+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

Date post: 14-Apr-2018
Category:
Upload: brilliantibnusina
View: 218 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 25

Transcript
  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    1/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 1

    Just Lying There, Remembering: Improving Recall of Prose in Amnesic

    Patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment By Minimizing Interference

    Sergio Della Sala

    Neuropsychology Research Group, Department of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, UK

    Nelson Cowan

    Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA

    Nicoletta Beschin

    Dipartimento di Riabilitazione, Azienda Ospedaliera Gallarate,

    Ospedale Somma Lombardo (Va), Italy

    Michele Perini

    Dipartimento di Neurologia, Azienda Ospedaliera S. Antonio Abate Gallarate (Va), Italy

    Corresponding author:

    Nelson Cowan

    Department of Psychological Sciences

    University of Missouri

    210 McAlester HallColumbia, MO 65211

    E-mail: [email protected]: 573-882-4232

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    2/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 2

    Abstract

    The hallmark of amnesia is poor explicit long-term memory along with normal short-term

    memory. It is often stated that information encountered by amnesic patients is forgotten within 1 min

    of presentation. However, previous work has not distinguished between forgetting as a function of

    time vs. the interfering material occupying that time. We show that there is a marked benefit of

    reduced interference in amnesic patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), a condition that is

    characterized by anterograde amnesia in the absence of other neuropsychological deficits and carries

    an increased risk for Alzheimer disease. The result suggests that long-term memory is encoded in

    these patients to a greater extent than had been realized but that their memory is highly vulnerable to

    interference.

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    3/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 3

    Just Lying There, Remembering: Improving Recall of Prose in Amnesic

    Patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment By Minimizing Interference

    The hallmark of anterograde amnesia is fast forgetting of material to be remembered.

    Typically amnesic patients retain verbal information for no longer than one minute. If the

    forgetting were inevitable, it would allow the possibility that the event has not been encoded into

    the memory system used for voluntary, conscious recollection. In contrast, if memory could be

    retrieved under conditions of reduced interference, this would indicate that the encoding did

    occur, at least in a weakened form.

    The shallow encoding could take two forms. The information could remain in an active

    or conscious form continually in memory (i.e., in working memory; see Baddeley, 1986; Cowan,

    1995), from the time of its presentation until the time of recall. Alternatively, the information

    could be encoded in a form that becomes dormant or inactive, leaving consciousness (i.e., in

    long-term memory), and yet could remain available for retrieval so long as subsequent events are

    too weak to interfere with the original encoding context.

    Every seasoned clinician may notice that anterograde amnesic patients are vulnerable to

    interference. However, there are few objective data on it and, in clinical situations, it is next to

    impossible to determine the role of interference as opposed to the passage of time because

    patients (like the rest of us) are rarely free of interference for more than a few minutes. To

    examine this issue, we investigated the role of interference in verbal recall. To minimize the

    possible role of active rehearsal we relied on prose memory with immediate and one-hour-

    delayed recall. This was prompted by the recent report that amnesiacs show a relative good

    performance on immediate recall of prose passages (Baddeley & Wilson, 2002). The critical

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    4/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 4

    manipulation was the use of ordinary interference and very-low-interference conditions during

    the hour-long retention interval. If recall in amnesiacs can be improved by reducing interference,

    that finding would show that amnesiacs do encode information into a system that can be used for

    deliberate, verbal recall, despite the popular view to the contrary. The suggestion that amnesiacs

    may suffer exaggerated effects of retroactive interference can be gleaned from the literature (e.g.,

    Moscovitch, 1994; Shimamura, Jurica, Mangels, Gershberg & Knight, 1995). However, until

    now, this hint has not been backed up empirically.

    Cowan, Beschin, and Della Sala (work in preparation) used this type of test with six

    amnesiacs who had a variety of brain lesions and found that four of them benefited profoundly

    from reduced interference during a 1-hour retention interval, whereas the other two showed no

    benefit. At this point it is unclear why only some amnesiacs benefited and it would help to

    observe the effect in a defined subcategory of amnesia. For that purpose, patients with Mild

    Cognitive Impairment (MCI) were recruited. They are well-suited to this test because they

    display anterograde amnesia in the absence of overt dementia or other neuropsychological

    deficits. Fortunately for this work, as well, their immediate recall appears to be fairly

    comparable to that of normal control participants, allowing valid comparisons of forgetting (see

    below). They are also of special practical concern because MCI carries an increased risk for

    developing Alzheimer disease (Petersen, Smith, Waring, Ivnik, Tangalos, & Kokmen, 1999).

    Methods

    Participants

    Ten patients with MCI (six women and four men, mean age = 67.40 years, SD = 8.96; mean

    education = 5.60 years, SD = 1.26) are described in Table 1, which gives ages, education, and criterion

    scores for the patients, and Table 2, which gives further psychometric test data for them. No MCI

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    5/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 5

    patients were included in Cowan et al. (submitted). A group of ten age-matched, healthy controls (five

    women and five men, mean age = 68.30 years, SD = 5.79, mean education = 5.4 years, SD = 0.96) also

    participated in this experiment. All MCI patients entering the study had a history of incipient

    anterograde memory deficits as evinced from clinical notes, relatives reports, and interview with the

    patients and their caregivers; no signs of focal lesions (computerized tomography or magnetic

    resonance imaging); no signs of abnormalities in the neurological examination; normal Mini-Mental

    Examination score; and normal blood tests (e.g., cell counts, thyroid hormones levels, syphilis

    serologic testing, vitamin B12 and folic acid levels). Moreover, none of the patients had a history of

    psychiatric illness or was assuming psychoactive drugs at the time of testing; and they show no hints

    of depression as assessed by the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, 1961). They all had a Clinical

    Dementia Rating Scale (CDR - Hughes, Berg, Danziger, Coben, & Martin, 1982) score equal to 0.5

    indicating an overwhelming deficit of memory as compared to all other cognitive domains and

    activities of daily living, and they scored above 9, i.e. within the normal range, in the Instrumental

    Activity of Daily Living (IADL Lawson & Brody, 1969). Moreover, they scored below cut-off in the

    delayed recall of the Rey-type Word List Recall Test (Carlesimo, Caltagirone, & Gainotti,1996). On

    the other hand, all patients scored above cut-off in tests assessing language comprehension (including

    normal performance with the long sentences of the Token Test) and production, verbal fluency,

    immediate copy of a geometrical figure, Trail Making, and verbal and spatial short-term memory (see

    Table 2). Finally, none of the patients showed signs of dementia when retested to this end after six

    months.

    ----- Insert Tables 1 and 2 about here -----

    Materials and Procedure

    All participants were presented with four stories orally (selected from a pool of seven

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    6/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 6

    standardized passages), were asked for immediate verbatim recall, and then were asked for delayed

    recall an hour later. The stories were in Italian, the mother tongue of all participants, and were derived

    from the available literature (Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987; Brazzelli, Della Sala & Laiacona, 1993;

    Capitani, Della Sala, Laiacona, Marchetti, & Spinnler, 1994). The length of the stories varied from 21

    to 64 content words and verbatim recall was scored with credit for all function words conveying the

    correct meanings. Cowan et al. (submitted) reported inter-rater reliabilities for recall scoring of >.95

    for each condition. Proportion of retention (delayed recall as a proportion of the preceding immediate

    recall on the same trial) was used in the analyses.

    Two of the stories were followed by a 1-h test delay filled with various psychometric tasks to

    create a standard amount of interference. The other two stories were followed by a delay in which the

    participant reclined in a dark, quiet room for the entire 1-h period to minimize interference. They were

    not told that delayed recall would be requested, although a participant theoretically might expect this

    after the first trial. Experimental testing took place in two sessions carried out on different days. The

    selection of stories for each condition and the order of conditions were random but proved to be well-

    matched across groups and conditions.

    Results

    Average proportions of immediate recall (with standard errors of the mean) were similar across

    groups: .49 (.05) and .46 (.04), respectively, for the healthy controls and the patient group. Retention

    in delayed tests was measured on each trial as the amount recalled following the delay divided by the

    amount recalled in the immediate test on the same trial (i.e., delayed / immediate). Healthy controls

    showed an average retention of .80 (.03) with the usual interference and .89 (.05) with minimal

    interference. The MCI patients showed a marked difference, from .20 (.03) with the usual interference

    up to .55 (.05) with minimal interference (see Figure 1).

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    7/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 7

    In an analysis of variance of the retention with the patient group as a between-participant

    factor and the interference condition as a within-participant factor, both main effects and the

    interaction all were highly significant, p < .001 in each case. [Group: F(1, 18) = 80.48, MSE =

    0.03; Interference, F(1, 18) = 76.96, MSE = 0.01; interaction, F(1, 18) = 27.19, MSE = 0.01.]

    We do not interpret the interaction because of ceiling effects in the controls and the poorer

    overall retention in amnesiacs simply reflects their amnesic deficit in delayed recall.

    The key finding is that separate analyses for each of the two participant groups showed

    that the retention was significantly larger for the minimal-interference condition than for the

    usual-interference condition in both groups (p < .001 in both cases). The improvement of

    retention by reducing interference perhaps would have been predicted by most investigators for

    normal controls, but this improvement is newsworthy and informative for amnesic patients.

    The performance characteristics of each patient on the story recall task are reported in

    Table 3. There was a large amount of individual difference in the extent to which patients

    benefited from minimization of interference. It is not yet clear why this difference occurs.

    Notice, for example, that Patient 4 went from a retention score of 0 with the usual interference

    to .38 with interference minimized, whereas Patient 6 went from 0 only to .08, and thus benefited

    much less than Patient 4. This cannot be easily attributed to differences in their levels of

    immediate recall, which were fairly similar as the table shows. Among all 10 patients, the

    relation between retention in the minimal-interference and usual-interference conditions was

    significant, r = .67. However, considering all of the variables presented in Tables 1 - 3, there

    were no other correlations with retention in the minimal-interference condition. (There was an

    almost-significant correlation between the level of immediate recall and retention in the usual

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    8/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 8

    interference condition, r = .61. Immediate recall also was significantly correlated with age, r =

    -.72, and performance on the Token Test, r = .66.)

    ----- Insert Table 3 about here -----

    A final, important issue is what patients did during the unfilled delay period. It is

    possible that some of them reminisced about the story for at least part of the interval and thus

    retained elements of the story in working memory. However, Patients 3 and 7 were observed to

    snore throughout portions of both unfilled delays and therefore could not have maintained the

    information in working memory, unless working memory is found to continue during sleep

    (Jenkins & Dallenbach, 1924, attributed to lack of interference the savings from forgetting they

    observed in two participants during sleeping periods compared to when they were awake). As

    Table 3 shows, these patients nevertheless appeared to benefit from the reduction of interference.

    The case is clearest for Patient 7, who went from .31 retention with usual interference to .61

    retention with minimal interference despite snoring under minimal interference.

    Discussion

    Minimal-interference conditions resulted in much better delayed recall than the usual-

    interference condition in the MCI patients. Since patients with Alzheimer Disease (AD) perform at

    floor in delayed retrieval of prose passages (e.g., Spinnler & Della Sala, 1987) and in our own pilot

    studies they did not benefit from the minimal interference condition, this procedure may add to the

    much sought (see e.g., Petersen et al., 1999) differential diagnosis between MCI and AD.

    These findings could hardly be accounted for by postulating the use of subvocal rehearsal

    during the no-interference condition on several grounds. First, the delay was long enough to

    discourage the strategy of sheer rehearsal. Second, several participants fell asleep (and, as noted, some

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    9/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 9

    snored) during the retention interval, as testified by the experimenters regular checks. Third during

    the debriefing session, only patient 7 volunteered that she used subvocal rehearsal as a strategy to

    optimize recall (and that attempt would have been affected by short-term memory loss during sleep).

    Hence, long-term retrieval, rather than only short-term rehearsal, seems improved by the lack of

    interference.

    In summary, we have shown that the deficit underlying amnesia in MCI is characterized

    by increased vulnerability to interference and that reducing the interference can result in a

    substantial increase in memory performance. These findings indicate that amnesia, at least that

    characterizing MCI, does not necessarily imply an absence of memory encoding and that new

    explicit memory representations can be constructed by MCI patients. However, these memory

    traces are highly susceptible to interference from intervening events.

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    10/25

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    11/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 11

    del test e per lo studio crosslinguistico dei disturbi afasici. Archivio di Psicologia

    Neurologia Psichiatria, 47, 209-237.

    De Renzi, E., & Faglioni, P. (1978). Normative data and screening power of a shortened version

    of the Token Test. Cortex, 14, 41-49.

    Giovagnoli, A.R, Del Pesce, M., Mascheroni, S., Simoncelli, M., Laiacona, M., & Capitani, E. (1996).

    Trail making test: normative values from 287 normal adult controls. Italian Journal

    Neurological Sciences, 17, 305-309.

    Hughes, C.P., Berg, L., Danziger, W.L., Coben, L.A., & Martin, R.L. (1982). A new clinical

    scale for the staging dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 140 , 566- 572.

    Jenkins, J.G., & Dallenbach, K.M. (1924) Oblivescence during sleep and waking. American

    Journal of Psychology, 35, 605-612.

    Lawson, M.P., & Brody, E.M. (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental

    activities of daily living. Gerontologist, 9, 179-186.

    1Measso, G., Cavarzeran, F., Zappala, G., Lebowitz, B.D., Crook, T.H., Pirozzolo, F.J., Amaducci,

    L.A., Massari, D. & Grigoletto, F. (1993). The Mini-Mental State Examination: Normative

    study of Italian random sample. Developmental Neuropsychology, 9, 77-85.

    1Moscovitch, M. (1994). Memory and working with memory: Evaluation of a component process

    model and comparisons with other models. In D.L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory

    systems 1994 (pp. 269 310). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    1Orsini, A., Grossi, D., Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1987). Verbal and

    spatial immediate memory span: Normative data from 1355 adults and 1112 children. Italian

    Journal of Neurological Science, 8, 539-548.

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    12/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 12

    Petersen R.C., Smith G.E., Waring S.C., Ivnik, R.J., Tangalos, E.G., & Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild

    cognitive impairment. Clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology, 56, 303-

    308.

    11Shimamura, A.P., Jurica, P. J., Mangels, J. A., Gershberg, F. B., & Knight, R. T. (1995).

    Susceptibility to memory interference effects following frontal lobe damage: Findings

    from tests of paired-associate learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 144-152.

    Spinnler, H., & Della Sala, S. (1988). The role of clinical neuropsychology in the neurological

    diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, 235, 258-271.

    Spinnler, H., & Tognoni, G. (Eds) (1987). Standardizzazione e Taratura italiana di test

    neuropsicologici. The Italian Journal Neurological Sciences , Suppl 8 to No. 6: 1-120.

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    13/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 13

    Table 1.

    Demographic Features of the MCI Patients Entering the Study and their Performance in theDiagnostic Tests.

    MCI/ Pt. No. Age Education(years) MMSEscore1 Rey-typeWord List

    Delayed2

    1 73 8 30 3.9

    2 66 5 30 1.8

    3 68 5 30 2.4

    4 80 5 26.03 3.1

    5 72 5 27.03 2.4

    6 76 5 30 3.1

    7 50 8 29.07 2.6

    8 56 5 28.24 0.7

    9 66 5 28.99 2.810 67 5 30 4.4

    Notes

    1 Score range, 0-30; cut-off for normal performance, 23.8 (for the Italian population). Scores

    adjusted for age and education (Measso et al., 1993).

    2 Fifteen-word lists learning delayed recall. Score range, 0-15; cut-off for normal performance,

    4.69 (Carlesimo et al., 1996).

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    14/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 14

    Table 2

    Neuropsychological Profile of the MCI Patients

    MCI

    Patient

    Number

    Naming Token

    Test

    Phonetic

    Fluency

    Semantic

    Fluency

    Word List

    Learning

    Immediate

    Digit

    Span

    Spatial

    Span

    Rey Figure

    Immediate

    Rey

    Figure

    Delayed

    Trail

    Making

    1 9 35 31.7 16.75 28.9 9.25 7.25 36 9.75 1362 9 33 34.9 21.25 18.1* 5.5 5.5 36 6.75* 145

    3 9 35 32.6 20.0 38.0 6.5 6.5 35 6.75* 92

    4 9 31 25.7 14.25 36.2 4.75 4.75 36 7.75* 81

    5 9 32 26.6 19.6 28.0* 4.5 4.5 36 9.75 116

    6 9 31 30.4 16.75 30.0 4.5 4.5 32.75 11.75 168

    7 9 35 24.1 18.5 23.3* 6.0 6.0 35.25 3.5* 124

    8 9 32 24.8 16.75 17.8* 4.5 4.25 36 5.5* 81

    9 9 32 21.25 10.25 20.1* 5.5 4.25 33.25 6.75* 122

    10 9 34 28.9 14.25 33.1 5.5 6.25 36 5.5* 114

    Note. Scores are age- and education-adjusted. Asterisk (*) indicates performances below the

    normal cut-off score. Naming test from theAachener Aphasie Test AAT, 120 stimuli, cut-off:8 (De Bleser et al., 1986). Token Test, score range: 0-36, cut-off score: 26.5 (De Renzi &

    Faglioni, 1978). Phonemic Verbal Fluency, words beginning with F, A or S, cut-off score: 17.35

    (Carlesimo et al., 1996). Semantic Verbal Fluency, names of colors, animals, fruit, cities, cut-offscore: 7.25 (Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987). Rey-Type Word List Learning(15 word list, repeated 5

    times), score range: 0-75, cut-off score: 28.53 (Carlesimo et al., 1996). Digit Span (Orsini et al.,

    1987), cut-off score: 3.5 (digit span in Italian is generally lower than in English due to the longerpronunciation times of the digits). Spatial Span (Orsini et al., 1987), cut-off score: 3.25. Rey

    Figure Immediate, score range: 0-36, cut-off score: 28.87 (Caffarra et al., 2002). Rey Figure

    Delayed, score range 0-36, cut-off score: 9.46 (Caffarra et al., 2002). Trail Making, time in

    seconds, section A section B, the higher the worse, cut-off score: 187 (Giovagnoli et al., 1996).

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    15/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 15

    Table 3.

    Individual Mean Performance Characteristics of the MCI Patients in the Experimental Prose

    Recall Tasks

    Patient Immediate

    Recall:Proportion

    Correct

    Interference:

    Retention(Delayed/

    Immediate)

    No

    Interference:Retention

    (Del./Imm.)

    1 .52 .14 .54

    2 .54 .25 .48

    3 .58 .40 .59

    4 .20 .00 .38

    5 .50 .33 .67

    6 .32 .00 .08

    7 .57 .31 .61

    8 .61 .11 .62

    9 .40 .32 .8810 .46 .14 .67

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    16/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 16

    Figure Caption

    Figure 1. Proportion retention in the experimental story recall task following usual

    interference (left) and minimal interference (right) for normal control participants (dark bars) and

    MCI patients (white bars). Retention is calculated on each trial as the proportion of correct

    verbatim recall in 1-hour-delayed recall relative to (divided by) immediate recall. Error bars are

    standard errors.

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    17/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 17

    Figure 1

    0

    0.1

    0.2

    0.3

    0.4

    0.5

    0.6

    0.7

    0.8

    0.9

    1

    Usual Minimal

    Amount of Interference

    Proportion

    Retentio

    n

    Normal Control

    MCI Patient

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    18/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 18

    Hanya Berbaring sana, Mengingat: Meningkatkan Recall of Prosa di amnesic

    Pasien dengan Penurunan Mild Cognitive

    Hanya Berbaring sana, Mengingat: Meningkatkan Recall of Prosa di amnesicPasien dengan Penurunan Mild Cognitive Dengan Meminimalkan Interferensi

    Sergio Della Sala

    Neuropsikologi Research Group, Departemen Psikologi, University of Aberdeen, InggrisNelson Cowan

    Departemen Ilmu Psikologi, Universitas Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA

    Nicoletta Beschin

    Dipartimento di Riabilitazione, Azienda Ospedaliera Gallarate,Ospedale Somma Lombardo (Va), Italia

    Michele Perini

    Dipartimento di Neurologia, Azienda Ospedaliera S. Antonio Abate Gallarate (Va), Italia

    Sesuai penulis:

    Nelson Cowan

    Departemen Ilmu PsikologiUniversity of Missouri

    210 McAlester Balai

    Columbia, MO 65211

    E-mail: [email protected]

    Telepon: 573-882-4232

    Abstrak

    Ciri dari amnesia adalah miskin eksplisit memori jangka panjang bersama dengan normalmemori jangka pendek. Hal ini sering menyatakan bahwa informasi yang dihadapi oleh pasien

    amnesic dilupakan dalam 1 menit presentasi. Namun, pekerjaan sebelumnya belum dibedakan

    antara melupakan sebagai fungsi waktu vs bahan campur menempati waktu itu. Kami

    menunjukkan bahwa ada manfaat ditandai gangguan berkurang pada pasien amnesic denganPenurunan Mild Cognitive (MCI), suatu kondisi yang ditandai dengan amnesia anterograde tanpa

    adanya defisit neuropsikologi lainnya dan membawa peningkatan risiko untuk penyakit

    Alzheimer. Hasilnya menunjukkan bahwa memori jangka panjang dikodekan pada pasiendengan tingkat yang lebih besar daripada yang telah menyadari tetapi memori mereka sangat

    rentan terhadap gangguan.

    Hanya Berbaring sana, Mengingat: Meningkatkan Recall of Prosa di amnesic

    Pasien dengan Penurunan Mild Cognitive Dengan Meminimalkan Interferensi

    Ciri amnesia anterograde cepat melupakan bahan untuk diingat. Biasanya pasien amnesic

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    19/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 19

    menyimpan informasi verbal untuk tidak lebih dari satu menit. Jika lupa tidak bisa dihindarkan,

    akan memungkinkan kemungkinan bahwa acara tersebut belum dikodekan ke dalam sistem

    memori yang digunakan untuk sukarela, ingatan sadar. Sebaliknya, jika memori dapat diambildalam kondisi gangguan berkurang, ini akan menunjukkan bahwa pengkodean itu terjadi,

    setidaknya dalam bentuk lemah.

    Pengkodean dangkal bisa mengambil dua bentuk. Informasi bisa tetap dalam bentuk aktif atausadar terus-menerus dalam memori (yaitu, dalam memori kerja, lihat Baddeley, 1986; Cowan,

    1995), dari saat presentasi sampai saat recall. Atau, informasi dapat dikodekan dalam bentuk

    yang menjadi aktif atau tidak aktif, meninggalkan kesadaran (yaitu, dalam memori jangkapanjang), namun bisa tetap tersedia untuk pengambilan selama kejadian setelah terlalu lemah

    untuk mengganggu konteks pengkodean asli .

    Setiap dokter berpengalaman mungkin memperhatikan bahwa pasien amnesic anterograde rentan

    terhadap gangguan. Namun, ada beberapa data yang obyektif di atasnya dan, dalam situasi klinis,itu adalah mustahil untuk menentukan peran interferensi yang bertentangan dengan berlalunya

    waktu karena pasien (seperti sisa dari kita) jarang bebas dari gangguan selama lebih dari

    beberapa menit. Untuk memeriksa masalah ini, kami meneliti peran gangguan ingat verbal.

    Untuk meminimalkan kemungkinan peran aktif latihan kita bergantung pada memori prosadengan recall langsung dan satu-jam-tertunda. Hal ini dipicu oleh laporan terbaru bahwa amnesia

    menunjukkan kinerja yang baik relatif pada ingat segera bagian prosa (Baddeley & Wilson,2002). Manipulasi penting adalah penggunaan gangguan biasa dan sangat-rendah-gangguan

    kondisi selama interval retensi selama satu jam. Jika recall di amnesia dapat ditingkatkan dengan

    mengurangi gangguan, yang menemukan akan menunjukkan bahwa amnesia yang mengkodekaninformasi ke dalam sistem yang dapat digunakan untuk disengaja, ingat verbal, meskipun

    pandangan populer yang bertentangan. Saran yang amnesia mungkin menderita efek berlebihan

    gangguan retroaktif dapat diperoleh dari literatur (misalnya, Moscovitch, 1994; Shimamura,

    Jurica, Mangels, Gershberg & Knight, 1995). Namun, sampai sekarang, petunjuk ini belumdidukung secara empiris.

    Cowan, Beschin, dan Della Sala (pekerjaan dalam persiapan) menggunakan jenis uji dengan

    enam amnesia yang memiliki berbagai lesi otak dan menemukan bahwa empat dari merekamanfaat mendalam dari gangguan berkurang selama interval retensi 1 jam, sedangkan dua

    lainnya menunjukkan tidak bermanfaat. Pada titik ini tidak jelas mengapa hanya beberapa

    amnesia manfaat dan akan membantu untuk mengamati efek dalam subkategori didefinisikanamnesia. Untuk itu, pasien dengan Penurunan Mild Cognitive (MCI) direkrut. Mereka cocok

    untuk tes ini karena mereka menampilkan amnesia anterograde tanpa adanya demensia terang-

    terangan atau defisit neuropsikologi lainnya. Untungnya untuk pekerjaan ini, juga, ingat

    langsung mereka tampaknya cukup sebanding dengan peserta kontrol normal, memungkinkanperbandingan yang valid lupa (lihat di bawah). Mereka juga menjadi perhatian khusus karena

    praktis MCI membawa peningkatan risiko untuk mengembangkan penyakit Alzheimer (Petersen,

    Smith, Waring, Ivnik, Tangalos, & Kokmen, 1999).

    Metode

    PesertaSepuluh pasien dengan MCI (enam perempuan dan empat laki-laki, usia rata-rata = 67.40 tahun,

    SD = 8.96, pendidikan rata-rata = 5,60 tahun, SD = 1,26) dijelaskan pada Tabel 1, yang

    memberikan usia, pendidikan, dan skor kriteria untuk pasien, dan Tabel 2, yang memberikan

    data uji lanjut psikometri untuk mereka. Tidak ada pasien MCI dimasukkan dalam Cowan et al.

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    20/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 20

    (Disampaikan). Sekelompok sepuluh usia-cocok, kontrol sehat (lima perempuan dan lima laki-

    laki, usia rata-rata = 68,30 tahun, SD = 5,79, berarti pendidikan = 5,4 tahun, SD = 0,96) juga

    berpartisipasi dalam penelitian ini. Semua pasien MCI memasuki studi itu memiliki sejarah barujadi defisit memori anterograde seperti yang tampak dari catatan klinis, laporan kerabat ', dan

    wawancara dengan pasien dan pengasuh mereka, tidak ada tanda-tanda lesi fokal (computerized

    tomography atau magnetic resonance imaging), tidak ada tanda-tanda kelainan pada pemeriksaanneurologis, yang normal Mini-Mental nilai Ujian, dan tes darah normal (misalnya, jumlah sel,

    kadar hormon tiroid, sifilis pengujian serologi, vitamin B12 dan kadar asam folat). Selain itu,

    tidak ada pasien memiliki riwayat penyakit jiwa atau berasumsi obat psikoaktif pada saatpengujian, dan mereka tidak menunjukkan petunjuk depresi sebagaimana dinilai oleh Beck

    Depression Inventory (Beck, 1961). Mereka semua memiliki Penilaian Skala Demensia Klinis

    (CDR - Hughes, Berg, Danziger, Coben, & Martin, 1982) skor sebesar 0,5 menunjukkan defisit

    besar memori dibandingkan dengan semua domain kognitif lainnya dan aktivitas sehari-hari, danmereka mencetak gol diatas 9, yaitu dalam kisaran normal, dalam Kegiatan Instrumental Hidup

    Harian (IADL - Lawson & Brody, 1969). Selain itu, mereka mencetak gol di bawah cut-off di

    recall tertunda dari Uji Rey-jenis Daftar kata Recall (Carlesimo, Caltagirone, & Gainotti, 1996).

    Di sisi lain, semua pasien mencetak di atas cut-off dalam tes menilai pemahaman bahasa(termasuk kinerja normal dengan kalimat panjang Test Token) dan produksi, kefasihan lisan,

    copy langsung dari sebuah figur yang geometris, Trail Making, dan verbal dan spasial memorijangka pendek (lihat Tabel 2). Akhirnya, tidak ada pasien menunjukkan tanda-tanda demensia

    ketika diuji ulang untuk tujuan ini setelah enam bulan.

    ----- Insert Tabel 1 dan 2 tentang di sini -----Bahan dan Prosedur

    Semua peserta disajikan dengan empat cerita lisan (dipilih dari kolam dari tujuh bagian standar),

    diminta untuk mengingat verbatim segera, dan kemudian diminta untuk mengingat tertunda satu

    jam kemudian. Cerita-cerita itu dalam bahasa Italia, bahasa ibu dari semua peserta, dan berasaldari literatur yang tersedia (Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987; Brazzelli, Della Sala & Laiacona, 1993;

    Capitani, Della Sala, Laiacona, Marchetti, & Spinnler, 1994) . Panjang cerita bervariasi 21-64

    kata konten dan mengingat verbatim itu mencetak gol dengan kredit untuk semua kata fungsimenyampaikan makna yang benar. Cowan et al. (Disampaikan) melaporkan reliabilitas antar-

    penilai untuk mencetak penarikan kembali> .95 untuk setiap kondisi. Proporsi retensi (recall

    tertunda sebagai proporsi dari recall langsung sebelumnya pada sidang yang sama) yangdigunakan dalam analisis.

    Dua dari cerita diikuti oleh penundaan uji 1-h diisi dengan tugas psikometri berbagai membuat

    sejumlah standar gangguan. Dua cerita lainnya yang diikuti dengan penundaan di mana peserta

    berbaring di ruangan gelap, tenang untuk periode 1-h keseluruhan untuk meminimalkangangguan. Mereka tidak diberitahu bahwa recall tertunda akan diminta, meskipun peserta secara

    teoritis mungkin berharap ini setelah sidang pertama. Pengujian eksperimental berlangsung

    dalam dua sesi dilakukan pada hari yang berbeda. Pemilihan cerita untuk setiap kondisi danurutan kondisi yang acak tetapi terbukti sangat cocok di seluruh kelompok dan kondisi.

    Hasil

    Proporsi rata recall langsung (dengan kesalahan standar dari mean) adalah serupa di seluruhkelompok: .49 (.05) dan .46 (.04), masing-masing, untuk kontrol yang sehat dan kelompok

    pasien. Retensi dalam tes tertunda diukur pada setiap percobaan karena jumlahnya ingat

    menyusul keterlambatan dibagi dengan jumlah ingat dalam tes langsung pada sidang yang sama

    (yaitu, tertunda / langsung). Kontrol yang sehat menunjukkan retensi rata-rata .80 (.03) dengan

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    21/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 21

    gangguan biasa dan .89 (.05) dengan gangguan minimal. Para pasien MCI menunjukkan

    perbedaan yang nyata, dari .20 (.03) dengan gangguan yang biasa sampai .55 (.05) dengan

    gangguan minimal (lihat Gambar 1).Dalam analisis varians dari retensi dengan kelompok pasien sebagai faktor-antara peserta dan

    kondisi gangguan sebagai faktor dalam-peserta, baik efek utama dan interaksi semua sangat

    signifikan, p

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    22/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 22

    minimal, prosedur ini dapat menambah banyak dicari (lihat misalnya, Petersen et al, 1999.)

    diferensial diagnosis antara MCI dan AD.

    Temuan ini hampir tidak bisa dipertanggungjawabkan oleh mendalilkan penggunaan latihansubvocal selama kondisi no-gangguan pada beberapa alasan. Pertama, penundaan itu cukup lama

    untuk mencegah strategi latihan belaka. Kedua, beberapa peserta tertidur (dan, seperti telah

    disebutkan, beberapa mendengkur) selama interval retensi, sebagaimana dibuktikan olehpemeriksaan reguler eksperimen itu. Ketiga selama sesi tanya jawab, hanya pasien 7 sukarela

    bahwa dia menggunakan latihan subvocal sebagai strategi untuk mengoptimalkan recall (dan

    upaya yang akan telah dipengaruhi oleh jangka pendek kehilangan memori saat tidur). Olehkarena itu, dalam jangka panjang pengambilan, bukan hanya jangka pendek latihan, tampaknya

    ditingkatkan dengan kurangnya gangguan.

    Singkatnya, kami telah menunjukkan bahwa amnesia defisit yang mendasari di MCI ditandai

    dengan meningkatnya kerentanan terhadap gangguan dan mengurangi gangguan dapatmengakibatkan peningkatan yang substansial dalam kinerja memori. Temuan ini menunjukkan

    bahwa amnesia, setidaknya itu mencirikan MCI, tidak selalu berarti tidak adanya pengkodean

    memori dan bahwa representasi eksplisit memori baru dapat dibangun oleh pasien MCI. Namun,

    jejak-jejak memori sangat rentan terhadap gangguan dari intervensi peristiwa.

    ReferensiBaddeley, M (1986). Memori kerja. Oxford Psikologi Seri # 11. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Baddeley, A., & Wilson, B.A. (2002). Prosa recall dan amnesia: Implikasi untuk struktur memori

    kerja. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1.737-1.743.

    Beck, A. T. A (1961). Sistematis penyelidikan depresi. Komprehensif Psychiatry, 2, 163-170.Brazzelli, M., Della Sala, S., & Laiacona, M. (1993). Taratura della versione Italiana del

    Rivermead Perilaku Memory Test: Un tes di valutazione ecologica della memoria. Supplemento

    al manuale del TMCR. Firenze (Florence, Italia): Organizzazioni Speciali.Caffarra, P., Vezzadini, G., Dieci, F., Zonato, F., & Venneri, A. (2002) angka Rey-Osterrieth

    kompleks: normatif nilai dalam suatu sampel populasi Italia. Neurologis Ilmu, 22, 443-447.

    Capitani, E., Della Sala, S., Laiacona, M., Marchetti, C., & Spinnler, H. (1994).Standardizzazione ed uso Clinico di un tes di memoria di PROSA. Bollettino di Psicologia

    Applicata, 209, 47-63

    Carlesimo, GA, Caltagirone, C., & Gainotti, G. (1996). Baterai kemunduran mental: normatif

    data, keandalan diagnostik dan analisis kualitatif penurunan kognitif. Kelompok untukstandarisasi Baterai Penurunan Mental. Eropa Neurologi 36: 378-384.

    Cowan, N. (1995). Perhatian dan memori: Sebuah kerangka kerja terpadu. Oxford Psikologi

    Series, No 26. New York: Oxford University Press.De Bleser, R., Denes, F., Luzzatti, C., Mazzucchi, A., Poeck, K., Spinnler, H., & WILLMES, K.

    (1986). L'Aachener Aphasie Test (AAT). Problemi e soluzioni per versione una uji italiana del e

    per lo studio crosslinguistico dei disturbi afasici. Archivio di Psicologia Neurologia Psichiatria,47, 209-237.

    De Renzi, E., & Faglioni, P. (1978). Normatif data dan daya pemutaran versi singkat dari Uji

    Token. Cortex, 14, 41-49.

    Giovagnoli, AR, Del Pesce, M., Mascheroni, S., Simoncelli, M., Laiacona, M., & Capitani, E.

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    23/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 23

    (1996). Trail membuat tes: normatif nilai dari 287 kontrol dewasa normal. Italia Jurnal Ilmu

    Neurologis, 17, 305-309.

    Hughes, CP, Berg, L., Danziger, WL, Coben, LA, & Martin, RL (1982). Sebuah skala klinis baruuntuk demensia pementasan. British Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 566-572.

    Jenkins, J.G., & Dallenbach, K.M. (1924) Oblivescence saat tidur dan bangun. American Journal

    of Psychology,, 35 605-612.Lawson, M.P., & Brody, E.M. (1969). Penilaian orang tua: aktivitas Self-Memelihara dan

    instrumental dari kehidupan sehari-hari. Gerontologist, 9, 179-186.

    Measso, G., Cavarzeran, F., 'Zappala, G., Lebowitz, BD, Crook, TH, Pirozzolo, FJ, Amaducci,LA, Massari, D. & Grigoletto, F. (1993). State Examination Mini-Mental: Normatif studi sampel

    acak Italia. Pembangunan Neuropsikologi, 9, 77-85.

    Moscovitch, M. (1994). Memori dan bekerja dengan memori: Evaluasi dari model proses

    komponen dan perbandingan dengan model lainnya. Dalam D.L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.),Memory sistem 1994 (hal. 269-310). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Orsini, A., Grossi, D., Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1987). Rentang

    memori verbal dan spasial langsung: Normatif data dari 1.355 orang dewasa dan 1.112 anak-

    anak. Italia Jurnal Ilmu Neurologis, 8, 539-548.Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik, RJ, Tangalos, EG, & Kokmen, E. (1999). Kerusakan

    kognitif ringan. Klinis karakterisasi dan hasil. Archives of Neurology,, 56 303-308.Shimamura, AP, Jurica, PJ, Mangels, JA, Gershberg, FB, & Knight, RT (1995). Kerentanan

    terhadap efek interferensi memori menyusul kerusakan lobus frontal: Temuan dari tes

    berpasangan-asosiasi belajar. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,, 7 144-152.Spinnler, H., & Della Sala, S. (1988). Peran neuropsikologi klinis dalam diagnosis neurologis

    penyakit Alzheimer. Journal of Neurology,, 235 258-271.

    Spinnler, H., & Tognoni, G. (Eds) (1987). Standardizzazione e Taratura italiana di tes

    neuropsicologici. The Journal Italia Neurologis Ilmu, Suppl 8 sampai No 6: 1-120.

    Tabel 1.Fitur demografi dari Pasien MCI Memasuki Studi dan Kinerja mereka di Tes Diagnostik.

    MCI / Pt. No Umur Pendidikan(Tahun) MMSE

    score1 Rey-jenis

    Daftar kata

    Delayed21 73 8 30 3,9

    2 66 5 30 1,8

    3 68 5 30 2,44 80 5 26,03 3,1

    5 72 5 27,03 2,4

    6 76 5 30 3,17 50 8 29,07 2,6

    8 56 5 28,24 0,7

    9 66 5 28,99 2,8

    10 67 5 30 4,4

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    24/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 24

    Catatan

    1 Skor kisaran, 0-30, cut-off untuk kinerja normal, 23,8 (untuk populasi Italia). Skor disesuaikan

    dengan usia dan pendidikan (Measso et al., 1993).

    2 Lima belas-kata daftar recall belajar tertunda. Jarak skor, 0-15, cut-off untuk kinerja normal,

    4,69 (Carlesimo et al, 1996.).

    Tabel 2

    Neuropsikologi Profil Pasien MCI

    MCI

    Pasien Nomor Penamaan

    Uji Fonetik Token Kefasihan Semantic Kefasihan Daftar kata Belajar

    Span Segera Digit Span Spatial Rey Gambar Segera Rey Trail Gambar Tertunda Membuat1 9 35 31,7 16,75 28,9 9,25 7,25 36 9,75 136

    2 9 33 34,9 21,25 18,1 * 5,5 5,5 36 6,75 * 1453 9 35 32,6 20,0 38,0 6,5 6,5 35 6,75 * 92

    4 9 31 25,7 14,25 36,2 4,75 4,75 36 7,75 * 81

    5 9 32 26,6 19,6 28,0 * 4,5 4,5 36 9,75 1166 9 31 30,4 16,75 30,0 4,5 4,5 32,75 168 11,75

    7 9 35 24,1 18,5 23,3 * 6,0 6,0 35,25 3,5 * 124

    8 9 32 24,8 16,75 17,8 * 4,5 4,25 36 5,5 * 81

    9 9 32 21,25 10,25 20,1 * 5,5 4,25 33,25 6,75 * 12210 9 34 28,9 14,25 33,1 5,5 6,25 36 5,5 * 114

    Catatan. Skor adalah usia dan pendidikan-disesuaikan. Asterisk (*) menunjukkan kinerja dibawah nilai cut-off normal. Penamaan uji dari uji Aphasie Aachener - AAT, 120 rangsangan,

    cut-off: 8 (De Bleser et al, 1986.). Token Test, rentang skor: 0-36, cut-off score: 26,5 (De Renzi

    & Faglioni, 1978). Fonemik Verbal Kefasihan, kata-kata yang dimulai dengan F, A atau S, cut-off score: 17.35 (Carlesimo et al, 1996.). Semantic Verbal Kefasihan, nama-nama warna,

    binatang, buah, kota, cut-off score: 7.25 (Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987). Rey-Type Daftar kata

    Belajar (15 daftar kata, diulang 5 kali), rentang skor: 0-75, cut-off score: 28,53 (Carlesimo et al,

    1996.). Digit Span (Orsini et al, 1987.), Cut-off score: 3,5 (rentang digit dalam bahasa Italiaumumnya lebih rendah daripada dalam bahasa Inggris karena semakin lama waktu pengucapan

    digit). Spasial Span (Orsini et al, 1987.), Cut-off skor: 3,25. Rey Gambar Segera, jarak skor: 0-

    36, cut-off score: 28.87 (Caffarra et al, 2002.). Rey Gambar Tertunda, jarak skor 0-36, cut-offscore: 9,46 (Caffarra et al, 2002.). Pembuatan Trail, waktu dalam detik, bagian A - Bagian B,

    semakin tinggi, buruk cut-off score: 187 (Giovagnoli et al, 1996.).

    Tabel 3.

    Kinerja Individu Karakteristik mean dari Pasien MCI dalam Tugas Recall Prosa Eksperimental

  • 7/29/2019 Della Sala Memory MCI Patients

    25/25

    Della Sala et al., Amnesia and Interference, Page 25

    Pasien Segera Recall: Proporsi Interferensi Benar:

    Retensi (Tertunda / Segera) No Interferensi: (.. Del / Imm) Retensi

    1 .52 .14 .542 .54 .25 .48

    3 .58 .40 .59

    4 .20 .00 .385 .50 .33 .67

    6 .32 .00 .08

    7 .57 .31 .618 .61 .11 .62

    9 .40 .32 .88

    10 .46 .14 .67

    Gambar Keterangan


Recommended