+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Closest Speaking Space During the Production of Sibilant Sounds

Closest Speaking Space During the Production of Sibilant Sounds

Date post: 28-Nov-2014
Category:
Upload: amar-bhochhibhoya
View: 198 times
Download: 2 times
Share this document with a friend
4
Closest Speaking Space During the Production of Sibilant Sounds and its Value in Establishing the Vertical Dimension of Occlusion C.A. BURNETT and T.J. CLIFFORD Department of Restorative Dentistry, School of Clinical Dentistry, The Queen's University of Belfast, Grosvenor Road, Belfast BT12 6BP, Northem Ireland, United Kingdom The purpose ofthis investigation was to determine whether the production of sibilant sounds involved adopting a jaw position that corresponded to the closest vertical speaking space (CSS), by analysis of the smallest vertical excursion of the mandible during the performance of different pho- netic exercises. A further objective was to establish the variability in the-CSS produced by individual sibilant phonemes. Thirty young adult subjects had their CSS determined during three separate phonetic tests, using a kinesiograph (Sirognathograph, Siemens A.G., Benshiem, Germany) and a Bio-Pak (BioResearch Associates Inc., Milwaukee, WI) jaw-tracking software program. The first test was a general phonetic articulation test containing all the sounds of the English language and specifically in- cluding all six sibilant word sounds. The second phonetic test contained the six sibilant sounds making up a short sentence. The third test included six single words, each expressing a different sibilant sound. No statistically significant difference among the mean CSS determined in each of the three exercises was demonstrable. A phonetic test containing all sibilant sounds produced a CSS equiva- lent to that of a test containing all speech sounds. The vertical component of the CSS was also independent of the form or duration of the phonetic tests containing the sibilant word sounds used in this investigation. The CSS determined for 5 of the individual sibilant phonemes in the third exercise differed (p < 0.05) from that calculated for the three complete exercises. It was concluded that voicing sibilant phonemes, or word sounds, does cause the subject to adopt the CSS. When a phonetic test is used in the determination of the vertical dimension of occlusion, one of short duration containing all the sibilant sounds appears to give a reliable guide to the CSS. It was also concluded that subjects varied with respect to which of the group of sibilant sounds produced the CSS, and that a single sibilant word sound does not give a reliable indica- tion of the smallest speaking vertical dimension. J Dent Res 72(6):964-967, June, 1993 Introduction. The use of phonetics is one of many techniques available as guides to the vertical dimension of occlusion in dentu- lous and edentulous patients. The phonetic method, by assessment of mandibular position during production of certain speech sounds, identifies the smallest speaking vertical dimension or closest speaking space (CSS) and was originally suggested by Silverman (1951), who re- Received for publication October 28, 1992 Accepted for publication January 19, 1993 ported that it gave constant and reproducible results. The aim of the CSS method in determining occlusal vertical dimension for edentulous subjects is to provide an interoc- clusal space of about 2 mm between the incisor teeth during the pronunciation of sibilant word sounds. Special emphasis is placed on the format for the sibilants because they are among the most frequently used in articulate speech. The sibilants are high-frequency sounds produced by a stream of air directed through a minimal incisal separation. They occur in two forms, the "surd" form and the "sonant" form. The surd form is that produced without laryngeal vibration, voicing, or phonation, while the sonant form is the same sound produced with phonation (West et al., 1947). The Is/, /sh/, and /ch/ sounds are the surd forms of the sibilants. When these sounds are made in conjunc- tion with laryngeal vibrations, the respective sonant forms /z/, /zh/, and /j/ are produced. All six sibilants have similar labial and incisal patterns in their formats, the major difference being a function of tongue position and phona- tion. Despite a lack of objective measurement to show that sibilant sounds do indeed produce the closest speaking level of the mandible to the maxilla, and that different sibilants produce the same levels, the 'closest speaking space technique' (Silverman, 1952) has come to be widely accepted and has often been used in clinical research (Gillings, 1973; Murrell, 1974; Pound, 1977). Morrison (1959) suggested the use of the words sixty- six and Mississippi and several short poems containing /s/ sounds for determination of an acceptable vertical dimension of occlusion in denture construction. Morrison preferred to use short poems, arguing that as the patient re-reads the passage and becomes familiar with its rhythm, involuntary muscle activity assumes greater influence and produces a more consistent closest speaking level. Mehringer (1963) adjusted occlusal registration rims to provide an incisal separation of 1 to 1.5 mm for sibilants, 2 to 4 mm for the nasal sonants, and 5 to 10 mm for the diphthongs. Pound (1977) proposed the Isl sound as the critical guide to establishing the vertical dimension of occlusion for edentulous patients. The purpose of this investigation was to determine whether the CSS occurs during the production of sibilant sounds. This was done by comparison of the smallest vertical components of speech-that is, the smallest ver- tical excursion of the mandible-produced during the performance of three different phonetic exercises. A fur- ther objective was to establish the variability in the CSS during the production of individual sibilant phonemes. Materials and methods. Thirty undergraduate dental students in the School of Clinical Dentistry, Queen's University of Belfast, volun- 964
Transcript
Page 1: Closest Speaking Space During the Production of Sibilant Sounds

Closest Speaking Space During the Production of Sibilant Soundsand its Value in Establishing the Vertical Dimension of Occlusion

C.A. BURNETT and T.J. CLIFFORD

Department of Restorative Dentistry, School of Clinical Dentistry, The Queen's University of Belfast, GrosvenorRoad, Belfast BT12 6BP, Northem Ireland, United Kingdom

The purpose ofthis investigation was to determine whetherthe production of sibilant sounds involved adopting ajawposition that corresponded to the closest vertical speakingspace (CSS), by analysis ofthe smallest vertical excursionof the mandible during the performance of different pho-netic exercises. A further objective was to establish thevariability in the-CSS produced by individual sibilantphonemes. Thirty young adult subjects had their CSSdetermined during three separate phonetic tests, using akinesiograph (Sirognathograph, Siemens A.G., Benshiem,Germany) and a Bio-Pak (BioResearch Associates Inc.,Milwaukee, WI)jaw-tracking software program. The firsttest was a general phonetic articulation test containing allthe sounds of the English language and specifically in-cluding all six sibilant word sounds. The second phonetictest contained the six sibilant sounds making up a shortsentence. The third test included six single words, eachexpressing a different sibilant sound. No statisticallysignificant difference among the mean CSS determined ineach of the three exercises was demonstrable. A phonetictest containing all sibilant sounds produced a CSS equiva-lent to that of a test containing all speech sounds. Thevertical component ofthe CSS was also independent oftheform or duration of the phonetic tests containing thesibilant word sounds used in this investigation. The CSSdetermined for 5 of the individual sibilant phonemes inthe third exercise differed (p < 0.05) from that calculatedfor the three complete exercises. It was concluded thatvoicing sibilant phonemes, or word sounds, does cause thesubject to adopt the CSS. When a phonetic test is used inthe determination of the vertical dimension of occlusion,one of short duration containing all the sibilant soundsappears to give a reliable guide to the CSS. It was alsoconcluded that subjects varied with respect to which ofthegroup of sibilant sounds produced the CSS, and that asingle sibilant word sound does not give a reliable indica-tion of the smallest speaking vertical dimension.

J Dent Res 72(6):964-967, June, 1993

Introduction.

The use of phonetics is one of many techniques availableas guides to the vertical dimension of occlusion in dentu-lous and edentulous patients. The phonetic method, byassessment of mandibular position during production ofcertain speech sounds, identifies the smallest speakingvertical dimension or closest speaking space (CSS) andwas originally suggested by Silverman (1951), who re-

Received for publication October 28, 1992Accepted for publication January 19, 1993

ported that it gave constant and reproducible results. Theaim of the CSS method in determining occlusal verticaldimension for edentulous subjects is to provide an interoc-clusal space of about 2 mm between the incisor teethduring the pronunciation of sibilant word sounds. Specialemphasis is placed on the format for the sibilants becausethey are among the most frequently used in articulatespeech. The sibilants are high-frequency sounds producedby a stream of air directed through a minimal incisalseparation. They occur in two forms, the "surd" form andthe "sonant" form. The surd form is that produced withoutlaryngeal vibration, voicing, or phonation, while the sonantform is the same sound produced with phonation (West etal., 1947). The Is/, /sh/, and /ch/ sounds are the surd formsof the sibilants. When these sounds are made in conjunc-tion with laryngeal vibrations, the respective sonant forms/z/, /zh/, and /j/ are produced. All six sibilants have similarlabial and incisal patterns in their formats, the majordifference being a function of tongue position and phona-tion.

Despite a lack of objective measurement to show thatsibilant sounds do indeed produce the closest speakinglevel of the mandible to the maxilla, and that differentsibilants produce the same levels, the 'closest speakingspace technique' (Silverman, 1952) has come to be widelyaccepted and has often been used in clinical research(Gillings, 1973; Murrell, 1974; Pound, 1977).

Morrison (1959) suggested the use of the words sixty-six and Mississippi and several short poems containing/s/ sounds for determination of an acceptable verticaldimension of occlusion in denture construction. Morrisonpreferred to use short poems, arguing that as the patientre-reads the passage andbecomes familiar with its rhythm,involuntary muscle activity assumes greater influenceand produces a more consistent closest speaking level.Mehringer (1963) adjusted occlusal registration rims toprovide an incisal separation of 1 to 1.5 mm for sibilants,2 to 4 mm for the nasal sonants, and 5 to 10 mm for thediphthongs. Pound (1977) proposed the Isl sound as thecritical guide to establishing the vertical dimension ofocclusion for edentulous patients.

The purpose of this investigation was to determinewhether the CSS occurs during the production of sibilantsounds. This was done by comparison of the smallestvertical components of speech-that is, the smallest ver-tical excursion of the mandible-produced during theperformance of three different phonetic exercises. A fur-ther objective was to establish the variability in the CSSduring the production of individual sibilant phonemes.

Materials and methods.Thirty undergraduate dental students in the School ofClinical Dentistry, Queen's University of Belfast, volun-

964

Page 2: Closest Speaking Space During the Production of Sibilant Sounds

CLOSEST SPEAKING SPACE AND SIBILANT SOUNDS

tarily took part in the investigation. There were 16 fe-males and 14 males, and their ages ranged from 20 to 23years. All had intact dentitions, except where extractionsfor orthodontic realignment had been performed or thirdmolars were missing. No subjects suffered from any speechdefect, and none reported any history or symptoms oftemporomandibular joint or muscular dysfunction. Therewas no restriction on participants with respect to skeletalor incisal relationships, and the horizontal and verticaloverlaps of the central incisor teeth were measured foreach individual.

Mandibular movement was measured by an electro-magnetic method of jaw tracking. The recording deviceused was a kinesiograph ( Sirognathograph, Siemens A.G.,Benshiem. Germany), which did not interfere with jawmovement and allowed a measurement of spatial dis-placements ofthe mandible to be made. In this technique,mandibular movements are recorded as changes in themagnetic field produced during the movement of a light-weight magnet (3 g) rigidly retained on the lower centralincisors bv an adhesive. In this investigation, SimplexRapid (Austenal Dental Products, Harrow, England), anauto-polymerizing acrylic resin, was used as the adhesive.All measurements recorded represented mandibular move-ment and were movements at the point of attachment ofthe magnet. in this case the lower central incisors. Aheadset which carried 8 magnetometers sensed the alter-ations in the magnetic field and transformed them intoelectrical potentials which were relayed directly to acomputer (Nimbus VX/2, Research Machines Limited,Oxford, England), which utilized a software system (Bio-Pak, BioResearch Associates Inc., Milwaukee, WI) torecord and display spatial co-ordinates in three planes,frontal, sagittal, and coronal, so permitting analysis ofmandibular movement. This system has been shownBalkhi and Tallents, 1991) to have linear output for up toalbout 40 mm ofvertical displacement ofthe magnet whenlateral movement is less than 10 mm, and these condi-tions were fulfilled in this study.

The CSS ofeach individual was determined during theiXeading of three exercises. The first phonetic test was asfolIows

How are you Tom Dope? and no oranges were grow-ing in Mexico and it is nice to see my grandfatherswim about here and George depends on Ruth tobake a big lemon cake and roses are red and violetsare blue and three lashings won't hurt poor Dick andTim show Harry where to wash your clothes tub andWednesday will be a laugh for all ofus and the sixty-five trucks leave the zoo each year and perhaps youn-eed to fire the man in England too and sweet PeggyNun measured the fur hat and the children weren't(catching anything.

This passage, containing 104 words, was specifically de-signed for use as a general phonetic articulation test andis closely modeled on that suggested by Kestenberg (1983)and formulated by the following criteria:

(1 The frequency of occurrence of the various consonantphonemes are approximately the same as their relativefrequency in spoken English.

(2 All the phonemes which occur in the English language

are used.(3) The words are kept as simple as possible and are all

very common or easily recognized.(4) The test is brief so that it can be performed quickly

(about 45-60 s).(5) A large number ofeach group of English consonants is

included for elimination of random error.(6) Most phonemes are used at the beginning, middle. and

end of at least one word.(7) Various blends (or co-articulations) of consonants are

included.(8) Each of the sibilant sounds is included.

The second speech exercise contained the six sibilantsounds in the form of a short sentence:

"The fish in the river Yes are if I am any judge themore difficult" said Buzz, "but I remember a dayafter church when I got their measure."

The third exercise contained six single words, eachcontaining a different sibilant phoneme in the same orderas the previous exercise (the sibilant phoneme expressedin each word is set in bold type):

FISHYESJUDGEBUZZCHURCHMEASURE

/shlIs!Ijl/lzl

/ch//zhl

From this exercise, it was possible to obtain an overallmean value for the CSS as well as individual measure-

E

a.L-(0-a)0

a1)

-1 0 1 2 3 4

Horizontal Overlap (mm)5 6 7

Fig. 1-Incisal overlap in vertical and horizontal planes forthe 30 subjects. The large 0 denotes coincidence of twosubjects' incisal relationships.

8. 0

6. * 0 0 *

* 0 * 0 0

4 0 0 0 0 0

* 0

2 60 0

0

0.

4.4

-6. . . . . .

V",l" 72 A"6, 965

Page 3: Closest Speaking Space During the Production of Sibilant Sounds

966 BURNETT& CLIFFORD

)5 10 15 20 25 30

4.

3.5

3.

2.5.

2.

1.5.

1.

.5

Subject Number

Fig. 2-Closest speaking space (CSS) for each subject ineach of the three phonetic tests [ - general articulationphonetic test; - single-sentence sibilant phonetic test; -

individual sibilant phonetic test].

ments of the CSS for each of the six sibilant sounds.The subjects were asked to recite the three speech

exercises at normal conversational rate and volume, andthey were given a few minutes to practice and becomefamiliar with them. Recordings were made with the sub-ject sitting and the head supported in an upright positionand the Frankfort horizontal plane parallel to the floor.The intercuspal position was recorded prior to the recita-tion, and the closest position to this, assumed by themandible during each speech exercise, represented theCSS. The three exercises were performed and recordedone after the other, and this sequence was repeated threetimes to give a mean value for the CSS for each exercise.The range ofmandibular movement during the reading ofthe general speech exercise was also recorded for eachsubject to give parameters of a speech envelope in thefrontal, sagittal, and coronal planes.

Statistical analysis ofthe data was performed by meansof a one-factor ANOVA test with repeated measures.Levels of p < 0.05 were considered to be statisticallysignificant.

Results.The 30 subjects of the investigation showed a wide varia-tion in their horizontal and vertical incisal relationships,as shown in Fig. 1. There was a range of 0 mm to 6 mm inthe horizontal plane and from a 5-mm anterior open biteto an 8-mm overlap in the vertical plane.

The dimensions of the speech envelope, described asthe mean (+ S.D.) of mandibular movement in threeplanes for the entire subject group as produced during thegeneral phonetic articulation test, were: frontal plane, 8.6± 1.7 mm (range = 4.5-11.1 mm); sagittal plane, 3.7 ± 1.3mm (range = 1.8-8.3 mm); and coronal plane, 2.1 ± 1.0 mm(range = 0.2-4.4 mm).

Discussion.Severeal phonetic tests are recommended in prosthodontictexts as guides to the occlusal vertical dimension. Thesetests are frequently based on the Is! sound produced in thepronunciation of the words "Mississippi" or "sixty-six", inrepeating the days of the week, or counting from 1 to 10.The aim of these tests is to permit detection of contactbetween teeth, or occlusal rims, which would indicate anexcessive occlusal vertical dimension. The vertical compo-nent of the CSS determined for the general phoneticarticulation test in this investigation had a mean value of2.0 mm with a range from 0.1 mm to 4.1 mm. Silverman(1951) described a CSS range of0-10mm for 208 patients,and from his results a mean of 2.1 mm can be calculated,although Silverman himself did not derive this value.Benediktsson (1958) documented the Is! position as hav-ing a range from 0 to 14 mm for 246 patients with a meanof 2.6 mm. Gillings (1973) determined the minimum jawopening for 22 young adults as they counted from 1 to 10as being 1.1 mm with a range from 0 to 3 mm. George(1983) recorded what he called a "near Is!" position, whichequates with the CSS, of 1.8 mm for 31 subjects who hadskeletal and occlusal Class I relationships. Howell (1986)published a value of 3.1 mm with a range of 0.0 mm to 8.0

TABLESUBJECT GROUP MEAN CLOSEST SPEAKING SPACE

(mm) FOR THE SIX SIBILANT PHONEMES

Mean S.D. Range

Fish /shb 2.6 + 1.3 0.1-6.0

Yes/sl 2.6 + 1.2 0.1-4.5

Judge /j/ 2.1 + 1.2 0.1-4.5

Buzz /z/ 2.3 + 1.2 0.1-4.0

Church /ch/ 2.3 + 1.2 0.1-4.0

Measure /zhb 2.6 + 1.5 0.1-6.8

0E3

AA al a

00 A

o o0A A

A A 0

0 0 a00A AA A

Aa00a 0

0 A 00 0

0A 0 a

0 A0

0 AD0 0 0A 0

A

0 030 a0 0 A 03 A

A 0 aA A

al 00 A ala A

Ea)C)

Q

CDQ

(nCOa)CD)

en00)

4.5

U

.

J Dent Res June 1993

The CSS determined for each ofthe 30 subjects for eachof the three exercises are shown in Fig. 2. The CSSdetermined in the three exercises for each individual wasseparated by less than 0.5 mm in 22 subjects. In a furtherfour subjects, the range was 0.75 mm, and the remainingfour subjects showed a range of 0.9-1.2 mm.

The CSS for the group of 30 subjects for the generalphonetic articulation test was 2.0 + 1.1 mm (mean + S.D.;range = 0.1-4.1 mm). In the single-sentence sibilant pho-netic test, CSS was 1.9 ± 1.1 mm (range = 0.2-4.0 mm), andthat of the individual sibilant phonetic test was 2.0 ± 1.2mm (range = 0.1-3.9 mm). No statistically significantdifference among the mean CSS obtained for the threeexercises was demonstrable.

The Table shows the CSS as elicited separately for eachof the six sibilants in the third exercise. The individualsibilant CSS were found to be statistically significantlydifferent from the CSS produced in each of the threecomplete exercises except with the phoneme /j/, which wasfound to be different from the second exercise only.

Page 4: Closest Speaking Space During the Production of Sibilant Sounds

CLOSEST SPEAKING SPACE AND SIBILANT SOUNDS

mnm for the mean CSS for his group of 95 subjects. Thework of Howell (1986) is unique in basing results on ageneral speech test and as such was not solely reliant onthe /s/ sound. The consensus from these studies wouldappear to support the clinical practice of providing a CSSof about 2 mm in complete denture construction. How-ever, it must be borne in mind that this is an averagevalue, and a range from zero to as high as 10 mm has beenreported (Silverman, 1951; Geissler, 1975).

The speech pattern described by the lower incisor teethfor the 30 subjects during the reading of the generalphonetic articulation test was remarkably similar to thatproduced by 95 subjects reading a standard passage oftext as a general articulation test, the so-called "RainbowPassage", in the work of Howell (1986), the only previousdocumentation of a general speech envelope.

The values ofthe mean CSS for 30 subjects recorded inthree separate phonetic exercises showed no significantstatistical difference. This suggests that the CSS deter-mined from a general phonetic articulation test contain-ing all speech sounds is likely to be the same as thatdetermined from a test containing only the sibilant sounds.It also indicates that the values for CSS determined froma long passage ofprose, a short sentence, and individuallyarticulated words each containing the sibilant soundswer-e uniform. The results imply that sibilant sounds doproduce the CSS, although it cannot be inferred that othersounds would not also produce a comparable CSS, sincethe sibilants were the only word sounds tested on anindividual basis.

Although the mean CSS determined for the 30 subjectsin the three, tests were similar, the CSS produced by aparticular sibilant sound varied from individual to indi-vidual. Thi's refutes previous suggestions that a singlesound, that, is, the /s/ sound, can be relied upon fordetermination of occlusal vertical dimension by the pho-netic method. For reliable determination of a particularindividtual.s CSS, it is necessary to use a phonetic testwhich covers the range of sibilant sounds. This investiga-

tion showed that it was unnecessary to have a subjectrecite long passages ofprose, since a phonetic test consist-ing of a short sentence or list containing all the sibilantword sounds could be satisfactorily used in determiningthe vertical dimension ofocclusion by the phonetic method.

REFERENCES

Balkhi KM, Tallents RT (19911. Error analysis of a jaw-trackingdevice. J Craniomandib Disord Facial Oral Paini 5:51-56.

Benediktsson E (1958). Variation in tongue and jaw position in "s'sound production in relation to front teeth occlusion. Acta Odloni -

tol Scand 15:275-303.Geissler PR (1975). Studies of mandibular movements in speech. J

Dent 3:256-260.George JP (1983). Using the kinesiograph to measure mandibular

movements during speech: A pilot study. JProsthet De;it 49:263-270.

Gillings BRD (1973). Jaw movements in young adult men duringspeech. J Prosthet Dent 29:567-576.

Howell PGT (1986). Incisal relationships during speech. J ProsthetDent 56:93-99.

Kestenberg JM (1983). Speech assessment in dentistry. Allst De7i t J28:98-101.

Mehringer EJ (1963). The use of speech patterns as anl aid inprosthodontic reconstruction. JProsthet Denit 13:825-836.

Morrison ML (1959). Phonetics as a method of determining verticaldimension and centric relation. JAmi Denit Assoc 59:690-695.

Murrell GA (1974). Phonetics, function and anterior occlusion. JProsthet Dent 32:23-31.

Pound E (1977). Let S/ be your guide. J Prostliet Deint 38:482-489.Silverman MM (1951). Accurate measurement of x ertical dimension

by phonetics and the speaking centric space. Part I. Denlt Digest57:261-265.

Silverman MM (1952). Vertical dimension must not be increased. JProsthet Dent 2:188-197.

West R, Kennedy L, CarrA (1947). The rehabilitation of speech. NewxYork: Harper & Brothers.

Vot1,-2 No. 967


Recommended