+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

Date post: 03-Oct-2016
Category:
Upload: cynthia
View: 212 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
19
This article was downloaded by: [University of Memphis] On: 03 September 2012, At: 16:28 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Early Education & Development Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/heed20 Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression Hey Jun Ahn & Cynthia Stifter Version of record first published: 08 Jun 2010 To cite this article: Hey Jun Ahn & Cynthia Stifter (2006): Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression, Early Education & Development, 17:2, 253-270 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed1702_3 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms- and-conditions This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
Transcript
Page 1: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

This article was downloaded by: [University of Memphis]On: 03 September 2012, At: 16:28Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH,UK

Early Education & DevelopmentPublication details, including instructions forauthors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/heed20

Child Care Teachers' Responseto Children's EmotionalExpressionHey Jun Ahn & Cynthia Stifter

Version of record first published: 08 Jun 2010

To cite this article: Hey Jun Ahn & Cynthia Stifter (2006): Child Care Teachers'Response to Children's Emotional Expression, Early Education & Development, 17:2,253-270

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed1702_3

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan,sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone isexpressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make anyrepresentation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up todate. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should beindependently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liablefor any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damageswhatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connectionwith or arising out of the use of this material.

Page 2: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

Child Care Teachers’ Response toChildren’s Emotional Expression

Hey Jun AhnDepartment of Curriculum and Instruction

Pennsylvania State University

Cynthia StifterHuman Development and Family Studies

Pennsylvania State University

This observational study examined practices through which child care teachers so-cialize children’s emotion. A specific aim was to describe strategies of teacher inter-vention in response to emotion displayed by children in child care centers, and to an-swer the question of differential interactions based on children’s age and gender.

The results of this study were as follows: (a) toddler teachers matched and en-couraged children’s positive emotion expression more often than did preschoolteachers; (b) in response to children’s negative emotion, toddler teachers used physi-cal comfort and distraction more often than did preschool teachers who relied moreon verbal mediation; (c) in response to girls’ negative emotional expressions, teach-ers provided more physical comfort and distraction whereas they were more likely toprovide boys with constructive ways to express negative emotion.

The results of this study also revealed relatively infrequent teaching about con-structive ways of expressing negative emotion and very few occurrences of teacher’sempathy, two developmentally appropriate methods for socializing emotion.Teachers may benefit from a training program focusing on facilitating emotionalcompetence.

Young children’s emotional competence has been investigated as an important as-pect of their early success (Raver, 2002). Research indicates that adult, specificallyparent, socialization attempts affect children’s emotional competence; their ability

EARLY EDUCATION AND DEVELOPMENT, 17(2), 253–270Copyright © 2006, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Correspondence should be addressed to Hey Jun Ahn, Department of Curriculum and Instruction,Pennsylvania State University, 446 Blue Course Drive # 504, State College, PA 16803. E-mail:[email protected]

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 3: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

to identify their own and others’emotion expressions as well as use emotion words(Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spinrad, 1998). Research on emotion socialization hasbeen concerned with the communicative interactions between individuals withinfamily contexts (Lewis & Saarni, 1985). However, other social contexts such asschools and child care centers provide important socialization opportunities andtheir role in the development of emotion has been sorely neglected. This study wasconducted to begin to fill this gap.

In addressing how parents contribute to young children’s emotional compe-tence, three mechanisms of emotion socialization have been proposed: modeling,contingency, and coaching (Denham, 1998). First, displays of positive and nega-tive emotion within the family provide children with opportunities to learn aboutemotional expressions, regulation, and display rules. Children’s emotion expres-siveness may reflect both their parents’ overall expressiveness and the patterns ofparticular emotion expression, for example, the prevalence of happiness, anger,and sadness (Denham, 1993). Negative emotional climates of the family, for exam-ple, have been linked with decreases in knowledge about emotions and lessprosocial behavior with siblings (Denham, Mitchell-Copeland, Strandberg, Auer-bach, & Blair, 1997). Second, there is evidence that supportive parental reactionsto children’s emotion expressions are associated with positive outcomes for chil-dren. In general, findings from parenting studies suggest that parents who are re-sponsive, warm, and accepting of children’s emotional reactions tend to have chil-dren who are emotionally well regulated and responsive themselves. In contrast,nonsupportive parental responses to children’s negative emotions such as punitive,minimizing, and parental distress have been associated with negative outcomes forchildren (Eisenberg et al., 1998). The third way in which parents socialize chil-dren’s emotion is through coaching or discussion of emotion. Parent-led conversa-tions about the names, causes, and consequences of emotions may help children inunderstanding why emotions occur and how they are to be expressed. Understand-ing of causes and consequences of emotions, in turn, help children communicatetheir feelings and regulate them. For instance, 3-year-olds who grew up in familiesin which feelings were frequently discussed were better than their peers at makingjudgments about others’ emotions at age 6 (Dunn, Brown, & Beardsall, 1991).

Despite the interest in children’s socialization of emotion by their own parents,social contexts outside the family still lack the attention of researchers. The familyis the first but not the only social context in which children become socialized(Denham, 2001). Teachers, like parents, are managing the emotional climate inwhich children learn about emotions (Mill & Romano-White, 1999) and it is ex-pected that caregivers and teachers are functioning as socializing agents of chil-dren’s emotions. Denham (1998) suggests that teachers have varied qualities thatmake them excellent socializers. They show new skills, provide interesting materi-als, and develop emotional bonds with children in their classrooms. Teachers arealso powerful role models for young children. When teachers express interest in

254 AHN AND STIFTER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 4: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

children’s feelings and show respect for children’s emotional experiences, chil-dren’s emotional development can be profoundly impacted.

The importance of teachers’ role in emotion socialization is underscored by thenumber of children experiencing nonparental care. With more mothers of youngchildren entering the work force, a parallel number of young children have en-rolled in child care centers. In 1999, 61% of mothers with children under 6 years ofage were in the labor force (National Research Council and Institute for Medicine,2000). Consequent to these demographic trends, researchers have conducted stud-ies to address the question of the effects of child care experiences on children’ssocioemotional development. The results from these studies are controversial. Forinstance, Vandell and Corasaniti (1990) reported that children with more extensivechild care experiences since infancy were rated as having poorer peer relationshipsand emotional health by their teachers. More recently, it was found that more timein nonparental care across the first 4 years of life predicted less social competencerated by mothers and caregivers and more externalizing problems reported bymothers and kindergarten teachers (National Institute of Child Health and HumanDevelopment [NICHD] Early Child Care Research Network, 2003). In contrast,other researchers have documented positive effects of early care experiences. Forexample, Crockenberg and Litman (1991) found that longer work hours by moth-ers, and therefore, more hours of nonparental care, were associated with greaterchild compliance at home and in the lab. Another study found that children who at-tended child care were more self-confident, outgoing, verbally expressive, self-sufficient, and comfortable, while acting less fearful, distressed, and timid in newsituations (Clarke-Stewart, Gruber, & Fitzgerald, 1994).

More recently, child care research has evolved from simple questions aboutgood or bad effects of child care into more detailed questions about the quality ofchild care. Quality of child care, that is, better trained teachers, lowerteacher–child ratio, has been identified as an important factor in emotional devel-opment of children. Higher quality is associated with socioemotional outcomes inchildren, such as lower rates of negative emotional expressions, and higher levelsof compliance, attention regulation, and sociability (NICHD Early Child Care Re-search Network, 1998, 2000, 2001; Peisner–Feinberg et al., 1999; Votruba-Drazal,Colety, & Chase–Lansdale, 2004). Vandell, Henderson, and Wilson (1988) foundthat children in higher quality child care were rated as more socially competent,had fewer unfriendly peer interactions, were happier, and received fewer shy nomi-nations from peers. In particular, high-quality child care experiences are beneficialto low-income children’s socioemotional functioning (Votruba-Drazal et al.,2004). Indeed, the consensus is that the effects of early child care are strikinglypositive, especially when children, of either low or high risk, are placed in qualitychild care centers (National Research Council and Institute for Medicine, 2000).

A component of high quality child care is the teacher’s role in facilitating chil-dren’s affect and achievement For example, Loeb, Fuller, Kagan, and Carrol

TEACHERS’ RESPONSE TO CHILDREN’S EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION 255

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 5: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

(2004) reported that children in child care exhibited stronger cognitive growthwhen child care teachers were more responsive and sensitive. Similarly, resultsfrom the Cost, Quality, and Outcomes Study and the NICHD Study of Early ChildCare indicated that children from all ethnic groups showed higher levels of socialskills on standardized assessments shown to predict school success when care-givers were sensitive and stimulating (Burchinal & Cryer, 2003).

Pianta (1999) asserts that child–teacher relationships also play a formative rolein emotional development and that a negative developmental history (i.e., de-pressed mother) of the child can be overcome by positive teacher–child interac-tions. Howes and Ritchie (2002) emphasized the roles of teacher in construction ofsecure attachment relationships and emotional coaching. In fact, children whowere assessed as having more secure child–teacher attachments were observed andrated to be more socially competent with peers (Howes, Hamilton, & Phillipsen,1998). Likewise, Hestenes, Kontos, and Bryan (1993) found that children whoseteachers showed high levels of classroom engagement displayed more intense pos-itive affect whereas low levels of classroom engagement by teachers predictedmore intense negative affect among children.

Given the importance of teachers’ role in children’s emotional development,it is surprising that so little is known about the socialization of emotion by pro-fessional caregivers. Very few studies have investigated teachers’ socialization ofemotion and majority of them are case studies using qualitative methodology.For instance, Pollak and Thoits (1989) studied the emotion socialization of dis-turbed 3- to 5-year-olds in a therapeutic child care center and found that theteachers explained and identified children’s feelings most frequently by associat-ing emotion words with a situational cause. Teachers also taught children toidentify emotional states in others, explained and validated children’s emotions,and helped children to learn appropriate displays and emotion management.More recently, DeMorat (1998) found that kindergarten children learned appro-priate ways of displaying and discussing emotions from their teacher. A contin-gent analysis examined how students responded to teachers’ displays of emotion.Students as a group matched the emotion of the teacher 42% of the time. Stu-dents often seemed to interpret the teacher’s affective state through his or her fa-cial expression, voice, and gestures.

As mentioned earlier, previous studies have not addressed how teachers in childcare centers respond to children’s emotional expressions using systematic obser-vation. Given that the increasing number of dual-career families has resulted inmuch of the socialization process being shared with substitute caregivers, it is im-portant to examine the emotion socialization process that occurs in the context of achild care center. That is, increased understanding is needed about how child careteachers serve as emotion socializing agents that facilitate the emotional develop-ment of young children. By observing systemically how the teachers respond tochildren’s negative and positive emotional expressions and how they teach emo-tion, important information would be gained that may inform educational practice.

256 AHN AND STIFTER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 6: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

This study aimed to observe teachers’emotion socialization practices and to an-swer thequestionofdifferential interactionsbasedonchildren’sageandgender.Pre-vious studies on parental emotion socialization have found that parents socializedtheir children differently based on children’s ages. For example, as a child developsthe responsibility of emotion regulation moves from the parent to the child (Kopp,1989). That is, as children come to have more effective regulatory abilities, parentsdecrease the frequency and intensity of their intervention. For example, Grolnick,Kurowski, McMenamy, Rivkin, and Bridges (1998) found that there was an age-re-lated decrease in children’s dependency on mother for emotion regulation and moth-ers initiated lessactiveengagementwith theiroldercompared toyounger toddlers.

Like parents, teachers have children of different ages under their care. To facili-tate children’s emotion development, teachers need to consider children’s develop-mental level. Thus, we expected teachers of toddlers to have different behavioraland verbal responses toward children’s emotion expressions than teachers of pre-schoolers. By observing different classrooms, age-appropriateness of teachers’emotion socialization practices was investigated.

There is some evidence that adults have different expectations with regards toboys’ and girls’ emotional expressions (Eisenberg et al., 1998). Several studieshave demonstrated differences in parents’ socialization behaviors as a function ofgender. Parents reported that they encourage their sons, more than their daughters,to control their emotions (Block, 1979), but that mothers discuss sadness morewith daughters, whereas they discuss anger and disgust more with their sons(Fivush, 1989).

Unclear, however, is how gender influences teachers’ socialization of children’semotion. Wittmer and Honig (1988) reported that 2-year-old male toddlers be-haved more negatively and elicited more negative behaviors from their caregiversthan did 2-year-old girls or 3-year-olds. Thus, it was expected that teachers wouldexhibit different socialization strategies for girls and boys.

The goal of this study was directed toward better understanding of teachers’emotion socialization practices through their everyday interactions with children.Through systematic observations, this study examined teachers’ responses to chil-dren’s emotional expressions, as well as, their strategies for intervening dependingon the emotion displayed. Finally, difference in teachers’ interactions with regardto children’s age and gender was investigated.

METHOD

Participants

The participants in the study were 12 primary full-time teachers from three privatechild care centers in a semirural area of Pennsylvania. Two teachers in each toddlerand preschool room were observed. One among 12 teachers was male. The mean

TEACHERS’ RESPONSE TO CHILDREN’S EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION 257

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 7: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

teachers’ level of education was 15. 8 years and they averaged 7.3 years of experi-ence in child care settings. The teachers’ mean age was 34 years.

Child care center A was a university-affiliated center located on the campus of alarge university. This center is also accredited by NAEYC (the National Associa-tion for the Education of Young Children). Child care centers B and C are pri-vate-for-profit child care centers, and both are nationally franchised. The numberof children ranged from 8 to 12 in the toddler room and 14 to 18 in the preschoolroom. Two classrooms at each center, one toddler (ages 2–3) and one preschool(ages 4–5), were observed. The ratio of teacher to child in the three toddler roomsis approximately 2:10, and 2:16 for the preschool rooms.

Child care center directors were contacted by telephone or in person and wereasked if they would be willing to receive information on a study designed to exam-ine teacher–child interactions. If the director agreed to receive information, a de-scription of the study was mailed to him or her. He or she was then given a fol-low-up telephone call and asked if any of the eligible teachers at the center agreedto participate.

Measures

Emotion socialization coding. Because previous studies have not used ameasurement system specifically designed to record different types of emotion so-cialization practices, it was necessary to create a coding system to address thegoals of this study. The coding of teachers’ reactions to children’s emotional ex-pressions was based on extensive pilot observations as well as existing parents’emotion socialization coding systems (Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1997; Honig &Wittmer, 1985; Roberts & Strayer, 1987). The coding scheme was then reviewedby several experienced educators and their feedback was incorporated into the fi-nal coding system.

Teachers’ responses to children’s negative emotion were coded as follows: (a)ignoring—the teacher intentionally or unintentionally does not attend to child’semotional display; (b) physical comforting—the teacher gives a hug or touches achild’s face or body; (c) negative response—the teacher restricts, threatens, ridi-cules, frowns, forbids, punishes, or minimizes the seriousness of the situation suchas scolding a crying child with the comment “Stop crying! You’re driving mecrazy;” (d) teaching constructive means of emotion regulation or alternative waysto express emotion such as guiding angry child to use her words instead of scream-ing to her friend; (e) intervening in the cause of negative emotion—the teachertries to help the child to solve the source of the problem; (f) showing empathy withor validating the child’s emotion; (g) distraction—the teacher draws the child’s at-tention to other activities; and (h) other. Teachers’ responses to children’s positiveemotion expressions were also coded as follows: (a) ignoring—the teacher inten-tionally or unintentionally does not attend to child’s emotion display, (b) matching

258 AHN AND STIFTER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 8: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

the emotion—the teacher shows the same emotion display, (c) encouragement ofemotion display—the teacher praises or encourages children’s positive emotiondisplays, (d) discouragement of emotion display—the teacher bans or discourageschild to express positive emotion, and (e) other. Examples of teachers’ responsescan be found in Ahn (2005).

Coding of children’s emotion. How the teacher responded to each child’semotion with another child or the teacher was coded using codes from a study byDeMorat (1998). Children’s display of sadness, anger, fear, and generalized cryingwas noted and then teacher responses coded. Children’s positive emotions of hap-piness, pride, interest, affection, and overexcitement were also noted and the teach-ers’ responses coded.

Procedures

Because the emotion socialization practices by the teachers were the focus of thisstudy, teachers’ responses to child emotion were observed in an unmanipulatedcontext with no constraints. Observations of the teacher and children were con-ducted during free play, teacher-led activities (both a large group and small groupactivity), transitions (i.e., clean up time, getting ready to go outside, etc.), andsnack hours. Individual teachers were observed continuously for 120 to 180 min,depending on the length of the free play period, structured activity, and outdoor ac-tivity on that day. Each teacher was observed for 30 hr. It usually took 2 weeks toobtain the minimum 30 hr of observation. Coding was done in real time using acoding grid.

This study used the event sampling method. After an emotion was displayed bya child or the teacher noticed a child’s emotion expression, then, behaviors and ver-balizations of the teacher were recorded with the contextual information on thecoding sheet. In addition, the time, child gender, and the type of emotion expressedwere noted. If there was no response from the teacher within 15 sec of a child’semotion expression, the teacher was coded as “ignoring” the child’s emotion ex-pression. Once the teacher responded to an individual child’s emotion expressionsin either a positive or negative manner, the researcher recorded the teacher’s re-sponse, class event (e.g., circle time), and verbalizations of the teacher. In caseswhere the teacher showed more than one response, all strategies or behavioral re-sponses were coded. If two children were expressing the same or different emo-tions, the child to whom the teacher directed his or her attention became the targetchild and the quality of the teacher’s behavior was coded.

Prior to coding, the researcher (HJA) was present in the child care center for aminimum of 2 weeks (25 hr) prior to formal data collection, so that the teacher andchildren would be accustomed to her presence during the study.

TEACHERS’ RESPONSE TO CHILDREN’S EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION 259

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 9: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

Interobserver reliability. A second observer observed 10% of the total ob-servations. Interobserver agreement for coding of teachers’ responses to children’semotional expressions ranged from .84 to .96, which reflects fairly high inter-observer agreement. The Cohen’s kappa levels were .84 for children’s positiveemotion expression, .87 for teachers’ responses to children’s positive emotion, .92for children’s negative emotional expressions, and .96 for teachers’ responses tochildren’s negative emotion expressions.

RESULTS

Data Analysis

Analyses of the data were conducted by using the Statistical Package for the SocialSciences (SPSS 10.0). In all of the statistical analyses, the conventional 0.05 alphalevel of significance was utilized. To examine the association between variables,the Crosstabs procedure of the SPSS program was used. A nonparametric correla-tion coefficient, Cramer’s V, was used because the variables to be correlated werenominal. The Cramer’s V was used because there were three child care centers andthe other variables had more than three levels.

Children’s Emotional Expressions

Analyses were carried out to examine the effects of gender and age on the fre-quency of children’s expressions of positive and negative emotion. Children’s pos-itive emotional expressions included smiling, happiness, pride, affection seekingbehavior, and excitement. Table 1 shows the frequency and percentages of chil-dren’s positive emotion expressions by gender and age of the child.

There was a statistically significant moderate relationship between age group(toddler, preschool) and children’s positive emotion expression (Cramer’s V = .38,p ≤ .05). In general, toddlers in the child care centers expressed more positive emo-tion than did preschoolers. Toddlers expressed happiness almost twice as manytimes as preschoolers. However, affection seeking behaviors and excitement weremore prevalent among preschoolers.

Girls expressed more positive emotion than did the boys (see Table 1). Therewas a statistically significant although low relationship between gender and chil-dren’s positive emotional expressions (Cramer’s V = .20, p ≤ .05). Girls displayeda substantially larger proportion of affection seeking behaviors toward their teach-ers than did boys. Moreover, girls smiled almost twice as many times as boys. Ex-citement, on the other hand, was seen more in boys than in girls.

The frequency of children’s negative emotional expressions by gender and ageof the child appear in Table 1. Although the older group expressed more anger and

260 AHN AND STIFTER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 10: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

261

TAB

LE1

Per

cent

age

and

Freq

uenc

yof

Chi

ldre

n’s

Exp

ress

ions

ofP

ositi

vean

dN

egat

ive

Em

otio

nby

Cen

ter,

byA

ge,a

ndby

Gen

der

Chi

ldre

n’s

Exp

ress

ion

ofPo

siti

veE

mot

ion

Chi

ldre

n’s

Exp

ress

ion

ofN

egat

ive

Em

otio

n

Smil

eH

appi

ness

Pri

deA

ffect

ion

Exc

item

ent

Tota

lSa

dA

ngry

Fus

syC

ryTo

tal

Age To

ddle

r55

.3(8

8)66

.6(2

73)

66.7

(10)

27.5

(11)

19.5

(32)

414

53.1

(52)

48.1

(141

)37

.0(1

7)67

.3(2

20)

430

Pres

choo

ler

44.7

(71)

33.4

(137

)33

.3(5

)72

.5(2

9)80

.5(1

32)

374

46.9

(46)

51.9

(152

)63

.0(2

9)32

.7(1

07)

334

Cra

mer

’sV

=.3

8,p

≤.0

5C

ram

er’s

V=

.20,

p≤

.05

Gen

der

Gir

l66

.0(1

05)

53.7

(220

)73

.3(1

1)65

.0(2

6)37

.8(6

2)42

459

.2(5

8)40

.3(1

18)

26.1

(12)

41.6

(136

)32

4B

oy34

.0(5

4)46

.3(1

90)

26.7

(4)

35.0

(14)

62.2

(102

)36

440

.8(4

0)59

.7(1

75)

73.9

(34)

58.4

(191

)44

0C

ram

er’s

V=

.20,

p≤

.05

Cra

mer

’sV

=.1

5,p

≤.0

5

Not

e.V

alue

inpa

rent

hese

sre

flec

tsfr

eque

ncy

ofoc

curr

ence

s.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 11: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

whining or fussiness, toddlers expressed their sadness more often than did pre-schoolers. In particular, a substantially larger proportion of toddlers (67.3%) criedmore often than did preschoolers (32.7%). Regarding gender and children’s nega-tive emotion expression, boys were angrier and fussier than were girls. Boys alsocried more than did girls. Conversely, girls expressed their sadness more often thandid boys.

Toddler and Preschooler Teacher Responsesto Children’s Emotional Expression

To examine the effect of age on teacher responses to children’s positive emotion,toddler teachers and preschooler teachers were compared. There was a statisticallysignificant moderate relationship between children’s age and teachers’ responses(Cramer’s V = .34, p ≤ .05). Table 2 reports the results. Toddler teachers matchedpositive emotion more frequently (77.3%) than did preschool teachers (55.9%).Whereas toddler teachers encouraged children’s positive emotional expressionsmore often than did preschool teachers, preschool teachers were more likely to dis-courage children’s positive emotion expression (33.7%) than toddler teachers(7.7%).

Teachers’ responses to children’s negative emotional expression by age groupwere found to be significantly different for some behaviors (Cramer’s V= .31, p ≤.05) and can be seen in Table 3. Compared to preschool teachers, toddler teachersused physical comfort and distraction more often in response to children’s expres-sion of negative emotions. They also taught constructive or alternative ways of ex-pressing negative emotion more often than did toddler teachers.

262 AHN AND STIFTER

TABLE 2Toddler and Preschool Teachers’ Responses

to Positive Emotional Expression

Age

Teacher Response Toddler Preschooler Total

Ignoring 16 (3.9%) 19 (5.1%) 35 (4.4%)Match 320 (77.3%) 209 (55.9%) 529 (67.1%)Encouragement 40 (9.7%) 14 (3.7%) 54 (6.9%)Discouragement 32 (7.7%) 126 (33.7%) 158 (20.1%)Others 6 (1.4%) 6 (1.6%) 12 (1.5%)Total 414 (100%) 374 (100%) 788 (100%)

Note. Cramer’ s V = .34, p ≤ .05.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 12: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

Teachers’ Responses to Emotional Expression by Gender

As can be seen in Table 4, teachers in all three child care centers responded to girls’and boys’ positive emotional expression in significantly different ways (Cramer’sV= .22, p ≤ .05). Teachers matched girls’ positive emotion expression more(74.1%) than they did boys’ (59.1%). The frequency of encouragement to girls wasalso higher whereas discouragement of positive emotions was greater for boysthan girls.

Table 5 reports how teachers responded to the negative emotional expressionsof girls and boys. There was a statistically significant although modest relationshipbetween children’s gender and teachers’ responses to negative emotional expres-

TEACHERS’ RESPONSE TO CHILDREN’S EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION 263

TABLE 3Toddler and Preschool Teachers’ Responses

to Negative Emotional Expression

Age

Teacher Response Toddler Preschooler Total

Ignoring 60 (14.0%) 35 (10.5%) 95 (12.4%)Physical comfort 98 (22.8%) 37 (11.1%) 135 (17.7%)Negative 32 (7.4%) 30 (9.0 %) 62 (8.1%)Constructive or alternative 38 (8.8%) 50 (15.0%) 88 (11.5%)Intervening the cause 95 (22.1%) 126 (37.7%) 221 (28.9%)Empathy or validation 24 (5.6%) 16 (4.8%) 40 (5.2%)Distraction 58 (13.5%) 16 (4.8%) 74 (9.7%)Others 25 (5.8%) 24 (7.2%) 49 (6.4%)Total 430 (100%) 334 (100%) 764 (100%)

Note. Cramer’s V = .31, p ≤ .05.

TABLE 4Teachers’ Responses to Positive Emotional Expression by Gender

Gender

Teacher Response Girl Boy

Ignoring 20 (4.7%) 15 (4.1%)Match 314 (74.1%) 215 (59.1%)Encouragement 33 (7.8%) 21 (5.8%)Discouragement 55 (13.0%) 103 (28.3%)Others 2 (0.5%) 10 (2.7%)Total 424 (100%) 364 (100%)

Note. Cramer’s V = .22, p ≤ .05.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 13: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

sion (Cramer’s V = .19, p ≤ .05). Teachers responded to girls with more physicalcomfort and distraction. Boys experienced more teacher indifference, empathy,and suggested constructive ways to express negative emotion.

DISCUSSION

The purpose of this study was to investigate child care teachers’ socialization ofemotion practices. Classroom observations generated data to describe teachers re-sponses to emotional displays of toddler and preschool children and whether theytake advantage of these situations to socialize emotions.

Toddler teachers matched and encouraged children’s positive emotion morefrequently than did preschool teachers. It is difficult to tell whether toddler teach-ers’ use of more active strategies of emotion socialization induced toddlers’ morefrequent positive emotion, or whether toddlers’ positive emotional expressions ledto their teachers’ more active intervention with matching and encouragement ofpositive emotions.

On the other hand, although preschool teachers matched children’s positiveemotion expression the majority of the time, they also discouraged these expres-sions more frequently than did toddler teachers. One possible explanation for thisresult is that preschoolers displayed much more overexcitement than did toddlers.To calm down overexcitement of preschoolers, their teachers may need to discour-age their overly excited feelings. Teachers were often heard to say “Remember weare inside” or “Please use your inside voice. I can’t hear you.”

Another possibility is that teachers of toddlers and preschoolers differ in theirexpectations for children’s ability to control their excitement. According to Hyson

264 AHN AND STIFTER

TABLE 5Teachers’ Responses to Negative Emotional Expression of Girl and Boy

Gender

Teacher Response Girl Boy

Ignoring 27 (8.3%) 68 (15.5%)Physical comfort 68 (21.0%) 67(15.2%)Negative 27 (8.3%) 35 (8.0%)Constructive 32 (9.9%) 56 (12.7%)Intervening the cause 95 (29.3%) 126 (28.6%)Empathy or validation 11 (3.4%) 29 (6.6%)Distraction 46 (14.2%) 28 (6.4%)Others 18 (5.6%) 31 (7.0%)Total 324 (100%) 440 (100%)

Note. Cramer’ s V = .19, p ≤ .05.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 14: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

(1994), as toddlers grow into preschoolers, they developmentally move towardbetter coordination and control of emotions and exhibit more varied, complex, andflexible ways of expressing emotions. Consequently, preschool teachers might be-lieve that children of this age group have a better ability or should have a betterability to control themselves than do younger children. As a result of these differ-ent expectations, preschool teachers might be more vigilant in tracking and con-trolling children’s high levels of excitement than toddler room teachers.

Teachers’ responses to children’s negative emotional expressions also differedby age. Teachers of toddlers were more frequently involved in children’s expres-sions of negative emotion than were teachers of preschoolers. Again, one possibleexplanation for this higher level of support for toddlers might be toddlers’ morefrequent negative emotional expressions in comparison with preschoolers. Foryounger children, who might be perceived by teachers as more vulnerable to nega-tive emotion, such supportive intervention would reflect age appropriate emotionsocialization practices.

This result is consistent with previous studies of parent socialization which in-dicated that, as children develop, the responsibility of emotion regulation movesfrom parents to children. That is, as children come to have more effective regula-tory abilities, their parents decrease the number of their interventions. An age-re-lated decrease in children’s dependency on mother for emotion regulation, for in-stance, was found in the study of Grolnick et al. (1998). With age, the ability toregulate emotion is developing from more passive or other-directed regulatory be-haviors to more active or self-directed regulatory behaviors. Bridges and Grolnick(1995) also found that 12- and 18-month-olds were less actively engaged with amother, compared to 24- and 32-month-olds, during delay and separation situa-tions. In fact, Grolnick et al. (1998) found that mothers of older toddlers initiatedless active engagement with their children compared to mothers of the youngerones.

In this study, toddler teachers used physical comfort and distraction more oftenthan did preschool teachers, whereas preschool teachers were more likely to inter-vene with the cause of the emotion. Moreover, preschoolers were taught construc-tive or alternative ways of expressing negative emotion more often than were tod-dlers. In short, teachers of younger children preferred more direct, quick responsessuch as physical comfort and distraction. Teachers may view younger children asless able to regulate negative emotion and chose their strategies accordingly. Incontrast, teachers of older children, recognizing and expecting more mature formsof cognitive ability, took the opportunity to teach constructive ways of dealing withnegative emotion.

The age-related differences found in teachers’ responses to children’s negativeemotion suggest that teachers in this study tailored their role in helping children toregulate their emotion to the developmental level of the children. With age, chil-dren need to develop more sophisticated skills and to acquire a growing repertoire

TEACHERS’ RESPONSE TO CHILDREN’S EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION 265

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 15: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

of strategies in managing their emotional experience and expression. According toSroufe (1989), the responsibility of managing emotions transfers from the care-giver to the child. At first, caregivers have primary responsibility for keeping emo-tional arousal tolerable. As the children grow, they play an active role in the pro-cess of emotion regulation by instigating regulatory assistance through deliberateefforts.

Gender also appeared to affect teacher responses to children’s expressions ofemotion. Teachers responded differently to girls’ and boys’ expressions of positiveemotion. Teachers matched the girls’ positive emotional expressions more oftenthan they did the boys’ positive emotional expressions. The frequency of encour-agement of positive emotion to girls was also higher than that given to boys. Thisresult is inconsistent with earlier studies of parental socialization of positive emo-tion. Malatesta and Haviland (1982) reported that mothers seemed to be more con-tingent in responding to their sons’ emotional expressions during play than to theirdaughters’ emotional expressions. Mothers matched their sons’ emotional expres-sions and responded more contingently to their sons’ smiles than to their daugh-ters’ smiles. Eisenberg and her colleagues (1998) suggested that the gender of thechild has an effect on mothers’ responses to children’s emotional expression be-cause mothers may perceive their boys to be more at-risk for dealing with emotionsand they may try to alter this trend by encouraging more positive emotion expres-sions.

Why are the results of this study different from prior studies with parents? First,teachers at child care centers are interacting with children in a different setting thanthat of the home. Group dynamics at child care centers may affect teachers’ re-sponses to children’s expressions of emotion in a different way from the parents athome. Another possibility is that because boys’ and girls’ different repertoires andintensity of positive emotion were not coded and thus statistically controlled for inthis study, the higher frequency of boys’ overexcitement may have resulted in theteachers’different reactions to them. That is, teachers may not have matched or en-couraged boys’positive emotional expression due to their discouragement of boys’overexcitement.

In response to girls’ negative emotional expressions, teachers provided morephysical comfort and distraction. In response to boys’ expressions of negativeemotion, teachers expressed more empathy, and they also more frequently pro-vided boys with constructive ways to express negative emotion. Teachers’differen-tial reactions to the negative emotions of boys and girls in this study may reflectdifferences in the children’s emotionality. There is evidence suggesting that adults’reactions are associated with high levels of children’s negative emotions.Gronlnick, McMenamy, Kurowski, and Bridges (1998) found that mothers of 1- to3-year-olds who used more reassurance and physical comforting had children whowere more likely to be more distressed in a frustrating situation than were childrenof less reassuring mothers. Boys in this study, therefore, may be provided more

266 AHN AND STIFTER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 16: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

empathy and instruction of constructive ways of expressing negative emotion be-cause they were more prone to cry and be angry.

It is interesting that the findings of this study are consistent with the evidencethat adults have different expectations with regard to boys’and girls’ emotional ex-pressions, and that adults actually may reinforce these emotional expressions as afunction of gender (Eisenberg et al., 1998). Several studies have reported differ-ences in parents’ socialization behaviors with boys compared to girls. Parents re-ported that they encouraged their sons to control emotions more than their daugh-ters (Block, 1979), which may explain why, in this study, teachers responded withphysical comfort more frequently to girls’ rather than boys’negative emotional ex-pressions. Teachers’ more frequent teaching of constructive ways to express nega-tive emotion with boys than with girls may also reflect their perception that boysneed to learn to control their emotions.

Despite this initial evidence of the teachers’ role in the socialization of chil-dren’s emotion, it is interesting to note the relatively low frequency with whichteachers taught children constructive ways in expressing negative emotion as wellas the low frequency of expressed empathy. These data imply that teachers’ emo-tion socialization practices may need attention. Through validation of emotion,children learn that teachers acknowledge that they are having negative emotionsand that it is okay to have such emotions. Although most teachers in this study en-couraged children to verbalize their anger and sadness, two toddler room teachersseldom taught children to use constructive ways of emotional expression. Becausechildren in child care centers may spend even more time with their teachers thanthey do with their own parents, teachers’ role in children’s emotion developmentshould be emphasized.

CONCLUSION

Teachers of younger children used more direct and active strategies such as givingphysical comfort and distraction, whereas preschool teachers tried to intervene inthe cause of the emotion and to teach constructive ways of emotional expression.The differences found in teachers’ responses by gender and age may reflect chil-dren’s different emotional expressions, or it may be that teachers’ perceptions ofchildren’s emotional vulnerability influences the way they react. That is, teachersof younger children may view them as more vulnerable and thus provide morephysical comfort. In contrast, teachers of older children may perceive children ashaving the ability to learn to express negative emotion constructively. However, itdoes appear that teachers are sensitive to the developmental needs of their chargesand that, in general, they respond appropriately to children’s expressions of bothpositive and negative emotions.

TEACHERS’ RESPONSE TO CHILDREN’S EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION 267

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 17: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

Because of the importance of emotional competence to social and academic suc-cess, and because young children are spending more and more time with teachers, itis recommended that a teacher training program focusing on facilitating emotionalcompetence be developed to encourage ways of socializing emotion. For example,using puppets to model appropriate responses to emotion is one example of class-room activity that promotes children’s emotional development. With younger chil-dren, the teachermightusepuppets tomodel theuseof languagerather thanhitting toexpress anger. With older children, the teacher might model different responses tofrustration such as not winning a game (Brewer, 2001). Preschool teachers may helpchildren to express their feelings through writing by selecting examples from litera-ture that illustrate how children have written about their negative feelings. Childrenmay learn to cope more effectively with negative emotion through the writing pro-cess.Asmoreandmorechildrenenterchildcare, the roleof the teacherhasexpandedto include thesocializationofemotion.Programs that facilitate thisprocesswill con-tribute significantly to positive child outcomes.

Limitation of the Study

Due to a small sample size and nonindependent sample, this descriptive study hasinherent limitations of generalizability. Although the researcher is aware that theresults from this study may not be generalized in other settings, these results mayspur other researchers to look at how teachers socialize their students’ emotionalcompetence. In addition, because positive or negative impacts on child outcomeswere not measured in this study, it is impossible to pinpoint the importance of aparticular response to children’s emotional expression.

REFERENCES

Ahn, H. J. (2005). Child care teachers’strategies in children’s socialization of emotion. Early Child De-velopment and Care, 175, 49–61.

Block, J. H. (1979). Another look at sex differentiation in the socialization behaviors of mothers and fa-thers. In J. Sherman & F. L. Denmark (Eds.), Psychology of women: Future of research (pp. 29–87).New York: Psychological Dimensions.

Bridges, L. J., & Grolnick, W. S. (1995). The development of emotional self-regulation in infancy andearly childhood. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology: Vol. 15. Socialdevelopment (pp.185–211). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Burchinal, M., & Cryer, D. (2003). Diversity, child care quality, and developmental outcomes. EarlyChildhood Research Quarterly, 18, 401–426.

Clarke-Stewart, K. A., Gruber, C. P., & Fitzgerald, L. M. (1994). Children at home and in day care.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Crockenberg, S., & Litman, C. (1991). Effects of maternal employment on maternal and two-year-oldbehavior. Child Development, 61, 930–953.

268 AHN AND STIFTER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 18: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

DeMorat, M. G. (1998). Emotion socialization in the classroom context: A functionalist analysis. Un-published doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Barbara.

Denham, S. A. (1993). Maternal emotional responsiveness and toddler’s social–emotional competence.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 715–728.

Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development of young children. New York: Guilford.Denham, S. A. (2001). Dealing with feelings: Foundations and consequences of young children’s emo-

tional competence. Early Education and Development, 12, 5–10.Denham, S. A., Mitchell-Copeland, J., Strandberg, K., Auerbach, S., & Blair, K. (1997). Parental con-

tributions to preschoolers’ emotional competence: Direct and indirect effects. Motivation and Emo-tion, 21, 65–86.

Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L. (1991). Family talks about feelings and children’s later understand-ing of other’s emotions. Developmental Psychology, 27, 448–455.

Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychologi-cal Inquiry, 9, 241–273.

Fivush, R. (1989). Exploring sex differences in the emotional content of mother–child conversationabout the past. Sex Roles, 20, 675–691.

Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: How families communicate emotion-ally. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Grolnick, W. S., Kurowski, C. O., McMenamy, J. M., Rivkin, I., & Bridges, L. J. (1998). Mothers’strat-egies for regulating their toddlers’ distress. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 437–450.

Hestenes, L., Kontos, S., & Bryan, Y. (1993). Children’s emotional expression in child care centersvarying in quality. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8, 295–307.

Honig,A.,&Wittmer,D.S. (1985).Toddlerbidsandteacher responses.ChildCareQuarterly,14,14–29.Howes, C., Hamilton, C., & Phillipsen, L. C. (1998). Stability and continuity of child–caregiver and

child–peer relationships. Child Development, 69, 418–426.Howes, C., & Ritchie, S. (2002). A matter of trust: Connecting teacher and learners in the early child-

hood classroom. New York: Teachers College Press.Hyson, M. C. (1994). The emotional development of young children: Building an emotion-centered

curriculum. New York: Teachers College Press.Kopp, C. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view. Developmental

Psychology, 25, 343–354.Lewis, M., & Saarni, C. (1985). Socialization of emotion. New York: Plenum.Loeb, S., Fuller, B., Kagan, S., & Carrol, B. (2004). Child care in poor communities: Early learning ef-

fects of type, quality, and stability. Child Development, 75, 47–65.Malatesta, C., & Haviland, J. M. (1982). Learning display rules: The socialization of emotion expres-

sion in infancy. Child Development, 53, 991–1003.Mill, D., & Romano-White, D. (1999). Correlates of affectionate and angry behavior in child care edu-

cators of preschool-aged children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 14, 155–178.National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network.

(1998). Early child care and self-control, compliance, and problem behavior at twenty-four andthirty-six months. Child Development, 69, 1145–1170.

National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network.(2000). Interaction of child care and family risk in relation to child development at 24 and 36 months.Applied Developmental Science, 6, 144–156.

National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network.(2001). Child care and children’s peer interaction at 24 and 36 months: The NICHD study of earlychild care. Child Development, 72, 1478–1500.

National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network.(2003). Does amount of time spent in child care predict socio-emotional adjustment during the tran-sition to kindergarten? Child Development, 74, 976–1005.

TEACHERS’ RESPONSE TO CHILDREN’S EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION 269

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12

Page 19: Child Care Teachers' Response to Children's Emotional Expression

National Research Council and Institute for Medicine. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: Thescience of child development. Washington, DC: National Academy of Science Press.

Peisner-Feinberg, E., Burchinal, M., Clifford, R., Yazejian, N., Culkin, M., Zelazo, J., Howes, C.,Byler, P., Kagan, S., & Rustici, J. (1999). The children of the Cost, Quality, and Outcomes Study go toschool. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina, Frank Porter Graham Child Development Center.

Pianta, R. C. (1999). Enhancing relationships between children and teachers. Washington, DC: Ameri-can Psychological Association.

Pollak, L. H., & Thoits, P. A. (1989). Processes in emotional socialization. Social Psychology Quar-terly, 52, 22–34.

Raver, C. C. (2002). Emotions matter: Making the case for the role of young children’s emotional de-velopment for early school readiness. Social Policy Report, 16, 3–19.

Roberts, W., & Strayer, J. (1987). Parental responses to the emotional distress of their children. Devel-opmental Psychology, 23, 415–422.

Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Pathways to adaptation and maladjustment: Psychopathology as developmentaldeviation. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology: Vol. 1(pp.13–24). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Vandell, D. L., & Corasaniti, M. A. (1990). Variations in early child care: Do they predict subsequentsocial, emotional, and cognitive differences? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 5, 555–572.

Vandell, D. L., Henderson, L. V., & Wilson, K. S. (1988). A longitudinal study of children withday-care experiences of varying quality. Child Development, 59, 1286–1292.

Votruba-Drazal, E., Colety, R. L., & Chase–Lansdale, P. (2004). Child care and low-income children’sdevelopment: Direct and moderated effects. Child Development, 75, 296–312.

Wittmer, D., & Honig, A. C. (1988). Teacher re-creation of negative interaction with toddlers. EarlyChild Development and Care, 33, 77–88.

270 AHN AND STIFTER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f M

emph

is]

at 1

6:28

03

Sept

embe

r 20

12


Recommended