Date post: | 03-Jun-2018 |
Category: |
Documents |
Author: | natalia-platon |
View: | 238 times |
Download: | 1 times |
of 26
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
1/26
Job satisfaction and life satisfactionrevisited: A longitudinal test of an
integrated model
Joseph C. Rode
A B S TRA C T Research indicates that job satisfaction is significantly related to life
satisfaction. However, previous studies have not included variables
that may confound the relationship. Fur thermore, the vast majority
of studies have relied on cross-sectional data. I tested a compre-
hensive model that examined the relationship between job and life
satisfaction and a broad personality construct called core self-
evaluations, as well as nonwork satisfaction and environmental vari-
ables, using a nationally representative (US), longitudinal data set.
Results indicated that core self-evaluations was significantly related
to both job satisfaction and life satisfaction over time, and that the
relationship between job satisfaction and life satisfaction was not
significant after taking into account the effects of core self-evaluations
and nonwork satisfaction. Implications for theory and practice arediscussed.
K E Y W O R D S job life nonwork personality satisfaction
Results of more than three decades of research have led researchers to
conclude that job satisfaction is significantly related to, or spills over into,overall attitudes towards life, or life satisfaction (see Rain et al., 1991 and
Tait et al., 1989 for reviews). We would expect the two to be related because
of the amount of time spent at work by full-time employees, and also
1 2 0 5
Human Relations
DOI: 10.1177/0018726704047143
Volume 57(9): 12051230
Copyright 2004
The Tavistock Institute
SAGE Publications
London, Thousand Oaks CA,
New Delhi
www.sagepublications.com
http://www.sagepublications.com/http://www.sagepublications.com/8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
2/26
because, for most people, work is a central life activity (Dubin, 1956).
Empirical studies have consistently reported moderate correlations between
job and life satisfaction, an average of .31 or .44 when corrected for atten-
uation (Tait et al., 1989).
However, the relationship is more complex than these numbers imply,
and at least three theoretical perspectives could account for the observed
zero-order correlations between job and life satisfaction. The first perspec-
tive, sometimes referred to as the bottom-up perspective (Brief et al., 1993;
Diener, 1984), proposes that job satisfaction has a casual influence on life
satisfaction because it is part of life satisfaction (Andrews & Withey, 1976;
Cambell et al., 1976; Rice et al., 1985). Life satisfaction is conceptualized,
in part, as the result of satisfaction with various life domains such as work,family, health, etc., and the effects of environmental conditions on life satis-
faction are assumed to be largely mediated by satisfaction with life domains.
Research indicates that satisfaction with work and nonwork domains
accounts for about 50 percent of the variance in life satisfaction (Andrews
& Withey, 1976; Cambell et al., 1976; Hart, 1999; Near et al., 1984).
The second perspective argues that the causal relationship between the
two variables is top-down (Diener, 1984), or that life satisfaction influences
job satisfaction (Judge & Watanabe, 1993; Schmitt & Bedeian, 1982). The
influence of life satisfaction on job satisfaction represents a dispositionaleffect (Staw et al., 1986), whereby the positive affect associated with life
satisfaction results in the recall of a greater number of positive job events
and more positive interpretations of job conditions, which leads to higher
job satisfaction (Bower, 1981; Judge & Hulin, 1993). Probably the strongest
empirical support for this perspective comes from Judge and Watanabe
(1993) who found that life satisfaction had a stronger relationship to job
satisfaction over a 5-year period than job satisfaction had on life satisfaction
over the same period.The third perspective has not been previously explicated, but is
suggested by the results of previous studies. It may be that much of the
relationship between job satisfaction and life satisfaction is spurious, result-
ing from common influences. The literature presents two possibilities. First,
research following the bottom-up tradition indicates that job satisfaction and
satisfaction with nonwork domains are influenced by many of the same
environmental variables (e.g. job income), and as a result, satisfaction with
nonwork domains may confound the relationship between job satisfaction
and life satisfaction. In fact, the percent of variance in life satisfactionuniquely attributed to job satisfaction often falls to 5 percent or lower
(Andrews & Withey, 1976; Campbell et al., 1976; Hart, 1999; Near et al.,
1984) when the effects of satisfaction in nonwork domains are controlled.
Human Relations 57(9)1 2 0 6
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
3/26
The second possibility is suggested by Judge et al. (1997) who proposed
a broad personality construct called core self-evaluations, which they devel-
oped specifically to explicate the process by which disposition may influence
both job satisfaction and life satisfaction. Unlike earlier top-down theories,
Judge et al. proposed that life satisfaction is not a dispositional variable per
se, but that it is influenced by disposition, as is job satisfaction. Empirical
research has found core self-evaluations to be related to both job and life
satisfaction, suggesting that it may be at least partially responsible for the
bivariate relationship between the two constructs (Heller et al., 2002; Judge
et al., 1998). To date, the possible confounding effects of both core self-
evaluations and satisfaction with nonwork domains on the relationship
between job satisfaction and life satisfaction have not been simultaneouslytaken into account.
Irrespective of the underlying theoretical perspective, the vast majority
of the empirical research on job and life satisfaction has utilized a cross-
sectional design. Although the use of longitudinal data in a non-experimental
study does not necessarily establish causality, it does provide stronger
support for causal relationships than can be inferred from analysis of cross-
sectional data (Menard, 1991). This is particularly relevant to studies of job
and life satisfaction, given that the perspectives described earlier are based
on differing causal assumptions. In this study, I tested a comprehensive modelthat enabled me to examine the relationship between job and life satisfaction
over time, while taking into account the possible confounding influences of
nonwork satisfaction, personality (i.e. core self-evaluations), and a set of
environmental and demographic variables. I tested the model using a nation-
ally representative, longitudinal sample of US workers.
Theoretical model and hypotheses
My model integrates the bottom-up perspective and Judge et al.s (1997)
dispositional perspective. Following bottom-up theorists, I propose that
satisfaction with specific life domains (i.e. work and nonwork in this study),
mediates the relationship between environmental conditions and life satis-
faction (e.g. Andrews & Withey, 1976; Campbell et al., 1976; Rice et al.,
1985). I also propose that core self-evaluations is related to both domain
satisfactions and life satisfaction as shown in Figure 1. I incorporated the
dispositional view proposed by Judge et al. over the dispositional proposi-tion that life satisfaction influences job satisfaction for two reasons. First,
Judge et al.s approach offered greater conceptual congruence with the
bottom-up approach which forms the basis of my model. Second, a growing
Rode Job and life satisfaction 1 2 0 7
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
4/26
body of research supports the idea that life satisfaction is not dispositional
in and of itself, but that it is influenced by disposition (Brief et al., 1993;
Diener et al., 1999). For simplicity, I do not explicitly address the indirect
relationships contained in the model, but I do take into account these
mediated effects in the structural equation modeling analysis described later.
Relationships between domain satisfactions and life satisfaction
Research indicates that life satisfaction can be viewed as the result, in part,
of satisfaction with various life domains (Andrews & Withey, 1976;
Campbell et al., 1976). This notion is based on the assumption that indi-
viduals evaluate the details of experience when making overall satisfactionjudgments (Rice et al., 1985). For example, evaluations of ones job are based
on how one evaluates ones pay, supervisor support, working conditions, etc.
relative to desired levels of these variables. The direction of influence is
assumed to be from the specific to the general, in this case from satisfaction
with specific life domains (e.g. work) to overall life satisfaction. From this
perspective, satisfaction with a life domain represents the aggregate evalu-
ations of the domains salient aspects that are taken into account when
making overall life satisfaction evaluations.
Empirically, satisfaction with major life domains (e.g. family, work,health, and leisure) explains about 50 percent of the variance in overall life
satisfaction (e.g. Andrews & Withey, 1976; Campbell et al., 1976; Hart,
1999), with the remaining 50 percent presumably the result of measurement
Human Relations 57(9)1 2 0 8
Controls
Working
Conditions Nonworking
Conditions Demographics
+
JobSatisfaction
NonworkSatisfaction
LifeSatisfaction
+
+
+
+
Core Self-Evaluations
Figure 1 Hypothesized model
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
5/26
error and individual differences (e.g. personality). The effects of domain
satisfactions on life satisfaction appear to be additive, with the most salient
life domains explaining the most variance; there is little evidence of inter-
action effects between life domains (Andrews & Withey, 1976; Campbell et
al., 1976). For parsimony, I classified satisfaction with all nonwork domains
as nonwork satisfaction. The hypotheses also consider the effects of core
self-evaluations, which, as described below, is also proposed to be related to
life satisfaction. Formally stated:
Hypothesis 1: Job satisfaction is positively related to life satisfaction,
controlling for nonwork satisfaction and core self-evaluations.
Hypothesis 2: Nonwork satisfaction is positively related to life satis-
faction, controlling for job satisfaction and core self-evaluations.
Relationships between core self-evaluations and satisfaction
variables
Core self-evaluations are basic conclusions or bottom-line evaluations
(Judge et al., 1997) people hold regarding their selves and their capabilities.Judge et al. proposed four criteria to determine the degree to which disposi-
tional traits were indicative of core self-evaluations: (i) reference to the self,
(ii) evaluation focus (i.e. the extent to which traits involve evaluations versus
descriptions), (iii) fundamentality to more specific surface traits (Cattell,
1965), and (iv) breadth or scope (Allport, 1961). Judge et al. identified four
existing constructs meeting these criteria: self-esteem, generalized self-
efficacy, locus of control, and neuroticism. However, these were not proposed
as comprehensive or exclusive measures of the construct.Judge et al. (1997) proposed that core self-evaluations affected the
perception of objective conditions and events and the norms against which
perceived conditions and events are appraised, which in turn influenced satis-
faction judgments. For example, persons who consider themselves to be
fundamentally incompetent may experience little satisfaction with a given set
of working conditions because they conclude their incompetence will eventu-
ally lead to failure, demotion, and disgrace when they do not perform up
to expectations. Conversely, persons with high core self-evaluations may
experience greater satisfaction with the same set of working conditionsbecause they are more confident in their ability to take advantage of those
conditions. Additional support for this notion comes from the finding that
individuals with low self-esteem (which is considered an indicator of core
Rode Job and life satisfaction 1 2 0 9
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
6/26
self-evaluations; Judge et al., 2002) tend to have unrealistically high perform-
ance level and influence expectations of themselves, which leads to greater
incidences of perceived failure and lower satisfaction (Brockner, 1988).
Several studies provide empirical support for the proposed relationship
between core self-evaluations and job satisfaction (e.g. Judge et al., 1998,
2000). Judge et al. (1997) did not explicitly discuss nonwork life domains,
but the same linking mechanisms should apply to satisfaction with nonwork
life domains as well. The hypotheses reflect the inclusion of a set of environ-
mental and demographic control variables, which, as described later, have
been associated with job and nonwork satisfaction. Formally stated:
Hypothesis 3: Core self-evaluations is positively related to job satis-faction, controlling for environmental conditions and demographics.
Hypothesis 4: Core self-evaluations is positively related to nonwork satis-
faction, controlling for environmental conditions and demographics.
Both Andrews and Withey (1976) and Campbell et al. (1976) proposed that
the self qualifies as a life domain in the same manner as work, family, health,
etc. Campbell et al. (1976) found that a measure of personal competence,
which they argued was closely related to satisfaction with the self, was signifi-cantly related to life satisfaction after controlling for other life domains.
Thus, to the extent that core self-evaluations represents satisfaction with the
self, it should influence overall life satisfaction evaluations in the same
manner as satisfaction with other life domains (Andrews & Withey, 1976;
Campbell et al., 1976). Formally stated:
Hypothesis 5: Core self-evaluations is positively related to life satis-
faction, controlling for job satisfaction and nonwork satisfaction.
Relationships between control variables and domain satisfactions
In an exhaustive study of the effects of working and nonworking conditions
on overall life satisfaction, Campbell et al. (1976) concluded that both the
realities of life and the perception of those realities influenced overall life
satisfaction, but that the effects were almost entirely mediated by satisfaction
with life domains. Subsequent studies found that some environmental
conditions are significantly associated with both interdomain and cross-domain satisfaction (e.g. working conditions affecting nonwork satisfaction;
Near et al., 1983, 1984), which may account for some of the observed corre-
lations between job satisfaction and satisfaction in nonwork domains.
Human Relations 57(9)1 2 1 0
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
7/26
I do not explicitly address the theoretical mechanisms linking each of
the control variables to job and nonwork satisfaction, as the effects of
environmental and demographic variables were not the focus of the study
(see Campbell et al., 1976; Diener et al., 1999; Rice et al., 1985; and Spector,
1997 for detailed discussions of the relationships between environmental
conditions and satisfaction judgments), but I included these variables for two
reasons. First, I wanted to provide a more comprehensive test of the bottom-
up causal flow assumption inherent in my model. Second, including these
variables allowed me to assess the relationship between core self-evaluations
and domain satisfactions, after controlling for the effects of a set of environ-
mental conditions and demographics, which has not been done in previous
studies involving core self-evaluations. I was limited to the measures includedin the data set, which, admittedly, do not represent all the environmental
conditions that could potentially influence satisfaction judgments. Still, the
variable set is reasonably comprehensive, compared with that utilized in
similar studies examining the relationships between environmental
conditions and satisfaction judgments (Andrews & Withey, 1976; Campbell
et al., 1976; Near et al., 1983, 1984).
Methods
The data for the study came from the first and second waves of the Ameri-
cans Changing Lives (ACL) survey, which were collected by the Institute
for Social Research at the University of Michigan (House, 1997). Data were
collected in two waves, 3 years apart, from the same cohort. The survey was
designed to provide a wide range of data on sociological, psychological, and
medical aspects of Americans daily lives.
Sample
The study population for the survey included the US household population,
aged 25 or older, exclusive of residents of Alaska or Hawaii. Residents
residing in households on military bases, in group quarters, or in institutions
were excluded. The survey design specified a two to one oversampling of
interviewees 60 years of age or older and of African Americans aged 2559,
and a four to one oversampling of African Americans 60 years of age or
older. The overall response rate for wave 1 was 67 percent. The attrition ratefor the follow-up survey was 20.7 percent. Subsequent analysis revealed no
significant differences in response rates across demographic variables, except
that Blacks had a slightly higher response rate than the overall sample (71
Rode Job and life satisfaction 1 2 1 1
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
8/26
percent; House, 1997). Trained interviewers conducted both surveys in the
interviewees place of residence. All cases were anonymous, identified with
only a random identification number.
Because I wanted to focus on full-time employees, I excluded inter-
viewees who were unemployed during either survey administration, or who
had worked fewer than 1500 hours in the year preceding either adminis-
tration. I also deleted 15 cases that showed strong evidence of containing
response sets (details available from the author) for a final n = 892. To
compensate for the oversampling of African Americans and people over 60
years old, each case was assigned a weight based on the sampling method-
ology employed: non-Blacks between 25 and 60 years old received a
sampling weight of 4; non-Blacks over 60 years old and Blacks between 25and 60 years old received a sampling weight of 2; and Blacks over 60 years
old received a sampling weight of 1. After applying this weighting scheme to
the data set (sample size was set to n = 892 for all statistical analyses
described later), the final sample was fairly representative of the overall US
workforce at the time, within three percentage points of the overall work-
force in terms of occupational categories, gender, ethnicity and marital status.
MeasuresMeasures available in this data set were not always as extensive as I would
have liked. However, possible measurement limitations not withstanding, the
ACL database provided the opportunity to examine a large, random national
data set, whose results could generalize to the population in much the same
way that presidential polls normally predict voting behavior. These data also
had two data collection points (referred to hereafter as Time 1 and Time 2),
thereby permitting longitudinal analyses, which is very rare in this field and
an important focus of this study. Unless otherwise noted, all multiple itemmeasures were modeled as latent variables in the structural equation
modeling (SEM) analyses described later, with each item serving as an indi-
cator of the latent construct.
Overall life satisfaction
I used two items ( = .75) as indicators of overall life attitudes for Time 2
data. The first item was, overall, how satisfied would you say you are with
your life these days?, rated on a 7-point scale, ranging from 1 (completelysatisfied) to 7 (completely dissatisfied). This item is practically identical to
the single life satisfaction item used by Campbell et al. (1976). The second
item, overall, how happy would you say you are these days?, measured on
Human Relations 57(9)1 2 1 2
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
9/26
a 3-point scale ranging from 1 (very happy) to 3 (not too happy), has been
used extensively in studies of subjective well-being, which is conceptually and
empirically very similar to life satisfaction (Diener, 1984). I recoded both
items so that higher scores corresponded to higher life satisfaction. Descrip-
tive statistics were calculated based on the average of the standardized values
of both items, because of the differing response formats.
Job satisfaction
I measured job satisfaction with two items ( = .78 and .79, at Time 1 and
Time 2, respectively): how satisfied are you with your job? and how much
do you enjoy your work? each recoded so that the rating scale ranged from1 (low) to 5 (high). Descriptive statistics were calculated using the average
of the two items.
Nonwork satisfaction
I created a composite measure of nonwork attitudes consisting of five items
related to satisfaction with various nonwork domains, including health,
marriage, family, finances, and home, all rated on either a 5- or 7-point scale,
with 1 = completely satisfied. Although finances are linked to ones jobthrough job income, satisfaction with finances was included in the nonwork
domain because it is influenced by contextual factors beyond job income (e.g.
spousal income, investment activity, material desires, current and future
perceived financial demands, etc.), which fall within the realm of nonwork
life. I do consider the influence of job salary on both job satisfaction and
nonwork satisfaction in the SEM models described later.
I modeled nonwork satisfaction as a formative variable in the SEM
analyses because the individual items were not intended to measure the sameunderlying construct, but were designed to measure separate aspects of
nonwork life. I was interested in the variance contributed by the items in
total, which is best accomplished by utilizing a single composite (i.e.
observed) variable in SEM analyses (Kline, 1998). I did not attempt to model
error variance in the SEM analyses, nor did I calculate internal reliability
statistics because these analyses assume the underlying construct to be unidi-
mensional, which was not the case here, given that the items focused on
differing, and largely unrelated, aspects of nonwork life. This consolidation
methodology is similar to that utilized in other studies employing generalnonwork satisfaction constructs (e.g. Hart, 1999; Near et al., 1984). I
recoded the items so that higher scores indicated higher satisfaction, and I
standardized the scores before averaging the items, because of the differing
Rode Job and life satisfaction 1 2 1 3
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
10/26
response formats. The resulting composite variable was used in all subse-
quent analyses.
Core self-evaluations
Core self-evaluations measures contained within the survey included the
following: 3 items from Rosenbergs (1965) 10-item self-esteem scale, 3 items
from Pearlin and Schoolers (1978) 7-item mastery scale (described below),
and 5 items from Eysenck and Eysencks (1968) 12-item neuroticism scale.
An example of a self-esteem item was I take a positive attitude toward
myself. An example of a mastery item was there is really no way I can solve
the problems I have. An example of a neuroticism item was are you aworrier?, with yes and no response categories. All mastery and self-esteem
items were scored on a 4-point scale with 1 = strongly agree and 4 =
strongly disagree. Items were recoded so that higher scores indicated higher
mastery and self-esteem levels. All neuroticism items were worded such that
a yes response indicated higher levels of neuroticism. In their original form,
yes responses were assigned a value of 1, and no responses were assigned a
value of 5. Also, in a few cases a neuroticism item had an original value of
3, indicating that the interviewee was unable to select yes or no and had
verbally indicated that the correct response was maybe. These values wererecoded with yes responses = 1, no responses = 0, and maybe responses =
1.5.
Although not specifically mentioned by Judge et al. (1997), mastery
appears to fit well with the core self-evaluations conceptualization in that it
is both fundamental and wide in scope (it refers to the self in general) and
contains a strong evaluative component. According to Pearlin et al. (1981:
340), mastery represents the extent to which people see themselves as being
in control of the forces that importantly affect their lives. This definition isvery similar to Rotters (1966) well-established locus of control concept,
which Judge et al. (1997) specified as a potential measure of core self-
evaluations. Moreover, the mastery construct was developed specifically to
complement self-esteem as a self-evaluations construct (Pearlin et al., 1981).
Unfortunately, although the neuroticism scale showed adequate
internal reliability ( = .69), the abbreviated self-esteem ( = .53) and mastery
( = .39) scales each had unacceptably low internal reliability coefficients. In
an attempt to construct core self-evaluations measures with greater construct
validity and higher levels of reliability, the items from all three scales weresubjected to a common factor analysis with orthogonal rotation. Factors
with eigenvalues > 1, before rotation, were retained. As shown in Table 1,
the results indicated the presence of three separate underlying factors. The
Human Relations 57(9)1 2 1 4
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
11/26
one item that displayed similar loadings on both factors one and two was
retained in factor two, because dropping the item would have significantly
reduced the internal reliability of the scale created from the items loading on
factor two.
The first factor contained two items from the abbreviated self-esteem
scale and two items from the abbreviated mastery scale. The four items loading
onto this factor were averaged to create a new scale which demonstrated an
acceptable internal reliability ( = .68). This new scale was labeled self-esteem
and mastery. The second factor consisted of the five neuroticism items. Unfor-
tunately, the scale formed by the two items loading on the third factor did not
display acceptable internal reliability ( = .45), so these items were dropped
from further analysis. In the SEM analyses, core self-evaluations was modeledas a latent variable, with each of the two derived scales serving as indicators.
This methodology is similar to that used in earlier studies (e.g. Judge et al.,
1998, 2000) which have modeled core self-evaluations as a higher order factor.
However, in this study core self-evaluations was modeled as a first-order latent
variable, because modeling higher order factors requires a minimum of
three first order factors (i.e. scales). Descriptive statistics for the two core
Rode Job and life satisfaction 1 2 1 5
Table 1 Results of core self-evaluations factor analysis
Item (original scale) Factor 1: Factor 2: Factor 3:
Self-Esteem Neuroticism Self-Esteem
and Mastery 1 and Mastery 2
Think I am no good (self-esteem) .60 .12 .14
Pushed around in life (mastery) .57 .12 .05
Feel I am a failure (self-esteem) .52 .09 .10No way to solve problems (mastery) .48 .13 .11
Often feel fed up (neuroticism) .40 .38 .04
Am a nervous person (neuroticism) .03 .74 .14
Am tense/high strung (neuroticism) .12 .53 .05
Am a worrier (neuroticism) .21 .51 .14
Mood goes up and down (neuroticism) .35 .38 .03
Can do anything (mastery) .08 .05 .64
Positive attitude toward self (self-esteem) .30 .25 .38
Eigenvalue 1.61 1.51 .65Percent variance explained 14.67 13.69 5.89
Note: bold indicates the factor on which items were retained for subsequent scale development.
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
12/26
self-evaluation scales were calculated using the average of the items contained
in each scale.
Controls
Most of the measures of environmental conditions were objective and quan-
titative (e.g. salary or household income), but one was perceptual and based
on Likert scales (i.e. perceptions of workplace autonomy). Despite its percep-
tual underpinnings, autonomy has been shown to be empirically distinct
from work attitudes (Glick et al., 1986). I measured autonomy with two
items ( = .64 and = .69 in Time 1 and Time 2, respectively), which were
similar to two of the items included in the widely used three-item scale devel-oped by Hackman and Oldham (1975). The items were I have a lot of say
about what happens in my work and I decide how to do my work rated
on a 4-point agreement scale. Items were recoded so that higher scores corre-
sponded to higher levels of autonomy. I included four other working
conditions measures: annual salary (corrected for non-normal distributions
with log transformations), average number of hours worked per week, self-
employment status (1 = self-employed, 0 = not self-employed), and an objec-
tive measure of job complexity derived by Roos and Treiman (1980) based
on the three-digit 1970 US Census occupation code. Unfortunately, the datawere classified by the 1980 Census occupation code for the follow-up survey,
so no job complexity measure was available at Time 2.
Nonworking conditions control variables were: (i) household size,
measured as the number of individuals related to the interviewee currently
living in the household; (ii) household income, measured as the midpoint
values on an 8-point interval scale (which resulted in a normal distribution
that did not require any transformation); (iii) social integration, measured as
the arithmetic mean of the standardized values of three items related tofrequency of attendance at formal social gatherings (five categories), and
participation in informal social gatherings with friends and relatives (six
categories); (iv) health conditions, measured as a count of 10 chronic health
conditions such as heart disease and arthritis; and (v) care hours, measured
as the number of hours spent in the past year (indicated by the midpoint
values of two separate 5-point interval scales) caring for non-household
persons who either had significant long-term health problems or who experi-
enced serious injury, illness, or personal crises. Social integration was
modeled as formative variable in the SEM analyses. I also controlled fordemographic variables, including age, gender (1 = female, 0 = male), marital
status (1 = married, 0 = not married), and ethnicity (1 = White, 0 = non-
White).
Human Relations 57(9)1 2 1 6
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
13/26
Analysis
I tested the above model in two steps. First, I tested the measurement model
using confirmatory factor analysis prior to estimation of the hypothesizedmodels to prevent interpretational problems inherent in the simultaneous
estimation of measurement and hypothesized models (Andersen & Gerbing,
1988). Second, I developed two structural models (described later) to test the
direct relationships included in the hypothesized model. I estimated all
models using covariance structural equation modeling (SEM) with AMOS 5,
which is comparable (Hox, 1995) to other SEM programs (e.g. EQS,
LISREL). Owing to the large sample size I gauged model fit through the
goodness-of-fit index (GFI), comparative fit index (CFI), and the root mean
squared error of approximation (RMSEA) measure, as well as traditional chi-square test results. All SEM analysis was performed using the maximum like-
lihood procedure.
Results
Intercorrelations and descriptive statistics are shown in Table 2. The corre-
lation between job satisfaction and life satisfaction at Time 2 (r = .30) wasvery close to average unadjusted correlation (r = .31) reported in the meta
analysis by Tait et al. (1989).
To confirm the factor structure of the latent variables (job satisfaction
in both Time 1 and Time 2, life satisfaction in Time 2, core self-evaluations
in Time 1, and autonomy in Time 1 and Time 2), I performed a confirma-
tory factor analysis with each indicator constrained to load on its respective
latent variable and the correlations between the latent variables uncon-
strained. The overall measurement model provided a good fit to the data:2(39, n = 892) = 159.57 (p < .01), GFI = .97, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .05. All
indicators had loadings of .50 or higher. Examination of the correlation
residuals and modification indices and did not find evidence of significant
cross-loadings of indicators, suggesting discriminant validity. Furthermore,
the average variance explained across the indicators of each latent variable
was higher than the variance shared by any two latent variables, thereby
passing a very stringent test of discriminant validity among the latent vari-
ables (Fornell & Larcker, 1981).
To test the relationships contained in the proposed model, I developedtwo separate structural models. In the first model all variables were measured
during Time 1, except life satisfaction, which was measured at Time 2. This
model was designed to assess the relative strength of the effects of core
Rode Job and life satisfaction 1 2 1 7
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
14/26
Table 2 Means or percentages, standard deviations, reliabilities, and intercorrelations (n = 892)
Mean/% SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19
1 Life satisfaction T2 0.00 0.90 75
2 Nonwork satisfaction T1 .00 0.61 42
3 Nonwork satisfaction T2 .01 0.62 57 58
4 Job satisfaction T1 4.11 0.80 24 30 30 78
5 Job satisfaction T2 4.04 0.79 30 31 37 49 79
6 Self-esteem and mastery T1 3.34 0.59 27 29 22 25 24 68
7 Neuroticism T1 .32 0.26 28 32 28 18 20 39 69
8 Job complexity T1 4.88 2.07 03 06 04 10 06 16 07
9 Autonomy T1 3.33 0.64 13 16 16 38 27 25 12 34 64
10 Autonomy T2 3.34 0.65 16 13 17 22 37 21 13 30 51 69
11 Self-employed T1 3% 07 03 04 12 09 04 00 15 07 15
12 Self-employed T2 3% 02 07 02 05 07 01 00 10 03 12 40
13 Salary T1a 9.95 .67 11 15 15 11 05 23 12 44 25 23 08 06
14 Salary T2a 10.18 .64 09 07 09 10 04 20 12 37 21 19 06 03 86
15 Work hours T1 46.43 10.72 08 09 08 14 14 08 00 18 18 18 10 09 33 24
16 Work hours T2 46.19 10.08 06 09 07 09 14 12 06 13 16 19 1 11 16 21 52
17 Household size T1 2.86 1.37 02 07 00 02 06 04 02 10 04 03 04 00 04 03 00 03
18 Household size T2 2.85 1.35 04 04 03 07 00 03 03 08 04 02 01 01 01 03 03 02 73
19 Family income T1 36725 24011 09 18 14 07 07 21 13 34 21 24 12 12 62 46 25 14 10 08
20 Family income T2 44323 27766 11 16 14 09 07 19 12 39 22 24 06 05 67 78 21 19 06 10 73
21 Social integration T1 0.03 0.93 10 07 08 06 10 03 04 13 13 09 00 01 04 06 08 03 00 03 03
22 Social integration T2 0.04 0.90 09 06 09 07 08 07 00 13 13 06 07 08 04 04 01 03 14 14 04
23 Health conditions T1 0.69 0.95 09 17 13 02 01 12 14 10 07 06 05 01 15 19 00 05 09 12 07
24 Health conditions T2 0.77 1.06 14 17 13 03 02 14 16 08 00 08 07 03 14 11 02 06 09 12 06
25 Care hours T1 33.28 62.44 04 05 07 00 07 04 04 02 02 03 05 01 03 01 01 01 01 03 02
26 Care hours T2 40.30 70.61 00 03 06 00 05 01 02 02 07 01 06 02 01 00 01 08 06 05 04
27 Married T1 64% 08 24 11 02 05 00 09 05 06 11 08 05 11 10 09 07 48 42 31
28 Married T2 66% 17 19 22 03 04 01 10 07 06 12 07 05 14 20 10 10 38 46 27
29 Age T1 42.67 12.41 04 14 15 21 16 06 08 00 11 07 05 01 04 03 03 07 21 33 1130 Female 42% 09 13 08 01 00 08 15 05 06 11 09 04 28 25 23 24 06 14 18
31 White 79% 06 06 05 10 01 08 03 26 15 18 12 05 16 16 12 08 10 09 15
Note. Cronbachs alpha appears on the diagonal; decimals not shown for correlations, to save space; all correlation values >.06 are significant, p < .05; all correlation values >.08 are signia Salary variables have been subjected to natural log transformations.
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
15/26
self-evaluations, job satisfaction, and nonwork satisfaction on life satis-
faction over time. In the second model, core self-evaluations measures were
taken from Time 1, and all other variables were measured at Time 2. This
model was designed to asses the longitudinal relationship of core self-
evaluations on job satisfaction, while controlling for environmental conditions
at the time of the job satisfaction measure, and also to examine the relation-
ship between job satisfaction and life satisfaction when the effects of core self-
evaluations on the two variables were subject to the same time lag.
Both models allowed the following paths to be freely estimated: the
paths from each of the control variables (i.e. environmental conditions and
demographics) to both job satisfaction and nonwork satisfaction, the paths
from core self-evaluations to job satisfaction, nonwork satisfaction, and lifesatisfaction, and the paths from both job satisfaction and nonwork satis-
faction to life satisfaction. I allowed the correlations between the disturbance
terms (i.e. the variance not accounted for by the respective exogenous vari-
ables) of job satisfaction and nonwork satisfaction to be freely estimated, as
well as the correlations among the exogenous variables. I did not initially
include paths between any of the control variables and life satisfaction in the
initial models because theoretically I expected that the effects of these vari-
ables on life attitudes would be indirect, mediated by job satisfaction and/or
nonwork satisfaction (Andrews & Withey, 1976; Campbell et al., 1976; Riceet al., 1985).
The models both displayed a good fit with the data; 2(86,
n = 892) = 174.59 (p < .01), GFI = .98, CFI = .98, RMSEA = .03 for Model
1, and 2(81, n = 892) = 144.82 (p < .01), GFI = .98, CFI = .98, RMSEA = .03
for Model 2. I examined the modification indices for the presence of poten-
tially significant paths (i.e. modification indices > 4) between the control vari-
ables and life satisfaction to ensure that all potentially significant effects on
life satisfaction had been taken into account. In neither model did the modifi-cation indices indicate the presence of additional potentially significant paths
to life satisfaction. For presentation simplicity, I excluded variables whose
path coefficients were not significant atp < .05 (e.g. gender) from Figures 2
and 3. In Model 1, three of the five working conditions measures, one of the
five nonworking conditions measures, and three demographic variables were
significantly related to one or more dependent variables. In Model 2, three
working conditions, three nonworking conditions, and two demographic
measures were significantly related to one or more dependent variables.
Contrary to the prediction made in Hypothesis 1, job satisfaction wasnot significantly related to life satisfaction in either model. As predicted in
Hypothesis 2, the standardized path coefficient between nonwork satis-
faction and life satisfaction was significant in both Model 1 (.32, p < .01)
Rode Job and life satisfaction 1 2 1 9
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
16/26
and Model 2 (.54,p < .01). However, core self-evaluations was also signifi-
cantly related to job satisfaction (standardized path coefficients = .24,
p < .01, and .32,p < .01, for Models 1 and 2, respectively) and to nonwork
satisfaction (standardized path coefficients = .44,p < .01 and .33,p < .01,for Models 1 and 2, respectively), as predicted by Hypotheses 3 and 4,
respectively. Finally, core self-evaluations was significantly related to life
satisfaction in both models (standardized path coefficients = .33,p < .01, and
.29,p < .01, for Models 1 and 2, respectively), as predicted by Hypothesis
5. Thus, the relationships posited in the hypothesized model were all
supported, except that no significant relationship was observed between job
satisfaction and life satisfaction.
I also performed two hierarchical multiple regressions in an attempt to
validate the unexpected finding that job satisfaction was not significantlyrelated to life satisfaction, using alternative statistical analysis. The hier-
archical multiple regression models followed the same time lag logic as the
structural equation models described above. Model 1 included all the control
Human Relations 57(9)1 2 2 0
LifeSatisfaction
(.32)
JobSatisfaction(.40)
.32
.49
JobComplexity
.11*
Age
.14
HealthConditions
.14
Time 1 Time 2
Married.22
Autonomy
.13
NonworkSatisfaction
(.31)
Core Self-Evaluations
.24
.33
.44
White
.07*
.19
Work Hours .11*
Figure 2 Results of path analysis of overall life attitudes, Model 1
Note: n = 892; standardized path coefficients are indicated by lines with arrows; correlation
between job satisfaction and nonwork satisfaction error terms is indicated by curved line;
*denotes path coefficients significant at p < .05, all other path coefficients significant at p < .01;
numbers in parentheses represent variance explained. Circles/ovals represent latent constructs.
Rectangles represent indicators.
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
17/26
variables and nonwork satisfaction from Time 1 entered at step 1, core self-
evaluations from Time 1 entered at step 2, and job satisfaction from Time 1entered at step 3. Model 2 included all the control variables and nonwork
satisfaction from Time 2 entered at step 1, core self-evaluations from Time
1 entered at step 2, and job satisfaction from Time 2 entered at step 3. As
shown in Table 2, the results indicated that in both models the incremental
variance predicted by job satisfaction entered in step 3 was not significant.
Thus, these results were consistent with those obtained in the SEM analyses.
Finally, I compared the relative strengths of the correlations of the same
variables over time. If core self-evaluations is dispositional, it should be more
stable than satisfaction measures, which are conceputuized not as disposi-tional, but as influenced by dispositions as well as other factors, including
environmental conditions. Unfortunately, it was not possible to make
detailed comparison of the relative strengths of the correlations of all core
Rode Job and life satisfaction 1 2 2 1
LifeSatisfaction
(.50)
JobSatisfaction
(.40)
.54
.46
Age
.14
HealthConditions
.10
Time 1 Time 2
Married.19
Autonomy
.10*
NonworkSatisfaction
(.21)
Core Self-Evaluations
.32
.29
.33
Hours
Worked.10*
Salary .12
Care Time
SocialIntegration
.10
.07*
.30
Figure 3 Results of path analysis of overall life attitudes, Model 2
Note: n = 892; standardized path coefficients are indicated by lines with arrows; correlationbetween job satisfaction and nonwork satisfaction error terms is indicated by curved line;
*denotes path coefficients significant at p < .05, all other path coefficients significant at p < .01;
numbers in parentheses represent variance explained. Circles/ovals represent latent constructs.
Rectangles represent indicators.
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
18/26
Human Relations 57(9)1 2 2 2
Table 3 Results of hierachical multiple regression (n = 892)
Model 1 Model 2
Independent variables Beta R2 F value Beta R2 F value
Step 1: Control variables .20 26.14** .36 61.43**
Job complexity T1 .04
Autonomy T1 .02
Autonomy T2 .02
Self-employed T1 .05
Self-employed T2 .00
Salary T1a .01
Salary T2a .02
Work hours T1 .03
Work hours T2 .03
Household size T1 .02
Household size T2 .04
Family income T1 .01
Family income T2 .07*
Social integration T1 .08**
Social integration T2 .04*
Health conditions T1 .03
Health conditions T2 .03
Care hours T1 .04
Care hours T2 .04
Married T1 .07*
Married T2 .12**
Age .02 .03
Female .02 .01
White .02 .02
Nonwork satisfaction T1 .34**
Nonwork satisfaction T2 .51**
Step 2: Core self-evaluations .04 35.19** .02 29.70**
Self-esteem/mastery .13** .13**
Neuroticism .12** .10**
Step 3: Job satisfaction .00 1.83 .00 1.61
Job satisfaction T1 .05
Job satisfaction T2 .03
Overall F 27.18** 56.04**
Note: Standardized regression weights are for the full model. Both models included life satisfaction from Time2 as the dependent variable. Model 1 included all predictor variables from Time 1. Model 2 included all predic-
tor variables from Time 2 except core self-evaluations, which was from Time 1.aSalary variables have been subjected to natural log transformations.
* p < .05; ** p < .01.
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
19/26
self-evaluations and satisfaction variables over time, because neuroticism was
not measured during Time 2, and also because no comparable life satisfaction
measure was administered in Time 1. The correlation between the self-
esteem/mastery scale measured at Time 1 and Time 2 was r = .59, which was
greater than the correlation between job satisfaction at Time 1 and Time 2
(r = .49), and similar to the correlation between nonwork satisfaction at Time
1 and Time 2 (r = .58). Thus, while these results with respect to nonwork
satisfaction are unclear, the finding that a core self-evaluations measure was
more stable than a satisfaction measure over time lends some support to the
conceptual underpinnings of the model.
Discussion
This study examined the longitudinal relationships among a broad person-
ality construct (i.e. core self-evaluations), job satisfaction, and life satis-
faction, within a theoretically derived model that also included nonwork
satisfaction, working conditions, nonworking conditions and demographic
measures. I discuss the major findings here.
Relationship between core self-evaluations and job satisfaction
Job satisfaction was found to be significantly related to a personality
variable measured 3 years earlier, after controlling for a number of working,
nonworking and demographic variables measured concurrently with job
satisfaction. This finding lends support to the dispositional theory of job
satisfaction popularized by Staw and his colleagues (Staw et al., 1986; Staw
& Ross, 1985). Although research on the relationships between disposi-
tional traits and job satisfaction has increased significantly over the pastdecade (see Judge & Larsen, 2001 for a review), very few have utilized a
longitudinal design. To my knowledge, only Judge et al. (2000) have
specifically examined the effects of core self-evaluations on job satisfaction
over time. My findings largely replicate those reported by Judge et al.
(2000), but with a more comprehensive list of control variables (Judge et
al. controlled only for the census occupational code based measure of job
complexity), and a nationally representative sample that can be generalized
to the US population. Thus, my results indicate that core self-evaluations
may be partially responsible for the surprising stability of job satisfactionover time observed in previous studies (e.g. Staw et al., 1986; Staw & Ross,
1985).
Rode Job and life satisfaction 1 2 2 3
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
20/26
Relationship between job satisfaction and life satisfaction
As predicted, nonwork satisfaction was significantly related to life satis-
faction in both models. However, contrary to prediction, job satisfaction wasnot significantly related to life satisfaction in either model, after controlling
for core self-evaluations and nonwork satisfaction. This result is notable
because previous studies have found significant relationships between job
satisfaction and life satisfaction, even after controlling for nonwork satis-
faction (Hart, 1999; Near et al., 1983, 1984). In those studies, the zero-order
correlations between job satisfaction and life satisfaction were very similar
to that found in this sample, suggesting that my finding is not due to any
anomaly in the data, but rather to the inclusion of a broad personality
variable (core self-evaluations) that was not included in those studies. Theresults imply that job satisfaction and life satisfaction may not be directly
related, but that the variance shared between the two variables is the result
of a common predictor variable (core self-evaluations) and the fact that both
are correlated with nonwork satisfaction. Why job satisfaction was not
significantly related to life satisfaction, whereas satisfaction with nonwork
domains showed relatively strong relationships with life satisfaction is
puzzling. It may be that for most people work is not a central life activity as
proposed by Dubin (1956). It may be that most Americans work primarilyto support their nonwork lives (George & Brief, 1990; Seeman, 1967), and
that the effects of job satisfaction on life satisfaction are simply too weak to
detect after taking into account life domains that are of greater importance
(e.g. family, health, self).
It should be noted that in both models the disturbance terms of job
satisfaction and nonwork satisfaction (i.e. portion of variance not accounted
for by core self-evaluations, environmental conditions, and demographics)
were significantly correlated, suggesting that the exogenous variables did not
account for all the variance shared between the two variables. This impliesthat job satisfaction and nonwork satisfaction are either influenced by
common variables not included in the model (e.g. additional environmental
or personality variables), or that a causal relationship exists between the two
variables. The theoretical model utilized in this study suggests the former,
although this is certainly an area for future research.
Implications
Although life satisfaction is certainly a desirable outcome by itself, the
positive organizational behavior view recently explicated by Luthans
(2002) suggests that life satisfaction or subjective well-being (of which life
Human Relations 57(9)1 2 2 4
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
21/26
satisfaction is a central component; Diener, 1984; Diener et al., 1999) may
be related to other important variables as well. For example, empirical
evidence suggests that life satisfaction may be directly related to in-role
performance (Rode et al., 2003), and also to supervisor ratings of employee
performance even when job satisfaction is not related to such ratings (Wright
& Cropanzano, 2000). Also, subjective well-being is associated with the
onset of heart disease (Booth-Kewley & Friedman, 1987), which has obvious
implications for employee absenteeism and healthcare costs.
The results of this study imply that organizations wishing to increase
employee life satisfaction may wish to consider at least two possibilities.
First, they could implement policies and benefits that allow employees to
attend to, and further develop, the nonwork domains of their lives (e.g.flexible working arrangements, employee assistance programs for personal
issues, and other family-friendly work practices), given that nonwork satis-
faction was strongly related to life satisfaction. Conversely, interventions
designed to increase job satisfaction, although certainly desirable, may have
negligible impacts on life satisfaction.
Second, the results indicated that core self-evaluations had significant
direct and indirect effects on life satisfaction. Although personality traits, by
definition, demonstrate stability across time, an encouraging body of
evidence suggests that some dispositions can change as a result of environ-mental conditions and learning, particularly those related to evaluations
regarding personal control (e.g. Brockner, 1988; Markus & Kunda, 1986),
which is a central component of core self-evaluations. Thus, designing jobs,
organizational structures, and human resource policies to facilitate a sense
of control in both work and nonwork domains may lead to higher core self-
evaluations which the results of the current study suggest may facilitate
higher levels of job satisfaction, nonwork satisfaction, and also life satis-
faction. However, very little is known about the existence or the strength ofthe relationship between workplace variables and core self-evaluations; this
appears to be a potentially rich area for research.
Limitations and conclusions
At least four limitations should be noted. First, the data set contained a
limited number of measurement items for several key constructs, which may
have impacted the reliability, and by extension, the validity of those
constructs. Specific measurement issues existed with respect to the job satis-faction, life satisfaction, and core self-evaluations variables. Both the job
satisfaction and life satisfaction variables were measured using two items,
instead of more robust scales which generally include four or more items to
Rode Job and life satisfaction 1 2 2 5
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
22/26
measure these constructs. However, a meta-analysis by Wanous et al. (1997)
reported minimum reliability levels to be at least .70 for the included single-
item job satisfaction measures. Also, the correlation between job satisfaction
and life satisfaction at Time 2 was nearly identical to the average unadjusted
correlation reported in the meta analysis by Tait et al. (1989), which provides
some, but certainly not conclusive, support for the validity of the abbrevi-
ated measures. Core self-evaluations was measured using 9 items taken from
three different scales, in contrast to previous studies which have utilized as
many as 45 items from four scales to measure the construct. The limited
number of items required that I model the construct as a first-order latent
variable, instead of a higher order factor, as in previous studies. Although
the items utilized in the current study fit well with the conceptual definitionof the construct, and the factor structure of the latent variables were vali-
dated by confirmatory factor analysis, it is impossible to determine the extent
to which the use of fewer indicators impacted the results. The strength of the
results in both of the tested models with respect to core self-evaluations
provides some confidence in the measure, but they do not provide con-
clusive proof of validity.
A second potential limitation concerns the possible effects of common
method variance, as both the independent and dependent variables were
subjective measures derived from survey instruments. However, the effects ofcommon method variance may have been limited in this study for at least
two reasons. First, the longitudinal data set provided 3-year lags between
many of the subjective measures (precisely which measures varied somewhat
by model) which removed common method variance resulting from mood
and situational cues. Second, in two similar studies involving core self-
evaluations and satisfaction measures, Judge et al. (1998, 2000) found only
minor differences in their results when measures where taken from a combi-
nation of self-reports and other reports versus all self-reports, suggesting thatthe primary variables of interest to this study may not be significantly
affected by common method variance.
Third, the generalizability of the results may be impacted somewhat by
the oversampling of African Americans and individuals over the age of 60.
While weighting schemes designed to compensate for oversampling are
commonly used in large-scale survey research, it is impossible to assess the
effectiveness of the weighting strategy employed in this particular sample on
variables other than readily available demographics.
Finally, some authors have noted high correlations between autonomyand job satisfaction (e.g. Fried, 1991), and have suggested that modeling
autonomy as a predictor of job satisfaction may partial out true variance in
job satisfaction. In the current study, autonomy was modeled as a predictor
Human Relations 57(9)1 2 2 6
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
23/26
of job satisfaction, but not of life satisfaction. Thus, any true variance
partialled out of the job satisfaction measure would have provided for a more
conservative test of the predictors of job satisfaction (i.e. core self-
evaluations), but would not have affected the relationship between job
satisfaction and life satisfaction.
Obviously, the finding that job satisfaction and life satisfaction may
not be directly related has important implications for both theory and
practice, especially given that research indicates that life satisfaction may be
related to several outcomes of interest to managers (Booth-Kewley &
Friedman, 1987; Wright & Cropanzano, 2000) beyond the desirability of life
satisfaction as an end in and of itself. Overall, the results indicated that
paying greater attention to employees personal characteristics and nonworklives may yield important benefits, for both managers and organizational
behavior researchers alike, that may not be realized by focusing only on
employee job satisfaction.
Acknowledgements
This article is based on my dissertation for which I would like to thank my disser-
tation committee. I would also like to thank Janet Near and three anonymousreviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier draft.
References
Allport, F.W. Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Wilson,1961.
Andersen, J.C. & Gerbing, D.W. Structural equation modeling in practice: A review andrecommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 1988, 103, 41123.
Andrews, F.A. & Withey, S.B. Social indicators of well-being in America: The developmentand measurement of perceptual indicators. New York: Plenum Press, 1976.
Booth-Kewley, S. & Friedman, H.S. Psychological predictors of heart disease: A quantita-
tive review. Psychological Bulletin, 1987, 101, 34262.Bower, G.H. Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 1981, 36, 12948.Brief, A.P., Butcher, A.H., George, J.M. & Link, K.E. Integrating bottom-up and top-down
theories of subjective well-being: The case of health.Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 1993, 74, 64653.
Brockner, J. Self-esteem at work: Research, theory, and practice. Lexington, MA: Lexing-ton Books, 1988.
Campbell, A., Converse, P.E. & Rodgers, W.L. The quality of American life: Perceptions,
evaluations and satisfaction. New York: Sage Foundation, 1976.Cattell, R.B. The scientific analysis of personality. Baltimore: Penguin, 1965.Diener, E. Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 1984, 95(3), 54275.Diener, E., Suh, R.E., Lucas, R.E. & Smith, H.L. Subjective well-being: Three decades of
progress. Psychological Bulletin, 1999, 25, 276302.
Rode Job and life satisfaction 1 2 2 7
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
24/26
Dubin, R. Industrial workers worlds: The Central Life Interests of industrial workers.
Journal of Social Issues, 1956, 3, 13142.Eysenck, H.J. & Eysenck, S.G. Manual for the Eysenck Personality Inventory. San Diego,
CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service, 1968.
Fornell, C. & Larcker, D.F. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable vari-ables and measurement error.Journal of Marketing Research, 1981, 18, 3950.
Fried, Y. Meta-analytic comparison of the job diagnostic survey and job characteristics
inventory as correlates of work satisfaction and performance. Journal of AppliedPsychology, 1991, 76(5), 6907.
George, J.M. & Brief, A.P. The economic instrumentality of work: An examination of the
moderating effects of financial requirements and sex on the pay-life satisfaction relation-
ship.Journal of Vocational Behavior, 1990, 37, 35768.Glick, W., Jenkins, G. & Gupta, N. Method versus substance: How strong are the under-
lying relationships between job characteristics and attitudinal outcomes? Academy ofManagement Journal, 1986, 29, 44164.
Hackman, J.R. & Oldham, G.R. Development of the job diagnostics survey. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 1975, 60, 15970.
Hart, P.M. Predicting employee life satisfaction: A coherent model of personality, work andnonwork experiences, and domain satisfactions.Journal of Applied Psychology, 1999,
84(4), 56484.Heller, D., Judge, T.A. & Watson, D. The confounding role of personality and trait affec-
tivity in the relationship between job and life satisfaction. Journal of OrganizationalBehavior, 2002, 23(7), 81535.
House, J.S. Americans changing lives: Waves I and II, 1986 and 1989. Ann Arbor:University of Michigan Press, 1997.
Hox, J.J. Amos, EQS, and LISREL for Windows: A comparative review. Structural
Equation Modeling, 1995, 2, 7991.Judge, T.A., Bono, J.E. & Locke, E.A. Personality and job satisfaction: The mediating role
of job characteristics.Journal of Applied Psychology, 2000, 85(2), 23749.Judge, T.A., Erez, A., Bono, J.E. & Thoresen, C.J. Are measures of self-esteem, neuroti-
cism, locus of control, and generalized self-efficacy indicators of a common core
construct?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2002, 83, 693710.Judge, T.A. & Hulin, C.L. Job satisfaction as a reflection of disposition: A multiple-source
causal analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 1993, 56,388421.
Judge, T.A. & Larsen, R.J. Dispositional affect and job satisfaction: A review and theor-etical extension. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 2001, 86(1),
6798.Judge, T.A., Locke, E.A. & Durham, C.C. The dispositional causes of job satisfaction: A
core evaluations approach. Research in Organizational Behavior, 1997, 19, 15188.Judge, T.A., Locke, E.A., Durham, C.C. & Kluger, A.N. Dispositional effects on job and
life satisfaction: The role of core evaluations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1998,83(1), 1734.
Judge, T.A. & Watanabe, S. Another look at the joblife satisfaction relationship.Journalof Applied Psychology, 1993, 78, 93948.
Kline, R.B. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: GuilfordPress, 1998.
Luthans, F. Positive organizational behavior: Developing and managing psychological
strengths. Academy of Management Executive, 2002, 16(1), 5775.Markus, H. & Kunda, Z. Stability and malleability of the self-concept. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 1986, 51, 85866.Menard, S. Longitudinal research. Sage university paper series on quantitative applications
in the social sciences. Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 1991.
Human Relations 57(9)1 2 2 8
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
25/26
Near, J.P., Smith, C.A., Rice, R.W. & Hunt, R.G. Job satisfaction and nonwork satisfactionas components of life satisfaction. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1983, 13,12644.
Near, J.P., Smith, C.A., Rice, R.W. & Hunt, R.G. A comparison of work and nonworkpredictors of life satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal, 1984, 27, 37794.
Pearlin, L., Lieberman, M., Menaghan, E. & Mullan, J. The stress process. Journal ofHealth and Social Behavior, 1981, 22, 33756.
Pearlin, L. & Schooler, C. The structure of coping.Journal of Health and Social Behavior,1978, 19, 221.
Rain, J.S., Lane, I.M. & Steiner, D.D. A current look at the job satisfaction/life satisfactionrelationship: Review and future considerations. Human Relations, 1991, 44, 287307.
Rice, R.W., McFarlin, D.B., Hunt, R.G. & Near, J.P. Organizational work and the perceived
quality of life: Toward a conceptual model. Academy of Management Review, 1985,10, 296310.
Rode, J.C., Arthaud-Day, M.L., Mooney, C.H., Near, J.P., Baldwin, T.T., Bommer, W.H.
& Rubin, R.S. Life satisfaction and performance. Submitted for publication, 2003.Roos, P.A. & Treiman, D.J. Worker functions and worker training for the 1970 U.S. census
classification. In P.S. Cain & P.A. Roos (Eds), A critical review of the Dictionary ofOccupational Titles. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 1980, pp. 33689.
Rosenberg, M. Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton UniversityPress, 1965.
Rotter, J.B. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement.
Psychological Monographs, 1966, 80(1).Schmitt, N. & Bedeian, A.G. A comparison of LISREL and two-stage least squares analysis
of a hypothesized lifejob satisfaction relationship. Journal of Applied Psychology,1982, 67, 80617.
Seeman, M. On the personal consequences of alienation of work. American SociologicalReview, 1967, 32, 27385.
Spector, P.E.Job satisfaction: Application, assessment, causes, and consequences. ThousandOaks, CA: Sage, 1997.
Staw, B.M., Bell, N.E. & Clausen, J.A. The dispositional approach to job attitudes: Alifetime longitudinal test. Administrative Science Quarterly, 1986, 31, 5677.
Staw, B.M. & Ross, J. Stability in the midst of change: A dispositional approach to job
attitudes.Journal of Applied Psychology, 1985, 70, 46980.Tait, M., Padgett, M.Y. & Baldwin, T.T. Job and life satisfaction: A re-examination of the
strength of the relationship and gender effects as a function of the date of the study.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 1989, 74, 5027.
Wanous, J.P., Reichers, A.E. & Hudy, M.J. Overall job satisfaction: How good are single-item measures?Journal of Applied Psychology, 1997, 82(2), 24752.
Wright, T.A. & Cropanzano, R. Psychological well-being and job satisfaction as predictorsof job performance.Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 2000, 5(1), 8494.
Rode Job and life satisfaction 1 2 2 9
8/12/2019 Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction
26/26
Human Relations 57(9)1 2 3 0
Joe Rode is an Assistant Professor of Management in the Richard T.
Farmer School of Business at Miami University in Oxford, Ohio. He
received his PhD in organizational behavior and human resources fromIndiana University, Bloomington in 2002. His research interests include
the relationships between work and nonwork domains and the effects of
individual differences on human performance.
[E-mail: [email protected]]