+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for...

Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for...

Date post: 21-Sep-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 0 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
101
Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in Children with Normal and Poor Word-Reading Skills by Erika Zapparoli A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Human Development and Applied Psychology University of Toronto © Copyright by Erika Zapparoli 2009
Transcript
Page 1: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in Children with Normal and Poor Word-Reading Skills

by

Erika Zapparoli

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts

Human Development and Applied Psychology University of Toronto

© Copyright by Erika Zapparoli 2009

Page 2: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

ii

Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in Children with

Normal and Poor Word-Reading Skills

Erika Zapparoli

Masters of Arts

Human Development and Applied Psychology University of Toronto

2009

ABSTRACT

This study examined written expression skills in children with attention problems with

and without word reading difficulties. The sample consisted of 28 children with attention

problems (AP) only, 18 children with coexisting attention and reading problems (ARP), and 34

children without attention or word reading difficulties (TYP). Curriculum-based measurement

(CBM) indices of accuracy and fluency, plus teacher ratings of handwriting, spelling, and

overall writing skills were used to assess children’s written expression skills. The analyses

indicated that the AP and ARP groups received significantly lower scores on all measures of

written expression than the TYP group. The ARP group scored significantly lower than the AP

group on the teacher ratings of writing and spelling. These findings suggest that inattention is

significantly related to written expression difficulties independent of word-reading skills.

Page 3: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

iii

TABLE OF CONTENTS Abstract ii List of Tables vi List of Figures vii List of Appendices viii Chapter 1: Introduction 1 1.1 Background and Overview of the Present Study 1.2 Literature Review

1.2.1 Academic and Cognitive Weaknesses Associated with ADHD 1.2.1.1 General Academic Underachievement and Learning Difficulties

1.2.1.2 Inattention and Reading Difficulties

1.2.1.3 ADHD and Writing Difficulties

1.2.1.4 Summary

1.2.2 Children’s Writing Development

1.2.2.1 Theoretical Framework for Writing Development

1.2.2.2 The Role of EF and WM in Writing Acquisition

1.2.2.3 The Role of Transcription in Writing Acquisition

1.2.2.4 The Role of Word-Reading Skills in Writing Acquisition

1.2.2.5 Summary

1.2.3 Assessment of Written Expression Skills

1.2.3.1 Issues and Considerations in Writing Assessment

1.2.3.2 Key Assessment Components According to Simple View

1.2.3.3 Curriculum-Based Measurement (CBM) of Written Expression

1.2.4. Summary of Literature Review and Rationale for Present Study

Page 4: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

iv

1.3 Objectives and Hypotheses

Chapter 2: Method 32

2.1 Participants

2.2 Procedure

2.3 Measures

2.4 Statistical Analyses

Chapter 3: Results 42

3.1 Group Differences in Written Expression Outcomes

3.2 Partial Correlations between Inattention vs. Hyperactivity-Impulsivity and Writing

Outcomes

3.3 Supplementary Analyses

Chapter 4: Discussion 49

4.1 Summary and Discussion of Results

4.1.1 Transcription Level Outcomes

4.1.2 Text Generation Level Outcomes

4.1.3 Language Arts Competence

4.1.4 General Summary and Discussion

4.2 Implications for Research, Theory, and Practice

4.3 Limitations and Directions for Future Research

4.4 Conclusion

References 68

Tables 80

Appendices 84

Figure Captions and Figures 92

Page 5: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

v

List of Tables Table 1. Sample Characteristics by Subgroup Table 2. Group Differences on the CBM Assessments of Written Expression Table 3. Group Differences on the Teacher Ratings of Written Expression and Language Arts Skills Table 4. Zero-order and Partial Correlations between Inattention and Hyperactivity with Writing Variables

Page 6: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

vi

List of Figures

Figure 1. Schematic Diagram for the Simple View of Writing Figure 4. Hypothetical Model of the Relationships between Inattention and Written Expression

Page 7: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

vii

List of Appendices

Appendix A. Scoring Procedures for CBM Writing Probe with Scored Examples Appendix B. Table of Correlations between Direct Measures of Writing and Teacher Ratings of

Writing Appendix C. Table of Means and Standard Deviations for Direct Measures of Writing by

Gender and Group Appendix D. Table of Means and Standard Deviations for Teacher Ratings of Writing by

Gender and Group Appendix E. Copyright Permission from Guilford Press for use of adapted schematic diagram

of Simple View of Writing (Berninger & Amtmann, 2003)

Page 8: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

1

CHAPTER ONE

INTRODUCTION

Page 9: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

2

1.1 Background and Overview of the Present Study

Children with a clinical diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are

at high risk for academic impairment, special education, grade repetition, and high school

dropout (Biederman et al., 2004; Currie & Stabile, 2006; Deshazo-Barry, Lyman, & Klinger,

2002; DuPaul et al., 2004; Rapport, Scanlan, & Denney, 1999; Spira & Fischel, 2005). Children

with ADHD display developmentally inappropriate symptoms of inattention and/or

hyperactivity in more than one context (American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and

Statistical Manual, 4th ed., TR, 2000). Prior research has demonstrated that written expression

disabilities are twice as common as learning disabilities (defined as an IQ-achievement

discrepancy) in reading or math among children with a clinical diagnosis of ADHD (Mayes &

Calhoun, 2006; 2007a; Mayes, Calhoun, & Crowell, 2000). Specifically, 65% of children with

ADHD have been found to meet criteria for a learning disability in written expression relative to

33% for reading and 26% for math (Mayes & Calhoun, 2006; 2007a; Mayes et al., 2000).

Despite the higher prevalence of written expression disabilities relative to reading and

math disabilities among students with ADHD, there is considerably more research concerning

the reading skills of children with ADHD than there is for written expression (Berninger,

Mizokawa, & Bragg, 1991; Elbert, 1993; Mayes & Calhoun, 2007a; Mayes et al., 2000).

Furthermore, previous research concerning ADHD and written expression has not typically

examined the specific role of inattention, nor has it explicitly tested and controlled for co-

existing reading difficulties. This thesis focuses on the inattention dimension of behaviour,

rather than ADHD per se, because a growing body of research indicates that symptoms of

inattention, but not symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity are associated with academic

underachievement in reading and mathematics in both clinical and community samples of

Page 10: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

3

children (Dally, 2006; DuPaul et al., 2004; Fuchs et al., 2006; Frick et al., 1991; Rabiner &

Coie, 2000; Todd et al., 2002; Warner-Rogers, Taylor, Taylor, & Sandberg, 2000).

Children with attention problems may be more likely to have written expression

difficulties than children without attention problems because they often exhibit weaknesses in

executive function (EF) and working memory (WM) (Barkley, 1997; Gathercole et al.,2008; Lui

& Tannock, 2007; Martinussen & Tannock, 2006; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Westerberg,

Hirvikoski, Frossberg, & Klingberg, 2004). Developing writers need high levels of attention and

memory resources, as well as linguistic and transcription skills in order to hold thoughts about

content in mind while remembering and writing letter forms, identifying correct spellings, and

thinking of suitable words and phrases (Berninger, 1999; Berninger et al., 1996; Graham &

Harris, 2003; McCutchen, 1996 ). Specifically, the writing process draws heavily on EF and

WM to coordinate, execute and regulate a number of component writing skills all at once

(Altemeier, Abbott, & Berninger, 2007; Berninger, 1999; Bourke & Adams, 2003; Hayes, 2005;

Hooper, Swartz, Wakely, de Kruif, & Montgomery, 2002; Kellogg, 1996; McCutchen, 1996;

Vanderberg & Swanson, 2007). Weaknesses in EF and WM have been found to be associated

with poor writing outcomes in children (e.g., Altemeier et al., 2007; Hooper et al., 2002;

Swanson & Berninger, 1996).

Furthermore, research has shown that weak readers generally become poor writers (Juel,

1988). Specifically, difficulties with the sub-skills that underlie spelling and early word level

reading are likely to produce writing difficulties at the level of transcription (Berninger, Abbott,

Abbott, Graham, & Richards, 2002; Berninger et al., 2006; Berninger, Cartwright, Yates,

Swanson, & Abbott, 1994; Berninger, Yates & Lester, 1991; Shanahan & Lomax, 1986; 1988).

Considering that attention problems have been found to predict poor reading skills (Dally, 2006;

Rabiner & Coie, 2000), it is possible that at least some of the writing difficulties demonstrated

Page 11: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

4

by children with ADHD in previous studies are due to coexisting weaknesses in reading skills.

Hence, it is important that research studies examining the written expression difficulties of

children with attention problems control for co-existing reading problems in order to verify

whether symptoms of inattention are related to difficulties with written expression independent

of weak reading skills.

Determining whether children with attention problems demonstrate significant

weaknesses in written expression independent of coexisting word-reading weaknesses has

important implications for understanding, preventing and re-mediating written expression

difficulties among these children. Although inattentive behaviour was found to be related to

word-level reading and writing skills in a sample of children with dyslexia (Thomson et al.,

2005), no previous research could be found that specifically assessed whether inattentive

behaviour is associated with written expression outcomes in a community sample of English-

speaking children. In summary, there is a conspicuous gap in the research regarding the role of

attention in children’s writing ability and whether attention problems are related to writing

difficulties independent of reading difficulties.

Epidemiological studies indicate that written expression difficulties are one of the most

common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993;

Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun, 2006). The ability to produce quality written work plays

an important role in academic success. Teachers often evaluate student knowledge via written

expression. Also, writing is a means for communication, self-regulation, problem solving, and

idea generation (Graham & Perin, 2007). Thus, it is important to identify factors that contribute

to writing difficulties in children. The primary objective of the present study was to determine

whether children with attention problems, with and without co-existing word reading

difficulties, exhibit weaknesses in written expression related to handwriting quality, spelling,

Page 12: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

5

compositional fluency and accuracy relative to children without attention and word reading

problems. Thus, in order to isolate and examine the specific effects of attention problems on

children’s written expression skills, the present study differentiated between children with

attention problems only and children with coexisting attention and word reading problems.

1.2 Literature Review

The theoretical rationale underlying the present investigation of written expression

difficulties in children with attention problems is comprised of four main sections. In the first

part, empirical evidence concerning the academic outcomes and cognitive characteristics

associated with attention deficits and ADHD is summarized to demonstrate a gap in the

literature regarding the relationship between inattention and written expression outcomes. The

second section consists of a review of the literature concerning children’s writing development

to provide a framework for understanding written expression difficulties in children with

attention problems. A gap in the literature is identified regarding the role of inattention in

children's written expression skills. The third section discusses the assessment of written

expression and describes the advantages of curriculum-based measures (CBM) of written

language, the tool that was used to assess children’s writing in the present study. Finally, the

literature review will be concluded by a summary of the key findings of prior research and the

questions which remain unanswered.

1.2.1 Academic and Cognitive Weaknesses Associated with ADHD

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a common behavioural disorder that

affects 3% to 7% of children (5.3% worldwide; Polanczyk, de Lima, Horta, Biederman, Rohde,

2007). Children with ADHD exhibit developmentally inappropriate symptoms of inattentive

behaviour, and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity that cause significant impairment (American

Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 4th ed., TR, 2000). Children with a

Page 13: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

6

diagnosis of ADHD have been reported to experience poor psychiatric, social, and academic

outcomes (Barkley, Fischer, Edelbrock, & Smallish, 1990; Biederman et al., 2006; Biederman et

al., 2004; DuPaul et al., 2004; Hart, Lahey, Loeber, Applegate, & Frick, 1995). Symptom

intensity on the hyperactive-impulsive dimension of ADHD tends to be associated with

oppositional behaviour and poor social outcomes (e.g., Chhabildas, Pennington, & Willcutt,

2001; Weiss, Worling, & Wasdell, 2003). In contrast, symptom intensity on the inattentive

dimension of ADHD has been reported to be associated with cognitive deficits in executive

function (EF) and working memory (WM) (Barkley, 1997; Chhabildas et al., 2001; Martinussen

& Tannock, 2006; Westerberg et al., 2004; Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, Pennington, 2005;

Wilding, 2003; 2005), academic underachievement (DuPaul et al.,2004; Loe & Feldman, 2007;

Mayes et al., 2000; Spira & Fischel, 2005), and comorbid learning disabilities (Mayes et al.,

2000; Weiss et al.,2003; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000).

The symptoms of ADHD have been found to occur along “a continuum of risk” (Levy,

Hay, McStephen, Wood, Waldman, 1997; Loe & Feldman, 2007; Merrell & Tymms, 2001;

Mayes et al., 2000; Rodriquez et al., 2007). Children who exhibit mild and moderate levels of

inattentive behaviour have been found to be at risk for academic under-achievement (Currie &

Stabile, 2006; Todd et al., 2002). It follows that there may be several students in any class who

exhibit mild to moderate symptoms of ADHD and experience academic risk; however, these

students would not meet the diagnostic criteria for ADHD.

1.2.1.1 General Academic Underachievement and Learning Difficulties

Children with a clinical diagnosis of ADHD often score below average on standardized

achievement tests (Barkley, DuPaul, & McMurray, 1990; Hinshaw, 1992, 1994) and the rate of

comorbid LD (as defined by IQ-achievement discrepancy) is particularly high (Barkley et al.,

1990). Reports of comorbid LD in children with ADHD range from 25% to 70% (Barkley, 1994;

Page 14: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

7

Mayes et al., 2000). Estimates of reading disorder in ADHD range from 15% to 50% (August &

Garfinkel, 1990; Barkley 1990; Mayes et al., 2000; Semrud-Clikeman et al., 1992; Willcutt &

Pennington, 2000). Approximately 24% to 60% of children with ADHD have a LD in math

(Barkley, 1990; Mayes et al., 2000; Semrud-Clikeman et al., 1992), and about 24% to 60% have

a spelling disorder (Barkley, 1990; Elbert, 1993; Mayes et al., 2000). Although research

concerning comorbid written expression disorders in children with ADHD is limited, data

suggest that the prevalence of LD in written expression among children with ADHD is higher

than is found in the non-ADHD population (Elbert, 1993; Mayes et al., 2000; Mayes &

Calhoun, 2006; 2007a). For example, 65% of children with ADHD were found to have a

comorbid LD in written expression compared to only 27% of non-ADHD children in a sample

of clinic-referred subjects (Mayes et al., 2000).

The relationship between the core symptoms of ADHD and academic risk has been

confirmed by a number of studies (Deshazo-Barry et al., 2002; DuPaul et al., 2004; Hinshaw,

1992; 1994; Merrell & Tymms, 2001; Rapport et al., 1999; Rodriquez et al., 2007). For

example, ADHD symptom severity has been found to predict academic and learning outcomes

over and above intelligence (DuPaul et al., 2004; Rapport et al., 1999), executive function

(Deshazo-Barry et al. 2002), and conduct problems (Frick et al., 1991). Inattention (as opposed

to hyperactivity-impulsivity) has been found to be a particularly strong predictor of academic

and learning outcomes among both clinical samples of children with ADHD (Dupaul et al.,

2004; Spira & Fischel, 2005) and community samples of children (Merrell & Tymms, 2001;

Rabiner & Coie, 2000; Rodriquez et al., 2007;Todd et al., 2002). Furthermore, Mayes &

Calhoun (2007b) found that weaknesses in attention, graphomotor skills, and processing speed

were associated with lower levels of academic achievement of children with ADHD.

Attention appears to be a major mediator in academic performance and learning

Page 15: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

8

disabilities (Mayes et al., 2000). Children with learning disabilities are often rated by their

teachers as exhibiting inattentive behaviour (McConaughy, Mattison, & Peterson, 1994; Speece,

McKinney & Applebaum, 1985), which has been supported by psychometric measures of

attention (Copeland &Wisniewski, 1981; Richards, Samuels, Turnure, &Ysseldyke, 1990; see

Soo & Bailey, 2006 for review; Swanson, 1983; Tarnowski, Prinz, & Nay, 1986). Furthermore,

inattentive behaviour has been found to be related deficits in EF and WM in both clinical

(Martinussen & Tannock, 2006; Westerberg et al., 2004) and community samples of children

(Gathercole et al., 2008; Lui & Tannock, 2007). Deficits in WM are also commonly found

among individuals with learning difficulties (Alloway et al., 2005; Gathercole, Alloway, Willis,

& Adams, 2006; Pickering & Gathercole, 2004; Swanson, Ashbaker, & Lee, 1996). In general,

data suggest that attention problems and learning problems are “overlapping, interrelated

spectrum disorders” that may require intervention even if diagnostic criteria for ADHD or LD

are not met (see also Levy et al., 1997; noted by Mayes et al., 2000; Merrell & Tymms, 2001;

Rabiner & Murray, 2002; Rodriguez et al., 2007).

1.2.1.2 Inattention and Reading Difficulties

Inattentive behaviour in the classroom has been reported to play a critical role in the

development of children’s reading skills (Dally, 2006; Rabiner & Coie, 2000; Rabiner, Malone

& CPPRG, 2004; Rabiner & Murray, 2002; Thomson et al., 2005; Warner-Rogers et al., 2000).

Data from longitudinal studies has demonstrated that attention problems in kindergarten are

predictive of reading difficulties in later grades (Dally, 2006; Rabiner & Coie, 2000). Moreover,

data from intervention studies indicate that attention plays a significant role in learning

outcomes (Chenault, Thomson, Abbott, & Berninger, 2006; Rabiner et al., 2004). For example,

Rabiner et al. (2004) found that students with coexisting attention and reading problems who

received reading remediation showed no improvements in reading relative to students with

Page 16: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

9

either attention or reading problems alone.

1.2.1.3 ADHD and Writing Difficulties

Existing data suggests that children who exhibit clinical levels of ADHD symptoms

demonstrate difficulties with handwriting legibility, spelling, compositional fluency, planning

and organization, sentence construction, and story composition (Adi-Japha, Landau, Frenkel,

Teicher, & Shalev, 2007; Elbert, 1993; Imhof, 2004; Mathers, 2006; Mayes & Calhoun, 2006;

2007a; 2007b; Re, Caeron, & Cornoldi, 2008; Re, Pedron, & Cornoldi, 2007). Difficulties with

handwriting have been well documented among children with ADHD (Adi-japha et al., 2007;

Imhof, 2004; Mayes & Calhoun, 2007b; see review by Racine, Majnemer, Shevell, & Snider,

2008; Schoemaker, Ketelaars, Zonneveld, Minderaa, & Mulder, 2005; Tucha & Lange, 2004).

Imhof (2004) reported that children with ADHD performed significantly below typical peers on

a copy task in terms of letter formation, alignment and neatness. There is some evidence that

weaknesses in fine motor control are related to symptoms of inattention, but not hyperactivity-

impulsivity (Fliers, Rommelse, Vermeulen, & Atlink, 2008; Martin, Piek, & Hay, 2006; Pitcher,

Piek, & Barrett, 2002; Pitcher, Piek, & Hay, 2003). These weaknesses may account for some of

handwriting difficulties among children with ADHD.

Although significant difficulties with handwriting quality are documented among

children with ADHD (see Racine et al., 2008 for a review), evidence suggests they are not

impaired in terms of handwriting fluency (i.e., speed) (Ross, Poidevant, & Miner, 1995; Re et

al., 2007). For example, no significant differences were found in handwriting fluency between

48 students with ADHD and a matched control group (Ross et al., 1995). It is important to

recognize that handwriting fluency is typically evaluated in isolation from the overall process of

composing. Thus, it is distinct from compositional fluency which is considerably more

complicated because it requires the fluent integration and execution of transcription skills

Page 17: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

10

together with higher level composing skills.

Children with ADHD have also been reported to exhibit difficulties with spelling, both

when writing compositions (e.g., Elbert, 1993; Re et al., 2007) and when completing context-

free tasks (i.e., dictation: Elbert, 1993; Kroese, Hynd, Knight, Meimez, & Hall, 2000; Mayes &

Calhoun, 2006; 2007a). In brief, children with ADHD symptoms appear to demonstrate

transcription difficulties related to handwriting quality and spelling, but not necessarily

handwriting fluency (Adi-japha et al., 2007; Re et al., 2007; Ross et al., 1995; Racine et al.,

2008).

Children with ADHD have also been found to exhibit written expression difficulties at

the level of composition. Relative to peers without ADHD symptoms, they exhibit weaknesses

in compositional fluency (e.g., story length under time constraints), punctuation, sentence

construction, planning and organization, and story composition (Mathers, 2006; Mayes &

Calhoun, 2007b; Re et al., 2007; 2008; Reid & Ortiz-Lienemann, 2006). For example, Re et al.

(2007) found that Italian children who exhibited elevated levels of ADHD symptoms in the

classroom had difficulties on all qualitative and quantitative measures of writing compared to

controls; specifically, they produced shorter texts with more spelling, grammar and punctuation

errors. In addition children with ADHD symptoms received significantly lower scores for

adequacy of product relative to task assignment, organization of text, and variety of words (Re

et al., 2007).

Similarly, another study that compared the writing skills of 21 boys with ADHD or

attention deficit disorder and 11 matched controls across three essays reported that boys with

ADHD performed significantly below controls on word complexity, written language,

productivity, and general writing ability (Resta & Eliot, 1994). Mathers (2006) reported that

children with ADHD produced more spelling and punctuation errors in writing compared to

Page 18: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

11

typical peers when required to produce three types of written text (i.e., story, recount, and

procedural). In summary, these studies indicate that children with ADHD symptoms exhibit

written expression difficulties related to spelling, compositional fluency (i.e., they tend to write

shorter stories under time constraints), and compositional accuracy (produce more errors with

punctuation, grammar, and sentence construction in their stories).

1.2.1.4 Summary and Implications of ADHD Section

Overall, these findings demonstrate that children with ADHD, and those with high levels

of ADHD symptoms, have significant difficulties with written expression related to transcription

(i.e., handwriting and spelling), compositional fluency (i.e., rate of text production), and

compositional accuracy (i.e., proportion of errors). Some critical issues however, remain

unexplored. Specifically, few of these studies examined the role of inattention, and none of these

studies explicitly tested and controlled for comorbid word level reading problems. As such it is

unclear whether inattention symptoms or comorbid word reading difficulties are associated with

the poor writing outcomes demonstrated by children with ADHD. Furthermore, previous studies

have examined handwriting fluency, but not handwriting quality, together with compositional

skills (e.g., Re et al., 2007) among children with ADHD or attention problems. Studies

examining written expression skills of children with ADHD provide a useful background for

examining the role of inattention in written expression difficulties; however, it is also necessary

to understand the developmental constraints that influence children’s writing. The next section

describes an influential model of writing development and outlines the key developmental

factors that influence children’s writing proficiency in order to provide a framework for

understanding and assessing written expression difficulties in children with attention problems.

1.2.2 Children’s Writing Development

“The physical act of transcribing written text is a major drain on cognitive resources of

Page 19: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

12

beginning writers, and text generation itself is also less fluent for younger writers.” -McCutchen, 1996, p319

Children's writing differs in many ways from adult writing, and thus some models are

more applicable to understanding early writing development than others. For example, Hayes

and Flower’s (1980) influential model of adult writing emphasizes the cognitive processes

involved in planning and revising rather than the processes involved in translating ideas into

written text. However, during the early elementary years, writing development consists mainly of

translating. Children must develop basic writing skills such as learning to print and spell (i.e.,

transcription skills) so that they can translate their ideas into written words, sentences and

discourse (i.e., text generation) (Berninger, Cartwright, et al., 1994; Berninger et al., 1996;

Berninger et al., 1992). In view of the fact that children’s writing tends to be focussed on

transcription and text generation rather than high levels of planning and revising (as does adult

writing), Berninger and colleagues (1996) modified Hayes & Flowers (1980) influential model

of adult writing to reflect the developmental constraints of children's writing. Subsequently a

developmental model of writing known as the Simple View of Writing was established

(Berninger & Amtmann, 2003; Berninger, Vaughn, et al., 2002).

1.2.2.1 Theoretical Framework for Writing Development

The Simple View of Writing (Berninger & Amtmann, 2003; Berninger, Vaughn, et al.,

2002), provides a developmental framework for understanding children’s writing that

corresponds to the key constraints (i.e., linguistic, neuro-developmental and cognitive) that

influence early writing development (Berninger et al., 1996). This developmental model of

writing consists of the following three key components: (a) text generation; (b) transcription,

including spelling and handwriting; and (c) executive function (EF) including attention control

and self regulation skills. With the support of working memory, these three key components

Page 20: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

13

interact throughout the composing process. Text generation involves producing ideas and

converting them into language (e.g., words or sentences) (Berninger et al., 1992; Berninger et

al., 1996; Berninger, Vaughn, et al., 2002). Transcription is the physical act of translating and

recording those language representations into decipherable text on the page. Transcription skills

depend upon orthographic motor integration and phonetic skills for handwriting and spelling.

According the model, EF is involved in regulating the overall writing process, and WM is

related to the efficiency of each component process; thus EF and WM correspond to cognitive

constraints of writing development. Schematically, the Simple View of Writing represents the

components of children’s writing in a triangle (see Figure 1). Transcription and EF are the

foundation of children’s writing. In the model, EF and WM support the goal of text generation.

WM has a central position in the model as it connects to and facilitates each component. As

depicted in the model, writing proficiency corresponds to the text generation component

positioned at the apex of the triangle and is enabled by available WM resources and the

transcription and self regulation components at the base.

The components in the Simple View of Writing are all engaged and interacting together

throughout the composing process. However, the relative influence of each component may

vary across development and across individuals. For example, the transcription component

tends to exert a strong influence on the early acquisition of writing because the low-order neuro-

developmental skills which underlie transcription are just emerging. Specifically, low-order

neuro-developmental skills such as the speed of sequential finger movements, rapid coding of

orthographic information, and rapid automatic production of alphabet letters have been shown to

be good predictors of beginning handwriting and composition skills (Berninger et al., 2006;

Berninger et al., 1992; Ritchey, 2008). Weaknesses in these low-level neuro-developmental

skills may constrain the development of proficient transcription skills. In turn, poor transcription

Page 21: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

14

skills can act as a major barrier to the text generation and fluent composing of beginning writers

during the translation process (Berninger et al., 1992). According the model, as the children’s

transcription and EF (e.g., attention regulation) skills become increasingly developed and

effortless, more WM resources become available for the text generation component of writing

and the fluency and quality of children’s writing improves (Berninger, 1999; Berninger & Fuller,

1992; Berninger et al., 1996; Berninger, Mizokawa, Bragg, Cartwright, & Yates, 1994;

Berninger, Vaughn, et al., 2002). Thus, children’s ability write fluently and proficiently depends

on automatic transcription skills, strong EF abilities and available WM resources.

In summary, this model illustrates that the children’s writing proficiency is strongly

influenced by the development of text generation and transcription abilities as well as the ability

to effectively regulate their attention (EF) between the demands of transcription and text

generation during the translating process (Berninger et al., 1996; Berninger, Vaughn, et al.,

2002). The Simple View of Writing provides a theoretical basis for the main hypothesis tested in

the present study as well as a framework for assessing children’s writing. The present study

tested the hypothesis that children with attention problems (poor attention-control) with or

without poor word reading skills would demonstrate written expression difficulties. Although

the role of inattention in children's writing has not been explicitly examined in previous studies

with typically developing children; the role of specific cognitive processes such as WM, and EF,

as well as the role of transcription and reading skills in children's writing have been

demonstrated in number of studies. Evidence for the relative roles of EF, transcription and word-

reading in children’s writing is reviewed in the next sections.

1.2.2.2 The Role of EF and WM in Writing Acquisition

Successful writing requires the effective allocation of a number of resources such as

attention, memory, organizational, and problem solving (Berninger, Vaughn, et al., 2002;

Page 22: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

15

Graham & Harris, 2000; Hayes, 2005; Hooper et al., 2002; Kellogg, 1996; Swanson &

Berninger, 1996). Executive function and WM processes, sometimes referred to as attention

control and self-regulation (SR), are attributed a central role in several descriptions of writing

for both adults and children (Berninger & Amtmann, 2003; Berninger, Vaughn, et al., 2002;

Graham & Harris, 2000; Hayes, 2005; Hooper et al., 2002; Kellogg, 1996; McCutchen, 1996;

2000). In these descriptions of writing, EF and WM are generally responsible for planning and

revising, maintaining the goals of the specific writing task, managing the writing environment,

and coordinating and executing the component skills involved in composing all at the same time

(see Berninger et al., 1996; Graham & Harris, 2000; Hayes, 2005; McCutchen, 1996). Research

has confirmed that EF and WM measures contribute to overall writing proficiency, as well as

planning, translation, revision and many sub-measures of written output such as punctuation and

grammar (Altemeier et al., 2007; Altemeier, Jones, Abbott, & Berninger, 2006; Berninger, 1999;

Bourke & Adams, 2003; Graham & Harris, 2000; Hooper et al., 2002; Swanson & Berninger,

1996; Vanderberg & Swanson, 2007). Evidence suggests that the controlled attention

component of WM is intricately tied to writing in school students (Vanderberg & Swanson,

2007). As children’s writing does not typically involve high levels of planning and revising, EF

skills and WM resources are primarily responsible for controlling attention and efficiently

coordinating component writing skills during the translating process in children’s writing.

Furthermore, individual differences in WM capacity are suspected to influence the

acquisition and fluency of early transcription skills (Berninger, 1999; Graham & Harris, 2000;

McCutchen, 1996; Swanson & Berninger, 1996). Transcription skills consume most of the

attentional resources of beginning writers (Berninger, 1999; Berninger et al., 1992; McCutchen,

1996). Limited attentional resources are thus likely to slow or impair the acquisition of fluent

transcription skills; in turn less attentional resources are likely to be available for higher level

Page 23: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

16

text generation processes (Berninger, 1999; Berninger & Amtmann, 2003; Graham & Harris,

2000; McCutchen, 1996).

Although it is generally accepted that the ability to control and sustain attention is

important for the efficient execution of quality written expression (Berninger & Amtmann,

2003; Berninger, Vaughn, et al., 2002; Graham & Harris, 2000; Hayes, 2005; Hooper et al.,

2002; Kellogg, 1996; Vanderberg & Swanson, 2007), the relationship between inattentive

behaviour and written expression outcomes has not been explicitly explored in a community-

based sample of English-speaking children. However, behavioural measures of attention were

found to play a significant role in the sentence and word writing skills of English-speaking

children with dyslexia in one study (Thomson et al., 2005). Furthermore, in another study of

English-speaking children with dyslexia - prior attention training was reported to improve the

effects of composition instruction (Chenault et al., 2006). Children with attention problems are

likely to demonstrate weaknesses in writing relative to children without attention problems

because the ability to control and sustain attention is essential in written expression.

Furthermore, given the domain-general role attributed to attention and related constructs (i.e.,

EF and WM) for regulating the writing process, the writing difficulties associated with attention

problems are also likely to be domain-general (Hooper et al., 2002; McCutchen, 1996; Swanson

& Berninger, 1996). As such, many aspects of writing may be undermined by problems with

attention such as compositional fluency and accuracy on a timed writing task as well as

transcription skills.

1.2.2.3 The Role of Transcription in Writing Acquisition

Children’s overall writing ability is strongly influenced by basic transcription skills

which consist of both handwriting and spelling skills (Berninger, 1999; Berninger et al., 1996;

Berninger, Vaughan, Abbott, Abbott, & Woodruff, 1997; Berninger et al., 1992; Graham, 1990;

Page 24: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

17

Graham et al., 1997; McCutchen, 1996). Orthographic (letter) knowledge, orthographic-motor

integration for letter writing, and phonemic segmentation support transcription skills (Abbott &

Berninger, 1993; Berninger, Cartwright, et al., 1994; Berninger et al., 1996; Berninger, Yates, et

al., 1991). Together handwriting and spelling draw on orthographic (letter) knowledge

(Berninger, Cartwright, et al., 1994; Berninger et al., 1992; Berninger, Yates, et al., 1991). Since

orthographic knowledge represents children’s internal representation of letter shapes, deficits in

this area may manifest themselves in both handwriting and/or spelling weaknesses. Handwriting

also draws on orthographic-motor integration skills to physically translate these orthographic

forms into written text (Berninger et al., 1992; Berninger, Cartwright, et al., 1994); whereas

spelling also draws on phonemic segmentation skills to sound out and identify letters and letter

clusters within the word being spelled. Although spelling generally draws on the same subskills

involved in word-level reading (Berninger, Abbott, et al., 2002; Berninger, Cartwright, et al.,

1994), it has been argued that learning to spell is generally more difficult for children than

learning to read (see Berninger, 1999 for discussion).

Both handwriting and spelling skills have both been reported to predict children's

compositional fluency and quality at the elementary level (Abbott & Berninger, 1993;

Berninger, 1999; Berninger et al., 1997; Berninger et al., 1992; Graham, 1990; Graham et al.,

1997; Graham, Harris, & Fink, 2000; McCutchen, 1996). Handwriting skills have been found to

contribute directly to compositional fluency (Berninger et al., 1997; Graham et al., 2000) and

quality in both the primary and intermediate grades (Berninger, Cartwright, et al., 1994;

Berninger et al., 1992; Graham et al., 1997). Graham et al. (1997) found that spelling

contributed directly to compositional fluency and indirectly – through handwriting – to

compositional quality in the primary grades. In the intermediate grades (i.e. grade four through

six), spelling was found to contribute to both compositional fluency and quality indirectly

Page 25: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

18

through its correlation with handwriting. Furthermore, the treatment of handwriting problems

has been found to transfer to improved compositional skills in elementary level students

(Berninger et al., 1997; Graham et al., 2000; Jones & Christensen, 1999).

Consistent with Berninger, Vaughn, et al.’s (2002) Simple View of Writing, the evidence

reviewed demonstrates that transcription skills play an important role in children’s writing

ability during the elementary years. Difficulties with basic handwriting and spelling skills,

particularly weaknesses in handwriting fluency, are often related to problems with text

generation such as poor compositional fluency and quality (e.g., Berninger et al., 1992;

Rosenblum, Weiss, & Parush, 2003). When letter production is not fully automatic, the act of

handwriting may place increased demands on memory and attentional resources, which, in turn,

may constrain the higher level cognitive processes required for composition (Graham & Harris,

2000; Jones and Christensen, 1999). Furthermore, it may be that children with limited

attentional resources are slower to develop automatized transcription skills, and thus experience

an elevated risk for transcription related writing difficulties.

1.2.2.4 The Role of Word-Reading Skills in Writing Acquisition

Early reading and writing development is closely tied because reading and writing

acquisition involve many of the same preliminary sub-skills (Berninger, Cartwright, et al. 1994;

Berninger, Yates, et al., 1991; Shanahan, 1984; 2006; Shanahan & Lomax, 1986; 1988). In

particular, transcription and word-reading skills draw on many of the same sub-processes

including orthographic (letter) and phonological (letter-sound) coding (Berninger, Cartwright, et

al. 1994; Berninger, Yates, et al., 1991; Shanahan, 1984; 2006; Shanahan & Lomax, 1986;

1988). Evidence shows that early word reading skills and related processes (e.g. orthographic

knowledge and phonological awareness) are strongly related to the acquisition of the ability to

write letters and spell words at the elementary (Abbott & Berninger, 1993; Berninger, Abbott, et

Page 26: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

19

al., 2002; Berninger et al., 2006; Berninger, Cartwright, et al., 1994; Olinghouse, 2008) and

kindergarten levels (Berninger et al., 2006; Ritchey, 2008). Deficits in word-reading or the

component processes of word-reading are thus likely to manifest themselves as spelling and

handwriting difficulties.

The relationship between word reading skills and various aspects of writing, including

transcription, compositional fluency and quality has been demonstrated in a number of studies

(Abbott & Berninger, 1993; Berninger, Abbott, et al., 2002; Berninger et al., 2006; Berninger,

Cartwright, et al., 1994; Olinghouse, 2008; Ritchey, 2008, Shanahan, 1984; 2006; Shanahan &

Lomax, 1986; 1988). At the elementary level, correlation coefficients reported between word

reading and writing skills range from .36 to .76 for spelling, .21 to .32 for handwriting, and .14

to. 52 for compositional fluency or quality (Berninger, Abbott, et al., 2002; Berninger,

Cartwright, et al., 1994; Berninger et al., 1992; Olinghouse, 2008). Evidence suggests that word-

reading is directly related to transcription and indirectly related to compositional fluency and

quality through transcription (e.g., Berninger, Abbott, et al., 2002; Berninger, Cartwright, et al.,

1994; Olinghouse, 2008). Accordingly, weak word reading skills may contribute to written

expression difficulties at the level of transcription; in turn, difficulties at the level of

transcription may have a negative impact on children’s fluency and quality of writing.

1.2.2.5 Summary of Children’s Writing Development Section

The Simple View of Writing (Berninger & Amtmann, 2003) identifies text generation,

transcription and EF skills as key components that each draw on WM resources to varying

degrees at different stages of writing development. Empirical evidence confirms the importance

of WM and EF processes, as well as transcription skills in children's writing. Weaknesses in EF

and WM may result in written expression difficulties related to compositional fluency, accuracy

and quality as well as transcription (Bourke & Adams, 2003; Hooper et al., 2002; McCutchen,

Page 27: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

20

1996; Swanson & Berninger, 1996; Vanderberg & Swanson, 2007). Poor handwriting or

spelling skills may also influence weaknesses in compositional fluency and quality during the

elementary years (Berninger, Cartwright, et al., 1994; Berninger et al., 1997; Berninger, Vaughn,

et al., 2002; Berninger et al., 1992; Graham et al., 1997; Olinghouse, 2008). Furthermore, weak

word-reading skills may impact compositional fluency and quality indirectly through problems

with transcription skills since word level reading skills are closely related to spelling and

handwriting skills (Berninger, Abbott, et al., 2002; Berninger, Cartwright, et al., 1994).

Children who exhibit inattentive behaviour have been found to have weaknesses in EF and WM

(Gathercole et al., 2008; Lui & Tannock, 2007) as well as reading (Dally, 2006; Rabiner & Coie,

2000), all of which are important to developing writing proficiency.

The Simple View of Writing provided the theoretical basis for the main hypothesis tested

in the present study. Specifically, children with attention problems (poor attention-control) with

or without poor word reading skills were expected to demonstrate written expression

difficulties. Children with coexisting attention and word-reading problems were expected to be

even more impaired than those with attention problems only. Given the general importance of

controlled attention and EF in the Simple View of Writing, and the EF and WM weaknesses

associated with inattentive behaviour, children with attention problems were expected to

demonstrate poor compositional fluency, quality and accuracy (i.e., text generation skills) on a

timed writing task as well as poor transcription skills.

Children with coexisting attention and word-reading problems were expected to be more

impaired than children with attention problems only on all these measures since transcription

and word-reading skills are closely related (Berninger, Cartwright, et al., 1994; Berninger,

Abbott, et al., 2002; Shanahan, 1984; 2006) and weak transcription skills tend to contribute to

higher level written expression difficulties (Berninger et al., 1992; Graham et al., 1997).

Page 28: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

21

Previous research has shown that children with ADHD symptoms have significant

writing difficulties (e.g., Re et al., 2007). However, the role of inattention in children's writing

ability has not been explicitly examined in previous studies with typically developing children.

The present thesis study therefore addresses a significant gap in the writing development

literature. The next section addresses issues and considerations relevant to assessing and

understanding children’s written expression and explains the advantages of employing objective

Curriculum-Based Measurement (CBM) of written expression for present study.

1.2.3 Assessment of Written Expression Skills

“…writing disabilities may be rarely identified but they certainly are not rare.” -Mayes & Calhoun, 2007a, p. 443

Despite the fact that written expression difficulties are one of the most common and

impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Berninger, Mizokawa, & Braggs,

1991; Cobb-Morocco, Dalton, & Tivnan, 1992; Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes

& Calhoun, 2006; 2007a; Mayes et al., 2000), many standardized tests of academic achievement

tend to be insensitive to writing disorders because they only assess written expression at the

word or sentence level (Berninger, Mizokawa, et al., 1991; Mayes & Calhoun, 2007a; Mayes,

Calhoun & Lane, 2005; Sandler et al., 1992; Wiener, 1986). In fact, specific writing difficulties

often are not identified and children may not receive interventions for these difficulties

(Berninger, Mizokawa, et al., 1991; Berninger et al., 2008a; Mayes et al., 2005; Mayes &

Calhoun, 2007a; Sandler et al., 1992; Wiener, 1986). Understanding and identifying those

factors that differentiate good writers from poor writers has important implications for

developing effective instructional practice, as well as for recognizing, preventing and

remediating written expression difficulties.

Page 29: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

22

1.2.3.1 Issues and Considerations in Writing Assessment

The assessment of written expression is a particularly challenging area of academic

assessment. In contrast to assessments of reading and math which typically require individuals

to respond in a way such that a correct response is easily evident, assessments of written

expression do not tend to have a single correct answer (Gansle, VanDerHeyden, Noell, Resetar,

Williams, 2006). Such responses are considerably more complicated to evaluate objectively and

efficiently. For example, as pointed out by Gansle et al. one student may write a text that is clear

and well-organized, but with many spelling errors; whereas another student may write a text that

lacks coherence, but with few spelling or punctuation errors. In addition to a writer’s writing

proficiency, several other factors may influence an individual’s score on a writing assessment.

Schoonen (2005) noted that various aspects of writing assessment such as which traits were

assessed (e.g., content or conventions) and the way in which these traits are scored (e.g., a

holistic rating scale or analytically) may influence the outcome of the assessment.

Furthermore, it is important to consider that different standardized achievement tests of

written expression have been found to yield different results regarding the presence or absence

of a writing disability depending on the specific skills measured (Mayes et al., 2005; Mayes et

al., 2007). For example, the reported rate of written expression disabilities identified is twice as

high when employing the written expression subtest of the Weschler Individual Achievement

Test (WIAT) (Psychological Corp., 1992), which assesses compositional writing skills,

compared to the written language subtests of the Woodcock Johnson, Psycho-Educational

Battery-revised (Woodcock & Johnson, 1989) in which the ability to produce individual words

and sentences is evaluated (e.g., see Mayes et al., 2005). This discrepancy in outcomes across

different tests suggests that assessments which involve composition writing are more sensitive

measures of writing ability than are assessments that require writing words and sentences in

Page 30: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

23

isolation (Berninger, Mizokawa, et al., 1991; Mayes et al., 2005).

Furthermore, evidence suggests there are considerable intraindividual differences in

terms of word, sentence and paragraph level writing skills (Berninger, Mizokawa, et al., 1994).

Thus, the assessment of written expression difficulties is further complicated by the

considerable variability that exists regarding individual strengths and weaknesses in writing

subskills (Berninger, Mizokawa, et al., 1991). Struggling elementary level writers tend to

produce written compositions with poor handwriting legibility, fewer words and sentences,

higher percentages of spelling, capitalization and punctuation errors, and less sensitivity to text

structure compared to proficient elementary writers (e.g., Graham, 1990; Graham, 1999;

Graham, Harris, MacArthur, & Schwartz, 1991; Graham & Harris, 2003; Houck & Billingsley,

1989; Thomas, Englert, & Gregg, 1987). However, the specific type or pattern of writing errors

and the underlying causes of writing problems may vary considerably among these students

(Berninger, Mizokawa, et al., 1991; Berninger, Mizokawa, et al., 1994; Mayes et al., 2005).

Cluster analysis techniques have been used to empirically identify a variety of written

expression subtypes that reflect distinct patterns of skills and impairment (Roid, 1994; Sandler

et al., 1992; Wakely, Hooper, de Kruif, & Swartz, 2006). For example, Sandler et al. (1992)

found that some children exhibited writing difficulties because of poor visual-spatial skills;

whereas other students had attention and memory problems. Because writing involves a diverse

array of multi-level skills, a variety of developmental dysfunctions (e.g., linguistic, neuro-motor,

cognitive) may contribute to a child’s written output (Berninger, Mizokawa, et al., 1991;

Berninger, Mizokawa, et al., 1994).

1.2.3.2 Key Assessment Components According to Simple View

It is important that assessments of written expression include measures of multiple skills

across levels because written expression is a multifaceted ability involving several high- and

Page 31: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

24

low-level subskills (Berninger, 1999; Berninger et al., 1996; Berninger, Mizokawa, et al., 1991).

Measures of transcription for example, (i.e., spelling and handwriting) assess low-level skills.

Measures of compositional fluency (i.e., rate of text production) and overall compositional

accuracy (i.e., correct use of grammar, punctuation and capitalization) represent the higher-level

text generation and EF skills as identified in the Simple View of Writing (Berninger &

Amtmann, 2003; Berninger, Vaughn, et al., 2002). Furthermore, transcription skills,

compositional fluency, and accuracy are strong predictors of overall writing proficiency at the

elementary level (Berninger et al., 1992; Berninger, Cartwright, et al., 1994; Berninger et al.,

1997; Brooks, Vaughan, & Berninger, 1999; Graham et al., 1997; Graham et al., 2000; Jones &

Christensen, 1999) and are thereby important components to include when assessing of writing

proficiency.

It is important to specifically evaluate spelling and handwriting because these may be the

first signs of writing difficulties in primary grade children (Berninger, et al., 1992). It is also

important to assess compositional fluency as poor compositional fluency is also one of the early

indicators of writing disability in the primary grades (Berninger, Mizokawa, et al., 1991;

Berninger et al., 1992). Although measures of compositional fluency (e.g., number of words

produced) theoretically reflect how easily the writer can find words to express ideas (i.e., text

generation), compositional fluency is also influenced by available WM resources, and the speed

and efficiency of EF and transcription skills during the composing process (Berninger, 1999;

Berninger et al., 1997; Berninger, Vaughn, et al., 2002; Berninger et al., 1992; Graham et al.,

1997). In summary, previous research and writing theory suggest that assessments of written

expression at the elementary level should specifically evaluate both text generation and

transcription skills within the context of the composing process (e.g., Berninger, Mizokawa, et

al., 1991; Mayes et al., 2005).

Page 32: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

25

A variety of approaches may be employed by writing researchers, teachers and school

psychologists to carry out the daunting task of objectively scoring written narratives or essays

and assessing written expression skills. Some examples include: holistic scoring, which entails

using an overall impression to rate a paper on a scale with anchor papers serving as scale

examples (e.g., see Cooper, 1997); primary trait scoring, in which features of a particular genre

are identified and rated; discourse scoring, in which T-units or other discourse units are counted

(e.g., Houck & Billingsley, 1989); and objective analytic scoring, which involves counting

micro-measures of writing (e.g., number of correct words, sentences, punctuation, etc., see

Deno, Marston, & Mirkin,1982; Parker, Tindal, & Hasbrouck, 1991b). In contrast to holistic

measures of writing and subjective judgements of quality which may require specialized training

in using a rating scheme, objective analytic measures of writing may be more useful for

identifying specific areas of difficulty and they may be more reliable (Berninger et al., 1992).

For example, holistic measures of writing may overlook specific deficiencies in handwriting or

spelling and may be more vulnerable to producing inconsistent results due to the subjectivity of

the scorer. Curriculum-based measurement (CBM) of written expression use standardized,

objective analytic scoring procedures. This type of assessment was used to assess writing in the

present study.

1.2.3.2 Curriculum-Based Measurement (CBM) of Written Expression

Curriculum-based measures were originally developed to provide an efficient and valid

means for teachers to assess the effects of instruction in special education settings (Deno, 1992;

Deno & Fuchs, 1987; Deno, Mirkin, & Wesson, 1984; Tindal & Parker, 1989). CBM

assessments use brief tasks in which correct and incorrect student responses to the tasks are

counted within a set time interval (usually in minutes). The CBM assessment system is built on

a set of common principles such that assessments are administered and scored according to an

Page 33: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

26

established set of rules (Hosp, Hosp, & Howell, 2006).

Curriculum-based measures of writing are useful because they provide objective

measures of writing that focus on the curriculum (i.e., relevant material). CBM measures of

written expression have been used to distinguish between skilled and poor writers and, to

identify students who require extra support (e.g., Parker et al., 1991a; Watkinson & Lee, 1992).

Furthermore, there is extensive empirical evidence supporting the reliability and validity of

curriculum-based measures to effectively assess of written expression proficiency (Deno,

Marsten, & Mirkin, 1982; Gansle et al., 2004; Gansle et al., 2006; Jewell & Malecki, 2005;

Malecki & Jewell, 2003; for review see McMaster & Espin, 2007; Parker et al., 1991a; 1991b;

Tindal & Parker, 1989; Weissenburger & Espin, 2005; Videen, Deno, & Marston, 1982).

Curriculum based measures have been described as “academic vital signs” because they can be

used as “general indicators” of student performance in an academic area (Gansle et al., 2006;

Hosp et al., 2006).

The CBM writing assessment typically indexes children’s ability to produce a

composition within a limited amount of time. Specifically, students are presented with a lined

sheet of paper with a story starter prompt at the top and instructed to think for 1 minute about a

possible corresponding story. They are then given 3 minutes (sometimes 5 minutes) to write the

story. CBM writing samples can be scored in various ways. For example, CBM writing samples

can be assessed for (a) fluency (total words written, words spelled correctly, correct writing

sequences); (b) accuracy (percentage of words spelled correctly, percentage of correct writing

sequences); and (c) accurate production indicator (correct minus incorrect writing sequences)

(Hosp et al., 2006). The CBM writing sample collected for the present study was only scored on

the first two indices (i.e., fluency and accuracy) as well as in terms of handwriting quality. Early

research on CBM writing assessment focused on the identification of reliable and valid

Page 34: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

27

indicators of written expression performance at the elementary school level (Deno et al., 1982;

Deno, Mirkin, & Marston, 1980; Marston & Deno, 1981; Videen et al., 1982).

The CBM indices for fluency and accuracy utilized in the present study have been

previously validated against standardized assessments of writing (e.g., SAT-9, Test of Written

Language; Woodcock Johnson Revised: Writing subtests), language arts grades, and teachers’

holistic ratings of writing (Deno et al., 1982; Gansle et al., 2004; Jewell & Malecki, 2005;

Malecki & Jewell, 2003; for review see McMaster & Espin, 2007; Parker et al., 1991b; Videen

et al., 1982; Weissenburger & Espin, 2005). Initially, research on CBM indices of writing

focussed on the fluency measures for total words written (TWW) and the number of words

spelled correctly (WSC) and provided evidence that these measures are reliable and valid

indicators of writing proficiency at the elementary school level (Deno et al., 1982; Deno et al.,

1980; Marston, Lowry, Deno, & Mirkin, 1981; Shinn & Marston, 1985). Subsequent research

concerning the reliability and validity of the fluency-based score for correct writing sequences

(CWS) and accuracy-based scores (i.e., %WSC and %CWS) indicates that these measures

correlate more strongly with criterion measures of writing and thus may be even more useful for

assessing general writing proficiency in elementary students than the fluency-based measures

for TWW and WSC (Jewell & Malecki, 2005; Parker et al., 1991b; Weissenburger & Espin,

2005; Videen et al., 1982). For example, among elementary level students the fluency score for

correct writing sequences (CWS) has been found to correlate significantly with holistic ratings

of writing, language arts achievement scores and the Test of Written Language (TOWL)

(Hammill & Larsen, 1983) with correlations ranging from .51 to .85 (Parker et al., 1991b;

Videen et al., 1982; Weissenburger & Espin, 2005). CBM accuracy-based measures for spelling

(%WSC) and overall writing (%CWS) have also been found to correlate significantly with

holistic ratings of writing, language arts grades, and standardized achievement tests in

Page 35: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

28

elementary level students (i.e., %WSC, r = .46 - .67; %CWS, r = .43 -.70) (Jewell & Malecki,

2005; Parker et al., 1991b). Accuracy-based CBM writing measures have also been used to

identify students at risk for writing difficulties (Parker et al. 1991a; Watkinson & Lee 1992).

In addition to the CBM indices of fluency and accuracy described above, the CBM

writing samples collected in the present study were assessed for handwriting quality using a

rubric. As well, each child’s classroom teacher was asked to rate the child’s writing skills and

overall language competence. Teacher ratings provided an impression of overall academic

functioning relevant to written expression and can provide information to confirm the validity of

the direct measures of writing.

1.2.4. Summary of Literature Review and Rationale for Present Study

Written expression problems are reported to occur at twice the rate of reading and math

difficulties among children with a clinical diagnosis of ADHD (Mayes & Calhoun, 2006;

2007a). Children who exhibit high levels of ADHD symptoms (including those with a clinical

diagnosis) have been reported to have difficulties with written expression related to handwriting,

compositional fluency, spelling, punctuation, grammar and vocabulary (e.g., Resta & Elliott,

1994; Re et al., 2007). The present study focused specifically on inattention symptoms because

children with elevated attention problems (e.g., sub-threshold to clinical levels) were expected

to be at high-risk for writing difficulties. For example, recent studies suggest that inattentive

behaviour is associated with underlying deficits in EF and WM among both community

(Gathercole et al., 2008; Lui & Tannock, 2007) and clinical (Chhabildas et al., 2001;

Martinussen & Tannock, 2006) samples of children. These same EF and WM processes are

specifically identified as key factors in children’s writing proficiency (Hooper et al., 2002;

Swanson & Berninger, 1996a).

Additionally, inattentive behaviour in the classroom has also been found to be predictive

Page 36: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

29

of weak reading skills among community samples of children (e.g., Rabiner & Coie, 2000).

Word-level reading skills have been found to be strongly related to transcription skills in

typically developing elementary level students (Berninger et al., 2002). Previous studies of

written expression difficulties in children have not specifically examined the role of inattentive

symptoms at the sub-threshold to clinical level and, explicitly tested and controlled for co-

existing word level reading problems (e.g., Elbert, 1993; Re et al., 2007; Resta & Elliot, 1994).

Therefore, research is needed to determine whether inattention is a risk factor for weaknesses in

written expression skills, and whether an association between inattention and writing difficulties

exists independent of co-existing reading difficulties. To address this gap in the literature, the

participants in the present study were not restricted to students with a categorical diagnosis of

ADHD. Rather, the present study examined writing skills in a community sample of children in

grades 1 to 4 with and without moderate to severe attention problems as rated by their classroom

teacher. Furthermore, the present study tested word-level reading skills and distinguished

between children with attention problems with and without coexisting word reading problems.

The Simple View of Writing (Berninger & Amtmann, 2003; Berninger, Vaughn, et al.,

2002) provided the theoretical framework for understanding and assessing written expression in

the present study of elementary grade children with attention problems because it describes the

key skills that influence the development of children’s writing proficiency. The Simple View of

Writing identifies text generation, transcription and self-regulation skills as key components that

each draw on WM resources to varying degrees at different stages of writing development

(Berninger & Amtmann, 2003; Berninger, Vaughn, et al., 2002). In the present study,

compositional measures of handwriting quality and spelling accuracy assess the transcription

component, measures of attention in the classroom reflect the self-regulation/EF/WM

component (see Thomson et al., 2005) and together, measures of compositional fluency and

Page 37: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

30

accuracy as well as teacher ratings of written expression skills assess the text generation

component.

1.3 Objectives and Hypotheses of the Present Study

(1) The primary objective of this study was to determine whether children with attention

problems with and without co-existing word level reading difficulties exhibit weaknesses in the

following written expression skills: i) direct measures of handwriting quality, spelling,

compositional fluency, and compositional accuracy as well as ii) teacher ratings of writing skills

and language arts competence.

(2) The second objective of this study was to explore whether children with co-existing

attention and reading problems exhibit greater weaknesses in written expression skills compared

to children with attention problems only.

(3) The third objective of this study was to explore whether writing outcomes (both

direct measures and teacher ratings), were related to inattentive behaviour in the classroom

independent of hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms.

Hypotheses tested:

(1) Relative to children without attention or word reading problems, children with

attention problems with and without word reading problems will exhibit significantly lower

written expression scores on the objective measures of handwriting quality, spelling,

compositional fluency, and accuracy, as well as on the teacher ratings of writing skills and

overall language arts competence.

(2) Children with attention problems and coexisting word-reading difficulties will be

significantly more impaired than children with attention problems only on all measures of

writing. This hypothesis is grounded in evidence that word reading skills are closely related to

transcription skills, and transcription difficulties have been found to negatively impact the

Page 38: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

31

fluency and quality of children’s writing (Berninger, Abbott, et al., 2002).

(3) It was predicted that poor writing outcomes would be significantly related to

attention problems independent of (i.e., controlling for) hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms.

Hyperactivity-impulsivity was predicted to be indirectly related to writing outcomes through its

correlation with symptoms of inattention; thus hyperactivity-impulsivity would not relate

significantly to writing outcomes after controlling for inattention. These hypotheses are

grounded in research documenting a relationship between inattentive behaviour and academic

impairment (Dally, 2006; Dupaul et al., 2004; Frick et al., 1991; Fuchs et al., 2006; Rabiner &

Coie, 2000; Todd et al., 2002; Warner-Rogers et al., 2000), as well as cognitive deficits in EF

and WM (Gathercole et al., 2008; Martinussen & Tannock, 2006; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996).

Page 39: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

32

CHAPTER TWO

METHOD

Page 40: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

33

2.1 Participants

Participants included 85 children in grades one through four from four schools in a rural

Ontario school board. Their ages ranged from 6 to 10 years (M = 8.2, SD=1.17), with 18

students in grade one, 26 students in grade two, 21 in grade three, and 20 in grade four. The

sample consisted of more boys (68%) than girls (32%). The average level of parental education

reported for the majority of participants was some college or higher such that 36.5% (n=31)

graduated from college or received some higher level education, 36.5% (n=31) graduated from

high-school or attended some college, 12% of parents (n=11) attended some high school or less,

and no parental education information was available for 15% (n=13).

Recruitment. Participants in this study were drawn from the baseline assessment pool of

a larger intervention study examining the effects of a training package for teachers on

instructional strategies for children with ADHD. For the larger study, each teacher identified six

students in their classrooms based on criteria outlined by the researchers (two students with

good attention skills; two average students, and two students with attention and academic

difficulties). Parents of identified students were provided with an information package regarding

the study including a consent form. Teachers completed the Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD

and Normal Behaviour (SWAN) rating scale and a brief academic rating scale for each selected

and consenting student in their class. Consenting parents also completed the parental version of

the SWAN and a brief questionnaire that was used to characterize the sample in terms of

parental education and proportion of students with a designated exceptionality (e.g., learning

disability) or diagnosis of ADHD.

Inclusionary criteria. Students were included in the larger study if they were attending a

general education classroom and their parent or guardian provided informed consent. All

Page 41: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

34

children with parental consent were also asked for their verbal assent to participate in the study.

Students were included in the present study if complete data for them was available in terms of

teachers’ assessment of attention, word reading skills, and curriculum-based measures (CBM) of

writing. As long as these conditions were met, students with identified exceptionalities were

included in the study.

Formation of subgroups. Participants were classified into subgroups based on the

presence of mild to severe attention and/or reading problems. For the present study, attention

problems were assessed with the SWAN teacher rating scale (Swanson et al., 2005). An average

inattention score was created by averaging the ratings on the 9 inattention items on the SWAN

rating scale. Children who had an average score on the inattention items that was greater than 0

were classified as inattentive. Reading problems were defined as a composite score of less than

90 on a standardized measure of word reading, the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE)

(Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 1999). Using these criteria, the sample was divided into four

groups consisting of: (a) children with typical attention and reading skills (TYP group n = 34),

(b) children with mild to severe attention problems only (AP group; n = 28), (c) children with

coexisting attention and reading problems (ARP group; n = 18), and (d) children reading

problems only (RP group; n = 5).

Group characteristics. As Table 1 illustrates, the groups were equivalent in terms of age,

nonverbal intelligence, auditory short term memory, average parental education level, and

gender distribution. By design, groups differed significantly on measures of attention and

reading; significantly lower levels of inattentive behaviour were evident for the TYP compared

to the attention problem groups (AP and ARP), and significantly lower reading scores were

evident for the ARP group than the TYP and AP groups. Descriptive statistics across groups are

presented in Table 1. The RP group was excluded from subsequent group analyses due to small

Page 42: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

35

sample size.

2.3 Procedure

Each student completed brief measures of memory, nonverbal intelligence, word reading

skills and written expression that were administered by trained research assistants in quiet rooms

in their school in random order. The CBM writing probe was administered to small groups of

two to five students from the same classroom, whereas the other measures were administered

individually.

2.4 Measures

Inattentive behaviour. Teacher ratings of inattentive behaviour using the SWAN teacher

rating scale were used to identify children with and without problems with attention (mild to

severe) in the current study. The SWAN ADHD scale (Swanson et al., 2005) includes 18

questions that correspond to the ADHD symptoms (nine for inattention and nine for

hyperactivity-impulsivity) listed in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the American

Psychiatric Association (DSM IV, 1994). Participants were rated on a scale of ‘-3 to +3’ for each

question, such that a score of zero reflects average behaviour and values above zero indicate the

presence of mild to severe attention problems (i.e., a score of +3 is rated as severe) while values

below zero indicate the absence of attention problems and good attending behaviour (i.e., a

score of -3 means above average levels of attention). Thus, unlike other ADHD symptom rating

scales, such as the SNAP IV, or the DSMIV criteria, the SWAN is designed to assess the full

range of attention skills (i.e., from strong to poor) (Swanson et al., 2005). Furthermore, a factor

analysis of the SWAN scale in a Canadian community sample identified the expected two

factors, inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity (Lui & Tannock, 2007). The teacher ratings on

the 9 inattention items were used to create an average inattention score and teacher ratings on

the 9 hyperactivity-impulsivity items were used to create an average hyperactivity-impulsivity

Page 43: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

36

score. These scores could range from +3 (“far below” average) to -3 (“far above” average

behaviour).

Parent ratings of ADHD behaviours using the SWAN parent scale were also collected.

Significant correlations between teacher and parent SWAN scores for inattention and

hyperactivity (inattention rho = .56, p <0.001, n = 69; hyperactivity rho = .42, p <0.001, n = 69)

reflect the reliability of these measures across environments and raters.

Reading skills. The Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE) (Torgesen, Wagner,

Rashotte, 1999) was used to assess word reading skills in the present study. Word recognition

skills and nonword decoding skills were assessed with the Sight Word Efficiency and Phonetic

Decoding Efficiency subtests of the TOWRE respectively. Children were required to read two

lists of words (one for real words and one for nonwords). The score for the Sight Word

Efficiency subtest is the number of real words correctly read in 45 seconds. The score for the

Phonetic Decoding Efficiency subtest is the number of nonwords correctly decoded in 45

seconds. The TOWRE is a standardized, nationally (i.e., U.S.A) normed measure of word

reading fluency and accuracy that assesses both word recognition and word decoding subskills.

Measures of reading fluency are considered a more sensitive measure of reading ability than

accuracy measures (Fuchs et al., 2002). The test developer reported that the test-retest reliability

of the TOWRE is .95 and the criterion validity is .92 with WRMT Word Identification

(Woodcock, 1998) for 8-year-olds. Raw scores were converted to age-based standard scores.

Auditory verbal short term memory. The Auditory Sentence Length (ASL) task utilized

in the present study provides a quick and reliable assessment of auditory short term memory

span. The ASL score is the number of words correctly recalled from orally presented sentences

of increasing length (see Rowe, Pollard, & Rowe, 2005).

Page 44: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

37

Non-verbal intelligence. The Matrices subtest from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale for

Intelligence (WASI) (Wechsler, 1999) measures nonverbal reasoning, abstract problem solving,

and inductive and spatial reasoning. The correlation between the Matrices subtest and WISC–III

Full Scale IQ reported by the test developer is r =.66.

Teacher ratings of writing related performance. Each student’s handwriting, spelling and

written expression skills were rated on a scale of 1 to 5 by their classroom teacher using the

Literacy Rating Scale (LRS) (see Fletcher, Tannock, & Bishop, 2001), a brief, non-standardized

measure. In addition, teachers’ overall perception of student skills and achievement-related

behaviours in language arts was assessed with the Reading and Language Arts subscale of the

Academic Competence Evaluation Scale (ACES) (Diperna & Elliot, 1999a; 1999b).These

measures provided an index of general language arts performance.

Curriculum-based measurement (CBM) of written language (Deno et al., 1982; Gansle et

al., 2002; Malecki & Jewel, 2003). For this measure, children were asked to write a story in

response to the written prompt “One day....” This prompt was both read aloud and typed at the

top of a lined piece of paper. Students were first given one minute to think of a story they

wanted to write and then three minutes to write their story. If a student stopped writing before

the three minutes were over the examiner encouraged the student to keep writing until his or her

time was up. Each child’s writing sample was scored for fluency, spelling, writing accuracy, and

handwriting quality. These measures were selected on the basis of their utility, reliability and

validity as reported in the literature (Gansle et al., 2004; Jewell & Malecki, 2005; Parker et al.,

1991a; 1991b; for review see McMaster & Espin, 2007). The scoring procedure for each of the

CBM writing measures utilized in the present study is briefly described here (see Appendix A

for a more detailed description of scoring procedures).

Page 45: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

38

Handwriting quality (HW). Quality of handwriting in the writing samples was evaluated

along two dimensions: (a) the letter legibility, and (b) the uniformity of text on the page using a

scale of one to five (1 = well below average, 5 = well above average). These two scores were

averaged to provide a score for the overall visual presentation and readability of the writing

sample. The development of this handwriting measure for the present study was informed by

empirical findings and a literature review that identify aspects of letter legibility and the overall

text as the best predictors of handwriting quality (see Graham, Struck, Santoro, & Berninger,

2006; Rosenblum et al., 2003). The scale and scoring instructions used to assess handwriting in

the present study are included in Appendix A; examples of two scored writing samples can be

found in Appendix B.

CBM measures of writing fluency. The total number of words written (TWW) during the

3-minute period, including incorrectly spelled words, was counted. TWW reflects compositional

fluency. The total number of words spelled correctly (WSC) was counted irrespective of

grammar or context, consistent with the rules reported by Gansle et al. (2002). The total number

of writing sequences (TWS), considered as all separate writing units such as words and

punctuation, was counted. The number of correct writing sequences (CWS) was calculated by

counting each successive pair of correct writing units from beginning to end of the sample. To

be counted as correct, writing sequences had to be correctly spelled and grammatically correct.

Also, words at the beginning of sentences had to be capitalized, and correct punctuation had to

be inserted at the end of a sentence. In contrast to the scores for TWW and WSC, which

specifically reflect word writing and spelling fluency, the CWS score reflects many aspects of

written expression, including fluency, spelling, grammar, capitalization, punctuation, and

spelling.

CBM measures of writing accuracy. The percentage of words spelled correctly (%WSC)

Page 46: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

39

was calculated by dividing the number of words spelled correctly (WSC) by total words written

(WSC/TWW) to provide an index of spelling accuracy. The percentage of correct writing

sequences (%CWS) was calculated by dividing the number of correct writing sequences (CWS)

by the number of total writing sequences (CWS/TWS) to provide an index of overall writing

accuracy including accuracy of spelling, use of grammar and writing conventions. The CBM

measures %WSC and %CWS have been reported to correlate strongly with holistic ratings of

writing (e.g., r = .73 for %WSC and r =.75 for %CWS, Tindal & Parker, 1989), thus providing

evidence for their validity in evaluating students’ writing.

Reliability and validity of writing measures. Inter-scorer reliability of the CBM writing

measures utilized in the present study was calculated for 30% (n = 35) of the writing samples.

These samples were scored independently by two scorers and the inter-scorer reliability for each

measure was computed by dividing the lower of the two scores by the higher score for each

double scored sample (Powell-Smith, & Shinn, 2004; Weissenburger & Espin, 2005). The

average inter-scorer reliability was 98.4% for TWW, 97.0% for WSC, 97.5% for CWS, 98.7%

for TWS and 92.7% for handwriting. The scoring procedures utilized and examples of scored

writing samples are presented in Appendix A. The CBM writing scores also correlated

significantly with teacher ratings of student writing skills and overall language arts performance

which provides further evidence of their validity in assessing written expression. The correlation

coefficients between CBM writing scores and teacher ratings of writing are presented in

Appendix B. The CBM scores for handwriting (HW) and overall writing accuracy (%CWS)

correlated best with teacher ratings (i.e., HW, rs= .49 to .59, p <.001; %CWS, rs = .40 to .54, p

<.001).

Page 47: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

40

2.5 Statistical Analyses

The shape of the distribution for each variable was assessed statistically with the

Kolmogorov-Smirnov (total sample) and Shapiro-Wilk (across groups, n < 50) tests of

normality, and graphically by inspection of histogram and normal Q-plots. A square root

transformation was applied to WSC and CWS in SPSS to attain a normal distribution of scores

for these variables across groups and total sample. Although the distributions for %WSC and

%CWS were normally distributed across the total sample, these variables deviated from the

normal curve across groups and could not be transformed to produce a normal distribution

across groups. Consequently nonparametric tests were employed to assess group differences for

these variables.

Hypotheses 1 & 2: To explore group differences in the handwriting quality and CBM

fluency measures (Hypotheses 1 and 2), two multivariate ANOVAs were conducted. The CBM

measures for handwriting quality and compositional fluency variables (HW, TWW, WSC, and

CWS) were entered as dependent variables into one MANOVA with group as the independent

variable. Follow-up univariate ANOVAs were conducted if the overall MANOVA was

significant. Pillai’s trace statistic was used for interpretation of multivariate analyses as it is

more robust to assumption violations than other statistics (Olson, 1976). Similarly the

Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons was used for post hoc pair-wise comparisons

for all follow-up univariate analyses to control for type I error. A second MANOVA was

conducted to examine group differences on the teacher ratings of handwriting, spelling, and

written expression skills and teacher perceptions of overall language arts competence (ACES

subscale). Non-parametric Kruskall-Wallis and follow up Mann Whitney U tests were

conducted to compare groups on percentage scores for spelling accuracy (%WSC) and overall

writing accuracy (%CWS).

Page 48: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

41

Hypothesis 3: The hypothesis that inattentive behaviour is related to writing outcomes

independent of its relationship with hyperactivity was tested using partial correlational analyses.

The relationship between attention and writing outcomes controlling for hyperactivity and age

was compared to the relationship between hyperactivity and writing outcomes controlling for

inattention and age.

Page 49: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

42

CHAPTER THREE

RESULTS

Page 50: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

43

3.1 Group Differences in Written Expression

CBM writing scores. A multivariate analysis of variance was conducted to examine

group differences (TYP, AP, and ARP) on handwriting quality and the CBM fluency variables

(TWW, WSC, CWS). There was a significant overall effect of group, Pillai’s trace = .36,

F(8,150) = .41, p<.001, η2 = .18. Follow up univariate ANOVAs revealed significant

differences between the groups on each variable with effect sizes ranging from η2 = .20 for total

words written (TWW), to η2 = .27 for correct writing sequences (CWS). Table 2 presents the

univariate ANOVA results and the means and standard deviations for each measure by subgroup.

Pair-wise comparisons (Bonferroni adjusted) indicated that the AP and ARP subgroups scored

significantly lower than the TYP group on all of the writing measures. There were no

significant differences between the two attention groups (AP vs. ARP) on measures of

handwriting quality (HW), or on any of the fluency measures (TWW, WSC, CWS).

Nonparametric analyses were also conducted for the accuracy scores for spelling and

overall writing (%WSC and %CWS) due to the atypical distribution of these variables within

each subgroup. Results of the Kruskal-Wallis k-independent samples test revealed a significant

difference between subgroups on both %WSC, χ2(2, N = 80) = 22.64, p <.001, and %CWS,

χ2(2, N = 80) = 19.48, p <.001. Mann-Whitney U tests were used to examine differences

between the subgroups. Three planned follow-up comparisons were conducted and a Bonferroni

correction was applied for the three comparisons (i.e., a significance level of p = .05/3 = .0167)

to minimize Type 1 error (see Field, 2005). Results of the Mann Whitney U comparisons

indicated that the TYP group scored significantly higher than the AP and ARP groups on %WSC

(TYP vs. AP, U = 253.50, p = .002; TYP vs. ARP; U = 76.00, p <.001) and %CWS (TYP vs. AP,

U = 297.00, p = .011; TYP vs. ARP, U = 79.00, p <.001). The Mann Whitney U comparisons

Page 51: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

44

for the AP and ARP groups were not significant for %WSC, (U = 166.50, p = .05) or %CWS (U

= 169.00, p = .06). See Table 2 for means and standard deviations.

Teacher ratings of writing. A multivariate analysis of variance was conducted to

examine group differences (TYP, AP, and ARP) on the teacher ratings of writing (handwriting,

spelling, and written expression) and overall language arts competence. A significant difference

was found between the groups, Pillai's Trace = .71, F(8,146) = 10.12, with a moderate effect

size of η2 = .36. Follow up post hoc univariate ANOVAs indicated that the subgroups differed

on teacher ratings of handwriting, spelling, written expression, and overall language arts

competence (see Table 3). Effect sizes were moderate to strong, ranging from η2 = .47 for

handwriting skills to η2 = .56 for written expression skills. Post-hoc Bonferroni comparisons

indicated that the AP and ARP subgroups scored significantly lower than the TYP group on the

teacher ratings of handwriting, spelling, written expression, and overall language arts score (see

Table 3). Children with coexisting attention and reading problems (ARP group) scored

significantly lower than children with attention problems only (AP group) on teachers’ ratings of

spelling and written expression skills as well as teachers’ perceptions of overall language arts

competence. The two attention problem groups (AP and ARP) did not differ significantly on

teacher ratings of handwriting skills.

3.2 Partial Correlation Analyses: ADHD Symptom Dimensions and Writing Outcomes

Zero-order correlation analyses revealed that the average scores for Inattention (SWAN-

ATT) and Hyperactivity (SWAN-HYP) were highly correlated, r = .82, p < .001 with each other.

Both the Inattention average score and the Hyperactivity average score were significantly

correlated with all measures of writing (see Table 4). After controlling for age and

Hyperactivity, Inattention was still significantly related to all writing outcomes with the

Page 52: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

45

exception of the direct measure of handwriting quality. After controlling for Inattention and age,

Hyperactivity was no longer significantly correlated to writing outcomes, with the exception of

the direct measure of Handwriting quality, r = .29, p <.05 (see Table 4). The lack of association

between Hyperactivity and most writing outcomes after controlling for Inattention and age

should be interpreted cautiously due to the high correlation between Inattention and

Hyperactivity and probable suppression effects. Specifically, after the variance associated with

Inattention and age is removed from the relationship between Hyperactivity and writing, the

remaining ‘residual’ for Hyperactivity may not actually represent the construct of Hyperactivity

very effectively because too much shared variance between Inattention and Hyperactivity has

been removed.

3.3 Supplementary Analyses

Gender Differences. Several studies have reported that girls outperform boys on

measures of compositional fluency (Jewell & Malecki, 2005; Malecki & Jewell, 2003) and

handwriting and orthographic automaticity (Berninger & Fuller, 1992; Berninger, Nielsen,

Abbott, Wijsman, & Raskind, 2008b). Although the subgroups did not differ in gender

distribution (see Table 1), an additional exploratory analysis was conducted to assess whether

boys and girls performed similarly within each group. Thus, gender differences in writing

outcomes were examined within groups using Mann Whitney U tests for the accuracy measures

(%WSC and %CWS) and t-tests for all other measures. Results of the within group tests

indicated that boys and girls were equivalent on most measures, however girls did perform

significantly better than boys on two measures: (a) handwriting quality within the TYP group,

t(32) = -3.32, p<.01, and within the AP group, t(26) = -3.79, p <.01; and (b) teacher ratings of

written expression skills in the TYP group, t(32) = .28, p <.01. No significant gender differences

were found for direct measures of spelling, compositional fluency, and accuracy or for teacher

Page 53: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

46

ratings of handwriting, spelling and language arts competence within any groups. Due to

insufficient sample size for girls across groups, gender was not included as a factor in group

analyses. The means and standard deviations by gender and group as are reported in Appendix C

for the direct measures of writing and Appendix D for the teacher ratings of writing.

Group differences were examined separately for boys and girls for the two measures in

which girls outperformed boys (i.e., handwriting quality and teacher ratings of written

expression). Analysis of variance was used to examine group differences for the boys. Due to

small sample sizes of girls across groups, nonparametric Kruskall-Wallis tests with Mann-

Whitney U post hoc comparisons were conducted for girls. All post hoc comparisons were

Bonferroni adjusted.

Consistent with the overall group results, a significant group effect was found for

handwriting quality when examined among boys only, F(2,53) = 9.43, p <.001, η2=.26. Follow-

up comparisons between the groups indicated that the AP group scored significantly lower than

the TYP group on the direct measure of handwriting quality; the ARP group also scored lower

than the TYP group but the difference was not statistically significant. As with the total sample,

there was no significant difference between the AP group and the ARP group for handwriting

quality.

A significant group effect was also found among boys for teacher ratings of written

expression, F(2,53)= 27.14, p <.001, η2 =.51. Consistent with the results for the total sample,

follow up comparisons indicated that both attention problem subgroups (AP and ARP) received

significantly lower teacher ratings than the typical (TYP) group, and the ARP group scored

significantly lower than the AP group.

For the girls, a significant group effect was also found among girls for handwriting

quality, χ2 (2, N = 24) = 9.21, p = .01, η2 = .40. Follow up Mann-Whitney U comparisons

Page 54: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

47

indicated that the ARP group received significantly lower scores for handwriting quality than the

TYP group (U = 4.00, p = .011). Although the AP group also scored lower than the TYP group

on handwriting legibility, the difference was not significant. There was no significant difference

between the AP and ARP groups. These findings are similar to those reported for the boys and

the total sample.

A significant group effect was also found among girls for teacher ratings of written

expression, χ2 (2, N = 24) = 17.04, p = .003, η2=.74. Again, consistent with the results for the

total sample, follow up group comparisons indicated that both attention problem subgroups (AP

and ARP) received significantly lower teacher ratings of written expression skills than the TYP

group (i.e., TYP vs. AP, U = 2.00, p <.001; TYP vs. ARP, U = 1.00, p = .003). However, no

significant differences were found between the attention problem subgroups (AP and ARP) for

girls (U = 11, p = .51). This finding may be due to the small number of girls within the ARP

group.

In summary, the results of this study reveal that inattentive behaviour is significantly

related to written expression outcomes independent of word reading skills and hyperactivity-

impulsivity. Findings for teacher ratings of handwriting, spelling, and written expression were

generally consistent with the outcomes for direct measures (CBM) of writing. Specific areas of

difficulty that were significantly related to attention problems include handwriting, spelling,

compositional fluency accuracy and quality. Although, there were no significant differences

between the attention groups (AP and ARP groups) on direct CBM measures of writing, the

group with coexisting attention and reading problems (ARP ) had significantly lower teacher

ratings of spelling and written expression skills as well as overall language arts competence than

the comparison and AR groups. In conclusion, results of the present study suggest that

Inattentive behaviour is directly related to all writing outcomes (see Table 4) with the exception

Page 55: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

48

of the direct measure of handwriting quality. Thus the results of the partial correlation analyses

suggest that handwriting quality is indirectly related to inattention through hyperactivity-

impulsivity.

Page 56: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

49

CHAPTER FOUR

D IS C U S S IO N

Page 57: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

50

This chapter is divided into four main sections. First, the specific findings are

summarized in terms of text generation and transcription level outcomes and discussed in

relation to previous research and possible underlying mechanisms of effect. The practical and

theoretical implications of these findings are discussed in the second section. The limitations of

the present study and directions for future research are discussed in the third section. The

conclusion is presented in the final section.

4.1 Summary and Discussion of Results

The purpose of this study was to examine the influence of inattention on children’s

writing skills and to assess whether attention problems were associated with weak writing skills

independent of reading difficulties and symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity. Three

hypotheses were tested. First, children with attention problems were expected have significantly

poorer writing outcomes independent of word-reading skills than those without attention and

reading problems. Second, children with coexisting attention and word-reading problems were

expected to be significantly more impaired than children with attention problems only. Third,

poor writing outcomes were expected to be significantly correlated to attention problems

independent of (i.e., controlling for) hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms. Three key findings

emerged from the results. First, children with attention problems with and without coexisting

word reading difficulties performed significantly poorer on all measures of written expression

than children in the typical group. Second, children with coexisting attention and word-reading

problems were only moderately more impaired than children with attention problems only.

Finally, attention problems were significantly associated with written expression skills after

controlling for hyperactivity-impulsivity. Hyperactivity was only significantly associated with

handwriting quality after controlling for inattention. The lack of significant correlations between

Page 58: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

51

Hyperactivity and most writing outcomes after controlling for Inattention and age is likely due

to a suppressor effect thus should be interpreted cautiously.

The results indicated that writing difficulties associated with attention problems tend to

be pervasive or otherwise nonspecific, such that multiple aspects of writing were impaired

across both transcription and text generation levels including measures of handwriting quality,

compositional fluency, spelling and compositional accuracy. More specifically, relative to

typically developing children without attention or reading problems, children with attention

problems (with or without reading problems) had lower quality handwriting, produced written

narratives that were shorter (lower TWW scores), with a higher rate of errors in spelling (lower

%WSC scores) and compositional accuracy (lower % CWS scores). Children with attention

problems (with or without reading problems) also received lower teacher ratings for writing

skills in handwriting, spelling and written expression as well as overall language arts

competence. The specific findings at the levels of (a) transcription and then (b) text generation

and their relation to previous research are discussed more thoroughly in the next section.

4.1.1 Transcription Level Outcomes

Spelling. Consistent with the first hypothesis, children with attention problems with and

without word reading problems scored significantly lower on the direct measure of spelling

accuracy (%WSC). They also wrote significantly fewer correctly spelled words (WSC), and

received significantly lower teacher ratings for spelling skills than peers without attention or

reading problems. These results are consistent with previous findings that children with ADHD

write stories with more spelling errors (Mathers, 2006; Re et al., 2007; Resta & Elliot, 1994).

Although no statistically significant differences were found between the attention problem

subgroups (AP and ARP) on the direct CBM measures of spelling, children with coexisting

attention and reading problems tended to receive lower scores than the attention problem only

Page 59: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

52

subgroup. This tendency was supported by significantly lower teacher ratings of spelling for

children with coexisting attention and reading problems than for those with attention problems

only. Thus, the second hypothesis that coexisting attention and word level reading problems

would be associated with significantly poorer spelling scores than attention problems only was

partially confirmed. Furthermore, inattention was significantly related to poor spelling

outcomes after controlling for hyperactivity-impulsivity as predicted by the third hypothesis,

The finding that children with attention deficits demonstrate significant spelling

problems in the written compositions despite normal word reading skills may be partially

explained by the results of a recent study that analyzed the transcription skills of Hebrew-

speaking children with ADHD (Adi-japha et al., 2007). Specifically, boys with ADHD, who had

normal reading skills, were found to display a unique pattern of spelling errors known as

graphemic buffer errors (e.g., letter insertions, substitutions, transpositions and omissions).

Graphemic buffer errors are thought to be due to attentional problems, whereas phonological

spelling errors are related to linguistic deficits (Adi-japha et al., 2007). According to this

explanation children with coexisting attention and linguistic (i.e., word reading) deficits would

be expected to produce more phonological and graphemic spelling errors relative to children

with either attention or wording reading problems alone. Although the present study did not

examine the types of spelling errors the children made in their compositions, children with co-

existing attention and reading problems had a slightly higher rate of spelling errors and

significantly lower teacher ratings of spelling skills than children with attention problems only.

Considering the findings of Adi-japha et al. it may be possible that some of the spelling errors

displayed by children with attention problems only in the present study were a product of

“attention-related grapheme buffer errors” (i.e., difficulty correctly forming written letters)

rather than “phonologically-related spelling errors”. Furthermore, children with attention

Page 60: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

53

problems and normal word-reading skills may struggle more with spelling when they are coping

with the demands of handwriting together with generating text due to the added strain on

working memory.

Handwriting. In the present study, handwriting quality was evaluated in terms of letter

legibility and overall uniformity of written text on the page. Handwriting skills in general were

also rated by each student’s classroom teacher. Compared to the typical group, the attention

problem subgroups (both AP and ARP) received significantly lower scores for handwriting (i.e.,

objective measures and teacher ratings) with no significant differences between the AP and ARP

groups. These results indicate that poor handwriting quality is associated with attention

problems independent of word reading difficulties. The present findings are consistent with

previous research identifying problems with handwriting in children with ADHD (Adi-japha et

al., 2007; see Racine et al., 2008 for a review, Schoemaker et al., 2005). For example, in a recent

review of research concerning handwriting in children with ADHD, Racine et al. concluded

children with ADHD have impaired handwriting characterized by poor legibility and

inappropriate speed of execution compared to typical children.

Schoemaker et al. (2005) examined whether the handwriting difficulties associated with

ADHD might be a result of impaired motor control processes, (i.e. motor planning and

parameter setting). They had children with and without ADHD copy figures of increasing

complexity under increasing accuracy levels on a digitizer. Although the motor planning

performance of children with ADHD appeared normal, Schoemaker et al. found evidence for a

specific deficit in parameter setting as well as a general fine motor control problem.

Specifically, these children were unable to successfully adapt task performance when more

accurate strokes were required (i.e., parameter setting). They also showed slower, more

inaccurate goal-directed strokes with higher axial pen force across all graphic tasks (i.e., general

Page 61: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

54

fine motor control). Poor inhibitory control (i.e., attention/EF weaknesses) and developmental

delays related to fine motor control were proposed as possible explanations for the graphic

output deficits displayed by children with ADHD (Schoemaker et al). None of the studies

reviewed by Racine et al. (2008), nor Shoemaker et al., specifically explored whether the

handwriting difficulties associated with ADHD were more associated with the inattentive or

hyperactive-impulsive symptom dimension of ADHD or controlled for coexisting reading

difficulties.

In the present study, the results of zero-order correlations initially revealed that poor

handwriting was strongly correlated with both inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity (i.e., r

=.60 and .61, p <.001); however inattention was no longer significantly related to handwriting

when the effects hyperactivity-impulsivity were controlled. These results suggest that

handwriting quality may be indirectly related to inattention through symptoms of hyperactivity-

impulsivity. The finding that poor handwriting quality is related to hyperactivity and not

inattention is inconsistent with a recent study that reported that that inattention was more related

to weaknesses in fine motor skills (as indexed by a parent rating scale) than hyperactivity (Fliers

et al., 2008). This inconsistency in findings may be because the present study directly assessed

handwriting quality, whereas Fliers et al. relied on a rating scale that addressed other skills. The

results of partial correlation analyses in the present study should be interpreted cautiously

considering that the zero-order correlations indicated that inattention and hyperactivity were

highly correlated with each other, and with each of the writing outcomes. In summary, the poor

handwriting quality demonstrated by children with attention problems in this study, and in

previous studies of ADHD, may be a result of developmental delays or deficits in a range of

processes including fine motor control, orthographic coding, or EF skills (see Schoemaker et al.,

2005; and Racine et al., 2008 for review).

Page 62: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

55

Few studies have examined the relationship between handwriting quality and

composition skills among children with attention problems. Instead, previous studies have

generally focussed on the relationship between compositional quality and handwriting fluency

as opposed to handwriting quality or legibility (e.g., Graham et al., 1997). For example, Re et al.

(2007) examined handwriting fluency (i.e., a timed copy task) in youth with and without ADHD

symptoms and found no significant differences on the timed copy task. However, the two groups

differed on measures of writing fluency and accuracy. Research is needed to examine whether

difficulties producing legible handwriting may influence other aspects of writing such as

accuracy and handwriting fluency (e.g. Jones & Christensen, 1999), and may therefore account

for some of the weaknesses in written spelling, compositional fluency, and accuracy

demonstrated by children with attention problems in the present study. Furthermore,

handwriting legibility and spelling can affect a teacher’s judgement about overall writing quality

and a students’ ability to compose (e.g., Chase, 1986; Graham, 1999; Rosenblum et al., 2003).

Research is needed to examine the relationship between standardized assessments of

handwriting quality and handwriting fluency and compositional quality in children with

attention problems; such research may help identify key areas to target for intervention.

4.1.2 Text Generation Level Outcomes

Compositional fluency and quality. In the present study, children with attention

problems, with and without word reading problems (ARP and AP groups), wrote stories with

significantly fewer words, and significantly more errors (including incorrect grammar and

punctuation) than children in the typical group. Teacher ratings of written expression skills were

also significantly lower for both attention problem groups (AP and ARP) than for the typical

group (TYP). Children with coexisting attention and reading problems received significantly

lower teacher ratings of written expression skills compared to children with attention problems

Page 63: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

56

only. Although there was a tendency for children with coexisting attention and reading problems

to have lower scores for compositional accuracy than the AP subgroup, these two subgroups did

not differ significantly on direct measures of compositional fluency or accuracy. Additionally,

attention problems were found to be significantly related to all measures reflecting text

generation skills (i.e., direct measures of compositional fluency, quality, accuracy and teacher

ratings of written expression skills) after controlling for the effects of age and hyperactivity-

impulsivity through partial correlation analyses. Thus, the results of the present study suggest

that poor written expression outcomes are directly related to attention problems independent of

word-reading skills and hyperactivity-impulsivity.

These results are generally consistent with previous findings that children with ADHD

write stories with fewer words and more errors (Re et al., 2007; Resta & Elliot, 1994); the

present study however, provides further evidence concerning the relative influence of word-

reading skills and ADHD symptom dimension. These results are also generally consistent with

recent findings regarding the prevalence and co-morbidity of learning disabilities in written

expression (defined using IQ-discrepancy) among children with ADHD (Mayes & Calhoun,

2007a). Mayes and Calhoun demonstrated that written expression disabilities frequently occur in

students with ADHD without co-morbid reading disabilities. In a sample of 242 children with

ADHD they found that almost 50% of the children demonstrated significant impairment in

written expression alone; whereas only 14% presented with both reading and written expression

disabilities, and 4% were identified with RD alone. Thus, even with average or better reading

skills, children with attention-deficits appear to be at significant risk for written expression

disabilities. Given the apparent prevalence of writing weaknesses in children with attention

deficits, it may be beneficial to assess the written expression skills of students who exhibit

inattentive behaviour and target this group for special instruction.

Page 64: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

57

4.1.3 Language Arts Competence.

In present study the attention problem groups, both with and without reading difficulties

(ARP and AP) received significantly lower scores for overall language arts competence

compared to the typical group (TYP). Children with co-existing attention and reading problems

scored significantly lower than children attention problems only. These results indicated that

children with attention problems experience significant academic risk in overall language arts

even when they have average or better word-reading skills. Poor overall language arts scores are

likely a consequence weaknesses in written language skills, or perhaps higher-level reading

skills (e.g., reading comprehension).

4.1.4 General Summary and Discussion

Importantly, the present findings indicate that even children who exhibit mild to

moderate (subclinical) levels of inattention, and normal word-reading skills demonstrate

significantly poorer written expression skills than children with normal levels of attention and

word-reading skills. It may be that children with attention problems are particularly impaired

when a timed measure of composing is used because it may place extra demands on their

cognitive resources. However, it bears consideration that the group differences and effect sizes

found for teacher ratings of writing skills were greater than those found for the timed measure of

writing, perhaps because most classroom based writing activities also tend involve time

constraints.

Furthermore, given that handwriting skills contribute to compositional skills (e.g.,

Graham et al., 1997; Graham et al., 2000; see Rosenblum et al., 2003 for review), the

handwriting difficulties exhibited by children with attention problems may negatively impact

overall written expression proficiency and academic performance of children with attention

problems. Poor quality handwriting has also been shown to negatively affect teacher’s

Page 65: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

58

perceptions of the content of written work (e.g., Graham & Perin, 2007). In general, difficulties

with handwriting have been reported to cause a significant degree of impairment in academic

life for children (Graham et al., 1997; see Rosenblum et al., 2003 for review). Future research is

needed to clarify the mechanisms through which inattentive behaviour is related to poor written

expression and identify target areas for intervention and accommodation.

4.2 Implications for Research, Theory and Practice

Implications for Research. The hypothetical inattention-writing model shown in Figure 2

attempts to represent and integrate the findings of the present thesis study together with the

literature concerning written expression development, and the literature on writing difficulties

and ADHD that was reviewed for this thesis. The purpose of the model in Figure 2 is to

describe how inattention may be related to poor writing outcomes, and to pose questions for

future research. Also, the figure identifies key areas to consider when assessing written

expression difficulties in children with attention problems. The relationships between attention

problems and writing outcomes illustrated in Figure 2 are not causal or exhaustive. The key

point demonstrated by this diagram is that the relationship between inattention and written

expression difficulties is likely multi-faceted and more research is needed to clarify this

relationship.

For example, as shown in Figure 2, inattention may be indirectly related to poor

transcription level outcomes either through coexisting reading weaknesses or coexisting

symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity. Alternatively, inattention may directly related to poor

writing outcomes independent of coexisting word reading weaknesses and symptoms of

hyperactivity-impulsivity. Given that attention problems are often highly correlated with

hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms (e.g., A.P.A. DSM-IV, 2000) and poor reading skills (e.g.,

Dally, 2006), bidirectional arrows are shown in Figure 2 joining attention problems to

Page 66: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

59

hyperactivity-impulsivity and reading weaknesses. In consideration of the evidence that word

reading ability is related to transcription skills, particularly spelling (e.g., Berninger, Abbott, et

al., 2002), Figure 2 also shows an arrow connecting reading weaknesses directly to written

spelling and transcription level difficulties to indicate that poor spelling may be due to

coexisting word reading weaknesses. Further research is needed to isolate and examine how

word reading weaknesses and hyperactivity-impulsivity influence written expression in children

with and without attention problems.

The results of the present study suggest that poor handwriting quality may be directly

related to hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms independent of inattention, and thus Figure 2 also

shows an arrow connecting hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms directly to handwriting quality

and transcription level difficulties. However, Figure 2 also shows an arrow connecting attention

problems directly to handwriting quality in consideration of conflicting evidence which suggests

that symptoms of inattention and not hyperactivity-impulsivity are related to poor fine motor

control (e.g., Fliers et al., 2008; Pitcher et al., 2002), which is important for handwriting (e.g.,

Feder & Majnemer, 2007). The arrow shown connecting handwriting quality to written spelling

represents the untested hypothesis that poorly formed letters or otherwise illegible writing (e.g.,

poor letter spacing and orientation) may impair the written spelling accuracy of children with

attention problems with or without underlying spelling difficulties. Further research is needed to

clarify the relationship between inattention and handwriting quality and examine the extent to

which handwriting quality contributes to written spelling and text generation skills.

Figure 2 shows a direct connection between attention problems and transcription level

difficulties (e.g., difficulty producing correct letter forms, or poor spelling), as well as direct

connection between attention problems and text generation level difficulties such as poor

compositional fluency (i.e., writing little under time constraints), or poor compositional

Page 67: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

60

accuracy (i.e., producing careless errors in written work). However, it is unknown whether

attention problems are related to text generation level difficulties (i.e., poor compositional

fluency and composition accuracy) because of transcription level difficulties, or in addition to

transcription level difficulties. Research is needed to examine the effects of transcription skill

deficits on writing fluency and quality in children with attention problems. Such research has

important implications for preventing and remediating written expression difficulties among

children with attention problems.

Finally, weaknesses in attention, graphomotor skills, and processing speed have been

reported to be associated with lower levels of academic achievement of children with ADHD

(Mayes & Calhoun, 2007b). It may be that writing tasks require more time and effort for

children who experience difficulties at the level of transcription (handwriting or written

spelling) and/or problems with compositional fluency and consequently reduce the amount of

written work completed relative to other children. This is reflected in Figure 2 by arrows

connecting transcription level difficulties and compositional fluency to ‘time and effort to

complete work’. Studies are needed to assess the contribution of writing proficiency to

children's productivity and overall academic performance, particularly among children with

attention problems. It is hoped that the Inattention-Writing model presented in Figure 2 may be

useful to educators and researchers interested in understanding and assessing written expression

difficulties in children with attention problems.

Implications for Theory and Practice. As predicted, based on the Simple View of

Writing (e.g., Berninger & Amtmann, 2003), children in the present study who demonstrated

poor attention control in the classroom demonstrated pervasive writing difficulties. The Simple

View of Writing postulates that written expression performance at the level of text generation

(see Figure 1) relies on high levels of WM, EF (attention-control), and transcription skills. Thus

Page 68: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

61

weaknesses in attention control, or any of the other components, may impair written expression

at the text generation level. Although the Simple View of Writing attributes an important role to

attention-control in children’s writing development (Berninger & Amtmann, 2003), few studies

have examined the role of inattention in children’s writing with English-speaking children. On

the other hand, inattention, but not hyperactivity, was found to be related to writing outcomes in

one study that examined reading and writing in a sample of children with dyslexia (Thomson et

al., 2005). The results of the present study therefore make a novel contribution to the literature

on children’s writing development by providing support for the role of attention in writing

theory in a community-based sample of children.

The results of the present study also have important clinical and educational implications

concerning the identification, prevention, remediation, and possible aetiology of written

expression difficulties in children with and without poor word-reading skills. Previous research

suggests that significant written expression difficulties frequently go “unrecognized and

untreated” (e.g., Berninger, Mizokawa, et al., 1991; Berninger et al., 2008a), and that the

prevalence of writing disabilities is particularly high in students with ADHD (Mayes et al.,

2005; Mayes & Calhoun, 2006; 2007a). The present results suggest that inattentive behaviour,

even at subclinical levels, may be an important predictor of written expression proficiency.

Thus, to improve the rate of identification, prevention and remediation of written expression

difficulties, it may be helpful to identify primary grade children with attention problems and

screen them for written expression difficulties. In order to promote and support the academic

success of students with attention problems and writing weaknesses, it may be helpful to

develop targeted instructional strategies for this group to prevent further deterioration of written

expression skills and academic impairments.

In view of the weaknesses demonstrated by children with attention problems in the

Page 69: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

62

present study on measures of transcription, compositional fluency and accuracy, these students

may benefit from targeted instruction to improve accuracy and automaticity of transcription

skills including handwriting and/or keyboarding skills (Berninger et al., 1997; Berninger,

Vaughn, et al., 2002; Graham, 1999; Graham et al., 1997; Jones & Christensen, 1999).

Instruction targeted to the accuracy and automaticity of transcription skills has been reported to

improve composition fluency and quality (Berninger et al., 1997; Berninger, Vaughn, et al.,

2002; Graham, 1999; Graham et al., 1997; Graham et al., 2000; Jones & Christensen, 1999).

Additionally, adaptations and accommodations that reduce or remove the mechanical constraints

of written expression may help to free up limited attentional and cognitive resources (i.e., EF

and WM) and facilitate text generation at the word, sentence and discourse level of composing

(Berninger et al., 1997; Berninger, Vaughn, et al., 2002; Cobb et al., 1992; Graham et al., 1997,

2000).

For example, students who are able to dictate their texts, or use technological assistance

(e.g.., word processor), may be able to focus more on higher level writing processes than

transcription (Graham & Harris, 2003). Furthermore, prior research has shown that students

with LD produce significantly longer and better quality stories when they dictate stories rather

than write them by hand (Graham, 1990; MacArthur, & Graham, & Scarvold, 1987). However,

children with ADHD have been found to exhibit significant deficits in producing coherent oral

narratives (e.g., Flory et al.,2006; Purvis & Tannock, 1997; Tannock, Purvis, & Schachar, 1993),

thus this strategy alone may improve their fluency, but not necessarily the quality of their

writing. Additional support or training related to producing cohesive narratives may be needed.

Recently, Re et al. (2008) demonstrated that a guided facilitation intervention for

composing a letter produced significant improvements in the length and quality of letters written

by children with and without ADHD symptoms. However, the quality of letter writing by

Page 70: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

63

children with ADHD symptoms was still significantly poorer than children without ADHD

symptoms after the guided facilitation training (Re et al., 2008). Some additional tools and

strategies that may be helpful include computer software for transcribing voice-to-text, reading

back typed text, and correcting spelling. Research, however, is needed to assess the impact of

these tools with children with attention problems (Berninger & Amtmann, 2003).

In addition, children with attention problems may benefit from direct instruction in

metacognitive strategies, staging of writing, self-monitoring, and mnemonic devices for

regulating the writing process and reducing memory demands during writing (e.g., De LaPaz,

2001; Graham & Harris, 2000, 2003; Graham, Harris, & Mason, 2005; Lienemann & Reid,

2008; Reid & Lienemann, 2006, Re et al., 2008). The effectiveness of self regulated strategy

development (SRSD) (Graham & Harris, 2000) among students with ADHD has been

demonstrated in small-scale intervention studies with moderate success (De LaPaz, 2001;

Lienemann & Reid, 2008; Reid & Lienemann, 2006). The efficacy of SRSD for improving

written expression of students with LD is widely documented (Graham & Harris, 2003; Graham

et al., 2005). Attention training paired with writing instruction has been reported to improve the

composition skills of children with dyslexia (Chenault et al., 2006). Research is needed to

identify instructional strategies that support the written expression development of children with

attention problems.

Finally, this study also has practical implications for assessment in both research and

education. The findings of the present study provide further evidence that CBM assessment of

written language is a valid measure of students' written expression performance (e.g.,

Weissenburger & Espin, 2005). Furthermore, the present findings also provide more evidence

for the validity of CBM writing measures to detect differences in writing proficiency among at

risk groups of children (e.g. Parker et al., 1991a). Also, teachers may be able to more easily

Page 71: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

64

interpret and make use of the present findings because the present study utilized CBM measures

of writing that can be easily administered and interpreted by teachers. Thus an advantage of the

present study is that it utilized measures with ecological validity and obvious educational utility.

4.3 Limitations and Directions for Future Research

Findings of the present study must be considered in light of the following limitations.

The first limitation of the study is related to the handwriting assessment. The direct measure of

handwriting was assessed solely by analyzing the visual presentation of the text in the writing

samples. Unfortunately, it was not possible to directly assess handwriting and its various

dimensions of independent of the composing process. Independent, objective measures of

handwriting subskills, such as orthographic coding and copy fluency, would have been useful to

assess in order to potentially isolate, understand, or rule-out specific skill deficits underlying

handwriting difficulties and attention problems. For example, difficulties with handwriting may

arise from a variety of different underlying deficits including difficulty coordinating the overall

writing process due to limited cognitive resources, poor fine motor control, poor orthographic

coding, poor executive control or a combination of such deficits (Feder & Majnemer, 2007).

Along a similar vein, future studies should distinguish between phonological spelling errors and

graphemic buffer errors to determine whether the transcription difficulties of children with

attention problems are related to linguistic errors, attention problems or both (Adi Japha et al.,

2007; Re et al., 2007).

A second limitation of this study was that due to insufficient sample size across groups

(i.e., n = 5 for Reading Problems only group) and gender (i.e., n = 4 for girls vs. n = 13 for boys

for ARP) it was not possible to: (a) assess the interaction between group and gender and (b)

assess the characteristics of the reading problem only group (RP) relative to the attention

problem groups (i.e., AP and ARP). A recent study examined gender differences and EF in

Page 72: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

65

writing among children with dyslexia and reported that boys were significantly more impaired

than girls with respect to both EF (i.e., sustained attention/switching tasks) and writing

(Berninger et al., 2008b). Further research is needed to examine gender differences in written

expression within samples of children with attention deficits or ADHD.

An additional limitation of this study was that only a single writing sample was collected

from each child, and the writing samples were quite short (about 3 to 5 sentences on average).

The brevity of the writing samples provided limited material to assess, and thus may have

reduced the sensitivity of the measures to subtle group differences, for example between the AP

and ARP groups. Furthermore, the use of more sophisticated writing measures (e.g., sentences,

content, cohesion, overall writing quality, etc.) was impractical given length of the samples

collected. It should be noted however, that CBM measures of writing have been found to

effectively capture students academic functioning in written expression skills (see review by

McMaster & Espin, 2007). Future research should consider obtaining multiple and/or longer

writing samples in different genres to provide for a more in-depth assessment of writing ability

in children with attention problems using a range of tasks (e.g., see Schoonen, 2005).

Furthermore, children with clinical levels of inattention (i.e., ADHD) often exhibit

weaknesses in oral language skills related to EF deficits and difficulties producing coherent oral

narratives (Flory et al., 2006; Purvis & Tannock, 1997; Tannock et al., 1993). Hence, it would be

advisable to investigate EF-related language factors such as planning, goals, organization,

cohesion, problem solving, and idea generation in writing among older students with attention

problems.

Finally, it bears consideration that the students with attention problems were selected for

the larger intervention study because they exhibited attention and academic problems; however,

they did not have to exhibit writing difficulties. Also, for the present study, the relations between

Page 73: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

66

reading and attention and writing within this sample were of particular interest, not just whether

they had deficits. Furthermore, the subjective teacher ratings of attention (SWAN scale) used to

differentiate the groups in the present study could have been confounded by students’ academic

performance (or vice versa), thereby contributing to the strong relationship between attention

scores and ratings of writing skills and language arts competence. However, the validity of these

measures in the present study was supported because teacher ratings of inattention correlated

significantly with parent ratings of inattention, and teacher ratings of writing skills were

consistent with objective CBM writing scores. Future studies should consider inclusion of

objective measures of attention (e.g., neuro-cognitive measures of WM, EF, and processing

speed). Such measures would be useful to validate the teacher attention scores as well as to

potentially isolate and explain the underlying mechanism through which inattentive behaviour is

related to poor writing outcomes.

4.4 Conclusion

The present study illustrates that children with mild to severe attention problems

exhibited weaknesses in all areas measured including handwriting quality, spelling,

compositional fluency, accuracy, and general written expression across both direct measures and

teacher ratings of writing. These weaknesses were evident even among those children with

attention problems who did not have word-level reading impairments. This finding suggests that

attention may be important in early written expression and it confirms the first hypothesis. The

second hypothesis, that coexisting attention and reading problems would be associated with

significantly greater impairment in written expression than attention problems alone, was only

partially confirmed as there were no significant differences between the attention groups (AP

and ARP groups) on the objective CBM measures of writing. However, children with coexisting

attention and reading problems did exhibit significantly lower teacher ratings of spelling and

Page 74: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

67

written expression skills as well as overall language arts competence compared to those children

in the AP group.

Finally, the third hypothesis that inattentive behaviour would be directly related to all

writing outcomes independent of hyperactivity-impulsivity was confirmed by for all but one

outcome measure. After controlling for age and hyperactivity-impulsivity, inattention was still

directly related all measures of writing except for direct measures of handwriting quality.

Furthermore, handwriting quality was the only measure that continued to be significantly related

to hyperactivity-impulsivity after controlling for age and inattention.

The results of the present study extend prior research concerning written expression

skills of children with ADHD symptoms (Elbert, 1993; Mayes et al., 2000; Mayes & Calhoun,

2007a; Re et al., 2007; 2008) by demonstrating an association between inattention and written

expression difficulties in a non-clinical English-speaking sample. It is hoped that the findings

from this study may help to understand and prevent written expression difficulties in students

with and without attention problems and encourage further exploration of this area.

Page 75: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

68

References

Abbott, R., & Berninger, VB. (1993). Structural equation modeling of relationships among

developmental skills and writing skills in primary and intermediate grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 478-508.

Adi-Japha, E., Landau, Y.E., Frenkel, L., Teicher, M., & Shalev, R.S. (2007). ADHD and dysgraphia: Underlying mechanisms. Cortex, 43, 700-709.

Alloway, T.P, Gathercole, S.E, Adams, A., Willis,C., Eaglen, R, Lamont, E. (2005).Working memory and phonological awareness as predictors of progress towards early learning goals at school entry. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 417–426

Altemeier, L.E., Abbott, R. D. & Berninger, V.W. (2007). Executive functions for reading and writing in typical literacy development and dyslexia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 588- 606

Altemeier, L.E., Jones, J., Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (2006). Executive functions in becoming writing readers and reading writers: Note taking and report writing in third and fifth graders. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 161-174.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.

Barkley, R.A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65-94.

Barkley, R., DuPaul, G., & McMurray, M. (1990). Comprehensive evaluation of attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity as defined by research criteria. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 775–789.

Barkley, R.A., Fischer, M., Edelbrock, C.S., & Smallish, L. (1990). The adolescent outcome of hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria, I: An 8-year prospective follow-up study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 546–557.

Berninger, V.W. (1999). Coordinating transcription and text generation in working memory during composing: Automatic and constructive processes. Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 22, 99-112.

Berninger, V.W., Abbott, R.D., Abbott, S.P., Graham, S., & Richards, T. (2002). Writing and reading: Connections between language by hand and language by eye. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 39-56.

Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Jones, J., Wolf, B. J., Gould, L., Anderson-Youngstrom, M., Shimada, S., & Apel, K. (2006). Early development of language by hand: Composing, reading, listening, and speaking connections: Three letter-writing modes and fast mapping in spelling.

Page 76: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

69

Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 61-92.

Berninger, V.W., & Amtmann, D. (2003). Preventing written expression disabilities through early and continuing assessment and intervention for handwriting and/or spelling problems: Research into practice. In H. L. Swanson, K.R. Harris & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of Learning Disabilities, (pp. 345-363). New York: Guilford Press.

Berninger, V.W., Cartwright, A.C., Yates, C.M., Swanson, H.L., & Abbott, R.D. (1994). Developmental skills related to writing and reading acquisition in the intermediate grades. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6, 161-196.

Berninger, V.W,. & Fuller, F. (1992). Gender differences in orthographic, verbal and compositional fluency: Implications for assessing writing disabilities in primary grade children. Journal of School Psychology, 30, 363-382.

Berninger, V.W., Fuller, F., & Whitaker, D. (1996). A process model of writing across the life span. Education Psychology Review, 8, 193-218.

Berninger, V.W, Mizokawa, D., & Bragg, R. (1991). Theory-based diagnosis and remediation of writing disabilities. Journal of School Psychology, 29, 57–79. Berninger, V.W., Yates, C., Lester, K. (1991). Multiple orthographic codes in reading and writing acquisition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 3, 115-149.

Berninger, V.W., Mizokawa, D.T., Bragg, R., Cartwright, A., & Yates, C. (1994). Intraindividual differences in levels of written language. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 10, 259-275.

Berninger V.W., Nielsen, K.H., Abbott R.D., Wijsman, E., & Raskind W. (2008a). Writing problems in developmental dyslexia: Under-recognized and under-treated. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 1–21.

Berninger V.W., Nielsen, K.H., Abbott R.D., Wijsman, E., & Raskind W. (2008b). Gender differences in severity of writing and reading disabilities. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 151 – 172.

Berninger, V.W., Vaughan, K, Abbott, R., Abbott, S., Woodruff, L. (1997). Treatment of handwriting problems in beginning writers: Transfer from handwriting to composition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 652-666

Berninger, V.W., Vaughan, K., Abbott, R.D., Begay, K., Coleman, K.B., Curtin, B. et al. (2002). Teaching spelling and composition alone and together: Implications for the Simple View of Writing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 291-304.

Berninger, V.W., Yates, C., Lester, K. (1991). Multiple orthographic codes in reading and writing acquisition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 3, 115-149.

Berninger, V.W, Yates, C., Cartwright, A., Rutberg, J., Remy, E., & Abbott, R. (1992). Lower-level developmental skills in beginning writing. Reading and Writing: An interdisciplinary Journal, 4, 257-280.

Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M.C., Doyle, A.E., Seidman, L.J., Wilens, T.E., Ferrero, F., et al. (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on

Page 77: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

70

academic outcomes in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 757–766.

Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Mick, E., Spencer, T., Wilens, T. E., Silva, J. M., et al. (2006). Young adult outcome of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A controlled 10-year follow-up study. Psychological Medicine, 36, 167–179.

Bourke, L., & Adams, A.M. (2003). The relationship between working memory and early writing assessed at the word, sentence and text level. Educational and Child Psychology, 20, 19-36.

Chase, C. (1986). Essay test scoring: interaction of relevant variables. Journal of Educational Measures 23, 33–41.

Chenault, B., Thomson, J., Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (2006). Effects of prior attention training on child dyslexics' response to composition instruction. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29(1), 243-260.

Chhabilidas, N., Pennington, B.F., & Willcutt, E. (2001). A comparison of the neuropsychological profiles of the DSM-IV Subtypes of ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 529–540.

Cobb Morocco, C., Dalton, B., Tivnan, T. (1992). The impact of computer-supported writing instruction on fourth-grade students with and without learning disabilities. Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 8, 87 – 113

Cooper, C. (1997). Holistic evaluation of writing. In C. Cooper & L. Odell (Eds.), Evaluating writing (pp. 3–31). Urbana IL: National Council of Teachers of English.

Copeland, A.P., Wisniewski, N.M (1981). Learning disability and hyperactivity: Deficits in selective attention. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 88-101.

Currie, J., & Stabile, M. (2006). Child mental health and human capital accumulation: The case of ADHD. Journal of Health Economics, 25, 1094-1118.

Dally, K. (2006). The influence of phonological processing and inattentive behaviour on reading acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 420-437.

Deno, S. (1992). The nature and development of curriculum-based measurement. Preventing School Failure, 36, 5-10.

Deno, S.L., & Fuchs, L.S. (1987). Developing curriculum-based measurement systems for data-based special education problem solving. Focus on Exceptional Children, 19, 1-16.

Deno, S., Marston, D., & Mirkin, P. (1982). Valid measurement procedures for continuous evaluation of written expression. Exceptional Children, 48, 368-371.

Deno, S.L., Mirkin, P.K., & Marston, D. (1980). Relationships among simple measures of written expression and performance on standardized achievement test (Research Report No. 22). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Institute for Research on Learning Disabilities.

Deno, S. Mirkin, P., & Wesson, C. (1984). How to write effective data-based IEPs. Teaching

Page 78: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

71

Exceptional Children, 16, 99-104.

DeShazo-Barry, T., Lyman, R.D., & Klinger, L.G. (2002). Academic underachievement and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The negative impact of symptom severity on school performance. Journal of School Psychology, 40, 259–283.

DiPerna, J.C., & Elliott, S.N. (1999a). Academic Competence Evaluation Scale. San Antonio, Texas: Harcourt.

DiPerna, J.C., & Elliott, S.N. (1999b). Development and validation of the Academic Competence Evaluation Scale. Journal of Psychoeduational Assessment, 17, 207-225.

DuPaul, G., Volpe, R., Jitendra, A., Lutz, G., Lorah, K., & Gurber, R. (2004). Elementary school students with AD/HD: Predictors of academic achievement. Journal of School Psychology, 42, 285-301.

Elbert, J. C. (1993). Occurrence and pattern of impaired reading and written language in children with attention deficit disorders. Annals of Dyslexia, 43, 26-43.

Feder, KP, & Majnemer, A. (2007). Handwriting development, competency, and intervention. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 49, 312–317.

Field, A. (2005). Discovering Statistics Using SPSS. United Kingdom: Sage

Fletcher, J., Tannock, R., Bishop, D. (2001).Utility of brief teacher rating scales to identify children with educational problems, Australian Journal of Psychology, 53, 63-71.

Fliers, E., Rommelse, N., Vermeulen, S.H.H., Altink, M., Buschgens, C.J, Faraone, S.V., & Sergeant, J.A.. (2008). Motor coordination problems in children and adolescents with ADHD rated by parents and teachers: effects of age and gender. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115, 211-220.

Flory, K., Milich, R., Lorch, E., Hayden, A., Strange, C., & Welsh, R. (2006), Online story comprehension among children with ADHD: Which core deficits are involved? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 853-865.

Frick, P.J., Kamphaus, R.W., Lahey, B.B., Loeber, R., Christ, M.G., Hart, E.L., et al. (1991). Academic underachievement and the disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 289 – 294.

Fuchs, L., Compton, D., Fuchs, D., Paulsen, K., Bryant, J., & Hamlett, C. (2005) The prevention, identification, and cognitive determinants of math difficulty. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 493–513.

Fuchs, L.S., Fuchs, D., Compton, D.L., Powell, S.R., Seethaler, P.M., Capizzi, A.M., et al. (2006). The cognitive correlates of third-grade skill in arithmetic, algorithmic computation, and arithmetic word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 29-43.

Fuchs, L.S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M.K., & Jenkins, J.R. (2001). Oral Reading Fluency as an Indicator of Reading Competence: A Theoretical, Empirical, and Historical Analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 239–256.

Page 79: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

72

Gansle, KA., Noell, GH., VanDerHeyden, AM., Naquin, GM., Slider, NJ. (2002). Moving beyond total words written: the reliability, criterion validity, and time cost of alternate measures for curriculum-based measurement in writing, School Psychology Review, 31, 477-497.

Gansle, K., Noell, G., Vanderheyden, A., Slider, N., Hoffpauir, L., Whitmarsh, E. et al. (2004). An examination of the criterion validity and sensitivity to brief intervention of alternate curriculum-based measures of writing skill. Psychology in the Schools, 41, 291-300.

Gansle, K.A., VanDerHeyden, A.M., Noell, G.H., Resetar, J.L., & Williams, K.L. (2006). The technical adequacy of curriculum-based and rating-based measures of written expression for elementary school students. School Psychology Review, 35, 435-450.

Gathercole, S., Alloway, T., Kirkwood, H., Elliott, J., Holmes, J., & Hilton, K. (2008). Attentional and executive function behaviours in children with poor working memory. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 214-223.

Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Willis, C. S., & Adams, A. M. (2006).Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 265−281.

Graham, S. (1990). The role of production factors in learning disabled students' compositions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 781- 791.

Graham, S. (1999). Handwriting and spelling instruction for students with learning disabilities: A review. Learning Disability Quarterly, 22, 78-98.

Graham, S., Berninger, V.B, Abbott, R.D., Abbott, S., & Whitaker, D. (1997). Role of mechanics in composing of elementary school students: A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 170-182.

Graham, S., & Harris, K.R. (2000). The role of self-regulation and transcription skills in writing and writing development. Educational Psychologist, 35, 3-12.

Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2003). Students with learning disabilities and the process of writing: A meta-analysis of SRSD studies. In L. Swanson, K. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 323–344). New York: Guilford.

Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Fink, B.( 2000). Is handwriting causally related to learning to write? Treatment of handwriting problems in beginning writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 4, 620-633.

Graham, S., Harris, K.R., & Mason, L. (2005). Improving the writing performance, knowledge, and self-efficacy of struggling young writers: The effects of self-regulated strategy development. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30, 207-241.

Graham, S., Harris, K., MacArthur, C., & Schwartz, S. (1991). Writing and writing instruction with students with learning disabilities: A review of a program of research. Learning Disability Quarterly, 14, 89–114

Graham, S., & Perin, D. (2007). A meta-analysis of writing instruction for adolescent students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 445–476.

Page 80: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

73

Graham, S., Struck, M., Santoro, J., & Berninger, V. W. (2006). Dimensions of good and poor handwriting legibility in first and second graders: Motor programs, visual-spatial arrangement, and letter formation parameter setting. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 43.

Hammill, D., Larsen, S., (1983). Test of Written Language (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro ed.

Hart, E. L., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Applegate, B., & Frick, P. J. (1995). Developmental changes in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in boys: A four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 729-749.

Hayes, J. R. (2005). New Directions in Writing Theory, Chapter 2. In C. MacArthur, S. Graham & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of Writing Research (pp. 28-40). New York, NY: Guildford Press.

Hinshaw, S.P. (1992). Academic underachievement, attention deficits, and aggression: Comorbidity

and implications for intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 893-903. Hinshaw, S.P. (1994). Attention Deficits and Hyperactivity in Children. Thousand Oaks, CA. Sage. Hooper, S. R., Swartz, C., Montgomery, J., Reed, M. S., Brown, T., Wasileski, T., & Levine, M. D.

(1993). Prevalence of writing problems across three middle school samples. School Psychology Review, 22, 610-22.

Hooper, S.R., Swartz, C.W., Wakely, M.B., de Kruif, R.E.L., & Montgomery J.W. (2002). Executive

functions in elementary school children with and without problems in written expression. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 57-68.

Hosp, M., Hosp, J., Howell, K.W. (2006). The ABC’s of CBM: a practical guide to curriculum-based measurement. Guilford Press.

Houck, C.K., Billingsley, B.S. (1989). Written expression of students with and without learning disabilities: differences across the grades. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 561-572

Imhof, M. (2004). Effects of color stimulation on handwriting performance of children with ADHD without and with additional learning disabilities. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 191-198.

Jewell, J., & Malecki, C. K. (2005). The utility of CBM written language indices: An investigation of production-dependent, production-independent, and accurate-production scores. School Psychology Review, 34, 27-44.

Jones, D., & Christensen, C. (1999). The relationship between automaticity in handwriting and students’ ability to generate written text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 44–49.

Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437–447.

Kellogg, R.T. (1996). A model of working memory in writing. In C.M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.). The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences and applications (pp. 57-71). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Page 81: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

74

Kroese J, Hynd G, Knight D, Heimenz J & Hall J. (2000). Clinical appraisal of spelling ability and its

relationship to phonemic awareness (blending, segmenting, elision, and reversal), phonological memory and reading in reading disabled, ADHD and normal children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13, 105–131.

Levy, F., Hay, D.A., McStephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1997). Attention-deficit hyperactivity

disorder: a category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 337 – 344.

Lienemann, T.O., Reid, R. (2008). Using SRSD to improve the expository story writing with students with ADHD. Exceptional Children, 74, 471-486.

Loe, I.M., Feldman, H.M. (2007). Academic and educational outcomes of children with ADHD. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 643–654.

Lui, M., Tannock, R. (2007). Working memory and inattentive behaviour in a community sample of children. Behavioral and Brain Functions. 3, 1-11. Retrieved on 10 May 2008 from http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/3/1/12

MacArthur, C., Graham, S., Skarvold, J. (1987). Learning disabled students’ composing under three methods of text production: Handwriting, dictation, and word processing and text production. Journal of Special Education, 21, 22-24.

Malecki, C. K., & Jewell, J. (2003). Developmental, gender, and practical considerations in scoring curriculum-based measurement writing probes. Psychology in the Schools, 40, 379-390.

Marston, D., Deno, S.L. (1981). The reliability of simple direct measures of written expression (Teach. Rep. No. 50.) Minniapolis, MN: University of Minnesota, Institute for Research on Learning Disabilities.

Marston, D., Lowry, L., Deno, S. L., & Mirkin, P. (1981). An analysis of learning trends in simple measures of reading, spelling, and written expression: A longitudinal study. (Research Report No. 49) Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, Institute for Research on Learning Disabilities.

Martin, N.C., Piek, J.P., Hay, D. (2006). DCD and ADHD: a genetic study of their shared aetiology. Human Movement Science, 25, 110-24.

Martinussen, R., & Tannock, R. (2006). Working memory impairments in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with and without comorbid language learning disorders. Journal of Clinical and Neuropsychology, 28, 1073-1094.

Mathers, M. (2006). Aspects of language in children with ADHD: Applying functional analyses to explore language use. Journal of Attention Disorders, 9, 523-533.

Mayes, S.D., & Calhoun, S.L (2006). Frequency of math, and writing disabilities in children with clinical disorders. Learning and Individual Differences, 16, 145-157

Mayes, S.D., & Calhoun, S.L. (2007a). Challenging the assumptions about the frequency and coexistence of learning disability types. School Psychology International, 28, 437–448.

Page 82: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

75

Mayes, S.D., & Calhoun, S.L. (2007b). Learning, attention, writing and processing speed in typical children and children with ADHD, autism, anxiety, depression, and oppositional defiant disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 13, 469-493.

Mayes, S.D., & Calhoun, S.L., & Crowell, E.W. (2000). Learning disabilities and ADHD: Overlapping spectrum disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 417-424.

Mayes S.D., & Calhoun, S.L., & Lane, S.E. (2005). Diagnosing children's writing disabilities: different tests give different results. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 101, 72-78.

McConaughy, S.H., Mattison, R.E., Peterson, R.L. (1994). Behavioral/emotional problems of children with serious emotional disturbances and learning disabilities. School Psychology Review, 23, 81-98.

McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory for writing: Working memory in composition. Education Psychology Review, 8, 299-325.

McCutchen, D. (2006). Cognitive Factors in the Development of Children's Writing. Ch8, in MacArthur, C., Graham, S., & Fitzgerald, J. Eds, Handbook of Writing Research (pp 115-130). New York, NY: Guildford Press.

McCutchen D., Covill A., Hoyne S. H., & Mildes K. (1994). Individual differences in writing: Implications of translating fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 256-266.

McMaster, K., & Espin, C. (2007). Technical features of curriculum-based measurement in writing: A literature review. The Journal of Special Education, 41, 68-84.

Merrell, C.H, Tymms, P. B. (2001). Inattention, hyperactivity and impulsiveness: Their impact on academic achievement and progress. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 43-56.

Myklebust, H. (1965/1973). Development and disorders of written language (Vol. 1). New York: Grune & Stratton.

Olinghouse, K. (2008). Student- and instruction-level predictors of narrative writing in third-grade students, Reading and Writing 21, 3–26.

Olson, C.L. (1976). On choosing a test statistic in multivariate analysis of variance. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 1350-1352.

Parker, R., Tindal, G., & Hasbrouck, J. (1991a). Countable indices of writing quality; their suitability for screening-eligibility decisions. Exceptionality, 2, 1-17.

Parker, R., Tindal, G., & Hasbrouck, J. (1991b). Progress monitoring with objective measures of writing performance for students with mild disabilities. Exceptional Children, 58, 61-73.

Pennington, B.F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.

Pickering, S. J., Gathercole, S.E. (2004). Distinctive working memory profiles in children with special educational needs, Educational Psychology, 24, 393-408.

Page 83: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

76

Pitcher, T.M., Piek, J. P., Barrett, N.C. (2002). Timing and force control in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Subtype differences and the effect of comorbid developmental coordination disorder. Human Movement Science, 21, 919-945.

Pitcher, T.M., Piek, J. P., Hay, D.A. (2003). Fine and gross motor ability is males with ADHD. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45, 525-535.

Powell-Smith, K.A. & Shinn, M.R. (2004) Administration and scoring of curriculum-based measures for use in general outcome measurement. Eden Prairie, M.N: Edformation.

Purvis, K.L. & Tannock, R. (1997). Language abilities in children with ADHD, reading disabilities and normal controls. Journal of Attention Disorders, 25, 133-144.

Rabiner, D.L., & Coie, J.D. (2000). Early attention problems and children's reading achievement: a longitudinal investigation. The Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 859-867.

Rabiner, D.L., Malone, P.S., & The Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2004). The impact of tutoring on early reading achievement for children with and without attention problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 273–284.

Rabiner, D. L., & Murray, D.W. (2002). Predictors of below grade level achievement among first grade students: The impact of attention problems. Unpublished manuscript, Duke University.

Racine B.M., Majnemer, A., Shevell, M., & Snider, L. (2008). Handwriting performance in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Child Neurology, 23, 399-406.

Rapport, M.D., Scanlan, S.W., & Denney, C.B. (1999). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and scholastic achievement: A model of dual developmental pathways. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 1169–1183.

Re, A.M., Pedron, M., & Cornoldi, C. (2007). Expressive writing difficulties in children described as exhibiting ADHD symptoms. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 244-255.

Re, A.M., Caeron, M., & Cornoldi, C. (2008). Improving expressive writing skills of children rated for ADHD symptoms. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 535-544.

Reid, R., Ortiz-Lienemann, T. (2006). Self-regulated strategy development for written expression with students with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Exceptional Children, 73, 53-68.

Resta, S., & Eliot, J. (1994) Written expression in boys with attention deficit disorder, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 1131-1138.

Richards GP, Samuels SJ, Turnure JE, Ysseldyke JE. (1990). Sustained and selective attention in children with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 129–136.

Ritchey, K. (2008). The building blocks of writing: Learning to write letters and spell words. Reading and Writing, 21, 27–47.

Rodriguez, A., Järvelin, M., Obel, C., Taanila, A., Miettunen, J., Moilanen, I., et al. (2007). Do

Page 84: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

77

inattention and hyperactivity symptoms equal scholastic impairment? BMC Public Health, 7, 327-336.

Roid, G. H. (1994) Patterns of writing skills derived from cluster analysis of direct-writing assessments, Applied Measurement in Education, 7, 159- 170.

Rosenblum, S., Weiss, P.L., & Parush, S. (2003). Product and process evaluation of handwriting difficulties. Educational Psychology Review, 15, 41-81.

Ross, Peter A., Poidevant, John M. & Miner, Claire Usher (1995). Curriculum-based assessment of writing fluency in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and normal children. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 11, 201-208.

Rowe, K., Pollard, J., & Rowe, K. (2005) Literacy, behavior, and auditory processing: Does teacher professional development make a difference? Australian Council for Educational Research. (Background paper to Rue Wright Memorial Award presented at the Royal Australasian College of Physicians Scientific Meeting Wellington, New Zealand, 8-11 May 2005.)

Sandler AD, Watson TE, Footo M, Levine MD, Coleman WL, Hooper SR. (1992) Neurodevelopmental study of writing disorders in middle childhood. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 13, 17–23.

Schoemaker, M.M., Ketelaars, C.E., van Zonneveld, M., Minderaa, R.B., & Mulder, T. (2005). Deficits in motor control processes involved in production of graphic movements of children with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 47, 390-395.

Semrud-Clikeman, M., Biederman, J., Sprich-Buckminster, S., Lehman, B. K., Faraone, S. V., & Norman, D. (1992). Comorbidity between ADHD and learning disabilities: A review and report in a clinically referred sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 439–448.

Shanahan, T. (1984). Nature of the reading-writing relation: An exploratory multivariate analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 1309-1324.

Shanahan, T. (2006). Relations among oral language, reading, and writing development. In C. A. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 171–186). New York: Guilford Press.

Shanahan, T. & Lomax, D. (1986). An analysis and comparison of theoretical models of the reading-writing relationship. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 116- 123.

Shanahan, T., & Lomax, R. G. (1988).A developmental comparison of three theoretical models of the reading–writing relationship. Research in the Teaching of English, 22, 196–212.

Shinn, M. R., & Marston, D. (1985). Differentiating mildly handicapped, low-achieving and regular education students: A curriculum-based approach. Remedial and Special Education, 6, 31–45.

Soo, C., & Bailey, J. (2006). A review of functioning of attentional components in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and learning disabilities. Brain Impairment, 7, 133–147.

Page 85: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

78

Speece, D.L., McKinney, J.D., Applebaum, M.I. (1985). Classification and validation of behavioural subtypes of learning disabilities. Journal of Education Psychology, 77, 67-77.

Spira, E.G., & Fischel, J.E. (2005). The impact of preschool inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity on social and academic development: A review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 755-773.

Swanson, H. L. (1983). A developmental study of vigilance in learning-disabled and nondisabled children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11, 415-428.

Swanson, H.L., Ashbaker, M.H., Sachse-Lee, C. (1996). Learning disabled reader’s working memory as a function of processing demands. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61, 252 - 275.

Swanson, H.L., & Berninger, V. (1996). Individual differences in children's working memory and writing skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 358–385.

Swanson J., Schuck, S., Mann, M., Carlson, C., Hartman, K., & Sergeant, J., et al. (2005). Categorical and dimensional definitions and evaluations of symptoms of ADHD: The SNAP and the SWAN Ratings Scales. Retrieved on January 2, 2009 from http://www.adhd.net/SNAP_SWAN.pdf.

Tannock, R., Purvis, K., & Schachar, R. (1993) Narrative abilities in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and normal peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 21, 103-117.

Tarnowski, K.J, Prinz, R.J., Nay, S.M. (1986). Comparative analysis of attentional deficits in hyperactive and learning-disabled children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 341-345.

Thomas, C., Englert, C., & Gregg, S. (1987). An analysis of errors and strategies in the expository writing of learning disabled students. Remedial and Special Education, 8, 21–30.

Thomson, JB., Chenault, B., Abbott, R., Raskind, W.H., Richards, T., Aylward, E., & Berninger, V.W. (2005). Converging evidence for attentional influences on the orthographic word form in child dyslexics. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 18, 93–126.

Tindal, G., & Parker, R. (1989). Assessment of written expression for students in compensatory and special education programs. The Journal of Special Education, 23, 169-183.

Todd, R.D., Sitdhiraksa, N., Reich, W., Ji, T.H., Joyner, C.A., Heath, A.C., et al. (2002). Discrimination of DSM-IV and latent class attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes by educational and cognitive performance in a population-based sample of child and adolescent twins. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 820-828.

Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., & Rashotte, C.A. (1999). Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Tucha, O. & Lange, K.W. (2004). Handwriting in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Motor Control, 8, 461-471.

Vanderberg, R., & Swanson, HL. (2007). Which components of working memory are important in the writing process? Reading and Writing, 20, 721-752.

Page 86: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

79

Videen, J., Deno, S., & Marston, D. (1982). Correct word sequences: A valid indicator of proficiency in written expression. Report No. 84. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, Institute for Research on Learning Disabilities.

Wakely, M. B., Hooper, S. R., de Kruif, R. E. L., & Swartz, C. (2006). Subtypes of written expression in elementary school children: A linguistics based model. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 125-160.

Warner-Rogers, J., Taylor, A., Taylor, E., & Sandberg, S. (2000). Inattentive behavior in childhood: Epidemiology and implications for development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 520-536.

Watkinson, J. & Lee, S. (1992). Curriculum-based measures of written expression for learning disabled and nondisabled students. Psychology in Schools, 29, 184-192.

Weiss, M., Worling, D. & Wasdell, M. (2003). A chart review study of the Inattentive and Combined Types of ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 7, 1-9.

Weissenburger, J., & Espin, C. (2005). Curriculum-based measures of writing across grade levels. Journal of School Psychology, 43, 153–169.

Weschler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Intelligence Scale (WASI). San Antonio, Texas: The Psychological Corporation

Westerberg, H., Hirvikoski, T., Frossberg, H., & Klingberg, T. (2004) Visuo-spatial working memory span: A sensitive measure of cognitive deficits in children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychology, 10, 155-161.

Wiener, J. (1986). Alternatives in the assessment of LD adolescent: A learning strategies approach. Learning Disability Focus, 1, 97-107

Wilding, J. (2003). Attentional difficulties in children: Weakness in executive function or problems in coping with difficult tasks? British Journal of Psychology, 94, 427–436.

Wilding, J. (2005). Is attention impaired in ADHD? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 487–505.

Willcutt, E. G., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Psychiatric comorbidity in children and adolescents with reading disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 1039-1048.

Willcutt, E.G., Doyle, A.E., Nigg, J.T., Faraone, S.V., Pennington, B.F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336–1346.

Wright, J. (1992). CBM Measurement: Directions for Administering and Scoring CBM Probes in Writing. Excerpt from: Curriculum-Based Measurement: Manual for Teachers. Retrieved on 24 November 2006 from www.interventioncentral.com

Page 87: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

80

TABLES

Table 1.

Sample Characteristics across Groups

TYP (1) AP (2) ARP (3) Differences

M SD n M SD n M SD n F(2, 77) Post hoc

Gender (%boys)

62% 75% 78% naa -

Parent Ed. b 4.33 1.02 29 3.72 1.17 25 4.38 0.89 13 3.08 -

Age 8.25 1.27 29 7.99 1.19 25 8.63 0.87 13 2.43 -

WASI 51.62 8.74 29 50.44 9.61 25 49.15 9.34 13 .71 -

ASL 15.03 3.29 29 13.80 4.37 25 15.08 4.05 13 1.24 -

TOWRE 105.66 12.44 29 102.04 8.49 25 79.15 7.53 13 46.78*** 1, 2 > 3

SWAN

Inattention

-1.33 0.97 29 1.10 0.61 25 1.25 0.51 13 102.89*** 2 , 3 >1

SWAN

Hyperactivity

-1.09 1.12 29 0.52 0.97 25 0.93 0.89 13 25.26*** 2 ,3 >1

Note: TYP=Typical attention and reading, AP=Attention problems only, ARP=Attention and reading

problems, WASI = Wechsler Abbreviated Scale for Intelligence-Matrices subtest measures nonverbal IQ,

TOWRE = Test of Word Reading Efficiency; ASL = Auditory Sentence Length Task; SWAN = Strength and Weaknesses of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity-Impulsivity Disorder and Normal

Behaviour. a Gender distribution was equivalent across groups: Pearson χ2(2, N = 80) = 1.95, p >.05 b Parent education values: 1 = Grades 0-8, 2 = Grades 9-11, 3 = High school/GED, 4 = Some college, 5 = College graduate, 6 = Post-college degree, value presented is the average *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

Page 88: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

81

Table 2.

Group Differences on Handwriting Quality and the CBM Assessments of Written Expression

1.TYP (n = 34)

2. AP (n = 28)

3. ARP (n = 18)

M SD M SD M SD df F b Post hocd η2b

Handwriting quality

HW 3.31 .83 2.39 .81 2.39 .63 2, 77 13.35*** 1 > 2, 3 .26

Fluency scores

TWW 32.03 15.19 20.07 9.79 20.28 8.31 2, 77 9.33*** 1 > 2, 3 .20

WSCa 28.47 15.20 15.75 8.71 13.94 6.60 2, 77 11.68*** 1 > 2, 3 .23

CWSa 25.47 15.46 12.21 7.93 10.00 6.37 2, 77 14.21*** 1 > 2, 3 .27

Accuracy scores df, N χ2 c

%WSC 86% .10 76% .15 66% .15 2, 80 22.64*** 1 > 2, 3 .29

%CWS 68% .17 54% .22 43% .15 2, 80 19.48*** 1 > 2, 3 .25

Note: TYP=Typical attention and reading, AP=Attention problems only, ARP=Attention and reading

problems, HW = Handwriting, TWW = words written, WSC = words spelled correctly, CWS = Correct

writing sequences, %WSC = WSC/TWW, %CWS = CWS/total writing sequences. a Square root transformation applied to normalize distribution but raw scores are presented to facilitate

interpretation. b F values and effect size are for follow-up univariate ANOVAs. c Reported results for group differences in the accuracy scores were computed using nonparametric

Kruskal-Wallis tests; Mann Whitney U were employed for post hoc pair-wise comparisons. d Bonferroni adjusted for multiple comparisons. *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

Page 89: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

82

Table 3.

Group Differences on the Teacher Ratings of Written Expression Skills and Language Arts

1.TYP (n = 34)

2. AP (n = 27)a

3. ARP

(n = 16)a Differences

M SD M SD M SD F b Post-hoc c η2 b

Literacy Rating Scale

Handwriting 3.71 0.84 2.33 0.83 1.81 0.75 33.64*** 1 > 2, 3 .47

Spelling 3.76 0.89 2.48 0.85 1.44 0.63 46.36*** 1 > 2, 3;2 > 3 .55

Written

expression 3.56 0.82 2.26 0.66 1.56 0.73 48.28*** 1 > 2, 3;2 > 3 .56

Academic Competence Evaluation Scale

Language Arts 40.26 7.77 28.52 6.45 22.69 5.55 44.35*** 1 > 2, 3, 2 > 3 .54

Note: TYP = Typical attention and reading, AP = Attention Problem only, ARP = Attention + Reading

Problems. a Teacher rating data was unavailable for 3 children. b F values and effect size are for follow-up univariate ANOVAs, df = 2, 77. c Bonferroni adjusted for multiple comparisons. *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

Page 90: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

83

Table 4.

Zero-order and Partial Correlations for Inattention and Hyperactivity across Writing Variables

Zero-order Correlations Partial Correlations

Age Inattention Hyperactivity Inattention b Hyperactivity c

Age _ .04 .06 _ _

Inattention _ _ .82*** _ _

CBM writing variables

HW .28* -.60*** -.61*** -.21 -.29*

TWW .58*** -.43*** -.33** -.30** .02

WSCa .59*** -.48*** -.37** -.33** .01

CWSa .57*** -.50*** -.37** -.38*** .06

%WSC .36** -.46*** -.39*** -.27* .02

%CWS .36** -.44*** -.33** -.30** .08

Teacher ratings of writing skills

Handwriting -.06 -.78*** -.63*** -.59*** .13

Spelling -.14 -.75*** -.58*** -.59*** .06

Written Expression -.10 -.80*** -.62*** -.65*** .11

Language Arts -.08 -.77*** -.56*** -.65*** .20

Note: HW = Handwriting, TWW = Total words written, WSC = Words spelled correctly, CWS = Correct

writing sequences, %WSC = WSC/TWW, %CWS = CWS/Total writing sequences, Inattention = teacher

ratings on Inattention Subscale of the Strength And Weaknesses of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity-

Impulsivity Disorder And Normal Behaviour (SWAN) scale, Hyperactivity = teacher ratings on

Hyperactivity-Impulsivity Subscale of SWAN. a Square root transformed. b Covarying age and hyperactivity-impulsivity. c Covarying age and inattention. *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

Page 91: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

84

APPENDIX A

Scor ing p rocedures and examples o f scored wr i t ing samples

Page 92: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

85

Scoring Procedures for CBM Writing Samples in Present Study Note: scoring procedures used in this study were compiled by the author based on published studies that utilized CBM measures or analyzed handwriting (e.g., Jewel & Malecki, 2005; Parker et al.,1991; Gansle et al., 2004; Rosenblum et al., 2003; Wright, 1992) Directions for scoring Children’s writing samples are to be scored using procedures described below to provide an index of children’s writing fluency (production dependent scores), accuracy (production independent scores), legibility (handwriting scale), and overall quality (CWS). Score samples from the same grade at a time. Start with the fastest and easiest measures: 1)Handwriting, 2)Total words and 3) Total words spelled correctly , 4) Correct and Total writing sequences (CWS & TWS). Staple the provided slips of paper to the sample to record the scores and please identify special characteristics of the writing sample not captured in the scores in the space for notes on the slip. Keep a log of any dilemmas/issues you have scoring the data; Include the subject number, type of issue and if and how you resolved it. 1. Handwriting (HW): Use the scale below to assess the visual presentation of the writing sample based on a) legibility of letters written and b) uniformity of written product; these two scores will be combined to provide a score for overall visual presentation of the writing sample. (1= well below avg, 2=below avg, 3=avg, 4=above avg, 5=well above avg)

Score Letter legibility (letter form, orientation, pencil control)

Uniformity of written text (letter size, spacing between & within words, alignment)

1= well below avg

Student appears to have considerable difficulty writing letters; letters are barely identifiable. Written product may contain scribbles, incorrect letter forms, reversals, rotations, letters with missing or extra parts, letters too small or printed too lightly to read, etc.

Difficult to read and understand because of inconsistent and/or inappropriate spacing within and between words, letter size, and/or alignment. May include: strings of letters and words not separated by spaces, overlapping letters, writing that doesn't follow or fit between lines on the page.

2=below avg

Student appears to have some difficulty shaping and orienting letters. Reader has some difficulty identifying letters, but most letters can be identified. Large proportion letters may be inappropriately capitalized.

Some difficulty reading and/or understanding content because of issues with uniformity of writing such as letter strings, words not separated by spaces, overlapping letters, or writing that doesn't follow or fit between lines on the page.

3=avg, Letters are clearly identifiable and basically formed correctly. Upper and lower case letter use is mostly correct. There are no (or few for Gr.1) letter reversals or rotations.

Written product demonstrates adequate letter size, spacing between letters and words, and alignment of words across lined page. (Consider sample relative to grade level)

4=above avg,

Letters are clearly identifiable and readable. They are clearly and correctly formed, well proportioned there are no reversals, rotations

Clear, consistent and appropriate spacing between letters and words; letter size and alignment across the page makes written product easy to read and understand.

5=well above avg

Letters have a pleasant appearance that enhances readability of the written product. Letters are well formed, clearly printed, nicely proportioned relative to each other and lines on the page

Communicative value of the written product is enhanced by consistent and appropriate letter size, spacing between letters and words and alignment through easy readability and pleasing visual presentation.

(Letter legibility score + Uniformity score ) /2 = Overall Handwriting Score

Page 93: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

86

2. Total Words Written (TWW): Count the total number of words written during the 3-minute period, including the words that are spelled incorrectly. Do not count numbers that are not spelled out (1987, 3, 29) as words. Be sure to count the title if written and proper names and nouns as words. If the student writes the story starter as part of the story, include those words in the count. Abbreviations count. Strings = one word but try to break apart any recognizable invented spelling words. Each line of letters strings counts as one word. Time needs be written as a word ; a.m. or p.m. are abbreviations so count as 1 word. 3. Words Spelled Correctly (WSC): Total number of words spelled correctly. Following Gansle et al. (2006) rules, consider words in isolation. Words do not have to be grammatically correct or make sense in the sentence. Draw a box around incorrectly spelled words. Words DO count as correct if they:

are correct abbreviations such as 'a.m.' and 'Mr.' contain incorrectly capitalized letters, but are spelled correctly such as 'fun' written as 'fuN' or

'Fun' Words DO NOT count as correct if:

• a letter reversal makes a word incorrect such as dog written as bog • the word includes as letter that is not recognizable • a proper noun that is not capitalized such as 'Sarah' written as 'sarah', or I written as 'i' • contractions missing punctuation such as 'didn't' written as 'didnt'

4. Correct Writing Sequences (CWS) and Total Writing Sequence (TWS): Count correct units of writing and their relation to one another. Mark the presence of a correct writing sequence with a caret (+) and mark missing or incorrect writing sequences with a (*) so that 'Total possible writing sequences' can be easily counted. Words and critical punctuation marks (e.g., periods) are considered separate writing units; each successive pair of correct writing units (writing sequence) from beginning to end of sample are counted. To receive credit, writing sequences must be correctly spelled and be grammatically correct. The words in each writing sequence must also make sense within the context of the sentence. The right form of the word (for example: our, are, their, there, they’re, etc.) must be used to count as a correct sequence. If two words should be an adjoined compound word (for example: outside, uptown, upstairs, etc.) and the two words are not connected, consider how the two words flow with the sentence separately, not as a compound. For compound words that are separate, give the student credit prior to and after the compound word but not the one between the two words (e.g. can not). Consider sentences separately; the word at the end of a sentence should never make a correct sequence with a word at the beginning of another sentence. This applies to when you determine a sentence should end and another begin and the punctuation to mark it as such is missing. 'Total number of possible writing sequences’ (TWS): Count all possible writing sequences, both incorrect & correct writing sequences in sample in order to calculate percentage and correct minus incorrect scores. For example: ^I^ played^ outside* *it ^was^ fun* is scored as TWS=8 / CWS=5 ^I^played^outside^.^It ^was ^fun^. CWS=TWS=8 Note: In practice (see example below) we found it more efficient and clear to use “+” and “o” instead of “^” and “*”.

Page 94: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

87

Appendix A cont’d – Examples of Scored CBM Writing Samples Sample 1 – Grade 3.

Sample 2 – Grade 2.

Page 95: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

88

APPENDIX B

Correlations between Curriculum Based Measures of Writing and Teacher Ratings of Writing

N = 85 HW TWW WSC CWS %WSC %CWS

Literacy Rating Scale (LRS)

Handwriting skills .587*** .229* .314** .376*** .473*** .489***

Spelling skills .568*** .309** .402*** .474*** .529*** .537***

Written expression skills .489*** .280** .335** .382*** .403*** .395***

Academic Competence Evaluation Scale (ACES)

Language Arts (N = 83) .523*** .297** .381*** .464*** .491*** .492***

Note: Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient values shown. HW = Handwriting, TWW = Words written,

WSC = Words spelled correctly, CWS = Correct writing sequences, %WSC = WSC/TWW,

%CWS = CWS/total writing sequences *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

Page 96: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

89

APPENDIX C

Means and Standard Deviations for Direct Measures of Writing by Gender and Group

Typical Attention Problems Attention and Reading Problems

M SD n M SD n M SD n

Boys

Handwriting quality HW 2.95 0.55 21 2.12 0.67 21 2.43 0.68 14 Fluency scores TWW 29.57 13.51 21 19.14 9.47 21 19.00 6.77 14 WSC 26.05 13.14 21 14.48 8.00 21 13.29 5.74 14 CWS 22.86 12.52 21 10.76 7.00 21 9.36 5.53 14 Accuracy scores % WSC 86% .09 21 73% .15 21 68% .14 14 %CWS 67% .15 21 49% .22 21 45% .13 14 Girls Handwriting quality HW 3.88 0.92 13 3.21 0.64 7 2.25 0.50 4 Fluency scores TWW 36.00 17.40 13 22.86 10.98 7 24.75 12.58 4 WSC 32.38 17.91 13 19.57 10.24 7 16.25 9.74 4 CWS 29.69 19.09 13 16.57 9.50 7 12.25 9.43 4 Accuracy scores %WSC 86% .12 13 84% .12 7 59% .20 4 %CWS 70% .20 13 67% .19 7 38% .24 4 Note: HW = Handwriting, TWW = Total words written, WSC = Words spelled correctly, CWS = Correct

writing sequences, %WSC = WSC/TWW, %CWS = CWS/Total writing sequences. A significant gender difference was found for handwriting within the TYP group, t(32) = -3.32, p<.01;

and the AP group, t(26) = -3.79, p<.01. *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

Page 97: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

90

APPENDIX D

Means and Standard Deviations for Teacher Ratings of Writing by Gender and Group

Typical Attention problems Attention+Reading

problems

M SD n M SD n M SD n

Boys

Literacy Rating Scale (LRS)

Handwriting skills 3.52 0.81 21 2.15 0.81 20 1.62 0.65 13

Spelling skills 3.67 0.86 21 2.40 0.94 20 1.33 0.49 13

Written expression skills 3.24 0.77 21 2.25 0.72 20 1.42 0.67 12

Academic competence evaluation scale (ACES)

Language arts subscale 38.81 7.55 21 28.65 7.24 20 21.33 5.03 12

Girls

Literacy Rating Scale (LRS)

Handwriting skills 4.00 0.82 13 2.86 0.69 7 2.25 0.96 4

Spelling skills 3.92 0.95 13 2.71 0.49 7 1.75 0.96 4

Written expression skills 4.08 0.64 13 2.29 0.49 7 2.00 0.82 4

Academic competence evaluation scale (ACES)

Language arts subscale 42.62 7.83 13 28.14 3.76 7 26.75 5.62 4

Note: A significant gender difference was found for teacher ratings of written expression within the TYP

group, t(32) = .28, p<.01. *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

Page 98: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

91

APPENDIX E - COPYRIGHT PERMISSION

Page 99: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

92

FIGURE CAPTIONS

Figure 1. Schematic Diagram for the Simple View of Writing Figure 2. Hypothetical Model of Relationships between Attention Problems and Written

Expression Outcomes

Page 100: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

93

Figure 1. Schematic Diagram of the Simple View of Writing

Note: The diagram in Fig. 1 was adapted from Berninger, V.W., & Amtmann, D. (2003). Preventing written expression disabilities through early and continuing assessment and intervention for handwriting and/or spelling problems: Research into practice, Fig. 21.1 on page 350 in Ch. 21 in H. L. Swanson, K.R. Harris & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of Learning Disabilities, 345-363. New York: Guilford Press.

The Simple View of Writing, adapted from Berninger et al., (2003) describes key developmental constraints that influence children’s writing during composing.

Compositional Fluency and Quality • Words, sentences, discourse • Overall writing accuracy • Cohesion and content

*Limited working memory resources hold and manipulate knowledge retrieved from long term memory to execute transcription, self regulation and text generation while composing.

Page 101: Inattention and Written Expression Difficulties in ......common and impairing academic problems for all school-aged children (Hooper et al., 1993; Hooper et al., 2002; Mayes & Calhoun,

94

Figure 2.

Hypothetical Model of the Relationships between Inattention and Written Expression

Compositional Fluency

Hyperactivity-impulsivity

Reading Weaknesses

Handwriting

Written Spelling

Compositional accuracy/quality

Attention Problems

Amount of written work completed

Inattention and related risk factors

Text generation level difficulties

Transcription level difficulties

Academic function level difficulties

Time & effort to complete written

work


Recommended