+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

Date post: 06-Apr-2018
Category:
Upload: mcjozz
View: 217 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 18

Transcript
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    1/18

    Keywords:

    Branding, childrens

    consumption,

    content analysis,

    Christmas

    Dr Aron OCass

    School of Marketing

    & Management,

    Griffith University-

    Gold Coast,

    PMB 50 Gold Coast

    Mail Center 9726

    Queensland, Australia

    Tel: +61 7 5594 8139

    Fax: +61 7 5594 8085

    E-mail: A.Ocass@

    mailbox.gu.edu.au

    Dear Santa, do you have my brand?A study of the brand requests,awareness and request styles atChristmas timeReceived (in revised form): 6th August, 2001

    Aron OCass

    is a senior lecturer at Griffith University in the school of marketing and management.

    He has a bachelor of commerce majoring in marketing, a master of business majoring

    in marketing and a PhD focusing on consumer behaviour. He has published on topics

    such as political marketing, voter choice, consumer brand associations, service choice

    behaviour. His publications appear in the European Journal of Marketing, Psychology and

    Marketing, Journal of Product & Brand Management, Economic Psychology, Journal of

    Services Marketing and Journal of Advertising.

    Peter Clarke

    has a Bachelor of Commerce majoring in Marketing and Human Resource

    Management with 1st Class Honours in Marketing. Peter Clarke has delivered papers

    on consumer behaviour at Anzmac and World Marketing Congress conferences.

    Research interests include consumer behaviour, gift giving and buyer behaviour at

    Christmas, and historical or cultural icons as consumption objects.

    AbstractThis study examines brand awareness (preferences) and request styles (communicationapproaches) at Christmas of Australian children and adults. The study is founded in the

    growing Santa Claus literature that has examined various issues related to gift giving andrequest behaviour at Christmas time. The study is based on the content analysis of 422letters written by children to Santa Claus. The results indicate that children are brand-orientated in their request behaviour, adopt fairly meaningful request strategies and usevisual imagery ie graphics in their communication strategies in their attempts to securetheir requested gifts and specific brands.

    INTRODUCTIONChristmas where the gum trees growThere is no frost and there is no snowChristmas in Australias hotCold and frosty is what its notWhen the bloom of the Jacaranda tree is

    hereChristmas time is nearTo ride around the bush where its dryTo cart all the presents piled so highA red nosed reindeer would never doSanta should jump on a kangaroo(Sabegal, undated)

    These words are from a favourite

    Australian Christmas song a

    reminder that snow, reindeer, elves and

    maybe even Santa Claus seem out of

    place in Australia. Yet this Northern

    hemisphere myth of Santa, symbolism

    and all the Christmas activities are just

    as evident at summer time in Australia

    as they are in in the norths coldest,

    darkest winters. Even more evident is

    the transition of this festive season of

    sharing and celebrating the birth of

    Jesus into the season of giving, and

    more often than not, giving and

    requesting lots. The new gods of

    Christmas now worshipped, are toy

    Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817 37

  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    2/18

    companies and their marketing armies

    (elves).

    Although Christmas is the

    cornerstone of Christianity, it is also the

    time of Santa and gift giving (Caplow

    and Williamson, 1980). With the

    complicity of parents, Santa Claus is theembodiment of a request culture that

    endorses and encourages hedonistic

    behaviour which contributes to the

    socialisation of children into the self-

    indulgent consumption associated with

    the materialism of Western cultures

    (Belk, 1987). Such requests are made

    known to Santa either during a visit to

    the Santa village located in a shopping

    centre or offered as a written wish list to

    Santa in a letter.

    SANTA, CHILDREN, PARENTS AND

    SOCIALISED REQUEST BEHAVIOUR

    The participation in request behaviour

    associated with (the tradition and myth

    of) Christmas is an important

    socialisation process where adults teach

    children to become consumers. Bahn

    (1986) describes socialisation as a

    process of attending to and knowing

    objects by, means of the senses, while

    Mochis (1985) argues that this process

    relies on the primary involvement of

    parents and the family in general. Thisprocess is important because it helps to

    develop preferences for purchasing and

    consuming certain products over others

    and may extend to brand preferences,

    and both parental and marketplace

    information contributes to a childs

    brand awareness, familiarity or

    knowledge. Children are increasingly

    participating in family decision

    processes (McNeal, 1992) and becoming

    important marketplace participants

    through a number of contemporary

    social factors such as increasing

    household affluence, growing

    independence allowed to children and

    greater consumer socialisation

    (Lackman and Lanasa, 1993; McNeal,

    1992). If there is previous experience or

    exposure, Phelps and Hoy (1996)

    suggest that either childrens or adults

    attitudes toward familiar brands have a

    strong relationship with a positive

    purchase request, purchase decision or

    both. This dominance of some brands

    over others at Christmas time has

    important social and marketing

    implications.In the area of childrens decision

    making, Bahn (1986) maintains that

    older children make judgments on

    multiple attributes while younger

    children discriminate on one or maybe

    two attributes. Research into childrens

    consumer activities as early as the 1960s

    has indicated that children influence

    purchase decisions in the general

    categories of confectionery, snacks,

    cereal and soft drinks (Ward and

    Wackman, 1968). Increases in the range

    of product available to children has

    occurred, however, because licensing

    arrangements are moving brands

    targeted at children into product

    categories of apparel or juveniles

    merchandise (Miller, 1990) and there

    has been a growth in products and

    brands now targeted at children. Thus, a

    broadening of what might be termed

    childrens product categories is being

    seen. Children are becoming more

    orientated toward fashion with popular

    character motifs (Haynes et al., 1993),rock paraphernalia (Otnes et al., 1994b)

    and sports icons. They also have a

    strong influence in the purchase

    decisions for shoes and video games

    (McNeal, 1992). In Australia, the

    Christmas period accounts for about 60

    per cent of the A$2.2bn annual sales in a

    leisure market which consists of

    product categories such as traditional

    toys, clothing, confectionery,

    audiovisual, books, stationery, sports,

    furniture and accessories (Meegan,

    1993). Although the traditional toys and

    clothing still dominate, audiovisual,

    sports and furniture are indicative of the

    increased range of products that appeal

    to children. Moreover, children are

    turning to contemporary products such

    as audiovisual equipment, electronics

    and computer-based games that account

    38 Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817

    Aron OCass and Peter Clarke

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    3/18

    for approximately 20 per cent of the

    Australian toy market (Vowles, 1996)

    and, in general, Australian children

    aged 14 to 17 own high-tech products in

    comparable percentages to Australian

    adults (Schiffman et al., 2001). Children

    now have an opportunity to expresstheir preferences in an increasingly

    expanding range of products and while

    request behaviour is not the sole

    domain of children, it is accentuated at

    Christmas time and adults are placed

    under enormous pressure to deliver the

    goods (the right brand).

    CHILDREN AND BRANDS

    Hite and Hite (1995) argue that children

    use product attributes to judge and

    create preferences for brands of

    products. They also express their

    preferences in personal purchases

    (McNeal, 1992) or request others to

    make purchase for them (Ward and

    Wackman, 1968). John and Sujan (1990)

    have indicated that children categorise

    products through visual cues of shape,

    package, colour and size. Generally,

    younger children cannot read and they

    often use brand names or symbols as

    attributes (Ward et al., 1977) or packages

    as brand cues (Hite and Hite, 1995).

    According to Percy and Rossiter (1992)such visual images are often enough to

    stimulate a response by children to a

    brand and use non-product attributes to

    identify differences in brands. Children

    appear to know the value and use of

    brands to nominate their preferences

    across a wide range of products. It has

    been argued that children over the age

    of two appear to have the capacity to

    recognise, classify and evaluate brand

    or product alternatives (Macklin, 1994)

    that satisfy their own desires and

    openly express these preferences

    through letters written to Santa.

    Toy brands have extended into

    product areas not previously considered

    as a childrens domain (Miller, 1990;

    Haynes et al., 1993; Otnes et al., 1994b).

    Children, however, often become highly

    familiar with nationally advertised

    brands at an early age (Hite and Hite,

    1995; Reda, 1995). Therefore, requesting

    a brand is not necessarily a restriction to

    the product category of toys but a

    continuation of that brand across other

    product categories that a child seeks to

    own or consume. According to Miller(1990), childrens recognition and

    knowledge of brands is such that they

    know one brand name can cover

    requests as diverse as cereal, soft drinks

    and confectionery to household linen,

    toiletries and magazines. Children may

    understand the concept of brand

    extension (Hite and Hite, 1995) and they

    should be able to nominate multiple

    requests for a brand name over a

    number of categories to display a

    multiple brand-orientation in their

    request behaviour. Children may be

    brand aware and parental

    encouragement in requesting and

    acquiring product and brand

    knowledge may be prominent at

    Christmas and communication in this

    period should cover such aspects.

    REQUESTS WRITTEN BY CHILDREN AS

    COMMUNICATION

    Similar semantic phrases that cover

    logical and affective appeals as well as

    position formation appear in both oraland written approaches to

    communication phraseology. For

    example, written lists can omit words or

    phrases to allow greater effect, with the

    sender sorting statements into some

    perceived order or grouping (Erftmier

    and Dyson, 1986). Young children often

    write about animals or people, but they

    prefer to write in the non-narrative

    format of lists, short notes, signs and

    labels (Shook et al., 1989). Such

    communication approaches allow key

    words, phrases and concepts to be

    sorted into a perceived order or

    grouping for stronger effect, and can be

    re-read and checked by them (Wetton,

    1996). Weiss and Sachs (1991) indicated

    that boys generally followed the

    strategy type of rules, fair play and

    reason, while girls used a format of

    Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817 39

    Dear Santa, do you have my brand?

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    4/18

    statements or questions. This gender

    effect indicates that boys use the art of

    reason, while girls simply expect the

    request to be honoured. Although there

    are differences between oral and written

    communications, children appear able

    to adapt their communication strategyto different situations such as a visit or a

    letter to Santa at Christmas.

    HYPOTHESIS FORMATION

    Given the lack of research on brand

    identification and request behaviours of

    Australian children at Christmas, there

    is a need to combine the request items,

    phraseology and illustrative content to

    help understand how children express

    their desires. Younger children do not

    have the access to purchase products

    directly and television advertising

    encourages request behaviour in this

    group through familiar messages such

    as ask mom to get one (Kunkel and

    Roberts, 1991). Hite and Hite (1995)

    argue, however, that children appear to

    be able to recognise and rely on brand

    name and package cues to nominate

    their preferences. Both boys and girls

    brand knowledge seems to be alike

    because they generally have similar

    exposure to a number of different and

    varied sources of information, learningand entertainment (McNeal, 1992).

    Caron and Ward (1975) suggest

    television is the major source of

    information for childrens request

    choices and children also accompany

    adults to shopping centres (McNeal,

    1992) and because of such exposure

    children know and use brands to

    express their wishes precisely. The

    similarities in exposure to products and

    brands as well as the encouragement of

    unrestricted request activities at

    Christmas for boys and girls suggest:

    H1: There will not be a significant

    difference between the number of

    brands requested by boys and girls

    when children write letters to Santa.

    Otnes et al. (1994a) refer to politeness as

    a socially accepted form of ingratiation

    and an integral part of any request

    strategy. Erftmier and Dyson (1986)

    infer politeness is an essential

    component in written communication

    and also a form of friendship or

    solidarity in the affective appeals viawritten and verbal approaches, If

    politeness is a socialised trait, then:

    H2: There will not be a significant

    difference between the politeness of

    girls gift request strategy and boys

    gift request strategy.

    Weiss and Sachs (1991) suggest boys

    oral strategy is based upon rules, fair

    play and reason which means they seek

    the why challenge of direct statements

    and do not use an indirect request

    communication style. On the other

    hand, girls expect any requests to be

    honoured through simple, yet indirect

    requests. Such views suggest that girls

    should be less direct in the way they

    write requests and therefore:

    H3: The letters girls write for

    themselves will contain

    significantly more indirect request

    strategies than letters boys write for

    themselves.

    Since lists and pictures constitutes a

    form of written communication and

    children generally have similar

    exposure to information or learning,

    expectations are that:

    H4: There will not be a significant

    difference between the numbers of

    illustrations contained in girls and

    boys letters to Santa.

    RESEARCH METHOD

    Some adults may want to know what

    their children desire as Christmas gifts

    and persuade children to write a letter

    to Santa, that openly describes,

    identifies or requests the gifts they

    desire . The letters seemingly act as a

    surrogate request to parents and

    40 Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817

    Aron OCass and Peter Clarke

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    5/18

    generally are considered to be a written

    expression of intent, behaviour and

    intrinsic values that openly express

    feelings within the broad topic of

    Christmas wishes (Richardson and

    Simpson, 1982). The awareness and

    knowledge of brands and gifts togetherwith semantic phrase and illustrations

    are identifiable and measurable within

    the content analysis research method.

    Content analysis is an acceptable

    research method used to evaluate the

    content of recorded information (Kolbe

    and Burnett, 1991; Zickmund, 1994).

    Content analysis is an unobtrusive

    method that negates biased responses

    and is used to gauge behavioural or

    affective consumer responses (Kolbe

    and Burnett, 1991; Malhotra et al., 1996).

    It has also been used in a variety of

    research contexts, such as television

    commercials (Olney et al., 1991) and

    printed advertisements (Belk and

    Pollay, 1985). Importantly, letters to

    Santa Claus have previously been used

    as a source of data for content analysis

    (Caron and Ward, 1975; Richardson and

    Simpson, 1982; Downs, 1983; Otnes

    et al., 1994a,b).

    THE STUDY

    A convenience sample of letters writtento Santa was obtained from a national

    retailer in the run-up to Christmas. The

    retailer enjoys a high level of patronage

    across its outlets and encourages adults

    and children to post a letter to Santa in

    the Christmas village precinct of its

    stores. These letters reflect the

    importance of the request tradition and

    are a suitable source of data. Individual

    stores within the retail chain forwarded

    convenience samples of letters received,

    and they constitute an adequate sample

    for the study. Following similar

    procedures and recommendations

    adopted by Otnes et al. (1994) and

    Richardson and Simpson (1982), a

    number of letters were excluded

    some because they were generated by

    keyboard and their true authorship

    could not be satisfactorily established

    and others because they were only

    scribble drawings or illegible scrawl.

    Letters requesting single items as a

    shared gift (for example, a trampoline

    for a sister and brother) caused

    difficulty in identifying a singular

    recipient. The current study alsoexcluded the non-committed request

    style that sought plenty of toys,

    surprises or something nice. Also,

    specific requests by children for gifts of

    pets, or gifts to their pets; requests by

    children for gifts for parents; requests

    by parents for gifts for themselves;

    requests for conspicuous consumption

    items like a million dollars, travel,

    boats or cars, which are not realistic

    gifts for a child; and requests for boy

    friends or calendar pin-ups were

    excluded.

    Otnes et al. (1994b) excluded letters

    written by adults because they were

    considered to hold an adults view. The

    present study also identifies that letters

    containing such statements as I am

    three years old and mummy is writing

    this letter for me are written entirely by

    adults, and as such, are an adult view

    irrespective of whether the letter is or is

    not dictated or influenced by the child.

    Finally, exclusion of the letters that

    combine efforts of both adult andchildren in writing the letter to Santa,

    which could be categorised as

    partnership letters, were undertaken

    because there is difficulty in assessing

    the instigator of the letter.

    The final sample of letters was

    categorised via five specific

    characteristics. The first was the gender

    of the child, second the authorship of

    the letter and the third was the

    aggregated brand request styles. The

    other two concern the use of semantic

    phrases and illustrations. The number of

    categories and the ease of

    implementation influence any approach

    to establishing intercoder reliability and

    agreement (Krippendorf, 1980; Perrault

    and Leigh, 1989). A simple approach to

    intercoder reliability requires fewer

    categories, and therefore less

    Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817 41

    Dear Santa, do you have my brand?

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    6/18

    opportunity for error. The authorship

    construct developed for this study is

    narrow with two elementary categories

    and the disagreement between judges

    was minimal. Any disagreement about

    authorship of a particular letter negated

    the use of that letter. All other categorieswere adequately summarised through

    previous construct guides and the

    coding process is explained in the

    following paragraphs.

    Child authors were identified through

    such statements such as I am five years

    old and writing to ask for . . .. A further

    indication of child authorship was the

    handwriting script that is currently

    taught to children and is noticeably

    different from previous generations

    writing styles. Children also tend to

    write in different colours and use

    pencils or crayons rather than blue or

    black pens. They also form larger

    figures and write in an erratic style.

    Therefore, a childs letter is readily

    identifiable as being solely written by a

    child. For letters authored by adults, it is

    noticeable that the continuity of these

    letters is mature in expression, the letter

    formation uniform and the writing

    script is different. Such letters are easily

    identifiable as being solely written by

    adults.The second construct of brand request

    style qualifies the manner in which

    brands are requested. The concept of

    branding identifies, defines and

    reinforces those unique characteristics

    and differences within product

    categories and therefore a specific name

    identified the request as a brand. A baby

    doll, for example, is a generic requestwithin a category of baby dolls, but a

    NewBorn baby doll is a specific doll

    within that doll category and therefore a

    brand. Branded requests also included

    store brands such as K Mart; sports

    team names such as Broncos (an

    Australian Rugby League team), CDs

    such as Spice Girls and low profile

    brands that may be catalogue promoted

    items. Confirmation of brand names

    was through an examination of store

    shelves and catalogue searches. One

    author has 17 years experience in the

    toy and gift industry with an additional

    12 years in Christmas retail operations

    and subsequently coded each request by

    allocation of a unique brand

    identification number or recorded as a

    generic gift request.

    This study uses the same categories to

    describe the construct of brand request

    styles as Otnes et al. (1994b) where they

    identified five brand request styles.

    Table 1 provides the descriptions and

    examples of the coding for the brandrequest style construct.

    The branded requests were coded

    Table 1 Examples of request style coding

    Request style Examples

    Brand-obsessed Requested one gift only, eg a Design-a-Mug brand(Mentions only one brand and no other gifts) Requested three gifts only, eg each branded Space

    JamSingularly branded(One brand mentioned along with other non-

    branded gifts)

    Requested two gifts, eg one for the brandBananas, the other unbranded

    Requested four gifts, eg one for Barbie, the rest

    unbrandedBrand majority(One brand is mentioned more than other

    brands)

    Requested four gifts, eg two for Barbie and oneeach for Bananas and Sky Dancer

    Requested 12 gifts, eg three branded Nike, onebranded Sony and the rest unbranded

    Pluralistic(Two or more brands mentioned equally) Requested seven gifts, eg one branded request

    each for Barbie, Fashion Avenue and PollyPocket, the others unbranded

    No brand mentioned Generic requests only such as a teddy bear, a doll ora truck

    42 Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817

    Aron OCass and Peter Clarke

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    7/18

    into the five categories as developed by

    Otnes et al. (1994b) and then combined

    to form the aggregated brand request

    construct that addresses request

    behaviours in this study. The creation of

    this summary set of data allows the

    retention of the authorship construct,but aggregates the brand request

    construct to three unique, generic

    categories that indicate the brand

    request styles at Christmas. The brand-

    fixated (brand-obsessed) and singularly

    branded categories nominate only one

    brand. These became a general category

    of one brand. The brand dominant

    (brand majority) and brand plural styles

    request more than one brand and attract

    the grouping name of many brands. The

    no brand mentioned category retains

    that name.

    Table 2 offers descriptions of selected

    communication strategy constructs,

    meanings and statement types taken

    from Otnes (1994a) and concentrates on

    communication and socialisation issues

    of politeness, direct and indirect (or

    compound) request strategies.

    When children write they use

    illustrations in the form of cutouts,

    stickers or drawings to express their

    ideas and thoughts, hence the final area

    of interest is the use of illustrations. Inline with content analysis procedures

    (Krippendorf, 1980), these illustrative

    forms determined three categories of

    product, Christmas or closure signature

    illustrations. Product illustrations

    included branded and unbranded

    drawings, cutouts, stickers or

    commercial stationery such as

    Thousand and One Dalmatians

    notepaper, and Christmas illustrations

    included individual images or collages

    of Christmas or family themes. The

    third illustration theme is the closing

    signature, which was simply noughtsand crosses or the words hugs and

    kisses.

    The data are quantitative and

    qualitative in line with a content

    analysis where judgment variables

    transpose to nominal data and

    appropriate analysis was via chi-square.

    Other data such as numbers of presents

    and brands requested is quantitative in

    nature and analysis via t-test was

    conducted. Further, chi-square and

    t-tests are analytical procedures in line

    with those used in other content

    analysis of letters to Santa

    (eg Richardson and Simpson, 1982;

    Robinson and Morris, 1986; Fisher-

    Thompson, 1993; Otnes et al., 1994a,b)

    and were also adopted in this study. In

    total 422 childrens letters in this study

    were suitable for analysis of which 173

    were written by boys and 249 by girls.

    RESULTS AND FINDINGS

    H1: There will not be a significant

    difference between the number ofbrands requested by boys and girls

    when children write letters to

    Santa.

    Hypothesis 1 (H1) was related

    specifically to brand awareness

    differences between boys and girls. In

    Table 2 Communication strategy construct and statement types

    Persuasive strategy Description Statement types

    1 Politeness Socially accepted forms ofingratiation

    How are youPlease (unaccompanied by request)Thank you

    2 Direct requests Appeals that blatantly statedthe desire for an item

    I wantBring me/get me/send me

    3 Compound requests Indirect requests accompaniedby qualifiers

    I would likePlease bring me/get me/send meCan I have/Can you get meWill you get me/bring me/send me

    Source: Otnes et al., 1994a

    Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817 43

    Dear Santa, do you have my brand?

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    8/18

    Figure 1 Joel is certain about the gender

    appropriateness of one brand and this

    letter reinforces the view that the child

    who requested the gift, or brand,

    assigned the gender appropriateness of

    the toy or gift request. This letter is also

    an example of a singularly brandedrequest style strategy. Figure 2 provides

    an example of letters that examine

    gender and generic unbranded present

    and specific branded requests.

    The results indicate that in the 422

    child-authored letters, children

    requested 44.8 per cent (787) of the

    presents for Christmas as branded

    products. In comparison, Otnes et al.

    (1994b) reported that 56 per cent (1,278)

    of 2,475 gift requests were for specific

    brand names. The average number of

    gifts requested by boys was 3.9 whilegirls requested an average of 4.3 gifts

    per letter, with no significant difference

    between the number of gifts requested

    by boys and girls when they write

    letters to Santa requesting presents

    (t 1.17 d.f. 411.15 p 0.243).

    Figure 1: Gender appropriateness of requests

    44 Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817

    Aron OCass and Peter Clarke

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    9/18

    Similarly, a t-test between boys and girls

    at the brand level also indicated no

    statistical difference (t 0.07

    d.f. 390.46 p 0.942), with both boys

    and girls requesting 1.9 branded gifts

    per letter. Therefore H1 is supported in

    that there was no significant difference

    between the number of brands

    requested by boys and girls when

    children write letters to Santa. Table 3

    indicates the patterns of request by

    children and their brand orientations.

    Perusal of the letters in the study also

    identified that letters to Santa written by

    children often indicate preferred

    choices. For example, Figure 2 indicates

    that not all requests will be successful

    and a strategy of identifying priority

    requests is important.

    H2: There will not be a significant

    difference between the politeness of

    girls gift request strategy and boys

    gift request strategy.

    Hypothesis 2 (H2) suggested that there

    is no significant difference between the

    politeness of boys and girls gift request

    strategy when children write the letters.

    Traits of politeness in both verbal and

    written strategies cover the affective

    appeals of friendship and simple pleas,

    Figure 2: Example of preferred choice

    Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817 45

    Dear Santa, do you have my brand?

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    10/18

    as well as the buddy style of written

    strategy. The politeness concept

    includes three elementary phrases of

    how are you; please; and thank you

    in both verbal and written

    communications strategies. In Figure 3

    Milindee asks Santa how are you? and

    uses the word please.

    Apart from requests for gifts, a single

    letter may contain direct, indirect and

    polite elements as semantic phrases as

    well as Christmas, product and

    signature graphics and Table 4 presents

    the distribution of request strategies and

    illustrations by authorship of letters.

    The use of politeness as a major

    communications strategy occurred in

    116 or 27.5 per cent of letters written bychildren and indicates children did not

    embrace a politeness strategy. There

    was an expectation that both boys and

    girls will embrace politeness similarly.

    Within the children authorship

    category, 36 boys and 80 girls used a

    politeness strategy and the results

    indicate a statistical difference in favour

    of girls (chi-square 6.71, d.f. p , 0.01).

    The hypothesis that there is no

    significant difference between the

    politeness of girls and boys is not

    supported. By comparison, Otnes et al.

    (1994) reported 36 boys and 57 girls

    used politeness elements in their letters

    and the difference approached

    statistical significance.

    H3: The letters girls write for themselves

    will contain significantly more

    indirect request strategies than

    letters boys write for themselves.

    Hypothesis 3 (H3) suggests the letters

    girls write for themselves contain

    significantly more indirect request

    strategies than letters boys write for

    themselves. Use of indirect statements is

    evident in 75.6 per cent of the letters, of

    which there were 115 (66.5 per cent)

    boys and 204 (81.9 per cent) girls. The

    difference between boys and girls use

    of indirect phrases is significant (chi-

    square 13.05, p , 0.01) and does not

    support Otnes et al. (1994) who reported

    no significant difference between the

    number of girls and boys using indirect

    requests.The findings show children act more

    in line with the nominal verbal

    communication strategies (Weiss and

    Sachs, 1991) that attribute the use of

    challenge or reason to boys and the

    simple expectation of having requests

    honoured to girls. Overall, girls use a

    request format of statements offered in

    the simple expectation of having

    requests honoured. The communication

    strategy construct places questionstyle

    statements like Will you bring me, can

    I have or please bring me as indirect

    requests. The letter from Melissa (Figure

    4) not only illustrates the use of the

    indirect phrase, I would like, it also

    demonstrates the polite use ofplease

    and the understanding that a choice

    from presents is expected.

    On the other hand, boys follow the

    Table 3 Patterns of requests by gender of authors

    Request style Boy Girl Total (%)

    Brand obsessed 32 33 65 15.4Only one brand and no other gifts mentioned

    Singularly branded 26 53 79 18.7One brand mentioned along with other non-

    branded giftsBrand majority 20 32 52 12.3

    One brand mentioned more than other brandsPluralistic 47 60 107 25.4

    Two or more brands mentioned equallyNo brand mentioned 48 71 119 28.2Total 173 249 422Percentage 41.0 59.0 100.0

    46 Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817

    Aron OCass and Peter Clarke

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    11/18

    strategy of rules, fair play and

    predominantly use the art of reason and

    direct challenge. Their communication

    consists of indirect strategies but also

    direct phrases like I want or bring me.

    Figure 5 shows Toms use of the strong

    phrase I want and does not use any

    element of politeness.

    Additionally, there is an observation

    about the use of direct phrases in letters

    that if girls use more indirect strategies

    than boys do, then boys should be

    expected to make more use of direct

    strategies than girls. Only 22 boys and

    19 girls letters made use of direct

    statement phrases such as I want or

    Figure 3: Politeness concept illustrated

    Table 4 Distribution of requests strategies by gender

    Author Politeness Indirect requests

    Boys 36 115Girls 80 204Significance of difference chi-squared 6.71, p , 0.01 chi-squared 13.05, p , 0.001

    Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817 47

    Dear Santa, do you have my brand?

    http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    12/18

    Figure 4: Indirect communication girl

    Figure 5: Direct communication boy

    48 Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817

    Aron OCass and Peter Clarke

  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    13/18

    bring me, and there is no significant

    difference between girls and boys

    usage of direct phrases. One possibility

    for minimal use of direct statements is

    that there is no immediate response or

    clarification of meaning as there is in

    speech and children understand thedifferences. Support is found for H3,

    however, because letters that girls write

    for themselves contain significantly

    more indirect requests than do boys

    letters.

    H4: There will not be a significant

    difference between the numbers of

    illustrations contained in girls and

    boys letters to Santa.

    When children write, they not onlyuse lists but also use illustrations in the

    form of cutouts, stickers or drawings as

    an examination method to express their

    ideas and thoughts. Of the 181 letters to

    Santa that contain illustrations 42.9 per

    cent of the letters were written by

    children and there is no significant

    difference between boys and girls

    overall use of illustrations, as indicated

    in Table 5. The analysis indicates no

    significant difference between boys and

    girls use of Christmas illustrations,

    however, boys use product illustrationsmore (chi-square 5.64, p, 0.05).

    Signature graphics also represents an

    affective appeal of friendship and an

    element of girls socialisation, and the

    results indicate girls use this illustrative

    format more than boys do (chi-square

    18.05, p , 0.001). Despite the differences

    within the illustration construct, H4 is

    supported because there is not a

    significant difference between the

    overall numbers of illustrations

    contained in girls and boys letters to

    Santa.

    DISCUSSION

    Christmas is many things to many

    people and the one aspect that mustinterest academic and general

    practitioners of marketing is the impact

    of brand names on request behaviour of

    children. Brand requests are a

    component of the overall gift request

    behaviour and the purpose of this

    research is to examine childrens brand

    identification and request styles at

    Christmas together with their written

    forms of communication. Past studies

    have focused on very subjective,

    product category-based constructs thatmake longitudinal comparisons

    untenable and the category definitions

    are problematic because of the use of

    different criteria to describe categories.

    As an example, Richardson and

    Simpson (1982) delineate specific

    categories of machines (models of work

    and construction equipment such as

    caterpillars), race cars and vehicles

    (representations of vehicles for carrying

    people or goods, eg cars, trucks). There

    is also a category for depots (places to

    store vehicles, eg garages, airports,space stations etc.). On the other hand

    Fisher-Thompson (1993), categorises

    these machines, race cars, vehicles and

    depots under the singular category of

    toy vehicles and includes other items

    such as trains and planes. Fisher-

    Thompson (1993), however, places farm

    equipment and barns in the category of

    farm and zoo animals, while Richardson

    and Simpson (1982) indicate toy animals

    consist of representational animals and

    Table 5 Distribution of illustrations

    Illustration element Boy Girl Significance of difference

    Christmas illustrations 43 65 N.S.D.Signature graphics 14 58 chi-square 18.05, df. 1, p, 0.001Product illustrations 17 10 chi-square 5.64, df. 1, p , 0.05

    Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817 49

    Dear Santa, do you have my brand?

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    14/18

    makes no mention of farm machinery

    and farm storage toys.

    Ultimately, a specific product or

    category could emerge, fade or fail as it

    moves through the product life cycle.

    Fisher-Thompson (1993) included

    categories of furnishings, clothing andaccessories, computer and videos that

    are not included in, or available to

    earlier studies (Richardson and

    Simpson, 1982; Robinson and Morris,

    1986). The nature of brands is such that,

    while they are subject to similar cyclical

    changes, they are a unique entity and

    identifier of specific products within

    categories. As an example, the Star Wars

    brand encompasses not just figures,

    space ships and space stations but is

    now a recognised brand in diverse

    categories such as confectionery,

    stationery, household linen and

    computer games, whereas the Barbie

    brand extends to magazines and a pop

    group. There is a multitude of

    subcategories within the broader

    category of toys, which are also a

    subcategory of Christmas requests.

    Therefore, brands are important and

    any research that seeks information on

    brand awareness and the prevalence of

    brand usage within the domain of

    childrens products is valuable.The rationale behind this stance is

    that a specific product or brand name

    request may not span time or location.

    What constituted a popular toy, or

    brand, in 1985 in America is not

    necessarily available in 2000 in Australia

    but the notion of using numbers of

    brand requests and the request strategy

    style remain immutable. The availability

    of particular brands in the marketplace

    at a given time will not affect the brand

    request theme of single brand, multiple

    brands and non-branded requests

    which are elements that are readily

    observable and measurable within in

    the Christmas request category and

    thereby, enable comparisons in the

    future.

    This study has focused on important

    issues relating to marketing of toys and

    the findings contribute to the

    understanding of brand request

    behaviour of adults and children at

    Christmas. It is quite illuminating to see

    the clarity with which children can

    identify specific brands and the method

    of manipulating the brands in theirwritten letters. Australian children are

    not unlike the American children that

    Otnes et al. (1994) indicated, have a

    strong awareness of the diversity of

    brand name goods available to them

    and the knowledge to use brand names

    and brand extensions in their request

    behaviour. Because children have a

    predisposition toward toys and their

    brands they possess the capability to

    practise either differentiation between

    brands or understand that brands can

    appear in different forms or categories.

    Children can clearly identify their

    preferred brand of toy and appear to

    understand the importance of explicitly

    requesting such brand names via clearly

    articulated request strategies.

    LIMITATIONS

    As is often the case with consumer

    research there are limitations, and while

    this study posed questions concerning

    the brand awareness and request styles

    of children and provided some answersto them, it does have some limitations

    that must be clearly expressed. This

    study is consistent with previous

    content analysis of letters written to

    Santa where the scope of analysis is

    limited to variables concerning the

    gender, requests and authorship content

    of the letters. Children of different ages

    would clearly have different brand

    experience levels which affect their

    brand nomination or usage, however, as

    in previous studies of this type, the age

    of the child is rarely cited in letters and

    absence of an age variable limited the

    scope of this study. Children may

    request different presents from different

    family members, as well as from Santa,

    and make use of a number of semantic

    phrases and request strategies.

    Therefore, the request behaviour

    50 Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817

    Aron OCass and Peter Clarke

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    15/18

    exhibited in this study is representative

    only of that population of children who

    write letters to Santa in Australia and

    are not generalisable outside the

    Australian Christmas period.

    IMPLICATIONSThis study shows there is a difference

    between boys and girls request

    behaviour and infers a low brand

    awareness and usage by adults in their

    request behaviour. The trend in

    marketing to children has seen their role

    move from one of demanders or

    influencers to important decision

    makers. Since brand owners seek

    growth of their brands in other product

    categories via brand extensions, that

    brand leaves the exclusive domain of

    the childrens market and moves into an

    adult-dominated market. If adults do

    not recognise, approve or trust the

    brand, then how can the brand transfer

    successfully to other categories without

    additional costs in the establishment of

    a position and image in this adult realm.

    Therefore, brand owners and

    advertisers of childrens brands must

    encourage adults to form a positive

    attitude toward the brand by use of a

    different promotional mix.

    FUTURE RESEARCH

    While this study has identified key

    issues relating to the brand awareness

    and request styles of children in letters

    to Santa, it also raises many related

    questions in need of future research

    which could be directed to understand

    what brands mean to children and to

    adults. Ideally, research could move

    from a childrens request focus to

    encompass assessment of parents

    attitude toward Christmas and the

    relationship with giving popular brand

    names as gifts to their children. In a

    similar vein, the relevance of giving

    both gifts and brands may also affect the

    appeal of brand as gifts and the usage of

    information sources by adults merits

    attention.

    The authors investigated the use of

    written semantic phrases and some

    related socialisation issues evident in

    letters to Santa and suggest that other

    issues could address how parents

    communicate with their children about

    requests and the relationship between

    this communication and a parentsevaluation of brands or use of

    information sources. Similarly, parents

    give gifts for different reasons and

    examination of the connection between

    these motives or roles and a parents

    attitude toward Christmas is

    appropriate.

    Research could also focus on learning

    and memory issues in Christmas gift

    request behaviour by adults and

    children, eg on the extent to which

    childrens requests are born from a

    long-term desire for the brand or are

    contingent on promotional activities as

    Christmas draws closer.

    Comparisons of the Christmas studies

    indicate boys made more requests in

    one study, girls in the other. Otnes et al.

    (1994b) suggest childrens exposure to

    the environment around them and their

    understanding of the relative financial

    wellbeing of their parents may reflect in

    numbers of presents requested, and in

    the use of brands. Therefore, a time

    series study could prove valuable inidentifying any variations of behaviour

    as the same population grows and

    qualify possible external factors that

    may influence Christmas request

    behaviour. Similarly, a cross-national or

    multicultural study would seek to

    qualify cultural differences, while a

    study that addresses segmentation by

    cultural background would provide an

    understanding of request behaviours in

    minority groups.

    CONCLUSION

    This study demonstrates that Australian

    children do not just request brands as

    gifts but requesting no brands is a

    feature of childrens request behaviour

    that probably reflects the tradition of

    requesting teddies, trucks or dollies

    along with those semantic phrases and

    Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817 51

    Dear Santa, do you have my brand?

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    16/18

    expressions that are bound in the

    Christmas wish list. Further, these

    young and budding consumers appear

    to be savvy to the importance of

    identifying clearly their desired brands

    because they are highly brand aware

    and understand that many brands exist,and different toy variants exist within a

    common brand name. Importantly, they

    seem to learn how to be successful

    through request strategies as

    Robinson and Morris (1986) indicate,

    children overtly request half of the gifts

    that are given at Christmas. The focus

    on brand names within the context of

    Christmas indicates that toy

    manufacturers and retailers who

    heavily promote not only their toys at

    Christmas, but also the Christmas myth

    and symbolism of giving their toys as

    gifts are gaining a competitive

    advantage. Perhaps many marketers

    and toy companies are getting their own

    Christmas wishes answered, via these

    budding, young, articulate, brand

    savvy, mini consumers.

    REFERENCES

    Bahn, K. (1986) How and When Do Brand

    Perceptions and Preferences First Form? A

    Cognitive Developmental Investigation,Journal of Consumer Research, 13, December,

    382393.

    Belk, R. (1987) A Childs Christmas in

    America: Santa Claus as Deity,

    Consumption as Religion, Journal of

    American Culture, 10(1), Spring, 87100.

    Belk, R. and Pollay, R. (1985) Images of

    Ourselves: The Good Life in Twentieth

    Century Advertising, Journal of Consumer

    Research, 11, 887897.

    Belk, R., Mayer, R. and Driscoll A. (1984)

    Childrens Recognition of Consumption

    Symbolism in Childrens Products, Journal

    of Consumer Research, 10, March, 386397.

    Caplow, T. and Williamson, M. H. (1980)

    Decoding Middletowns Easter Bunny. A

    Study in Iconography, Semiotica, 32 3/4,

    2123.

    Caplow, T., Bahr, H., Chadwick, B., Hill, R.

    and Williamson, M. (1982) Middletown

    Families: Fifty Years of Change,

    Minnesota Press.

    Caron, A. and Ward, S. (1975) Gift Decisions

    by Kids and Parents, Journal of Advertising

    Research, 15(24), August, 520.

    Clarke, T. (1984) Situational Factors

    Affecting Preschoolers Responses toAdvertising, Journal of the Academy of

    Marketing Science, 12(4), Fall, 2540.

    Downs, C. (1983) Letters to Santa Claus:

    Elementary School-Age Childrens Sex-

    Typed Toy Preferences in a Natural

    Setting, Sex Roles, 9(2), 159163.

    Erftmier, T. and Dyson, A. H. (1986) Oh,

    ppbbt~: Differences Between the Oral

    and Written Persuasive Strategies of

    School-Aged Children, Discourse-

    Processes, 9(1), JanMar, 91114.

    Fisher-Thompson, D. (1993) Adult Toy

    Purchases for Children: Factors Affecting

    Sex-Typed Toy Selection, Journal of

    Applied Development Psychology, 14,

    385406.

    Haynes, J., Burts, D., Dukes, A. and Cloud,

    R. (1993) Consumer Socialization of

    Preschoolers as Related to Clothing

    Consumption, Psychology and Marketing,

    10(2), March/April, 151161.

    Hite, C. and Hite, R. (1995) Reliance on

    Brand By Young Children, Journal of the

    Market Research Society, 37(2), 185193.

    John, D. and Sujan, M. (1990) AgeDifferences in Product Categorization,

    Journal of Consumer Research, 16(4),

    452460.

    Kolbe, R. and Burnett, M. (1991) Content

    Analysis Research: An Examination of

    Applications with Directives for

    Improving Research Reliability and

    Objectivity, Journal of Consumer Research,

    18, September, 243250.

    Krippendorf, K. (1980) Content Analysis:

    An Introduction to its Methodology, Sage,

    Beverly Hills, CA.

    Kunkel, D. and Roberts, D. (1991) Young

    Minds and Marketplace Values: Issues in

    Childrens Television Advertising, Journal

    of Social Issues, 47(1), Spring, 5772.

    Lackman, C. and Lanasa, J. (1993) Family

    Decision-Making Theory: An Overview

    and Assessment, Psychology & Marketing,

    10(2), March/April, 8193.

    52 Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817

    Aron OCass and Peter Clarke

    http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    17/18

    Macklin, C. (1994) The Impact of

    Audiovisual Information on Childrens

    Product-Related Recall, Journal of

    Consumer Research, 21, June, 154164.

    Malhotra, N., Hall, J., Shaw, M. and Crisp,

    M. (1996) Marketing Research: An

    Applied Orientation, Prentice Hall,Sydney.

    McNeal, J. (1992) Kids as Consumers: A

    Handbook for Marketing to Children,

    Lexington, NY.

    Meegan, G. (1993) News Update: World for

    Kids Targets Service, The Toy and Hobby

    Retailer, 6(4), July, 5.

    Miller, P. (1990) Kid Crazy, Catalog-Age,

    7(1), December, 1, 30.

    Mochis, G. (1985) The Role of Family

    Communication in Consumer

    Socialization of Children and Adolescent,

    Journal of Consumer Research, 11, March,

    868913.

    Olney, T., Holbrook, M. and Batra, R. (1991)

    Consumer Responses to Advertising: The

    Effects of Ad. Content, Emotions, and

    Attitude toward the Ad. on Viewing

    Time, Journal of Consumer Research, 17,

    March, 440453.

    Otnes, C., Kim, K. and Kim, Y. (1994a) Yes,

    Virginia, There is a Gender Difference:

    Analyzing Childrens Requests to Santa

    Claus, Journal of Popular Culture, 28:1,

    Summer, 1730.Otnes, C., Kim, Y. and Kim, K. (1994b) All I

    Want for Christmas: An Analysis of

    Childrens Brand Requests to Santa Claus,

    Journal of Popular Culture, 27(4), Spring,

    183195.

    Percy, L. and Rossiter, J. (1992) A Model of

    Brand Awareness and Brand Attitude

    Advertising Strategies, Psychology and

    Marketing, 9(4), July/August, 263274.

    Perrault, W. and Leigh, L. (1989) Reliability

    of Nominal Data Based on Qualitative

    Judgements, Journal of Market Research, 26,

    May, 135148.

    Phelps, J. and Hoy, M. (1996) The Aad-Ab-

    PI Relationship in Children: The Impact of

    Brand Familiarity and Measurement

    Timing, Psychology and Marketing, 13(1),

    January, 77105.

    Reda, S. (1995) Barbie and Board Games,

    Stores, 77(2), February, 3436.

    Richardson, J. and Simpson, C. (1982)Children, Gender, and Social Structure:

    An Analysis of the Contents of Letters to

    Santa Claus, Child Development, 53,

    429436.

    Robinson, C. and Morris, J. (1986) The

    Gender Stereotyped Nature of Christmas

    Toys received by 36, 48, and 60 month old

    Children: A Comparison Between Non-

    requested and Requested Toys, Sex Roles,

    15(1/2), 1-32.

    Sabegal, L. (date unknown) Christmas

    where the gum trees grow.

    Schiffman, L., Bednall, D., Coweley, E.,

    OCass, A., Watson, J. and Kanuk, L.

    (2001) Consumer Behaviour, 2nd edition,

    Prentice-Hall, Australia.

    Shook, S., Marrion, L. and Ollila, L. (1989)

    Primary Childrens Concepts About

    Writing, Journal of Educational Research,

    82(3), JanFeb, 133138.

    Vowles, E. (1996) Editorial Comment, The

    Toy and Hobby Retailer, 7(8), September, 5.

    Ward, S. and Wackman, D. (1968)

    Childrens Purchase Influence Attempts

    and Parental Yielding, in Kassarjian,H. H. and Scott, R. S. R. (eds) Perspectives

    in Consumer Behaviour, Foresham and

    Company, Ill.

    Ward, S., Wackman, D. and Wartella, E.

    (1977) How Kids Learn to Buy: The

    Development of Consumer Information

    Processing Skills, Sage, Beverly Hills.

    Weiss, D. and Sachs, J. (1991) Persuasive

    Strategies used by Preschool Children,

    Discourse Process, 14(1), JanMar, 5572.

    Wetton, N. (1996) Safe in the Sun, Health

    Education, 96(4), 1316.

    Zickmund, W. (1994) Exploring Marketing

    Research, Dryden Press, Fort Worth.

    Journal of Consumer Behaviour Vol. 2, 1, 3753 #Henry Stewart Publications 1472-0817 53

    Dear Santa, do you have my brand?

  • 8/3/2019 Dear Santa, Do You Have My Brand-18p

    18/18


Recommended