+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner...

Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner...

Date post: 27-Mar-2015
Category:
Upload: riley-gill
View: 220 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
Popular Tags:
25
Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1 , Friedo W. Dekker 1,2 , Christoph Wanner 3 , Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology and Physiopathology of Renal Diseases and Hypertension of Reggio Calabria, Italy 1 ERA–EDTA Registry, Department of Medical Informatics, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. 2 Department of Clinical Epidemiology, Leiden University Medical Centre, Leiden, The Netherlands 3 University of Würzburg, Division of Nephrology, University Clinic, Würzburg, Germany Kidney International: Series on epidemiology
Transcript
Page 1: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

Bias in clinical research

Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager1, Friedo W. Dekker1,2,

Christoph Wanner3, Carmine Zoccali

CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology and Physiopathology of Renal Diseases and Hypertension of Reggio Calabria, Italy

1ERA–EDTA Registry, Department of Medical Informatics, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

2Department of Clinical Epidemiology, Leiden University Medical Centre, Leiden, The Netherlands3University of Würzburg, Division of Nephrology, University Clinic, Würzburg, Germany

Kidney International: Series on epidemiology

Page 2: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

If an experiment was conducted to find out the distribution of sizes of fish in a lake,If an experiment was conducted to find out the distribution of sizes of fish in a lake, a net could be used to catch a representative sample of fish. If net had a mesh size of a net could be used to catch a representative sample of fish. If net had a mesh size of

1 cm then no fish with sizes less than 1cm would be found. This is a result of the 1 cm then no fish with sizes less than 1cm would be found. This is a result of the method of selection (selection bias) - from the experiment, there is no way of method of selection (selection bias) - from the experiment, there is no way of

knowing whether there are any fish smaller than 1 cm.knowing whether there are any fish smaller than 1 cm.

From Wikipedia: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Selection_biasFrom Wikipedia: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Selection_bias

Page 3: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

IntroductionIntroduction

The quality of a clinical study depends on internal and external factors. Studies have internal The quality of a clinical study depends on internal and external factors. Studies have internal validity when, random error apart, reported differences between exposed and unexposed validity when, random error apart, reported differences between exposed and unexposed individuals can be attributed only to the exposure under investigation. Internal validity can be individuals can be attributed only to the exposure under investigation. Internal validity can be affected by two types of error: random error and systematic error. affected by two types of error: random error and systematic error.

- - Random error Random error depends on chance and can be minimized by increasing the sample size (1). For example if we depends on chance and can be minimized by increasing the sample size (1). For example if we measure serum creatinine three times in the same individual by using the same instrument and the same measure serum creatinine three times in the same individual by using the same instrument and the same laboratory reagents we can obtain slightly different values as a result of clinical fluctuations in the measurement. laboratory reagents we can obtain slightly different values as a result of clinical fluctuations in the measurement.

- Systematic errorsSystematic errors depend on any flaw that systematically leads to an overestimation or underestimation of the depend on any flaw that systematically leads to an overestimation or underestimation of the measurement and this type of error is independent of sample size (1). Systematic error occurs if the device we measurement and this type of error is independent of sample size (1). Systematic error occurs if the device we use, for example a sphygmomanometer for measuring blood pressure (BP), systematically overestimates or use, for example a sphygmomanometer for measuring blood pressure (BP), systematically overestimates or underestimates the “true” BP of the subject being evaluated. underestimates the “true” BP of the subject being evaluated. Systematic error is also referred to as biasSystematic error is also referred to as bias ..

The list of biases is a long one (2). We describe three types of selection biases (incidence-The list of biases is a long one (2). We describe three types of selection biases (incidence-prevalent bias, loss-to–follow-up bias and publication bias) and a series of information biases prevalent bias, loss-to–follow-up bias and publication bias) and a series of information biases [i.e. misclassification bias (recall bias, interviewer bias, observer bias and regression dilution [i.e. misclassification bias (recall bias, interviewer bias, observer bias and regression dilution bias) and lead time bias].bias) and lead time bias].

1) 1) Rothman KJRothman KJ. Random error and the role of statistics. In Epidemiology. An Introduction. Oxford University Press. New York. . Random error and the role of statistics. In Epidemiology. An Introduction. Oxford University Press. New York. USA; pp. 113-142, 2002.USA; pp. 113-142, 2002.2) 2) Sackett DLSackett DL. Bias in analytic research. J Chronic Dis 1979; 32: 51-63. . Bias in analytic research. J Chronic Dis 1979; 32: 51-63.

Page 4: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

SELECTION BIAS (OR BERKSONIAN BIAS)SELECTION BIAS (OR BERKSONIAN BIAS)

- This type of error occurs when a systematic error in the - This type of error occurs when a systematic error in the enrollment of individuals in a study – cases or controls in enrollment of individuals in a study – cases or controls in case-control study, or exposed or unexposed individuals in case-control study, or exposed or unexposed individuals in a prospective cohort study – determines a biased a prospective cohort study – determines a biased association between exposure and outcome. association between exposure and outcome.

- Here we consider three types of selection bias: - Here we consider three types of selection bias: the the incidence-prevalence bias, the loss-to-follow-up bias and incidence-prevalence bias, the loss-to-follow-up bias and the publication bias.the publication bias.

Page 5: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

Incidence - prevalence biasIncidence - prevalence bias

An important selection bias is An important selection bias is the incidence-prevalence bias the incidence-prevalence bias (or Neyman bias or survival bias)(or Neyman bias or survival bias), i.e. a bias that occurs when , i.e. a bias that occurs when we try to estimate the risk of a disease on the basis of data we try to estimate the risk of a disease on the basis of data collected at a given time point in a series of survivors rather collected at a given time point in a series of survivors rather than on data gathered during a certain time period in a group than on data gathered during a certain time period in a group of incident cases (of incident cases (Example 1Example 1) or when the sample of cases ) or when the sample of cases offers a distorted frequency of the exposure (offers a distorted frequency of the exposure (Example 2Example 2). ). This type of bias is of particular relevance in cross-sectional This type of bias is of particular relevance in cross-sectional studies.studies.

SELECTION BIAS (OR BERKSONIAN BIAS)SELECTION BIAS (OR BERKSONIAN BIAS)

Page 6: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

Incidence - prevalence bias (Example 1)Incidence - prevalence bias (Example 1)

We consider a cohort of 20 individuals with a follow-up from t0 - t2. During this follow-up, 4 individuals developed a moderate disease and 4 individuals got a severe form of the same disease. Therefore, the true risk ratio between the risk of severe disease and that of moderate disease is 1. However, people with severe disease have a higher risk to die, and before the time “t1”, 2 individuals died. If at time “t1” we perform a cross-sectional analysis (survey) we get a prevalence ratio of severe versus moderate disease of 0.5 (in fact at time “t1” we have only one case with severe disease and two cases with moderate disease), a figure that does not reflect the true risk ratio that is 1.

Therefore, whenever possible we should estimate the occurrence of a disease in terms of incidence, i.e. by counting all cases that occur in a sample in a given time interval rather than in a given time point.

Page 7: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

Incidence - prevalence bias (Example 2a)Incidence - prevalence bias (Example 2a)As further example of incidence-prevalence bias we can consider the following one.As further example of incidence-prevalence bias we can consider the following one.

In a case-control study, Ibanez et al. (3) assessed the association between chronic use of Aspirin and occurrence of end stage renal disease (ESRD).

Disease status

With ESRD(cases)

Without ESRD(controls)

Users of Aspirin 81 94

Nonusers of Aspirin 439 888

Total 520 982

In this study, the odds ratio (OR) of the use of Aspirin is: In this study, the odds ratio (OR) of the use of Aspirin is: OROR=(81/439)/(94/888)= 1.74=(81/439)/(94/888)= 1.74

An odds-ratio of 1.74 means that the odds of exposure to Aspirin were 74% higher An odds-ratio of 1.74 means that the odds of exposure to Aspirin were 74% higher in individuals with ESRD than in those without this complication. In this study, the in individuals with ESRD than in those without this complication. In this study, the selection process of cases and controls as well as the assessment of exposure selection process of cases and controls as well as the assessment of exposure status were performed in an appropriate manner, thus we can assume that this OR status were performed in an appropriate manner, thus we can assume that this OR reflects the situation in the reference population, i.e. that we can look at this OR as reflects the situation in the reference population, i.e. that we can look at this OR as at the “true” OR.at the “true” OR.

3)3) Ibanez L, Morlans M, Vidal X, et al. Ibanez L, Morlans M, Vidal X, et al. Case-control study of regular analgesic and nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory use and end- Case-control study of regular analgesic and nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory use and end-stage renal disease. Kidney Int 2005; 67: 2392-2398.stage renal disease. Kidney Int 2005; 67: 2392-2398.

Page 8: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

Incidence - prevalence bias (Example 2b)Incidence - prevalence bias (Example 2b)

We now consider a hypothetical situation where the selection of cases from the target We now consider a hypothetical situation where the selection of cases from the target population, but not that of controls, is biased because affected by previous knowledge population, but not that of controls, is biased because affected by previous knowledge by the investigator of the exposure status. In other words, the investigator, influenced by the investigator of the exposure status. In other words, the investigator, influenced by his knowledge of the exposure status, tends to gather cases mainly among by his knowledge of the exposure status, tends to gather cases mainly among individuals known to be Aspirin users.individuals known to be Aspirin users.

Disease status

With ESRD(cases)

Without ESRD(controls)

Users of Aspirin 130 94

Nonusers of Aspirin 390 888

Total 520 982

The OR in this situation is: The OR in this situation is: (130/390)/(94/888)= 3.15(130/390)/(94/888)= 3.15

Bias in the selection of cases produces a considerable alteration in the estimate of the OR Bias in the selection of cases produces a considerable alteration in the estimate of the OR (3.15 vs 1.74).(3.15 vs 1.74). To avoid this problem, the selection process of cases and controls from the To avoid this problem, the selection process of cases and controls from the target population should be rigorously the same and should be independent from the target population should be rigorously the same and should be independent from the exposure status (i.e. the investigator should be blinded to the exposure status). This type of exposure status (i.e. the investigator should be blinded to the exposure status). This type of bias, which we herein describe in relationship with case-control studies, can occur with any bias, which we herein describe in relationship with case-control studies, can occur with any type of study design. type of study design.

Page 9: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

Loss-to–follow-up biasLoss-to–follow-up biasThis bias occurs in prospective cohort studies when individuals lost to follow-up do not This bias occurs in prospective cohort studies when individuals lost to follow-up do not have the same probability of having the clinical outcome of interest in comparison with have the same probability of having the clinical outcome of interest in comparison with individuals who remain under observation. individuals who remain under observation.

• In the recent literature, a potential source of loss-to-follow-up bias can be found in the CHOIR study (a randomized clinical trial testing the effect of anemia correction in patients with chronic kidney disease not receiving dialysis) (4). The CHOIR protocol terminated from the follow-up all individuals who achieved ESRD, thus potentially generating a bias. In fact, although the relative impact of the intervention was unaffected by this bias (the proportion of excluded ESRD patients was similar between the two CHOIR study arms: 18% vs 15%), this was not true for the cumulative incidence of events in the two groups that was influenced by censoring ESRD patients.

• We consider a hypothetical prospective study aimed at determining the incidence rate of renal insufficiency in hypertensive and normotensive individuals. It is well known that in this type of study the follow-up duration must be extended to several years. As a consequence, normotensives who did not develop any disease after several years of observation may be less stimulated to continue the study while hypertensives, that most likely develop comorbid conditions, can be more motivated to continue the study participation. Thus, loss to follow-up bias may distort the true risk of renal insufficiency in hypertensive versus normotensive individuals.

4) 4) Singh AK, Szczech L, Tang KL, Barnhart H, Sapp S, Wolfson M, Reddan D; CHOIR InvestigatorsSingh AK, Szczech L, Tang KL, Barnhart H, Sapp S, Wolfson M, Reddan D; CHOIR Investigators . Correction of anemia . Correction of anemia with epoetin alfa in chronic kidney disease. N Engl J Med 2006; 355:2085-2098.with epoetin alfa in chronic kidney disease. N Engl J Med 2006; 355:2085-2098.

SELECTION BIAS (OR BERKSONIAN BIAS)SELECTION BIAS (OR BERKSONIAN BIAS)

Page 10: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

Publication biasPublication bias

This bias is generally considered as a selection bias. Indeed, the acceptance of the validity of published findings as applied to a given reference population is conditional not only on the fact that each published study is unbiased but also that published studies constitute an unbiased sample of all studies performed. When these assumptions are not met, a literature review based on either meta-analytic or conventional narrative approaches will give a distorted view of the exposure-outcome association of interest.Publication bias is generated in the selection process of the information that is eventually published. Several factors influence publication, the most important being study size and design, quality, funding and prestige (5).

5) 5) Fouque DFouque D. Producing systematic reviews of best quality: a prerequisite for evidence-based nephrology. J Nephrol 1999; 12: . Producing systematic reviews of best quality: a prerequisite for evidence-based nephrology. J Nephrol 1999; 12: 314-317.314-317.6) 6) Fried LF, Orchard TJ, Kasiske BL for the lipids and renal disease progression meta-analysis study group. Fried LF, Orchard TJ, Kasiske BL for the lipids and renal disease progression meta-analysis study group. Kidney Int Kidney Int 2001; 59: 260-269.2001; 59: 260-269.

Kasiske and al. collated 13 studies on lipid lowering and renal outcomes in patients with chronic kidney disease (6). In this systematic review, only 2 studies out of 11 (i.e. 18%) reported a negative effect of treatment on albuminuria or proteinuria. A possible explanation for this phenomenon is that journal editors might have been biased in accepting studies reporting a negative effect of treatment or, alternatively, that authors of negative studies might have not submitted them because of the low probability for these studies to be accepted.

SELECTION BIAS (OR BERKSONIAN BIAS)SELECTION BIAS (OR BERKSONIAN BIAS)

Page 11: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

Publication biasPublication biasThe simplest and most commonly used method to detect publication bias is the funnel plot (7-8). A funnel plot is a graph where the effect of a given treatment of each trials is plotted against some measure of its size, such as the precision, the standard error or the overall sample size. These plots are referred to as funnel plots because they should be shaped like a funnel if no publication bias is present. This shape is expected because the estimate of the effect of a treatment has a larger variability in smaller studies. Since smaller and negative studies are less likely to be published, trials in the bottom left hand corner of the graph are often omitted, creating a degree of asymmetry in the funnel.

7) 7) Begg CBBegg CB. Publication bias. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, eds. The handbook of research synthesis. New York: Russell Sage . Publication bias. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, eds. The handbook of research synthesis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; pp. 399-409, 1994.Foundation; pp. 399-409, 1994.8) 8) Sterne JA, Egger M, Smith GD.Sterne JA, Egger M, Smith GD. Systematic reviews in health care: Investigating and dealing with publication and other Systematic reviews in health care: Investigating and dealing with publication and other biases in meta-analysis. BMJ 2001; 323:101-105.biases in meta-analysis. BMJ 2001; 323:101-105.

SELECTION BIAS (OR BERKSONIAN BIAS)SELECTION BIAS (OR BERKSONIAN BIAS)

Page 12: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

Publication biasPublication bias

In the figure we report a hypothetical example of publication bias. In the left part of figure the shape of the plot suggests that smaller studies indicating a worse outcome with the treatment being tested, might have been not reported (just one study where treatment produced worse outcome). In the right part of figure there are smaller studies showing either beneficial or no beneficial effects of the treatment. In this case the plot is appropriately funnel-shaped, making publication bias less likely.

SELECTION BIAS (OR BERKSONIAN BIAS)SELECTION BIAS (OR BERKSONIAN BIAS)

Page 13: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

INFORMATION BIASINFORMATION BIAS

Information bias occurs during data collection. There are two (main) types of Information bias occurs during data collection. There are two (main) types of information bias: misclassification bias and lead time bias. information bias: misclassification bias and lead time bias.

Misclassification bias originates when the process to detect the exposure status (exposure identification bias) and/or the outcome (outcome identification bias) is imperfect, i.e. exposed/diseased individuals are classified as non-exposed/non diseased and vice-versa. A potential source of misclassification can be the use of an inaccurate instrumentation, such as using only one size blood pressure cuff to take measurements on both lean and obese adults. As an effect of the inappropriate size of cuff we can classify a normotensive individual as hypertensive and vice-versa. Misclassification can be nondifferential or differential. In the example below we refer to misclassification of exposure.

MisclassificationMisclassification

Page 14: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

INFORMATION BIASINFORMATION BIAS

Non differential misclassification occurs when the misclassification of exposure is Non differential misclassification occurs when the misclassification of exposure is independent of disease status, i.e. it is the same in diseased individuals (cases) and independent of disease status, i.e. it is the same in diseased individuals (cases) and non-diseased individuals (controls). non-diseased individuals (controls).

Consider a hypothetical case-control study investigating the association between Consider a hypothetical case-control study investigating the association between hypertension and renal dysfunction. hypertension and renal dysfunction.

With renal dysfunction(cases)

With normal renal function (controls)

Hypertensives 40 10

Normotensives 40 50

OR= (40/40)/(10/50)=5.00OR= (40/40)/(10/50)=5.00

The OR indicates that the odds of exposure to hypertension are 5.00 times higher The OR indicates that the odds of exposure to hypertension are 5.00 times higher in cases than in controls and, in the absence of misclassification, we consider this in cases than in controls and, in the absence of misclassification, we consider this odds ratio as the true OR.odds ratio as the true OR.

Nondifferential misclassificationNondifferential misclassification

Page 15: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

INFORMATION BIASINFORMATION BIASNow, we can hypothesize a situation in which the misclassification of the exposure is equally Now, we can hypothesize a situation in which the misclassification of the exposure is equally frequent in cases and controls, i.e. in 30% of hypertensives with renal dysfunction (n=12) and frequent in cases and controls, i.e. in 30% of hypertensives with renal dysfunction (n=12) and in 30% of hypertensives with normal renal function (n=3). Since the misclassification of the in 30% of hypertensives with normal renal function (n=3). Since the misclassification of the exposure is identical in cases and controls it is said “nondifferential”. This new situation is exposure is identical in cases and controls it is said “nondifferential”. This new situation is illustrated belowillustrated below::

With renal dysfunction(cases)

With normal renal function (controls)

Hypertensives 40-12=28 10-3=7

Normotensives 40+12=52 50+3=53

OR: (28/52)/(7/53)=4.10OR: (28/52)/(7/53)=4.10

This OR is lower than the “true” OR (4.10 instead of 5.00). In fact, nondifferential This OR is lower than the “true” OR (4.10 instead of 5.00). In fact, nondifferential misclassification of exposure always leads to an underestimation of the strength of the misclassification of exposure always leads to an underestimation of the strength of the association between the exposure and the disease, i.e. the RR and the OR tend toward 1. association between the exposure and the disease, i.e. the RR and the OR tend toward 1.

Page 16: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

Differential misclassification occurs when the misclassification of exposure differs Differential misclassification occurs when the misclassification of exposure differs between cases and controls. For example, we consider that there is a 30% between cases and controls. For example, we consider that there is a 30% exposure misclassification only in controls: exposure misclassification only in controls:

INFORMATION BIASINFORMATION BIAS

With renal dysfunction(cases)

With normal renal function (controls)

Hypertensives 40 10-3=7

Normotensives 40 50+3=53

Odds ratio: (40/40)/(7/53)=7.58Odds ratio: (40/40)/(7/53)=7.58

Here, differential misclassification leads to an overestimation of the strength of the Here, differential misclassification leads to an overestimation of the strength of the association between exposure and disease. By contrast, if misclassification occurs in association between exposure and disease. By contrast, if misclassification occurs in cases but not in controls the resulting OR leads to an underestimation of the strength cases but not in controls the resulting OR leads to an underestimation of the strength of the same association. In summary, differential misclassification may either increase of the same association. In summary, differential misclassification may either increase or decrease the strength of reported associations depending on the direction of the or decrease the strength of reported associations depending on the direction of the misclassification. Differential misclassification can also occur in cohort studies if misclassification. Differential misclassification can also occur in cohort studies if exposed and unexposed individuals are misclassified. exposed and unexposed individuals are misclassified.

Differential misclassificationDifferential misclassification

Page 17: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

INFORMATION BIASINFORMATION BIAS

TheThe most common biases producing misclassification are recall bias, most common biases producing misclassification are recall bias, interviewer bias, observer bias and regression dilution bias. interviewer bias, observer bias and regression dilution bias.

Recall biasRecall biasRecall bias results from imprecise memory of past exposure and it is of particular concern in Recall bias results from imprecise memory of past exposure and it is of particular concern in case-control studies. For example, a patient with glomerulonephritis may have gone through case-control studies. For example, a patient with glomerulonephritis may have gone through all his exposures in his head once the diagnosis was made to try to understand “Why me?”. all his exposures in his head once the diagnosis was made to try to understand “Why me?”. An individual without illness might not have that memory due to limited reason to stretch to An individual without illness might not have that memory due to limited reason to stretch to recall exposures. Methods used to prevent recall bias include: a) verification of responses recall exposures. Methods used to prevent recall bias include: a) verification of responses from study individuals by using hospital records or other reliable sources of clinical from study individuals by using hospital records or other reliable sources of clinical information; b) use of diseased controls. Patients (cases) are much more prone to remember information; b) use of diseased controls. Patients (cases) are much more prone to remember relevant exposures than healthy controls. Therefore, a control group composed by individuals relevant exposures than healthy controls. Therefore, a control group composed by individuals affected by a disease different from that of cases can be used to introduce a similar bias also affected by a disease different from that of cases can be used to introduce a similar bias also in the odds of exposure of controls; c) use of objective markers of exposure. in the odds of exposure of controls; c) use of objective markers of exposure.

For example in a hypothetical case-control study investigating the association between exposure to hydrocarbons and the risk of glomerulonephritis we can estimate exposure to hydrocarbon by measuring metabolites of this compound in biological samples.

Page 18: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

INFORMATION BIASINFORMATION BIAS

Interviewer biasInterviewer biasThis is the tendency of the interviewer to obtain answers that support preconceived notions. This is the tendency of the interviewer to obtain answers that support preconceived notions. Interviewer bias may happen as a consequence of trying to “clarify” questions when such Interviewer bias may happen as a consequence of trying to “clarify” questions when such clarifications are not part of the study protocol or when certain words are more emphasized clarifications are not part of the study protocol or when certain words are more emphasized during the interview of cases but not of controls (or vice-versa). For example an interviewer during the interview of cases but not of controls (or vice-versa). For example an interviewer testing the hypothesis that hydrocarbon exposure may cause glomerulonephritis may adopt a testing the hypothesis that hydrocarbon exposure may cause glomerulonephritis may adopt a more inquisitive attitude in cases than in controls thus making the ascertainment of exposure more inquisitive attitude in cases than in controls thus making the ascertainment of exposure more likely in diseased than in non diseased individuals. To avoid interviewer bias we should more likely in diseased than in non diseased individuals. To avoid interviewer bias we should carefully standardize the interview and use an identical approach in cases and controls. carefully standardize the interview and use an identical approach in cases and controls. Furthermore, the interviewer should be blinded to the case-control status. Furthermore, the interviewer should be blinded to the case-control status.

Observer biasObserver biasKnowledge of exposure status by the outcome assessor may influence the assessment Knowledge of exposure status by the outcome assessor may influence the assessment process and therefore produce biased results. An obvious example is the assignment of a process and therefore produce biased results. An obvious example is the assignment of a kidney biopsy specimen to a diagnosis of hypertensive nephropathy where the pathologist kidney biopsy specimen to a diagnosis of hypertensive nephropathy where the pathologist knows that the patient has a long history of hypertension. Possible remedies to observer bias knows that the patient has a long history of hypertension. Possible remedies to observer bias are: 1) blinding the outcome assessor to exposure status; 2) when applicable, labelling the are: 1) blinding the outcome assessor to exposure status; 2) when applicable, labelling the outcome as “possible”, “probable” and “definite”. Observer bias should be suspected if results outcome as “possible”, “probable” and “definite”. Observer bias should be suspected if results emerge in the “possible” category only; 3) involving multiple outcome assessors. emerge in the “possible” category only; 3) involving multiple outcome assessors.

Page 19: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

INFORMATION BIASINFORMATION BIAS

Regression dilution biasRegression dilution biasIt is well known that a variable that shows an extreme value It is well known that a variable that shows an extreme value on its first measurement will tend to be less extreme on on its first measurement will tend to be less extreme on subsequent assessments (‘regression to the mean’ subsequent assessments (‘regression to the mean’ phenomenon). The regression dilution bias is related to phenomenon). The regression dilution bias is related to regression to the mean and originates in longitudinal studies regression to the mean and originates in longitudinal studies investigating the association between baseline investigating the association between baseline measurements of a continuous variable and the risk of a measurements of a continuous variable and the risk of a given outcomegiven outcome..

Page 20: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

As a consequence of the “regression to the mean”, individuals in the top category of blood pressure level frequently show higher systolic pressure at the first visit than at the second, while individuals in the bottom category have a systolic pressure at the first visit that is somewhat lower than that at the second visit: in other words many values converge (regress) toward the mean (see figure). Thus, if patients would be classified as hypertensive and normotensive on the basis of blood pressure measurement at the first visit we had have a misclassification of exposure (normotensives erroneously classified as hypertensives and vice-versa).

A classical example of this bias is the association between systolic pressure and the risk of myocardial infarction. During the baseline assessment of systolic pressure we have two sources of variations: variations in the measurement of arterial pressure among individuals and deviations between the baseline and the usual arterial pressure level within each individual. Regression to the mean refers to the noise introduced by within individual variation. It is the tendency for high values of continuous variables (like blood pressure) measured at a given time point to decrease and for low values to increase, when repeated measurements are performed.

Regression dilution bias (Example 1)Regression dilution bias (Example 1)

Page 21: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

As further example we can consider a sample of individuals where everyone usually has a creatinine of 0.9 mg/dL. However, at this particular moment of blood sampling some values were higher due to a steak from last night’s dinner or volume depletion, whereas others were lower due to pregnancy or steroid use for muscle building. If we were to take only those individuals with creatinine > 1 mg/dL and follow them, they will all fall back to their true baseline values of 0.9 mg/dL eventually and we would erroneously claim that their kidney function improved. The regression dilution bias always produces an underestimation of the true effect. To avoid the regression dilution bias the classification of patients should be based on more than one blood pressure measurement.

Regression dilution bias (Example 2)Regression dilution bias (Example 2)

Page 22: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

INFORMATION BIASINFORMATION BIAS

Lead time biasLead time bias

Another important information bias which may occur Another important information bias which may occur in prospective studies aimed at evaluating the efficacy in prospective studies aimed at evaluating the efficacy of screening is the lead time bias. This is the added of screening is the lead time bias. This is the added time of illness attributable to the fact that we apply to time of illness attributable to the fact that we apply to selected patients different criteria for the diagnosis of selected patients different criteria for the diagnosis of the disease (i.e. diagnosis made in the latency period the disease (i.e. diagnosis made in the latency period or in early phase versus diagnosis made at an or in early phase versus diagnosis made at an advanced stage).advanced stage).

Page 23: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

INFORMATION BIASINFORMATION BIAS

Lead time bias (Example 1)Lead time bias (Example 1)

A subject gets diseased after a given time and the disease is diagnosed because of symptoms. The disease is a serious one and the patient dies quite soon after diagnosis. The dead person was young and this causes sorrow and concern in the community where he/she lived. Experience with this first case prompts a screening program to detect the disease at an early, pre-clinical stage. In the second subject disease is diagnosed during the screening program at an asymptomatic stage. The second patient receives some form of treatment but the disease progresses until death. The survival from diagnosis in this case is much longer than in the first case and we may be tempted to conclude that early diagnosis was useful. In fact this is so because we applied different criteria for the detection of the disease, i.e. we diagnosed it at a symptomatic phase in the first patient and at a pre-clinical, asymptomatic phase in the second patient. It is therefore possible that had we diagnosed the disease at a pre-clinical stage in both patients the survival might have been identical. Thus, if we are going to assess the usefulness of a screening program we should take into proper account the “lead time”.

Page 24: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

INFORMATION BIASINFORMATION BIAS

Lead time biasLead time bias

Lead time bias may be a problem in studies examining the Lead time bias may be a problem in studies examining the effect of early referral on the outcome of chronic kidney disease effect of early referral on the outcome of chronic kidney disease (CKD) ((CKD) (99). If we do not appropriately account for the phase of ). If we do not appropriately account for the phase of disease when CKD is diagnosed we may overestimate the disease when CKD is diagnosed we may overestimate the beneficial effect of early referral.beneficial effect of early referral. To avoid the lead time bias we can calculate the risk (or rate) To avoid the lead time bias we can calculate the risk (or rate) of mortality in all screened and control individuals rather than the of mortality in all screened and control individuals rather than the cumulative probability of survival from diagnosis in cases.cumulative probability of survival from diagnosis in cases.

9) 9) Korevaar JC, Jansen MA, Dekker FW, et al.Korevaar JC, Jansen MA, Dekker FW, et al. Netherlands Cooperative Study on the Adequacy of Dialysis Study Group. Netherlands Cooperative Study on the Adequacy of Dialysis Study Group. When to initiate dialyses: effect of proposed US guidelines on survival. Lancet 2001; 358:1046–1050. When to initiate dialyses: effect of proposed US guidelines on survival. Lancet 2001; 358:1046–1050.

Page 25: Bias in clinical research Giovanni Tripepi, Kitty J. Jager 1, Friedo W. Dekker 1,2, Christoph Wanner 3, Carmine Zoccali CNR-IBIM, Clinical Epidemiology.

SUMMARYSUMMARY

Bias appears a pervasive problem in clinical research. Bias appears a pervasive problem in clinical research. Bias can be prevented at two levels: 1) by choosing the Bias can be prevented at two levels: 1) by choosing the appropriate study design for addressing the study appropriate study design for addressing the study hypothesis and 2) by carefully establishing the procedures hypothesis and 2) by carefully establishing the procedures of data handling and the definitions of exposures and of data handling and the definitions of exposures and outcomes. outcomes. Suspecting bias is an important aspect of critical Suspecting bias is an important aspect of critical appraisal and this attitude is precious to properly appraisal and this attitude is precious to properly evaluate clinical and epidemiological studies.evaluate clinical and epidemiological studies.


Recommended