    families, relatively little is known about the scientific, psychological, or social issues surrounding these

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    6/24

    children and the adoption process. Therefore, our purpose in this study was to profile select behavioral

    characteristics of young Chinese adoptees. The information provided may contribute to an emerging

    understanding of the demographic, psychological, and social characteristics of Chinese adoptees. In

    addition, knowledge can be used to address the general lack of awareness and understanding about (1) the

    theoretical relevance, (2) potential applied and policy issues, and (3) developmental trajectories of social

    relations and attachment patterns currently associated with intercountry adoption.2

    Given the contradictory

    nature of the literature, it was difficult to establish a priori hypotheses, although the trend toward finding

    increased behavioral-emotional problems during adolescence influenced our belief that relatively fewer

    behavioral problems would be encountered with young Chinese adoptees.

    Method

    Participants

    A total of 61 research packets were mailed to adoptive families who had adopted children from China

    within the past 5 years and completed their adoptions through a state-licensed, church-sponsored agency

    located in the Southeast. Only parents with adopted children 2 years of age or older were included in the

    original data pool. A total of 44 families returned completed research materials representing a 71.1%

    return rate. Responding families resided throughout the United States. All parents were Caucasian and

    between the ages of 35-50 years. Most families were in middle or upper socioeconomic levels.

    Responses for 45 children (1 family had two children adopted from China) were included in the final

    data pool including 39 females and 6 males. Thirty-eight children were under the age of 6 years (female, n

    = 33; male, n = 5), while 7 children (female, n = 6; male, n = 1) were 6 years of age or older. The mean

    current age of adoptees was slightly under 4 years of age (M= 46.9 months, SD = 19.1,Mdn = 43.0) with

    ages ranging from 22 months to 116 months (9 years, 8 months) of age. The mean age at adoption was

    found to be 21.7 months (SD = 17.9), although the median of 15.0 months may provide a more accurate

    description of the sample as 86.7% of children were adopted on or before their 3rd birthday. Ages at

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    7/24

    adoption ranged from 1 month to 84 months (7 years). The average length of time adoptees had spent in

    their adoptive homes was 25.5 months (SD = 15.6) ranging from a low of 3 months to a high of 62 months

    (4 years, 2 months.). Interestingly, the mode of 38 months (3 years, 2 months) was reported by 20% of the

    sample (n = 9).

    Instrumentation

    The Parent Rating Scale (PRS) of the Behavior Assessment System for Children (BASC)20

    was used

    to collect data regarding parental perceptions of adoptees behavior. The BASC provides a multimethod,

    multidimensional assessment of emotional disorders, personality constructs, and behavioral problems of

    children. The BASC contains five separate components that can be used individually or in any combination

    including a childs self-report scale (for older children), parent rating scale, teacher rating scale, structured

    developmental history, and a system for recording directly observed behavior.21

    The PRS contains 131

    items that measure parents perceptions of their childs adaptive and problem (clinical) behaviors in

    community and home settings. Three forms are available for different age groupspreschool, child and

    adolescent. The PRS uses a four-choice response format ranging from neverto almost always and takes

    approximately 15-20 minutes to complete.

    The following 9 clinical scales were used: Aggression, Anxiety, Attention Problems, Atypicality

    (psychoticism), Conduct Problems, Depression, Hyperactivity, Somatization, and Withdrawl. The

    Hyperactivity, Aggression, and Conduct Problems scales can be combined to reflect an Externalizing

    Problems composite score. Hyperactivity refers to the tendency to be overly active, rush through activities,

    and act impulsively. Aggression is defined as verbal or physical behavior that is hostile or threatening to

    others. Conduct Problems consist of antisocial or rule -breaking behaviors including the destruction of

    property (scores are available only for children 6 years of age or older). Internalizing Problems are

    reflected in results of Anxiety, Depression, and Somatization scales. Anxiety is a tendency to be nervous,

    fearful, or worried about real or imagined problems. Depression includes feelings of unhappiness, sadness,

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    8/24

    or stress that bring about an inability to carry out daily activities or bring on thoughts of suicide.

    Somatization describes being overly sensitive to relatively minor physical problems or discomforts. Three

    additional scalesAttention Problems, Atypicality, and Withdrawlwere also included for analysis.

    Attention Problems denote a tendency to be easily distracted and unable to concentrate for extended

    periods. Atypicality refers to immature behavior considered odd or associated with psychosis. Withdrawl is

    the tendency to avoid social contact.21

    The BASC has generally received high marks as a reliable instrument. Past studies on the reliability of

    the PRS, in particular, have found internal consistency coefficients ranging from the low .60s to the high

    .80s with median correlations near .80. Merenda22

    indicated that in general, the coefficients of internal

    consistency [for the BASC] are quite impressive . . . at least half of them are in the range .80-.89 (p.

    231). Test-retest estimates are less impressive, although most studies have reported correlations ranging

    from around .70 to .90. Sandoval and Echandia23

    urged users to exercise caution when using the BASC

    with preschool children as instrument reliability scores tended to be lower. Reasonable evidence of

    instrument validity has been established in several studies showing moderate correlations with similar types

    of scales, e.g., Conners Rating Scales, and the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist.24

    Reynolds and

    Kamphaus20

    reported the results of both exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses for the PRS that

    provide additional evidence of the underlying structure (i.e., validity) of BASC results.

    The BASC provides two major types of norms for the PRS, general and clinical. The general norms

    were used in this study and are considered representative of the general U.S. population. PRS norms are

    based on maternal ratings only; relatively small differences exist between mothers and fathers scores.24

    Norms are subdivided by age and gender to reflect actual differences reported between males and

    females. Specifically, females tend to score higher on the Depression scale, while males tend to obtain

    higher scores on Aggression, Conduct Problems, Hyperactivity, and Attention Problems. Reynolds and

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    9/24

    Kamphaus20, 21

    recommended using the general (combined) norms for most situations to allow these

    gender differences to be reflected in normative scores

    Prior to the interpretation of an individuals results, the raw scores on the BASC-PRS are

    standardized by calculating raw scores for each behavior scale into linear T-scores (M= 50, SD = 10).

    This procedure is advisable because many of the constructs being measured are . . . not normally

    distributed, and this metric preserves underlying dimensions (p. 422).23

    The T-score range, 41-59 T, is

    considered average. Conversely, T-scores of 60-69 Tare considered within the at-riskrange, while scores

    greater than 70 Tare deemed clinically significant. Percentile equivalent scores are also provided for all

    standardized scores.20,24

    Procedure

    A copy of the computer-scored BASC-PRS (1 page front and back) was included in a packet of

    information sent by the adoption agency to all parents who had adopted children from China in the past 5

    years and whose child was 2 years of age or older at the time of the investigation. The packet contained

    information about a planned China families reunion weekend sponsored by the agency and scheduled for

    later in the year. A cover letter informed participants about the behavior scale and that information

    obtained from the BASC was being used in preparation for several workshops scheduled as a part of the

    reunion weekend. Confidentiality of response was assured to all participants. A period of four-weeks was

    established for receiving responses, at which time data collection ceased. Reminders to respond to the

    instrument were included in several agency mailings that offered additional information about the summer

    reunion.

    Data Analysis

    Several analyses were chosen to address our research questions. First, the entire sample was used to

    calculate descriptive statistics for parent responses to the 9 BASC scales. In addition to an overall

    description, we examined scores by gender as differences in behavior between male and female children

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    10/24

    have been reported.20,21,24

    Next, we removed male adoptees scores from the remainder of our analysis as

    males represented a small proportion of our sample (n = 6). The small number of male adoptees in our

    sample reflects the current situation regarding adoption from China, i.e., the vast majority of children

    adopted from China are female.9,10,6

    While the removal of male adoptees scores did reduce our overall

    sample size, the remaining pool of female scores can be interpreted without the potentially confounding

    influence of gender and also reflects the fact that most Chines adoptees are female.

    The final analysis included three sets of comparisons designed to examine parent ratings of adoptees

    behavior on the 9 BASC scales (dependent variables) based on three independent variables: age of child at

    adoption (2 groupschildren adopted before 18 months of age or after 18 months of age); current age of

    adoptees (2 groupschildren 3 years of age or younger and those over 3 years of age); and, the length of

    time since placement with adoptive family (2 groupschildren in adoptive homes less than 2 years or

    those longer than 2 years). Only 2 groups were constructed for each set of statistical comparisons

    primarily because of the small sample size. The alpha level for significance tests was set at .05, even

    though this raised the possibility of Type I error because of probability pyramiding. The decision was made

    because of the exploratory nature of this research and our desire to identify differences if they existed.

    Effect size (ES) coefficients (Cohens d, standardized difference) were calculated for statistically

    significant comparisons to examine the magnitude, or practical importance, of observed differences.

    Knowledge of effect size can help clarify the implications that statistically significant results have for

    theory and practice.25

    Results

    Description of Adoptees Behaviors

    Descriptive data was generated for the entire sample, as well as by gender. However, since only a

    small number of males were represented, these findings must be interpreted with caution. Even so, we felt

    it was important to include data about male adoptees because of a lack of available literature describing

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    11/24

    adopted children from China. Using Reynolds and Kamphaus20

    guidelines for interpretation, all but one of

    the BASC-PRS scales ranged in the average, or normal, range. Thus, as a group, no deviations from

    normal behavior were revealed (see Table 1). However, when looking at the variability of scores (using

    1 SD to account for roughly 68% of the sample), ratings for several scalesHyperactivity, Aggression,

    Conduct problems, and Attention problemsindicate greater potential for at-risk behavior (see Fig. 1).

    Table 1

    Parental Ratings (T-Scores) of Childrens Behavior on 9 BASC Scales

    BASC scales Female Male Total

    M SD %-tile M SD %-tile M SD %-tile

    Hyperactivity 46.92 11.87 43rd

    54.33 13.92 69th

    47.91 12.26 46th

    Aggression 47.97 10.28 47th

    50.00 11.98 55th

    48.24 10.40 47th

    Conduct problemsa

    52.00

    15.40 67th

    60.00

    86th

    53.14 14.38 70th

    Anxiety 44.92 7.45 32nd

    44.17 6.79 32nd

    44.82 7.29 32nd

    Depression 45.54 8.34 35th

    45.00 9.47 35th

    45.47 8.38 35th

    Somatization 46.03 9.85 36th

    39.50 4.46 17th

    45.16 9.54 36th

    Atypicality 47.97 8.75 50th

    47.00 9.82 50th

    47.84 8.79 50th

    Withdrawl 49.85 10.31 52nd 39.33 9.18 12th 48.31 10.82 47th

    Attention problems 45.79 10.57 33rd

    54.83 11.57 54th

    47.00 11.01 37th

    Note. Some percentile scores have been rounded or estimated to accommodate use ofT-scores rather

    than raw scores.aScores for Conduct Problems were available only for children 6 years of age or older (n

    = 6 for females, n = 1 for males).

    Scales indicating slightly elevated risk of behavior problems. While none of the mean ratings fell into

    at-risk or clinically significant categories, four scales had enough score variability to indicate the presence

    of an elevated risk of behavior problems. Hyperactivity T-scores ranged from 23 to 76 T. The mean score

    for boys was approximately three-fourths a standard deviation higher than for girls. Additionally, when

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    12/24

    score variability was considered, boys were at a higher risk of being identified as hyperactive. Eight

    adoptees scores were rated at or above the at-riskthreshold, T 60 (female, n = 6; male, n = 2).

    BASC-PRS scales

    Figure 1. Mean T-scores and standard deviations on BASC-PRS clinical scales for children from China.

    As a group, parent ratings of aggressive behavior were within normal limits. Scores ranged from 33 to

    73 Tand were similar for boys and girls. Five children (female, n = 4; male, n = 1) had scores above the

    range reflecting normal behavior. It seems that when parents indicated aggressive behavior in their child,

    they were likely to rate it as a serious concern. Four of these 5 children had Aggression scores that placed

    them in the clinically significant range (T 70). As a group, boys were at slightly greater risk of being

    identified as aggressive than girls.

    Parents ratings on conduct-related behavior was obtained only for the 7 adoptees who were 6 years

    of age or older; this may have been a contributing factor to the larger variability of scores observed for

    girls. Only one boy was included in this group which did not allow for any meaningful comparison of mean

    Conductproblems

    (6+yearsofage)

    Attentionproblems

    10

    20

    30

    40

    50

    60

    70

    80

    90

    100

    Hyperactivity

    Aggression

    Anxiety

    Depression

    Somatizatio

    Atypicality

    Withdraw

    At-risk range

    Clinically significant

    risk

    T-scores

    Female mean T-scores

    and standard deviations

    Male mean T-scores and

    standard deviations

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    13/24

    scores based on gender. Two adoptees (female, n = 1; male, n = 1) had behavioral ratings that placed

    them in the at-risk range for conduct problems.

    Scores on the Attention problems scale ranged from 25 to 71 Twith the overall mean at the 37th

    percentile. Boys had a considerably higher mean score on the Attention problems scale (+.82 SD) than

    their female peers. Scores of 2 boys (out of 6) and 3 girls were situated in the at-risk range.

    Scales within normal ranges of behavior. With only one exception, results on the remaining 5 BASC-

    PRS scales are interesting for their similarities rather than differences. Mean scores on the Anxiety,

    Depression, and Atypicality scales were almost identical for girls and boys, as was the variability of these

    scores expressed in standard deviation units. Scores on the Anxiety scale ranged from 29 to 59 T. None

    of the individual Anxiety ratings were at or above the at-risk threshold of 60 T. Depression scores ranged

    from 30 to 71 T; although, the behavior of only one girl was rated high enough to be identified as at-risk.

    Parent ratings on the Atypicality scale ranged from 29 to 79 Twith 2 scores in the at-risk range.

    Ratings obtained for Somatization and Withdrawl scales were higher for boys than girls; the only two

    with this response pattern. Scores on the Somatization scale ranged from 16 to 71 T. And, while the mean

    Somatization score for girls was .69 SD greater than for boys, both ratings were within acceptable

    behavior ranges. Only 3 scores, all girls, were above the specified range for normal behavior.

    Scores for adopted girls on the Withdrawl scale were almost a full standard deviation higher than for

    boys. This large difference can probably be attributed to several issuesa very low mean score for boys

    located at the 12th percentile, and although not reflective in the overall distribution of scores (see Figure 1),

    Scores for five girls were in the at-risk range and one was in the clinically significant risk range. Parent

    perceptions on this behavior scale ranged from 25-71 T.

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    14/24

    Influence of Age on the Perceived Behavior of Girls Adopted form China

    A series oft-tests were used to examine the possible influences of three variablesage of child at

    time of adoption, current age of adoptees, and the length of time children had lived with their adoptive

    familyon the behavior ratings of adoptees using to the 9 BASC scales as dependent variables.

    Age at adoption. Two age groups were constructedchildren adopted before 18 months (n = 25) and

    children adopted after 18 months of age (n = 13). None of the t-tests comparing these two groups on the 9

    BASC-PRS scales were statistically significant at the .05 level. This seems to indicate that age of adoption

    from China was not a significant influence on parents perceptions of adoptees behavior. The behaviors

    of children were similar regardless of their age at the time of adoption.

    Current age of adoptees. A second series of analyses examined perceived behaviors of adoptees

    according to current age: 3 years of age or younger (n = 17) and over 3 years of age (n = 22). Results of

    the t-tests revealed statistically significant differences on Hyperactivity, Aggression, and Withdrawl scales.

    Although differences were noted, mean scores for both groups were still well within normal ranges for

    behavior. Even so, the behavior of older adoptees was rated more hyperactive (M= 51.68) than that of

    their younger peers (M= 40.76), t(37) = -3.17,p = .003. The magnitude of this difference was

    substantial. AnES coefficient of 1.02 indicated that approximately 84% of younger children scored at or

    below the mean score established for the older adoptees group. Similarly, older adoptees were rated higher

    on the Aggression scale (M= 51.91) than younger adoptees (M= 42.88), t(37) = -2.99,p = .005,ES =

    1.00. Conversely, younger children were rated significantly higher in withdrawl behavior (M= 53.82) than

    their older peers (M= 46.77), t(37) = 2.23,p = .03,ES = .71.

    Length of time with adoptive family. A final series oft-tests examined the potential differences

    between children placed with their adoptive family for less than 2 years (n = 22) and those placed for

    more than 2 years (n = 16). Statistically significant differences were found on the Hyperactivity,

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    15/24

    Aggression, and Somatization scales. Consistent with earlier findings, the mean scores of both groups

    were within normal behavior ranges. Children placed over 2 years ago were rated higher on the

    Hyperactivity scale (M= 51.63) than adoptees in their homes less than 2 years (M= 43.32), t(36) = -2.21,

    p = .03,ES = .73. Likewise, children with longer residence had higher aggression scores (M= 52.19) than

    children in their adoptive homes for shorter lengths of time (M= 45.23), t(36) = -2.14,p = .04,ES = .66.

    Children placed with their adoptive family more than 2 years ago were also rated higher (M= 49.75) on

    the Somatization scaleoverly sensitive to minor physical problemsthan children in adoptive homes for

    2 years or less (M= 43.09), t(36) = -2.13,p = .04,ES = .77.

    Discussion

    This study sought to describe the behavior of young children, primarily preschoolers, adopted by U.S.

    parents from China and to examine the potential influence of age and placement on behavior. The

    importance of analyses like this one is the improved understanding they can bring to the practice of

    intercountry adoption in general and to children adopted from China in particular. Even so, several

    delimitations should be kept in mind as these results are interpreted. First, we measured and examined

    parent perceptions of their adopted childs behavior. While parents perceptions are critical in determining

    the appropriateness of behavior, other sources of information were not used. Future studies may

    incorporate additional information sources such as childcare providers or social workers. Regardless of

    how data was collected, the positive results described in this study cannot be used as a predictor of future

    behavior. In fact, some researchers3,5

    have indicated that most behavior problems in adopted children

    occur during adolescence. Third, our results were obtained with a relatively small sample and may not be

    reflective of the population of U.S. adopted children from China. However, many past ICA studies have

    also relied on small sample sizes given the difficulty in locating and following up with adoptive

    families.1,12,26

    Finally, many issues of particular interest and importance in determining long-term behavior

    of intercountry adopteescultural identity formation, self-concept, attachment and bonding, or experiences

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    16/24

    of racism and discriminationwere not addressed. Additional study is needed to provide information on

    these issues.

    Despite these potential constraints, we feel our results do contribute to a better understanding of the

    behavior patterns of Chinese adoptees in early childhood. Data can be added to the existing literature on

    ICAs to form a preliminary baseline of behavior and behavioral expectations for children from China; not

    only for current adoptees but for future adoptees, as well.

    Past literature1, 3, 5, 6, 8, 12

    describes the vulnerability of intercountry adoptees to a variety of problems

    including developmental delays, social difficulty, and behavioral/emotional problems as a result of maternal

    deprivation and abandonment, lack of an attachment figure in country of origin, visible differences between

    adoptive family and adoptee, racism-discrimination-prejudice, and concern or confusion with cultural

    identify formation. Although these are important concerns and could contribute to behavior-related

    problems, our results show that, in general, adoptees, regardless of their gender, did not deviate from

    normally expected behaviors to a significant degree. These findings support the past work of researchers

    like Kim12

    who reported no significant behavioral or mental health problems with a sample of South

    Korean adoptees. Other studies8,13,14,15

    have found concurring results for children adopted from a variety

    of countries including Latin America, Thailand, India, and Vietnam. Our findingsthat some adoptees

    were at slightly greater risk of hyperactivity, aggression, and conduct or attention problemsextends the

    results of these previous studies.

    The normalcy of BASC-PRS ratings might be the result of several factors, either alone or in

    combination. Typically, families able to adopt internationally have the economic resources that allow

    access to needed services, institutions, and support.2, 27

    Conceivably, parents would have greater access to

    medical care and psychological counseling should behavior problems be identified The nature of parenting

    may also be reflected in these findings. Most parents adopting from China, or any other international

    locale, tend to be older (at least 35 years of age for China) and have made conscious decisions about

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    17/24

    bringing an adopted child into their family. This situation probably results in highly motivated, interested,

    and actively involved parenting that may reduce, distinguish, or overlook minor behavior problems. Third,

    while speculative, it is possible that adoptive families share similar values and beliefs (e.g., middle/upper-

    class values) that are reflected in daily interactions between adoptive parents and child, e.g., discipline and

    nurturing.

    Another possible explanation for these positive findings may be the preponderance of girls in our

    sample. Kim28

    claimed that the positive results from behavioral studies on intercountry adoptees,

    particularly children from South Korea, was probably due to the overrepresentation of adopted girls. He

    observed that, regardless of adoptive status, most epidemiological studies have shown a lower risk of

    emotional and behavioral problems for girls than boys. Future study may focus on the characteristics of

    boys adopted from China to determine if they are at greater risk of emotional or behavioral problems.

    Several other possible explanations exist, such as the one advanced by Jenista,29

    Bazzoli30

    and others

    who noted that a majority of infants being adopted from China have been in excellent physical and mental

    condition. Kim28

    attributes the good health of Chinese adoptees to a reflection of Confucian beliefs held by

    orphanage workers that place high value on children. These views result in good pre-adoption care.

    Further, since most children are young when adopted, the long-term, maladaptive effects of

    institutionalization, if any, are less apparent. Even for children that many have suffered from

    institutionalization, Tizard3

    reported that most intercountry adoptees make rapid progress within the first

    year of their arrival, especially younger children.

    The influence of adoptee age on parents behavior ratings was somewhat mixed. The childs age at

    time of adoption did not make a difference in assessment of behavior. Girls adopted earlier or later in life

    exhibited similar types of behavior patterns. When current age was examined, older adoptees were found

    to have higher ratings for hyperactivity and aggression, while younger adoptees rated higher in withdrawl

    behavior. When length of placement in adoptive home was examined, adoptees placed for longer periods

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    18/24

    of time had higher hyperactivity, aggression, and somatization scores than their more recently placed

    peers. As these differences are considered, it is important to note that group mean scores were all within

    normal behavior ranges.

    Several possible explanations for these findings might be advanced. For example, the lack of at-risk

    behavior in our sample of adoptees might reflect Tizards3

    belief that the earlier adoptions occurred, the

    smaller chance of educational or behavioral problems. It is also possible that our participants were simply

    too young (most under 5 years of age) for us to be able to detect behavior problems or differences. Many

    studies assert that behavior problems become more evident as adoptees grow older. Jenista and

    Chapman16

    found that while adoptees behavior problems increased with age, they were within normally

    acceptable limits. For our group, it is possible that the short time span between younger and older adoptees

    was not sufficient to reveal many differences. Deacon5

    noted that significant increase in counseling of

    intercountry adoptees for psychological and behavior problems between ages 11 to 15 years. Follow-up

    study is important to determine the long-range implications of age and placement issues like those studied.

    Differences we found generally support and extend the conclusions of past researchers. Adoptees

    who were older or had been placed in their adoptive home longer generally had higher scores, although

    behavior ratings were still within normal ranges. Kim12

    found that age at placement and length of

    placement did not significantly relate to formation of adoptees self-concept. Similarly, Verhulst and

    Versluis-Den Bieman18

    reported that the age or time of placement could not explain increases in

    maladaptive behaviors of adopted adolescents. It is possible that the behavior differences we observed are

    precursors to the increased chances of behavior problems many intercountry adoptees experience as

    adolescents. However, as Serbin2

    noted, the developmental trajectories of such patterns are currently

    unknown (p. 89). This, again, points to the need for long-term research and follow-up of adoptees.

    Another possible explanation is that behaviors (hyperactivity and aggression in particular) may be initiated

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    19/24

    by adoptees as preliminary efforts to differentiate self from family members but are misinterpreted by

    parents as inappropriate behavior5.

    International adoption from China, or any other country, is a complex and oftentimes controversial

    practice.6,13

    Difficulties are compounded by limited and inconclusive data about the effects of international

    adoption on childrens adjustment and well being. While discrepancies exist, evidence from this study

    suggests that Chinese adoptees exhibit generally normal behavior patterns similar to international adoptees

    from other countries examined in prior studies. Although older children had higher hyperactive and

    aggressive behavior patterns than their younger peers, these behaviors were still well within normal

    behavioral expectation for preschoolers. The results are positive in that, at least at this relatively early

    stage of life, adoptees did not experience significant behavioral problems as a result of their experience.

    Continued study and follow-along will be important to document developmental obstacles, beneficial

    supports, or behavior changes that occur as adoptive preschoolers enter elementary school, become

    adolescents, and enter adulthood.

    Summary

    The purpose of our study was to profile select behavioral characteristics of children adopted from

    China. The Parent Rating Scale of the Behavior Assessment Scale for Children (PRS-BASC) was

    completed and returned by 44 of 61 families (71.1%) with children adopted from China. Results indicated

    that, as a group, no deviations from normal behavior were found. When the variability of scores was

    examined, several scalesHyperactivity, Aggression, Conduct problems, and Attention problems

    revealed the presence of an elevated risk for behavior problems. The age at which children were adopted

    was not a significant factor on parents perceptions of behavior. Older children were more likely to be

    rated hyperactive or aggressive than younger children, while younger children were more likely to exhibit

    withdrawl. Even so, the majority of respondents scores placed adoptees behavior well within normal,

    acceptable ranges. While discepancies exist, our evidence is positive, suggesting that Chinese adoptees

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    20/24

    exhibit generally normal behavior patterns similar to international adoptees from other countries. Continued

    study and follow-along will be important to document the changes that may occur in behavior, self-identity

    and adjustment in future years.

    References

    1. Kim SP, Hong S, Kim BS: Adoption of Korean children by New York area couples: a preliminary

    study. Child Welfare 63: 419-428, 1979.

    2. Serbin LA: Research on international adoption: implications for developmental theory and social

    policy.Int J Behav Dev 20: 83-92, 1997.

    3. Tizard B: Intercountry adoption: a review of the evidence.J Child Psych Psychiatry Allied Discp 32:

    743-756, 1991.

    4. Wilkinson HS: Psycholegal process and issues in international adoption. Am J Family Therapy 23:

    173-183, 1995.

    5. Deacon SA: Intercountry adoption and the family life cycle.Am J Family Therapy 25: 245-260, 1997.

    6. Riley NE: American adoptions of Chinese girls: the sociopolitical matrices of individual decisions.

    Womens Studies Intl Forum 20: 87-102, 1997.

    7. Immigration and Naturalization Service:International adoptions1989-1998 [WWW document].

    URL: http://www.hopeforchildren.org/adoptionsgraph.html, 1999.

    8. Bagley C: The psychology of adoption: case studies of national and international adoptions.Bull of

    Hong Kong Psych Soc 28/29: 95-115, 1992.

    9. Liedtke JA, Brasseur LE:New American Families: Chinese Daughters and Their Single Mothers.

    Bloomington, IL: StarNet Digital Publishing, 1997.

    10. OBrian CP: International adoption in China.Adopt Fostering 21(4): 6-13, 1997.

    11. FCCChina Adoption Statistics: Statistical information on adoptions from China [WWW

    document]. URL: http://fwcc.org/statistics.html, 1999.

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    21/24

    12. Kim DS: Issues in transracial and transcultural adoption. Soc Casework59: 477-486, 1978.

    13. Levy-Shiff R, Zoran N, Shulman S: International and domestic adoption: child, parents, and family

    adjustment.Intl J Behav Dev 20: 109-129, 1997.

    14. Hoksbergen RAC: Intercountry adoption coming of age in The Netherlands: basic issues, trends, and

    development. In:Intercountry Adoption: A Seven Country Perspective, eds. Alstein H, Simon RJ.

    New York: Praeger Publishing, 1990.

    15. Kuhl W: When Adopted Children of Foreign Origins Grow Up. Osnabruck: Terre des Hommes,

    1985.

    16. Jenista JA, Chapman D: Medical problems of foreign-born adopted children.Am J Diseases Child

    141: 293-302, 1987.

    17. Verhulst FC, Althaus M, Versluis-Den Bieman HJM: Problem behavior in international adoptees: I. an

    epidemiological study.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 29(1): 94-103, 1990.

    18. Verhulst FC, Versluis-Den Bieman HJM: Developmental course of problem behaviors in adolescent

    adoptees.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 29: 51-159, 1995.

    19. Hoksbergen RAC: Turmoil for adoptees during adolescence?Intl J Behav Dev 20: 33-46, 1997.

    20. Reynolds CR, Kamphaus RW:Manual for the BASC: Behavior Assessment System for Children.

    Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, 1992.

    21. Kamphaus RW, Reynolds CR, Hatcher NM: Treatment planning and evaluation with the BASC: the

    Behavior Assessment System for Children. In: The Use of Psychological Testing for Treatment

    Planning and Outcome Assessment, 2nd ed., ed.Maruish M. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

    Associates, 1999.

    22. Merenda PF: BASC: behavior assessment system for children.Meas Eval Counsel Dev 28: 229-232,

    1996.

    23. Sandoval J, Echandia A: Behavior assessment system for children.J School Psych 32:419-425, 1994.

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    22/24

    24. Kline RB: New objective rating scales for child assessment, I. parent- and teacher-informant

    inventories of the Behavior Assessment System for Children: the Child Behavior Checklist, and the

    Teacher Report Form.J Psychoeduc Assess 12: 289-306, 1994.

    25. Thompson B: Editorial policies regarding statistical significance tests: Further comments.Ed

    Researcher26(5): 29-32, 1997.

    26. Trolley BC, Walling J, Hansen J: International adoption: Issues of acknowledgement of adoption and

    birth culture. Child and Adolesc Soc Work J12: 465-479, 1995.

    27. Ramos JD: Counseling internationally adopted children.Elem School Guid Counsel 25: 147-152,

    1990.

    28. Kim WJ: International adoption: A case review of Korean children. Child Psychiatry Human Dev 25:

    141-154, 1995.

    29. Medical Column:A letter to families with children from China from Jerri Ann Jenista, MD

    [WWW

    document]. URL http://www.fwcc.org/healthconcerns.html, 1999.

    30. Health of Chinese Adoptees:Health of Chinese adoptees [WWW document]. URL

    http://www.night.net/rosie/health.html: 1999.

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    23/24

  • 8/3/2019 Behavior Study

    24/24


Recommended