+ All Categories
Home > Documents > ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an...

ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an...

Date post: 19-Oct-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 0 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
40
Journal of REHABILITATION MEDICINE ISSN 1650-1977 Official journal of the – International Society of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (ISPRM) – UEMS European Board of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (EBPRM) – European Academy of Rehabilitation Medicine (EARM) Published in association with the – European Society of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (ESPRM) – Canadian Association of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation (CAPM&R) – Asia Oceania Society of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (AOSPRM) – Baltic and North Sea Forum for Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (BNFPRM) Rehabilitation Information T www.medicaljournals.se/jrm Vol. 44. No. 6 May 2012 Special issue CONSCIOUSNESS AND THE VEGETATIVE STATE: TODAY Articles from the workshop held in July 6, 2010 in Salerno, Italy
Transcript
Page 1: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

Journal of

REHABILITATIONMEDICINE

ISSN 1650-1977

Official journal of the– International Society of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (ISPRM)– UEMS European Board of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (EBPRM)– European Academy of Rehabilitation Medicine (EARM)Published in association with the– European Society of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (ESPRM)– Canadian Association of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation (CAPM&R)– Asia Oceania Society of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (AOSPRM)– Baltic and North Sea Forum for Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (BNFPRM)

Rehabilitation InformationT

www.medicaljournals.se/jrm

Vol. 44. No. 6May 2012

Special issue

ConsCiousness and the vegetative state: today

Articles from the workshop held in July 6, 2010 in Salerno, Italy

Page 2: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

All correspondence concerning manuscripts, editorial matters and subscription should be addressed to:Editorial Manager: Mrs Agneta Andersson, Editorial assistant: Hanna Bergström, [email protected] [email protected] For postal address: see inside back cover

Publication information: Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine (ISSN 1650-1977) volume 44 comprises ten regular issues published in January, February, March, April, May, June, July, September, October and November. Each issue comprises approximately 96 pages. Subscr rates vol 44: - for institutions: paper incl. electronic access: EUR 430, electronic only: EUR 370

- for individuals: paper incl. electronic access: EUR 175, electronic only: EUR 145 - for the organizations above: paper incl. electronic access: EUR 75, electronic only: EUR 50

Indexing: Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is indexed in Index Medicus/MEDLINE, Excerpta Medica/EMBASE, Biological Abstracts, Current Contents/Clinical Practice, Allied and Alternative Medicine Database (AMED), Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts, Er-gonomic Abstracts, Psychological Abstracts PsycINFO, PSYCLIT DATABASE, Cumulative Index to Nursing & Allied Health Literature (CINAHL), Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, Exceptional Child Education Resources, Periodicals Scanned and Abstracted: Life Sciences Collection, Faxon Finder, Focus On Sports Science & Medicine, Research Alert, SCISEARCH, SportSearch.

Journal of Rehabilitation MedicineJournal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It is owned by a Swedish nonprofit organization: Foundation for Rehabilitation Information. Journal of Rehabilita-tion Medicine was former called Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, which was founded by Olle Höök in 1968. The name was changed to Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine in 2001.Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine aims to be a leading worldwide forum for research in physical and rehabilitation medicine, aiming to increase knowledge in evidence-based clinical rehabilitation. Contributions from all parts of the world and from differ-ent professions in rehabilitation are encouraged. Original articles, Reviews (including Educational reviews), Special reports, Short communications, Case reports, and Letters to the Editor are published. Clinical studies on rehabilitation in various patients groups, within neurological and musculoskeletal as well as in other relevant rehabilitation areas, reports on physical and behavioural treat-ment methodology, including rehabilitation technology, development and analysis of methodology for outcome measurements, epidemiological studies on disability in relation to rehabilitation, and studies on vocational and socio-medical aspects of rehabilita-tion will be considered for publication. The journal emphasizes the need for randomized controlled studies of various rehabilitation interventions, the use of the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF) as a background for reports when appropriate, and the use of modern psychometric methodology in treating and reporting data from ordinal scales.

Editor-in-ChiefGunnar Grimby, Göteborg, Sweden

Co-EditorBengt H. Sjölund, Malmö, Sweden

Advisory Co-EditorHenk Stam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands

Associate EditorsKristian Borg, Stockholm, Sweden Ian Cameron, Sydney, Australia Franco Franchignoni, Veruno, ItalyGerard E. Francisco, Houston, USABjörn Gerdle, Linköping, SwedenJianan Li, Nanjing, ChinaLeonard Li, HongkongGerold Stucki, Nottwil, SwitzerlandAlan Tennant, Leeds, United Kingdom

Editorial BoardOlavi Airaksinen, Kuopio, FinlandMasami Akai, Saitama, JapanJari Arokoski, Kuopio, FinlandAniko Bartfai, Stockholm, SverigeLinamara R. Battistella, Sao Paulo, BrazilBipin Bhakta, Leeds, United Kingdom Fin Biering-Sørensen, Copenhagen, DenmarkJörgen Borg, Stockholm, SwedenHelena Burger, Ljubljana, SloveniaAnne Chamberlain, Leeds, United KingdomAlain Delarque, Marseilles, FranceJan Ekholm, Stockholm, SwedenVeronika Fialka-Moser, Vienna, AustriaMartin Grabois, Houston, USACristoph Gutenbrunner, Hannover, GermanyKenji Hachisuka, Kitakyushu, JapanAndrew Haig, Michigan, USAKarin Harms-Ringdahl, Stockholm, SwedenMarta Imamura, São Paulo, BrasilSusanne Iwarsson, Lund, SwedenAlan Jette, Boston, USAYun-Hee Kim, Seoul, KoreaAyse Küçükdeveci, Ankara, Turkey

Gert Kwakkel, Amsterdam, The NetherlandsJan Lexell, Lund, SwedenMeigen Liu, Tokyo, JapanNancy Mayo, Montreal, CanadaFrans Nollet, Amsterdam, The NetherlandsJean-Michel Mazaux, Bordeaux, FranceJohn Olver, Melbourne, AustraliaChang-il Park, Seoul, KoreaMichael Quittan, Vienna, AustriaCarol Richards, Quebec, CanadaNicola Smania, Verona, ItalyJohan Stanghelle, Oslo, NorwayKatharina Stibrant Sunnerhagen, Göteborg, SwedenTodd P. Stitik, Newark, USASimon F.T. Tang, Tao-Yuan, TaiwanRobert Teasell, London, CanadaJean-Louis Thonnard, Bruxelles, BelgiumLeanne Togher, Sydney, Australia Lynne Turner-Stokes, London, United KingdomGuy Vanderstraeten, Gent, BelgiumMaobin Wang, Beijing, ChinaAnthony B. Ward, Stoke on Trent, United KingdomGünes Yavuzer, Istanbul, TurkeyAlain Yelnik, Paris, France

Contact persons for the organizations:International Society of Physical Rehabilitation Medicine (ISPRM): Franco Franchignoni, Veruno, ItalyUEMS European Board of Physical and Rehabilitation Medi-cine: Rolf Frischknecht, Lausanne, SwitzerlandEuropean Academy of Rehabilitation Medicine: Guy Vander-straeten, Gent, BelgiumEuropean Society of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine: Elena Milkova Ilieva, Plovdiv, Bulgaria and Calogero Foti, Rome, ItalyCanadian Association of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation: Colleen O’Connell, Fredericton, New Brunswick, CanadaAsia Oceania Society of Physical and Rehabiliation Medicine: Simon F.T. Tang, Tao-Yuan, TaiwanBaltic and North Sea Forum for Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine: Christoph Gutenbrunner, Hannover, Germany

Page 3: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 481–516

J Rehabil Med 44© 2012 The Authors. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0992Journal Compilation © 2012 Foundation of Rehabilitation Information. ISSN 1650-1977

ConsCiousness and the vegetative state: today

articles from the workshop held in July 6, 2010 in salerno, italy

Guest Editors

Giuliano Dolce, MD, Lucia F. Lucca, MD and Walter G. Sannita, MD

Page 4: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It
Page 5: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 483

J Rehabil Med 44© 2012 The Authors. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0967Journal Compilation © 2012 Foundation of Rehabilitation Information. ISSN 1650-1977

FOREWORD

Progress in intensive care has improved the outcome of patients with severe brain damage and increased their chances of sur-viving and developing a severe disorder of consciousness such as the vegetative or minimally conscious states. Recent data have documented early recovery from the vegetative state in a significant proportion of patients; late recovery is also possible years later. There is sound neuroimaging evidence that residual responsiveness is also detectable in subjects who have been unambiguously diagnosed as being in a vegetative state and, by definition, isolated from the environment. This evidence blurs further the diagnostic distinction between the vegetative and minimally conscious states, and brings into question the current diagnostic criteria, alters the perspective of health care and neurorehabilitation on this issue, and has led to increased interest amongst the scientific community in the mechanism sustaining consciousness. This issue is attracting the attention of scientists with diverse research backgrounds, due to progress in the investigation of higher brain function, advances in arti-ficial intelligence, and diffuse perception of the inadequacy of traditional mind/body separation.

The workshop “Consciousness and the vegetative state: to-day” was held in Salerno, Italy, on 6 July 2010, in the framework of the 2nd Conference on Consciousness and Coma, with the par-

ticipation of leading scientists in neuroscience. The chairs were G. G. Celesia (Chicago) and W. G. Sannita (Genova/New York). The objectives of the workshop were to update the current char-acterization of consciousness and related terms (which remain to a significant extent ambiguously defined), focus attention on methodological and applicative problems, and promote multi-disciplinary interaction and collaboration. It is hoped that the workshop and its proceedings will facilitate sharing of relevant information on this issue and promote further research.

Thanks are due to the Institute S. Anna – RAN for the suc-cessful organization, financial support and publication on this special issue as part of the program for advanced teaching and professional upgrading “Le giornate di Crotone, yrs. XIII and XIV”.

Giuliano Dolce, MD1, Lucia F. Lucca, MD1 and Walter G. Sannita, MD2

From the 1Institute S. Anna – Research in Advanced Neuro-rehabilitation (RAN), Crotone, Italy and 2Department of

Neuroscience, Ophthalmology and Genetics, University of Genova, Genova, Italy and Department of Psychiatry, State

University of New York, Stony Brook, NY, USA. E-mail: [email protected]

Included in the special issue: CONSCIOUSNESS AND THE VEGETATIVE STATE: TODAY

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 481–516

Page 6: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

© 2012 The Authors. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0939Journal Compilation © 2012 Foundation of Rehabilitation Information. ISSN 1650-1977

J Rehabil Med 44

introduCtion

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 484–486

CONSCIOUSNESS: TODAY

“To demonstrate existential characters of neurons, any theo-retically conceivable net embodying the possibility will serve.” (Pitts, McCulloch, 1948)

Consciousness is a multifaceted concept combining awareness and wakefulness. In everyday neurology it is usually equated to the waking state, and fluctuations in the level of vigilance are thought to reflect changes in brain (cortical) activation. It is believed to imply (subjective) experience and awareness of self (self-consciousness, self-detection, awareness of awareness, self-knowledge) and of environment (1). Jackson (2) restricted consciousness to the momentary relationship between the sub-ject and the object or (in his anatomical-physiological terms) to the organism adjustment to the environment. However, perception and behaviour are possible without formal aware-ness (3–8) and some sustained (self)consciousness also exists during sleep, as we remain ourselves in the most unrealistic dreams and are also aware of our dreaming (9).

Today, research on consciousness is expanding, with a major focus on its understanding in relation to cortical/brain activation or functional complexity, long-range connectivity, neuronal synchronization in selected frequency ranges, uni/multimodal perception, motor activation, focused attention, etc. The major current theories about consciousness involve large-scale information processing, social processes, or neurobiologi-cal mechanisms (1). Distinctions between consciousness and attention have been documented (10–12), with implication in the cognitive neuroscience that consciousness could be distinct from other higher brain functions (13). The brain structures and processes thought to mediate in sustaining consciousness nevertheless are identified by the impairment of varying sever-ity that results from local damage. Consciousness thus appears to be the result of a complex functional arrangement in which sustained sensory input, activation of non-specific ascending systems and primitive motor systems, activation of cortical neurones at due frequency, sensory-motor interaction, and balanced metabolism and neurotransmitters modulation are crucial (14, 15). This complex functional set-up conceivably also accounts for some specificity of the neurological signs predicting the outcome from the vegetative state (also referred to as unresponsive wakefulness syndrome) (16) and its evolu-tion into a minimally conscious state (17–23).

Further investigation is needed to define the extent to which the reported electrophysiological, functional magnetic resonance imaging, positron emission tomography scan or autonomic changes imply some specificity of response or have clinical or prognostic relevance. This caveat notwithstand-ing, neuroimaging has documented retained connectivity in segregated networks in response to stimulus conditions in both minimally conscious and vegetative state subjects, with

indication of the capability of the severely damaged brain to express surviving modular functions in the absence of the integrative processes necessary to consciousness (24–28). Although restricted to a relatively small portion of patients (29), this evidence further promoted research on the neuronal correlates of (un)consciousness (30) and expanded the clinical scenario. As a result, the vegetative and minimally conscious states appear today neither static nor homogeneous, and a tacit revision of the anatomo-functional set-ups underlying these conditions is de facto underway, warranting a formal nosographic revision of the current descriptive categories or accuracy of diagnosis (16, 31).

Regionally-mediated micro-consciousness processes have been proposed based on evidence of local neuronal organiza-tion in visual perception (32). On the other hand, increased synchronization between large neuronal populations of distinct areas related to perceptual dominance has been documented during conscious visual perception (33). The observation is consistent with evidence suggesting that neuronal activity syn-chronizes across cortical areas at conscious perception and with the theories of neural integration and complexity accounting for the properties of conscious experience and consciousness itself (13, 34–37). Long-range synchronization (e.g. in the gamma range) is thought to mediate in conscious perception (33) as it does in binding visual features and in all conditions in which neurones are selectively assembled to respond to any momentary functional requirement (38–44). However, its role in sustaining consciousness remains undocumented (45). In this respect, the major unsolved problem of biology is how billions of nerve cells work together to create consciousness (46, 47).

Consciousness is topical and is increasingly attracting scientists in neuroscience, medicine, neurocomputing, artifi-cial intelligence, and robotics. Interest is increasing with the rapid progress in the investigation of higher brain function, advances in artificial intelligence, and diffuse perception of the inadequacy of traditional mind/body separations. The issue is also crucial in methodological and bioethical controversies pertaining to medicine and public or private healthcare (16, 31, 48). However, consciousness and related terms remain to a significant extent ambiguously defined and inadequately characterized. Peculiar conditions, such as epilepsy or the vegetative and minimally conscious states, may question the correlation between wakefulness and awareness and the available computational models of brain activity (30, 49, 50). Research attempting to correlate the contents of conscious experience with representations in specific neural populations relies to a relevant extent on the linguistic neutrality of “cor-relates” when the experimental paradigms and explanatory canons of neuroscience are not neutral about the mechanical relations with the brain and are supposed to investigate causes

Included in the special issue: CONSCIOUSNESS AND THE VEGETATIVE STATE: TODAY

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 481–516

Page 7: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

485Consciousness: today

(51). A taxonomy of conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing is still needed (52).

Neuroscience has advanced to the point that it appears that we can now treat consciousness as a scientific problem like any other (53), disregarding objections that it is epiphenomenal, not evolutionary in function, unaccountable by brain proc-esses, unsuitable to objective investigated, etc. (53). To this end, a proper definition of consciousness and an up-to-date scrutiny of its descriptors are needed in order to be able to think scientifically about consciousness and to design experi-mental studies.

REFERENCES

Zeman A. Consciousness. Brain 2001; 124: 1263–1289.1. Jackson JH. On the scientific and empirical investigation of epilep-2. sies. Med Pres Cir 1876; 1: 63–65, 129–131, 173–176, 313–316.Weiskrantz L, Cowey A, Le Mare C. Learning from the pupil: a 3. spatial visual channel in the absence of V1 in monkey and human. Brain 1998; 121: 1065–1072.Grill-Spector K, Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N. The lateral occipital 4. complex and its role in object recognition. Vis Res 2001; 41: 1409–1422. Marois R, Yi DJ, Chun MM. The neural fate of consciously per-5. ceived and missed events in the attentional blink. Neuron 2004; 41: 465–472.Sergent C, Baillet S, Dehaene S. Timing of the brain events un-6. derlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink. Nat Neurosci 2005; 8: 1391–1400.Celesia GG, editor. Disorders of visual processing. In: J Daube, F 7. Mauguire, series editors. Handbook of clinical neurophysiology, vol 6. Elsevier: Amsterdam; 2005.Radoeva PD, Prasad S, Brainard DH, Aguirre GK. Neural activity 8. within area V1 reflects unconscious visual performance in a case of blindsight. J Cogn Neurosci 2008; 20: 1927–1939.Kahn D, Gover T. Consciousness in dreams. Int Rev Neurobiol 9. 2010; 92: 181–195.Koch C, Tsuchiya N. Attention and consciousness: two distinct 10. brain processes. Trends Cogn Sci 2008; 12: 44.van Boxtel JJ, Tsuchiya N, Koch C. Opposing effects of attention 11. and consciousness on afterimages. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010; 107: 8883–8888.Dienes Z, Seth AK. Measuring any conscious content versus 12. measuring the relevant conscious content: comment on Sandberg et al. Conscious Cogn 2010; 19: 1079–1080.Tononi G, Koch C. The neuronal correlates of consciousness: an 13. update. Ann NewYork Acad Sci 2008; 1124: 239–362.Dolce G, Sazbon L, editors. The posttraumatic vegetative state. 14. Stuttgart: Thiene; 2002.Dolce G, Quintieri M, Serra S, Lagani V, Pignolo L. Clinical 15. signs and early prognosis in vegetative state: a decisional tree, data-mining study. Brain Inj 2008; 22: 617–623.Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, León-Carrión J, 16. Sannita WG, et al. European Task Force on Disorders of Conscious-ness. Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med 2010; 8: 68.Multi-Society Task Force on PVS. Statement on medical as-17. pects of the persistent vegetative state. N Eng J Med 1994; 330: 1499–1508.Giacino JT, Kalmar K. The vegetative and minimally conscious 18. states: a comparison of clinical features and functional outcome. J Head Trauma Rehabil 1997; 12: 36–51.Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DJ, 19. et al. The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic

criteria. Neurology 2002; 58: 349–353.Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK Coma Recovery Scale – 20. revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2004; 85: 2020–2029.Jennett B. The vegetative state. Cambridge, UK: University 21. Press; 2002.Dolce G, Lucca LF, Rogano S, Candelieri A, Pignolo L, Sannita 22. WG. Visual pursuit in the severe disorder of consciousness. J Neurotrauma 2011; 28: 1149–1154. Monti MM, Laureys S, Owen AM. The vegetative state. BMJ 23. 2010; 341: c3765. Plum F, Schiff N, Ribary U, Llinás R. Coordinated expression in 24. chronically unconscious persons. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B, Biol Sci 1998; 353: 1929–1933.Schiff ND, Ribary U, Moreno DR, Beattie B, Kronberg E, Blas-25. berg R, et al. Residual cerebral activity and behavioral fragments can remain in the persistently vegetative brain. Brain 2002; 125: 1210–1234.Kotchoubey B, Lang S, Mezger G, Schmalohr D, Schneck M, 26. Semmler A, et al. Information processing in severe disorders of consciousness: vegetative state and minimally conscious state. Clin Neurophysiol 2005; 116: 2441–2453.Laureys S, Perrin F, Schnakers C, Boly M, Majerus S. Residual 27. cognitive function in comatose, vegetative and minimally con-scious states. Curr Opin Neurol 2005; 18: 726–733. Riganello F, Sannita WG. Residual brain processing in vegetative 28. state. J Psychophysiol 2009; 23: 18–26.Monti MM, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Coleman MR, Boly M, Pickard JD, 29. Tshibanda L, et al. Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. N Engl J Med 2010; 362: 579–589.Laureys S, Piret S, Ledoux D. Quantifying consciousness. Lancet 30. Neurol 2005; 4: 789–790.Dolce G, Sannita WG, the European Task Force On The Vegetative 31. State. The vegetative state: a syndrome seeking revision? Brain Inj 2010; 24: 1628–1629. Zeki S. The disunity of consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci 2003; 32. 7: 214–218.Srinivasan R, Russell DP, Edelman GM, Tononi G. Increased 33. synchronization in neuromagnetic responsess during conscious perception. J Neurosci 1999; 19: 5435–5438.Koch C, Laurent G. Complexity and the nervous system. Science 34. 1999; 284: 96–98.Tononi G, Edelman GM. Consciousness and complexity. Science 35. 1998; 282: 1846–1851.Tononi G. Consciousness, information integration, and the brain. 36. Progr Brain Res 2005; 150: 109–126.Moreno D, Schiff ND, Giacino J, Kalmar, K, Hirsch J. A network 37. approach to assessing cognition in disorders of consciousness. Neuroloy 2010; 75: 1871–1978.Bressler SL. The gamma wave: a cortical information carrier? 38. Trends Neurosci 1990; 13: 161–162.Engel AK, König P, Kreiter AK, Schillen TB, Singer W. Temporal 39. coding in the visual cortex: new vistas on integration in the nervous system. Trends Neurosci 1992; 15: 218–226.Gray CM. The temporal correlation hypothesis of visual feature 40. integration: still alive and well. Neuron 1999; 24: 111–125.Jefferys JG, Traub RD, Whittington MA. Neuronal networks for 41. induced ‘40 Hz’ rhythms. Trends Neurosci 1996; 19: 202–208.Singer W. Synchronization of cortical activity and its putative 42. role in information processing and learning. Ann Rev Physiol 1993; 55: 349–374.Traub RD, Jefferys JGR, Whittington MA. Fast oscillations in 43. cortical circuits. Computational neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1999.Sannita WG. Stimulus-specific oscillatory responses of the brain: 44. a time/frequency-related coding process. Clin Neurophysiol 2000; 111: 565–583.Carozzo S, Garbarino S, Serra S, Sannita WG. Function-related 45.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 8: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

486 W. G. Sannita

gamma oscillations and conscious perception. J Psychophysiol 2010; 24: 102–106.Sannita WG. Neuronal functional diversity and collective behav-46. iors. J Biol Phys 2008; 34: 267–278.Arecchi FT, Meucci R, Sannita WG, Farini A, guest editors. 47. Neuroscience today: neuronal functional diversity and collective behaviors. Cogn Proc, Suppl 1, 2009.Racine E, Amaram R, Seidle M, Karczewska M, Illes J. Media 48. coverage of the persistent vegetative state and end-of-life decision-making. Neurology 2008; 71: 1027–1032.Seth AK, Dienes Z, Cleeremans A, Overgaard M, Pessoa L. Meas-49. uring consciousness: relating behavioural and neurophysiological approaches. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008; 12: 314–321. Overgaard M, Timmermans B, Sandberg K, Cleeremans A. Op-50. timizing subjective measures of consciousness. Conscious Cogn 2010; 19: discussion 682–684.Neisser J. Neural correlates of consciousness reconsidered. Con-51. scious Cogn 2011 Apr 12 [Epub ahead of print].

Dehaene S, Changeux JP, Naccache L, Sackur J, Sergent C. 52. Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends Cogn Sci 2006; 10: 204–211.Crick F, Koch C. Consciousness and neuroscience. Cereb Cortex 53. 1998; 8: 97–107. Searle JR. How to study consciousness scientifically. Philos Trans 54. R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1998; 353: 1935–1942.

Key words: consciousness; descriptors; neuronal mechanisms; research.

Walter G. Sannita, MDFrom the Department of Neuroscience, Ophthalmology and

Genetics, University of Genova, Genova, Italy and Department of Psychiatry, State University of New York,

Stony Brook, NY, USA

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 9: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

originaL rePort

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 487–494

J Rehabil Med 44© 2012 The Authors. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0940Journal Compilation © 2012 Foundation of Rehabilitation Information. ISSN 1650-1977

METABOLIC ACTIVITY IN EXTERNAL AND INTERNAL AWARENESS NETWORKS IN SEVERELY BRAIN-DAMAGED PATIENTS

Aurore Thibaut, MSc1*, Marie-Aurélie Bruno, PhD1*, Camille Chatelle, MSc1, Olivia Gosseries, MSc1, Audrey Vanhaudenhuyse, PhD1, Athena Demertzi, MSc1, Caroline Schnakers, PhD1,

Marie Thonnard, MSc1, Vanessa Charland-Verville, MSc1, Claire Bernard, Ir2, Mohammed Ali Bahri, MD, PhD1, Christophe Phillips, Ir, PhD3, Mélanie Boly, Ir, PhD1, Roland Hustinx, MD,

PhD2 and Steven Laureys, MD, PhD1

From the 1Coma Science Group, Cyclotron Research Centre and Neurology Department, University and University Hospital of Liège, 2Nuclear Medicine Department, University Hospital of Liège and 3Cyclotron Research Centre, Univer-

sity of Liège, Liège, Belgium. *These authors contributed equally to the writing of this paper.

Objective: an extrinsic cerebral network (encompassing lateral frontoparietal cortices) related to external/sensory awareness and an intrinsic midline network related to inter-nal/self-awareness have been identified recently. This study measured brain metabolism in both networks in patients with severe brain damage. Design: Prospective [18F]-fluorodeoxyglucose-positron emis-sion tomography and Coma recovery scale-revised assess-ments in a university hospital setting.Subjects: healthy volunteers and patients in vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (vs/uWs), minimally conscious state (MCs), emergence from MCs (eMCs), and locked-in syndrome (LIS).Results: a total of 70 patients were included in the study: 24 vs/uWs, 28 MCs, 10 eMCs, 8 Lis and 39 age-matched controls. VS/UWS showed metabolic dysfunction in extrinsic and intrinsic networks and thalami. MCS showed dysfunc-tion mostly in intrinsic network and thalami. EMCS showed impairment in posterior cingulate/retrosplenial cortices. LIS showed dysfunction only in infratentorial regions. Coma Re-covery scale-revised total scores correlated with metabolic activity in both extrinsic and part of the intrinsic network and thalami.Conclusion: Progressive recovery of extrinsic and intrin-sic awareness network activity was observed in severely brain-damaged patients, ranging from vs/uWs, MCs, EMCS to LIS. The predominance of intrinsic network im-pairment in MCS could reflect altered internal/self-aware-ness in these patients, which is difficult to quantify at the bedside. Key words: vegetative state; minimally conscious state; positron emission tomography; consciousness; self-awareness; traumatic brain injury.J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 487–494

Correspondence address: Steven Laureys, Coma Science Group, Cyclotron Research Centre and Neurology Depart-ment, University and University Hospital of Liège, 4000 Liège, Belgium. E-mail: [email protected]

Submitted September 29, 2011; accepted December 5, 2011

INTRODUCTION

The assessment of consciousness in severely brain-damaged patients remains a major challenge (1). For clinicians, consciousness has two main components: arousal (i.e. wakefulness or vigilance) and awareness (i.e. comprising all subjective perceptions, feelings and thoughts) (2). Aware-ness has recently been subdivided into “external or sensory awareness” (i.e. perceptual awareness of the environment) and “internal or self awareness” (i.e. stimulus-independent thoughts, mental imagery, inner speech, daydreaming or mind wandering) (3). At the bedside, arousal is typically measured by examining eye opening. External awareness is assessed by showing the presence of reproducible command following or “non-reflex”/voluntary movements (4). After severe brain damage and the acute setting of coma, 4 different clinical entities can be disentangled: (i) patients who “awaken” but remain without reproducible signs of command following (i.e. vegetative state (VS), now also called “unresponsive wakefulness syndrome” (UWS) (5); (ii) minimally conscious state (MCS) patients showing reproducible, albeit fluctua-ting, signs of consciousness, but without functional commu-nication (6); (iii) patients who emerge from MCS (EMCS) recovering functional communication or object use (6); and (iv) locked-in syndrome (LIS) patients who are fully aware yet completely paralysed with the exception of small eye-movements permitting an eye-coded communication (7).

The behavioural assessment of consciousness in non-com-municative brain-damaged patients is difficult because move-ments can be very small, inconsistent and easily exhausted (8, 9). This issue is further complicated when patients have underlying deficits in the domain of verbal or non-verbal communication functions, such as aphasia, agnosia or apraxia (4, 10, 11). Quantifying internal or self-awareness is even more difficult than the assessment of external awareness in these patients. Most, if not all, of the employed conscious-ness scales mainly assess command-following or the presence of non-reflex movements (i.e. orientation to pain or visual pursuit) (12, 13). Regarding the latter behaviour, some scales,

Included in the special issue: CONSCIOUSNESS AND THE VEGETATIVE STATE: TODAY

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 481–516

Page 10: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

488 A. Thibaut et al.

such as the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R) (14) explicitly require the use of a mirror (15), hence possibly assessing some form of self-recognition/internal awareness. Similarly, presentation of the patient’s own name, another auto-referential attention-grabbing stimulus, has been em-ployed by some consciousness scales (e.g. the Wessex Head Injury Matrix (16)). Most behavioural scales, however, mainly, if not totally, assess external or sensory awareness and give little or no information about any possible form of internal or self-consciousness (17).

Recent studies have started to identify the neural corre-lates of internal and external awareness. An increasing body of evidence, mainly coming from functional neuroimaging (positron emission tomography (PET) and functional mag-netic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies) and electrophysio-logy point to the critical role of a widespread fronto-parietal network in the emergence of conscious awareness, also called “global neuronal workspace” (18–20). Within this widespread fronto-parietal network, two separate systems can be identi-fied: (i) an extrinsic/lateral network encompassing lateral parietal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices, mainly related to external awareness (i.e. stimulus-dependent or perceptual awareness of the environment) and (ii) an intrinsic/midline network encompassing midline precuneus/posterior cingulate and mesiofrontal/anterior cingulate cortices, mainly related to internal awareness (i.e. stimulus-independent thoughts and self-related thoughts) (3). Given our clinical limitation to objectively measure internal awareness, we here employed objective brain metabolism data obtained from PET in pa-tients with disorders of consciousness (i.e. VS/UWS, MCS, EMCS) and conscious LIS and controls, aiming to measure differences in activity in extrinsic and intrinsic network activity.

METHODSBrain metabolism was studied by means of [18F]-fluorodeoxy-glucose-PET (FDG-PET). The clinical diagnosis was based on the best response obtained by repeated CRS-R (14) assessments the day of the PET study and the two days before and after the PET acquisi-tion. We applied the diagnostic criteria, as published by the Multi Society Task Force on PVS (21), the Aspen Neurobehavioral Confer-ence Workgroup (22) and the American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine (7). Exclusion criteria for the present study were: (i) the presence of pre-morbid neurological disease; (ii) the presence of am-biguous behavioural signs not permitting reliable clinical diagnosis; (iii) the presence of large structural brain damage exceeding 25% of the whole brain volume not permitting reliable spatial normalization to the standardized stereotaxic brain template; and (iv) the absence of good quality PET data not permitting reliable image reconstruction or correction for attenuation. The control population consisted of age-matched healthy volunteers (n = 39; mean age 45 years (median 45) (range 18–80); 18 men).

FDG-PET data were acquired after intravenous injection of 5–10 mCi of FDG on a Siemens CTI 951 R16/31 scanner (as described in 23) at the University Hospital of Liege, Belgium. Data were pre-processed and analysed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) as described elsewhere

(24–26). In brief, FDG-PET data from each subject were normalized to a standard stereotactic space (using a spatial template adapted to severe brain damage, as previously described in 27) and smoothed with a 14-mm full-width half-maximum isotropic kernel. The design matrix included the VS/UWS, MCS, EMCS and LIS patients’ and control subjects’ scans. Global normalization was performed by ap-plying proportional scaling. The analyses identified brain regions where glucose metabolism was lower in each patient population compared with the control group. The resulting set of voxels values for each contrast, constituting a map of the t statistics (SPMt), was transformed to the unit normal distribution (SPMZ) and thresholded at p < 0.001. Results were considered significant at p < 0.01 family-wise correction for multiple comparisons. Next, we identified brain areas showing a linear correlation with CRS-R total scores. Here, results were thresholded for significance at p < 0.001 with small volume correction (8 mm radius) for multiple comparisons around the previously identified areas (24–26).

Informed consent was obtained from all control subjects and for LIS and EMCS patients, and from the legal representative of all non-com-municative patients. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University and University Hospital of Liege, Belgium.

RESULTS

A total of 132 patients were prospectively enrolled, of whom 62 were excluded because of: (i) pre-morbid neurological disease (8 patients); (ii) ambiguous behavioural signs not permitting reliable clinical diagnosis (12 patients); (iii) large structural brain damage (19 patients) and (iv) technical problems related to the FDG-PET acquisition (23 patients). Hence, 70 patients of the initial cohort were included for further analysis: 24 VS/UWS (mean age 51 years (median 50.5) (range 20–78); 10 men, 2 traumatic), 28 MCS (mean age 41 years (median 36.5) (range 17–81); 19 men, 16 traumatic), 10 EMCS (mean age 41 years (median 41) (range 14–76); 8 men, 4 traumatic) and 8 LIS (mean age 40 years (median 43) (range 22–53); 2 men, 1 traumatic). Patients were studied after a median of 26 months (interquartile range 24 months). Demographic and clinical data are summarized in Table I.

VS/UWS patients showed metabolic dysfunction in both thalami and in a widespread cortical network encompassing the extrinsic/lateral network (i.e. bilateral posterior parietal and prefrontal areas) and the intrinsic/medial network (i.e. the precuneus and adjacent posterior cingulate cortex and mesiof-rontal and adjacent anterior cingulate cortex), compared with controls (Fig. 1). MCS patients showed metabolic dysfunction in both thalami and in the intrinsic/medial network. EMCS pa-tients showed metabolic dysfunction in the posterior cingulate cortex and adjacent retrosplenial cortex. LIS patients showed metabolic dysfunction only in infratentorial regions (i.e. the cerebellum) (Table II).

At the group level, CRS-R total scores showed a positive correlation with a widespread cortical network encompassing both extrinsic/lateral network (i.e. bilateral posterior parietal and prefrontal areas) and part of the intrinsic/medial network (i.e. the precuneus and adjacent posterior cingulate cortex) (see Table III).

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 11: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

489Awareness network activity in severe brain damage

Table I. Patient demographic, clinical and Coma Recovery Scale-Revised subscore data

StateAge, sex Aetiology Time of PET Audition Visual Motor Verbal Comm Arousal

VS/UWS 1 30, M ARCA 25 months Startle reflex None Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

VS/UWS 2 44, M ARCA 11 days None None Abnormal posturing to pain

None None With stimulation

VS/UWS 3 69, M ARCA 24 days None None Abnormal posturing to pain

Oral reflexes None With stimulation

VS/UWS 4 62, M Anoxia 9 months Startle reflex None Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

VS/UWS 5 53, M Basilar stroke 16 days None None Flexion to pain

None None With stimulation

VS/UWS 6 34, F ARCA 18 months Startle reflex Blink to threat

Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

VS/UWS 7 47, M ARCA 55 days Startle reflex None None Oral reflexes None With stimulation

VS/UWS 8 63, F ARCA 40 months Startle reflex None Abnormal posturing to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

VS/UWS 9 65, F Anoxia 12 months Startle reflex Blink to threat

Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None With stimulation

VS/UWS 10 54, M ARCA 6 months Startle reflex None Abnormal posturing to pain

Oral reflexes None With stimulation

VS/UWS 11 42, M Anoxia 20 days Startle reflex None Abnormal posturing to pain

Vocalization None Without stimulation

VS/UWS 12 43, M ARCA 29 days Startle reflex None Abnormal posturing to pain

None None Without stimulation

VS/UWS 13 73, F Stroke 45 days Startle reflex None Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None With stimulation

VS/UWS 14 41, M ARCA 6 months Startle reflex Visual fixation

Abnormal posturing to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

VS/UWS 15 56, F ARCA 43 days Startle reflex None Abnormal posturing to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

VS/UWS 16 70, F Anoxia 52 days Startle reflex None Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None With stimulation

VS/UWS 17 49, F ARCA 4 months Startle reflex None None Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

VS/UWS 18 52, M Anoxia 10.5 months Startle reflex Blink to threat

Abnormal posturing to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

VS/UWS 19 78, F Aneurysm 32 days None None Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None None

VS/UWS 20 48, M Anoxia 30 months Startle reflex Blink to threat

Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None With stimulation

VS/UWS 21 53, M Stroke 66 days Startle reflex Blink to threat

Flexion to pain

None None Without stimulation

VS/UWS 22 46, F Traumatism 37 days Startle reflex None None Oral reflexes None With stimulation

VS/UWS 23 34, F Anoxia 260 months Startle reflex Blink to threat

Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

VS/UWS 24 20, M Traumatism 15 days None None Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None None

MCS 1 35, F Traumatism 101 months Reproducible movement to command

Visual pursuit

Flexion to pain

Vocalization None Without stimulation

MCS 2 28, F Traumatism 80 months Localization to sound

Visual pursuit

Automatic motor reaction

Vocalization None Without stimulation

MCS 3 81, F Stroke 44 days Reproducible movement to command

Object localization

Automatic motor reaction

Vocalization Intentional Without stimulation

MCS 4 37, M Traumatism 87 months Reproducible movement to command

Visual pursuit

Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes Intentional Without stimulation

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 12: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

490 A. Thibaut et al.

Table I. Condt.

StateAge, sex Etiology Time of PET Audition Visual Motor Verbal Comm Arousal

MCS 5 33, M ARCA 39.5 months Startle reflex Visual pursuit

Automatic motor reaction

Vocalization None Without stimulation

MCS 6 64, M Aneurysm 6 months Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Automatic motor reaction

Intelligible verbalization

Intentional With stimulation

MCS 7 50, F Aneurysm 28 days Startle reflex Visual pursuit

Flexion to pain Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

MCS 8 38, M Anoxia 4 months Reproducible movement to command

Visual pursuit

Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

MCS 9 81, M meningitis encephalopathy

46 days Localization ton sound

Visual pursuit

Localization to pain Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

MCS 10 19, F Traumatism 30 months Startle reflex Visual pursuit

Flexion to pain

Vocalization None Without stimulation

MCS 11 46, M Traumatism 17 months Startle reflex Visual pursuit

Flexion to pain

None None Without stimulation

MCS 12 36, M Traumatism 270 months Reproducible movement to command

Visual pursuit

Automatic motor reaction

None None Without stimulation

MCS 13 29, M Traumatism 46 days Startle reflex Visual pursuit

Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

MCS 14 50, F ARCA 65 days Reproducible movement to command

Blink to threat

Flexion to pain

Vocalization None With stimulation

MCS 15 40, M Traumatism 70 days Reproducible movement to command

Visual fixation

Localization to pain None None Without stimulation

MCS 16 50, M ARCA 7 months Reproducible movement to command

Object localization

Automatic motor reaction

Intelligible vocalization

Intentional Without stimulation

MCS 17 56, F Hydrocephaly 75 days Startle reflex Visual pursuit

None Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

MCS 18 63, F Stroke 17 days Consistent movement to command

Visual fixation

None None None With stimulation

MCS 19 17, M Traumatism 4 months Reproducible movement to command

Visual fixation

Localization to pain Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

MCS 20 32, F Anoxia 15 months Startle reflex Visual pursuit

Abnormal posturing to pain

Oral reflexes None With stimulation

MCS 21 50, M Anoxia 85 months Reproducible movement to command

Object localization

Automatic motor reaction

None Intentional With stimulation

MCS 22 23, M Traumatism 11 months Reproducible movement to command

Visual pursuit

Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

MCS 23 22, M Traumatism 99 months Startle reflex Visual fixation

Automatic motor reaction

None None Without stimulation

MCS 24 27, M Traumatism 4 months Reproducible movement to command

Visual pursuit

Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

MCS 25 30, M Traumatism 131 months Reproducible movement to command

Visual pursuit

Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None With stimulation

MCS 26 36, M Traumatism 4 months Reproducible movement to command

Visual pursuit

Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None Without stimulation

MCS 27 65, M Traumatism 21 months Reproducible movement to command

None Abnormal posturing to pain

Vocalization None With stimulation

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 13: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

491Awareness network activity in severe brain damage

Table I. Condt.

StateAge, sex Etiology Time of PET Audition Visual Motor Verbal Comm Arousal

MCS 28 23, M Traumatism 73 months Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Automatic motor reaction

Intelligible vocalization

Intentional Attention

EMCS 1 38, M ARCA 45 months Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Automatic motor reaction

Intelligible vocalization

Functional Attention

EMCS 2 45, F Traumatism 6 months Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Automatic motor reaction

Intelligible vocalization

Functional Attention

EMCS 3 32, M Traumatism 26 months Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Functional use of object

Intelligible vocalization

Functional Attention

EMCS 4 37, M ARCA 9 months Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Functional use of object

Intelligible vocalization

Functional Attention

EMCS 5 14, M Traumatism 14 months Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Functional use of object

Intelligible vocalization

Functional Attention

EMCS 6 56, M Stroke 64 days Consistent movement to command

Object localization

Functional use of object

Intelligible vocalization

Intentional Attention

EMCS 7 25, M Traumatism 9 months Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Functional use of object

Intelligible vocalization

Functional Attention

EMCS 8 44, M Stroke 7.5 months Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Functional use of object

Intelligible vocalization

Functional Attention

EMCS 9 44, M ARCA 88 days Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Automatic motor reaction

Intelligible vocalization

Functional Attention

EMCS 10 76, F Intoxication 81 days Reproducible movement to command

Object recognition

Automatic motor reaction

Intelligible vocalization

Functional Attention

LIS 1 53, M Basilar stroke 81 days Reproducible movement to command

Visual pursuit

Abnormal posturing to pain

Vocalization Intentional None

LIS 2 47, F Basilar stroke 20 days Reproducible movement to command

Object recognition

Flexion to pain Oral reflexes Intentional Without stimulation

LIS 3 39, M Traumatism 51 months Reproducible movement to command

Object recognition

Flexion to pain Oral reflexes Intentional Attention

LIS 4 44, F Basilar stroke 52 months Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Functional use of object

None Functional Attention

LIS 5 44, F Basilar stroke 19 days Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes Functional Attention

LIS 6 22, F Basilar stroke 14 days None None Flexion to pain

Oral reflexes None None

LIS 7 27, F Basilar stroke 71 months Consistent movement to command

Object recognition

Functional use of object

Intelligible vocalization

Functional Attention

LIS 8 42, F Brain stem haemorrhage

56 days Reproducible movement to command

Visual pursuit

Flexion to pain

None Intentional With stimulation

PET: positron emission tomography; VS/UWS: vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome; MCS: minimally conscious state; EMCS: emergence from MCS: LIS: locked-in syndrome; M; male; F: female; Comm: communication; ARCA: cardiac arrest.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 14: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

492 A. Thibaut et al.

form of internal/self-awareness: visual pursuit in response to a moving mirror (36).

In our view, the current data could shed some light on im-paired internal/self-awareness in MCS via the study of patients’ residual brain function. An increasing body of evidence points to the critical role of the intrinsic network in the emergence of internal/self-awareness including stimulus-independent cogni-tive processes, such as daydreaming, mental imagery, inner speech and self-oriented thoughts (37–40). In fMRI studies, the latter network, recorded during the so-called “resting state” condition has also been coined “default mode network” (41–43). In both VS/UWS and MCS patients a significant thalamic metabolic impairment was identified, in line with previous PET (29, 30, 44) and diffusion tensor imaging (45) MRI studies, and post-mortem neuropathology (46). This finding can also be related to the clinical observation that both patient groups have fluctuating arousal levels. Indeed, in our cohort 10 out of 24 (42%) VS/UWS and 7 out of 28 (25%) MCS showed CRS-R

DISCUSSION

Our results in VS/UWS of different aetiologies show a wide-spread fronto-parietal cortical dysfunction, in agreement with previous studies (9, 28–30). We observed a hypometabolism in the external network encompassing left and right lateral parietal and lateral prefrontal cortices and in the internal network encompassing midline precuneus/posterior cingu-late and mesiofrontal/anterior cingulate cortices. In MCS patients it seems that the extrinsic/lateral network is less impaired than is the intrinsic/medial network. This result is consistent with the clinical finding that these patients show evidence of external/sensory awareness, known to depend upon the functional integrity of the extrinsic/lateral fronto-parietal system (3, 31–35). The predominance of intrinsic/midline network impairment in MCS could reflect an impaired internal/self-awareness in these patients, which is very difficult to quantify at the bedside. Indeed, CRS-R assessments only have one item possibly assessing some

Table II. Coordinates of peak voxels of hypometabolic areas in vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (VS/UWS), minimally conscious state (MCS), emergence from MCS (EMCS) and locked-in syndrome (LIS)

Areas x (mm) y (mm) z (mm) Z p

VS/UWSRight thalamus 8 –18 4 5.21 < 0.0001Left thalamus –2 16 2 4.94 < 0.0001Right lateral parietal 50 18 0 4.5 < 0.0001Left lateral parietal –38 –72 42 7.29 < 0.0001Right lateral prefrontal 52 –4 52 Inf < 0.0001Left lateral prefrontal –34 4 54 7.56 < 0.0001Precuneus/posterior cingulate

2 –36 34 Inf < 0.0001

Mesiofrontal/anterior cingulate

2 –36 34 Inf < 0.0001

MCSRight thalamus 4 –18 2 7.37 < 0.0001Left thalamus –4 –20 2 4.2 < 0.0001Precuneus/posterior cingulate

0 –36 32 Inf < 0.0001

Mesiofrontal/anterior cingulate

6 18 30 6.22 < 0.0001

EMCSPosterior cingulate/restrosplenial

–2 –48 22 5.49 < 0.0001

LISCerebellum –38 –68 –38 3.88 < 0.0001

Inf: inferior than 0.0001.

Table III. Coordinates of peak voxels from areas showing a linear positive correlation with Coma Recovery Scale-Revised total scores

Regions x (mm) y (mm) z (mm) Z p

Right lateral parietal 50 18 0 4.5 <0.0001Left lateral parietal –58 –50 38 4.85 <0.0001Right lateral prefrontal 52 –4 52 Inf <0.0001Left lateral prefrontal –34 4 54 7.56 <0.0001Precuneus/posterior cingulate 2 –36 34 Inf <0.0001

Inf: inferior than 0.0001.

Fig. 1. Areas with significant metabolic impairment (blue) in vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (VS/UWS, n = 24), minimally conscious state (MCS, n = 28), emergence from MCS (EMCS, n = 10) and locked-in syndrome (LIS, n = 8) compared with age-matched controls (n = 39) (thresholded at p < 0.01 family-wise correction for multiple comparisons). The lower panel shows the areas where metabolic activity correlated with Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R) scores (thresholded at uncorrected p < 0.001; red). Note that in VS/UWS there is a metabolic dysfunction in the thalamus (T) external network encompassing left and right lateral parietal (LP) and lateral prefrontal (LF) cortices and in the internal network encompassing midline precuneus/posterior cingulate (MP) and mesiofrontal/anterior cingulate (MF) cortices. In MCS the thalamus (T) and intrinsic network is impaired (MP, MF). EMCS shows partly impaired intrinsic network activity (MP) and LIS fully preserved awareness networks, with only impairment in the cerebellum (C). The behavioural assessment scores correlate with activity in the extrinsic network (LP, LF) and part of the intrinsic network (MP).

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 15: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

493Awareness network activity in severe brain damage

arousal subscores of 1, meaning that patients needed tactile or noxious stimulation at least once during the examination in order to obtain sustained eye opening (47).

EMCS patients showed a near-normal brain metabolism with preserved extrinsic network activity and only dysfunc-tion of posterior cingulate cortex and adjacent retrosplenial cortex. This area, part of the intrinsic network, is known to be involved in autobiographical memory and self-reflexion (48, 49). Clinically, EMCS patients indeed classically experi-ence confusion and amnesia syndromes (50, 51). Finally, our studied LIS patients failed to show metabolic dysfunction in any supratentorial brain area. Both the extrinsic and intrinsic network activity was preserved in LIS and only the cerebellum was shown to be impaired, in line with previous studies (52, 53). Previous neuropsychological studies have indeed shown that classical LIS patients have no deficit in cognitive func-tioning (54). Despite the fact that 6/8 LIS patients experienced basilar artery stroke and showed structural lesions on MRI in the ventral pontine region (encompassing the corticospinal and adjacent corticobulbar pathways) the resulting metabolic impairment was localized not in the brainstem, but in the cerebellum. This can be explained by the fact that PET-FDG functional imaging, in contrast to MRI structural imaging, does not show white matter structural damage (i.e. in brainstem), but rather the cortical metabolic consequences (i.e. in cerebellar hemispheres), reflecting de-afferentation.

The observed progressive recovery of intrinsic network metabolic activity, as measured by FDG-PET in severely brain-damaged patients, ranging from VS/UWS, MCS, EMCS to LIS, corroborates previous fMRI “resting state” studies showing a pro-gressive recovery of functional connectivity in the “default mode network” in these patients (55). The latter study also identified a linear correlation between CRS-R total scores and functional con-nectivity in the default mode network. We expand these findings here, showing an additional correlation with the extrinsic/lateral network metabolic activity and CRS-R total scores.

In conclusion, the objective measurement of extrinsic/lateral and intrinsic/midline metabolic activity in severely brain-injured patients following coma, permits us to better understand the residual external/sensory and internal/self-awareness in disorders of consciousness. Our data show, for the first time, that patients with MCS, in contrast to those with VS/UWS, show cortical dysfunction of the intrinsic/internal awareness system more than of the extrinsic/external awareness networks. If confirmed, these findings indicate an impairment of a clinically barely measurable dysfunction of internal or self-awareness in MCS.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This study was supported by the Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique (FRS), Fonds pour la Recherche Industrielle et Agronomique (FRIA), French Speaking Community Concerted Research Action, University and University Hospital of Liege, James S. McDonnell Foundation, Mind Science Foundation and European Commission (Mindbridge, DISCOS, DECODER & COST).

REFERENCES

Schnakers C, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Giacino J, Ventura M, Boly 1. M, Majerus S, et al. Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: Clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment. BMC Neurol 2009; 9: 35.Zeman A. Consciousness. Brain 2001; 124: 1263–1289.2. Vanhaudenhuyse A, Demertzi A, Schabus M, Noirhomme Q, 3. Bredart S, Boly M, et al. Two distinct neuronal networks mediate the awareness of environment and of self. J Cogn Neurosci 2011; 23: 570–578.Majerus S, Gill-Thwaites H, Andrews K, Laureys S. Behavioral 4. evaluation of consciousness in severe brain damage. Prog Brain Res 2005; 150: 397–413.Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, Leon-Carrion J, San-5. nita WG, et al. Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med 2010; 8: 68.Giacino J, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz D, 6. et al. The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology 2002; 58: 349–353.American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine Recommenda-7. tions for use of uniform nomenclature pertinent to patients with severe alterations of consciousness Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1995; 76: 205–209.Gosseries O, Bruno M, Chatelle C, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers 8. C, Soddu A, et al. Disorders of consciousness: what’s in a name? NeuroRehabilitation 2011; 28: 3–14.Bruno M, Gosseries O, Ledoux D, Hustinx R, Laureys S. Assess-9. ment of consciousness with electrophysiological and neurological and neurological imaging techniques. Curr Opin Crit Care 2011; 17: 146–151.Majerus S, Bruno MA, Schnakers C, Giacino JT, Laureys S. The 10. problem of aphasia in the assessment of consciousness in brain-damaged patients. Prog Brain Res 2009; 177: 49–61.Bruno M-A, Fernández-Espejo D, Lehembre R, Tshibanda L, 11. Vanhaudenhuyse A, Gosseries O, et al. Multimodal neuroimaging in patients with disorders of consciousness showing “functional hemispherectomy”. Prog Brain Res 2011; 193: 323–333. Bruno M, Ledoux D, Lambermont B, Damas F, Laureys S. Com-12. parison of the Full Outline of Responsiveness (FOUR), the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) and the Glasgow Liege Scale (GLS) in an inten-sive care unit population. Neurocrit Care 2011; 15: 447–453.Schnakers C, Giacino J, Kalmar K, Piret S, Lopez E, Boly M, et 13. al. Does the FOUR score correctly diagnose the vegetative and minimaly conscious states? Ann Neurol 2006; 60: 744–745.Giacino J, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK Coma Recovery Scale-14. revised. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2004; 85: 2020–2029.Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Brédart S, Laureys S. Assess-15. ment of visual pursuit in post-comatose states: use a mirror. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008; 79: 223.Shiel A, Horn S, Wilson B, Watson M, Campbell M, McLellan D. 16. The Wessex Head Injury Matrix (WHIM) main scale: a preliminary report on a scale to assess monitor patient recovery after severe head injury. Clin Rehabil 2000; 14: 408–416.Laureys S, Perrin F, Brédart S. Self-consciousness in non-commu-17. nicative patients. Conscious Cogn 2007; 16: 722–741.Baars B, Ramsoy T, Laureys S. Brain, conscious experience and 18. the observing self. Trends Neurosci 2003; 26: 671–675.Dehaene S, Naccache L. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of 19. consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cog-nition 2001; 79: 1–37.Laureys S. The neural correlate of (un)awareness: lessons from the 20. vegetative state. Trends Cogn Sci 2005; 9: 556–559.The Multi-Society Task Force on PVS. Medical aspects of the per-21. sistent vegetative state. N Engl J Med 1994; 330: 1499–1508.Giacino JT. Disorders of consciousness: differential diagnosis and 22. neuropathologic features. Semin Neurol 1997; 17: 105–111.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 16: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

494 A. Thibaut et al.

Laureys S, Faymonville M, Moonen G, Luxen A, Maquet P. PET 23. scanning and neuronal loss in acute vegetative state. Lancet 2000; 355: 1825–1826.Laureys S. Functional neuroimaging in the vegetative state. Neuro-24. Rehabilitation 2004; 19: 335–341.Laureys S, Lemaire C, Maquet P, Phillips C, Franck G. Cerebral 25. metabolism during vegetative state and after recovery to conscious-ness. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999; 67: 121.Bruno M, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Boly M, Gosseries O, 26. Demertzi A, et al. Visual fixation in the vegetative state: an obser-vational case series PET study. BMC Neurol 2010; 10: 35.Phillips C, Bruno M, Maquet P, Boly M, Noirhomme Q, Schnakers 27. C, et al. “Relevance vector machine” consciousness classifier ap-plied to cerebral metabolism of vegetative and locked-in patients. Neuroimage 2011; 56: 797–808.Laureys S, Goldman S, Phillips C, Van Bogaert P, Aert J, Luxen 28. A, et al. Impaired effective cortical connectivity in vegetative state: preliminary investigation using PET. Neuroimage 1999; 9: 377–382.Nakayama N, Okumura A, Shinoda J, Nakashima T, Iwama T. Re-29. lationship between regional cerebral metabolism and consciousness disturbance in traumatic diffuse brain injury without large focal lesions: an FDG-PET study with statistical parametric mapping analysis. JNNP 2006; 77: 856–862.Juengling F, Kassubek J, Huppertz H, Krause T, Els T. Separating 30. functional and structural damage in persistent vegetative state us-ing combined voxel-based analysis of 3-D MRI and FDG-PET. J Neurol Sci 2005; 228: 179–184.Boly M, Balteau E, Schnakers C, Degueldre C, Moonen G, 31. Luxen A, et al. Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict somato-sensory perception in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2007; 104: 12187–12192.Bornhovd K, Quante M, Glauche V, Bromm B, Weiller C, Buchel 32. C. Painful stimuli evoke different stimulus-response functions in the amygdala, prefrontal, insula and somatosensory cortex: a single trial fMRI study. Brain 2002; 125: 1326–1336.Fuhrmann A, Hein G, Tsai N, Naumer M, Knight R. Temporal 33. characteristic of audiovisual information processing. J Neurosci 2008; 28: 5344–5349.Rees G. Neural correlates of the contents of visual awareness in 34. humans. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Biol Sci 2007; 362:877–886.Sergent C, Dehaene S. Neural process underlying conscious per-35. ception: experimental findings and global neuronal workspace framework. J Physiol Paris 2004; 98: 374–384.Vanhaudenhuyse A, Giacino J, Schnakers C, Kalmar K, Smart C, 36. Bruno M, et al. Blink to visual threat does not herald consciousness in the vegetative state. Neurology 2008; 71: 1374–1375.Mason M, Norton M, Van Horn J, Wegner D, Grafton S, Marcrae 37. C. Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 2007; 315: 393–395.Knauff M, Fangmeier T, Ruff C, Johnson-Laird P. Reasoning, 38. models, and images: behavioral measures and cortical activity. J Cogn Neurosci 2003; 15: 559–573.Lou H, Luber B, Crupain M, Keenan J, Nowak M, Kjaer T, et al. 39. Parietal cortex and representation of mental self. Proc Natl Acad

Sci 2004; 101: 6827–6832.Goldberg I, Harel M, Malach R. When the brain loses its self: 40. prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing. Neuron 2006; 50: 329–339.Raichle M, MacLeod A, Snyder A, Powers W, Gusnard D, Shul-41. man G. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2001; 98: 676–682.Boly M, Phillips C, Tshibanda J, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schabus 42. M, Dang-Vu T, et al. Intrinsic brain activity in altered states of consciousness: how consciousness is the default mode of brain function? Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008; 1129: 119–129.Soddu A, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Bahri M, Bruno M, Boly M, 43. Demertzi A, et al. Identifying the default-mode component in spatial IC analyses of patients with disorders of consciousness. Hum Brain Mapp 2011 Apr 11 doi: 10.1002/hbm.21249. [Epub ahead of print].Lull N, Noé E, Lull J, Garcia-Panach J, Chirivella J, Ferri J, et al. 44. Voxel-based statistical analysis of thalamic glucose metabolism in traumatic brain injury: relationship with consciousness and cognition. Brain Inj 2010; 24:1098–1107.Fernandez-Espejo D, Bekinschtein T, Monti M, Pickard J, Junque 45. C, Coleman M, et al. Diffusion weighted imaging distinguishes the vegetative state from the minimally conscious state. Neuroimage 2010; 54: 103–112.Graham D, Adams J, Murrays L, Jennet B. Neuropathology of 46. vegetative state after head injury. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2005; 15: 198–213.Cologan V, Schabus M, Ledoux D, Moonen G, Maquet P, Laureys 47. S. Sleep in disorders of consciousness. Sleep Med Rev 2010; 14: 97–105.Vogt B, Vogt L, Laureys S. Cytology and functionnally correlated 48. circuits of human posterior cingulate areas. Neuroimage 2006; 29: 452–466.Vogt B, Laureys S. Posterior cingulate, precuneal and retrospinal 49. cortices: cytology and components of the neural network correlates of consciousness. Prog Brain Res 2005; 150: 205–217.Machado C, Perez-Nellar J, Rodriguez R, Scherle C, Korein J. 50. Emergence from minimally conscious state: insights from evalu-ation of posttraumatic confusion. Neurology 2009; 74: 1156.Katz D, Polyak M, Coughlan D, Nichols M, Roche A. Natural 51. history of recovery from brain injury after prolonged disorders of consciousness: outcome of patients admitted to inpatient rehabilita-tion with 1–4 year follow-up. Prog Brain Res 2009; 177: 73–88.Laureys S, Owen A, Schiff N. Brain function in coma, vegetative 52. state, and related disordres. Lancet Neurol. 2004; 3: 537–546.Laureys S, Pellas F, Van Eeckhout P, Ghorbel S, Schnakers C, Perrin 53. F, et al. The locked-in syndrome: what is it like to be conscious but paralysed and voiceless? Prog Brain Res 2005; 150: 495–511. Schnakers C, Majerus S, Goldman S, Boly M, Van Eeckhout P, Gay 54. S, et al. Cognitive function in the locked-in syndrome. J Neurol 2008; 255: 323–330.Vanhaudenhuyse A, Noirhomme Q, Tshibanda J, Bruno M, Ledoux 55. D, Brichant J, et al. Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-communicative brain-damaged patients. Brain 2010; 133: 161–171.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 17: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

revieW artiCLe

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 495–501

J Rehabil Med 44© 2012 The Authors. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0975Journal Compilation © 2012 Foundation of Rehabilitation Information. ISSN 1650-1977

HEART RATE VARIABILITY AND THE CENTRAL AUTONOMIC NETWORK IN SEVERE DISORDER OF CONSCIOUSNESS

Francesco Riganello, PhD1, Giuliano Dolce, MD1 and Walter G Sannita, MD2,3

From the 1S. Anna Institute and RAN – Research in Advanced Neurorehabilitation, Crotone, 2Department of Neu-roscience, Ophthalmology and Genetics, University of Genova, Genova, Italy and 3Department of Psychiatry, State

University of New York, Stony Brook, NY, USA

Objective: to review the applicability of heart rate variabili-ty measures in research on severe disorder of consciousness.Methods: the available evidence on the correlation between heart rate variability measures and the outcome or residual functional state/responsiveness of severely brain-injured pa-tients (including those in vegetative or minimally conscious states) are reviewed and discussed with reference to the cen-tral autonomic network model.Results and conclusion: heart rate variability analyses ap-pear to be applicable to assess residual or emerging (higher level) function in brain-injured patients with disordered con-sciousness and to predict outcome. In this regard, the central autonomic network model is heuristic in the understanding of heart rate variability descriptors of the central nervous system/autonomic systems relationship.Key words: disorder of consciousness; brain injury; heart rate variability; HRV; vegetative state; minimally conscious state; central autonomic network.J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 495–501

Correspondence address: Giuliano Dolce, S. Anna Institute and RAN – Research in Advanced Neurorehabilitation, Crotone, Italy. E-mail: [email protected] September 29, 2011; accepted February 16, 2012

INTRODUCTION

Subjects in a vegetative state (VS; today also referred to as unresponsive wakefulness syndrome) after severe brain injury are, by definition, disconnected from the environment, with no indication of awareness, voluntary or otherwise, purpose-ful movement, or communication (1–5). Autonomic functions are thought to prevail on central nervous system activities. In contrast, research by advanced positron emission tomography (PET) or functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) techniques has documented stimulus- or condition-related regional brain activation that reflects retained connectiv-ity in segregated networks. These observations are deemed indicative of surviving sensory, emotional and “cognitive” modular processing at varying levels of functional complex-ity in the absence of the integrative processes necessary to consciousness (5–14). The clinical scenario and perspective have expanded significantly, with far-reaching implications and requirements as to healthcare and neurorehabilitation

of subjects in the VS. Emerging evidence suggests that the autonomic system can also mediate in patterns of brain acti-vation at varying levels of complexity, and measures of heart rate variability (HRV) are applicable in the description of the brain functional organization in homeostasis and homeostatic response (15–18).

METHODSThe US National Library of Medicine Database and Google Scholar databases were used to trace published reports on HRV, VS, minimally conscious state (MCS), and autonomic system/function over the pe-riod 1993–2011, using appropriate keywords and their combinations. Cohort studies, case control studies, case reports and case series of adult or paediatric brain-injured patients were included in this review. Animal studies were not included.

HEART RATE VARIABILITY: MEASURES AND MEASUREMENTS

Measures of the HRV reportedly indicate or anticipate car-diac disorders (19–21) and reflect the action of physiologi-cal factors modulating the heart rhythm and its adaptation to changing conditions. The dynamic interplay between the autonomic subsystems enables efficient cardiovascular re-sponses to endogenous/exogenous influences (22–24) and the efficiency of these responses can be quantified by appropriate data processing.

HRV recording techniques are non-invasive and HRV signals (the heart tachogram, i.e. the variation over time of the interval between consecutive heartbeats) have excellent signal-to-noise ratio compared with most brain signals in use in neuroscience or clinical neurophysiology, but are not periodic. Stimulus- or condition-related changes occur within the heart rate physio-logical range of variability in the absence of cardiac disorders and are seldom detectable without appropriate data treatment. To this purpose, the tachogram needs processing in the time or frequency domains or by geometrical or non-linear methods, as suggested by the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology (25, 26). HRV fluctuations are conventionally measured in the time domain by calculating indices based on statistical operations on RR intervals; fast Fourier transform (FFT) or autoregressive models (26) are of common use in analyses of frequency. The

Included in the special issue: CONSCIOUSNESS AND THE VEGETATIVE STATE: TODAY

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 481–516

Page 18: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

496 F. Riganello et al

HRV spectral profile is characterized by 3 main components: the high-frequency interval (0.15–0.5 Hz; HF), mainly associ-ated with activation of the parasympathetic nervous system; the low-frequency interval (0.04–0.15 Hz; LF), reflecting contributions from both the parasympathetic and sympathetic systems; and the very-low-frequency bandwidth (< 0.04 Hz; VLF), thought to reflect temperature, vasomotor, hormonal, and metabolic regulation. The LF/HF ratio is typically used as a measure of the sympathovagal balance.

HRV descriptors are also derived by non-linear methods, such as entropy analysis, in order to describe the complex-ity, irregularity or randomness of HRV and its changes (27–30). Developments in the non-linear analysis theories provide new instruments of the data analysis in the entropy domain, such as the approximate entropy (ApEn) and the simple entropy (SapEn), which are thought to provide global information on autonomic system functioning and complex-ity (Table I).

HRV measures are now being regarded with increasing in-terest as reliable descriptors of autonomic reaction to events with emotional resonance, and there is evidence that HRV can reflect the CNS/autonomic functional interaction under conditions involving motor, cognitive, emotional, behavioural or stressful tasks or adaptation to environmental change (16, 27, 31, 32). Clinical application is mainly in the investigation of subjects with psychiatric disorders, traumatic brain injury (TBI), impaired emotion-specific processing, and personality or communication disorders (33–41). The (partial) independ-ence of HRV parameters from conscious experience also makes application possible when the requirements for active collaboration need to be limited (e.g. during monitoring) or continuous collaboration is questionable even in simple experimental paradigms (e.g. in subjects with severe brain damage). In this respect, the approach appears to be suitable for privileged application in the study of subjects with severe disorder of consciousness, such as those in a VS or MCS.

HEART RATE VARIABILITY AND BRAIN INJURY

Two patterns of autonomic hyperactivity have been described, namely a paroxysmal sympathetic hyperactivity in the absence of parasympathetic major contribution, and the combined sympathetic/parasympathetic hyperactivity (“mixed autonomic hyperactivity disorders”) (42). Non-neurological organ dys-function (with paroxysmal sympathetic hyperactivity resulting in respiratory/cardiovascular dysfunction) seems to be associ-ated with brain injury (43, 44) and the risk of death increases in patients with severe cardiac uncoupling and depressed HRV (45, 46). Sympathetic hyperactivity and over-responsiveness to afferent stimuli have been observed in a HRV study on TBI patients with dysautonomia (42, 47–50). A parallel increase in the vagal activity and intracranial pressure (possibly due to compression of the vagal nuclei or brainstem) has been documented in patients changes in the LF power (51–53). A significant decrease in the LF/HF ratio was observed in TBI children at intracranial pressure above 30 mmHg (54). Lowensohn et al. (55) observed a HRV decrease with rising intracranial pressures in subjects with severe brain injury. Subacute studies have shown comparable changes in the LF/HF ratio compared with controls or a decrease in the HF power (56, 57) (Table II).

HEART RATE VARIABILITY AND PREDICTION OF OUTCOME

HRV has been proposed as a useful predictor of outcome in brain-injured patients (27, 58, 59). Reduced LF/HF ratios have been associated with low scores on the Glasgow Coma Scale and increased risk of brain death (54). A correlation between LF, severity of neurological dysfunction and outcome has been reported in TBI children (60, 61) and adults (62). The global HRV and parasympathetic tone were higher in TBI patients who later died than in those who survived; during the awak-

Table I. Heart rate variability (HRV) measures

HRV analyses Description Output variables

Time domain Statistical processing of consecutive intervals HR, SDHR, NN, SDNN, RMSDD SDNN, pNN50Frequency distribution TINN (baseline width of the RR interval histogram), HRV triangular

index (integral of the RR interval histogram divided by the height of the histogram)

Frequency domain Frequency spectrum FFT and AutoRegressive AnalysisPower: Total, ULF (< 0.003 Hz), VLF (0.003–0.04 Hz), LF (0.04–0.15 Hz), HF (0.15–0.4 Hz), Normalized Unit (LF, HF)Time spectrum analysis

Non-linear analyses Detrended fluctuation analysis (measures the correlation within the signal)

Typically the correlations are divided into short-term (α1) and long-term (α2) fluctuations

Poincare plot (graphical representation of the correlation between successive RR intervals)

SD1 (short-term variability)SD2 (long-term variability)

Entropy Measures of the complexity or irregularity of the signal (ApEn, SampEn)

ApEn: approximate entropy; SampEn: sample entropy; pNN50: proportion greater than 50 ms; RMSDD: root mean square of standard deviation; SD: standard deviation; SDNN: standard deviation of 5 min means; HF: high frequency; ULF: ultralow frequency; VLF: very low frequency; LF: low frequency; FFT: fast Fourier transform; HR: heart rate.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 19: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

497HRV in severe disorder of consciousness

ing period, the global HRV and parasympathetic tone were lower in those patients whose neurological condition later worsened compared with patients with a good recovery (58, 62–64). Attenuated parasympathetic tonus and low HF were found to correlate with the severity of brainstem damage, while very low LF and HF power was associated with progression towards brain death in TBI patients (65). Amelioration of the HRV total power in the first 3 months after TBI was correlated with recovery of autonomic function in a prospective study (56). Changes in autonomic reactivity, namely decrease in parasympathetic activity (normalized unit of high-frequency (nuHF)) and increase in sympathetic activity (normalized unit of low-frequency (nuLF)), were found to parallel the recovery of consciousness in TBI patients (66) (Table III).

HEART RATE VARIABILITY AND RESPONSIVENESS

HRV measures are used to assess the contributions of the autonomic nervous system in sustaining consciousness and its functional re-organization during recovery in subjects with severe disorder of consciousness. The nuLF descriptor of sympathetic activity was found to increase in VS subjects interacting with relatives (the “mom effect”) (Fig. 1) in the absence of any activation in control conditions (67). Higher HRV and HF values were recorded in a comparable study (68, 69), with minor differences conceivably depending on different stimulus paradigms and HRV data processing (70). Consistent patterns of variation in HRV (e.g. in the nuLF values) were observed in healthy controls and TBI patients

Table II. Heart rate variability (HRV) and brain injury

Author Subjects Results

Perkes et al., 2010 (42) 349 The core clinical features of PSH-heart rate were correlated with, blood pressure, respiratory rate, temperature, sweating, and motor hyperactivity.

Riordan et al., 2007 (46) 4,116 Reduced HRV was associated with an increase in mortality; beta B exposure appears associated with increased survival across all stratifications of cardiac uncoupling.

Riordan et al., 2009 (45) 2,178 Reduced HR multiscale entropy was significantly associated with increasing mortality and is a reliable predictor of mortality in TBI patients.

Baguley et al., 2009 (50) 27 HRV measures differentiate between (TBI) subjects with normal and elevated autonomic activity. HRV and event-related heart rate changes help in the diagnosis of dysautonomia. The comparison of HRV and heart rate parameters suggested an over-responsivity to nociceptive stimuli in dysautonomic subjects.

Kawahara et al., 2003 (51) 42 HRV analysis showed enhanced parasympathetic activity, probably associated with increased intracranial pressure in patients with acute subarachnoid haemorrhage.

Mowery et al., 2008 (51) 291 Cardiac uncoupling increases with ICP, cardiac uncoupling and ICH predict mortality.Morris et al., 2006 (53) 1,425 Reduced heart rate variability is a new biomarker reflecting the loss of command and control of the

heart (cardiac uncoupling).Keren et al., 2005 (56) 20 Change towards HRV normalization predicts recovery of the autonomic nervous system in patients

with TBI.

HR: heart rate; PSH: paroxysmal sympathetic hyperactivity; TBI: traumatic brain injury; ICP: intracranial pressure; ICH intracranial hypertension.

Table III. Heart rate variability (HRV) and prediction of outcome

Author Subjects Results

King et al., 2009 (58) 75 HRV triages and discriminates the severely brain injured patients during helicopter transport better than routine trauma criteria or en-route pre-hospital vital signs.

Cooke et al., 2006 (59) 84 Heart period variability analyses discriminate patients with poor prognosis (death) from those surviving TBI.

Biswas et al., 2000 (54) 15 HRV power spectral analysis (e.g. LF/HF ratio) as a useful ancillary test in determining the severity of brain insult and prognosis in children with traumatic brain injury.

Goldstein et al., 1993 (60) 11 Damaged sympathetic cardiovascular system in children with severe brain injury and complete interruption of the autonomic cardiovascular pathways in brain death.

Goldstein et al., 1996 (61) 36 Sequential changes in heart rate, respiratory rate, blood pressure, heart rate power spectra, and plasma catecholamine concentrations in patients with acute brain injury identify disruption of the autonomic nervous system control on heart rate proportionally to the degree of neurological insult in children with brain injury.

Rapenne et al., 2001 (62) 20 HRV provides useful information in the early prognosis of patients with severe brain trauma.Norris et al., 2005 (64) 1,316 HRV independently predicts death in TBI patients and detects early differences in the mortality rate of

groups of patients.

LF: low frequency; HF: high frequency; TBI: traumatic brain injury.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 20: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

498 F. Riganello et al

listening to classical music of different authorship aimed at evoking distinct emotional responses. The responses were classified as “positive” or “negative” based on the controls’ subjective reports; the nuLF patterns during listening differed from baseline and among musical samples, with a relationship with the music structure (71). Changes in the HRV patterns comparable to those observed in brain-injured subjects and in controls were detected in the same experimental conditions in subjects unambiguously diagnosed as being in a VS (72–74) and a relationship was observed between the HRV nuLF and LF peak and the occurrence of a visual pursuit response, a neurological marker of the subject’s evolution from the VS to the MCS (75–77) (Table IV).

HEART RATE VARIABILITY AND THE CENTRAL AUTONOMIC NETWORK

The central control of autonomic function and the complex interplay between the CNS and the autonomic system and between the sympathetic and parasympathetic subsystems

is modulated by direct/indirect descending, ascending and bidirectional connections among neural structures (24, 78, 79). A functional integrated model (usually referred to as the central autonomic network, or CAN) has been proposed and would include cortical components (medial prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and insular cortex), the paraventricular, amygdala central and lateral hypothalamic nuclei, and structures in the midbrain (the periacqueductal gray region) and pons (nucleus of the tractus solitarius, nucleus ambiguus and ventrolateral medulla), with primary outputs from stellate ganglia and vagus nerve to the sinoatrial node of the heart (24, 31) (Fig. 2). Tel-encephalic structures are connected with the hypothalamus and brainstem and contribute in the control of the autonomic or-

Table IV. Heart rate variability (HRV) and responsiveness

Author Subjects Results

Wijnen et al., 2006 (66) 16 TBI subjects Autonomic reactivity provides useful information on the severely damaged brain responsiveness to environmental changes.

Dolce et al., 2008 (67) 12 VS subjects HRV changes in response to a relative’s presence or voice (the “mom effect”) suggest residual rudimentary personal interaction in VS subjects.

Gutiérrez et al., 2010 (68) Case report Auditory stimulation induced recordable changes in HRV in VS subjects, suggesting residual preserved cognitive function detectable by cardiovascular descriptors.

Machado et al., 2011 (69) Case report Changes of HRV related to the emotional response to the mom’s voice (the “mom effect”).

Riganello et al., 2011 (70) 12 VS subjects Modifications in the HRV (nuLF) in response to emotional stimuli (voice of relatives), but not to controls.

Riganello et al., 2008 (71) 16 TBI subjects26 healthy controls

HRV described autonomic concomitants of emotional responses to complex sensory stimuli with emotional relevance (symphonic music).

Riganello et al., 2010 (72) 9 VS subjects16 healthy controls

Comparable autonomic changes with emotional relevance were induced by complex stimuli (music) in VS subjects and controls.

Candelieri et al., 2011 (77) 7 VS subjects8 MCS subjects

Two parameters obtained by HRV analysis (nuLF and peak of LF) proved highly correlated to eye-tracking.

TBI: tramatic brain injury; VS: vegetative state; nuLF: normalized unit of low-frequency.

Fig. 2. Schematic outline of the central autonomic network (adapted from Benarroch (92)).

Fig. 1. The “mom’s effect” in a subject in the vegetative state (VS): heart rate variability measures (fast Fourier transform (FFT) and auto regressive (AR)) in resting condition (baseline), while the subject’s mother was trying a personal interaction (test condition) and with an unfamilial person repeating the mother’s approach (control) (71).

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 21: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

499HRV in severe disorder of consciousness

ganization (24, 80). The insula (visceromotor area) is involved in the control of sympathetic and parasympathetic outputs (via a relay in the lateral hypothalamic area and through the amygdala) and in the autonomic and endocrine responses and motor activation needed to express the emotional response (78). The anterior cingulated cortex and its projections to the prefrontal cortex, amygdala, hypothalamus and brainstem are involved in the modulation of autonomic output in response to pain and emotional or behaviourally significant stimuli (81). The hypothalamus is thought to integrate autonomic and endocrine responses and to sustain vital homeostatic mecha-nisms, such as thermoregulation, osmoregulation, response to stress, etc. (82).

The CAN is essentially a dynamic system, with its activ-ity depending on initial state (83). A functional relationship between HRV measures, the CAN operational status and the activity in the neural structures involved in affective and autonomic regulation has been first suggested by Thayler (84–86). Parasympathetic activation decreases the firing rate of pacemaker cells and HR, while sympathetic activity results in an increase of HR and firing rate of the pacemaker cells in the heart sinoatrial node (87). Autonomic, attentional, and af-fective systems can be integrated in a functional model with the cardiac vagal tone (23, 88, 89). The autonomic nervous system, in general, and the CAN, in particular, are thought to be indexed by HRV measures.

CONCLUSION

HRV is an output measure with potentially wide applica-tion, but its use in neuroscience and medicine is occasion-ally questioned (90–92). A number of autonomic functional tests, including plasma and urinary catecholamines, provide indirect information on the sympathetic or parasympathetic function (93), and direct measures of sympathetic activity have been obtained from the cardiac norepinephrine spillover and by microneurographic techniques or direct recording from skeletal muscle (94–95). However, these approaches are invasive and inapplicable on large subjects’ samples, and only indirect methods are available today to obtain informa-tion on the parasympathetic system (96, 97). In this respect, HRV methodologies benefit from being non-invasive, with high benefit/cost ratio. HRV measures are obtained at limited costs, labour and accuracy of recording and information on the autonomic system functional condition or response, albeit indirect, is obtainable also when voluntary reports would be distracting, in the absence of the subject’s collaboration (as in cases of the severe disorder of consciousness), whenever sophisticated experimental designs and data recording pro-cedures are impracticable (e.g. in the intensive care unit), or when observation needs to be non-invasive and must cause no discomfort (e.g. in psychiatry or in sports medicine), or long-term observation is necessary.

HRV remains a suitable, although indirect, tool to assess residual or emerging sensory/cognitive function and to predict outcome of subjects with severe brain injury, including subjects

in a VS or MCS. The CAN model provides an independent approach in the understanding of the HRV measures as descrip-tors of the integrated function of, and interaction between, the CNS and autonomic (parasympathetic and sympathetic) system. There is evidence of applicability in the study of severe disorder of consciousness.

REFERENCES

Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, León-Carrión J, 1. Sannita WG, et al. Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med 2010; 8: 68.Multi-Society Task Force on PVS. Statement on medical aspects 2. of the persistent vegetative state. New Engl J Med 1994; 330: 1499–1508, 1572–1579.Jennett B, Plum F. Persistent vegetative state after brain damage; 3. a syndrome in search of a name. Lancet 1972; 1: 734–737.Laureys S, Owen AM, Schiff ND. Brain function in coma, vegeta-4. tive state, and related disorders. Lancet 2004; 3: 537–546.Monti MM, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Coleman MR, Boly M. Wilful 5. modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. N Engl J Med 2010; 18: 579–589.Riganello F, Sannita WG. Residual brain processing in the vegeta-6. tive state. J Psychophysiol 2009; 23: 18 26.Perrin F, Schnakers C, Schabus M, Degueldre C, Goldman S, 7. Brédart S, et al. Brain response to one’s own name in vegetative state, minimally conscious state, and locked-in syndrome. Arch Neurol 2006; 63: 562–569.Machado C, Korein J, Aubert E, Bosch J, Alvarez MA, Rodríguez 8. R, et al. Recognizing a mother’s voice in the persistent vegetative state. Clin EEG Neurosci 2007; 38: 124–126.Di HB, Yu SM, Weng XC, Laureys S, Yu D, Li JQ, et al. Cerebral 9. response to patient’s own name in the vegetative and minimally conscious states. Neurology 2007; 68: 895–899.Qiu J. Probing islands of consciousness in the damaged brain. 10. Lancet Neurol 2007; 6: 946–947.Owen AM, Coleman MR, Boly M, Davis MH, Laureys S, Pickard 11. JD. Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science 2006; 313: 1402.Coleman MR, Rodd JM, Davis MH, Johnsrude IS, Menon DK, 12. Pickard JD, et al. Do vegetative patients retain aspects of lan-guage comprehension? Evidence from fMRI. Brain 2007; 130: 2494–2507.De Jong MJ, Randall DC. Heart rate variability analysis in the 13. assessment of autonomic function in heart failure. J Cardiovasc Nurs 2005; 20: 186–195; Quiz 196–197.Boly M, Phillips C, Tshibanda L, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schabus 14. M, Dang-Vu TT, et al. Intrinsic brain activity in altered states of consciousness: how conscious is the default mode of brain func-tion? Ann NY Acad Sci 2008; 1129: 119–159.Mashin V, Mashina M. Analysis of the heart rate variability in 15. negative functional states in the course of psychological relaxation sessions. Hum Physiol 2000; 26: 420–425.Appelhans BM, Luecken LJ. Heart rate variability as an index 16. of regulated emotional responding. Rev Gen Psychol 2006; 10: 229–240.Appelhans BM, Luecken LJ. Heart rate variability and pain: as-17. sociations of two interrelated homeostatic processes. Biol Psychol 2008; 77: 174–182.Chambers AS, Allen JJ. Vagal tone as an indicator of treatment 18. response in major depression. Psychophysiology 2002; 39: 861–864.Garan H. Heart rate variability in acute myocardial infarction. 19. Cardiology 2009; 114: 273–274.Nolan J, Batin PD, Andrews R, Lindsay SJ, Brooksby P, Mullen 20.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 22: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

500 F. Riganello et al

M, et al. Prospective study of heart rate variability and mortality in chronic heart failure. Circulation 1998; 98: 1510–1516.Carney RM, Blumenthal JA, Freedland KE, Stein PK, Howells 21. WB, Berkman LF, et al. Low heart rate variability and the effect of depression on post-myocardial infarction mortality. Arch Intern Med 2005; 165: 1486–1491.Friedman BG. An autonomic flexibility-neurovisceral integration 22. model of anxiety and cardiac vagal tone. Biol Psychol 2007; 74: 185–199.Thayer JF, Friedman BH. A neurovisceral integration model of 23. health disparities in aging. In: Anderson NB, Bulatao RA, Cohen B, editors. Critical perspectives on racial and ethnic differences in health in late life. Washington DC: The National Academic Press; 2004, p. 567–603.Benarroch EE. Functional anatomy of the central autonomic 24. network. In: Benarroch EE, editor. Central autonomic network: functional organization and clinical correlations. Armonk: Futura Publishing Co. Inc.; 1997, p. 29–59.Van Leeuwen P, Bettermann H. The status of nonlinear dynamics 25. in the analysis of heart rate variability. Herzschrittmachertherapie und Elektrophysiologie 2000; 11: 127–130.Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North 26. American Society of Pacing and electrophysiology of Circulation. Heart rate variability: standard of measurement, physiological in-terpretation, and clinical use. Circulation 1996; 93: 1043–1065.Ryan ML, Thorson CM, Otero CA, Vu T, Proctor KG. Clinical 27. applications of heart rate variability in the triage and assessment of traumatically injured patients. Anesthesiology Research and Practice 2011; 2011 Feb 10 [E-pub ahead of print]. Papaioannou VE, Maglaveras N, Houvarda I, Antoniadou E, Vret-28. zakis G. Investigation of altered heart rate variability, nonlinear properties of heart rate signals, and organ dysfunction longitudi-nally over time in intensive care unit patients. J Crit Care 2006; 21: 95–103.Seely AJ, Macklem PT. Complex systems and the technology of 29. variability analysis. Critical Care 2004; 8: R367–R384.Vanderlei LCM, Pastre CM, Hoshi RA, de Carvalho TD, de Godoy 30. MF. Basic notions of heart rate variability and its clinical applica-bility. Rev Bras Cir Cardiovasc 2009; 24: 205–217.Lane RD, McRae K, Reiman EM, Chen K, Ahern GL, Thayer JF. 31. Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. Neu-roimage 2009; 44: 213–222.Napadow V, Dhond R, Conti G, Makris N, Brown EN, Barbieri R. 32. Brain correlates of autonomic modulation: combining heart rate variability with fMRI. Neuroimage 2008; 42: 169–177.Draper K, Ponsford J, Schönberger M. Psychosocial and emotional 33. outcomes 10 years following traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil 2007; 22: 278–287.Lippert-Grüner M, Kuchta J, Hellmich M, Klug N. Neurobehav-34. ioural deficits after severe traumatic brain injury (TBI). Brain Inj 2006; 20: 569–574.Wijnen VJ, van Boxtel GJ, Eilander HJ, de Gelder B. Autonomic 35. reactivity to sensory stimulation is related to consciousness level after severe traumatic brain injury. Clin Neurophysiol 2006; 8: 1794–1807.Allerdings MD, Alfano DP. Neuropsychological correlates of 36. impaired emotion recognition following traumatic brain injury. Brain Cognition 2006; 2: 193–194.Keren O, Yapatov S, Radai MM, Elad-Yarum R, Faraggi D, Ab-37. boud S. Heart rate variability (HRV) of patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI) during the post-insult sub-acute period. Brain Inj 2005; 19: 605–611.Gehi A, Mangano D, Pipkin S, Browner WS, Whooley MA. De-38. pression and heart rate variability in patients with stable coronary heart disease: findings from the heart and soul study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 6: 661–666.Valkonen-Korhonen M, Tarvainen MP, Ranta-Aho P, Karjalainen 39. PA, Partanen J, Karhu J, et al. Heart rate variability in acute psy-

chosis. Psychophysiology 2003; 5: 716–726.Cohen H, Benjamin J, Geva AB, Matar MA, Kaplan Z, Kotler M. 40. Autonomic dysregulation in panic disorder and in post-traumatic stress disorder: application of power spectrum analysis of heart rate variability at rest and in response to recollection of trauma or panic attacks. Psychiatry Res 2000; 96: 1–13.King ML, Litchtman SW, Seliger G, Ehert FA,Steinberg JS. Heart 41. rate variability in chronic traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 1997; 6: 445–453.Perkes I, Baguley IJ, Nott MT, Menon DK. A review of paroxysmal 42. sympathetic hyperactivity after acquired brain injury. Ann Neurol 2010; 68: 126–135.Kemp C, Johnson J, Riordan W, Cotton BA. How we die: the impact 43. of non-neurological organ dysfunction after severe traumatic brain injury. Am Surg 2008; 74: 866–872.Zygun D, Kortbeek J, Fick G, Laupland KB, Doig CJ. Non-44. neurologic organ dysfunction in severe traumatic brain injury. Crit Care Med 2005; 33: 654–660.Riordan B, Norris P, Jenkins J, Morris JA Jr. Early loss of heart rate 45. complexity predicts mortality regardless of mechanism, anatomic location, or severity of injury in 2178 trauma patients. J Surg Res 2009; 156: 283–289.Riordan B, Cotton B, Norris P, Waitman LR, Jenkins JM, Morris 46. JA Jr. Beta-blocker exposure in patients with severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) and cardiac uncoupling. J Trauma 2007; 63: 503–510.Lemke DM. Sympathetic storming after severe traumatic brain 47. injury. Crit Care Nurse 2007; 27: 30–37.Cuny E, Richer E, Castel JP. Dysautonomia syndrome in the acute 48. recovery phase after traumatic brain injury: relief with intrathecal baclofen therapy. Brain Inj 2001; 15: 917–925.Dolce G, Quintieri M, Leto E, Milano M, Pileggi A, Lagani V. 49. Dysautonomia and clinical outcome in vegetative state. J Neuro-trauma 2008; 25: 1079–1082.Baguley IJ, Nott MT, Slewa-Younan S, Heriseanu RE, Perkes 50. IE. Diagnosing dysautonomia after acute traumatic brain injury: evidence for overresponsiveness to afferent stimuli. Arch Phys Med Rehab 2009; 90: 580–586.Kawahara E, Ikeda S, Miyahara Y, Kohno S. Role of autonomic 51. nervous dysfunction in electrocardio-graphic abnormalities and cardiac injury in patients with acute subarachnoid hemorrhage. Circ J 2003; 67: 753–756.Mowery NT, Norris PR, Riordan W, Jenkins JM, Williams AE, 52. Morris JA Jr. Cardiac uncoupling and heart rate variability are associated with intracranial hypertension and mortality: a study of 145 trauma patients with continuous monitoring. J Trauma 2008; 65: 621–627.Morris JA, Norris PR, Ozdas A, Waitman LR, Harrell FE, Williams 53. AN, et al. Reduced heart rate variability: an indicator of cardiac uncoupling and diminished physiologic reserve in 1,425 trauma patients. J Trauma 2006; 60: 1165–1173.Biswas A, Scott W, Sommerauer JF, Luckett PM. Heart rate vari-54. ability after acute traumatic brain injury in children. Crit Care Med 2000; 28: 3907–3912.Lowensohn R, Weiss M, Hon E. Heart-rate variability in brain-55. damaged adults. Lancet 1977; 309: 626–628.Keren O, Yupatov S, Radai MM, Elrad-Yarum R, Faraggi D, Ab-56. boud S, et al. Heart rate variability (HRV) of patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI) during the post-insult sub-acute period. Brain Inj 2005; 19: 605–611.Baguley IJ, Heriseanu RE, Felmingham KL, Cameron ID. Dysau-57. tonomia and heart rate variability following severe traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 2006; 20: 437-444.King DR, Ogilvie MP, Pereira BM, Chang Y, Manning RJ, Con-58. ner JA, et al. Heart rate variability as a triage tool in patients with trauma during prehospital helicopter transport. J Trauma 2009; 67: 436–440.Cooke WH, Salinas J, McManus JG, Ryan KL, Rickards CA, 59.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 23: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

501HRV in severe disorder of consciousness

Holcomb JB, et al. Heart period variability in trauma patients may predict mortality and allow remote triage. Aviat Space Environ Med 2006; 77: 1107–1112.Goldstein B, DeKing D, DeLong DJ, Kempski MH, Cox C, Kelly 60. MM, et al. Autonomic cardiovascular state after severe brain injury and brain death in children. Crit Care Med 1993; 21: 228–233.Goldstein B, Kempski MH, DeKing D, Cox C, DeLong DJ, Kelly 61. MM, et al. Autonomic control of heart rate after brain injury in children. Crit Care Med 1996; 24: 234–240.Rapenne T, Moreau D, Lenfant F, Vernet M, Boggio V, Cottin Y, 62. et al. Could heart rate variability predict outcome in patients with severe head injury? A pilot study. J Neurosurg Anesthesiol 2001; 13: 260–268.Cooke WH, Salinas J, McManus JG, Ryan KL, Rickards CA, 63. Holcomb JB, et al. Heart period variability in trauma patients may predict mortality and allow remote triage. Aviat Space Environ Med 2006; 77: 1107–1112.Norris PR, Morris JA Jr, Ozdas A, Grogan EL, Williams AE. 64. Heart rate variability predicts trauma patient outcome as early as 12 h: implications for military and civilian triage. J Surg Res 2005; 129: 122–128.Su CF, Kuo TB, Kuo JS, Lai HY, Chen HI. Sympathetic and 65. parasympathetic activities evaluated by heart-rate variability in head injury of various severities. Clin Neurophysiol 2005; 116: 1273–1279.Wijnen VJ, van Boxtel GJ, Eilander HJ, de Gelder B. Autonomic 66. reactivity to sensory stimulation is related to consciousness level after severe traumatic brain injury. Clin Neurophysiol 2006; 118: 597–605.Dolce G, Riganello F, Quintieri M, Candelieri A. Personal inter-67. action in vegetative state: a data-mining study. J Phychophysiol 2008; 22: 150–156.Gutiérrez J, Machado C, Estévez M, Olivares A, Hernández H, 68. Perez J, et al. Heart rate variability changes induced by auditory stimulation in persistent vegetative state. Int J Disabil Hum Dev 2010; 9: 357–362.Machado C, Estévez M, Gutiérrez J, Beltrán C, Machado Y, 69. Machado Y, et al. Recognition of the mom’s voice with an emo-tional content in a PVS patient. Clin Neurophysiol 2011; 122: 1059–1060.Riganello F, Candelieri A, Dolce G, Sannita WG. Residual emo-70. tional processing in the vegetative state: a scientific issue? Clin Neurophysiol 2011; 122: 1061–1062.Riganello F, Quintieri M, Candelieri A, Conforti D, Giuliano D. 71. Heart rate response to music: an artificial intelligence study on healthy and traumatic brain injured subjects. J Psychophysiol 2008; 22: 166–174.Riganello F, Candelieri A, Quintieri M, Conforti D, Golce G. Heart 72. rate variability: an index of brain processing in vegetative state? An artificial intelligence, data mining study. Clin Neurophysiol 2010; 121: 2024–2034.Riganello F, Candelieri A. Data mining and the functional rela-73. tionship between HRV and emotional processing: comparative analysis and application. Healthinf 2010: Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Health Informatics; 2010, Jan 20–23; Valencia, Spain. Portugal: INSTICC Press; 2010.Hui-Min W, Hou MC, Sheng-Chieh H, Lei-Chun C, Shao-You 74. H, Tzu-chia H, et al. The relationship between music processing and electrocardiogram (ECG) in vegetative state (VS). Circuits and systems (ISCAS), proceedings of 2010 IEEE international symposium. 2010.Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK coma recovery scale-75. revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch

Phys Med Rehabil 2004; 85: 2020–2029.Vanhaudenhuyse A, Giacino J, Schnakers C, Kalmar K, Smart C, 76. Bruno A, et al. Blink to visual threat does not herald consciousness in the vegetative state. Neurology 2008; 71: 1374–1375.Candelieri A, Cortese MD, Riganello F, Sannita WG. Functional 77. status and the eye-tracking response. A data mining classification study in the vegetative and minimaly conscious states. Heathinf Rome Proc 2011, p. 138–141.Le Doux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Ann Rev Neurosci 78. 2000; 23: 155–184.Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA. Contributions of the anterior 79. cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain 1995; 118: 279–306.Saper CB. The central autonomic nervous system: conscious 80. visceral perception and autonomic pattern generation. Annu Rev Neurosci 2002; 25: 433–469.Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Josephs O, O’Doherty J, Zanini S, 81. Dewar B-K, et al. Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain 2003; 126: 2139–2152.Toni R. The neuroendocrine system: organization and homeostatic 82. role. J Endocrinol Invest 2004; 27 (6 Suppl): 35–47.Glass L, Mackey MC. From clocks to chaos: the rhythms of life. 83. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1988.Thayer JF, Lane RD. A model of neurovisceral integration in 84. emotion regulation and dysregulation. J Affect Disord 2000; 61: 201–216.Thayer JF. What the heart says to brain (and vice versa) and why 85. we should listen. Psychol Topics 2007; 16: 241–250.Thayer JF, Lane RD. The role of vagal function in the risk for 86. cardiovascular disease and mortality. Biol Psychol 2007; 74: 224–242.Levy MN. Neural control of cardiac function. Baillieres Clin 87. Neurol 1997; 6: 227–244.Porges SW. The polyvagal theory: new insights into adaptive reac-88. tions of the autonomic nervous system. Cleve Clin J Med 2009; 76 (Suppl 2): S86–S90.Thayer JF, Lane RD. Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connec-89. tion: further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2009; 33: 81–88.Thayer JF, Hansen AL, Saus-Rose E, Johnsen BH. Heart rate vari-90. ability, prefrontal neural function, and cognitive performance: the neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adapta-tion, and health. Ann Behav Med 2009; 37: 141–153.Porges SW. The polyvagal perspective. Biol Psychol 2007; 74: 91. 116–143.Benarroch EE. The autonomic nervous system: basic, anatomy 92. and physiology. Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2007; 13: 13–22.Berne C, Fagius J, Pollare T, Hjemdahl P. The sympathetic response 93. to euglycaemic hyperinsulinaemia. Evidence from microelectrode nerve recordings in healthy subjects. Diabetologia 1992; 35: 873–879.Wallin BG, Charkoudian N. Sympathetic neural control of integrat-94. ed cardiovascular function: insights from measurement of human sympathetic nerve activity. Muscle Nerve 2007; 36: 595–614.Esler M. Clinical application of noradrenaline spillover methodol-95. ogy: delineation of regional human sympathetic nervous responses. Pharmacol Toxicol 1993; 73: 243–253.Grossman P, Taylor EW. Toward understanding respiratory sinus 96. arrhythmia: relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobe-havioral functions. Biol Psychol 2007; 74: 263–285.Eckberg DL. The human respiratory gate. J Physiol 2003; 548: 97. 339–352.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 24: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

J Rehabil Med 44

originaL rePort

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 502–504

© 2012 The Authors. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0981Journal Compilation © 2012 Foundation of Rehabilitation Information. ISSN 1650-1977

DECREASING INCIDENCE OF PAROXYSMAL SYMPATHETIC HYPERACTIVITY SYNDROME IN THE VEGETATIVE STATE

Loris Pignolo, Eng, Stefania Rogano, MD, Maria Quintieri, MD, Elio Leto, MD and Giuliano Dolce, MD

From the S. Anna Institute and RAN – Research in Advanced Neurorehabilitation, Crotone, Italy

Objective: to update knowledge of the incidence of parox-ysmal sympathetic hyperactivity (Psh, also referred to as dysautonomia), an emergency condition tentatively attribut-ed to sympathetic paroxysms or diencephalic-hypothalamic disarrangement associated with severe diffuse brain axonal damage or hypoxia. This condition is reportedly common in the vegetative state, threatens survival and affects outcome. Methods: the results of a retrospective study on 333 subjects in a vegetative state admitted to a dedicated unit in 1998–2005 are compared with a survey on patients admitted to the same unit in 2006–2010. Results and comment: in the 1998–2005 period, the incidence of Psh was 32% and 16% in post-traumatic and non-trau-matic patients, respectively. It decreased to 18% and 7% in the 2006–2010 period. The PSH duration and the time spent in emergency units before admission and in the dedicated unit for the vegetative state after admission also decreased significantly. Incidence was greater among post-traumatic patients; its effect on outcome does not appear to have changed.

Key words: paroxysmal sympathetic hyperactivity; dysautono-mia; incidence; vegetative state; outcome.J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 502–504

Correspondence address: Loris Pignolo, S. Anna Institute and RAN – Research in Advanced Neurorehabilitation, Crotone, Italy. Email: [email protected]

Submitted September 27, 2011; accepted March 23, 2012

INTRODUCTION

The critical association of signs such as tachycardia (> 120 beats/min), tachypnea (> 30/min), systolic hypertension (> 160 mmHg), hyper/hypothermia, excessive sweating, decerebra-tion/decortication, increased muscle tone, horripilation and/or flushing is collectively referred to as “dysautonomia” or “paroxysmal sympathetic hyperactivity” syndrome (PSH) (1, 2). PSH is reportedly a common event in the vegetative state (VS, also referred to as “unresponsive wakefulness syndrome” or UWS) (3) and threatens these subjects’ survival and recov-ery. It is tentatively attributed to sympathetic paroxysms or diencephalic-hypothalamic disarrangement associated with severe diffuse brain axonal damage or hypoxia, and, by all criteria, is classed as an emergency condition (1–9).

A total of 333 subjects with severe disorder of consciousness following massive traumatic (n = 213; 64%) or non-traumatic (vascular, anoxic-hypoxic, infective or others) acute brain damage were retrospectively surveyed in a previous study (8). All patients had been referred to the S. Anna Institute – RAN in the years 1998–2005 for being in a VS/UWS condition (8). PSH occurred in 26.1% of them, with greater incidence after traumatic than non-traumatic brain injury (31.9% vs 15.8%). Outcome was worse following non-traumatic brain damage irrespective of PSH and worst among non-traumatic subjects with PSH. Occurrence of PSH and outcome were accounted for by the variance explained by variables (such as aetiology, age and sex) that are already known to be predictors of outcome for patients with severe disorder of consciousness, such as the VS/UWS (10–14). However, the mathematical model correlating the occurrence of PSH with the subjects’ clinical characteristics accounted for only 40% of the overall data variance (8). In this respect, the study was inconclusive and the natural his-tory of the PSH remains poorly understood. Incidence is also unclear due to the lack of studies on large samples and over time (1, 4, 5). The purpose of this paper was to compare the incidence of PSH in 1998–2005 with a new group of subjects in VS/UWS admitted to and cared for in the same institute in the period 2006–2010.

PATIENTS AND METHODSA new database of 169 patients admitted to the dedicated semi-intensive care unit of the S. Anna – RAN institute over a 4-year period (January 2006 to May 2010) was compared with the subjects’ group of the first survey (8). Eighty-eight subjects (52%) were in a VS/UWS due to traumatic brain injury; non-traumatic aetiologies (major vascular insults, anoxia-hypoxia, etc.) were documented for 81 subjects (48%). Subjects were diagnosed at admission as being in a VS/UWS according to the current clinical criteria (11, 16–19) and established evaluation scales. The scores of subjects in VS/USW were lower than 25 on the Loewenstein Scale (20), lower than 2 on the Level of Cognitive Function scale (21), and higher than 21 on the Disability Rating Scale (22). The revised Coma Recovery Scale (CRS-r) (23) global score was assessed retrospectively and found to be lower than 8 in all subjects. In all cases, subjects with infection, metabolic disorder, lung diseases affecting the cardio-respiratory function, or drug side-effects were excluded from the study. Outcome was defined in full accordance with the Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS) (23, 24). The rank-ing classes were: 1 = death; 2 = VS/UWS exceeding 1 year in duration; 3 = recovery, with severe disabilities; 4 = recovery, with mild disabilities; and 5 = full recovery or recovery with minimal disabilities not interfering with everyday life (24–27).

Included in the special issue: CONSCIOUSNESS AND THE VEGETATIVE STATE: TODAY

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 481–516

Page 25: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

503Decreasing incidence of PSH syndrome in vegetative state

With the exception of the revised Coma Recovery Scale (which was not in use in this country before 2007), the criteria by which patients have been diagnosed as being in a VS/UWS and by which PSH was identified were the same in the two studies, as were the monitoring, healthcare, pharmacological treatment (27) and rehabilitative proce-dures and protocols in the S. Anna – RAN unit; in addition, the attend-ing physicians were the same. The data from the 1998–2005 and the 2006–2010 periods were compared using the exact Fisher’s test.

The study is retrospective and was approved by the local public healthcare ethics committee. Regulations about subjects’ privacy and the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (1964) of the World Medical Association concerning human experimentation were followed.

RESULTS

The incidence of PSH was found to have decreased from the 1998–2005 period to the 2006–2010 period, with a reduction from 32% to 18% and from 16% to 7% among subjects with post-traumatic and non-traumatic brain damage, respectively.

Both the time spent in the emergency or intensive care units before admission to the S. Anna – RAN dedicated unit for the VS/UWS and the permanence in this unit have decreased irrespective of aetiology; the PSH duration decreased among non-traumatic subjects (Table I). Outcome did not change significantly (χ2, Pearson’s = 0.332, p = 0.56423), although the percentage of subjects with outcome in the GOS rank 1 (death) appears to have increased (Fisher’s exact test: p = 0.8053633) (Fig. 1).

DISCUSSION

Epileptogenic mechanisms cannot be excluded a priori in all cases (29), but the pathophysiological processes starting and sustaining PSH remain a matter of speculation. Two main un-

derlying mechanism have been suggested, notably a functional disconnection or unbalanced activation of structures usually under the control of higher brain centres (30), and an excita-tory/inhibitory ratio model of paroxysms resulting from the abnormal processing of and over-responsiveness to the affer-ent stimuli from the medulla (31). A residual neuro endocrine reactivity is suggested by the lower incidence among anoxic-hypoxic patients with diffuse brain damage (9); its remission following treatment with serotonin or GABA modulators (32, 33) suggests hypothalamic dysregulation (34, 35). A multi-factorial origin appears conceivable and would be consistent with the variability of the PSH clinical picture as to number, relevance, variability or development over time, and spontane-ous or drug-mediated remission of clinical signs.

Undetected (interactions among) factors possibly modifying the clinical picture or affecting its incidence may have ac-counted for the differences observed in the two subject groups and are not necessarily compensated for by the group sizes. This caveat notwithstanding, the comparison between two large patient groups monitored in the same unit for a short time interval suggests that the incidence of PSH may be decreas-ing, and that the condition has somehow become less severe and/or is better managed, at least in subjects with VS/UWS of non-traumatic aetiology. A more effective (although not neces-sarily intentional) prevention and better focused treatment in intensive care units appears possible; improved procedures to reduce brain oedema and control intracranial hypertension and early sedation in intensive care units are possible factors that may help reduce the incidence of dysautonomia (36).

Outcome does not seem to have improved in recent years, however. The differences between the two subject groups in a VS/UWS of non-traumatic aetiology suggests a higher per-centage of subjects who died during the observation after PSH (i.e. with outcome to be rated as GOS 1), but in all cases death

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

GLASGOW OUTCOME SCALE

SCALE 1 SCALE 2 SCALE 3 SCALE 4–5

%

POSTTRAUMATIC VEGETATIVE STATE 1998–2005POSTTRAUMATIC VEGETATIVE STATE 2006–2010NON-TRAUMATIC VEGETATIVE STATE 1998–2005 NON-TRAUMATIC VEGETATIVE STATE 2006–2010

Fig. 1. Outcome of subjects in a vegetative state with paroxysmal sympathetic hyperactivity syndrome. Comparison between the 1998–2005 and 2006–2010 subject groups.

Table I. Incidence of paroxysmal sympathetic hyperactivity (PSH) syndrome in vegetative state (VS)/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (UWS) following traumatic and non-traumatic brain injury. Comparison between the subjects admitted in the 2006–2010 period with those of the previous survey (1998–2005)

Traumatic brain injuryNon-traumatic brain injury

1998–2005 2006–2010 1998–2005 2006–2010

Subjects with PSH, % 32 18 16 7Age, years, mean (SD) 25.0 (9) 25.5 (9) 31.0 (15) 50 (15)Time in emergency/intensive care units before admission to the dedicated unit for VS, days, mean (SD) 77.0 (71) 44.3 (26)* 74.0 (65)

58.6 (15)***

Time in the dedicated unit for VS, days, mean (SD) 186 (69)

164 (104.5)** 224.0 (88)

201 (125)**

Duration of PSH, days, mean (SD) 162 (90) 70 (34) 190.0 (50)

116 (146)***

Fisher’s exact test vs 1998–2005. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.SD: standard deviation.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 26: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

504 L. Pignolo et al.

resulted from clinical events unrelated to the pathophysiology of the VS/UWS or PSH. The relevance of PSH as a negative prognostic indicator remains confirmed; patients should be monitored for its occurrence and ad hoc therapeutic procedures should be devised.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The study has been carried on at the S. Anna – RAN Institute with support from the institute; authors are all employees of the institute.

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

Perkes I, Baguley IJ, Nott MT, Menon DK. A review of paroxysmal 1. sympathetic hyperactivity after acquired brain injury. Ann Neurol 2010; 68: 126–135. Rabinstein AA. Paroxysmal sympathetic hyperactivity in the neu-2. rological intensive care unit. Neurol Res 2007; 29: 680–682.Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, León-Carrión J, 3. G Sannita WG, et al. Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med 2010; 8: 68.Baguley IJ. Nomenclature of “paroxysmal sympathetic storms”. 4. Mayo Clin Proc 1999; 74: 105.Baguley IJ, Nicholls JL, Felmingham KL, Crooks J, Gurka JA, 5. Wade LD. Dysautonomia after traumatic brain injury: a forgotten syndrome? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiat 1999; 67: 39–43.Baguley IJ, Heriseanu RE, Felmingham KL, Cameron ID. Dysau-6. tonomia and heart rate variability following severe traumatic brain injury. Brai Inj 2006; 20: 437–444.Gennarelli TA. Mechanisms and pathophysiology of cerebral 7. concussion. J Head Trauma Rehabil 1986; 1: 23–29.Dolce G, Quintieri M, Leto E, Milano M, Pileggi A, Lagani V, 8. et al. Disautonomia and clinical outcome in vegetative state. J Neurotrauma 2008; 25: 1079–1082.Baguley IJ, Heriseanu RE, Cameron ID, Nott MT, Slewa-Younan S. 9. A critical review of the pathophysiology of dysautonomia follow-ing traumatic brain injury. Neurocritical Care 2008; 8: 293–300.Hendricks HT, Heeren AH, Vos PE. Dysautonomia after severe 10. traumatic brain injury. Eur J Neurol 2010; 17: 1172–1177.Multi-Society Task Force On PVS. Statement on medical as-11. pects of the persistent vegetative state. N Eng J Med 1994; 330, 1499–1508.O’Brien PB, Butt W, Suhr H, Bimpeh Y, McKenna AM, Bailey MJ, 12. et al. The functional outcome of patients requiring over 28 days of intensive care: a long-term follow-up study. Crit Care Resusc 2006; 8: 200–204.Whyte J, Katz D, Long D, Dipasquale MC, Polansky M, Kalmar K, 13. et al. Predictors of outcome in prolonged posttraumatic disorders of consciousness and assessment of medication effects: a multicenter study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2005; 96: 453–462.Young GB, Wang JT, Connolly JF. Prognostic determination in 14. anoxic-ischemic and traumatic encephalopathies. J Clin Neuro-physiol 2004; 21: 379–390.Zandbergen EG, De Haan RJ, Hijdra A. Systematic review of predic-15.

tion of poor outcome in anoxic-ischaemic coma with biochemical markers of brain damage. Intensi Care Med 2001; 27: 1661–1667.Dolce G, Sazbon L. The posttraumatic vegetative state. Stuttgart: 16. Thieme; 2002.Giacino JT, Kalmar K. The vegetative and minimally conscious 17. states: a comparison of clinical features and functional outcome. J Head Trauma Rehabil 1997: 12; 36–51.Jennett B, Plum F. Persistent vegetative state after brain damage: 18. a syndrome in search of a name. Lancet 1972; 1: 734–736.Jennet B. The vegetative state. J Neurol Neurosur Psychiat 2002; 19. 73: 355–356.Borer-Alafi N, Gil M, Sazbon L, Korn C. Loewenstein com-20. munication scale for the minimally responsive patient. Brain Inj 2002; 16: 593–609.Hagen C, Malkmus D, Durham P. Levels of cognitive functioning. 21. Downey, CA: Rancho Los Amigos Hospital; 1972.Rappaport M, Hall KM, Hopkins K, Belleza T, Cope DN. Disability 22. rating scale for severe head trauma patients: coma to community. Arch Phys Med Rehab 1982; 63: 118–123. Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK Coma Recovery Scale – 23. Revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2004; 85: 2020–2029.Jennett B, Bond M. Assessment of outcome after severe brain 24. damage. Lancet 1975; 1: 480–484.Jennett B, Snoek J, Bond MR, Brooks N. Disability after severe 25. head injury: observations on the use of the Glasgow outcome scale. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiat 1981; 44: 285–293.Wijdicks E F. Clinical scales for comatose patients: the Glasgow 26. Coma Scale in historical context and the new FOUR Score. Rev Neurol Dis 2006; 3: 109–117. Pignolo L, Quintieri M, Sannita WG. The Glasgow outcome scale 27. in vegetative state: a possible source of bias. Brain Inj 2009; 23: 1–2.Baguley IJ, Cameron ID, Green AM, Slewa-Younan S, Marosszeky 28. JE, Gurka JA. Pharmacological management of dysautonomia fol-lowing traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 2004; 18: 409–417.Metz SA, Halter JB, Porte D Jr, Robertson RP. “Autonomic epi-29. lepsy: clonidine blockade of paroxysmal catecholamine release and flushing.” Anns Int Med 1978; 88: 89–93.Baguley IJ. The excitatory:inhibitory ratio model (EIR model): an 30. integrative explanation of acute autonomic overactivity syndromes. Med Hypotheses 2008; 70: 26–35.Baguley IJ, Nott MT, Slewa-Younan S, Heriseanu RE, Perkes 31. IE. Diagnosing dysautonomia after acute traumatic brain injury: evidence for overresponsiveness to afferent stimuli. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2009; 90: 580–586. Kang YM, Chen JZ, Ouyang W, Qiao JY, Reyes-Vazquez C, Dafny 32. N. Serotonin modulates hypothalamic neuronal activity. Int J Neurosci 2004; 114: 299–319.Ong J, Kerr DL. Clinical potential of GABAB receptor modulators. 33. CNS Drug Re 2005; 11: 317–334.Baguley IJ, Bailey KM, Slewa-Younan S. Prolonged anti-34. spasticity effects of bolus intrathecal baclofen. Brain Inj 2005; 19: 545–548.Luther MS, Krewer C, Müller F, Koenig E. Orthostatic circulatory 35. disorders in early neurorehabilitation: a case report and manage-ment overview. Brain Inj 2007; 21: 763–767.Talman WT, Florek G, Bullard DE. A hyperthermic syndrome in 36. two subjects with acute hydrocephalus. Arch Neurol 1988; 45: 1037–1040.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 27: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

originaL rePort

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 505–511

J Rehabil Med 44© 2012 The Authors. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0982Journal Compilation © 2012 Foundation of Rehabilitation Information. ISSN 1650-1977

RECOVERY OF COGNITIVE FUNCTION DURING COMPREHENSIVE REHABILITATION AFTER SEVERE TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY

José León-Carrión, PhD1,2, María Rosario Domínguez-Morales, MD1, Juan Manuel Barroso y Martín, PhD2 and Umberto Leon-Dominguez, MA1

From the 1Center for Brain Injury Rehabilitation (C.RE.CER.)® and 2Department of Experimental Psychology, University University of Seville, Seville, Spain

Objective: to explore the course and timing of functional re-covery in patients who have emerged from coma after un-dergoing severe traumatic brain injury. Methods: an observational study involving 19 patients with traumatic brain injury recovered from coma who under-went holistic, intensive and multidisciplinary neurorehabili-tation. Daily performance in each cognitive function (long-term memory, short-term memory, orientation, calculation, attention, mental control, automation, and planning) was clinically scored and compared at admission and discharge. Results: the course of cognitive recovery after post-trau-matic coma is not uniform, offering a curve with many ups, downs and plateaus. To achieve a good response and out-come nearing normalcy, a patient needs over 300 h of inten-sive rehabilitation. Conclusion: the consolidation of functional recovery in patients with traumatic brain injury requires time and ad-equate training, and discharge is not recommended until cognitive improvement is established. Key words: cognitive functions; neuropsychological rehabilita-tion; neurorehabilitation; traumatic brain injury.J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 505–511

Correspondence address: José León-Carrión, Center for Brain Injury Rehabilitation (C.RE.CER.), c/ Torneo 23, ES-41002 Seville, Spain. E-mail: [email protected] September 27, 2011; accepted March 15, 2012

INTRODUCTION

Functional disorders affecting daily living activities are frequent in patients who emerge from coma after sustaining severe traumatic brain injury (TBI). These disorders usually result in impairment to memory, attention, reasoning, mental imagery, language, problem-solving abilities or executive functioning, as noted by León-Carrión (1), and require treatment to achieve functionality. Recent studies have proven the efficacy of func-tional rehabilitation for patients who have emerged from deep coma. As shown by Cicerone et al. (2), there is substantial evidence supporting interventions for attention, memory, so-cial communication skills, and executive functioning, and for comprehensive neuropsychological rehabilitation after TBI, designed to help the person recover maximum functionality near-ing pre-injury level. However, the timing and duration of these

interventions has not been established. Prigatano (3) reports that “cognitive rehabilitation is labor intensive. Patients must spend hours at cognitive remediation tasks before any notable change can be achieved. No matter how well-randomized or designed, studies that employ less than 100 hours of cognitive rehabilita-tion will most likely be associated with minuscule results. This reality exists because we do not know how to deliver re-training activities systematically in a cost-efficient manner”.

In the search for TBI treatment, insurance companies, healthcare professionals, families, and patients are concerned with the duration of neurorehabilitation and whether it will be worthwhile. Different systematic reviews, most notably Rohling et al.’s (4), have demonstrated that in-hospital cogni-tive rehabilitation for patients with moderate-to-severe TBI is more effective than at-home rehabilitation or no rehabilitation post-injury. Studies by Cicerone et al. (5) and Yu (6) have also shown that a certain degree of spontaneous recovery occurs during the first few weeks, and even months, after injury. A pre-vious study by Leon-Carrion & Machuca-Murga (7) analysed the course of post-TBI cognitive deficits in patients who did not receive neuropsychological rehabilitation, and endeavoured to establish the point at which cognitive deficits ceased to present signs of spontaneous recovery. Our study involved 28 subjects with severe TBI who were neuropsychologically assessed at 8 months post-TBI and again, 19 months later. Results showed no significant differences between the two neuropsychological exams and no spontaneous recovery beyond the 8 month post-TBI. Neurocognitive deficits consequential to TBI appeared to be established within the first 8 months post-trauma.

The present study reports on the outcome of 19 adults with severe TBI in the post-acute phase after undergoing a holistic, intensive, and multidisciplinary programme in a highly special-ized neurorehabilitation centre in Europe.

METHODS

Subjects Nineteen patients with severe head trauma (3 female, 16 male; mean age 23.57 years) and a median Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) score of 5 (interquartile range (IQR): 4 –7) at admission. Patients were recruited from the Center for Brain Injury Rehabilitation (C.RE.CER) in Seville, Spain. No control group was used in this descriptive study. Inclusion criteria included emergence from coma, a GCS score of ≤ 8 within 24 h post-TBI, and the presence of at least 3 impaired cognitive functions

Included in the special issue: CONSCIOUSNESS AND THE VEGETATIVE STATE: TODAY

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 481–516

Page 28: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

506 J. León-Carrión et al.

FG% = × 100MI – M010 – M0

Table I. Patient demographic data: age, Glasgow Coma Score (GCS) score within 24-h post-traumatic brain injury (TBI) and time from injury to programme admission

Patient datan = 19 Mean (SD) Median (IQR)

Age 23.57 (7.04) 23 (19–28)GCS Score 5.37 (1.89) 5 (4–7)Time from injury to programme admission, months

23.94 (58.62) 11 (4–17)

GCS: Glasgow Coma Scale; SD: standard deviation; IQR: interquartile range.

Table II. Classification for Center for Brain Injury Rehabilitation Clinical Outcome Scale (CRECERCOS). The first column shows CRECERCOS scores; the second indicates percentage of cognitive functionality compared with pre-morbid levels of normalcy; the third shows level of impairment associated with each score

CRECERCOS score

Impairment score, % Specific function/s

1–2 10–20 Severe impairment (almost no response) in a specific function

3–4 30–40 Impaired, inconsistent response5–6 50–60 Consistent response7 70 Good response8–9 80–90 Near normal response in quantity

and quality, but not pre-morbid level10 100 Previous functioning level

(deterioration of mental process involving symbolic operations, such as orientation, memory, attention, mental control, automation, and planning). All patients began the neurorehabilitation programme ap-proximately 24 months post-injury. Patients’ mean GCS score, mean time from brain injury to programme admission and demographic data are shown in Table I.

Treatment programmePatients enrolled in a holistic, intensive and multidisciplinary reha-bilitation programme at Center for Brain Injury Rehabilitation (7–11). Patients underwent daily 4-h rehabilitation, 4 days a week, for 6 months. Each rehabilitation session lasted 60 min, and was given by a specialized therapist (neuropsychologist, physical therapist, speech therapist, or occupational therapist) in accordance with the patient’s needs. In general, patients received a combination of these rehabilitation sessions, which were specifically tailored to meet the physical, emotional, behavioural and cognitive needs of each patient, and could include pharmacological treatment, as reflected in previous studies by León-Carrión (1, 12, 13). Cognitive rehabilitation included exercises in orientation, memory, attention mechanisms (automation and mental control), calculation, planning and executive functioning (14) (Appendix I).

Outcome scoring system Each cognitive function was clinically scored on a scale from 1 to 10 by the therapist who conducted the session. Baselines for cognitive functions were obtained at admission, using the CRECER Clinical Outcome Scale (CRECERCOS) and neuropsychological assessments prior to rehabilitation (Table II). Patients received a score of normalcy when performance achieved pre-morbid levels of functioning. This normalcy was clinically established through interviews with the patients’ families and closest associates. A score of 1–2 was assigned to subjects with severe impairment (almost no response) in a specific function (10–20% normalcy); 3–4 indicated impaired, although in-consistent, response (30–40% normalcy); 5–6 showed consistent, but scarce, response (60% normalcy); 7 indicated a good response, but too scarce to be considered at normal level (70% normalcy); 8–9 reflected near normal response in quantity and quality, but not at pre-morbid levels (80–90% normalcy). A score of 10 was assigned when patient performance showed either his/her previous level of functioning (100%) or statistical normalcy.

Statistical and data analysisThe following analyses were carried out: comparison of initial scores with scores after discharge; mean number of sessions completed for each cognitive function; percentage of functional gain obtained after rehabilitation, and percentage of functionality at discharge compared with admission. The percentage of functional gain is calculated from the CRECERCOS baseline at admission and the final level of func-tionality obtained after neurorehabilitation, with a maximum score of 10. For example, a patient with a 6 on the CRECERCOS scale has a potential gain of 4 points to achieve the maximum score of 10. If the functional gain of this patient after treatment is 2 points, his/her

percentage of functional gain is 50% (half of the potential 4 points). The equation used to determine the latter is as follows:

Ml is the score obtained by the patient in the last month of rehabili-tation. M0 represents the patient’s score at admission. FG% is the percentage of functional gain for each specific function obtained in the final assessment. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 15.0 software for Windows, with alpha set at 0.05 for all tests. Fisher’s exact test was applied to analyse categorical variables. Given the asymmetri-cal distribution of most of the variables, non-parametric analyses were performed. Means, standard deviations, medians and interquartiles are displayed in Tables I, III, IV and V. We applied the Mann-Whitney U tests to analyse independent samples and the Wilcoxon test for related samples. Correlation analysis was carried out using the Spearman’s rank order correlation (rho). We used mean values and standard de-viations (SD) to summarize our results due to their higher illustrative capacity for presenting and comparing our data.

RESULTS

CRECERCOS score analysesTable III displays patients’ mean scores on the CRECERCOS scale at admission and discharge from the neurorehabilitation programme. At admission, the group mean for the different cognitive functions was 4.59. The lowest mean scores were for automation and short-term memory, while orientation received the highest score. At discharge, the group mean for all areas increased to 7.52, with calculation scoring the lowest, and orientation the highest mean score.

Functional gain increased in all areas. The global mean score reached 56.24%, with patients achieving the highest gains in orien-tation and automation and the least gain in calculation and mental control. Comparative analysis between areas showed the most significant gain in orientation, particularly compared with calcula-tion and mental control (p < 0.01). Significant differences were also found between short-term memory and calculation (Table III).

Statistical comparisons were carried out between number of rehabilitation sessions and cognitive function. Table IV illustrates the number of sessions (60 min per session) which patients underwent during the rehabilitation programme. The mean number of sessions was 43. Planning received the most rehabilitation sessions, whereas mental control received the

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 29: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

507Functional rehabilitation after coma

Table IV. Number of sessions, time elapsed from brain injury to rehabilitation programme admission and functional gain (FG)

Cognitive functions

Mann-Whitney U testNumber of sessions Differences between cognitive functions

Spearman correlation (rho)Sessions, n Correlation FG%–number of

sessionsCorrelation FG%–time from injury to programme admissionMean (SD) Median (IQR)

Long-term memory 46.53 (26.44) 54 (23–70) f** –0.08 –0.63**Short-term memory 50.16 (23.63) 58 (43–70) d** 0.03 –0.29Orientation 55.67 (31.05) 53 (22–88) d*, f** –0.48 –0.55Calculation 28.29 (27.65) 14.5 (4–53.5) b**, c*, h** 0.36 –0.13Attention 37 (32.89) 25.5 (7.25–72) 0.38 –0.25Mental control 23 (18.62) 13.5 (10.25–40.5) a**, c**, h** 0.26 0.09Automation 42.2 (33.97) 43 (5.75–76.5) 0.2 –0.16Planning 69 (42.39) 53.5 (41.75–83.75) d**, f** 0.63** –0.62**

a: significant differences for long-term memory; b: significant differences for short-term memory; c: significant differences for orientation; d: significant differences for calculation; e: significant differences for attention; f: significant differences for mental control; g: Significant differences for automation; h: significant differences for planning. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01. IQR: interquartile range: SD: standard deviation.

least. The comparative study between cognitive functions showed significant differences between calculation and the following: short-term memory (p < 0.01), orientation (p < 0.05), and planning (p < 0.01). Significant differences were also found between number of sessions for mental control compared with long-term memory, orientation, and planning (p < 0.01). Correlation analysis between functional gain and number of sessions was also performed for each cognitive function (see Table IV). Only planning showed a linear correlation between the two variables, as more sessions associated with greater functional gain (rho = 0.63, p < 0.01).

Correlation analysis between patients’ total functional gain and time from injury to programme admission was carried out for each cognitive function. The analysis revealed significant negative correlations between these variables for long-term memory (rho = –0.63) and planning (rho = –0.62). No other functions correlated with the time from injury to programme admission (Table IV).

To determine whether the initial state of a patient affected his/her subsequent rehabilitation, we relied on the GCS score at time of injury. We were able to obtain this information for 16 of the 19 patients in our study. All scores fell below 8 on

Table III. Classification for Center for Brain Injury Rehabilitation Clinical Outcome Scale (CRECERCOS) scores at admission and discharge, and overall functional gain

Cognitive functions n

CRECERCOS at admission CRECERCOS at discharge FG%

CRECERCOS Differences admission–discharge Wilcoxon (Z value)

FG% Differences between cognitive functionsMann-Whitney U test

Mean (SD) Median (IQR)

Mean (SD)

Median (IQR)

Mean (SD)

Median(IQR)

Long-term memory

19 4.27 (1.68)

4 (3–6)

7.7 (1.44)

8 6.75–8.625)

57.26 (24.4)

56.47 (33.33–72.32)

–3.73** c*

Short-term memory

19 3.86 (1.97)

4 (2–5.5)

7.4 (61.16)

7.5(7–8)

56.69 (18.03)

55.55 (47.77–71.4)

–3.82** c*, d*

Orientation 15 7.45 (3.15)

8.5 (6.1–10)

9.47 (1.46)

10 (9.875–10)

88.33(20.61)

100 (77.5–100)

–2.66** a*, b*, d**, e*, f**, h*

Calculation 14 4.3 (1.77)

4.5 (3.75–5.35)

6.87 (1.61)

7 (7–7.75)

43.32 (20.06)

40 (31.78-58.33)

–3.18** b*, c**

Attention 8 4.58 (1.60)

5 (4.12–5.8)

7 (1.64)

7 (5.75–7.87)

46.3(25.69)

44.44(26.25–56.25)

–2.52* c*

Mental control 16 4.53 (1.73)

4.5 (4–6)

7.03 (1.71)

7.5(6.12–8)

44.6 (30.54)

50 (22.72–66.66)

–3.24** c**

Automation 8 3.28 (2.27)

4.5 (1–5)

7.28 (2.15)

7 (6–10)

60.43 (33.79)

66.66 (25–100)

–2.37*

Planning 18 4.34 (1.46)

4.3 (3–5.5)

7.41 (1.43)

7.5 (7–8)

53.05 (24.76)

53.84 (37.5–72.5)

–3.77** c*

a: significant differences for long-term memory; b: significant differences for short-term memory; c: significant differences for orientation; d: significant differences for calculation; e: significant differences for attention; f: significant differences for mental control; g: Significant differences for automation; h: significant differences for planning. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01. SD: standard deviation; FG: functional gain; IQR: interquartile range.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 30: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

508 J. León-Carrión et al.

Table V. Between-group comparison of lowest and highest GCS scores

Patient demographic data

Low GCS score (n = 7) High GCS score (n = 9)

Between-group differences Mann-Whitney U test (z score)Mean (SD) Median (IQR) Mean (SD) Median (IQR)

Gender (M/F) 5/2 8/1 0.55a

Age, years 21.57 (4.81) 22(19–23) 22.89 (8.27) 25(16.5–28) –0.48GCS score 3.57 (0.53) 4 (3–4) 7.22 (1.92) 7 (5.5–8) –3.38**Time from injury to programme admission (months)

10.85 (8.39) 15 (1–16) 39 (84.72) 11 (3.5–21.5) –0.42

Cognitive functions(nlow GCS/nhigh GCS)

Functional gain % Between-group differences Mann-Whitney U test (z score)

Low GCS score (n = 7) High GCS score (n = 9)

Mean (SD) Median (IQR) Mean (SD) Median (IQR)

Long-term memory(7/9)

60.9 (27.80) 62.5 (33.3–85.07) 50.47 (19.68) 50 (29.16–69.04) –0.79

Short-term memory(7/9)

58.78 (16.11) 58.33 (50–64.28) 58.52 (12.72) 55.55 (48.88–72.38) –0.05

Orientation(4/4)

88.75 (13.14) 90 (76.25–100) 85 (30) 100 (55–100) –0.33

Calculation(6/6)

47.45 (25.13) 50.86 (25.45–64.58) 35.79 (12.77) 36.92 (28.57–43.33) –0.96

Attention(2/5)

54.16 (5.89) 54.1 (50–58.33) 42.41 (33.06) 30 (21.59–69.4) –1.16

Mental control(6/8)

28.40 (28.94) 38.18 (–3.57 to 50.08) 59.56 (23.78) 58.33 (50–72.9) –2.02*

Automation(5/2)

49.60 (33.49) 36.36 (22.5–83.33) 87.5 (17.67) 87.5 (75–100) –1.37

Planning(7/9)

44.53 (33.30) 45.94 (33.33–75) 60.23 (14.24) 62.5 (49.65–69.58) –1

aFisher’s exact test.*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.GCS: Glasgow Coma Scale; SD: standard deviation; IQR: interquartile range; M: male; F: female.

the GCS. We divided these patients into two groups: the low GCS group (n = 7), with scores ≤ 4, and the high GCS group (n = 9), with scores > 4. As shown in Table V, both groups had similar distributions of gender and age (p > 0.05), as well as time from injury to programme admission (p > 0.05). However, mean GCS scores between the two groups (3.57 for low GCS and 7.22 for high GCS) did show significant differences (p < 0.01).

Table V displays the percentage of functional gain obtained by both GCS groups in each cognitive function throughout the rehabilitation programme. The low GCS group showed a mean functional gain of 53.09%, whereas the high GCS group mean reached 61.74%. The highest functional gain for both groups was in orientation. The lowest gain was found in mental control in the low GCS group and in calculation in the high GCS group. We also compared the mean functional gain of each group in these cognitive functions, as shown in Table V. Significant differences were found in mental control (p < 0.05), with the highest gain shown by the high GCS group.

DISCUSSION

The main results of this study may be summed up as follows. Firstly, percentage of functional gain in all cognitive areas did

not differ between low and high GCS score groups, with the exception of mental control. Secondly, cognitive functions improved significantly from rehabilitation admission to dis-charge. Thirdly, functional gain was related to the number of sessions the patient underwent during the course of rehabilita-tion. Fourthly, not all cognitive functions required the same number of sessions to recover statistic or clinical normalcy. Finally, total functional gain and time from injury to cognitive rehabilitation showed an inverse relationship between long-term memory and planning.

The first aim of this study was to ascertain whether the sever-ity of the lesion at admission determined the severity of the cognitive sequelae observed as a consequence of the physical damage to the brain. To determine the severity of the lesion, we used the patient’s worst GCS score during the first 24 h post-injury. The GCS score, since its introduction, has been considered one of the most important predictors of outcome after head injury, although different studies have demonstrated that a correlation does not always exist after brain trauma (15). Our results showed a partial correlation between severity of lesion (GCS) at admission and patients’ cognitive functional gain. This correlation was only found in mental control. Our data did indicate that patients scoring higher within the GCS 5–8 range tended to achieve higher functional gain than those with lower scores, although a comparison of mean functional

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 31: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

509Functional rehabilitation after coma

gain between groups in each cognitive function only showed significant differences in mental control. As a cognitive func-tion, mental control is related to the part of executive function-ing that engages and directs different mental activities (16). This function is directly related to an individual’s capacity to be independent (17).

Our CRECERCOS analysis of scores at rehabilitation ad-mission (4.59) and discharge (7.52) found significant differ-ences between number of treatment sessions and the patient’s cognitive functional gain. This functional gain is observed in all cognitive areas, with a global mean of 56.24%. Our results support those of other authors, who maintain that the period of cognitive rehabilitation may vary (18).The course of cognitive recovery after post-traumatic coma is irregular, with many ups, downs, and plateaus. Our results indicate that, to achieve a good response and outcome nearing normalcy, a patient needs over 300 h of intensive rehabilitation. This data supports and validates Prigatano’s (3) earlier statement that the effects of cognitive rehabilitation are not observed in patients with TBI who receive less than 100 hours of treatment. Our data is also in accordance with Cicerone et al. (2), whose comprehensive review of the empirical literature on cognitive rehabilitation found evidence supporting this treatment and its advantages over conventional forms of rehabilitation.

It is important to note that patient scores increased and de-creased throughout the treatment period. Progress during any rehabilitation programme, whether it is physical or cognitive, is not uniform. In our study, each cognitive function required a mean of 43 training sessions, with planning requiring the most (69), and mental control the fewest (23). Our results also indicate that not all cognitive functions require the same number of sessions to recover statistic normalcy. For example, long-term memory, orientation and planning differed in terms of time and effort needed to achieve recovery.

In a previous study, we found that consolidation after an ini-tial gain required more rehabilitation time. Each achievement must be consolidated, and this takes time and repetition, which is reported to have significant physiological effects on learning and working memory (19). In clinical practice, we have observed that if the patient is discharged as soon as s/he obtains a score of 7 or 8, the possibility of a drop or regression persists. Time is also required for structural and functional reorganization in the brain. Training cannot be given all at once, although it should be consistent and progressive. Hence, we recommend that this rehabilitation period be scheduled as 4-h daily sessions, 4 days a week. Treatment should not be abandoned if for a short period of time the patient does not show improvement, or if s/he regresses somewhat. Nonetheless, if regression or stalls persist, their causes should be sought before continuing with the rehabilitation programme. Our results indicate that not all cognitive functions require the same type of treatment; some are more costly to recovery in terms of time and effort.

Another finding is of particular relevance to the planning and timing of TBI rehabilitation. We found that the sooner patients receive treatment after injury, the better their cogni-tive outcome, especially in long-term memory and planning.

However, this treatment requires time, especially to consolidate recovery. Memory is a time-dependent process, as shown by McGaugh & James (20). Furthermore, the duration of post-traumatic memory problems, such as amnesia, has traditionally been a better predictor of cognitive outcome than admission GCS score, as shown by Miller et al. (21).

In conclusion, the rehabilitation of cognitive deficits in TBI patients who have emerged from deep coma is advisable when a holistic, intensive and multidisciplinary programme is applied. However, the course of cognitive recovery after TBI is not uniform, and depends on which cognitive functions are impaired, and on the severity of this impairment. Successful treatment of these deficits varies in terms of time and effort. The number of sessions needed to rehabilitate impaired cognitive functions differs from function to function. For example, our results showed that planning and memory require the highest number of rehabilitation sessions to achieve near normalcy. We should also note that cognitive functions are interrelated, and their rehabilitation must be structured to maximize out-come. Furthermore, the consolidation of cognitive gain also requires time, proper training, and well-programmed therapy. We suggest that patient discharge should occur only after cognitive improvements are consolidated. This study provides an approximation of recovery time after TBI. More studies, involving different technology and theoretical bases, could help expand our knowledge of effective post-TBI cognitive rehabilitation.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This research has been funded by a contractual agreement between the Center for Brain Injury Rehabilitation (C.RE.CER) and the Human Neu-ropsychology Laboratory at the University of Seville, Spain. We would also like to extend our appreciation to Dr Fernando Machuca Murga for his assistance in collecting data, and Ignacio Solís Marcos for his contri-butions to data organization and analyses.

REFERENCES

León-Carrión J. Rehabilitation of cognitive disorders after acquired 1. brain injury. In: León-Carrión, von Wild, Zitnay G, editors. Brain injury treatment: theories and practices. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis; 2006, p. 385–500.Cicerone KD, Langenbahn DM, Braden C, Malec JF, Kalmar K, 2. Fraas M, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 2003 through 2008. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2011; 92: 519–530. Prigatano GP. Commentary: beyond statistics and research design. 3. J Head Trauma Rehabil 1999; 3: 308–311; discussion: 322–324.Rohling M, Faust M, Beverly B, Demakis G. Effectiveness of 4. cognitive rehabilitation following acquired brain injury: a meta-analytic re-examination of Cicerone et al.’s (2000, 2005) system-atic reviews. Neuropsychology 2009; 23: 20–39.Cicerone K, Dahlberg C, Malec J, Langebahn D, Felicetti T, Kneipp 5. S, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 1998 through 2002. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2005; 89: 1681–1692.Yu JY. Functional recovery with and without training following 6. brain damage in experimental animals: a review. Arch Phys Med

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 32: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

510 J. León-Carrión et al.

Rehabil 1976; 57: 38–41. xLeón-Carrión J. Rehabilitation models for neurobehavioral disor-7. ders after brain injury. Brain Inj Source 1998; 2: 16–32.León-Carrión J. Rehabilitation of memory. In: León-Carrión J, editor. 8. Neuropsychological rehabilitation: fundamentals, innovations and directions. Delray Beach, FL: St Lucie Press; 1997, p. 371–398.León-Carrión J. An approach to the treatment of affective disor-9. ders and suicide tendencies after TBI. In: León-Carrión J, editor. Neuropsychological rehabilitation: fundamentals, innovations and directions. Delray Beach, FL: St Lucie Press; 1997, p. 415–430.Machuca F, Martín-Carrasco JM, Martín González A, Rodríguez-10. Duarte R, León-Carrión J. Training for social skills after brain injury. In: León-Carrión J, editor. Neuropsychological rehabilita-tion: fundamentals, innovations and directions. Delray Beach, FL: St Lucie Press; 1997, p. 453–468.León-Carrión J, Machuca Murga F, Murga Sierra M, Domínguez-11. Morales MR. Outcome after an intensive, holistic and multidisci-plinary rehabilitation program after traumatic brain injury: medico-legal values. Rev Español de Neuropsicol 1999; 1: 49–68.León-Carrión J, Domínguez-Roldán JM, Murillo-Cabezas F, 12. Domínguez-Morales MR, Muñoz-Sánchez, MA. The role of citicholine in neuropsychological training after traumatic brain injury. NeuroRehabilitation 2000; 14: 33–40.Leon-Carrion J. Computerized Sevilla Neuropsychological Test 13.

Battery (BNS) [Internet]. Sevilla, Spain: Neurobirds; 2010. Avail-able from: www.neurobirds.com. Balestreri M, Czosnyka M, Chatfield DA, Steiner LA, Schmidt A, 14. Smielewski P, Matta B, Pickard JD. Predictive value of Glasgow coma scale after brain trauma: change in trend over the past ten years. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004; 75:161–162.Fuster JM. Cortex and mind. Unifying cognition. Oxford Univer-15. sity Press: New York; 2003.Lezack MD. Neuropsychological assessment. 3rd edn. Oxford 16. University Press: New York; 1995.Lincoln N, Nair R.17. Outcome measurement in cognitive neuroreha-bilitation. In: Stuss D, Winocur G, Robertson IH, editors. Cognitive neuro-rehabilitation, second edition: evidence and application. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1999, p. 203–217.León-Carrión J, Izzetoglu M, Izzetoglu K, Martín-Rodríguez JF, 18. Damas-López J, Barroso y Martin JM, et al. Efficient learning produces spontaneous neural repetition suppression in prefrontal cortex. Behav Brain Res 2010; 208: 502–508.McGaugh, James L. Time-dependent processes in memory storage. 19. Science 1966; 153: 1351–1358.Miller EH , McCarter R, Hameed B, Curran AL, Hunt L, Sharples 20. PM. Duration of post-traumatic amnesia is a better predictor of cognitive outcome after traumatic brain injury than admission Glasgow Coma Score. Arch Dis Childh 2007; 92: A3.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 33: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

511Functional rehabilitation after coma

Appendix I. The Center for Brain Injury Rehabilitation integral, intensive and multidisciplinary model of rehabilitation for people with acquired brain injury

interdisciplinary holistic and intensive programmes - involve brain damage specialists from different fields: neuropsychologists, speech therapists, neurologists, psychiatrists, neurosurgeons, physical therapists, etc.- ecologically-valid- divided into synchronized phases (aims, methods and professionals)use quantitative and qualitative methodseach deficit is allotted the necessary time and dedication to provide best possible outcomeBasic operational requirements:- rehabilitation treatment designed by specialized personnel, adapted to patient’s needspatient/specialist – at least one professional per three patients- adequate installations, apparatus and rehabilitation techniques for efficient treatmentMultidisciplinary Programme structure:Neuropsychological Rehabilitation- based on neurological evaluation of patient’s cognitive capacities and emotional state - outcome goals based on clinical and statistic results of this evaluation - main goal: patients attains maximum degree of functional independence- treatment sessions include individual (and family) psychotherapy – rehabilitation may continue when patient goes home

Speech rehabilitationfluidity, auditory comprehension, denomination, reading, writing, repeating, automatic mechanisms, comprehension of written language and presence of paraphasic errors: - fluency tasks: articulatory agility, length of phrases, verbal agility, etc. - auditory tasks: differentiating, identifying/obeying orders- denomination tasks: visual confrontation, free association, etc. - deficits appearing in reading/writing process are re-taught

Physical rehabilitationspasticity, posture control, balance, trembling, emotional reactivitywe use NeuroBird system of computerized muscular training as well as other physiotherapy techniques (e.g. Bobath)

Occupational therapy/functional therapyfocus on patient’s environment, his/her interests and motivation, culture, values, beliefs and the role the patient plays in his/her surroundings

Efficiency of treatmentCRECER programmes undergo daily evaluation and progress control- neurofunctional state of patient- efficacy of methods applied

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 34: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

J Rehabil Med 44

originaL rePort

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 512–516

© 2012 The Authors. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0968Journal Compilation © 2012 Foundation of Rehabilitation Information. ISSN 1650-1977

NEUROREHABILITATION FOR SEVERE DISORDER OF CONSCIOUSNESS: THE S. ANNA – RAN OPERATIONAL MODEL

Giuliano Dolce, MD, Lucia F. Lucca, MD, Maria Quintieri, Elio Leto, Stefania Rogano, MD, Francesco Riganello, PhD and Loris Pignolo, Eng

From the S. Anna Institute and Research in Advanced Neurorehabilitation (RAN), Crotone, Italy

the operational model and strategies designed for use in the S. Anna – Research in Advanced Neurorehabilitation Insti-tute for the care and neurorehabilitation of subjects in the vegetative or minimally conscious states are described here. a total of 722 patients were admitted, cared for and dis-charged from the institute in the period 1998–2009. Applica-tion of the model approach has progressively shortened the time of hospitalization and rehabilitation and reduced costs.Key words: severe disorder of consciousness; vegetative state; min-imally conscious state; healthcare; neurorehabilitation, outcome.

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 512–516

Correspondence address: Giuliano Dolce, S. Anna Institute and RAN – Research in Advanced Neurorehabilitation, Crotone, Italy. E-mail: [email protected] September 27, 2011; accepted January 30, 2012

INTRODUCTION

The S. Anna Institute – Research in Advanced Neurorehabilita-tion (RAN) for the care and neurorehabilitation of subjects with acquired severe brain damage and disorder of consciousness has been operative in Crotone, Italy, since 1998. The institute aims to meet the needs of a local population of 3–4 million; to date it has admitted, treated and discharged a total of 722 subjects. In the process, dedicated units have been designed and set up to care for subjects with different clinical conditions and at different stages of evolution after brain injury. The functional organization and care and neurorehabilitation procedures in each unit have been designed to respond to the subjects’ needs, particularly for those patients who cannot be discharged or treated at home, who need long-term hospitalization. The aim of continuous reorganization since 1998 was to achieve a progressive, cost-efficient reduction in the length of hospitalization in the semi-intensive units for acute patients and in the duration of the rehabilitation protocols, and to help improve outcomes. The objective of this paper is to describe the model and the strategies designed to operate it.

PATIENTS AND DIAGNOSIS

Subjects with severe acquired brain damage and disorder of consciousness are routinely admitted to the institute upon

referral from intensive care or neurology/neurosurgery units. There are no pre-determined admission criteria, other than autonomous breathing, stability of vital parameters, and ab-sence of indications for further (neuro)surgery. Patients are classified as being in a vegetative state (VS; also referred to as unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (UWS)) by the cur-rent clinical criteria and applicable scales; evolution into a minimally conscious state (MCS)1 (1–6) is diagnosed when reproducible or sustained behavioural patterns associated with evidence of awareness of self or environment are observed (7–11). Outcome is conventionally assessed with the Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS) (12, 13) despite occasional ambiguities in this scale in the classification of VS or MCS (14, 15).

A total of 722 patients were admitted in the period 1998–2009. Of these, 503 were diagnosed as being in VS/UWS according to the current criteria; demographics, aetiology and outcome are summarized in Table I. At admission approxi-mately 25% of referred subjects (n = 219; 30.3%) featured some consistent, although not constant, behavioural responses com-patible with the diagnostic criteria for the atypical VS or MCS. The percentage is consistent with the reported misdiagnosis between the VS and MCS (up to 25–40%) (16, 17); however, the continuous interaction between the S. Anna Institute and the staff of intensive care or neurology/neurosurgery units in the area appears to be incompatible with such a percentage of error. These subjects’ demographics, aetiology and outcome are summarized in Table I and compared with the subjects in VS/UWS at admission in order to infer about evolution and outcome.

1The MCS (8–10) was not defined until 2002 and the revised Coma Recovery Scale (7) was not in use in Italy before 2008 (18). Subjects admitted to the S. Anna–RAN in 1998–2002 were initially diagnosed as being in a VS with (“atypical” VS) or without any consistent behavioural responsiveness; in this regard, the Aspen Neuro-behavioral Conference Workgroup guidelines (9, 10) were informally followed. The clinical records have been revised for the present study and the diagnosis of VS and MCS reformulated according to these guidelines, but this re-classification did not change the perspective of the study. The VS is currently also referred to as UWS (19); this label is intended to help characterize a condition with somehow unclear boundaries, that shares aetiology and underlying pathophysiology with the MCS, but differs as to prognosis, medical, legal, or popular perception of the bioethical issues (20), allocated resources, healthcare policies, etc.

Included in the special issue: CONSCIOUSNESS AND THE VEGETATIVE STATE: TODAY

J Rehabil Med 2012; 44: 481–516

Page 35: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

513Neurorehabilitation for severe disorders of consciousness

INSTITUTE STRUCTURE AND ORGANIZATION

The institute units were designed and sequentially organized in compliance with the country regulation, and in order to guarantee clinical care and neurorehabilitation programmes that meet patient’s individual pathophysiological conditions, evolution during rehabilitation, and needs.

Operative unitsThe following units are operative: a 10-bed (2 rooms with 6 and 4 beds, respectively) semi-intensive care unit, also termed the “Awaking Unit” (Semi-Intensive Care Unit for the severe disorder of consciousness, Fig. 1), is dedicated to subjects with severe disorder of consciousness who meet the criteria for diagnosis of VS/UWS in the acute phase at admission. Three dedicated physicians, 5 therapists and nurses rotate to provide

a total of 10 h’ assistance per day. Temperature and humidity are kept constant and sterile air is circulated (8 times/h). All beds can be moved to upright positions to promote the patients’ adaptation to a vertical position and to help recover autonomic balance. The staff schedule and rotation guarantee an overall level of 8-h/day/patient medical, nursing and neurorehabili-tation assistance. Each patient is monitored by conventional procedures. All subjects undergo a 3 h/day minimum neu-rorehabilitation, compatible with their clinical condition and stability. The protocols for neurorehabilitation are purported to: (i) favour the recuperation of circadian rhythms by providing changes in illumination; start feeding with regular timing as early as possible; schedule all activities during the 24-h period; (ii) minimize all problems due to bedding; and (iii) transfer the subject from bed to wheelchair; adapt the subject to an upright position; and start the procedures or assisted mobili-zation as soon as possible. All subjects are treated regularly in a swimming pool at 38ºC to help counterbalance spasticity and provide the muscle relaxation needed for all rehabilitative procedures to be carried out. The neurorehabilitation protocols include assisted passive mobilization, postural positioning, orthoses, relaxation, stimulation of buccal mucosa, single and group mirror excercises, assisted drawing (Fig. 2a), train-ing in swallowing, training in breathing (clapping, assisted coughing), inhibition of pathological postures, hydrotherapy, automatic walking (Fig. 2b), protocols to withdraw the tra-cheal cannula, etc. Uni- or multi-modal sensory stimulations are presented regularly to help provide communication with the environment.

In the framework of the MIMERICA2 project, an ambient intelligence platform combining traditional and innovative sen-

EMERGENCY and NEUROSURGERY INTENSIVE CARE UNITS

S. ANNA INSTITUTE–RAN (91 beds in total)

SEMI-INTENSIVE CARE UNIT for DOCS (10 beds)

REHABILITATION UNIT for SEVERE BRAIN INJURY (20 beds)

DEDICATED UNIT FOR LONG-TERM

CARE (36 beds)

INTEGRATED BEHAVIOURAL-

COGNITIVE REHABILITATION UNIT

(10 beds)

UNIT for NEURO-REHABILITATION

(15 beds)

DAY-HOSPITAL MONITORING in REMOTE at HOME or

DISCHARGE

Fig. 1. S. Anna–RAN model for the care and neurorehabilitation of subjects with severe acquired brain damage and disorder of consciousness. RAN: Research in Advanced Neurorehabilitation; DOC: severe disorder of consciousness.

Table I. Demographics and outcome of 503 subjects diagnosed and 219 not diagnosed as being in a vegetative state (VS) at admission. The length of time in the intensive care units before admission and in the dedicated semi-intensive care units for VS are shown. The Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS) ranking classes were: 1 = death; 2 = VS exceeding 1 year in duration; 3 = recovery, with severe disabilities; 4 = recovery, with mild disabilities; and 5 = full recovery or recovery with minimal disabilities not interfering with everyday life (12, 13)

Subjects n (%)Age, yearsMean (SD)

Time in intensive care unit before admission, days Mean (SD)

Time in the semi-intensive care unit for VS, daysMean (SD)

GOS rating at discharge, %

1 2 3 4 5

Diagnosed (n = 503)All patients 503 39 (15) 58 (45) 154 (117) 17 16 23 24 20Post-traumatic 302 (60) 29 (14) 50 (47) 140 (118) 5 16 20 29 29Vascular 160 (32) 56 (15) 56 (39) 144 (113) 34 15 31 18 4Anoxic-hypoxic 37 (7) 45 (19) 63 (53) 174 (127) 45 21 18 9 6Others 4 (1) 59 (12) 34 (16) 63 (33)

Not diagnosed (n = 219)All patients 219 44 (19) 37 (20) 74 (72) 5 3 16 30 46Post-traumatic 120 (55) 39 (21) 39 (22) 72 (61) 5 3 14 19 59Vascular 81 (37) 49 (18) 36 (19) 81 (55) 4 4 20 44 28Anoxic-hypoxic 5 (2) 36 (12) 40 (24) 46 (42) 0 0 50 25 25Others 13 (6) 56 (16) 24 (20) 61 (46) 25 0 0 50 25

SD: standard deviation; GOS: Glasgow Outcome Scale.

2The project and development of MIMERICA were supported by the Italian Ministry of University and Research with dedicated funds for competitive pre-industrial research (2004–2007).

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 36: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

514 G. Dolce et al.

sors for the ambient (temperature and humidity, oxygen, light/dark cycles, noise, etc.) and the relevant functional parameters (body temperature, heart rate and systolic/diastolic blood pres-sure, breathing, oxygen saturation level, spontaneous move-ments, voicing, eye movements and blinking, and heart rate variability) of a sub-sample of subjects has been implemented for monitoring. Ambient intelligence collectively indicates pervasive and non-invasive hardware/software infrastructures allowing two-way human interaction with, and full control of, the environment at varying levels of functional complexity. Research into the effects of spontaneous or environment-induced changes in non-neural factors on brain function (e.g. responsiveness) or evolution is in progress. To this end, the platform architecture is interfaced for compatibility and in-terplay with advanced tools for knowledge management and knowledge discovery, processing data to infer new knowledge and potentiate intelligent processing through intensive and iterative processes (21–23).

Subjects emerging from the VS/UWS and recovering into a MCS clinical condition (7–9) are transferred to the 20-bed unit dedicated to the patients with acquired severe brain injury (brain injury care in Fig. 1). In this unit, monitoring is limited to the vital parameters, depending on the patient’s clinical needs; and assistance is provided for a total of 7 h/day/patient. Subjects are treated with standard motor, speech therapy and cognitive rehabilitation procedures, depending on the disabilities ob-served when consciousness is (partially) recovered.

A 36-bed unit is dedicated to the long-term care of patients who have not evolved from a VS/UWS or MCS and are unsuitable for discharge or homecare (long-term care in Fig. 1). Transfer to this unit is made at a time after brain injury that depends on aetiology: 12 months for post-traumatic subjects, 6 for those with major vascular injury and 3 for those who have had mas-sive anoxia-hypoxia. Full nursing and medical assistance, proper feeding/hydration, adaptation to a wheelchair, and passive motor treatments are guaranteed and the possible evolution towards a (partial) recovery of consciousness is monitored by ad hoc protocols. When practicable, the family is trained to be able to take care of the subject at home for limited periods of time, with the aim of re-adjusting the patient to the home environment. Fol-lowing an increase in the number of beds in this unit from 16 to

36, the turnover along the institute units increased significantly (black vertical bar in Fig. 3) (χ2 = 3.679, p = 0.05).

Subjects further evolving from a MCS and (partly) recover-ing consciousness with residual cognitive impairment and/or behavioural disorders that are incompatible with discharge or untreatable at home are transferred to the 10-bed inte-grated unit for cognitive-behavioural rehabilitation, with appropriate nursing and psychological support and cognitive rehabilitation.

Subjects (partially) recovering consciousness with residual major motor disabilities are transferred to the 15-bed unit for motor functional rehabilitation and trained to adjust to future, fully-monitored, remote treatment, at home.

This re-organization has progressively reduced the length of hospitalization in the semi-intensive unit for subjects with VS/UWS and has increased the turnover rate, therefore combining an optimal utilization of the institute facilities with the fulfil-ment of each patient’s needs (Fig. 3).

Work to extend healthcare and neurorehabilitation to patients at home under remote control is in progress. To this end, col-laboration between the S. Anna – RAN and the local govern-ment and healthcare organization (the Oberon project) has been

Fig. 2. Examples of rehabilitation treatment of patient with disorder of consciousness in S. Anna Institute–Research in Advanced Neurorehabilitation. (A) Assisted drawing. (B) Automatic walking.

(A) (B)

Fig. 3. Mean length of hospitalization (days) in intensive care or neurology/neurosurgery units of local hospitals and in the Institute S. Anna–Research in Advanced Neurorehabilitation dedicated units. Note how the increased number of patients in institute acute and the long-term units has contributed to reducing hospitalization in local hospitals. The turnover between units increased significantly after increasing the number of available beds in the long-term unit (black vertical bar) (χ2 = 3.679, p = 0.05).

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 37: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

515Neurorehabilitation for severe disorders of consciousness

established, in order to develop and test the potentialities of remote monitoring and homecare of 54 subjects in a persistent VS/UWS or MCS over a 3-year period.

EARLY RECOVERY AND OVERALL OUTCOME

The evolution from VS/UWS to MCS to recovery and the overall outcome were studied retrospectively by referring to two established major descriptors, namely the GOS (12–13) and the re-appearance of a visual pursuit response (24, 25). In general (and in agreement with previous evidence), post-traumatic patients had better outcomes than vascular patients, and anoxic-hypoxic subjects had the worst outcome irrespec-tive of their condition at admission (24–26).

Subjects not in VS/UWS at admission because of the short time between their emerging from coma and their referral to the S. Anna had shorter hospitalization times, both in the intensive and dedicated semi-intensive care units, and better GOS rat-ings at discharge than those in VS, irrespective of aetiology (χ2= 27.6, p < 0.0001), with a higher probability of scoring a GOS class 5 (χ2 = 11.375, p = 0.0004) and a lower probability of scoring a GOS class 1 (χ2 = 3.309, p = 0.03). Comparable results were obtained when considering post-traumatic and vascular subjects separately (χ2 = 22.26, p = 0.0002, and χ2 = 61.31, p = 0.0001, respectively) (Table I).

Visual pursuit (“the pursuit eye movement or sustained fixation that occurs in direct response to moving or salient stimuli”) is a predictor of favourable outcome, with recovery of consciousness in 73% of subjects in VS/UWS (45% in the absence of eye tracking); it is an established key descriptor of the subject’s evolving from the VS/UWS into the MCS (8–11, 24–27). No differences were observed by testing for a visual pursuit response in the evolution of subjects in VS/UWS due to traumatic or vascular brain injury, who were found to have developed into a MCS in 46% and 49% of cases, respectively, after 50 days. These percentages had increased by 8 months after brain injury, to 89% and 88%, respectively, and had increased further to approximately 90% at discharge or at the end of follow-up (> 235 days). The evolution of subjects with brain anoxia-hypoxia was less favourable, with percentages of evolution increased to a MCS up to 63% at the end of follow-up. Only 12.6% of subjects were diagnosed 8 months after brain injury as still being in a VS/UWS; a later evolution (2 years or more) was observed in 7% of the total group of subjects classified as being in a VS/UWS at admission (25).

The visual pursuit response reflects (partial) recuperation af-ter severe brain injury of the brainstem-cortical interaction and functional organization, which are thought to sustain conscious-ness and are interfered with by the pathophysiological discon-nection resulting in a VS/UWS (25). Its early re-appearance (deemed equivalent to early evolution into a MCS) correlates with a better outcome, confirming the predicting role of this neurological sign (24). However, evolution from the VS to the MCS (at least as indicated by recovered visual tracking) also appears possible several months after brain injury (25).

COMMENT

The extent to which the neurorehabilitative procedures now in use at the S. Anna – RAN Institute or elsewhere are individually or collectively capable of promoting an evolution from the VS to the MCS to recovered consciousness remains, to a relevant extent, undocumented, but a role of the therapeutic milieu, i.e. the synergic effects of the environment and the trainers’ and nurses’ assistance, appears indisputable. Following this rationale, units dedicated to the care and neurorehabilitation of subjects with severe brain injury and consciousness disorders, such as the VS or MCS, are operative in developed countries. The commitment as to resources, logistics, dedicated nurs-ing, rehabilitation and medical care has substantially reduced mortality and the percentage of the so-defined persistent (>1 year) VS. It has improved the chance of favourable outcome, which, in our experience, nevertheless remains worse than for patients with severe acquired brain damage who have never entered into a VS. In our institute, approximately 80% of sub-jects in VS/UWS due to brain trauma recovered consciousness, while 60% attained recuperation to levels compatible with autonomy or allowing quasi-normal life conditions. To this end, healthcare and neurorehabilitation in dedicated units should be made available as early as possible, with a flexible therapeutic continuum congruent to the functional brain organization at-tained at each phase during the evolution from coma to a VS or MCS, to recovered consciousness. In our operative model, hospitalization only exceptionally exceeds 6 months, unless cognitive/behavioural disturbances occur after recovery of con-sciousness. Later evolution from a VS/UWS to a MCS, further improvement to higher levels of functional brain organization, or recovery of consciousness are also possible.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This study was supported by the Institute S. Anna – RAN. The authors thank Professors Leon Sazbon (University of Tel Aviv, Israel) and Walter G. Sannita (University of Genova, Genova, Italy, and State University of New York, Stony Brook, NY, USA) for continuing support and valuable advice.

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

REFERENCES

Jennett B, Plum F. Persistent vegetative state after brain damage: 1. a syndrome in search of a name. Lancet 1982; 1: 734–736.Multi-Society Task Force on PVS. Statement on medical as-2. pects of the persistent vegetative state. N Eng J Med 1994; 330: 1499–1579.Royal College of Physicians. Guidance on diagnosis and manage-3. ment: report of a working party of the Royal College of Physicians. London: Royal College of Physicians; 1996.Jennett B. The vegetative state. Cambridge, UK: University 4. Press; 2002.Dolce G, Sazbon L. The posttraumatic vegetative state. Stuttgart: 5. Thiene; 2002.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 38: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

516 G. Dolce et al.

Saatman KE, Duhaime AC, Bullock R, Maas AI, Valadka A, 6. Manley GT; Workshop Scientific Team and Advisory Panel Mem-bers. Classification of traumatic brain injury for targeted therapies. J Neurotrauma 2008; 25: 719–738.Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK Coma Recovery Scale – 7. revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2004; 85: 2020–2029.Giacino JT, Kalmar K. The vegetative and minimally conscious 8. states: a comparison of clinical features and functional outcome. J Head Trauma Rehabil 1997; 12: 36–51.Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N. The minimally conscious state: 9. definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology 2002; 58: 349–353.Giacino JT, Whyte, J. The vegetative state and minimally conscious 10. state: current knowledge and remaining questions. J Head Trauma Rehabil 2005; 20: 30–50.Taylor C, Aird V, Tate R, Lammi M. Sequence of recovery during 11. the course of emergence from the minimally conscious state. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2007; 88: 521–525.Jennett B, Bond M. Assessment of outcome after severe brain 12. damage. Lancet 1975; 1: 480–484.Jennett B, Snoek J, Bond MR, Brooks N. Disability after severe 13. head injury: observations on the use of the Glasgow outcome scale. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiat 1981; 44: 285–293.Wijdicks E F. Clinical scales for comatose patients: the Glasgow 14. Coma Scale in historical context and the new FOUR Score. Rev Neurol Dis 2006; 3: 109–117.Pignolo L, Quintieri M, Sannita WG. The Glasgow outcome scale in 15. vegetative state: a possible source of bias. Brain Inj 2009; 23: 1–2.Andrews K, Murphy L, Munday R, Littlewood C. Misdiagnosis 16. of the vegetative state: retrospective study in a rehabilitation unit. BMJ 1996; 313: 13–16.

Schnakers C,17. Vanhaudenhuyse A, Giacino JT, Ventura M, Boly M, Majerus S, et al. Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment. BMC Neurol 2009; 9: 35–39.Lombardi F, Gatta G, Sacco S, Muratori A, Carolei A. The Italian 18. version of the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R). Funct Neurol 2007; 22: 47–61.Laureys S, Celesia19. GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, León-Carrión J, San-nita WG, et al.. Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med 2010; 8: 68.Racine E, Amaram R, Seidle, M, Karczewska M, Illes J. Media 20. coverage of the persistent vegetative state and end-of-life decision-making. Neurology 2008; 71: 1027–1032.Aarts E, Harwing H, Schuurmans M. Ambient intelligence: the 21. invisible future. New York: MacGraw-Hill; 2001.Remagnino P, Foresti G, Ellis T. Ambient intelligence, a novel 22. paradigm. USA: Springer; 2005, p. 63–87.Riva G. Ambient intelligence in healthcare. Cyberpsychol Behav 23. 2003; 6: 295–300.Dolce G, Quintieri M, Serra S, Lagani V, Pignolo L. Clinical 24. signs and early prognosis in vegetative state: a decisional tree, data-mining study. Brain Inj 2008; 2: 617–623.Dolce G, Lucca LF, Rogano S, Candelieri A, Pignolo L, Sannita 25. WG. Visual pursuit in the severe disorder of consciousness. J Neurotrauma 2011; 28: 1149–1154.Pignolo L, Quintieri M, Sannita WG. The Glasgow outcome 26. scale in vegetative state: a possible source of bias. Brain Inj 2009; 23: 1–2.Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Brédart S, Laureys S. Assess-27. ment of visual pursuit in postcomatose states: use a mirror. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiat 2008; 79: 223.

J Rehabil Med 44

Page 39: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

information and instructions to the authors – Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine(extended instructions are available at http://www.medicaljournals.se/jrm)

Preparing for submissionSubmission of a manuscript is held to imply that it has not previously been published and is not otherwise submitted for publication, except as an abstract (which in that case has to be stated).

Authorship. All persons designated as authors must participate suf-ficiently in the work to take public responsibility in its contents. For criteria for authors see “Uniform requirements for manuscripts submitted to biomedical journals” Ann Intern Med 1997; 126: 36–47, or JAMA 1997; 277: 927–34. The full document is available at www.icmje.org. If 5 authors or more, a statement on each author’s contribution must be described in the accompanying letter.

Length and number of copies. Articles should not exceed 9 printed pages (approx. 25 double-spaced manuscript pages incl. the reference list).

Submission should be made online at http://www.medicaljournals.se/jrm. For further information please contact the Editorial office.

Language. All papers should be written in English. It is essential that the language is irreproachable.

Conflict of interest and funding. Authors are responsible for recognising and disclosing financial and other conflicts of interest that might bias their work. They should acknowledge in the manuscript all financial support for the work and other financial or personal connections to the work.

Ethics and consent. When reporting studies on human subjects, indicate whether the procedures followed have been approved by an ethic com-mittee (in accordance with ethical standards on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 1983). Do not use patients’ names, initials, or hospital numbers, especially in illustrative material. Identifying information should not be published in written de-scriptions, photographs, and pedigrees unless the information is essential for scientific purposes and the patient (or parent or guardian) gives writ-ten informed consent for publication. Informed consent for this purpose requires that the patient be shown the manuscript to be published.

Copyright. It is a condition of publication that author sign “An exclusive licence to publish” and transfer the rights for layout and publication to Foundation of Rehabilitation Medicine, but will maintain the ownership of copyright. For further information regarding copyright please visit http:// www.medicaljournals.se/jrm (Instructions to authors).

Arrangement of manuscriptPapers should normally be divided into Title page, Abstract, Introduction (including clear statement of the aim of the study), Methods, Results, Discussion, and References.

Title page: A separate title page is necessary and should beara) the title of the article,b) names of the authors,c) the institutions of origin,d) a short title.

Abstract: Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine requires structured ab-stracts for all original articles. The following headlines should be used when appropriate: Objective, Design, Subjects/Patients, Methods, Results and Conclusion. The total number of words should not exceed 200. A list of from 3–8 key-words or terms from Medical Subject Headings (Bethesda, Md. 1966) suitable for indexing terms should be typed at the bottom of the abstract page.Below the abstract: Title of journal; The guarantor’s or the principal author’s complete address, including e-mail.

Statistics. Reporting study results should follow CONSORT for clinical trials with its extension to non-pharmacological treatments (Moher et al., BMJ 2010; 340: c869, Boutron et al., Ann Intern Med 2008; 148: 295–309) and STROBE for observational studies (Vandenbroucke et al., Ann Intern Med 2007; 147: W163–194).

Describe the statistical methods with enough detail to enable a knowledgeable reader with access to the original data to verify the reported results. Give references for advanced or specialized statistical methods and specify any general-use computer programs (including version no.) used. Specify which statistical methods that were used for group comparisons of primary and secondary outcome measures, and those used to adjust for differences between the groups compared. Explain how the study size was arrived at. Present results from a for-mal power calculation only if the calculation was conducted when the

study was planned. A diagram, such as the CONSORT flow diagram, showing the participant flow is recommended. Present background and baseline characteristics (e.g. demographic, social and clinical data) of participants in each study group in a table. Give enough details such that the comparability of the groups and the risk of confounded results in direct group comparisons can be judged.

Mean and SD should only be used to summarize quantitative vari-ables, and with a symmetrical distribution. Use median and percentiles to summarize ordinal scales and quantitative variables that have an asymmetrical distribution. Present dichotomous, categorical and quali-tative data, as well as ordinal scales with a small number of categories, with absolute and relative frequencies (numbers and proportions). When the outcome measures are rating scales, methods appropriate for ordinal data are strongly recommended, such as nonparametric methods for direct group comparisons in treatment evaluations or Rasch analysis.

Present the outcomes in each study group together with the con-fidence intervals, as 95% confidence level. Report estimated effect sizes (e.g. mean or median differences as appropriate) with confidence intervals. P-values can be provided in addition to confidence intervals but should not replace them. The actual p-value should always be reported, at least if in range 0.001–0.30.

References should be numbered consecutively in the order in which they are first mentioned in the text. Identify references in text by Arabic numerals in parenthesis as follows: “as shown by Smith (5)”:... if two authors; “Smith & Jones (6)”;... if more than two authors; “Smith et al. (7)”. The style of references must follow the Vancouver system and for the abbreviations of journal titles; please consult the List of Journals Indexed in Index Medicus, published annually as a list in the January issue of Index Medicus, also accessible at www.nlm.nih.gov.).

Reference to periodicals: Shah S, Vanclay F, Cooper B. Improving the sensitivity of the Barthel Index for stroke rehabilitation. J Clin Epidemiol 1989; 42: 709–710.

Reference to books: Keith RA, Granger CV, Hamilton BB, Sherwin FS. The functional independence measure: a new tool for rehabilitation. In: Eisenberg MG, Grzesiak RC, editors. Advances in clinical rehabilita-tion. New York: Springer Publishing; 1987, p. 6–18.

For journal article in electronic format use the following style: Morse SS. Factors in the emergence of infectious diseases. Emerg Infect Dis [serial online] 1995 Jan–Mar [cited 1996 Jun 5]; 1(1): [24 screens]. Available from: URL: http://www.cdc.gov\ncidod\EID\eid.htm

The number of references for original reports should not exceed 35–40; for other papers see Instructions for authors on the website of the journal.

Tables and figures: Tables are to be numbered consecutively with roman numerals. Each table should be typed on a separate sheet and should have a descriptive heading which is self-explanatory.

All illustrations should be considered as figures. Each graph, drawing, or photograph should be numbered in sequence with Arabic numerals. All figures should have legends, listed on a separate sheet. Line drawing and lettering should be large enough to sustain photo-reduction.

Page chargeUpon acceptance the authors will be charged a fee per printed page. Please see http://www.medicaljournals.se/jrm for further information.

Proofs and offprintsPage proofs are sent directly from the printing office to the corresponding author, via e-mail as pdf-files. It is the authors full responsibility to read and check the proofs against the manuscript. The proof should be returned to the Editorial Office within four days. Offprints may be ordered on the offprint order form accompanying the page proof.

SupplementsLengthy papers may be published as supplements, the full cost being borne by the author. Supplements are subject to editorial revision before publication.

Address of the Editorial OfficeJournal of Rehabilitation MedicineS:t Johannesgatan 22 A, SE-753 12, Uppsala, SwedenE-mail: [email protected]

Page 40: ISSN 1650-1977 REHABILITATION MEDICINE€¦ · Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine is an international peer-review journal published in English with ten regular issues per year. It

ContentsDolce, G., Lucca, L.F. & Sannita, W.G.: Foreword 483

Sannita, W.G.: Consciousness: Today 484

Thibaut, A, Bruno, M.-A. Chatelle, C., Gosseries, O., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Demertzi, A., Schnakers, C., Thonnard, M., Charland-Verville, V., Bernard, C., Bahri, M.A., Phillips, C., Boly, M. Hustinx, R. & Laureys, S.: Metabolic activity in external and internal awareness networks in severely brain-damaged patients 487

Riganello, F., Dolce, G. & Sannita, W.G.: Heart rate variability and the central autonomic network in severe disorder of consciousness 495

Pignolo, L., Rogano, L., Quintieri, M., Leto, E. & Dolce, G.: Decreasing incidence of paroxymal sympathetic hyperactivity syndrome in the vegetative state 502

León-Carrión, J., Domínguez-Morales, M.R., Barroso y Martín, J.M. & Leon-Dominguez, U.: Recovery of cognitive function during comprehensive rehabilitation after severe traumatic brain injury 505

Dolce, G., Lucca, L.F. , Quintieri, M., Leto, E., Rogano, S., Riganello, F. & Pignolo, P.: Neurorehabilitation for severe disorder of consciousness: the S. Anna – RAN operational model 512


Recommended