+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category...

Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category...

Date post: 04-Jun-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 1 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
12
Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827 Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category implicitly? Andrea Bozoki a , Murray Grossman b , Edward E. Smith c,a Michigan State University, MI, USA b University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA c Department of Psychology, 402B Schermerhorn Hall, Columbia University, 1190 Amsterdam Avenue, New York, NY 10027, USA Accepted 1 August 2005 Available online 14 October 2005 Abstract Can a person with a damaged medial-temporal lobe learn a category implicitly? To address this question, we compared the performance of participants with mild Alzheimer’s disease (AD) to that of age-matched controls in a standard implicit learning task. In this task, participants were first presented a series of objects, then told the objects formed a category, and then had to categorize a long sequence of test items [Knowlton B. J., Squire L. R. (1993). The learning of categories: parallel brain systems for item memory and category knowledge. Science, 262, 1747–1749]. We tested the hypotheses that: (1) both Control and AD participants would show evidence for implicit learning after the unwanted contribution of learning during test is removed; (2) the degree of implicit learning is the same for AD and Control participants; (3) training with exemplars that are highly similar to an unseen prototype will lead to better implicit category learning than training with exemplars that are less similar to a prototype. With respect to the first hypothesis, we found that both AD and Control participants performed better on tests of implicit learning than could be attributed to just learning on test trials. We found no clear means for evaluating our second hypothesis, and argue that comparisons of the degree of implicit learning between patient and control groups in this paradigm are confounded by the contribution of other memory systems. In line with the third hypothesis, only training with similar exemplars resulted in significant implicit category learning for AD participants. © 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s disease 1. Introduction 1.1. Implicit learning and the medial-temporal lobe Learning to classify items into categories according to com- mon or overlapping features is a fundamental cognitive task. Exposure to members of a category facilitates later categoriza- tion of similar but novel instances of that category. Presumably, this is one of the means by which children learn about the world, and how adults refine and add to their knowledge of previously learned categories. Starting in the late 1970s, many studies of category acquisition demonstrated that learning often relied on the explicit memorization of category exemplars (e.g., Estes, 1994; Medin & Schaffer, 1978; Nosofsky, 1991). Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 212 854 1789. E-mail address: [email protected] (E.E. Smith). Such explicit memory however, may not be necessary for all forms of category learning. In a classic study, Knowlton and Squire (1993) presented both medial-temporal lobe amnesics and normal controls with a series of dot patterns; all the patterns were transformations of a prototype pattern, but during presen- tation nothing about a category was mentioned to either group of participants. After presentation, however, all participants were informed that the patterns they had just seen were instances of a category, and that they were to determine which of a sequence of test patterns also belonged to that category. Both amnesiac and Control participants performed this unexpected categorization task with above-chance accuracy, and remarkably the amnesi- acs were as accurate as the controls. The conclusion drawn was that both groups of participants had learned the category implic- itly rather than explicitly, where implicit memory is known not to rely on the medial-temporal lobe (e.g., Schacter, 1992). The idea of implicit category-learning was born, and added to the list of kinds of implicit learning. Since the initial experiment, sev- 0028-3932/$ – see front matter © 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.08.001
Transcript
Page 1: Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category ...ftd.med.upenn.edu/uploads/media_items/can-patients... · Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s

Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827

Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learna category implicitly?

Andrea Bozoki a, Murray Grossman b, Edward E. Smith c,∗a Michigan State University, MI, USA

b University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USAc Department of Psychology, 402B Schermerhorn Hall, Columbia University, 1190 Amsterdam Avenue, New York, NY 10027, USA

Accepted 1 August 2005Available online 14 October 2005

Abstract

Can a person with a damaged medial-temporal lobe learn a category implicitly? To address this question, we compared the performance ofparticipants with mild Alzheimer’s disease (AD) to that of age-matched controls in a standard implicit learning task. In this task, participants werefirst presented a series of objects, then told the objects formed a category, and then had to categorize a long sequence of test items [Knowlton B.JWlhWaot©

K

1

1

mEttalct1

0d

., Squire L. R. (1993). The learning of categories: parallel brain systems for item memory and category knowledge. Science, 262, 1747–1749].e tested the hypotheses that: (1) both Control and AD participants would show evidence for implicit learning after the unwanted contribution of

earning during test is removed; (2) the degree of implicit learning is the same for AD and Control participants; (3) training with exemplars that areighly similar to an unseen prototype will lead to better implicit category learning than training with exemplars that are less similar to a prototype.ith respect to the first hypothesis, we found that both AD and Control participants performed better on tests of implicit learning than could be

ttributed to just learning on test trials. We found no clear means for evaluating our second hypothesis, and argue that comparisons of the degreef implicit learning between patient and control groups in this paradigm are confounded by the contribution of other memory systems. In line withhe third hypothesis, only training with similar exemplars resulted in significant implicit category learning for AD participants.

2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

eywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s disease

. Introduction

.1. Implicit learning and the medial-temporal lobe

Learning to classify items into categories according to com-on or overlapping features is a fundamental cognitive task.xposure to members of a category facilitates later categoriza-

ion of similar but novel instances of that category. Presumably,his is one of the means by which children learn about the world,nd how adults refine and add to their knowledge of previouslyearned categories. Starting in the late 1970s, many studies ofategory acquisition demonstrated that learning often relied onhe explicit memorization of category exemplars (e.g., Estes,994; Medin & Schaffer, 1978; Nosofsky, 1991).

∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 212 854 1789.E-mail address: [email protected] (E.E. Smith).

Such explicit memory however, may not be necessary for allforms of category learning. In a classic study, Knowlton andSquire (1993) presented both medial-temporal lobe amnesicsand normal controls with a series of dot patterns; all the patternswere transformations of a prototype pattern, but during presen-tation nothing about a category was mentioned to either group ofparticipants. After presentation, however, all participants wereinformed that the patterns they had just seen were instances of acategory, and that they were to determine which of a sequence oftest patterns also belonged to that category. Both amnesiac andControl participants performed this unexpected categorizationtask with above-chance accuracy, and remarkably the amnesi-acs were as accurate as the controls. The conclusion drawn wasthat both groups of participants had learned the category implic-itly rather than explicitly, where implicit memory is known notto rely on the medial-temporal lobe (e.g., Schacter, 1992). Theidea of implicit category-learning was born, and added to the listof kinds of implicit learning. Since the initial experiment, sev-

028-3932/$ – see front matter © 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.oi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.08.001

Page 2: Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category ...ftd.med.upenn.edu/uploads/media_items/can-patients... · Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s

A. Bozoki et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827 817

eral studies have replicated the findings that medial-temporallobe amnesics perform normally on implicit category learning(e.g., Kolodny, 1994; Reed et al., 1999; Squire & Knowlton,1995—see Keri, 2003 for a review).

1.2. Strengthening the case for implicit learning

But the existence of implicit category learning has been chal-lenged by a number of related papers including Nosofsky andZaki (1998), Palmeri and Flanery (1999) and Zaki (2004). Allof these articles argue that results with medial-temporal lobepatients can be explained in terms of explicit and working-memory systems, without any appeal to an implicit system.While these papers offer a number of arguments, the most com-pelling is the Palmeri and Flanery (1999) demonstration thatthe results obtained in the implicit category learning paradigmmay be entirely due to working memory. These authors did notpresent any training stimuli, but told participants that such itemshad been presented subliminally, and then gave the participantsthe same kind of test – a sequence of members and non-members– that is used in neuropsychological studies of implicit learning.Remarkably, the participants scored above chance. This findingindicates that category learning can occur on test trials alone,likely mediated by working-memory mechanisms that detect andamalgamate similar patterns into a category representation. Thisin turn implies that above-chance performance on the standardtte

wbtahtctspiCsts

al

cpgcfinct

performance on the categorization test before there has been suf-ficient chance for working-memory-based learning to occur ontest trails. Accordingly, we measured categorization accuracyafter the first 10 test trials (as well as at the end of test trials).Again the main hypothesis is that Control and AD participantswill manifest implicit learning, and again a stronger hypothe-sis is that AD patients manifest normal implicit learning evenwhen this stringent criterion is used. The obvious problem withthis criterion, though, is that 10 trials may not provide sufficientstatistical power to detect a smallish effect.

What precludes amnesiacs from using an explicit, exemplarstrategy in categorization is damage to the medial-temporal lobe,particularly the hippocampal system. But surgery and encephali-tis are not the only kinds of neurological damage leading tofunctional impairment of this system with consequent memoryloss. Specifically, the defining feature of early Alzheimer’s dis-ease (AD) is impairment of explicit, hippocampally-mediatedmemory due to neuronal loss from amyloid deposits and neu-rofibrillary inclusions. Thus, this disease provides an alternatemodel system for testing implicit categorization in the absenceof an intact mechanism for explicit learning, and AD patientswere employed in the present study. If patients with AD canimplicitly learn a category it is almost certainly on the basisof extra-hippocampal circuitry, or at least involving the recruit-ment of additional brain regions to augment the working of thedamaged medial-temporal lobe.2

1

tta

Ao(svswaooaAiistn

s&Bt

est of “implicit learning” cannot be unequivocally attributedo implicit learning, and hence that the data do not offer muchvidence for category learning by implicit memory.

One main goal of the present paper is to test whether patientsith medial-temporal lobe damage learn a category implicitly ory means of working memory. Toward this end, we introducedwo stringent criteria of implicit category learning, criteria thatssess learning when the contribution of learning-during-testas been subtracted or minimized. According to the first cri-erion, one needs to a demonstrate that, in a standard implicitategory-learning condition, performance on test trials exceedshat obtained in a condition in which no training stimuli are pre-ented (see Keri, 2003, for a similar point). Accordingly, in theresent experiment AD patients and normal controls were testedn both conditions. The main hypothesis of interest is that bothontrol and AD participants manifest implicit learning when a

tringent criterion of learning is used. A stronger hypothesis ishat AD patients manifest normal implicit learning even when atringent criterion is used.1

In addition to comparing categorization performance withnd without training items, another stringent test of implicitearning is to determine whether participants show above-chance

1 Knowlton and Squire (1993) and Reed et al. (1999) did try to assess theontribution of learning-during-test by instructing a separate group of controlarticipants to “imagine” that a set of training trials had been presented, and theniving them a standard categorization test. Neither study found any evidence forategory learning in the absence of training items. Presumably these failures tond learning-during-test were due to the fact that the “imagine” instructions didot sufficiently convince the participants that they could learn the category, inontrast to the Palmeri & Flanery cover story about subliminal presentation ofraining items (see Palmeri & Flanery, 1999).

.3. Effects of similarity of training items

A second goal of the present paper is to test the hypothesishat implicit memory makes a contribution to category learningo the extent that the members of a category are similar to onenother.

There have been relatively few categorization studies withD patients, and fewer still that examine dot-pattern learningr other forms of non-semantic categorization. But Keri et al.1999, 2001) examined just this type of learning in two succes-ive papers. In both papers, their task required participants toiew sequences of dot patterns, all of which were created byystematic distortions of a prototype dot pattern; participantsere then told that the patterns they had seen all belonged tocategory, and that they were to indicate which of a sequencef new test patterns also belonged to this category. The resultsf Keri et al. (1999) suggested implicit category learning (asssessed by the standard lenient criterion) was impaired inD patients; in particular, patients’ categorization of prototype

tems during test was notably impaired. Their follow-up study,n a larger cohort of 72 individuals with AD, showed “relativelypared” categorization (impairment was demonstrated only inhe sub-group with moderately severe disease). Thus, althoughumerous researchers have found that amnesic patients perform

2 Patients with early Alzheimer’s disease show some deficits in other memoryystems as well, including working-memory and semantic-memory (e.g., Perry

Hodges, 1999). Still the impairment of explicit memory is the most profound.ut the impairment in working memory might reduce the ability of these patients

o learn a category during test.

Page 3: Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category ...ftd.med.upenn.edu/uploads/media_items/can-patients... · Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s

818 A. Bozoki et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827

relatively normally on this task, the results for individuals withAD are less clear.

Importantly, in the Keri et al. studies participants were pre-sented with relatively dissimilar training stimuli during the train-ing phase; that is, training consisted of either a mixture of “lowdistortion” and “high distortion” displacements of a (unseen)prototype dot pattern (1999 study), or all “high distortion” stim-uli (2001 study), resulting in a wide range of exemplars. Theauthors describe their system for producing these stimuli as fol-lows:

“An initial pattern of dots was used as a prototype of thecategory. During stimulus preparation, virtual shells with dif-ferent size were constructed around the dots of the prototype.Distortions were generated by placing each dot in one of theshells with a certain probability. [. . .] In the case of high dis-tortions, dots were placed in the outer shells with a higherprobability.”

Keri et al.’s use of relatively dissimilar training stimulimay have impeded the development of an implicit prototype(which may be the mechanisms that underly implicit categorylearning—see Smith & Jonides, 2000). Individuals may betterlearn an underlying prototype if individual exemplars during thelearning phase are highly similar to one another. Thus, a secondpurpose of the present study was to determine if AD (and Con-trol) participants are more likely to implicitly learn a categorywd

1

nvid1tanttn

2

2

wrNgtan4

m

(memory-enhancing drugs for AD) were permitted for AD participants, andselective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (a popular and non-sedating class ofanti-depressant medications) were permitted for all participants. All participantswere screened for residual depression with the Geriatric Depression Scale andexcluded for GDS > 11.

Each participant was administered a battery of neuropsychological tests:Barona Estimated Demographic IQ (Barona, Reynolds, & Chastain, 1984);Folstein Mini Mental Status Exam (MMSE) (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh,1975) to assess overall cognitive function; Non-verbal Continuous PerformanceTest (CPT X and CPT XOX) to assess simple attention and working memory(Glosser & Goodglass, 1990); nine-item California Verbal Learning test (CVLT)to assess verbal explicit memory (Libon et al., 1996); Rey-Osterreith ComplexFig. (ROCF) to assess visuospatial explicit memory (Spreen & Strauss, 1998);Pyramid-Palm Trees test (PPT) to assess visual object processing (Howard &Patterson, 1992 Thames Valley Test Company, Bury St. Edmonds, UK); theBenton Visual Form Discrimination test (VFD) to assess visuospatial process-ing (Benton et al., 1994). Healthy older participants scoring below 25 on theMMSE, or below the 10th percentile (corrected for age and education) on anycomponent test, were excluded from further consideration. We chose these testsin an effort to examine several areas of cognitive processing relevant to acquiringa novel category, including explicit long-term memory and working memory.

Table 1 provides a comparison of the AD and Control participants on a num-ber of demographic variables. Despite our efforts to match participants accordingto age, there was a marginally significant difference in that variable, as well as inlevel of education, favoring Controls. As expected, there was a highly significantdifference in mean MMSE scores, with the AD patients performing substantiallylower than the Controls. All t-values are given, along with significance levels inTable 1.

2.2. Materials

(docteettvsGm1

2

2

raTpt

TC

SAEEM

hen trained on highly similar exemplars rather than relativelyissimilar exemplars.

.4. Materials

Instead of dot patterns, we use as stimuli a set of imagi-ary animals that vary on 1 or more of 10 features (spottedersus striped body, snout versus trunk, etc.). With dot patterns,t is unclear what exactly is learned (a prototypical pattern, aescription of parts of the pattern, etc.—see Smith & Medin,981, Chapter 6), but with these imaginary animals there is lit-le doubt that what is learned are feature representations of thenimals. Further, Reed et al. (1999) have replicated the basiceuropsychological findings with these imaginary animals, ashey showed that amnesiacs performed as well as normal par-icipants on the categorization test, but substantially poorer thanormals on a test of episodic memory of the training items.

. Methods

.1. Participant selection and characterization

A total of 86 participants were recruited; 44 participants were diagnosedith mild to moderate clinically probable AD (Folstein MMSE > 12), and were

ecruited through the Michigan Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center. Each metINCDS-ADRDA criteria for the diagnosis (McKhann et al., 1984). A controlroup of 42 healthy adult volunteers, “Controls,” who were age and sex matchedo the AD subjects, were recruited from the Ann Arbor, MI community via paiddvertisements. Four AD and two Control participants were later excluded forot completing the task or the neuropsychological testing, leaving 40 AD and0 Control participants in the study.

All participants were screened for a history of psychosis, neurological co-orbidities, and CNS-acting medications. However, cholinesterase inhibitors

AD participants were randomly assigned to receive High-Similarity trainingHS), Low-Similarity training (LS), or No Training (NT). Controls were ran-omly assigned to one of these conditions after age and sex matching. A totalf 10 AD and 10 Control participants were assigned to each of the HS and LSonditions, and twice that many were assigned to the NT condition (becausewo different test lists were used in this condition—see below). Stimuli for cat-gorization consisted of a set of novel cartoon animals used earlier by Reedt al. (1999) (see Fig. 1). To form the set, a prototype animal was created andhen transformed on 1 or more of 10 attributes in a binary fashion – either the pro-otypic value (or feature) of the attribute was maintained or the non-prototypicalue was substituted – for a total of 100 possible stimuli. The experiment con-isted of three phases, each presented on a Macintosh PowerPC or Powerbook3 computer: categorization training, categorization testing, and recognitionemory. All cartoon animal images were approximately 2 in.2, presented on a

5 in. monitor.

.3. Conditions

.3.1. High versus low similarity training conditionsDuring the training phase, 20 animals were each presented twice in pseudo-

andom order for a total of 40 trials. Each animal was presented for 3 s with500 ms inter-stimulus interval during which a fixation cross was presented.

otal duration of training was 140 s. In the HS condition, each of the 40 animalsresented had either 8 or 9 features in common with the (unseen) prototype. Inhe LS condition, each of the 40 animals had 6 or 7 features in common with

able 1haracterization of AD and Control participants

AD (n = 40) Controls (n = 40) t-Value p-Value

ex distribution 60% F 65% F 0.46 nsge 73.9 ± 7.9 70.5 ± 6.9 2.00 0.05ducation 14.7 ± 3.6 16.1 ± 2.9 2.01 0.05FSIQ 111.0 ± 8.0 1114.0 ± 7.0 1.84 nsMSE 20.6 ± 3.5 28.4 ± 1.5 12.96 <0.0001

Page 4: Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category ...ftd.med.upenn.edu/uploads/media_items/can-patients... · Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s

A. Bozoki et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827 819

Fig. 1. Examples of stimuli used in the categorization experiment. Prototype “Peggle,” example of a high-similarity (eight prototypic features) item, a low-similarity(six prototypic features) item, and a non-Peggle item (0 prototypic features).

the (unseen) prototype. Instructions to participants were given on the screen:

You will see a bunch of animal cartoons now. Look at each animal as itappears on the screen and think about its appearance.

2.3.2. No Training conditionThere was no training phase in the NT condition; that is, no animal presen-

tations occurred prior to test. Instead, after participants were administered theCPT (part of the neuropsychological battery), and prior to beginning the cate-gorization test, they were given the following instructions (based on Palmeri &Flanery, 1999):

During the previous task–images of imaginary animals were quickly flashedon the computer screen so as to be perceived subliminally (without consciousawareness). All of these animals belonged to a single category in the samesense that if a series of dogs had been presented, they would all belong tothe category “dog”. While you probably have no conscious recollection ofthe images, we would like you to try as hard as possible to figure out whichof the following are members of the same category which was displayedearlier and which are not. About half of the animals you are about to see aremembers of this category and about half are not. Press the ‘Y’ button if youthink the animal is a category member, and the ‘N’ button if you think it isnot. Try to go with your first impression.

2.3.3. Categorization testAll conditions culminated with a test phase. This phase consisted of 65 self-

paced test item presentations, given 2–5 min after the completion of training (forHS and LS conditions). The test animals ran the gamut from 0 to 10 features incommon with the prototype, presented in a pseudo-randomized order that wasthe same for all participants within a training group. For the HS group, therew1eiatosd

These two test lists were also used in the NT condition. We used different testlists for HS and LS conditions, and for the NT condition, because: (1) we wantedto include more tests of high-similarity items in the HS condition (namely oldones as well as new ones), and more tests of low-similarity items in the LScondition (old as well as new ones); (2) when comparing the HS (LS) and NTcondition, we wanted the test lists to be identical. (As it turned out, though, therewas no difference between the old and new test items.) Stimuli were self-paced,and participants in the HS and LS conditions were instructed as follows (theinstructions for the NT condition have already been described):

All of the animals you just saw belong to the category “Peggles”. You willnow see a new bunch of animals. About half will be Peggles and about halfwill not. Press the ‘Y’ button if you think the animal is a Peggle, and the ‘N’button if you think it is not. Try to go with your first impression.3

Categorization data were scored according to how often each type of test animalwas endorsed—judged to be a member of the category Peggle. Categorizationaccuracy (CatAcc) was calculated as the percentage of all trials in which theparticipant endorsed an item with six or more features in common with theprototype, or rejected an item with less than five prototype features. (Items withexactly five prototype features were dropped from this particular calculation asit was felt to represent an indeterminate condition.) Six features was chosen asthe cutoff for category membership, to maintain consistency with our training,during which low-similarity items were defined as category members and hadeither six or seven features in common with the prototype. We also calculated anaccuracy score based on just the first 10 trials (CatAcc10) for each participant,using the same criteria as those for CatAcc. Recall that this score could be usedas a second stringent criterion for implicit category learning.

Another measure of category learning was the slope of the function relat-ing endorsement rate to number of features in common with the prototype(typicality)—the steeper the slope, the better the category has been learned.

adScs3

ere: 10 stimuli (5 new and 5 old) with 9 features in common with the prototype,0 stimuli (5 new and 5 old) with 8 features in common with the prototype; 5 forach of the other animal types (e.g., 6 features in common with the prototype),ncluding 5 presentations of the prototype itself and 5 of the anti-prototype (thenimal that has zero features in common with the prototype). For the LS group,he pattern was the same, except that now there were 10 stimuli (5 new and 5ld) with items that had 6 and 7 features in common with the prototype, and 5timuli of each of the other animal types. The order of item presentation alsoiffered between HS and LS groups.

3 Because different AD patients were tested in the NT conditions, as wells in the HS and LS conditions, it is important to establish that there were noemographic or cognitive differences between these four groups of patients.eparate analysis of variance were performed on each of the five demographic-ognitive measures listed in Table 1. Not one of the five measure showed aignificant difference across patient groups (the maximum F achieved was F (3,6) = 1.44, ns).

Page 5: Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category ...ftd.med.upenn.edu/uploads/media_items/can-patients... · Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s

820 A. Bozoki et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827

We defined endorsement rate (CatEnd) as the percentage of trials at a givenlevel of typicality – e.g., seven features in common with the prototype – that anitem was designated as a category member. In order to improve our power todetect differences between groups, we collapsed each pair of successive typi-cality levels – e.g., 6 or 7 features in common with the prototype – into a singlelevel, leaving the prototype (all 10 features in common with the prototype) asits own level. We therefore ended up with six levels of typicality.

2.3.4. Recognition testAs a check on their ability to explicitly recognize individual exemplar ani-

mals, participants in the HS and LS conditions underwent a recognition task,immediately after concluding the categorization task. For expediency, the NTgroups did not perform this task. In the recognition task, a second set of car-toon animal exemplars, not previously seen by the participants, were presentedduring a study phase. These images looked qualitatively different from the itemsused during the categorization task (that is, they were not just different exem-plars from the Peggle group of animals). However, other parameters were thesame as in the earlier categorization task (i.e., 10 binary attributes with a pro-totype consisting of a particular value of each attribute). A set of 20 animalswere presented twice each in pseudo-random order for a total of 40 trials. Eachanimal was presented for 3 s with a 500 ms inter-stimulus interval during whicha fixation cross was presented. Total duration of presentation was 140 s. In theHS condition all animals presented had either eight or nine features in commonwith the (unseen) prototype; in the LS condition all animals had six or sevenfeatures in common with the prototype.

Prior to presentation, participants were instructed:

You will see a new bunch of animal cartoons now. Look at each animal as itappears on the screen and try to memorize its appearance. You will be testedon your ability to recognize them soon.

raa

m(wtrn

3

aed

3

twmmsos

Table 2Percentage of correct categorizations (CatAcc), with standard errors

AD (n = 10 per cell) Controls (n = 10 per cell)

High similarityTraining 73 ± 2.5 76 ± 3No Training 59 ± 4 71 ± 4Implicit learning 14 5

Low similarityTraining 65 ± 3 67 ± 3.5No Training 66 ± 3 71 ± 4Implicit learning 1 −4

is significantly greater than that for AD patients (t (18) = 2.53,p = 0.02). For the LS condition, the CatAcc10 scores for ADs andControls were 53% and 67%; the results for Controls is signif-icantly greater than chance (t (9) = 3.26, p < 0.01). For both HSand LS conditions, then, we have stringent evidence for implicitcategory learning, but only in Controls. There is no evidence forimplicit category learning in AD patients by this measure.4

Table 2 presents overall categorization accuracy – CatAccscores – and leads to a different conclusion. The scores arepresented for Control and AD participants in the HS, LS, andNT conditions (with scores for the NT condition being pre-sented separately for the two different test lists). Starting withthe results for the HS condition (top half of Table 2), notefirst that we have replicated the standard finding of compara-ble categorization accuracy for Control and Patient participants(76% and 73%, respectively). Of greater interest is our secondstringent measure of implicit learning, the difference betweenperformance with and without training items, HS–NT. Thereappears to be evidence for implicit learning, particularly in ADparticipants (HS–NT = 14%). These scores were submitted to atwo-factor analysis of variance (diagnosis—AD versus Control;condition—HS versus NT). There was a main effect of diagno-sis, Controls outperforming AD participants (F (1, 36) = 4.15,p = 0.05), but the training effect apparent in Table 2 failed toreach significance (F (1, 36) = 2.27, ns) as did the interactioneffect (F (1, 36) = 1.34, ns). In view of this, and given that thecwapfb

TfTtl

ocsp

Immediately following the presentations, participants were given an explicitecognition test. To keep procedures parallel to the categorization task, 55 newnd 10 previously shown items were presented during the test phase. Thesenimals were not given a category name. Participants were instructed:

You will now see some more animals. Some of these animals will be identicalto the ones you just saw but most of them will be new. Press the ‘Y’ buttonif you think the animal is one you just saw, and ‘N’ if you think it is new.

Because there was a tendency on the part of all participants to make theiremory judgments on the basis of features shared with the (unseen) prototype

that is, to respond as they did in the categorization task), recognition accuracyas assessed only by comparing new and old items with the same degree of

ypicality. That is, we compared accuracy of correct identification and correctejection of stimuli with, say, eight features in common with the prototype (5ew versus 5 old).

. Results and discussion

In what follows, we first discuss the results for over-ll accuracy of implicit category learning, next consider thendorsement-rate measure of category learning, and finallyescribe the results for the explicit memory task.

.1. Categorization accuracy

Consider the categorization accuracy for the initial 10 testrials—CatAcc10 scores. These scores provide an indication ofhether Control or AD participants in the HS and LS conditionsanifested any implicit learning before learning-during-test haduch time to manifest itself. For the HS condition, CatAcc10

cores for Controls and ADs are 79% and 60%, respectively;nly the result for Controls is significantly greater than a chancecore of 50% (p < 0.05). Furthermore, the score for Controls

ontrast between HS and NT is central to the present study,e tested for the training effect by directly comparing the HS

nd NT conditions for each group of participants. For the ADatients there is a significant difference (t (18) = 2.7, p < 0.02);or the Control participants the difference is in the right directionut is not significant (t (18) = 1.0, ns).

The results for the LS condition are in the bottom half ofable 2. Our stringent measure of LS–NT offers no evidenceor implicit category learning in either group of participants.his difference from the HS results is not due to a variation in

he NT conditions (see Table 2), as the two different test listsed to comparable results (for the AD patients, t (18) = 1.1, ns;

4 Another test for implicit category learning in AD patients is the comparisonf CatAcc10 scores between the HS condition (60%) and its comparable NTondition (49%). (This is a mixture of our two stringent criteria.) This compari-on is not significant (t (18) = 1.72, ns). Thus no test involving CatAcc10 scoresrovides evidence for implicit learning in AD patients.

Page 6: Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category ...ftd.med.upenn.edu/uploads/media_items/can-patients... · Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s

A. Bozoki et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827 821

for the Controls, t (18) = 0.00). Rather the difference betweenthe two sets of results in Table 2 is that HS training resultedin substantially greater learning than LS training. A two-wayANOVA – AD versus Control X type of training – was appliedto the CatAcc scores. There was a beneficial effect of type oftraining – HS better than LS (F (1, 36) = 7.13, p < 0.01) – but noeffect of group, as both AD and Control participants performedcomparably. Based on our second stringent criteria, then, thereis clear evidence for implicit category learning, but only whenthe training items are highly similar.

Thus, our first stringent test (CatAcc10 > 50%) provides evi-dence that Controls manifest implicit learning, whereas oursecond stringent test (CatAcc difference between HS and NT)provides evidence for implicit learning in AD participants. Whythis asymmetry in outcomes? There are two questions here. (1)Why did the AD patients not pass our first stringent criterion (atleast in the HS condition)? (2) Why did the Controls not showmore evidence for implicit learning by our second stringent cri-terion (at least in the HS condition)? With regard to question (1),one possibility is that the test has insufficient power to obtainsignificance for the AD patients (note that their CatAcc10 score,0.60, was in the right direction). A CatAcc10 score for a partic-ular condition was based on a total of only 100 observations (10trials × 10 participants), whereas a CatAcc score for a conditionwas based on 650 observations (65 trials × 10 participants). Inaddition, the CatAcc scores may have showed more evidencefptiWmbrbtaHH

3

adtr

TP

CCL

b

CatAcc, separately for Control and AD participants. It appearsthat both groups learned during test trials, as both groups showperformance increases with trials, and these increases are com-parable. An analysis of variance cannot be performed on thesedata because CatAcc and CatAcc10 scores are not independent.But a direct comparison of CatAcc to CatAcc10 shows signifi-cant effects for both AD participants (t (18) = 5.58; p = 0.001),and Control participants (t (18) = 5.89; p = 0.001). (Although theperformance increases are comparable for the two groups, thereis suggestive evidence that overall performance is better for Con-trols than AD participants—t (18) = 1.82, p < 0.10.)

3.3. Category endorsement

Our other categorization measure is category endorsement(CatEnd), the slope of the function relating the percentage oftimes an item is endorsed as a member of the category to thenumber of features it shares with the prototype. The functionsfor the AD and Control participants in the HS and NT condi-tions are shown in Fig. 2. Note that these functions provide moredetailed information than our CatAcc and CatAcc10 measures;the functions tell us whether categorization accuracy increasesmonotomically with the similarity of the test item to the cate-gory prototype, and how sensitively tuned categorization is tosimilarity (the steepness of the function). Category endorsementfunctions are routinely treated as the most sensitive measuresoPFl

tcoitugdgn(ftlfpdtil

d(fwC

or implicit learning than the CatAcc10 scores because the ADatients continued to learn implicitly over the course of the 65est trials (25 of the 65 test trials in the HS test list containedtems with at least 8 features in common with the prototype).

ith regard to question (2), our Controls may not have shownore evidence for implicit learning by our second stringent test

ecause of a “strategic” effect. Controls may have chosen toely less on learning-during-test in the HS than the NT conditionecause they had relatively little need of learning-during-test inhe HS condition—their implicit learning was sufficient. Yet were assuming “equal need” by subtracting the NT score from theS score. Hence, we may have “subtracted too much” from theS scores for Controls.

.2. Learning during test

Do both groups of participants show learning during test,nd if so, in comparable amounts? Table 3 presents the relevantata. The data are drawn from only NT trials (collapsed overhe HS and LS test lists, to increase power), and compare accu-acy for the first 10 trials, CatAcc10, to accuracy for all trials,

able 3ercentage correct categorizations, with standard errors: no-training

AD (n = 20 per cell) Controls (n = 20 per cell)

atAcc 62 ± 3* 71 ± 3*

atAcc10 54 ± 4 60 ± 4earning-during-test 8 11

* These entries differ from the corresponding No-Training entries in Table 2ecause they collapse over the data for the HS and LS test lists.

f implicit category learning (e.g., Knowlton & Squire, 1993;almeri & Flanery, 1999), and a comparison of the functions inig. 2 offers our most detailed stringent test for implicit category

earning.Consider first the contrast between HS and NT for AD par-

icipants in Fig. 2A. The results present support for implicitategory learning in AD patients. At every one of the six pointsn the x-axis, training led to better performance than no train-ng (remember: good performance for the points correspondingo 0–1 and 2–3 features requires not endorsing the item). Wesed regression analysis to calculate a best-fitting line for eachroup and condition, and then compared slopes with t-tests. Theifference between the slopes of the trained and untrained ADroups was significant (t (8) = 2.63; p < 0.05), though there waso such evidence in the equivalent comparison in the Controlst (8) = 1.18; ns). It is also worth noting that while the slopeor the AD participants in the NT condition leaves little doubthat the patients are capable of learning during test, overall theyearned less during test than did the Controls: the NT slopeor the Controls was significantly greater than that for the ADarticipants (t (8) = 5.72, p < 0.01). There was no comparableifference in slopes between Controls and AD participants forhe HS condition (t (8) = 0.67, ns), which indicates that theres no evidence for superiority of Controls over AD in implicitearning.

Fig. 3 compares endorsement rates in the HS and LS con-itions, separately for AD (Panel A) and Control participantsPanel B). The slope was steeper in the HS than the LS conditionor both AD and Control participants; the difference in slopesas significant for both AD patients (t (8) = 2.26, p < 0.05) andontrols (t (8) = 3.5, p < 0.01). However, there was no difference

Page 7: Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category ...ftd.med.upenn.edu/uploads/media_items/can-patients... · Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s

822 A. Bozoki et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827

Fig. 2. Endorsement ratings for test items (“Yes, it belongs to the category”) asa function of the typicality of the test item (i.e., the number of features the testitem shares with the prototype), separately for the HS and NT conditions. Thefunctions in 2A are for AD patients, the functions in 2B are for Controls.

in slopes between the two groups of participants, either in theHS or LS condition.5

3.4. Accuracy for episodic memory (recognition task)

Neither AD nor Control participants were significantly dif-ferent from chance (50%) in their ability to determine old fromnew test items. For the HS condition, recognition accuracieswere 39% and 52% for AD and Control participants, respec-

5 In keeping with our earlier finding that the CatAcc score for LS did notexceed that for the NT condition (no evidence of implicit category learning), theslope for the LS condition did not exceed that for the NT condition, for eitherAD or Control participants. In fact, for both groups of participants, the slope ofthe NT function was slightly steeper!

Fig. 3. Endorsement ratings for test items (“yes it belongs to the category”) asa function of the typicality of the test item (i.e., the number of features the testitem shares with the prototype), separately for the HS and LS conditions. Thefunctions in 3A are for AD participants, the functions in 3B are for Controlparticipants.

tively. For the LS condition, recognition accuracies were 36%and 44% for AD and Control participants, respectively. Whenadjusted for degree of similarity to the prototype, there was nodifference between old and new items, for either HS or LS par-ticipants. That is, there was no difference in accuracy for newversus old items at each level of typicality in which both newand old items were presented at test (six and seven features forLS; eight and nine features for HS). Rather, both groups of par-ticipants seemed to base their responses on the similarity of thetest item to the prototype (as in the categorization task). Theresulting floor effect for explicit memory makes further analysisof these data of little use.

To determine whether there was a dissociation betweenexplicit retrieval and the mechanisms subserving categorization,we examined the neuropsychological data for both verbal (CVLTtotal recall score) and visuospatial (Rey-Osterreith 5 min delay)episodic memory tasks, collected concurrently with the cate-gorization experiment (see Table 4). These data served as ourindicators of explicit memory. As might be expected, Controlsfar outperformed AD participants in both verbal and visuospatialepisodic memory function. There were also significant differ-ences in the other tested cognitive domains—simple attention

Page 8: Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category ...ftd.med.upenn.edu/uploads/media_items/can-patients... · Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s

A. Bozoki et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827 823

Table 4Neuropsychological data for AD and Control participants

AD Controls t-Value p-Value

CVLT total recall 31.1 ± 10.7 72.5 ± 10.3 17.6 <0.0005Rey-Osterreith copy* 24.2 ± 11.4 33.1 ± 3.3 4.74 <0.0005Rey-Osterreith 5 min recall* 2.7 ± 3.6 15.6 ± 7.5 9.75 <0.0005CPT-X false alarms* 2.4 ± 3.3 1.1 ± 1.3 2.6 0.013CPT-X misses* 3.9 ± 7.8 0.3 ± 0.7 2.89 0.006CPT-XOX false alarms 3.4 ± 5.0 0.9 ± 4.6 2.3 0.023CPT-XOX misses 10.2 ± 5.4 5.6 ± 4.1 4.23 <0.0005Benton visual form disc* 24.9 ± 4.8 30.4 ± 1.9 6.74 <0.0005Pyramid-palm trees* 45.3 ± 7.1 50.4 ± 1.6 4.4 <0.0005

* Unequal variances.

(CPT-X), working memory (CPT-XOX), visual object process-ing (PPT) and visuospatial processing (BVFD). Means, t-values,and significance levels for all administered neuropsychologicaltests are provided in Table 4.

4. General discussion

As outlined in the introduction, we set out to explore sev-eral questions. First, and most important, are AD patients andmatched Controls able to implicitly learn novel categories, whenstringent criteria for learning are used to rule out the contribu-tion of working memory? Second, assuming the answer to thefirst question is positive, is implicit learning in AD participantsequivalent to that in Control participants? Third, are AD par-ticipants able to learn a novel category solely through learningon the test trials, and if so, is this learning comparable to thatin Control participants? Fourth, does the similarity of traininginstances matter? That is, do individuals learn novel categoriesbetter if the exemplars are closely related to one another thanif they are more diverse in their features? We start by brieflydiscussing the last question, then move on to a more extendeddiscussion of the first three questions.

4.1. What are the effects of similarity of training?

porscw(cw(hsci

si

et al. (1999) finding of impaired implicit-category-learning inAD participants may have been due to their use of relatively dis-similar training items, i.e., Keri et al. used only high-distortionsof the prototype dot pattern. However, it is difficult to compareour variation in similarity among artificial animals with theirvariation in similarity of dot patterns. What is clear, though,is that in these kinds of paradigms implicit learning occurs tothe extent the exemplars are similar; in our study we obtainedevidence for implicit learning in AD participants only with high-similarity training items.

A cautionary note is in order, though, in interpreting anyof these findings as showing less implicit learning with low-similarity training items. It is possible that any lack of evidencefor learning – e.g., the fact that the difference between LSand NT was not significant for either group of participants –was due to a strategic effect. Specifically, in the LS condition,participants could have implicitly learned something about thecategory structure, but what they learned may have been suf-ficiently impoverished that they chose to ignore it and relyinstead on what they learned during test. This possibility ofstrategic choice arises because the paradigm permits multipleways for category learning to occur, which means that partic-ipants had the option of choosing to emphasize one way overanother.

4.2. Do AD and Control participants show evidence fori

atoictcbit

tCaptlfweidciptbh

Consider first the effects of similarity of training with Controlarticipants. When the measure of performance was accuracyn the first 10 test trials, Controls showed above-chance accu-acy with both high- and low-similarity training items, but withomewhat greater accuracy in the high- than the low-similarityondition (79% versus 67%). When the measure of performanceas accuracy on all test trials with versus without training

HS–NT or LS–NT), Controls showed no evidence for implicitategory learning with either kind of training, but their accuracyith training was greater for high- than low-similarity items

see Table 2). Thus, even in the latter case, there is evidence thatigh-similarity training leads to better implicit learning. Theafest conclusion seems to be that, though Control participantsan learn a category implicitly even with low-similarity trainingtem, they may learn more with high-similarity items.

The results are more clear-cut for AD participants. Theyhowed evidence for implicit learning only when the trainingtems were similar. This result raises the possibility that Keri

mplicit learning with stringent criteria?

This is the major question motivating our research. Inddressing the question, we focus on the high-similarity condi-ion. Using the standard criterion for implicit category learning –verall categorization accuracy on the test trials following train-ng – both groups of participants showed evidence for implicitategory learning. Categorization accuracy was 76% for Con-rols and 73% for AD participants; both numbers are signifi-antly greater than chance, and there is no significant differenceetween them. But, as noted at the outset, this standard criterions too weak given the Palmeri and Flanery (1999) demonstrationhat category learning can occur during test trials.

One stringent criterion for implicit learning was that par-icipants showed above-chance accuracy on the first 10 trials.ontrols passed this criterion; but AD patients did not, presum-bly because of the insensitivity of the measure or because ADatients needed additional implicit learning during test to raiseheir categorization accuracy. Our second stringent criterion forearning was the overall difference between categorization per-ormance when it was preceded by actual training trials andhen it was not. Under this criterion, AD participants showed

vidence for implicit category learning. To our knowledge, thiss the first demonstration of implicit category learning using aemanding criterion like that routinely applied in studies of per-eptual priming (e.g., in the standard fragment-completion task,mplicit memory is assumed only when performance is better onrimed items than on unprimed items, e.g., Schacter, 1992). Con-rol participants did not pass this stringent criterion, presumablyecause they chose not to use learning-during-test when theyad already learned something substantial from the training; this

Page 9: Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category ...ftd.med.upenn.edu/uploads/media_items/can-patients... · Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s

824 A. Bozoki et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827

strategic effect undermined our subtracting learning-during-testfrom their overall CatAcc scores.

Hence the results based on our two stringent criteria are some-what mixed. While we have offered explanations of why ADpatients failed to pass the first stringent criterion and Controlsthe second, we have no independent support for these accounts.But there is a third source of evidence—the functions relating theprobability of endorsing a test item as a category member to thetypicality of that test item. The slope of such a function offers adetailed measure of the degree of learning, a measure that goesbeyond our CatAcc scores. For AD patients, the slope of thefunction for the HS condition significantly exceeded that of theNT condition (see Fig. 2). This is the strongest evidence we havefor implicit category learning in patients with medial-temporallobe damage. Still, the fact that our results are somewhat mixedmeans that the issue is still open.

The preceding evidence has implications for the larger issueabout whether all category learning can be accounted for in termsof just episodic and working memory (e.g., Nosofsky & Zaki,1998; Palmeri & Flanery, 1999), or whether an implicit learningsystem needs to be posited as well. The categorization perfor-mance of normal controls in paradigms like the one we used can-not offer strong evidence about this larger issue. This is becausecontrol performance could be based on episodic memory of thetraining items (even though they were learned unintentionally).Stronger evidence about the issue is provided by the categoriza-tPmbaosl

4C

gtacAtt

hAtisttp&f

between the learning items and the test items, even though sucha connection was obscured. This priming paradigm is typicallyassumed to be free of intentional learning. Second, in an fMRIstudy using the standard implicit-category-learning paradigmwith normals, Reber, Stark, & Squire (1998) found increasedactivation in anterior frontal areas during the categorization test;these areas are routinely associated with intentional retrievalfrom episodic memory (Lepage, Ghaffar, Nyberg, & Tulving,2000). In sum, for normal participants, the standard paradigmfor assessing implicit category learning may involve all threemajor memory systems—implicit and explicit long-term mem-ory, as well as working memory (for learning during test trials).

4.4. Are AD and Control participants comparable inlearning during test?

The answer to this question is a qualified “yes.” Withouttraining, AD participants performance on the categorization testimproved from 54% on the first 10 trials to 62% on overall accu-racy at the end of 65 trials. Similarly, the Controls improvedfrom 60% to 71%. There was no statistical difference betweenthese estimates of improvement (though the Controls started ata higher level).

This kind of learning has been attributed to a working-memory mechanism (Palmeri & Flanery, 1999). How might thismechanism operate? In addressing this question, it is useful toh1isofttO

eeye&lwbiSAbC

lIpTulk

ion performance of patients with medial-temporal lobe damage.atient performance is much less likely to be due to explicitemory of the training items, and if working memory can also

e ruled out then categorization performance must be due tonother memory system—implicit memory. Thus, to the extentur results demonstrate accurate categorization by AD patients,uch results offer evidence for the existence of implicit categoryearning.

.3. Is AD implicit-category learning equal to that of theontrols?

This turns out to be difficult to answer, given that the tworoups passed different criteria for learning. It appears that thesewo groups used a different mix of (at least) two different mech-nisms to achieve comparable overall accuracy scores on theontrast between HS and NT. Specifically, in the HS conditionD participants may have relied on both implicit learning during

raining and some learning during test, whereas Controls seemo have relied mainly on implicit learning.

Even this view may be too simple, as the Controls may alsoave relied on their explicit memory of the training instances.lthough studies of implicit category learning routinely assume

hat they have eliminated this possibility by disguising the train-ng trials as something that would block intentional learning, andubsequent conscious retrieval, there is no direct evidence forhis assumption. Indeed there is now evidence that the assump-ion of no-explicit-learning is mistaken. First, in a recent study ofriming in a fragment-completion task in normals, May, Hasher,Foong (2005) interviewed their participants after the study and

ound that a sizeable number of them were aware of the relation

ave an example at hand, and accordingly Fig. 4 provides the first0 test items in the HS test list. One possibility is that after see-ng the first few items, participants notice that successive itemshare features, and then select one or two as the ones definingf category membership (e.g., striped body); they then use thiseature (or features) to determine their subsequent categoriza-ion decisions. This is an hypothesis-testing strategy, a strategyhat is known to rely heavily on working memory (Ashby &’Brien, 2005).This interpretation has some drawbacks, though. There is no

vidence that participants use a single feature to guide their cat-gorization decisions in this task (Reed et al., 1999). And evenoung normal participants typically do not spontaneously gen-rate hypotheses that involve more than one feature (e.g., Ashby

O’Brien, 2005). Furthermore, there is a more general prob-em with any hypothesis that attributes learning-during-test toorking memory: we have shown that AD patients are capa-le of such learning, but AD patients are known to have somempairment in working memory (e.g., Perry & Hodges, 1999).uch an impairment would explain why Controls outperformedD participants on the first 10 trials of test (61% versus 54%),ut the impairment is not in line with the finding that AD andontrol participants improved over test trials at the same rate.

If working memory is not the sole mechanism underlyingearning-during-test, what other memory system is involved?t cannot be explicit long-term memory for both groups ofarticipants, because this system is severely impaired in AD.his leaves us with implicit long-term memory, but this seemsnlikely for Controls because during the test trials they wouldikely have attempted to learn the category explicitly as theynew there was a category present. We are left then with

Page 10: Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category ...ftd.med.upenn.edu/uploads/media_items/can-patients... · Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s

A. Bozoki et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827 825

Fig. 4. The first 10 test items in the HS test list.

the (unparsimonious) hypothesis that Control and AD partici-pants may have used different mechanisms in learning-during-test—primarily working memory for Controls, and possibly acombination of working memory and implicit learning for ADpatients. Note that this hypothesis also offers another reasonwhy Control participants learned more than their AD counter-parts on the first 10 test trials (the CatAcc10 scores). Only theControls could make extensive use of working memory, andworking memory alone could accomplish some learning duringthe first 10 test trials without training (see Fig. 4). If this hypoth-esis is correct, then AD and Control participants used differentlearning mechanisms, or combinations of mechanisms, duringtest as well as during training.

5. Study limitations

There are a number of limitations of our study that preventus from drawing strong conclusions about certain questions. Wenote five such limitations in what follows.

5.1. Learning during test

We used the standard implicit category learning task, whichincludes a long series of uninterrupted test trials. Researchershave been aware from the start that there was a possibility oflearning during test trials (see Knowlton & Squire, 1993), butPalmeri and Flanery (1999) were the first to demonstrate that,

Page 11: Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category ...ftd.med.upenn.edu/uploads/media_items/can-patients... · Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s

826 A. Bozoki et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827

given sufficiently motivating instructions, participants couldlearn the category structure solely during test. While of someinterest in itself, learning-during-test makes it difficult to iso-late implicit learning, making it harder to study. In future workit would be useful to offset such learning by making the testlist as uninformative as possible. For example, our test listfor the HS condition contained 25 of 65 items that shared atleast eight features in common with the category prototype(we wanted multiple copies of certain items of interest), andthese items might have made it possible for participants to useworking memory to learn during the test. In subsequent exper-iments the test list could be composed of an equal numberof items at each possible level of similarity-to-the-prototype,which would made it more difficult to learn the categorystructure.

5.2. Test lists for HS, LS, and two NT conditions

We used different test lists for the HS and LS conditions,and for the NT conditions to which they were compared. Recallthat we did this because we wanted to include more tests ofhigh-similarity (low-similarity) items in the HS condition (LScondition), and when comparing the HS (LS) and NT conditionswe wanted the test lists to be identical. In retrospect, this designchoice proved problematic. It compromised a direct comparisonof HS minus NT versus LS minus NT, which is the comparisono

5

t5pcbtmisaastddebt

5

oria

are both necessarily between-subject variations.) Note that dou-bling the sample size would have required testing a total of 80AD patients, a very time-consuming endeavor. Given our lim-ited sample size, some of our statistical comparisons lackedpower and resulted in only borderline significance. Further-more, the lack of power limited our ability to treat the CatAcc10scores as definitive. (Recall that the fact that such scores did notexceed chance for AD patients may have been due to a lack ofpower.) Further experiments with AD patients and matched Con-trols will be needed to increase one’s confidence in the presentresults.

5.4.1. MedicationsThe use of cholinesterase inhibitors by a large subset of

our AD participants could conceivably be a confound in ourfindings, contributing to an artificial boost in the performanceof AD participants. However, from concomitant neuropsycho-logical profiling, it is clear that these AD participants retain aprofound deficit of explicit episodic memory for both verbal andvisuospatial stimuli. Although it is possible that cholinesteraseinhibitors exert a selectively greater effect on implicit memorymechanisms, there is no evidence for this in the neuropsycho-logical literature to date.

6

tloepeCcsal

A

cptvawatNCGTw

f most interest.

.3. Categorization versus recognition memory

On our explicit recognition test, participants saw 20 itemswice each for 3 s, then had to identify 10 of those from among5 fairly similar foils. We chose this design, which had a pre-onderance of new items, in order to mimic the structure of ourategorization test as closely as possible. But the test proved toe too difficult even for our control group, perhaps because ofhe low frequency of old items. Our intention, and the typical

ethod employed by researchers in this area, was to demonstratentact explicit recognition memory in Controls, thereby demon-trating a dissociation between preserved categorization skillnd degraded recognition memory in the AD patients. Instead,s evidence for this dissociation we offered the results of moretandard memory testing, in the form of the delayed recall por-ions of the CVLT and the Rey-Osterreith figure. While the largeifferences between the groups on these measures indicate aissociation between explicit memory and categorization, thevidence is not as strong as that which would have been affordedy a performance dissociation on the novel animal recognitionask.

.4. Sample size

Though this experiment tested a total of 80 participants, 40f them AD patients, the between-subject nature of the designesulted in there being only 10 participants in most of the crit-cal comparisons. (The difference between participant groupsnd the difference in training – HS versus LS versus NT –

. Conclusions

The most striking aspect of these results is that our AD par-icipants showed some evidence for implicit-learning, and forearning-during-test. Individuals with AD can perform this typef categorization task, with or without training; but the dis-ase burden may affect the mechanisms by which the task isreferentially accomplished—AD participants may use a differ-nt combination of implicit memory and working memory thanontrols do. If true, this would be an example of an adaptiveompensatory strategy occurring in the setting of a progres-ive and devastating disease, enabling patients to maintain somespects of cognitive performance within the normal range for aonger period of time.

cknowledgements

We would like to thank Nathalie James for her significantontributions to data collection and statistical analysis for thisroject. Her efforts were integral to the final paper. We alsohank Phyllis Koenig for her helpful comments on a recentersion of the manuscript. We are particularly indebted to thenonymous reviewers of an earlier version of this manuscript,hose insightful and incisive comments led us to reconceptu-

lize our research, which in turn led us to different conclusionshan we had initially drawn. This research was supported byIH grants: AG08671 (Michigan Alzheimer’s Disease Researchenter), AG08808 (Pepper Geriatrics Research and Trainingrant), and P32-AG00114 (U. of M. Institute of Gerontologyraining Grant) and R01-036827, and AG15116. This researchas carried out at the University of Michigan.

Page 12: Can patients with Alzheimer’s disease learn a category ...ftd.med.upenn.edu/uploads/media_items/can-patients... · Keywords: Implicit memory; Categorization; Aging; Alzheimer’s

A. Bozoki et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 816–827 827

References

Ashby, F. G., & O’Brien, J. B. (2005). Category learning and multiple mem-ory systems. Trends in Cognitivie Sciences, 9, 83–89.

Barona, A., Reynolds, C. R., & Chastain, R. (1984). A demographically basedindex of premorbid intelligence for the WAIS-R. Journal of Consultingand Clinical Psychology, 52(5), 885–887.

Benton, A. L., Sivan, A. B., Hamsher, K., et al. (1994). Visual form discrim-ination. In Contributions to neuropsychological assessment: A clinicalmanual (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press, pp. 65–72.

Estes, W. K. (1994). Classification and cognition. New York, NY: OxfordUniversity Press.

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state:A practical method of grading the cognitive state of patients for theclinicians. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.

Glosser, G., & Goodglass, H. (1990). Disorders in executive control functionsamong aphasic and other brain-damaged patients. Journal of Clinical andExperimental Neuropsychology, 12(4), 485–501.

Howard, D., & Patterson, K. E. (1992). Pyramid and palm trees test. BurySt. Edmonds, UK: Thames Valley Test Company.

Keri, S. (2003). The cognitive neuroscience of category learning. BrainResearch Reviews, 43, 85–109.

Keri, S., Kalman, J., Kelemen, O., et al. (2001). Are Alzheimer’s dis-ease patients able to learn visual prototypes? Neuropsychologia, 39,1218–1223.

Keri, S., Kalman, J., Rapcsak, S. Z., et al. (1999). Classification learning inAlzheimer’s disease. Brain, 122, 1063–1068.

Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1993). The learning of categories: Parallelbrain systems for item memory and category knowledge. Science, 262,1747–1749.

K

L

L

M

McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., et al. (1984). Clinical diagnosis ofAlzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINDS-ADRDA Work Group underthe auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Forceon Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 34, 939–944.

Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). A context theory of classificationlearning. Psychological Review, 85, 207–238.

Nosofsky, R. M. (1991). Tests of an exemplar model for relating perceptualclassification and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychol-ogy: Human Perceptual Performance, 17, 3–27.

Nosofsky, R. M., & Zaki, S. R. (1998). Dissociations between categorizationand recognition in amnesic and normal individuals: An exemplar-basedinterpretation. Psychological Science, 9(4), 247–255.

Palmeri, T. J., & Flanery, M. A. (1999). Learning about categories in theabsence of training: Profound amnesia and the relationship between per-ceptual categorization and recognition memory. Psychological Science,10(6), 526–530.

Perry, R. J., & Hodges, J. R. (1999). Attention and executivedeficits in Alzheimer’s disease: A critical review. Brain, 122, 383–404.

Reber, P. J., Stark, C. E. L., & Squire, L. R. (1998). Cortical areas supportingcategory learning identified using functional MRI. Proceedings of theNational Academy of Sciences, 95, 747–750.

Reed, J. M., Squire, L. R., Patalano, A. L., et al. (1999). Learning about cat-egories that are defined by object-like stimuli despite impaired declarativememory. Behavioral Neuroscience, 113(3), 411–419.

Schacter, D. L. (1992). Understanding implicit memory: A cognitive neuro-science perspective. American Psychologist, 47, 559–569.

Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (2000). The cognitive neuroscience of catego-rization. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed.).Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Smith, E. E., & Medin, E. L. (1981). Categories and concepts. Cambridge,

S

S

Z

olodny, J. A. (1994). Memory processes in classification learning: An inves-tigation of amnesic performance in categorization of dot patterns andartistic styles. Psychological Science, 5, 164–169.

epage, M., Ghaffar, O., Nyberg, L., & Tulving, E. (2000). Proceedings ofthe National Academy of Sciences: USA, 97, 506–511.

ibon, D. J., Mattson, R. E., Glosser, G., et al. (1996). A nine-word dementiaversion of the California Verbal Learning Test. Clinical Psychologist,10(3), 237–244.

ay, C. P., Hasher, L., & Foong, N. (2005). Implicit memory, age, and timeof day. Psychological Science, 169, 96–100.

MA: Harvard University Press.preen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). Rey-osterreith complex figure test. In A

compendium of neuropsychological tests (2nd ed.). New York, NY: OxfordUniversity Press, pp. 341–363.

quire, L. R., & Knowlton, B. (1995). Learning about categories in theabsence of memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA, 92, 12470–12474.

aki, S. R. (2004). Is categorization performance really intact in amnesia?A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 1048–1054.


Recommended