+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Imitative problem solving: Why transfer of learning often fails to occur

Imitative problem solving: Why transfer of learning often fails to occur

Date post: 01-Feb-2017
Category:
Upload: ian-robertson
View: 215 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
28
Imitative problem solving: Why transfer of learning often fails to occur Author(s): IAN ROBERTSON Source: Instructional Science, Vol. 28, No. 4 (2000), pp. 263-289 Published by: Springer Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/23371449 . Accessed: 28/06/2014 18:15 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. . Springer is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Instructional Science. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Transcript

Imitative problem solving: Why transfer of learning often fails to occurAuthor(s): IAN ROBERTSONSource: Instructional Science, Vol. 28, No. 4 (2000), pp. 263-289Published by: SpringerStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/23371449 .

Accessed: 28/06/2014 18:15

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range ofcontent in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new formsof scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

.

Springer is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Instructional Science.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Instructional Science 28: 263-289, 2000. Ifâ

© 2000 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands. €> 2000 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the M ether lands.

Imitative problem solving: Why transfer of learning often fails to occur

IAN ROBERTSON University of Luton, Department of Psychology, Luton, LUI 3JU, U.K.,

e-mail: ian. robertson@luton. ac. uk

Received: 9 March 1998; in final form: 2 August 1999; accepted: 16 August 1999

Abstract. The paper presents empirical evidence for imitative problem solving and the

Interpretation Theory described in Robertson and Kahney (1996). According to the theory

beginners use imitation as their primary problem solving method when learning about an

unfamiliar domain. Imitative problem solving can explain much of the evidence that ana

logical transfer even within a domain is often hard to find. The paper presents an analysis of algebra word problems to predict in detail exactly where solvers will have difficulty in

using a worked out example to solve either a close or distant variant of the problem type. In a 2x2 between-groups design, secondary school students were given an explanation of an

algebra word problem taken from Reed et al. (1985) or an explanation of the problem that

also included information about how the solution could be adapted to solve a distant variant.

They were then given either a close or distant variant to solve. Results were in line with the

predictions derived from the Interpretation Theory analysis.

Keywords: analogical problem solving, imitative problem solving, task analysis, transfer,

instructional design

Introduction

The issue of transfer of learning from one situation to another is of a perennial interest to psychologists and educationalists. Much research has focused on

the conditions under which it occurs or fails to occur both between domains

(e.g., Bassok & Holyoak, 1989; Catrambone & Holyoak, 1989; Gentner et

al., 1993; Gick & Holyoak, 1980; 1983; Pierce & Gholson, 1994) and within

domains (e.g., Anderson et al., 1984; Reed et al., 1985; Reed & Ettinger,

1987; Ross, 1989; Singley & Anderson, 1989). The general paradigm used in

much of this research is that of analogical problem solving (APS). Psycholo

gists have attempted to identify the cognitive processes involved when people

attempt to solve a current (target) problem using an analogy or example

(source) problem, and how the structure of the problems themselves influ

ences those processes (Falkenhainer et al., 1989; Gentner, 1983; Gentner &

Medina, 1998; Holyoak & Thagard, 1990).

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

264

There are a number of assumptions built into the APS account that have to be borne in mind especially when applied to the kinds of academic prob lem solving that students are often required to engage in. First, an important assumption is that the source problem is represented in long-term memory. For example, in Gick and Holyoak's, (1980, 1983) experiments, participants had already studied the Fortress Problem, where a general has to divide his

army into groups that converge simultaneously on a fortress. When asked to solve Duncker's (1945) Radiation Problem, where a surgeon has to focus a number of weak rays on a tumour, most of them did not relate it to the Fortress Problem since there is no obvious reason why one should look to a problem of

military strategy to solve a problem in surgery (Medin & Ross, 1992). When

given a hint to use the previously presented problem the vast majority were able to retrieve and use it to solve the Radiation Problem.

Second, APS assumes the student understands the source problem; that

is, the student has at least an implicit knowledge of the solution structure in the source. Analogies work when we can understand or make inferences about a new domain because we already understand the source domain. When

Jacques Chirac called currency speculation the AIDS of the world economy we can infer from our knowledge of AIDS that he probably meant that cur

rency speculation was debilitating and there was no cure for it. Third, APS assumes that the student understands the target domain well enough to adapt the solution. In the Fortress Problem, dividing an army into groups (in Gick and Holyoak's studies) becomes reducing the intensity of rays and finding more of them in the Radiation Problem. Fourth, most models of APS assume that analogical mapping involves a hierarchical system of relations (Gentner, 1989; Gentner & Markman, 1997) that have to be put into correspondence. Objects in the source problem are mapped onto the objects in the target. There are some constraints on this mapping, governed by the relations between the objects and the relations between those relations. Constraints other than structural ones (such as semantic and pragmatic constraints) are also assumed to influence analogical mapping (Holyoak & Thagard, 1989) and the retrieval of a relevant source from long-term memory (Thagard et al., 1990).

The examples so far have referred to between-domain analogising. It

might be argued that in within-domain analogising the problem of retriev

ing an example is reduced since there are likely to be more relevant cues available. Within-domain analogising is often known as case-based reason

ing (Kolodner, 1993) which is essentially a sub-type of APS since the same

processes apply. A current example reminds one of an earlier problem of the same type which is used as the basis for reasoning about the current situation. Here again, solvers are assumed to have a 'case' in long-term memory that

they can be reminded of and that they already understand (otherwise they

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

265

would not be able to reason from it). However, when students are learning a new topic for the first time, the examples they are presented with may be poorly represented in long-term memory. They may not have a 'case' to reason from.

Anderson, Fincham & Douglas (1997) suggest that case-based reasoning is the final stage of their four-stage model of learning a new domain. The

first stage involves analogy where a training problem is used to help solve a test problem. Solving a problem in this way allows the learner to abstract out the principles underlying the problems. These abstractions can be used

to generate production rules that can be used in future to solve problems of the same kind. Finally, specific examples can be retrieved that remind the solver what principles to apply, and hence what production rules are relevant. Note that retrieving specific cases to reason from is the final stage because

examples take time to learn. It is the first stage of Anderson et al.'s four stage model that the present

paper investigates. The first steps in novice problem solving involve imitative

problem solving (IPS) (Robertson & Kahney, 1996), which is a subtype of APS. It makes fewer assumptions about the knowledge the student is sup

posed to have and makes different assumptions about the processes that take

place in the early stages of problem solving. First, IPS does not assume

that the student has an example problem in long-term memory. In textbook

problem solving, students are likely to go back and look at examples in

the preceding section of a textbook rather than trying to retrieve one from

memory. Second, students are assumed to pay attention to the superficial features

of the problems rather than the structural features. Assuming the student has

been given an example to model, the first task is to identify objects that

appear to play the same role in the two problems (Gentner, 1989; Gentner

et al., 1993; Ross, 1987, 1989). When students have little understanding of

the example, this process is guided more by the semantic similarity of the

objects involved than by the solution structure. Furthermore, there are often

gaps in the explanations of textbook problems where the reader is left to

make inferences. An inability to make those inferences is likely to prevent the construction of a complete representation of the problem (Britton et al., 1990; Neumann & Schwarz, 1998).

Third, the student tries to identify and apply the same operator in the target that appears to be applied to the mapped objects in the source. There may be

little attempt to relate this process to what Van Dijk and Kintsch (1983) have

called the 'situation model' - a mental model of the situation represented

by the text. Furthermore, when students have a poor representation of the

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

266

structure of the source, the ability to adapt it depends on the amount of domain

knowledge the student possesses (Novick & Holyoak, 1991).

Fourth, as in the traditional APS account, learning comes about as a result

of the process of using the example to solve the current problem. By using and implicitly (or explicitly) comparing examples, students can make a start

at generalizing from the two problems (Dellarosa-Cummins, 1992; Ross &

Kennedy, 1990).

Although an example is the salient aspect of instruction as far as the

student is concerned, 'the disadvantage of an example is that it may not be

very helpful for solving problems that are slightly different' (Reed & Bolstad,

1991, p. 753). Thus, when Reed, Dempster and Ettinger (1985) gave students

the solution to an algebra problem, the latter could only solve a problem that

was almost identical to the example even when they had the example and

an explanation of the solution in front of them. When their participants were

asked to solve more distant variants of the examples they were given they often did no better than if they had been given a completely unrelated training

problem. Ross (1984, 1987) also found that his participants were often poor at using solution principles such as equations to solve problems even when

the equation was in front of the student at all times.

The main aim of the present paper is to see the extent of the difficulties

novices have in textbook problem solving as predicted from the structure of

the textual explanations themselves. A solution explanation can be couched

in terms specific to the example or at a more abstract or general level. A more

general explanation should help indicate how the solution can be applied to a broader range of examples than the specific one provided. Reed and

Bolstad enhanced transfer to more distant problem variants by providing an

explanation at a general level along with specific examples. This approach is also advocated by Nathan et al. (1992). One would therefore expect that

a student attempting a problem that is very similar to a training example

(a 'close variant' of the example problem) would be relatively uninfluenced

by the type of explanation given. Students faced with an exercise problem that requires a degree of adaptation of the example (a 'distant variant') should

be helped by an example explanation couched at a more general level. The

present study therefore looks at the effect that a specific explanation and a

general explanation can have on a student's ability to solve either a close or a

distant variant of an example. A second aim is to show how this research fits into the Interpretation

Theory described in Robertson and Kahney (1996). This is a theory of text, task and protocol analysis that provides a unified notation for describing both

the structure of a text, or task embedded in a text, and the behaviour of the

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

267

Figure 1. Example problems in textbooks represent a category of problems of which there can

be close and distant variants.

solver. One of its underlying assumptions is that novices in the early stages of learning a new domain engage in imitative problem solving.

The rest of this paper begins with a brief description of the Interpretation

Theory notation used to analyse texts and problem structure. Based on this

analysis, it is possible to predict where students are likely to have difficulty either because of the structure of the example problem or because of the

mappings between the example and the exercise problem. A full description of the Interpretation Theory as it applies to text and problem analysis can be

found in Robertson & Kahney (1996). (A summary diagram of the notation

is presented in Appendix A.) In this paper, I look at empirical investigations of IPS; particularly the types of errors that students make and how instruction

can be improved to reduce them, as revealed by the Interpretation Theory

analysis. This improvement can be produced in two ways: first by removing the gaps where students have to make inferences, and second by providing a

general explanation at a more abstract level alongside the specific example to

aid transfer to more distant variants of the problem. Before going on to the study itself, the Interpretation Theory is applied to

the analysis of specific problems based on those used in Reed et al. (1985).

The interpretation theory notation

In the early stages of problem solving in a novel domain, it is the solution

procedure that the novice tries to understand and which characterises the

problem. Exercise problems can be either very similar in terms of their solu

tion procedure (the 'close variants' in Figure 1) or they may require some

adaptation (the 'distant variants' in Figure 1).

Solving an exercise problem involves accessing or identifying a relevant

example problem, mapping the features of the example problem that appear to play the same role in the exercise problem, and applying the solution pro

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

268

4. application

3 . relational structure

solution procedure

A B

Example problem Example problem statement solution

2. mapping

5. (adapt)

Exercise problem Exercise problem statement solution

1

1. access

Figure 2. Some of the processes involved in using an example problem to solve an exercise

problem.

cedure instantiated in the example problem step by step to generate a solution.

These three processes are indicated in Figure 2, which is based on a simplified version of Sternberg's (1977) model of the component processes involved in

analogical reasoning. The A term represents an example problem statement, the B term is the example solution, the C term represents the statement of

an exercise problem and the D terms is the goal - the solution the solver is

seeking. It should be emphasised that the notation described here is not intended to

be a model of how a student represents a problem. It is a way of analysing texts and problems based on IPS theoiy.

In Reed et al.'s (1985) third experiment, there is a condition in which a

group of students is presented with an 'elaborated' solution to a problem

designed to enhance performance on subsequent test problems (Figure 3). The first paragraph presents the problem statement (the A term box in Fig ure 3). The goal is to generate an equation based on the information in the

problem statement (the B term). (Solving the equation to find't' is a different

type of problem and is not dealt with here.) Between the A term and the B

term is an explanation of the solution (Be). The explanation includes a table

as a way of represenaing the problem in terms of the variables in the equation. This is coded as B' which stands for any form of intermediate representation.

There are several sections highlighted in bold in the problem statement.

These represent the problem givens that play a role in the solution procedure. That is, a number of operators have to be applied to those problem givens to

generate the solution. The goal ('in how many hours... ') is also highlighted

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

269

A car travelling at a speed of 30 miles per hour (mph) left a certain place at 10.00 a.m. At 11.30 a.m. another car departed from the same place at 40

mph and travelled the same route. In how many hours will the second car overtake the first car?

The problem is a distance-rate-time problem in which

Distance = Rate x Time

Be

We begin by constructing a table to represent the distance, rate, and time for each of the two cars. We want to find how long the second car travels before it overtakes the first car. We let t represent the number that we want to find and enter it into the table. The first car then travels t + 3/2 hr because it left 1V2 hrs earlier. The rates are 30 mph for the first car and 40 mph for the second car. Notice that the first car must travel at a slower rate if the second car overtakes it We can now represent the distance each car travels by multiplying the rate and the time for each car. These values are shown in the following table.

B'

Distance Rate Time Car (miles) (mph) (hr)

First 30(t+3/2) 30 t+3/2 Second 40xt 40 t

B

Because both cars have travelled the same distance when the second car overtakes the first, we set the two distances equal to each other:

30(t + 3/2) = 40t

Solving for t yields the following:

30t + 45 = 40t; 45t = lOt; t = 4.5 hr.

Figure 3. An 'elaborated' explanation used by Reed et al. (1985).

since it implies the specific sequence of operators necessary to reach it. Thus

the A term (the example problem statement) can be seen as containing within

it those problem givens that play a role in the solution process - particularly in generating a sequence of subgoals (Figure 4).

One other feature of Figure 4 is worth mentioning. To understand the

solution fully, the solver has to be able to infer why both cars travel the same

distance. This is not stated in the problem. It can only be inferred from a

mental model (Nathan et al., 1989). The dotted line represents an inference

that has to be made based on the situation described in the problem statement.

It should also be pointed out that the solver does not need to make such

an inference - a close variant can still be solved by imitating the example

problem without understanding the underlying structure.

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

270

1

A'

JA

A2

A||A5 A4

A' speed carl: 30 nVh

ILd carl leaves: 10.00 am

N car2 leaves: 11.30am

A4 speed car2: "

40 m/h /

A5

\ overtake

M

goal: find time of car2

Figure 4. Representation of the example problem statement as givens nested within the A

term box.

The explanation of the solution also requires the solver to make a number of other inferences. First, the explanation states:

The first car then travels t + | hr because it left 1 | hrs earlier.

The solver has to infer which problem givens produce the t + | hr. They also have to infer the missing operators that produce the | hr. (One of the difficulties in analyzing tasks in the detail presented here is finding that state of ignorance which allows us to identify where inferences are built into a text and therefore where novices are likely to encounter problems. For this reason no inference is considered trivial.) Where operators are not explicitly stated, it is represented by a 'black box' in Figure 4. The two black boxes in the

figure represent the missing operators (subtract A2 [10 am] from A3 [11:30 am]) and those used to convert 11 to |.

The overall goal (the B term) can be construed as having subgoals 'nested' within it just as the problem statement has givens nested within it. On the

right of Figure 4, for example, the subgoal (B4) is to generate the time taken

by car 1 (t + |) which can be slotted into the equation. Using this notation it is

possible to analyse the text of a problem in such a way that the inferences that solvers have to make can be identified and where adaptations are required to solve the problem.

Figure 5, therefore, represents the complete analysis of the elaborated distance problem used by Reed et al. in their study of transfer. There are several places where there is the potential for solvers to misunderstand the

problem explanation. Row d has already been discussed. In row g there is no

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

271

explanation of why the distance can be represented by multiplying the rate

by the time for each car since the reader is not referred back to the distance

equation, nor is the relevant equation given. For this reason B7 appears as a

'ghost' term (in outline format) since it is not explicitly stated.

There are 6 values represented in the table and the student may have diffi

culties mapping them onto Distance = Rate x Time equation particularly as it

is not the relevant one. The relevant equation is Ratecaii x Timecas\ = Ratecai2 x Timecar2 but it is not provided anywhere. The black boxes between B7 and B8 and B9 in Figure 5 indicate where this information is missing and hence where solvers may experience difficulties.

When students attempt an exercise problem, which is a close variant of

the example problem, we can predict that any misunderstandings will be in

those places marked by the inference lines and black boxes in Figure 5d to i.

Thereafter, they are obliged to rely on their prior knowledge of mathematics

to understand the transformations that take place to arrive at the overall solu

tion (rows i to 1). The focus of this study, however, is on the students' ability to generate the equation in row i. If they can successfully substitute elements in the target for elements in the source to generate the equation, then solving a close variant should not cause much difficulty.

Predicted sources of difficulty in mapping and adapting the practice problem to solve a close variant

In Reed et al.'s experiments, participants were presented with an 'equivalent' exercise problem (a close variant of the example problem - see the statement

of the problem in Figure 7). As Figure 6 shows, there is no direct match

between the '2 hours' mentioned in the exercise problem and the times given in the example problem (represented by A2 and A3 respectively). To solve a

problem by analogy to an earlier one, the solver has to map the objects that

play the same roles in both problems (departure times, speeds of vehicles, and

so on). The mapping line between C2 and A2 and A3 is therefore important in

representing the need to identify and map relevant objects. However, since the

objects mentioned in the problem givens do not map directly, the access and

mapping line from C2 to A2 and A3 is shown as being blocked (represented by the vertical bars). Participants have to infer that they have to find the

difference in departure times (represented by D4). In fact, they would have

to map the B4 term (representing the difference of | hours) onto the C2 one.

Some of the example problem givens can be mapped directly onto the

exercise problem. The first car in both problems travels at 30mph. This

information is represented as A1 and C1 in row e of Figure 7. By imitating the example, problem solvers can map the relevant values onto the variables

in the relevant equation (B5 and D5 in row e). By changing the rate of the

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

272

ENCODE

Acartraveiingataspeedof30 mph left a certain point at 10.00 «.m. At 11.30 a.m., another car departed fiom the ssmeplaceat 40 mph and traveled the same route. In how ma ny hours willthe second car overtake the first?

EE

A2

A*

distance-rate-time problem

B3 Distance= Rate x time

b: constiuct table

r W

distance

Bl Met t represent number to be foird B3 ii = t

Bi carl leaves 1'/,

ej hours earlier B" time carl = t +3/2

B~TI ra(e car1 =

e[ 30 mph B rate carl = 30 mph

iatecar2= 40 mph

B rate cai2 = 40 mph

Be represert aisbnceDy miJtipMng rete & tine toreachcar

d1 = rtxtl d2= r2*t2

f rar

Bl values shown in table

Jd'8 1 30(1+%) 4a

30 40

t+% « ) M, V

b)

c)

<S)

e)

f)

"!g)

h)

B® BBtrresinBya rare—■ distance- set 2distance! eqial to each other B9 30(t+»/j = 4M _ J)

P

B^° solve fort B

1C 30t+ 45= 40t

B1 45" 101

j)

k)

B1 12 t =4.5 hours

RESPONSE: time taken by second car = 4.5 hrs

B' B2 B3 B4

B1J

B" B

B5

B?

B1( B9 B8 B7

m)

Figure 5. Interpretation Theory analysis of the elaborated distance problem from Reed et al.

1985.

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

273

4>

^ BLOCK [

Figure 6. Mapping and adapting times in test and practice problems.

second car in the example from 40mph to 45mph in the test problem (A4 and

C3 in row f), the solver could also map this onto the rate term in the equation

(D6). When using the example to solve the test problem, the solver 'inherits'

all the difficulties mentioned above in understanding the example problem.

Apart from the added difficulty of mapping the times in row d, solvers will

have difficulties where there are dotted lines and black boxes. The summary Figure 7 demonstrates where mappings are blocked and where the solver is

still obliged to make inferences about which operators should be used in order

to relate the problem givens to the solution. We can therefore make further

predictions about the possible sources of difficulty for the participants:

Difficulties in mapping (Hypothesis 1). The major difficulty in solving a close variant of the problem is one of mapping corresponding values.

Where the exercise problem is literally similar (that is, where the relations

between the objects are identical in both problems and where the objects in

the problems are semantically similar), we would not expect mapping to pose a particular problem. On the other hand, any move away from isomorphism, however slight, will be a potential source of error. Some participants will

therefore have difficulty at line d since the times in the example do not map

directly onto the one in the exercise problem. Difficulties in mapping will also

lead to more errors in mapping from distant variants than from close variants, and more errors where a specific explanation is given than where there is a

general explanation since a more general explanation will give at least some

indication of how to map values to a broader range of problems.

Predicted sources of difficulty in mapping and adapting the practice problem to solve a distant variant

The statement of Reed et al.'s 'similar' exercise problem (a distant variant) is

shown at the top of Figure 8.

Mapping the example problem onto this distant variant gives quite a dif

ferent picture. None of the values in the problem statement (C1, C2 and C3 in

Figure 8) maps onto anything in the earlier problem.

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

274

ENCODE A car travels south at the rate of 30 mph. Two hour s

later, asecondcar leaves to overtake the first car, using the same route and gang 45 mph. In how many hours will the second car overtake the first?

C1 c?

c c3

C59 C4

B1 B1

B2 B<

B

— , r_-_-_7

A3 A3 B"

A1 BE

Be

i

B7f

B8,

A5 B9

D1 Distance=

Speed x Time

(I car distance 'ate time ^

CM □ 1

Z

i

How many hours. .

Distance= Speed xTime

car2 leaves 2 his laier

D4 time car2 = t + 2

f=i - -

r1 rate carl = nb Map

FH 30mph 30mph => rl

I

c5 > i

rate carl =

45mph D6 Map

45mph => r2 ,

c4 Heaves to overtake

n7 di =d2 u rlxti= i2>4 2

v_, V

r-l—I rar distance rate time

^

P'8 1 30(t+2) 30 W2

2 45t 45 >

b)

c)

d)

e)

f)

g)

h)

S .

D9 a)(t+°/2)= 4ot

0

3Q + 60 = 43

d1 d2|d3 d4 RESPONSE: rr2 d5 time taken bv second car = 4 hrs

D d11 d6

d10 d9|d8 d7

k)

1)

m)

Figure 7. Mapping of a close transfer problem onto the training problem.

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

275

en: cde A pick- ip truck lea/es 3 hr after a large delivery truck but overtakes it by traveling 1 5 mph faster. If it takes the pick-up truck 7 hr to reach the delivery truck, find the r ate of each vehicle.

|c1 c2 —► C c3

c4

B1 n

Distance= Speed x Time

B2 B!

I. car distance rate time ^

D* i v 2 -/

|A®'

IA3"'

B3 find rates goal =rl goal =r2

B4. BA r1 3 hoirs after fry ■ time>u;ltl = 7hr

+ 3hr= 10hr -i_ >

A1 B6

A5 B

c3 1

true k2 takes 7fr

D5

A TimefccW= "Ttir

C* true k2 travels "

DB 15mph faster Rate^ + 15 J ! i" V

ft Heaves overtakes

D7 di =d2

r1xtt= i2xt2 u

'ch car distance rate time

b'8 1 lor ' 7+3 Ar+15) r+15 7 )

a)

b>

c)

d)

e)

0

g)

h)

Rate of first truck = 35 mph Rate of second truck = 50 mph

D1 D2 D3 D4

n12

D

D5

D11 D14 D6

D1C D9 D8 D7

o)

Figure 8. Summary figure of mappings of a more distant variant onto the practice problem.

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

276

Figure 8 shows not only the inferences inherited from understanding the

example problem (the A and B terms) but also specifies where solvers would

have difficulties making the necessary mappings. From the analysis of the

problem, we can predict that participants will have difficulty at the following lines:

(c) since the goals of the two problems are different;

(d, e) since the problem elements do not map and the participant is not told

what operator to apply;

(J) nothing in the earlier problem maps onto a difference in rates and the

participant is not told what operator to apply;

(g) for the reasons already given in the discussion of the close variant, par

ticipants may have difficulty generating the two halves of the relevant

equation;

(h) the types of elements in the table in the example problem do not match

those in the exercise problem (there are variables in the time column

in the example but not in the exercise problem, and variables in the

exercise problem under rate but not in the example);

(i) solvers have to solve for r rather than t and then find the rate of the

other vehicle in subgoal m.

In fact, very little of the explanation given in the original example problem is of any use at all.

Because of the difficulty in mapping values across from source to target, and because there is no general procedure presented for adapting the example

problem to this distant variant, we can make further predictions about the

difficulties facing students attempting to imitate a distant variant:

Overt imitation and 'over-transfer' (Hypothesis 2). There will be more

'quantity errors' (errors due to misassigning values in the problem to vari

ables in the equation) in the distant variant than in the close variant, since

some values are not given in the exercise problem which are given in the

example and vice versa. We would also expect that imitation would lead to

'over-transfer' from the source problem. In particular, we would expect to

find participants attempting to use the variable 1 in the solution to the distant

variant because that is what it says to do in the example, even though the

value of t can be readily derived from the problem statement.

Frame errors (Hypothesis 3). There will be more 'frame errors' (errors

occurring when attempting to generate and adapt the relevant equation) in

the distant variant when no information is provided about how to adapt the

equation. That is, there should be a significant difference in the number of

correct equations between the participants given a specific explanation and

those given a general explanation but only in the Distant conditions.

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

277

It follows from the above analysis of the word problems that, if the infer

ences inherent in the Reed et al. explanation are removed and a more general procedure provided, then participants will find it easier to map values across

and adapt the procedure to more distant variants.

Effects of a general procedure (Hypothesis 4). Removing inferences and

providing a more general procedure will enhance transfer to more distant

variants of a problem type. This should be evident in the participants' ability to produce the relevant solution procedure and identify the elements of the

relevant equation when solving distant variants.

Method

Participants

The participants were 40 Scottish secondary school students, aged between

14 and 16, who took part in the experiment during their normal mathemat

ics classes. They were divided into four groups of 10 by the mathematics

teacher so that there was the same general spread of mathematics ability in each group. The groups corresponded to four conditions: Specific/Close,

Specific/Distant, General/Close and General/Distant.

Materials

In the Specific/Close and Specific/Distant conditions, participants received an

example problem along with the explanation used by Reed et al. The only dif

ference was that the word 'rate' was replaced by 'speed'. In the General/Close

and General /Distant conditions, participants received the same problems but

the explanations had the identified inferences removed and included a general

explanation of the solution (one at a sufficient level of abstraction that it

might be applied to a more distant variant) as well as a specific one (see

Appendix B).

Design and procedure

The experiment used a 2x2 between-subjects design. The independent vari

ables were the type of instruction provided with the example problem

(Specific or General) and the type of exercise problem to be solved (Close or

Distant). The participants were asked to take part in a study of instructional

materials and were told that the study was not a test of their own problem

solving abilities. They were then presented with three sheets containing the

instructions, the example problem and associated solution procedure, and the

exercise problem. The instructions were as follows:

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

278

In a moment you will be asked to read the next sheet (sheet B) which

gives an example problem and an explanation of how to solve it. Please

read it carefully. Once you have read sheet B, turn to the third sheet (sheet

C) and write your name and class at the top. Then try to solve the problem which you find there.

Please use the example problem on sheet B to help you solve the

problem on sheet C.

There was no time limit imposed but the experiment was conducted

within the normal lesson time of approximately 35 minutes. Participants were

allowed to consult the example solution throughout the experiment.

Coding of solutions

Responses were analysed according to the following criteria:

Elements identified. This refers to the number of elements the student

identified as being part of the equation. That is, students were given a score

of 1 of they identified all four elements of the equation (the rates and times

for both cars). Correct equation. A score of 1 was given when the student generated

the correct equation by filling in the values in the equation: speedvcb,c\e] x

//mevchiciei =

speedveiàc\e2 x timeve\ùcie2- Note that an equation can be classified

as correct even though it may contain quantity or matching errors (see the

coding of errors below). This classification is in conformity with that used by Reed et al. (1985).

Correct solution. A score of 1 was given to a solution that included the

correct equation and that used the correct procedure. A correct solution also

contains the correct values in all variable slots in the equation. The correct

procedure here is defined as the procedure given in the training example. Note that the aim of the experiment was to identify how well participants

could generate the correct equation using the correct procedure. It did not concern itself with the participants' ability to manipulate the equation to

generate the correct answer once the equation had been generated.

Coding of errors

Responses were also examined to identify the types of errors made. The errors were classified as follows:

Nonalgebraic solution. In some cases, participants used both an algebraic and a nonalgebraic solution method; in such cases, the nonalgebraic solution

method was ignored for the purposes of coding. Otherwise participants are

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

279

90%

80%

^ 70%

S 60% 0

1 50%

■3 40% * £ 30% *

ZOH

10%

0% correct solution correct

equation

elements

identified

□ Specific/Close

0 General/Close

h Specific/Distant

a General/Distant

Figure 9. Success in the Specific and General Conditions.

coded as using a nonalgebraic method whether or not the method gives the

correct answer.

Wrong equation. Participants are coded as using the wrong equation when

it appears they are using any equation other than the 'correct' one as defined

above.

Quantity errors. A quantity error refers to an error produced when the

participant maps the wrong value from a target problem to the source. An

example would be mapping the difference between the speeds of the vehicles

(e.g. 15m/h) onto the speed of one of the cars in the source.

Matching errors. A matching error occurs when the participant substitutes

a value in the source problem for a value in the target without any attempt at

adapting it.

Frame errors. A frame error is an error in the form of the equation. If 30(7 + 2) = 451 is the correct equation, then an equation such as 30(/ + 2) = 45

would be a frame error since it does not preserve the form of the original

speed x time equation (the time of the second vehicle is missing).

Results

Figure 9 and Figure 10 present the results of the four groups. As expected the close variant is easier to solve than the distant variant and

the general explanation was more effective than the specific one at eliciting transfer to both near and distant variants of the training example (see the

correct solution columns in Figure 9). These results were found to be sig

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

280

1 6

1 4

12"

1 0

8

6

4

2

0

□ Specific/Close E General/close □ Specific/distant s General/distant

quantity matching frame non-aigebraic wrong equation total < solution

Figure 10. Errors in Specific and General Conditions.

nificant using a 2-factor ANOVA. There was a significant effect of problem distance (Fi 38 = 24, Mse = 3.6, p < .0001) and instructional type (F| 38 = 6, Mse = .9, p < .02). However, there was no significant interaction effect.

The total errors columns in Figure 10 are a measure of the overall likeli

hood of making an error in each condition. Again there was a main effect of

problem distance (Fi 38 = 13.241, p < .001) and of instructional type (F\ 3$ = 10.138, p < .003) but no significant interaction effect.

Difficulties in mapping (Hypothesis 1)

It was predicted in Hypothesis 1 that solving a close variant of an example problem by imitation would not cause significant difficulties although any variations, however slight, may lead to error. Participants in the Spe cific/Close condition made more errors than those in the General/Close condition where fewer inferences are required.

There was some confusion in both Distant conditions in assigning vehicle labels (vehicle 1, vehicle2) to the correct vehicles. The pickup truck in the 'distant' problem is mentioned first, even though it left after the delivery truck, and was therefore assigned to the vehicle 1 role by two participants in the Specific/Distant condition and two in the General/Distant Condition. Failure to map the correct vehicles led to quantity errors in the equation. The

misassignment of vehicle labels should therefore have been predicted since the participants were imitating the source.

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

281

'Overt ' imitation and over-transfer (Hypothesis 2)

Figure 9 shows that participants in the 2 Distant conditions were more likely to make quantity errors than those in the Close conditions. There were also

fewer quantity errors in the General conditions (fis = 1.852, p = .04). In some

cases, participants attempted to adapt the exercise problem in order to make

it perceptually similar to the earlier problem. Such 'overt' imitation can take

various forms. Examples would be: ensuring that the solution to the exercise

problem has the same number of '=' signs or same number of lines as the

example when solving for t. Overt imitation is a form of over-transfer, where

the solver transfers too much information from the source. Matching error is

the most overt form of imitation where the participant copies a value from

the source problem across to the target without any attempt at adapting it. For

instance, one participant in the Specific/Close condition copied across t + | from the example.

Examples of over-transfer were found in all conditions. In the distant con

ditions, some students attempted to incorporate 7' into the equation instead

of a more appropriate variable name since that was the variable used in the

training problem. This was despite the fact that the times for the vehicles were

given, or easily obtained by adding 7 and 3, in the problem statement. Again students were trying to copy the earlier procedure without adapting it. Reed &

Ettinger (1987) found the same effect in their studies, where their participants

copied over structures from the example problem which were unnecessary in the target problem. Such 'structure matching errors' are a form of over

transfer and played a great part in frustrating Reed & Ettinger's attempts to

induce transfer in their participants. In the Close conditions, over-transfer was

apparent in five participants who converted the 2 hours difference between

the departure times of the cars in the exercise problem to ~ so that it looked

like the | in the example. Similarly three participants in the Specific/Distant Condition converted the 3 to | or the 7 to In the latter case, the conversion

naturally led to incorrect answers.

Frame errors (Hypothesis 3)

There were no significant differences between the Specific and General

groups' ability to generate the correct equation. Almost all generated a correct

equation in the Close conditions and about half were successful in the Distant

conditions. Where frame errors occurred they were generally due to an inab

ility to identify the correct equation in the first place. In particular they tended

to use distance = rate x time or a variation of it, such as rate = distance /

time. The latter was usually generated by the participants themselves from the

distance-rate-time triangle:

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

282

which they had previously learned. Alternatively, they may have used the

distance = speed x time equation given in the explanations in the Specific condition.

Effects of a general procedure (Hypothesis 4)

The type of instruction had a significant effect on the likelihood of using the correct solution procedure and avoiding errors. In the Distant conditions, the general instructions were more effective at helping students identify the

relevant elements (t\% = -1.897, p < .04) and at finding the overall solution

(?i8 = -2.449, p = .01) since more information was provided in the General

conditions about how the problem givens should be mapped to the underlying

equation, and how the example relates to other problems of the same type.

Discussion

It would appear that the Interpretation Theory analysis of the problems and of

the mappings between them correctly predicted the areas where participants would have difficulty. It also shows that attempting to improve explanations in areas of potential difficulty, by removing the need for inferencing and

presenting a more general procedure, produces a greater degree of transfer

to variants, both near and distant, of a problem type. The better performance of participants in the General Conditions indicates

the beneficial effect of removing inferences when participants are reading an

example problem, and of providing an explanation at a level of generality sufficient to allow them to adapt the procedure to suit the target problem. This finding is in accordance with the results of Reed & Bolstad's (1991) experiments. The results clearly show where some participants had difficulty in adapting the source. One of the predictions from an analysis of Reed et al.'s text was that participants would find problems mapping elements onto the relevant equation. Half of the participants in the Specific conditions managed to fill in the times and speeds in the table. However, fewer than 10% managed to map these values onto the correct equation. These results contrast with those of Reed and Ettinger (1987) who found that their participants were able to generate the equation from the values in the table when those values were

given. Another notable aspect of the behaviour of the participants was the degree

to which they tried to imitate the example. Although it was predicted that

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

283

some participants would have difficulty in understanding where the | came

from in the example, and therefore that it would be hard to substitute the relevant value in the exercise problem, the lengths to which some participants went to adapt their solution to make it look like the example was often quite marked. Those who saw the correspondence between the 2 in the exercise

problem and the | in the example still adapted it to make it look like The

participants seemed to be imitating the example problem in the belief that

the closer the exercise problem resembled the example superficially the more

likely they were to get a correct answer.

According to Reed et al. (1985), the purpose of their experiments was

'to investigate how well students could use the solution of an algebra word

problem to solve other problems belonging to the same category.' The cat

egory (distance-rate-time problems) is one which has been imposed by the

authors and not one which is necessarily understood by the students (Hall et al., 1989). Similarly, stating that the earlier problem had to be 'slightly modified', as Reed et al. claim, is a judgement that only a relative expert can

make. In their experiments, only 22% of the participants given an 'elaborated'

solution to work from successfully solved the distant variant compared with

17% using a completely unrelated example problem. The difference between

these results is not statistically significant. According to Reed et al., the low

level of performance was due to participants' inability to generate the correct

equation rather than an inability to solve it. This does not explain why the

participants performed no better when an explanation was provided which

removed many of the inferences. The results can readily be explained if one

assumes that they are capable of imitating an example. Reed et al. were

aware of this and called it a 'syntactic pattern-matching procedure'. How

ever, the solution explanation provided by Reed et al. gave no information

to the participants about how to solve any other kind of problem, such as

one in which the rates are unknown. Figure 8 demonstrates that very little

in the example problem was of any use in solving the transfer problem. In

fact, it demonstrates that it was a different problem altogether, hence Reed

et al.'s participants were unable to imitate the example to solve the distant

variant.

This paper has argued that gaps in the explanations of example prob lems are often the source of error and misunderstanding. However, some

students can improve their understanding of worked examples when asked

to 'self-explain' (essentially filling in some of those gaps themselves). There

has been much recent work on the role of such self-explanations (Chi &

Bassok, 1989; Chi et al., 1994; Neumann & Schwarz, 1998; VanLehn et

al., 1992). For example, good students exhorted to 'self-explain' produce more clarification, justification and inference statements than poor students

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

284

who tend to produce more reformulation statements (Neumann & Schwarz,

1998). Beginners, however, may not know what inferences to make, espe

cially if their general domain knowledge is poor. The degree to which

students should be left to make their own inferences is an empirical one

and depends on the assumed prior knowledge of the student and whether the

writer wants only 'good' students to understand the text or most students to

understand it.

The theory of Imitative Problem Solving does not deal only with well

structured problems as exemplified here. In ill-structured problems such as

essay writing (another favourite task of students) imitation is often the pre ferred method. This does not just refer to outright plagiarism but rather to the

fact that the student will preferentially follow the structure of a textbook and

use the same examples the textbook uses, and so on. While the Interpretation Theory notation provides an analysis of the structure of well-structured prob lems, it is less well suited to such ill-structured problems. Nevertheless, it can

be modified to apply to the analysis of textbooks so that the behaviour of the

student can be mapped onto the text structure and the degree of convergence or divergence from the structure can be revealed (based on unpublished work

by the late Hank Kahney of the Open University, U.K.). The general aims of this research are to provide a unified methodology

for representing the structure of texts, problems and problem solving beha

viour; to improve thereby the structure of teaching texts; and to gain a better

understanding of the early stages of 'academic' problem solving. A unified

methodology or notation should allow us to see how the structure of a text (at a coarse grain of analysis) or the structure of an individual problem (at a fine

grain) affects problem solving behaviour (see Robertson & Kahney, 1996). As a result we should be in a better position to design instructional materials that are more explicit and that help teach students better.

The main pedagogical implication of IPS is that imitating an example is

the predominant method used by beginners in a domain with which they are unfamiliar. Imitation is very useful for two main reasons: first, it tends to allow the student to get an answer which may well be correct without

requiring the student to fully understand why (Reeves & Weisberg, 1993); second, it forms the basis for future learning. Comparing and using examples is the basis of understanding and learning through generalization (Reeves &

Weisberg, 1994; Ross & Kennedy, 1990). At a more practical level the Inter

pretation Theory demonstrated here can be used to help generate material which ensures that students do not need to make 'text external' inferences in

unfamiliar domains. Problem solving can also be speeded up by simply telling students which earlier example to use when solving an exercise problem in a

textbook. This would obviate the need for pointless searches through the book

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

285

for a relevant example. Finally, shifting the focus of attention to the structure of the expository text rather than looking at the 'failure' of particular students to understand might help us advance our understanding of problem solving behaviour, and may help maximize the rate at which all students learn.

Appendix A

Summary diagram of Interpretation Theory notation

0

0

©

Ba

B

example problem statement

exercise problem statement

intermediate representation

comprehensive explanation of solution

solution template

'ghost' term: no statement of the problem

numbered problem soling ^episode'

B

D

B,

example problem solution

exercise problem solution

explanation of solution

incomplete explanation of solution

unknovm processes; unspecified operators

I mappings are blocked/ I an impasse is reached

subject reaches an impasse and goes back to an eranple for inspiration

Be

ID

problem solving episode vtfiere subject attempts to understand why a solution works or fails to work

B.

example solution (B) showing subgoals (a. b & c) 'nested' within it IMJ

experimenter generated example

EXPERIMENTER 1 experimenter intervention

GO

0 GO

solid line indicates relation between a problem statement and its solution

dotted line means subject has to make an inference to understand the relation between a problem statement and its solution

indicates where the subject goes in the textbook to find information

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

286

Appendix B

General explanation of the overtaking vehicles problem

A car travelling at a speed of 30 miles per hour (m/h) left a certain place at 10:00 a.m. At 11:30 a.m. another car departed from the same place at

40 m/h and travelled the same route. Both cars therefore travelled the same

distance. In how many hours will the second car overtake the first car?

As you can see, the two cars travel at different speeds and leave at different

times: The first car leaves 1 \ hours before the second car (11:30-l0:00); The

second car travels 10 m/h faster than the first one (40 m/h-30 m/h). Since the first car has already been travelling for 1 \ hours you can work

out the distance it travelled. If it goes 30 miles in an hour then it will have

travelled 45 miles in 11 hours (30 m/h x 11 hours). The problem now becomes: How long will it take the second car to make

up this 45 miles by travelling 10 m/h faster?

In 1 hour the second car will travel 10 miles more than the first car, so it

will take 4.5 hours to make up the 45 miles (45 h -f- 10 m/h). In other words it will take 4.5 hours for the second car to overtake the first car.

1. Another way of solving this type of distance-speed-time problem is to

use an equation:

a] a) If you know the speed and time of a vehicle, then you can find the

distance it travelled by using the equation:

Distance = Speed x Time

For example, if a car travels at 40 m/h (Speed) for 2 hours {Time) then

it goes 80 miles {Distance - found by multiplying 40 by 2).

b] b) In problems like the one above, there are two vehicles (let's call them vehiclel and vehicle2). The equation for each vehicle would

be:

distance = speedyeucie\ x ft°/neVehiciei

and

distance = speedvchiclc2 x timevehicie2

c] c) But the two vehicles travel the same distance. So the distance travelled by one vehicle is equal to the distance travelled by the other. If the distances are equal then the speed x time for each vehicle must be equal too, so the equation to use when there are two vehicles travelling the same distance is:

Speedy^hiclel ^ ^//M£'Vehic!el Sp€€dvehicle2 X tiffîCvehick'2

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

287

2. We can now fill in some of the information that the problem gives us into this equation.

d] The problem gives the speeds of both vehicles but not the time taken

by either of

e] them. We want to find the time taken by vehicle2, so for the moment

we will call it f] t.

30 m/h 40 m/h t

.J, 4^ 4^ Speedy^ hiclel ^ tittle vehicle f ^^^^vehicle2 ^ timevehick'2

3. The next thing to do is to look at any differences between the two vehicles

which we can then use to fill in the blanks in the equation.

g] We know the two cars left at different times, so that the first car

travels l\ hours longer than the second one. The second car takes

t hours so the first car takes t + 1 \ hrs. We can now add this to the

equation:

30 (/+ if) 40 t

Jr 4" 4* 4' SpCi'd% chicle 1 X titftCyQhiclel ^P^^vehicle2 ^ ///rt^vehicleî

You end up with:

i] 30 x (t + 1 \ = 40 x /) Solving for t gives you the time taken by vehicle2 which is what you have to find.

30* + 45 = 40/

45 = 40/-30*

45 = 10/

j] t = 4.5 or 4\ hours

References

Anderson, J.R., Farrell, R. & Sauers, R. (1984). Learning to program in LISP. Cognitive Science 8: 87-129.

Anderson, J.R., Fincham, J.M. & Douglas, S. (1997). The role of examples and rules in the

acquisition of a cognitive skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,

and Cognition 23(4): 932-945.

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

288

Bassok, M. & Holyoak, K.J. (1989). Interdomain transfer between isomorphic topics in

algebra and physics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cog nition 15: 153-166.

Britton, B.K., Van Dusen, L., Glynn, S.M. & Hemphill, D. (1990). The impact of inferences on

instructional text. In A.C. Graesser & G.H. Bower, eds, The Psychology of Learning and

Motivation: Inferences and Text Comprehension, pp. 53-70. London: Academic Press.

Catrambone, R. & Holyoak, K.J. (1989). Overcoming contextual limitations on problem

solving transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition

15(6): 1147-1156. Chi, M.T.H. & Bassok, M. (1989). Learning from examples via self-explanations. In L.B.

Resnick, ed., Knowing, Learning, and Instruction: Essays in Honor of Robert Glaser, pp. 251-282. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Chi, M.T.H., de Leeuw, N., Chiu, M.H. & LaVancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanations

improves understanding. Cognitive Science 18: 439-478.

Dellarosa-Cummins, D. (1992). The role of analogical reasoning in the induction of problem

categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18(5): 1103-1124.

Van Dijk, T.A. & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York, NY:

Academic Press.

Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs 58: Whole no. 270.

Falkenhainer, B., Forbus, K.D. & Gentner, D. (1989). The structure-mapping engine:

Algorithm and examples. Artificial Intelligence 41: 1-63.

Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping: a theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science 7: 155-170.

Gentner, D. (1989). The mechanisms of analogical reasoning. In S. Vosniadou & A. Ortony,

eds, Similarity and Analogical Reasoning. London: Cambridge University Press.

Gentner, D. & Markman, A.B. (1997). Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. American

Psychologist 52(1): 45-56.

Gentner, D. & Medina, J. (1998). Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition 65: 263-297.

Gentner, D., Rattermann, M.J. & Forbus, K.D. (1993). The roles of similarity in transfer:

Separating retrievability from inferential soundness. Cognitive Psychology 25: 524-575.

Gick, M.L. & Holyoak, K.J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive Psychology 12:

306-356.

Gick, M.L. & Holyoak, K.J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive

Psychology 15: 1-38.

Hall, R., Kibler, D., Wenger, E. & Truxaw, C. (1989). Exploring the episodic structure of

algebra story problem solving. Cognition and Instruction 6(3): 223-283.

Holyoak, K.J. & Thagard, P. (1989). Analogical mapping by constraint satisfaction. Cognitive Science 13(3), 295-355.

Holyoak, K.J. & Thagard, P. (1990). A constraint satisfaction approach to analogical mapping and retrieval. In K.J. Gilhooly, M.T. Keane, R. Logie & G. Erdos, eds, Lines of Thinking:

Reflections on the Psychology of Thought (Vol. 1). Chichester: John Wiley.

Kolodner, J. (1993). Case-Based Reasoning. San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.

Medin, D.L. & Ross, B.H. (1992). Cognitive Psychology. Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace

Jovanovich.

Nathan, M.J., Johl, P., Kintsch, W. & Lewis, C. (1989). An unintelligent tutoring system for

solving word algebra problems. Paper presented at the artificial intelligence and education:

proceedings of the fourth international conference, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

289

Nathan, M.J., Kintsch, W. & Young, E. (1992). A theory of algebra-word-problem com

prehension and its implications for the design of learning environments. Cognition and

Instruction 9(4): 329-389.

Neumann, Y. & Schwarz, B. (1998). Is self-explanation while solving problems helpful? The

case of analogical problem-solving. British Journal of Educational Psychology 68: 15-24.

Novick, L.R. & Holyoak, K.J. (1991). Mathematical problem solving by analogy. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 17(3): 398—415.

Pierce, K.A. & Gholson, B. (1994). Surface similarity and relational similarity in the

development of analogical problem solving: Isomorphic and nonisomorphic transfer.

Developmental Psychology 30(5): 724—737.

Reed, S.K. & Bolstad, C.A. (1991). Use of examples and procedures in problem solving.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 17: 753-766.

Reed, S.K., Dempster, A. & Ettinger, M. (1985). Usefulness of analogous solutions for solv

ing algebra word problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology; Learning, Memory, and

Cognition 11(1): 106-125.

Reed, S.K. & Ettinger, M. (1987 ). Usefulness of tables for solving word problems. Cognition

and Instruction 4(1): 43-58.

Reeves, L.M. & Weisberg, R.W. (1993). On the concrete nature of human thinking: Con

tent and context in analogical transfer. Special Issue: Thinking. Educational Psychology

13(3—4): 245-258. Reeves, L.M. & Weisberg, R.W. (1994). The role of content and abstract information in

analogical transfer. Psychological Bulletin 115: 381—400.

Robertson, S.I. & Kahney, H. (1996). The use of examples in expository texts: Outline of an

interpretation theory for text analysis. Instructional Science 24(2): 89-119.

Ross, B.H. (1984). Remindings and their effects in learning a cognitive skill. Cognitive

Psychology 16: 371—416.

Ross, B.H. (1987). This is like that: The use of earlier problems and the separation of similar

ity effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition 13(4):

629-639. Ross, B.H. (1989a). Distinguishing types of superficial similarities. Different effects on

the access and use of earlier problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,

Memory, & Cognition 14: 510-520.

Ross, B.H. & Kennedy, P.T. (1990). Generalizing from the use of earlier examples in problem

solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 16: 42-55.

Singley, M.K. & Anderson, J.R. (1989). The Transfer of Cognitive Skill. Cambridge, MA:

Harvard University Press.

Sternberg, R.J. (1977). Component processes in analogical reasoning. Psychological Review

84: 353-378.

Thagard, P., Holyoak, K.J., Nelson, G. & Gochfield, D. (1990). Analog retrieval by constraint

satisfaction. Artificial Intelligence 46: 259-310.

VanLehn, K., Jones, R.M. & Chi, M.T.H. (1992). A model of the self-explanation effect. The

Journal of the Learning Sciences 2: 1-59.

This content downloaded from 141.101.201.167 on Sat, 28 Jun 2014 18:15:54 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions


Recommended