+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Creating the environment for driver distraction: A thematic framework … · 2019. 12. 16. · 1...

Creating the environment for driver distraction: A thematic framework … · 2019. 12. 16. · 1...

Date post: 27-Jan-2021
Category:
Upload: others
View: 1 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
43
1 Creating the environment for driver distraction: A thematic framework of sociotechnical factors Katie J. Parnell* a , Neville A. Stanton a , and Katherine L. Plant a *Corresponding author a Transportation Research Group, Faculty of Engineering and the Environment, Boldrewood Innovation Campus, University of Southampton, Burgess Road, Southampton, SO16 7QF, United Kingdom. Abstract As modern society becomes more reliant on technology, its use within the vehicle is becoming a concern for road safety due to both portable and built-in devices offering sources of distraction. While the effects of distracting technologies are well documented, little is known about the causal factors that lead to the drivers’ engagement with technological devices. The relevance of the sociotechnical system within which the behaviour occurs requires further research. This paper presents two experiments, the first aims to assess the drivers self-reported decision to engage with technological tasks while driving and their reasoning for doing so with respect to the wider sociotechnical system. This utilised a semi-structured interview method, conducted with 30 drivers to initiate a discussion on their likelihood of engaging with 22 different tasks across 7 different road types. Inductive thematic analysis provided a hierarchical thematic framework that detailed the self-reported causal factors that influence the drivers’ use of technology whilst driving. The second experiment assessed the relevance of the hierarchical framework to a model of distraction that was established from within the literature on the drivers use of distracting technologies while driving. The findings provide validation for some relationships studied in the literature, as well as providing insights into relationships that require further study. The role of the sociotechnical system in the engagement of distractions while driving is highlighted, with the causal factors reported by drivers suggesting the importance of considering the wider system within which the behaviour is occurring and how it may be creating the conditions for distraction to occur. This supports previous claims made within the literature based model. Recommendations are proposed that encourage a movement away from individual focused countermeasures towards systemic actors. Key words: In-vehicle technology, Driver distraction, Qualitative methods, Sociotechnical systems. 1. Introduction Technological developments are largely driven by industrial or commercial requirements which, Dorf (2001) claims, are harnessed by mankind to change or manipulate their environment. The driving environment has changed markedly through the implementation of technology over recent decades (Walker et al, 2001). This has had ramifications for the design and use of vehicles (Wierwille, 1993; Walker et al, 2001). Drivers now
Transcript
  • 1

    Creatingtheenvironmentfordriverdistraction:Athematicframeworkof

    sociotechnicalfactors

    KatieJ.Parnell*a,NevilleA.Stanton

    a,andKatherineL.Plant

    a

    *Correspondingauthor

    aTransportationResearchGroup,FacultyofEngineeringandtheEnvironment,BoldrewoodInnovation

    Campus,UniversityofSouthampton,BurgessRoad,Southampton,SO167QF,UnitedKingdom.

    Abstract

    Asmodernsocietybecomesmorereliantontechnology,itsusewithinthevehicleisbecomingaconcernfor

    roadsafetyduetobothportableandbuilt-indevicesofferingsourcesofdistraction.Whiletheeffectsof

    distractingtechnologiesarewelldocumented,littleisknownaboutthecausalfactorsthatleadtothedrivers’

    engagement with technological devices. The relevance of the sociotechnical system within which the

    behaviouroccursrequiresfurtherresearch.Thispaperpresentstwoexperiments,thefirstaimstoassessthe

    driversself-reporteddecisiontoengagewithtechnologicaltaskswhiledrivingandtheirreasoningfordoing

    so with respect to the wider sociotechnical system. This utilised a semi-structured interview method,

    conductedwith30driverstoinitiateadiscussionontheirlikelihoodofengagingwith22differenttasksacross

    7differentroadtypes.Inductivethematicanalysisprovidedahierarchicalthematicframeworkthatdetailed

    the self-reported causal factors that influence the drivers’ use of technologywhilst driving. The second

    experiment assessed the relevance of the hierarchical framework to a model of distraction that was

    established from within the literature on the drivers use of distracting technologies while driving. The

    findingsprovidevalidationforsomerelationshipsstudiedintheliterature,aswellasprovidinginsightsinto

    relationships that require further study. The role of the sociotechnical system in the engagement of

    distractions while driving is highlighted, with the causal factors reported by drivers suggesting the

    importanceof considering thewider systemwithinwhich thebehaviour is occurringandhow itmaybe

    creating theconditions fordistraction tooccur.This supportspreviousclaimsmadewithin the literature

    basedmodel.Recommendationsareproposedthatencourageamovementawayfromindividualfocused

    countermeasurestowardssystemicactors.

    Keywords:In-vehicletechnology,Driverdistraction,Qualitativemethods,Sociotechnicalsystems.

    1. Introduction

    Technologicaldevelopmentsarelargelydrivenbyindustrialorcommercialrequirementswhich,Dorf(2001)

    claims,areharnessedbymankindtochangeormanipulatetheirenvironment.Thedrivingenvironmenthas

    changedmarkedlythroughtheimplementationoftechnologyoverrecentdecades(Walkeretal,2001).This

    hashadramificationsforthedesignanduseofvehicles(Wierwille,1993;Walkeretal,2001).Driversnow

  • 2

    expect thedesignof thevehicle to include technological facilities thatenableentertainment,navigation,

    communication,connectivity,efficiencyandcomfortwhiledriving.Yet,thereisaneedtoensurethatthe

    implementationof such technologiesdoesnotadverselyaffect roadsafety (Leeetal,2008;Youngetal,

    2011).

    The distractive effects of hand-held phones have been evidenced,with adverse consequences to driver

    performancemetrics,suchasvehiclecontrol(Tsimhonietal,2004),attentiontunnelling(Reimer,2009),and

    hazarddetection(Summalaetal,1998)amongothers.Yet,despitebeingmadeawareoftherisksposedby

    mobile phoneswhile driving and legislation to ban their use acrossmany countries, drivers continue to

    engagewiththem(Dingusetal,2006;Lerneretal,2008;Walshetal,2008;Zhouetal,2012;Young&Lenné,

    2010;Metz et al, 2015; Tivesten&Dozza, 2015).While previous research has informed on the adverse

    consequencesofmobilephones,thecontextualandmotivationalfactorsthatleadtoengagementinother

    technological tasks isunder-researched (Young&Regan,2007;Youngetal,2008;Young&Lenné,2010;

    Tivesten&Dozza,2015;Horreyetal,2017).

    Some research has been conducted into the decisions that driversmake to engagewith distractions in

    simulators(Metzetal,2011;Schömig&Metz,2013),ontesttracks(Horrey&Lesch,2009)andthroughthe

    analysisofdataderivedfromnaturalisticstudies(Metzetal,2015;Tivesten&Dozza,2015).Achallengein

    the assessment of driver distraction research is the dichotomy between high levels of control and the

    naturalistic studyofbehaviour (Youngetal,2008), thus thebenefitsand limitationsof thesestudiesare

    inherenttothevalidityofthefindings.Whilesimulatorsoffercontroloverexternalvariables,suchasroad

    type and other road users, capturing realistic behaviour is compromised (Young et al, 2008). Yet, in

    naturalistic studies the focus of data collection is on the driver and their triggered engagement with

    secondary tasks as they allow very little control, and thusmeasurement of, the contextual factors that

    influence drivers’ engagement with secondary tasks (Metz et al, 2015). TheWorld Health Organisation

    (WHO)nowacknowledgesthesociotechnicalsystembasedapproachwhichidentifiesdriverbehaviour,not

    asaproductoftheindividual,butasaproductofsystemicelementssuchastheroadlayout,roaddesign,

    vehicledesign,andthecontextsurroundingthedrivingtask(WHO,2004).Despitethis,theapplicationof

    systemsbasederrormanagementapproacheshavebeenlargely ignored(Salmonetal,2010).Thecausal

    errortaxonomysuggestedbyStantonandSalmon(2009)statesfivekeyelementswithinthesociotechnical

    systemwhichinfluencetheconditionsthatleadtoerror;thedriver,thevehicle,roadinfrastructure,other

    roadusersandenvironmental conditions.Thus, it canbesuggested that thecauseofdistraction related

    errorsisnotlimitedtothedriver,insteaditisinfluencedbyamultitudeofothersystemicactors.

    Reviewingdistractionwiththesociotechnicalsystems‘riskmanagementframework’(RMF)developedby

    Rasmussen (1997) revealed the impact that hierarchical levels of the systemhave on the emergence of

    distraction.Actorswererevealedfromtheinternationalandnationalcommittees(Parnelletal,2017a)who

  • 3

    setthelawsthatareenforcedbylocalgovernmentsandregulatorsthatthenfeeddowntheframeworkto

    themanufacturersofdevicesandtheinteractiontheyhavewiththeenduser(Young&Salmon,2012;Parnell

    etal,2017a).Ratherthanfocusingonthedrivers’decisiontoengageastheinitiationoferror,thesystems

    approach gives an insight into the conditions through which the driver was permitted to engage with

    distractingtechnologiesandhowthisbehaviourinfluencestheemergenceofsafetywithinthesystemasa

    whole.Yet,appropriatemethodsarerequiredtoassessthesociotechnicalsystem(Youngetal,2013;Salmon

    etal,2017).

    Inafirstattempttoassessandmodeldriverdistractionfromasociotechnicalsystemsapproach,Parnellet

    al(2016)developedthePARRC(Priority,Adapt,Resource,Regulate,Conflict)modelofdistraction,thefirst

    model of the behaviour to account for the contribution of systemic factors. This encompasses five key

    mechanismsthroughwhichin-vehicletechnologymayleadtodistractionacrossthesociotechnicalsystem,

    including‘goalpriority’,‘adapttodemand’,‘resourceconstraints’,‘behaviouralregulation’and‘goalconflict’

    (Parnell et al, 2016). The PARRC model was developed through grounded theory methodology which

    determined thekey factors involved in theemergenceofdistractionasevolved from the literature. The

    interconnections made between these mechanisms were shown to influence how distraction related

    behaviouremergedfromthesystem,aswellastherelevanceofothersystemicactorsonthemechanisms.

    Readers are directed to Parnell et al (2016) for further information. Application of the PARRC model

    mechanismstoanAccimapanalysissuggestedhowactorsinthesystemmaybepreventingtheemergence

    ofdistractionorconverselyleavingthesystemopentodistraction(Parnelletal,2017a).Thishighlightedthe

    roleoflegislation,developedthroughinternationalandnationalcommitteesthatisthenenforcedthrough

    national laws, that targets hand-held mobile phone use but is more ambiguous on the use of other

    technologies.Theambiguityinlegislationwasshowntohaveledtotheadvancementoftechnologiesand

    theirimplementationwithinthevehicle,despitealackofevidencetosuggestthemtobesaferthanhand-

    heldmobilephones(Parnelletal,2017a).Yet,themechanismsofthePARRCmodelweredrawnfromthe

    literatureusinggroundedtheoryandthereforerequirevalidationthroughtheirapplicationtootherdata

    sources,methodsand/or investigatorsthroughtheprocessoftriangulation(Hignett,2005;Raffertyetal,

    2010).

    Thispaperseekstogaindatafromdriversontheirselfreportedreasonsforengagingwithtechnologywhile

    driving. Questionnaires and online surveys have strived to gather responses on drivers’ frequency of

    engagingwithdistractionsandtheirviewsontherisksindoingso(e.g.McEvoyetal,2006;Young&Lenné,

    2010;Walshetal,2008;Zhouetal,2009;Zhouetal,2012;).Yet,theyareoftenprescriptive,posingclosed

    questionsthatmaylimitthedatatotheagendaoftheresearcher(O’Cathain&Thomas,2004).Instead,the

    causalfactorsthatdriversdeemtoinfluencetheirdecisiontoengagewithdistractions,andhowthismay

    resultindistractionrelatedevents,isofinterest(Youngetal,2008;Young&Lenné,2010;Lee,2014).The

    first experiment within this paper sought to obtain the drivers self-reported reasons for engaging with

  • 4

    technology while driving using a semi-structured interview method to engage drivers in open-ended

    discussionsonwhytheymaybemore,orless,likelytoengagewithvarioustypesoftechnologywhiledriving.

    The inductive thematic analysis thatwas applied during the data analysis aimed to develop factors that

    driversthemselvesdeemto influencetheirengagementwithtechnologicaltasks.Thesecondexperiment

    aimedtoassesshowthecausalfactorsderivedfromthedriversintheinterviewstudyrelatedtothecausal

    factorsthatweredevelopedfromtheliteratureinthedevelopmentofthePARRCmodel(Parnelletal,2016).

    Thismodelisusedforitsabilitytoassessthesociotechnicalsystemsurroundingthebehaviour(Parnelletal,

    2016;2017a).Thefindingsseektoassistintheprovisionofcountermeasuresthattargetthesourceofthe

    issue,ratherthanobservingwithhindsighttheeffectsofdistraction.

    2.Experiment12.1Aim

    This experiment aimed to understand the drivers self-reported reasons for engaging with technological

    deviceswhiledrivingandtheinvolvementofthesociotechnicalintheirdecision-makingprocess.Previous

    researchhassoughttocapturethedrivers’useoftechnologiesusingquestionnairesandonlinesurveys,yet

    thisstudyaimstocapturethedrivers’subjectiveperspectiveintheirownwords.Thiswillinvolvetheuseof

    semi-structured interviews toelicitdiscussionswithdriverson their likelihoodofengagingwithdifferent

    technologicaltasksacrossdifferentroadtypes.

    2.2.Method

    2.2.1Participants

    DriverswithexperienceofUKroadswerespecifiedastheroadtypesincludedwithinthesemi-structured

    interviewsrelatedtothosecomprisingtheUKroadwaysystem(Walkeretal,2013).Atotalof30participants

    wererecruited(15females,15males),acrossthreeagecategories(18-30,31-49,50-65),withfivefemales

    andfivemalesineachcategory.ParticipantswererecruitedundertherequirementthattheyheldafullUK

    drivinglicenseandhadaminimumof1-yearsexperiencedrivingonUKroads(meanyearsexperience=19.5,

    SD=13.08).Theywerealsorequiredtobefrequentdrivers,drivingonaregularweeklybasisinorderforthem

    tobeexposedtosituationswheretheymaybeinclinedtoengagewithtechnology(meanhoursspentdriving

    aweek=9hrs45min,SD=6hrs20mins).Participationwasvoluntary.

    2.2.2Datacollection

    To obtain the drivers own views on why they engage with technological devices while driving, semi-

    structuredinterviewswereconducted.Semi-structuredinterviewshavebeenusedeffectivelytoinvestigate

    otheraspectsofdrivingbehaviour(Simon&Corbett,2007;Gardner&Abraham,2007;Tonetto&Desmet,

    2016),buttheyhavenotbeenappliedtostudyhowdriverdistractionisviewedbydrivers.Theirapplication

    within this research allowed for open-ended questions that enabled drivers to generate concepts they

  • 5

    deemedimportanttotheiruseoftechnologicaldeviceswhilstfacilitatingastructureddatacollectionmethod

    that could be reliably applied across all interviewees. They also allowed the researcher to probe into

    interestingconceptsastheyarose(Cohen&Crabtree,2006).

    The interviewswere structured around a table that encouraged the driver to discuss their likelihood of

    engagingwitharangeofdifferenttechnologicaltaskswhiledrivingacrossdifferentUKroadtypesinorder

    toprovideadiscussionsurroundingthesituationsandenvironmentswhichmayinfluencetheuseofdifferent

    technologicaldevices.Table1presentsthelistoftechnologicaltasksposedtoparticipantsintheinterview.

    Thetasksweredrawnfromthecurrentliteratureinvestigatingdistractionfromin-vehicletechnology(e.g.

    Young&Lenné,2010;Nealeetal,2005;McEvoyetal,2006;Harveyetal,2011a),aswellasreportsfrom

    roadsafetyorganisationsandpolicereports(RAC,2016;DepartmentforTransport,2015a).Theroadtypes

    presentedhavebeenshowninpreviousresearchtoinfluencedrivers’situationalawareness(Walkeretal,

    2013)andcrashrates(Bayliss,2009).Theyincludedmotorways,majorA/Broads,urbanroads,ruralroads,

    residential roads and junctions. Participants were presented with a road type classification sheet with

    definitions,imagesandcontextualinformationrelatingtoeachoftheroadtypesforclarity.

    Table1.Listoftechnologies/specifictasksthatdriverswereaskedtoratetheirlikelihoodofengagingwith.

    Technology Task

    NavigationsystemMonitorroute

    Enterdestination

    Hands-freesystem

    Findnumberfromaddressbook

    Answeracall

    Talktoother

    In-vehiclesystem

    Changeclimatecontrol

    Changesong/radiostation

    Adjustvolume

    Listentomusic

    Verballycommunicatewithin-builtsystem

    Mobilephone/Portabledevice

    Enterdestinationintonavigationapp

    Monitornavigationapp

    Write/sendatext

    Readatext

    Answerphonecall

    Talkonthephone

    Enter/Findanumber

    Changesong/audiotrack

    Usevoiceassistfeatures

    Takeaphoto

    Usesocialmediaapps

    Checkyouremail

    Participantswerefreetogeneratetheirownreasoningbehindwhytheymayormaynotengagewitharange

    ofdifferenttechnologicaltaskswhiledrivingacrosstheroadtypes.Theresearcherprobedtheparticipantto

  • 6

    expandontheirdiscussionpoints forclarityandfurther informationwherenecessary.Thesameprimary

    researcherconductedtheinterviewswithallparticipantsforconsistency.

    Apilotstudywasconductedtodetermineifthetechnologicaltaskswererepresentativeofthoseusedby

    driversandtoestablishagreementonthedescriptionsoftheUKroadtypes.Thisrevealedanoverlapinsome

    of thetechnological tasks, suchassearching forapointof interestandadestination inasat-nav. Italso

    revealedthatwhendriversdiscussedtheirbehaviouratajunctiontheyseemedtodifferintheirdiscussions

    surrounding the use of technologywhen stopped at a junction e.g. at traffic lights compared to driving

    throughanintersection.Therefore,thejunctionroadtypewassplitintwotorepresentbothdrivingthrough

    ajunctionandstoppedatajunction.

    Theinterviewslastedapproximately30minutesalthoughthisvarieddependingonthediscussionsengaged

    bytheparticipantandtheresearcher(average=34.21mins,SD=14.07).Interviewswereaudiorecordedand

    transcribed. Due to the sensitive content, i.e. if they were likely to engage in an activity that may be

    consideredillegalunderUKlawssuchasusingamobilephonewhiledriving,confidentialitywasensuredto

    allow theparticipant to talkopenly.The interviewstudywasapprovedby the research institutesEthical

    ResearchandGovernanceOrganisation(ERGOreference:24937).

    2.2.3Dataanalysis

    Transcriptionsoftheinterviewsprovidedthedatasetfromwhichtoanalyseanddrawinferencesonthe

    causalfactorsthatdriversreportedtoinfluencetheirdecisiontoengageinthetechnologicaltaskswhile

    drivinganddeterminetherelationshipofthefactorstothewidersociotechnicalsystem.Thematicanalysis

    wasusedtoorganise,analyseandinterpretkeythemeswithinthedata(Braun&Clarke,2006).Atheme

    wasdefinedas“…somethingimportantaboutthedatainrelationtotheresearchquestion,andrepresents

    somelevelofpatternedresponseormeaningwithinthedataset”(Braun&Clarke,2006;p10).The

    flexibilityofthemethodisbothanadvantage,infacilitatingadaptabilityacrossdifferentapproaches,anda

    limitationduetocommentsoflimitedrigourfromunclearmethodologies(Braun&Clarke,2006).Yet,

    BraunandClarke(2006)commentthatwithclearlydefinedmethodsandcommentary,thematicanalysis

    canbeahighlyusefultoolindrawingmeaningfromqualitativedata.Themethodologyappliedtothedata

    setisthereforegivenindetailwithinthispaper.

    ThethematiccodingprocesswasconductedinNvivo11softwaretoaddrigourtothequalitativeresearch

    (Richards&Richards,1991;Welsh,2002).Italsoassistedinreviewingthesub-themes(seeExperiment2),

    andallowedforqueriestoberunonthecodeddatatointerrogatethecodesaftertheyweredeveloped.

  • 7

    2.2.3.1Inductivethematiccoding

    Thethematicanalysisutiliseda‘bottom-up’approach,wherebycontentwascodedwithoutapre-existing

    framework, rather the frameworkdevelopedthroughtheanalysisof thedata (Boyatzis,1998).Thus, the

    themesthatdevelopedwerestronglylinkedtothesourceofthedata(Patton,1990).Incontrasttodeductive

    thematicanalysis,whichseekstolookataspectsofthedatathatrelatetotheresearchframeworkunder

    investigation, inductive analysis provides a richer insight into the data set as a whole, using naturally

    occurring themes (Braun & Clarke, 2006). It was not desirable to impose a framework on the drivers’

    verbalisations,butratherdrawontheconceptsthattheparticipantsdeemedtobeimportant.Braunand

    Clarke(2006)statethattheclarityofthemethodologyusedtodevelopthematiccodes isessential to its

    validity, which led to the development of their own guidelines on conducting thematic coding. Their

    guidelineswerefollowedandtheprocessofapplyingthemtothedatacollectedfromtheinterviewsisshown

    inFigure1.Thesameprimaryresearcherthatconductedtheinterviewsalsoconductedthethematicanalysis,

    astheywerefullyimmersedinthedataset.

    Figure1.Stagesoftheinductivethematicanalysis.

    Theiterativenatureofinductivethematicanalysismeantthattheinitial,descriptive,subthemeswerecoded

    asmultiple individualconceptstodrawasmuchinformationfromthetranscriptsaspossible(Stages1-3,

    Figure1).Thesewerecodedinthedriversownwords,in-vivo,tostaytruetothedata(Richie&Lewis,2003)

    If framework does not fit data set return to further review and refine codes.

    Stage 1Collect data - Interviews

    Stage 2Transcribe data and familiarisation with data set

    The primary researcher transcribed approximately 33% of interviews with the remainder sent to a transcription agency. The primary researcher spent time reading through all transcripts before coding

    Stage 3Generate initial descriptive codes

    The primary researcher conducted all interviews for consistency

    Initial codes were generated based on the descriptions of the responses using the drivers own words where applicable. Braun and Clarke’s (2006) advice was followed: • Code as many themes as possible• Include context surrounding each excerpt• Allow exerts to be coded at more than one theme

    Stage 4Review and collate themes into semantic themes

    Stage 5Review and collate themesinto systemic themes

    Re-read the the data extracts coded to each of the initial ‘descriptive’ themes generated at stage 3 and review to assess their coherence. Collate themes on their semantic meaning.

    Once the descriptive and semantic themes are set, review for patterns across the themes that relate to systemic elements. This requires some interpretation.

    Stage 6Contrast themes to data set as a whole

    Go back to the initial data set with the developed thematic structure to observe its fit with the data set as a whole and code any further extracts that may have been missed.

    DescriptionStageOutput

    Audio recordings of the interviews with all participants

    Complete transcriptions of all the interviews. Ready for input into Nvivo 11.

    Extensive list of initial descriptive codes

    Revised list of descriptive codes aggregated into semantic themes

    Organised list of descriptive and semantic themes with systemic high-level themes that identify the patterns of the data

    Refined theoretical framework encompassing the patterns observed in the data set

    Stage 7Define and name themes appropriately

    Review content coded to each theme to construct a meaningful definition of each theme and what it represents in the data. A codebook was generated encompassing the descriptive, semantic and systemic themes.

    Clearly defined themes and subthemes that are easily identifiable and collated into a codebook.

  • 8

    and allow an emerging framework to reflect the real-life experiences and thought processes of drivers

    (Tonetto&Desmet,2016).Themultipledescriptivethemeswerethenanalysed,organisedandrefinedinto

    semanticthemes(Stage4).Thisrequiredsomeinterpretationandinductivetheorisingonthesignificanceof

    conceptsandtheirbroadermeaning(Patton,1990).

    2.2.3.2Inter-raterreliabilitytest

    Allowingothers,independenttotheresearchproject,toapplythecodethathasbeenappliedtoadataset

    isausefulwayofassessingthereliabilityofthecoding(Boyatzis,1998).Totestthecodingframework10%

    oftheparticipants’transcriptswererandomlychosenforinter-raterreliabilitytesting.Twocolleagueswithin

    the Human Factors research team were recruited to test inter-rater reliability. They were given the

    codebook, which presented the full thematic framework (see the Online Appendix), during a 45minute

    trainingbriefingwheretheinterviewstudywasexplainedandthethemesweredescribedbeforetheywere

    askedtoindependentlycodethesame10%ofthetranscripts.Theinter-raterswereonlyrequiredtocodeat

    thesemanticlevel,whilereferencingthelowerleveldescriptivethemestoaidtheircoding.Nvivo11software

    wasusedfortheinter-ratercodingwiththeresearcherscodinghidden.Percentageagreementwasusedto

    assessthereliabilityscoresoftheinter-raters incontrasttotheresearchers initialcoding.Thismethodis

    widelyappliedininter-raterreliabilitystudies(Boyatzis,1998;Plant&Stanton,2013)andwhilethereare

    stillnoestablishedstandardsontheacceptablelevelofagreementbetweenraters,Boyatzis(1998)deems

    70%agreementasanecessary levelofagreement.Bothratersreachedagreementpercentagesoverthis

    level(rater1=81.24%,rater2=74.87%).Thusindicatingthattheraterswereabletousetheframeworkto

    codethedataata level thatwasmuchhigherthanchance,attestingtothereliabilityof theresearchers

    codingandtheapplicationofthethematicframework.

    2.3Results

    Inductivethematicanalysisresultedinthedevelopmentofahierarchicalframeworkofthemesthatreflected

    thedrivers’self-reportedlikelihoodtoengageineachofthetechnologicaltasksoneachoftheroadtypes.

    AnoverviewofthehighlevelsystemicandsemanticthemesisshowninTable2.Thefullthematicframework,

    includingthedescriptive,semanticandsystemiclevelsthatweredevelopedthoughtheprocess(shownin

    Figure1)ispresentedintheOnlineAppendix.

  • 9

    Semanticsubthemes Description ExampleQuote

    Driver Referencesmadebythedriverincludingtheirmental/physiologicalstate,experience,knowledge,skills,abilitiesandcontext-relatedbehaviour

    (D1)Attitudeofdriver

    Negative:Referencetonegativeattitudesofthedrivertowardsperformingthetaskwhiledriving “Because,Ijustthinkitistheworstthingintheworld,Ijustwouldn’tdoit,it’sterrible”Positive:Referencetopositiveattitudesofthedrivertowardsperformingthetaskwhiledriving “Idon’tseeanyproblemwithitpersonallywhatsoever”

    Unnecessary:Referencetothedriverperceivingthetasktobeunnecessarytoperformwhiledriving “That’ssomething–it’sjustsomethingthatcanwaituntilwhenyougethomeIthink”

    (D2)Tendency Referencetothedriversstatedtendencytoperformthetaskinthepastand/orthefutureasanindicatoroftheirlikelihoodtoengage “Ihavebeenknowntodothat”

    (D3)Viewofself Referencetothedriversstatedviewofthemselvesandtheirownbehaviouraltendencieswhenstatingtheirlikelihoodtoperformthetask ‘ifIamstoppedIgenerallyamalittlebitmorenaughty”

    (D4)Influenceofothers Referencetootherpeopleandtheirinfluenceonthedriverandtheirlikelihoodofperformingthetaskwhiledriving “theshameifyoudidsomethingbad,thateveryonewouldthinkyouaresostupid”Infrastructure Referencetothespecificroadtypewithintheroadtransportsystem,includingthelayout,contents,policy,andregulatedconditions

    (I1)Perceptionsofsurroundingenvironment

    Referencetothecontextsurroundingtheroadenvironmentofaspecificroadtypethatisinterpretedasaninfluencingfactorinthelikelihoodtoperformthetaskinthespecificroadenvironment

    “fortheseroadsandjunctions,itwouldrequirealotmoreconcentration”

    (I2)RoadLayout Referencetofeaturesofthespecificfixedroadenvironmentthatinfluencethedriver’slikelihoodtoperformthetask“becausetomeamotorway,onceyouareonit,itisallmovinginthesamedirectiongenerally”

    (I3)Illegality Referencetothelegislationontheuseofthetaskwhiledriving “Iusuallyholditinalowposition,sothepolicecan’tsee”

    (I4)Task-roadrelationshipReferencetotheinteractionbetweenfeaturesoftheroadandthetaskthatinfluencehowthetwomaybecompatiblesuchthatthelikelihoodofperformingthetaskisinfluenced.

    “Yeahitwouldbestilted,Iwouldprobablymakethepersononthephoneaware,sayhangonaminutebutIwouldprobablysoundnotasengage”

    (I5)RoadrelatedbehaviourReferencetotheactionsandresponsesthataretypicalorrequiredofthespecificroadtypewhichinfluencesthelikelihoodofperformingthetaskondifferentroads.

    “urbanroadIthinkismorebusyaswellsoIthinkthemoresortofdecisionsyou’vegottomake”

    Task Referencetothedetailssurroundingthespecifictaskandengagementwithit

    (T1)Complexity Referencetothedifficultyoreaseofperformingthetaskwhiledriving “ifyouhavetounlockthephonescreenorwhateveretc.,itisnotassimple–wellitisquitedistracting”

    (T2)Interaction Referencetophysicalfeaturesofthetaskthatrelatetotheinteractionrequiredtoperformthetaskwhiledriving.Thisrelatestotheinterfacedesign,devicelocationanddriverrequiredactionsinordertoengagewiththetask.“It’sonlyonebuttontopress,sothat’snotanissue”

    (T3)Duration Referencetothetimeand/orlengthofthetask “ifit’salongtextyoumightnotreadit”

    (T4)Desirability Referencetofeaturesofthetaskthatinfluencehowdesirableitmaybetoperformwhiledriving.Thismayincludeitsuse,performanceorqualityandoptionsforalternativemethodsofcompletingthetask.“Idon’treallyusemyphoneverymuchanywaysoit’sneverbeensomethingthatIhavefeltIhaveneeded”

    (T5)EngagementregulationReferencetothefactorsthatinfluencetheconditionssurroundingtheonsetofthetask.Theymayrelatetothephysicality’softhetaskand/orthedrivers’motivationrelatingtothetask.

    “IwillalwaysfigureoutwhatI’mgoingtolistentoandsetitgoingbeforeIleave”

    (T6)Abilitytocomplete Referencetofeaturesofthetaskwhichinfluenceitsabilitytobecompletedinfullwhiledriving “becauseI’vehadthecarforages,Iknowwheretheswitchesare”

  • 10

    Table2.Thesystemsandsemanticsubthemesofthethematicframework.

    Semanticsubthemes Description ExampleQuote

    Context Referencetothecircumstancessurroundingthebehaviourdescribed

    (C1)JourneyContext Referencetocircumstancesthatformthesettingforajourneythatmayinfluencethelikelihoodtoperformthetask.“ifIaminastrangecity,IwouldbelesslikelytomessaroundbecauseIdon’tknowwhereIamgoing”

    (C2)TaskContext Referencetocircumstancesthatformthesettingfortheuseofthetaskthatinfluencethedrivers’likelihoodtoengagewithit.

    “It’sstuffwhenIactuallyfeellikeIneedtosendamessagequickly,soifI’veagreedtocomehomeatacertaintimeandI’mrunninglateforinstance”

    (C3)Roadcontext Referencetocircumstancesthatformthesettingsurroundingtheroadingeneral(notrelatedtospecificinfrastructure)thatinfluencethelikelihoodtoperformthetask“Ithinkitwouldbesituationaldependent,justhowbusyisit?Ithink”

  • 11

    ItisevidentfromthefulltablepresentedintheAppendixthattherewasanextensivelistofreasonsthat

    drivers gave for engaging, or not engaging, with the technological tasks while driving. A total of 168

    descriptive themes were iteratively generated and revised into 18 semantic thematic categories. The

    generationofthesethemeswasalengthybutin-depthprocess.Clusteringthesesemanticthemesintohigher

    level systemic actors that contribute to the occurrence of the causal factors gave another level to the

    frameworkthatreadilydemonstratesthecontributionofthesystemwithinwhichdriverdistractionoccurs.

    Inlinewithpreviousindividualfocusedapproaches,thedriveremergedasakeyactor.Thedrivercategory

    suggests that the driver is influenced by their own attitudes (D1), perceptions of themselves (D3) and

    tendencies(D2)intheirengagementwithdistractionswhiledriving,aswellashowtheyfeeltheymaybe

    viewedbyothers(D4).Yet,thedevelopmentoftheothercategoriessuggeststhattheyarealsoinfluenced

    byothersystemicactors.

    Theroleofinfrastructurewasalsoreportedwhenrespondingtothedifferentroadtypesthatwereposedto

    thedriverduringthesemi-structuredinterviews.Theirperceptionsoftheroadenvironment(I1)alteredhow

    likelytheywouldbetoengageduetotherequirementsofthedrivingtaskintheseconditionse.g.increased

    concentrationrequiredatjunctionsorreducedperceptionofriskonmotorways.Roadlayoutacrossroad

    types(I2)wasalsowidelydiscussedwiththediscussionofcorners,roadturnings,androadvisibilitystated

    aselementscontributingtothedecisiontoengage.Driversalsomadeconnectionsbetweentheroadand

    theirbehaviouronit(I5),aswellasbetweenthetaskanditsuseinrelationtotheroad(I4).Forexample,the

    speedofparticularroadsortheavailabilityofplacestostopwasdiscussedascontributingtotheirdecision

    to engage while driving. Furthermore, the behaviour required in the driving task was also reported to

    influence their ability to engage in the different secondary taskswhile driving, for example driving on a

    motorwaywasdeemedtobeeasierbysomedriverswhichmotivatedtheirengagementinmorecomplex

    secondarytasks,thaniftheywereinamorecomplexroadenvironment.Illegality(I3)wasathemethatis

    alsomentionedbydriversandisincludedintheroadinfrastructuretheme,asitisinStantonandSalmon

    (2009). Interestingly, the lawwasonlyoneof the168other factors thatdriversstated to influence their

    decision to engage. This highlights the potential for the development of other techniques to tackle the

    numerousothercontributingfactors,asafearofthelawwasonlyasmallcontributiontothedriversself-

    reportedcausalfactors.

    Thetaskitselfgenerateddiscussiononhowitinfluencedthelikelihoodofthedrivertoengagewithitwhile

    driving.Notably,thisincludedhowlongthetaskwouldtaketocomplete(T3),themethodthroughwhich

    theycouldinteractwithit(T2),thecomplexityofthisinteraction(T1),itsdesirability(T4),theabilityforthe

    task tobecompleted (T6)aswell ashow itsonsetmaybe regulated (T5). The task themehas themost

    semantic subthemes,highlighting thenumberof variables relating to the task that influence thedrivers’

    engagement with it. It was evident that drivers were aware of the differing complexities and ways of

    interactingwiththedifferenttechnologicaltasksandhowtheycouldmanagethesewhiledriving.Therole

  • 12

    of the manufacturer in facilitating engagement and the influence of developments in Human Machine

    Interface(HMI)designconceptswasparticularlyevidentthroughoutthesediscussions.

    Theothersystemiccategorywasthewidercontextwithinwhichdriversdiscussedtheirengagementwith

    thetechnologicaltasks.Theydiscussedthetypeofjourney(C1)thattheymaybegoingonthatmayrequire

    themtoengagewiththetaskmore.Forexample,theuseofasat-navwasmorelikelyonalongerjourney

    whentheydidn’tknowwheretheyweregoingoraphonecallmaybemorelikelyiftheywerecommuting.

    The context of the task itself (C2)was also discussedwith the importance of the task to their priorities

    referenced frequently. Notably, when discussing phone based tasks drivers reporting who was

    communicatingwiththemandtheirperceivedimportanceofthecommunicationtogreatlyinfluencetheir

    engagementwiththetask.

    2.4Discussion

    ThedevelopmentofthethematicframeworkinTable2isthefirstattempttodevelopanextensivelistofthe

    drivers self-reported reasons for engagingwith technological tasks. Previous efforts to assess the causal

    factorsofdrivererrorhavesuggestedthatthekeyfactorsrelatetothefollowingsystemicelements:road

    infrastructure,thevehicle,thedriver,otherroadusersandenvironmentalconditions(Stanton&Salmon,

    2009; Salmon et al, 2010). Thus far, the development of error taxonomies has been heavily theoretical,

    emergingfromtheaggregationofpreviousliteratureandaccidentreports(e.g.Stanton&Salmon,2009).

    Thehierarchicallevelsoftheframeworkthatwereinductivelygeneratedgaveaninsightintothehigherlevel

    factorsthatarecloselytiedtothedriversowncommentsanddiscussions.Thehigh-levelfactorssuggestthe

    importanceofthedriver,theroadinfrastructure,thetaskandthewidercontextonthedrivers’decisionto

    usethetechnologicaltaskswhiledriving.DifferencestothecausalfactorstaxonomypresentedbyStanton

    andSalmon, (2009) includeamorespecific focusonthetask in thethematic frameworkrather thanthe

    vehicleasawhole.Thisislikelyduetothedesignandaimsofthestudywhichrequireddriverstotalkthrough

    theirlikelihoodofengagingwithavarietyofdifferenttechnologicaltasks.Hadtheparticipantsbeenasked

    todriveortalkmoreabouttheinteractionbetweencompletingthetaskwhiledriving,morereferencesto

    thevehiclemayhaveemerged(Pedic&Ezrakhovich,1999).‘Otherroadusers’isalsoabsentfromthehigh-

    level themes of the thematic framework, but does appear within the infrastructure theme under the

    ‘perceptionsofsurroundingenvironment’semanticsubtheme(seetheOnlineAppendix).Thiscouldsuggest

    thatthedrivers’viewofotherroadusersistightlylinkedtoinfrastructureandthesurroundingenvironment

    whendecidingtoengagewithtechnologicaltasks.Otherresearchconductedinanaturalisticdrivingstudy

    has suggested that other vehicle in front of thedriver donot influencedrivers’ decision to engagewith

    technologicaltasks(Tivesten&Dozza,2015).

    Whiletherewasanevidentinvolvementofsystemicactorsthatinfluencedengagement,thedriveremerged

    asakeysystemicthemeduetothereferencesthatparticipantsmadetotheirattitudes,perceptionsand

  • 13

    viewsof engagingwith the technological tasks. This compliments other research that utilised surveys to

    identifythatdrivers’intentiontoengageisstronglyinfluencedbytheirattitudetowardsthebehaviourand

    theirperceived riskof the task (Welshetal,2008;Zhouetal, 2012).Thevoluntaryaspectofdistraction

    (Beanland et al, 2013) and its self-regulatory association (Tivesten & Dozza, 2015) are inherent to the

    behaviour, yet this should not be studied independently to the wider context and system within the

    behaviouroccurs(Young&Salmon,2015;Parnelletal,2016).

    Theroad infrastructurewasdiscussedextensively leading tomultiple themeswithin the framework.This

    compliments theresearchconducted inanaturalisticstudybyTivestenandDozza (2015)whofoundthe

    drivers’abilitytoanticipatetheroadinfrastructure,suchastightcornersorstraightroads, influencesthe

    drivers’engagementwithdistractingtasks.Whiletheysuggestedthatotherroadconditionsdidnotinfluence

    thedrivers’engagementastheycouldnotbeanticipated,thefindingsfromthisinterviewstudysuggestthat

    road environment and the relationship between the task and the road is discussed as a causal factor in

    engagementandthusdriverscananticipatetheeffectismayhaveontheirdrivingperformance,withinthe

    interviewsetting.Otherthemesintheframeworkhavealsobeensuggestedintheliteraturesuchastask

    context(Lerneretal,2008),taskcapabilities(Zhouetal,2009),journeycontextandtheinfluenceofothers

    (Tivesten&Dozza,2015).Yet,theaggregationoffactorsthatwereinductivelygeneratedfromasampleof

    driversisnovelandhasstrongtheoreticalapplications.

    Theextensiverangeofcausalfactorswithintheframeworkincludesthecontributionof legislationtothe

    drivers’decisiontoengage,yetthereareahostofothercontributingfactorsthatsuggestthepotentialfor

    other measures through which to tackle the drivers’ engagement with technologies that can lead to

    distractionrelatedevents.ThePARRCmodelofdistraction(Parnelletal,2016)highlightstherelevanceof

    systemic actors to the causal factors that are attributed to driver distraction in the literature. The

    developmentofthethematicframeworkinExperiment1offersthepossibilitytocontrastthecausalfactors

    thatdriversreportintheinterviewstudytothosethatarereportedintheliterature.Furthermore,thisoffers

    theopportunitytodeterminethefurtheravenuesfordistractionmitigation,asthePARRCmodelhassought

    toachieveinthepast(Parnelletal,2016;17).ThisisexploredwithinExperiment2.

    3.Experiment23.1.Aim

    The inductive thematic analysis conducted in Experiment1allowed the causal factors that influence the

    drivers’likelihoodofengagingwithtechnologiestobedirectlylinkedtothedrivers’discussions.ThePARRC

    modelofdistractionsoughtcausalfactorsdirectlyfromtheliteratureusinggroundedtheorymethodology

    (Parnell et al, 2016). Yet, like the thematic framework developed in Experiment 1, it highlights the

    involvementofthewidersociotechnicalsysteminthedevelopmentofdistraction.TheaimofExperiment2

    willbetodeterminetherelationofthedriversreportstotheclaimsmadeintheliteraturebyapplyingthe

  • 14

    thematicframeworkinTable2tothePARRCmodelofdistractiondevelopedinParnelletal(2016).Thiswill

    seektoassessthevalidationofthePARRCmodelthroughtriangulationwith itsapplicationtoalternative

    datasources(Hignett,2005).Itwillalsoassessifthereareconceptsthatarereportedbythedriversthat

    havenotbeenstudiedintheliterature.Itthereforeseekstopromotefutureresearchaswellasproviding

    sociotechnicalsystemsrecommendationstothemitigationofdistraction,ashasbeenachievedwithprevious

    applicationsofthePARRCmodel(Parnelletal,2016;2017b).

    3.2.Method

    The PARRCmodel of distraction (Parnell et al, 2016) was reviewed to assess how the literature-driven

    mechanisms relate to causal factors stated by drivers. The process through which this was achieved is

    detailedinFigure2.

    Figure2.StagesintheapplicationofthethematicframeworktothePARRCmodelofdistraction.

    The relationship of the semantic causal factors stated by drivers to the key factors identified from the

    literatureinthePARRCmodelwasreviewedthroughdiscussionswithsubjectmatterexpertswithover40

    yearsofHumanFactorsexperience(Stage1,Figure2).Therelationshipofthesemanticfactorstosystemic

    Stage 1Code the thematic framework to the PARRC model at the semantic level using Nvivo11.

    Stage 2Asses the interconnections made between the PARRC mechanisms across the causal framework using a matrix query in Nvivo11.

    The semantic themes developed from the thematic coding of the interview transcripts were coded to the key mechanisms of the PARRC model; ‘Goal priority’, ‘Adapt to demand’, ‘Resource constraints’, ‘Behavioural regulation’ and ‘Goal priority’. This was conducted with the discussion of subject matter experts.

    Coding of the transcripts to the PARRC model mechanisms with the semantic themes in Nvivo11 allowed a matrix query to be run in the software to calculate the number of references to co-occurring PARRC mechanisms. The frequency of connections made between the mechanisms can be used to infer the strength of the connections which can then be compared to the original PARRC model interconnections that were derived from observations in the literature.

    Stage 3Review the content of the transcripts coded to interconnecting themes.

    From the matrix queries in Nvivo11 the context of the transcripts that were coded to co-occurring PARRC mechanisms can be reviewed to understand what the interconnections mean to the driver themselves. These can then be contrasted to the interconnections that were identified by researchers in the literature that the PARRC model was grounded within.

    DescriptionStageOutput

    Expansion of the PARRC mechanisms to include drivers self-reported causal factors

    Frequency counts on the number of references in the interview transcripts that make connections between mechanisms of the PARRC model.

    An understanding of what the interconnections of the PARRC model relate to in the drivers discussion.

    Stage 4Contrast the original PARRC model developed from the literature on driver distraction from technology to the model developed from the drivers self-reported reasons for engaging with technological tasks.

    The PARRC model was constructed from the interconnections that were referenced by the drivers, with the frequency of the interconnections visualised through the strength of the interconnections. This was then contrasted to the original PARRC model whose interconnecting mechanisms were constructed from the number references and significant relationships made between the mechanisms in the literature. Comparisons between the drivers stated behaviour and the behaviour studied in the literature.

    A version of the PARRC model developed from the literature on driver distraction from technology and a version of a PARRC model developed from the drivers self reports in the interview.

  • 15

    actors(stage5oftheframeworkinFigure1)meantthatthecontributionofthesystemicactorstothecausal

    mechanismsofthePARRCmodelcouldbeidentified.Furthermore,interconnectionsareimportantwithin

    systemsmodelsassociotechnicalsystemsemphasiseemergenceofsafety fromthecomplex interactions

    betweensystemicelements(Leveson,2004).TheinterconnectionsintheoriginalPARRCmodelwerederived

    fromempiricallytestedconnectionsmadeintheliteratureaswellasassociationsmadebyauthorsinrelating

    conceptstooneanother.Connectionsbetweenthecausalfactorsreportedbythedriversintheinterview

    studyinExperiment1wereidentifiedusingamatrixqueryinNvivo11softwarethatwasusedtocodethe

    data.Matrixqueriesallowthenumberofco-occurringcodedthemestobequantifiedandhighlighted,to

    determinethenumberandtypeofdataexcerptsthatrelatetoco-occurringthemesofinterest.Stage3of

    theinductivethematicprocess(Figure1),statestheprocessforgeneratingtheinitialdescriptivecodeswithin

    thedata.Thisprocessrequiresexcerptstobecodedatmultiplethemes(Braun&Clarke,2006),allowingco-

    occurringthemestobereviewedafterwards.ThelinkingofthesemanticsubthemestothePARRCmodel

    mechanismsinFigure2,allowedthelinksbetweenthesubthemesrepresentingeachofthePARRCfactors

    to be explored. The total number of interconnecting statements in the interviews between the PARRC

    mechanism subthemes was calculated. The connections could then be reviewed through the Nvivo11

    softwaretofurtheranalysetheconceptsthatwerecodedtotheinterconnectingPARRCmechanisms(Stage

    3,Figure2).ComparisonscouldthenbemadebetweentheoriginalPARRCmodel,groundedintheliterature,

    and the reconstructedPARRCmodelbuilt from thedrivers self-reporteddiscussionson their reasons for

    engagingwithtechnologywhiledriving.

    3.3Results

    Application of the semantic themes detailed in Table 2 to the PARRC factors and assessment of the

    interconnectionsreferencedbythedriversledtotheconstructionofthePARRCmodeldevelopedfromthe

    drivers’selfreportedreasonsforengagingwithdistractivetechnologieswhiledriving(Figure2,Stage4).This

    ispresentedinFigure3.InsightsthatweregainedfromtheapplicationofthePARRCmodelframeworkto

    thethemesidentifiedandinductivelygeneratedfromtheinterviewsarediscussed.

  • 16

    Figure3.Applicationofthethematicframework,referencedinTable2,tothePARRCmodelofdistraction.

    EachofthePARRCfactorswerefoundtoberepresented inthethematicframeworkdevelopedfromthe

    driversself-reportedreasonsforengagingwithdifferenttechnologicaltaskswhiledriving.Therelevanceof

    thesethemestothefactorsisdiscussedbelow.

    Adapttodemands:Driverssupportedthenotionthattheyadaptboththeirbehaviourinthedrivingtask

    andthesecondarytaskinlinewithincreasedmentalandphysicaldemandwhendiscussingtheirlikelihood

    ofengagingwithatechnologicaltaskwhiledriving.Thesemanticsubtheme‘Roadcontext’(C3)highlights

    theneedtoalterandadapttheirdrivingbehaviourinlinewiththechangingdemandsoftheroad

    environment.Thesemanticsubtheme‘Road-taskrelationship’(I4)suggeststhatdriversalsoadapttheir

    behaviouracrossdifferentroadtypes,suchthattheyareawareofthedifferentdemandsofdifferentroads

    andaltertheirengagementwithtechnologyaccordingly.Intermsoftheadaptionofthetechnologicaltask,

    thesubtheme‘abilitytocomplete’(T6)suggeststhatdriversadaptthefunctionalityofthetask,adjusting

    thecompletenessofitasillustratedintheexamplequote,inordertomeetthedemandsofthedriving

    task.

    Behaviouralregulation:Theinterviewsprovidedinformationonthecognitivethoughtprocessesofthe

    driverandtheirperceptionsoftheirownbehaviourrelativetothesurroundingenvironment.Ithasshown

    thatthedrivers‘attitude’(D1),‘tendencies’(D2),‘viewofself’(D3),andthe‘influenceofothers’(D4)are

    keyfactorsthatrelatetotheregulationoftheirbehaviourwithrespecttoengagingintechnologywhilst

    Examples:Not worth it

    Too dangerousHappily do it

    Examples:Drawn in IgnoreTriggered responseExamples:

    Eyes off the roadHands on the wheel

    Examples:Task TimeLength of text

    Examples:Required attentionOther road users

    Examples:Poor visibilityCorners

    Examples:SpeedDriving task difficulty

    Examples:Journey lengthJourney TypeFamiliarity

    Examples:UrgencyNecessityIn a hurry

    Examples:Do it all the timeDon’t like doing itTry not o

    Examples:GuiltyStupidToo clumsy

    Examples:Others do it tooSocial peer pressureOthers seeing you

    Examples:Know I shouldn’t

    Examples:Concentration required from drivingBusyness of road environment

    Examples:Driving SlowerWait for right conditions

    Examples:Adapt for driving functionalityLimited completion

    Examples:Ease of taskCognitive processing required Examples:

    UtilityReliabilityUse outside of car

    Attitude

    Driver (D1)

    Engagement regulation

    Task (T5)

    Task (T6)Ability to complete

    Task (T2)Interaction

    Road Infrastructure (I5)

    Road Related behaviour

    Road Infrastructure (I2)

    Road layout

    Road Infrastructure (I1)Perceived surrounding

    environment

    Context (C2)

    Task Context

    Context (C1)

    Journey Context

    Tendency

    Driver (D2)

    View of self

    Driver (D3)Influence of others

    Driver (D4)

    Context (C3)

    Road Context

    Road Infrastructure (I4)

    Road-Task relationship

    Illegality

    Road Infrastructure (I3)Task (T3)

    Duration

    Task (T1)

    Complexity

    Desirability

    Task (T4)

    Adapt to demands

    Behavioural Regulation

    Goal Priority

    Resource constraints

    Goal Conflict

  • 17

    driving.Furthermore,asisdemonstratedintheexamplequote(Table2,I4),thebehaviourofthedriveris

    alsoshowntoberegulatedbytheroadinfrastructurewithinthetask-roadrelationshiptheme(I4),with

    driversdiscussinghowtheyregulatetheirbehaviourinrelationtotheroadenvironmentandattainmentof

    thedrivinggoal,whichisalteredacrossroadtypes.

    Goalconflict:Driversdiscussedthelimitationsofrespondingtoco-occurringdrivingandtechnologicaltask

    goalswithrespecttothefeaturesofthetechnologicaltasks(T4&T5)andtheirknowledgeonthelaws(I3)

    whichstatetheconflictinggoalsshouldnotbeachievedinunison(i.e.drivingwhileusingahand-held

    phoneunderUKlaw).Featuresofthetechnologicaltaskwhichrelatetoitspotentialtoconflictwiththe

    drivingtaskwere‘desirability’(T4)and‘engagementregulation’(T5).Technologieswithinthevehiclehave

    developedovertimetoprovidenovelinteractionsandfunctionalitiestothedriverthatwerenotpreviously

    available.Thismakesthemdesirabletowould-beusers(Walkeretal,2001)andthereforeplacesthemin

    conflictforattentionwiththemaindrivingtask.Driversdiscusstheutility(T4)ofthetechnologiesandhow

    thisrelatestotheirusewhiledriving,asillustratedintheexamplequote(Table2,T4).Theyalsodiscussthe

    featuresofthetaskthatdeterminehowabletheyaretoregulatetheonsetofthetask(T5)andthearising

    conflictthismayhavewiththedrivingtask.Forexample,manydriverscommentedonthetriggered

    responsethatoccurredwhentheyreceiveatextwhiledriving,e.g.“’Readingatext’,youseeIwouldreada

    textjustbecauseofthenatureofthefactthatitflashesuponyourphone.”[Participant:7,Task:Readtext

    onmobilephone].Thissuggeststhatdriversdidnotalwayswishtoengageinthetaskbutthedesignofthe

    deviceallowedittocompeteforthedrivers’attention,divertingitawayfromthedrivingtask.

    Goalpriority:Theprioritisationofthegoalswasfoundtobeinfluencedbythecontextandcircumstance

    whichsurroundtheinteraction,notjusttheroadinfrastructurebutthespecificcircumstancesthatmay,or

    maynot,leadtointeractionwithtechnology.Thejourneytypeand/orlength(C1)influencedthe

    requirementtoengage.Forexample,longerjourneysmayleadtomoreinteractionswiththemusicsystem.

    Thefamiliarity(C1)withtheroutewasalsosuggestedtoalterconfidenceinprioritisingthetechnological

    task.Thecircumstancessurroundingthetechnologicaltaskwerealsoimportant(C2),suchashow

    importantorurgentthetaskwas,ashighlightedintheexamplequote.Thecontextualfactorssuggestthere

    aremanysituationsinwhichengagementwithtechnologyismoreorlesslikelytooccur.Thesedonot

    relatetoroadtypeorenvironmentbutspecificmomentsandcircumstancesthatcannotbeforeseenand

    directlyimpactondriver’swillingnesstoengage.

    Resourceconstraints:Theattentionalresourcesofthedriverwerereportedtobeconstrainedbyfeatures

    ofthetask(T1-T3)andfeaturesoftheroadenvironment(I1-I2).Theroadtypespresentedtothedrivers

    ledthemintoadiscussiononthefeaturesoftheroadthatinfluencestheirdecisiontoengagewith

    technology.Discussionsontheroadlayout(I2)andtheirinterpretationofthesurroundingenvironment(I1)

    highlightedthedrivers’awarenessfortheelementsoftheroadenvironmentwhichmaylimittheir

  • 18

    attention.Likewise,therewasalsoanin-depthdiscussiononthecharacteristicsofthetechnologicaltasks

    presentedtodriversandhowfeaturessuchasits‘complexity’(T1),methodof‘interaction’(T2),and

    ‘duration’(T3)influencedtheattentionthatitrequired.Insomecases,suchastheexamplequote,the

    perceivedresourcesrequiredtointeractareminimalwhichincreasesthelikelihoodofengaging,whereas

    forothertaskstheperceivedresourcesaretoogreattocompletethetaskwhiledriving.

    Interconnections:JustasthemechanismsofthePARRCmodelwerefoundedingroundedtheory

    methodology,theinterconnectionsbetweenthemwereidentifiedfromtheempiricallytestedconnections

    madeintheliterature(Parnelletal,2016).Thetotalnumberofinterconnectingstatementsinthe

    interviewsbetweenthePARRCmechanismsubthemesisshowninFigure4b.Inlinewithstage4inthe

    applicationofthePARRCmodeltothethematicframeworkinFigure2,thisiscontrastedtothe

    interconnectionsfoundduringthegroundedtheoryapproachintheoriginaldevelopmentofthePARRC

    model(Figure4a).ThenumbersontheinterconnectionsinFigure4arelatetothenumberofstudies

    empiricallytestingtherelationshipbetweenthefactors.Thesizeoftheconnectinglinesrepresentsthe

    numberofconnectionsmadeinbothdiagrams.Thematrixcodingoftheinterviewdataonlystatesthe

    frequencyofco-occurringsubthemes,notthedirectionofanyrelationshipthatmayoccur,sothe

    connectionsareshownaslinesratherthandirectionalarrowsinFigure4b.

    a) b)

    Figure4.PARRCmodelscreatedusinga)empiricallytestedrelationshipsintheliterature(takenfromParnelletal,2016)b)interviewtranscripts.

    Figure4contraststheinterconnectionsbetweenthePARRCfactorsintheoriginalmodel(Figure4a)and

    theonebasedonthedriverscodedtranscripts(Figure4b).Asidefromthequantitiesoftotal

    interconnectionsbeinghigherintheinterviewdrivenconnections(Figure4b),whichislikelytobedueto

    richdatasourceoftheinterviewsincontrasttothe33studiesidentifiedintheliteraturereview(Parnellet

    al,2016),therearesimilaritiesintheconfigurationsoftheinterconnections.Thissuggestssupportforthe

    underlyingliteraturethatthePARRCmodelwasgroundedin,astheresearchistargetingconceptsthatare

    alsogeneratedtobeimportantbydrivers.

  • 19

    Connection Description Quote

    Resourceconstraints GoalConflictConstrainedresourcesmeansdriverscannotperformtwotasksatoncesotheyareinconflictwitheachother.

    Well,againitisjustIwouldfindittoodistracting,Iwouldn’tbeabletodriveandoperatethesoftwareonmyphoneinordertoenterthedestination,becauseIwouldn’tbeabletoseeproperly!

    Resourceconstraints Adapttodemands Limitedresourcesrequiresadaptationofbehaviour.

    Sothepicturethatyou’vegothaslotsofcarsonthesideoftheroadforexampleandhousesandI’dbethinking,“Ahacarisgoingtopulloutinfrontofme”or,“I’mgoingtogetveryclosetoacar”sotherefore100%needstobeontheroadatthatpoint.

    Resourceconstraints Behaviouralregulation Driversmustregulatebehaviourinlinewiththeirlimitedresources.

    Yes,Idon’tthinkthatcanbedoneinasafeamountoftime.ItwouldhavetobelessthanasecondIthinkandeventhen,eventhatonamotorwayisrisky.ButIdon’tthinkIcoulddothatinlessthanabout20seconds,evenifIknewthepostcodeandeverything.SoIjustwouldn’t

    GoalConflict GoalPriority Driversprioritisetoovercomeconflict.Soalsodependingonwhoiscalling,soifforexampleworkiscallingI’llprobablyturnthatofforsomenumberthatIdon’tknow,whereasifit’ssomeonethatI’mmorelikelytoknoworsomeonewho’smorelikelytotellmesomethingimportantthenIwillanswermorereadily

    GoalConflict Adapttodemands Driverscanadapttothedemandsoftheconflictingtasks.

    Iwouldbehighlylikelytodothisonamotorway,becauseIthinkitisquiteeasy,againlikeusingaphoneIwoulddoitstaggeredtouchsomethinglookup,touchsomethingelse,lookup.

    GoalConflict BehaviouralregulationTheabilitytoregulatebehaviourtowardsnewtasksthatconflictsthedrivingtaskinfluencestheconflictforattentionbetweenthetasks.

    Justbecauseit’smoreofa–forme,forsomereasonit’sanautomaticresponse,sothephoneflashesandyounaturallyjustlookoverandthenyoureadthetext.

    Adapttodemands GoalConflict Driversadaptbehaviourwhichcanleadtofurtherconflictbetweengoals.

    Yeah,intothecarbutIlistentoplaylistssoit’sneverreally…actually,onthemotorwayit’sfindingasong,ifI’mfindingasong,searchingonSpotifywhereasifIwasonaruralroadoraresidentialarea,I’djustbeskippingasong.

    Adapttodemands GoalPriority Driverscanadapttodemandsbyprioritisingtheirgoals.

    TheonlytimeImightdoitisatajunction,stopped;there’sachanceImightifIreallyneedtomakeacall,butotherwiseIwouldn’tmoving,no.

    Adapttodemands Behaviouralregulation Driverscanregulatetheirbehaviourtoadapttothedemandsoftheenvironmentandthetask.yeah,oksochangingclimatecontrolsIdefinitelywouldonthemotorway,aslongasyouarejustcruisingalong

    Goalpriority AdapttodemandsTheprioritisationofgoalsrequiresadaptionoftheprimaryandsecondarytasksinlinewithcurrentdemands.

    Ruralroadprobably,itdependshowrural,ifitareallyreallytinyroadIprobablydon’tansweritbecauseitisquitenicetobeabletoheartheroad

    Additionalconnections

    BehaviouralRegulation GoalConflict

    Thedriversattitudetowardsthetaskandstatedintentiontoengagewithitwhiledrivinginfluencethepotentialforthetechnologicaltasktocomeintoconflictwiththedrivingtask

    No,never,never,never,never,never.Readatest?Honestly,Iwouldneverdothisstuff.

    Resourceconstraints GoalPriority Driverslendresourcestothedrivingtasktodetermineitsprioritybeforedecidingtoengagewithit.Iwouldneveropenupawholemessage,butImightglancedownandlookatwhoitisfromatleastandwhatiswrittenonit.

  • 20

    Table3.InterconnectionsbetweenthePARRCfactorsasdrawnfromtheinterviewdata.

    Theevidencefortheseconnectionsfoundwithinthedriversself-reportedlikelihoodofengagingwithtechnologies

    providessupportforthePARRCmodel,andtheliteraturewithinwhichitwasdeveloped.Yet,somedifferencescanbe

    seenbetweenthetwomodelsinFigure4.Thesewereassessedbylookingtowardsthetranscriptsthatwerecodedto

    theconnectingthemesinthematrixqueryandunderstandinghowthedrivers’reportsmaydiffertothosestudiedin

    theliterature.Theyincludethefollowing:

    • Areductionintheprominenceoftheconnectionbetween‘goalconflict’and‘goalpriority’.

    InthedevelopmentoftheoriginalPARRCmodelconnections,theliteraturewasfoundtofrequentlyconsidertheneed

    toprioritise thegoalsof thedriving taskand thesecondary task inorder to resolveanygoalconflict thatmaybe

    occurringbetweenthetwo(e.g.Doganetal,2011).Yet,thetranscriptsofthedriversverbalisedthoughtprocesson

    theirlikelihoodtoengageintechnologicaltaskssuggestsprioritisationofgoalstobelessofaconcern.

    • Anincreasedprominenceoftheconnectionbetween‘resourceconstraints’and‘adapttodemands’.

    Adaption of behaviour tomanage resources was a well represented notion in the interview transcripts, with 72

    statements connecting ‘resource constraints’ themes to adapt to demand themes. The connectionwas originally

    deemedtoreflecttheideathatadaptionrelatestotheattentionalresourcesthatareavailable(Parnelletal,2016).

    ThePARRCmodel alignswith the theory that the attentional resourcesof drivers are limited, such that both the

    primaryandsecondarytasksmustcompeteforavailableresourcestofacilitatetheireffectiveperformance(Wickens,

    2002).Onewayofensuringthatattentionalresourcesareefficientlyallocatedacrosstheprimaryandsecondarytask

    isthroughadaption(Noyetal,1989;Cnossenetal,2000).Figure4bandtheexamplequotegiveninTable3suggest

    thatdriversdoreporttheadaptionoftheirbehaviourinordertofacilitatetheirsecondarytaskgoal.Italsosuggests

    thatthisadaptiontofacilitatemultiplegoalsismoreimportanttodriversthantheneedtoprioritiseonegoalover

    another.

    • Theadditionofaconnectionbetween‘resourceconstraints’and‘goalpriority’,whichwasnotfoundinthe

    literaturethattheoriginalPARRCmodelwasdevelopedfrom.

    AnotherkeydifferencebetweenFigure4aand4bistheadditionofanextraconnectionbetween‘resourceconstraints’

    and‘goalpriority’,whichwasnotfoundintheliteraturethatthePARRCmodelwasdrawnfromandisthereforeabsent

    intheoriginalmodel(Figure4a).Parnelletal(2016)reasonedthattheabsenceofthisconnectioncouldstemfroma

    number of causes which relate to the other connections in themodel. These include the prominent connection

    between‘goalpriority’,‘adapttodemands’and‘resourceconstraints’,whichpreventsresourcesfromconstraining

    theprioritisedgoal,andfacilitatesadaptionofbehaviourinlinewithresourceavailabilityinordertoprioritiseone

    goal over another (Parnell et al, 2016). Yet, the findings from the interview data suggest that drivers domake a

    connection between ‘resource constraints’ and ‘goal priority’, although it is minimal. Table 3 gives an example

    statement of the connection made by the drivers and suggests the connection relates to drivers lending some

  • 21

    attentionalresourcestowardsthetaskinordertodetermineifitrequiresprioritising.Thisisofinterestasitsuggests

    a concept that was not previously been considered in the literature from which the original PARRC model was

    conceived.

    • Anincreasedprominenceofconnectionbetween‘goalconflict’and‘behaviouralregulation’.

    Figure 4a suggested the connection between ‘behavioural regulation’ and ‘goal conflict’ reflected the bottom-up

    processthroughwhichcertaingoalsmayresultintriggeredresponsesthatrealignstheconflictinggoals(Parnelletal,

    2016). The interview data presents a stronger connection between ‘goal conflict’ and ‘behavioural regulation’, in

    Figure4b.Areviewofthecodedtranscriptsconnectingthemechanismssuggestitmayreflectaconnectioninthe

    oppositedirectiontotheoriginalPARRCmodel,goingfrom‘behaviouralregulation’to‘goalconflict’.Theexample

    quotefromTable3highlightstheinfluencethatdrivers’attitudestowardsthetechnologicaltaskscanhaveontheir

    potentialtoconflictwiththedrivingtask.Thedriverintheexampleisadamantthattheywillneverusethedevice

    while driving, stating theywill turn it off so that itwill not pose a conflict to their driving goal. This illustrates a

    reoccurring theme within the interview transcripts, many drivers held strong attitudes towards the use of

    technologicaltaskswhiledrivingstatingthattheywouldnevereverattempttoengageinthetaskwhiledriving.

    3.4.Discussion

    Experiment2haspresentedtheapplicationofthethematicframeworkthatwasdevelopedinExperiment1tothe

    PARRCmodelofdistractionwhichpresentsthecausalfactorsinvolvedindriverdistractionasstatedintheliterature.

    ThishasvalidatedandextendedtheoriginalPARRCmodelandtheliteraturewithinwhichitiscomprised.Ithasshown

    howthedrivers’verbalreportsrelatetothestudyofthebehaviourintheliterature.Driverdistractioncanbeadifficult

    behaviour tostudy in itsnaturalenvironmentduetootherconfounding factorsandtheethical issuesofexposing

    participantstodistractingactivities(Carstenetal,2013).Therefore,capturingthedriversreportedbehaviourthrough

    open-endeddiscussionsandestablishingitsrelationtotheexistingliteratureprovidesmuchneededvalidationofthe

    research.

    ThecausalfactorsreportedwithintheinterviewsandtheirassociationwiththePARRCmodelfactorssupportsother

    researchfromnaturalisticdrivingstudies.Thenotionofdriveradaptionisparticularlyevidentwithstudiesthathave

    lookedatdrivers’ engagementwith secondary tasks,withdrivers slowingdown (Metzet al, 2015) and increasing

    headway(Tivesten&Dozza,2015)whenengagingwithsecondarytasks.Thissupportstheadaptionofbehaviourat

    thecontrollevelthathasalsobeenfoundinsimulatorstudies(Schömig&Metz,2013;Cnossenetal,2004).Yet,there

    wasalsoasuggestionthatdriversstrategicallyplantheirengagementwiththetechnologicaltasksinadvance.The

    notionthatdrivers’engagementwithtechnologydependsonjourneytypeandroadinfrastructurehavealsobeen

    foundinnaturalisticdrivingstudies(Tivesten&Dozza,2015).

    Therewere,however,commentsmadebydriversthatwerenotincludedinthedevelopmentoftheoriginalPARRC

    model from the literature,which furtherhighlights the importanceofassessing thevalidationofmodelswith the

    applicationtodifferentsources(Hignett,2005).Thisincludesanadjustmentwithinthestructureofthemodelformed

  • 22

    fromtheinterviewdatawhichsuggestedlessofafocusonprioritisinggoalsdirectlyandmorereportsofadapting

    theirlimitedresourcepoolandattributingsomeresourcestodeterminingthepriorityofthesecondarytaskbefore

    lendingitfurtherresourcestocarryoutthetask.Thishighlightstheeffectthattaskssuchastextmessageshavewhen

    theydrawthedrivers’attentiontowardsthedeviceandleadthedrivertomakeadecisiontoprioritisethedrivingtask

    orthetexttask.Ifthenotificationdidnotarisewhilethevehiclewasinmotiontheywouldnotbealertedtoitand

    theywouldnotneedtore-establishtheirpriorities.

    Conversely,theconnectionbetweenthe‘behaviouralregulation’and‘goalconflict’factorssuggeststhatsomedrivers

    wereabletocontrolthegoalsthatconflictedwiththedrivingtaskduetotheirattitudes,perceptionsandbehaviour

    that regulated their engagementwith the taskswhile driving. This reflects otherswho suggest the role of driver

    attitudeintheirintentiontoengagewithdistractionswhiledriving(Walshetal,2008;Zhouetal,2009;Zhouetal,

    2012).The suggestion thatdrivers’engagementwithdistractionsare largelyvoluntary (Lee,2014;Beanlandetal,

    2013), suggest that they do have an element of control over their behaviour but that they choose to become

    distracted.ThedevelopmentofthethematicframeworkanditsapplicationofthePARRCmodelaimstosuggestthat

    thedrivers’decisiontoengageisnotentirelystraightforwardandthatbanningthebehaviourthroughlegislationis

    nottheonlyoption.Insteadtherearenumerousfactorsandactorstoconsiderwithinthesociotechnicalsystem,which

    arecomplexlyinterconnectedindeterminetheemergenceofdistraction.

    4.GeneralDiscussion

    It isunderstoodfrompreviousresearchthatthemisuseoftechnologicaldevices,suchassat-navs (Tsimhonietal,

    2004),musicplayers(Youngetal,2012),hands-freephones(Horrey&Wickens,2006)mobilephones(McCarttetal,

    2006)andwearabletechnologies(Sawyeretal,2014)poseathreattoroadsafety.Whilst,researchfocusesonthe

    adverseeffectsoncethetechnologyisengagedwithbythedriver,itfailstoaccountforwhythedriverschooseto

    engagewiththetechnology inthefirstplace.Anunderstandingofthekeyunderlyingcausalfactorsthatmotivate

    driverstoengagewithtechnologiesiscriticalinprovidingrecommendationsandcountermeasurestolimittheadverse

    effectsofdriverdistraction(Walshetal,2008;Atchleyetal,2011;Atchleyetal,2012;Zhouetal,2009;Young&Lenné,

    2010).Furthermore,therelevanceofsystemsbasedmeasurestocounteringdriverdistractionarerequiredtoimprove

    thesafetyoftheroadtransportsystemasawhole(Young&Salmon,2012;Salmonetal,2012;Parnelletal,2016).

    Thesystemiclevelofthethematicframeworksuggestshowactorsoutsideofthedrivers’controlmaybecreatingthe

    conditions for distractions to be engaged with. Application of the thematic framework to the PARRC model in

    Experiment2suggestedhowtheunderlyingthemesrelatetothePARRCfactors,validatingthemwithconceptsderived

    fromdriversreportsoftheirownbehaviour.Furthermore,explorationoftheinterconnectionsbetweenPARRCfactors

    hassuggestedsomedifferentstructuralconnectionswithinthemodel.Astheoriginalmodelreflectedtheempirically

    testedrelationshipsintheliterature,thedifferenceintheinterconnectionsfoundinthispapersuggestpotentialgaps

    intheliterature.Theinductiveanalysishasprovidedfactorsthatdriversthemselvesreporttobeimportantintheir

    decisiontoengagewithtechnologicaltaskswhiledriving.Thishassupportedtheagendaofpreviousresearchthathas

    appliedbehaviouralintentionliteraturetodriverdistraction(Welshetal,2008;Zhouetal2009;Zhouetal,2012).Yet,

  • 23

    it has also highlighted the importance of the interacting elements in the road transport system in creating the

    conditionsfordriverstomakethedecisiontoengagewithtechnologicalsecondarytaskswhiledriving.

    4.1Recommendationstopractise

    The adverse implications of using specific technological tasks while driving are known (Horrey &Wickens, 2006;

    Tsimhonietal,2004;McCarttetal,2006;Youngetal,2012),yetthefacilitatingconditionsarelessacknowledged.By

    targetingthecausalfactorsofdistraction,countermeasurescanbedevelopedthatfocusontheunderlyingcausesof

    theissue,ratherthanlimitingitseffectsonceengagementhasbeeninitiated.Thethematicframeworkdevelopedin

    Experiment 1 highlights the importance of the wider road transport sociotechnical system and its influence on

    technologyuse,includingthedriver,task,contextandroadinfrastructure.Thissupportstheimportanceoflooking

    beyondindividualfocusedmethodsoftargetingdriverdistraction(Young&Salmon,2012;Salmonetal,2012;Parnell

    etal,2017).

    Todeterminetheimportanceofsystemicactorstoissuesthatarefoundwithinsociotechnicalsystems,thehierarchy

    ofthesystemcanbemappedusingtheRMF(Rasmussen,1997).Thisrepresentationofthesystemshierarchyisuseful

    indetermininghowactorsinteractwitheachother(Rasmussen,1997),whichcanthenbeusedtoassessthepotential

    forincidentaswellasidentifyingfuturesolutions(e.g.Young&Salmon,2012;Parnelletal,2017).Thelocationofkey

    actors inthehierarchycanfacilitatetheprovisionofcountermeasuresthattargetelementshigherup,toproduce

    widespreadchangeatlowerlevels(Branfordetal,2009).TheadaptedRMFhierarchy(Parnelletal,2017)thatincludes

    anadditional twohigh level themes, thenational and international committees, alongside theoriginal levels (the

    government,regulators,companyandmanagement;Rasmussen,1997)wasapplied.Theactorsrelevanttoeachof

    thesystemiclevelsofthethematicframeworkwereassessedtodeterminetheactorsacrossthehierarchicallevels

    thatcouldbetargetedforfuturecountermeasures.

  • 24

    Figure4. Systemsactorsacross thehierarchyof the sociotechnical systems thatare related to the fourhigh-level

    systemicthemesidentifiedinthethematicanalysis.

    4.1.1Driver

    Thedrivers’attitudetowardstheuseofthetechnologywasfoundtoplayanimportantroleinlimitingitsconflictwith

    thedrivinggoal.Elementsofthesystemthatimpactonthedriverandtheirattitudestowardstechnologicaldevices

    appearacross thehierarchy fromthosedirectly interactingwiththedriver,suchaspassengersor thepresenceof

    otherroaduserswhomaybewatching,tohigherlevelactorsuchaseducationalproviders,themediaandroadsafety

    charitieswhocancontrolattitudesinamoretopdownmanner.ArecentroadsafetycampaignbyTHINK!intheUK

    withthetag line‘maketheglovecompartmentthephonecompartment’,guidesthedriverawayfromplacingthe

    phonegoal intoconflictwiththedrivinggoal (THINK!,2017).Thiswouldalsopreventthedriverfromdetermining

  • 25

    wherethetaskliesintheirgoalprioritiesandhowtheymayadapttheirbehaviouraccordingly.Furthermore,theviews

    wehaveontheuseoftechnologywhiledrivingasasocietyareinfluencedbynationalcommitteeswhodeterminethe

    importanceofbehaviourwithinnationalculture.Theissueofroadsafetyisasocialresponsibilitythatshouldbeshared

    bythetoplevelofthesystem(Larssonetal,2010)andthereforetheuseoftechnologieswhiledrivingneedtobe

    portrayedasanti-socialwhenitconflictswiththesafemonitoringofthedrivingtask.

    4.1.2Infrastructure

    Asan integralpartofthetransportationsystem,road infrastructure isregulatedatthe international,nationaland

    governmental levels with the aim to develop an efficient road transport system (e.g. Department for Transport,

    2015b).Theinteractionofroadtypewithtechnologicalengagementhasbeenexploredheretoidentifythatdrivers

    doconsidertheroadenvironmentwhentheydecidetousetechnologywhiledriving,ashasalsobeenidentifiedin

    naturalisticdrivingstudies (Tivesten&Dozza,2015).Onmotorways,descriptivethemessuchas ‘justcruising’and

    ‘consistent’roadlayoutssuggestthatdriversdeemthedrivingtasktobelessdemandingontheseroadscomparedto

    ruralroadswhere‘poorvisibility’and‘corners’mayincreasedemandinthesecondarytask.Yet,attheindustrialand

    resource providers level there are no actors directly influencing which tasks are compatible with different road

    infrastructures (Salmonetal, 2012). Future research shoulddetermine if certain tasksand technologiesaremore

    compatiblewithcertainroadenvironments.Forexample,interactingwithasat-navmaybeeasieronamotorwaybut

    ithaslimitedusehereasthereisgenerallymoreroadsideinformationandthedistancebetweenjunctionsisgreater

    thanonruralorurbanroads,whichmaybemoredemandingbutholdagreaterrequirementtoengagewiththesat-

    nav to navigate through fast changing environments. Tivesten andDozza (2015) also came to similar conclusions

    suggestingthepotentialforsomeroadareaswherephoneusemayberegulated,ratherthanbanned.

    4.1.3Task

    Thetechnological task isassociatedwithanumberofsystemselements fromtheverytopofthehierarchytothe

    bottom.Thedesignoftechnologicaltasksisinfluencedbyanumberofguidelines,standardsandcriteriathatstem

    frominternationalandnationalactors,whicharethenfeddowntotheindividualmanufacturersanddevelopers.Yet,

    thereisalsoaneedtorepresenttheviewsoftheenduserandapplyiterativedesignproceduresthatallowforthe

    evaluationofin-builtsystemsusabilitywithrespecttothedriverandthecontextofuse(Harveyetal,2011b).The

    driversthatwereinterviewedmadenumerousreferencestothetaskfeatures,suchashowitmaytaketheireyesoff

    theroadortheirhandsoffthewheelunderthe‘interaction’subtheme,aswellasreferencingthe‘complexity’and

    ‘duration’ofthetask.Thissuggestsdrivershadanunderstandingoftheattentionalrequirementsofthetechnological

    tasks. Design standards and guidelines have aimed to inform what is achievable while driving, yet they do not

    necessarilytakeintoconsiderationthedesirethatdrivershavetousethetechnologyattheenduserlevel.Indeed,

    otherresearchpresentedinthisjournalsuggeststhatdifferentdriversneedanddesiredifferentinformationunder

    different contexts (Davidsson & Alm, 2014). By facilitating the functioning of the technology in the vehicle, the

    temptationforthedrivertoengagewillendure,thisisparticularlytrueofmobilephones(Nelsonetal,2009).The

    multi-functionalityofphonesprovidesextratemptationforthedrivertoengagewithitwhiledrivingandthisshould

    berespondedtobydevicedevelopersbylimitingfunctionalitywhiledriving.Therewerenumerouscommentsrelating

  • 26

    tothenotificationsreceivedonmobilephonewhendriversreceivedtextsorphonecallswhichtriggeraresponse

    fromthedriver.Thepresenceoftechnologieswithcapabilitiestotriggeraresponsethattakesthedriversattention

    awayfromthedrivingtask,evenmomentarily,shouldberevisedbydevicemanufacturersasitforcestheenduserto

    assess theirprioritieswhichshouldpredominately focusonthedriving taskandroadsafety (Leeetal,2008).The

    manufacturerApplehastakenstepstowardsthiswiththeirrecentphoneupdate(ios11,releasedSeptember2017)

    thatincludesa‘donotdisturbwhiledriving’modethatcansensewhenthedeviceisinamovingvehicleand,once

    promptedbytheuser,willturnoffnotifications(Apple,2017).Thereis,therefore,thepotentialfornewregulations

    that target the desires and engagement regulation of technology use to stem from the very top levels of the

    sociotechnicalsystemandfocusontheinfluencesacrossthelevelsofthesystem,notjustthedriver.

    4.1.4Context

    The framework highlighted the importance of circumstance in the drivers’ decision to engage. There are many

    complexlyinteractingfactorsinfluencingtheuseoftechnologywhiledrivingthatrelatetotheroadenvironment,the

    driverandthetaskitself.Theinformationthatdriversrequireanddesireunderdifferentcontextualdemandsislikely

    todiffer(Davidsson&Alm,2014;Tivesten&Dozza,2015).Theeffectsofcontextoccurinapredominantlybottomup

    fashionwithinthesystemhierarchyastheyaredeterminedbytheinteractionswiththesurroundingenvironmental

    conditions.Figure5showsthelackofhighlevelactorsoncontextwithinthesociotechnicalsystemwhichisreflective

    ofthelimitedcontroloverthecomplexlyintegratedfactorsthatcompriseindividualcircumstances.Roadconditions

    arehardtocontrolastheyareinfluencedbyenvironmentalconditionssuchastimeofday,roadtypeandweather

    conditions.Taskconditionsrelatingtotheurgencyornecessitytointeractsuggestthatdriversassesstheirpriorities

    astasksandrequirementsarise.Yet,determiningandsettingprioritiesinadvance,orpredefiningsituationswhere

    engagementwouldbemoreor lessnecessarycouldlooktocontroltechnologyengagementathigher levels inthe

    system.Researchcentresofferingfacilitiestotestdifferentcontextualfactorsthroughtheuseofdrivingsimulators

    andhighlycontrolledenvironmentscanofferpromisinginsightsintotheroleofcontextondriverengagement(e.g.

    Konstantopoulosetal,2008)andfutureworkshouldassesthecomplexlyinteractingconditionsthatinfluencedrivers

    desiretoengage.

    4.2EvaluationandFuturework

    Theframeworkwasdevelopedfromtheself-reportedbehavioursofasampleof30participants,which issmall in

    contrast to the number of participants that can be recruited from online surveys that facilitate far-reaching

    recruitmentdatabases.However, thedataobtainedwasmuchricher,withover17hoursofaudio recordingsand

    transcribeddata.Furthermore,thesamplestrivedtoincludedriversofanequalrangeofageandgenderstogenerate

    a framework based on a representative sample.While the research focused on UK drivers, the laws relating to

    technologyuseinthevehiclearesimilaracrossEurope,Australia,NewZealand,JapanandIndiawhospecificallyban

    theuseofmobilephones.However,futureworkshouldseektoexplorehowtheframeworkofcausalfactorsmay

    alterwithindividualandculturaldifferences,asthismayinfluencetheuseoftechnologybothinsideandoutsideof

    thevehicle(e.g.McEvoyetal,2007;Shineretal,2005;McEvoyetal,2006;Young&Lenné,2010;Horberryetal,2006).

  • 27

    Anadvantageofthethematicframework is itsgrounding intheself-reportedbehavioursofdrivers,suchthatthe

    causalfactorsaredirectlyinformedbythosewhoexperiencethem.Theuseoftheinterviewsettingallowedthedrivers

    toopenlydiscussallfactorsthattheyfeltinfluencedtheirlikelihoodofengaging.Whileotherstudieshaveexplored

    thedrivers’willingness toengagewithdistractions insimulators (Metzetal,2011;Schömig&Metz,2013)and in

    naturalistic driving settings (Metz et al, 2015; Tivesten & Dozza, 2015) they have looked at the drivers’ physical

    engagementwiththetaskrelativetothecontextoftheroadenvironment,ratherthanenablingthedriverstodiscuss

    theirmotivationstoengage.ThestudyconductedbyLerneretal(2008)facilitateddriverstodiscusstheirwillingness

    toengagewithdistractionswithinfocusgroups,yetthismayhavebeeneffectedbythesocialdynamicsandbiases

    whendiscussingbehavioursthatbeundesirable(Smithson,2000;Lajunen&Summala,2003).Thedrivers’discussions

    inExperiment1wereconductedwithconfidentialityandanonymitywhichencourageddriverstorevealtheirtrue

    views.Thisdiscussionalsoallowedforinsightsintothewidersociotechnicalsystemanditsinvolvementintheissue

    ofdriverdistractiontoberevealed,aconceptthatwasnotobtainedbytheobjectivemeasurementsinsimulatoror

    naturalisticstudies(e.g.Metzetal,2011;Schömig&Metz,2013;Metzetal,2015;Tivesten&Dozza,2015).Whileit

    is important to review the drivers’ willingness to engagewithin the context that the behaviour arises, the open

    discussionswithdrivers,promptedwithdifferentroadtypesandtechnologicaltasks,hasuncoveredavarietyoffactors

    that influence theirwillingness to engage that has not been explored previously. Furthermore, this has provided

    validationofpreviousresearch,aswellasproposingthepossibilityoffutureexplorationsofthebehaviourandways

    tomitigateagainstit.

    5.Conclusions

    Thispaperhaspresentedthedevelopmentofathematic frameworkofthecausal factorsthatmotivatedriversto

    engagewithtechnologywhiledriving.Theuseofsemi-structuredinterviewsenableddriverstodiscusstheirlikelihood

    ofengagingwithavarietyoftechnologicaltasksacrosscommonUKroadtypes.Thestructureoftheinterviewsallowed

    allparticipantstodiscussthesametasksandroadtypeswhilefreelygeneratingthekeyconceptsthatwereimportant

    to their perceived likelihood of performing the task. Inductive thematic analysis facilitated the development of a

    hierarchicalframeworkofcausalfactorsthatwasdrivenfromthedriverowninterpretations,ratherthanapplying

    predefinedtheories.Thishasshowntheinfluenceofsystemicactorsonthecausalfactorsinfluencingtechnologyuse

    whiledriving,highlightinghowtheroadtransportsystemmaybecreatingtheconditionsfordriverdistractiontooccur.

    Thisshouldencourageamovementawayfromindividualfocusedcountermeasuresandtowardstheroleofsystemic

    actors,whichhasbeenhighlightedthroughtheapplicationofthethematicframeworktotheinteractingmechanisms

    ofdistractionfromthePARRCmodel(Parnelletal,2016).ThethematicanalysishasshowntherelevanceofthePARRC

    factors to thesystemicandsemantic themesthatdrivers report to influencetheirengagementwith technological

    devices. This has supportedprevious literature in the field,whilst also suggesting additional concepts of interest.

    Assessmentoftheactorsimpactingonthesystemicthemesidentifiedfromtheinterviewsacrossthehierarchyofthe

    sociotechnicalsystemshashighlightedfutureareasforresearchandcountermeasureimplementation.

  • 28

    6.Acknowledgements

    This researchwas fundedby theEngineeringandPhysical ScienceResearchCouncil (EPSRC)grantEP/G036896/1,

    undertheIndustryDoctoralTrainingCentreinTransportandtheEnvironment.

    7.ReferencesAltman,D.G.(1990).PracticalStatisticsforMedicalResearch.CRCpress.Apple(2011).HowtousetheDoNotDisturbwhileDrivingfeature.https://support.apple.com/en-gb/HT208090Atchley,P.,Atwood,S.,&Boulton,A.(2011).Thechoicetotextanddriveinyoungerdrivers:Behaviourmayshapeattitude.AccidentAnalysis&Prevention,43(1),134-142.Atchley,P.,Hadlock,C.,&Lane,S.(2012).Stuckinthe70s:Theroleofsocialnormsindistracteddriving.AccidentAnalysis&Prevention,48,279-284Bayliss,D.(2009).AccidentTrendsbyRoadtype.MotoringTowards2050–RoadsandRealityBackgroundPaperNo.9.March. Royal Automobile Club Foundation. Bayliss, D. (2009).http://www.racfoundation.org/assets/rac_foundation/content/downloadables/roads%20and%20reality%20-%20bayliss%20-%20accident%20trends%20by%20road%20type%20-%20160309%20-%20background%20paper%209.pdfAccessed01/08/2017.Boyatzis,R.E.(1998).Transformingqualitativeinformation:Thematicanalysisandcodedevelopment.Sage.Branford, K., Hopkins, A., & Naikar, N. (2009). Guidelines for AcciMap analysis. In Learning from High ReliabilityOrganisations.CCHAustraliaLtd.Braun,V.,&Clarke,V.(2006).Usingthematicanalysisinpsychology.QualitativeResearchinPsychology,3(2),77-101Carsten,O.,Kircher,K.,&Jamson,S.(2013).Vehicle-basedstudiesofdrivingintherealworld:Thehardtruth?AccidentAnalysis&Prevention,58,162-174.Cnossen,F.,Meijman,T.,&Rothengatter,T.(2004).Adaptivestrategychangesasafunctionoftaskdemands:astudyofcardrivers.Ergonomics,47(2),218-236.Cohen D, Crabtree B. (2006) Qualitative Research Guidelines Project. July http://www.qualres.org/HomeSemi-3629.htmlAccessed22/04/2017.Davidsson,S.,&Alm,H.(2014).Contextadaptabledriverinformation–Or,whatdowhomneedandwantwhen?.AppliedErgonomics,45(4),994-1002.DepartmentforTransport(2015a).ReportedRoadCasualtiesGreatBritain:2015.AnnualReport.https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/568484/rrcgb-2015.pdfAccessed28/04/2017.DepartmentforTransport(2015b).2010to2015GovernmentPolicy:Roadnetworkandtraffic.May.TheStationaryOffice,London.Dingus,T.A.,Klauer,S.G.,Neale,V.L.,Petersen,A.,Lee,S.E.,Sudweeks,J.D.,...&Bucher,C.(2006).The100-carnaturalisticdrivingstudy,PhaseII-resultsofthe100-carfieldexperiment(No.HS-810593).WashingtonUSA.Dogan,E.,Steg,L.,&Delhomme,P.(2011).Theinfluenceofmultiplegoalsondrivingbehaviour:Thecaseofsafety,timesaving,andfuelsaving.AccidentAnalysis&Prevention,43(5),1635-1643.

  • 29

    Dorf,R.C.(2001).Technology,humans,andsociety:towardasustainableworld.AcademicPress.Gardner,B.,&Abraham,C.(2007).Whatdrivescaruse?Agroundedtheoryanalysisofcommuters’reasonsfordriving.TransportationResearchPartF:TrafficPsychologyandBehaviour,10(3),187-200.Harvey,C.,Stanton,N.A.,Pickering,C.A.,McDonald,M.,&Zheng,P.(2011a).Totwistorpoke?Amethodforidentifyingusabilityissueswiththerotarycontrollerandtouchscreenforcontrolofin-vehicleinformationsystems.Ergonomics,54(7),609-625.Harvey,C.,Stanton,N.A.,Pickering,C.A.,McDonald,M.,&Zheng,P.(2011b).Ausabilityevaluationtoolkitforin-vehicleinformationsystems(IVISs).AppliedErgonomics,42(4),563-574.Hignett,S.2005.“Qualitativemethodologyinergonomics”.InEvaluationofhumanwork,Editedby:Wilson,J.R.andMegaw,E.TaylorandFrancis.London,UK.Horberry,T.,Anderson,J.,Regan,M.A.,Triggs,T.J.,&Brown,J.(2006).Driverdistraction:Theeffectsofconcurrentin-vehicletasks,roadenvironmentcomplexityandageondrivingperformance.AccidentAnalysis&Prevention,38(1),185-191.Horrey,W.J.,&Wickens,C.D.(2006).Examiningtheimpactofcellphoneconversationsondrivingusingmeta-analytictechniques.Humanfactors:TheJournaloftheHumanFactorsandErgonomicsSociety,48(1),196-205.Horrey,W.J.,Lesch,M.F.,Garabet,A.,Simmons,L.,&Maikala,R.(2017).Distractionandtaskengagement:howinterestingandboringinformationimpactdrivingperformanceandsubjectiveandphysiologicalresponses.AppliedErgonomics,58,342-348.Jamson,A.H.,Westerman,S.J.,Hockey,G.R.J.,&Carsten,O.M.(2004).Speech-basede-mailanddriverbehaviour:Effectsofanin-vehiclemessagesysteminterface.HumanFactors:TheJournaloftheHumanFactorsandErgonomicsSociety,46(4),625-639.Konstantopoulos,P.,Chapman,P.,&Crundall,D.(2010).Driver'svisualattentionasafunctionofdrivingexperienceandvisibility.Usingadrivingsimulatortoexploredrivers’eyemovementsinday,nightandraindriving.AccidentAnalysis&Prevention,42(3),827-834.Lajunen,T.,&Summala,H.(2003).Canwetrustself-reportsofdriving?Effectsofimpressionmanagementondriverbehaviourquestionnaireresponses.TransportationResearchPartF:TrafficPsychologyandBehaviour,6(2),97-107.Larsson,P.,Dekker,S.W.,Tingvall,C.,(2010).Theneedforasystemstheoryapproachtoroadsafety?SafetyScience.48(9),1167–1174Lee,J.D.,Young,K.L.,Regan,M.A.,(2008).Definingdriverdistraction.DriverDistraction:Theory,EffectsandMitigation,pp.31–40.BocaRaton,FL:CRCPress.Lerner,N.,Singer,J.,&Huey,R.(2008).Driverstrategiesforengagingindistractingtasksusingin-vehicletechnologies(No.HS-810919).NationalHighwayTrafficSafetyAdministration.Leveson,N.G.,(2004).Anewaccidentmodelforengineeringsafersystems.SafetyScience.42(4),237–270.Leveson,N.G.,(2011).Applyingsystemsthinkingtoanalyseandlearnfromevents.SafetyScience49(1),55–64.McCartt,A. T.,Hellinga, L.A.,&Bratiman,K.A. (2006).Cell phonesanddriving: reviewof research.Traffic InjuryPrevention,7(2),89-106.McEvoy,S.P.,Stevenson,M.R.,&Woodward,M.(2006).Theimpactofdriverdistractiononroadsafety:resultsfromarepresentativesurveyintwoAustralianstates.InjuryPrevention,12(4),242-247.

  • 30

    McEvoy,S.P., Stevenson,M.R.,&Woodward,M. (2007).Theprevalenceof,and factorsassociatedwith, seriouscrashesinvolvingadistractingactivity.AccidentAnalysis&Prevention,39(3),475-482.Metz,B.,Schömig,N.,&Krüger,H.P. (2011).Attentionduringvisualsecondarytasks indriving:Adaptationtothedemandsofthedrivingtask.TransportationResearchPartF:TrafficPsychologyandBehaviour,14(5),369-380.Metz,B.,Landau,A.,&Hargutt,V. (2015).Frequencyandimpactofhands-freetelephoningwhiledriving-Resultsfromnaturalisticdrivingdata.TransportationResearchPartF:TrafficPsychologyandBehaviour,29(0),1-13.Neale,V.L.,Dingus,T.A.,Klauer,S.G.,Sudweeks,J.,&Goodman,M.(2005).Anoverviewofthe100-carnaturalisticstudyandfindings.NationalHighwayTrafficSafetyAdministration,(Paper05-0400).Nelson,E.,Atchley,P.,&Little,T.D.(2009).Theeffectsofperceptionofriskandimportanceofansweringandinitiatingacellularphonecallwhiledriving.AccidentAnalysis&Prevention,41(3),438-444.Noy,Y.I.(1989).Intelligentrouteguidance:willthenewhorsebeasgoodastheold?.InVehicleNavigationandInformationSystemsConference,September.Toronto,Ontario,Canada(pp.49-55).IEEE.O'Cathain,A.,&Thomas,K.J.(2004)."Anyothercomments?"Openquestionsonquestionnaires–abaneorabonustoresearch?BMCMedicalResearchMethodology,4(1),25.Parnell,K.J.,Stanton,N.A.,&Plant,K.L.(2016).Exploringthemechanismsofdistractionfromin-vehicletechnology:ThedevelopmentofthePARRCmodel.SafetyScience,87,25-37.Parnell,K.J.,Stanton,N.A.,&Plant,K.L.(2017).What’sthelawgottodowithit?Legislationregardingin-vehicletechnologyuseanditsimpactondriverdistraction.AccidentAnalysis&Prevention,100,1-14.Patton,M.Q.(1990).Qualitativeevaluationandresearchmethods.SAGEPublications,Inc.Pedic,F.,&Ezrakhovich,A.(1999).Aliteraturereview:thecontentcharacteristicsofeffectiveVMS.Road&TransportResearch,8(2),3.Plant,K.L.,&Stanton,N.A. (2013).What isonyourmind?Usingtheperceptualcyclemodelandcriticaldecisionmethodtounderstandthedecision-makingprocessinthecockpit.Ergonomics,56(8),1232-1250.RAC,(2016).RACReportonMotoring2016.TheRoadtotheFuture.https://www.rac.co.uk/pdfs/report-on-motoring/rac-report-on-motoring-2016-outline.pdfAccessed28/04/2016.Rafferty,L.A.,Stanton,N.A.,&Walker,G.H.(2010).Thefamousfivefactorsinteamwork:acasestudyoffratricide.Ergonomics,53(10),1187-1204.Reimer,B.(2009).Impactofcognitivetaskcomplexityondrivers'visualtunnelling.TransportationResearchRecord:JournaloftheTransportationResearchBoard,(2138),13-19.Richards,L.&Richards,T.(1991).TheTr


Recommended