+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in...

Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in...

Date post: 08-May-2018
Category:
Upload: vuongquynh
View: 225 times
Download: 1 times
Share this document with a friend
17
Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise Menn, University of Colorado Ma4hew Goldrick, Northwestern University
Transcript
Page 1: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

Language and the Brain, 1924-2014

Developments in Neurology/Neuroscience,

Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics

Lise  Menn,  University  of  Colorado  Ma4hew  Goldrick,  Northwestern  University  

Page 2: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

1924: Disciplines isolated, language data only via M.D.’s

descriptions •  Neurology: Best available analogy for brain

function was telephone switchboard. Understood that information is relayed from some parts of brain to other parts, but no clue about nature of the sources of that information.

•  Linguistics: not in the picture – no corpora! Recording devices clumsy, used by ethnomusicologists but by few other scientists (c.f. Bloomfield, via Keating; also what Barbara Partee said about semantics).

•  Psychology: Stimulus-response behaviorism beginning to dominate (cf. Bever!); introspection discredited (and not much help anyway)

3  jan  2014   2  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

Page 3: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

Neurology: Trying to link brain damage to behavior change •  Time lag: brain injuries mapped at autopsy

had to be compared to descriptions of language behavior, possibly from years earlier. –  during that time, the brain damage could

have partly healed, or gotten worse

•  The  only  in  vivo  evidence  for  locaGon  of  brain  damage  came  from  figuring  the  trajectories  of  penetraGng  brain  wounds    

•  or  from  noGng  damage  to  sensory  and  motor  abiliGes  (the  motor  and  sensory  cortex  had  been  fairly  well  mapped  by  1924).  

3  jan  2014   3  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

Page 4: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

The most comprehensible theorists were the ‘localizationists’ •  The localizationists, also called ‘connectionists’, envisioned the parts of the brain that

they mapped out as a collection of ‘centers’ containing motor or sensory images, connected by bundles of nerve fibers which transmitted information from one to another (telephone switch-board model) and eventually to muscles.

•  Severing specific connections ( ), e.g. between visual input and visual memory for letters, could explain puzzles like patients who can write but not read (‘alexia without agraphia’). But it drastically oversimplified many cases.

B  =  Begriff  ‘concept’  (large    interconnected  set      of  memory  images)  

M  =  Motor  memory    images  for  arGculaGon  

m  =  motor  output  pathway    for  speech  

A  =  Auditory  sensory  images    for  word  sounds  

a  =  pathway  for  auditory    informaGon  

slide  based  on  Graves  (2009),  ‘The  Legacy  of  the  Wernicke-­‐Lichtheim  Model’  3  jan  2014   4  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

✖✖  

✖  

Page 5: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

Complicated (but not rare!) cases Head (1926:179), clinical example •  Young officer, blunt trauma, left parieto-occipital region, “perpetually at

a loss for names”. Asked to name the color of a black patch: …people who are dead…the other people who are not dead, they have this color.

“Choice of colours to oral command [was] slow.” – Explaining slowness of comprehension in terms of some kind of disconnection requires a lot of ad hoc apparatus!

•  Pointed to matchbox on seeing printed word MATCHES, but unable to get meaning from WATCH until he spotted a wall clock. When I look at that big one (clock) that helps me…If you say it to me, I see it at once; if you show it to me like that (printed), I have to think, I don’t get the picture easily. –  Information from seeing a printed word and seeing an object it can refer to - or a

semantically/visually related object - have to combine for this patient to understand the word he is looking at. Localizationist brain model has no way to combine different types of information.

3  jan  2014   5  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

Page 6: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

Many localizationists failed to apply the basic logic of troubleshooting complex systems

If you have an amplifier not giving you normal sound and you see a loosely connected component, you can guess that that component is involved in delivering the sound. You don’t assume, without further evidence, that it’s the source of the sound. But many localizationists did the equivalent: if a disability was associated with a lesion (injury) in a particular area, they concluded that that area was responsible for that ability - e.g. ‘Exner’s center’, supposed to be ‘the location’ for reading.

3  jan  2014   6  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

Page 7: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

Some localizationists got the logic right: Head (1926) expounds Arnold Pick:

“When … he states that syntactical deficits [agrammatism] are caused by a lesion of the left temporal lobe, he does not mean that

“grammatism”, or the correct use of syntax, is centred within this region. He implies solely that a lesion, situated in this part of the brain, can disturb the processes of normal speech in such a way that the phenomena of agrammatism become apparent.”

3  jan  2014   Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014   7  

But cautious, complex statements like this don’t make good sound bites. The people who made them were overshadowed by those who said confidently “Here is grammar, there is speech, over there is reading.”

Page 8: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

No corpora, no linguistics

Working with aphasic speakers is not like field work with normal speakers. Aphasic speakers have much less control, can’t voluntarily ‘say it again’. Transcription on the fly is

difficult, especially with ‘fluent’ aphasic speakers like this person, who were described by doctors as having ‘normal syntax’.  

•  Apparently, no recordings were made of aphasic speakers.

✖  

VIDEO  HERE  

3  jan  2014   8  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

§  “Normal” label was probably cued by use of formulaic expressions like the ones in this clip: “Pretty good, actually…” “Yeah, I guess…”

Page 9: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

Recording Speech Makes Linguistic Analysis Possible

Spoken  words  with  morpheme  boundary  markers  (-­‐  ⎦    ⎣)  

Und  es  rinn-­‐t⎦            der  Hahn    ⎣über    

TranslaGon  with    labeled  grammaGcal  morphemes  

And  it  flow-­‐PRES,3SG⎦      the  faucet          ⎣over-­‐SEP  PREF    

Colloquial  equivalent   ‘And  the  faucet  is  overflowing’    

Stark & Dressler (1990) in Menn & Obler (eds.) Agrammatic Aphasia:

Target  picture:  water  is  running  from  faucet,  overflowing  a  sink.  “Blend”  of  two  intenGons:    

The  faucet  is  running    +    The  water  is  overflowing  

3  jan  2014   9  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

Page 10: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

Measurement Makes Linguistic Analysis Possible •  Reveals sub-perceptual variation—in principle

inaccessible to transcription

•  Building on techniques developed for medical imaging (e.g., cardiac), real-time MRI allows imaging of global configurations of vocal tract (Narayaran et al. 2004). –  Example movie with articulator tracings.

•  Hagedorn et al. (2012): Real-time MRI of apraxic repetition reveal covert gestures not visible in acoustic signal –  Frame from /f/ in “federation,” heard and

transcribed as [r]. –  Imaging reveals labial closure is present,

but is obscured, hard to hear because of the simultaneously produced /r/ gesture.

Figure 3: Acoustic waveform (top) and time-aligned estimated constriction functions (labial, coronal, dorsal) in second /kɒp-tɒp/ repetition trial: apraxic speaker. Labial and tongue tip gestures coordinated in-phase (synchronously) (arrows). Dorsal gestures are missing at expected times (boxes).

Figure 4: Audio signal (top) and time-aligned labial, coronal & dorsal constriction functions reveal covert tongue tip intrusion during labial closure for /b/ in apraxic utterance “I can type ‘bow know’ five times.”

Some of the individual words produced by the subject also reveal evidence of gesture intrusions. Acoustic analysis of one of the subject’s responses to the stimulus item “federation” suggests a form which might be represented in close transcription as [ɹɛd&ɹeɪʃ&n]. Articulatory analysis of the same utterance using rtMRI provided additional insights: the segment transcribed as [ɹ] was found not to arise from simple “anticipatory substitution” (segment [ɹ] replaces segment [f]); rather, the initial labial gesture of target /f/ was observed to be synchronously produced with an anticipatory lingual intrusion gesture for [ɹ]. Target and erroneous productions of the initial portion of the word “federation” are compared in Figure 5.

3.1 Multiple Initiation Gestures in Spontaneous Speech

Consistent with earlier findings [5], multiple initiation gestures by this subject were found to be more frequent in spontaneous speech than in imitated speech. Segments used word-initially by the apraxic subject in spontaneous speech included /t/, /d/, /g/, /k/, /b/, /dʒ/, /w/, /f/, /s/, /n/, /m/, /l/, /ð/, and /h/. Tokens exhibiting multiple initiation gestures are defined as those in which a visible articulatory gesture occurs at least once before complete production of the word.

Token 1 Token 2

Figure 5: Two productions by apraxic subject of initial fricative in lexical item “federation.” Left: target production; Right: labial constriction co-produced with intrusive lingual gesture corresponding to tongue posture observed during [ɹ] production later in same word. Labialization not discernible in acoustic signal of second token.

Words exhibiting multiple initiations included those starting with /w/ (100% of /w/ tokens), /dʒ/ (50% of tokens), /s/ (16%), /m/ (20%), /l/ (20%), /t/ (50%), and /d/ (50%). It is noteworthy that the majority of these segments that were problematic for the subject require more than one vocal tract gesture. This suggests that the added complexity of gestural coordination required for the production of multi-gestural segments might present additional challenges in planning, with the result that they exhibit false starts, or perhaps explicit articulation as part of the planning process. In many cases, the repeated initiation gestures are covert, being articulated without any phonation, and could not, therefore, be captured by acoustic analysis or adequately represented in standard phonetic transcription.

In Figure 6, a silent tongue tip gesture (arrow) can be seen to precede full (and audible) production of the coronal-initial word no [noʊw] in the utterance “I can type ‘bone no’ five times”. Under acoustic analysis, the duration of the covert gesture might be interpreted as a pause, however using rtMRI, we find that a complete tongue tip gesture is present.

Evidence for covert gestural rehearsal is also shown in Figure 7, where three silent tongue tip gestures can be observed in the interval before the vocalized production of the coronal stop which initiates successful production of the complete lexical item temperatures.

Figure 6: Covert tongue tip gesture during first (silent) attempt at producing coronal-initial word ‘no’ in the utterance “I can type ‘bone no’ five times.”

0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5

Dorsal

Time (sec)

I can type (TT) bow know0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5

Con

stric

tion

Coronal

I can type (TT) bow know0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5

Deg

ree Labial

I can type (TT) bow know0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5

I can type (TT) bow know

p b

nt

k

oUoU

n

3  jan  2014   10  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

Page 11: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

‘The Cognitive Revolution’: Psycholinguistics •  Basic elements of mind/brain are complex computational

processes manipulating structured mental representations. •  Behavior reflects the coordinated interaction of these

processes. Ø  Complexity of component processes and interactions moved far

beyond simple stimulus-response theories.

•  Integrated into study of language disorders –  Marshall and Newcombe (1966) –  Semantic errors (CANARY à“parrot”) reflect

cognitive consequence of brain injury: Disrupted access to stored “lexical entries,” specifying syntactic, semantic categories and features (explicitly referencing proposal of Katz & Fodor, 1963)

Freda  Newcombe,  1925-­‐2001    3  jan  2014   11  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

Page 12: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

Complex Computation: Parallel Activation •  Complex computational structure allows us to understand

complex behavioral phenomena. •  Parallel activation: At many (all?) stages of processing, in

perception and production, computation involves simultaneous activation of multiple linguistic representations.

•  Parallel activation of alternative formulations of message provides an account of syntactic blends –  Aphasia example from above: The faucet is overflowing

§  The faucet is running + The water is overflowing –  Normal speaker’s speech error:

§  The road to Chicago is straight as a pancake §  straight as an arrow + flat as a pancake (Cutting & Bock, 1997)

–  Constrained by complex internal structure of idioms, collocations (≈ frequent word sequences; Menn & Duffield, 2013)

3  jan  2014   12  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

Page 13: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

From Static Pathology to the Living Brain •  Quantitative shift since 1924: Imaging technology allows a

precise view of brain structure –  In both pathological and healthy brains.

•  Qualitative shift: We can now observe the healthy brain in action. –  Electrical activity of ensembles of neurons –  Metabolic activity correlated with neuronal processing

First language-related fMRI: Heat map of increase in blood flow across tasks includes Broca’s area (McCarthy et al. 1993)

Earliest human EEGs: Patient of Hans Berger, 1924

3  jan  2014   13  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.USA90(1993)4953

axC1) Ccn.r_U-i60)C,,

8%7%6%5%4%3%2%1%0%-1%-2%-3%

C

C;C)Un

C,CDcUZ-i C)CO in

01)c

._Ue

C'LONcr)XLOs0n'-C)U)N0 .~~~NNcsNNC'

ImageNumberFIG.1.(A)Thecoloroverlaysrepresentz-scoredeviationsofthe

firstGenerateconditionfromthefirstBaselinecondition(seetext).Inthisandallsubsequentfigures,increasingz-scorevaluesarerepresentedbywarmercolors.zscoreswithassociatedprobabilitiesbelow0.05arenotrepresented.TheoverlaysaresuperimposeduponaTl-weightedimageacquiredduringthesameimagingsession.ThedotsrepresentregionsfromwhichlittlesignalwasobtainedfollowingshimmingoptimizedfortheLIFC.TheROImarkedas"1"includesareas47and10.(B)ThecoloroverlaysrepresentadirectcomparisonoftheGenerateandRepeatconditions.(C)TimecourseoftheactivationeffectmeasuredasAS/SoforGenerate,Repeat,andMotormeasuredforROI1ofA.Thehorizontalbarbelowtheabscissarepresentstheactivetaskperiodbeginningafterimage5andendingatimage17.thecenteroftheacquisitiontogivesusceptibilityrelatedcontrast.Theimagematrixsizewas64x64withnominalin-planeresolutionof6x4.5mmandslicethicknessof10mm.ATRof3swasusedbetweensuccessiveimagestoreducesaturationeffects.

Tasks.Subjectswereengagedinfourcoreconditionsthatwererepeatedinrandomordertwoorthreetimespersession.Thirty-twoimageswereacquiredoftheselectedplaneineachconditionforatotaltimeof96s.InBaseline,subjectsrestedfortheentire32-imagesetwithnostimuli.InMotor,subjectsrestedduringimages1-5(pre-task),movedtheirtongueandlipsorjaws(butrefrainedfromspeechandsubvocalizing)duringimages6-17(activetask),andrestedduringimages18-32(post-task).Theremainingtasksfol-lowedthesameprotocol,withtheactivetaskalwaysper-formedduringimages6-17.InRepeat,subjectswerereadalistofcommonnouns(approximately1wordper1.5s)andaskedtorepeateachwordimmediately.InGenerate,nounswerereadatthesamerateastheRepeattask,butsubjectswereaskedtorespondwitharelatedverb(e.g.,experimenterread"volcano,"subjectresponded"erupt").AdditionalcontroltasksusedinsomesubjectswereListen,inwhichsubjectswerereadcommonnounsandlistenedpassively;Nonwords,inwhichsubjectswerereadletters(e.g.,"z")andlistenedpassively;andCovertgenerate,inwhichsubjects

25%20%15%10%5%0%-5%-10%

Run1

Run2

25%20%15%10%5%1

-5%-10%i

25%T20%l15%{-10%5%0%ZN

-10%I

AverageofRuns1and2

E~~Listen|Covert

)LONCJ)'-ar)NC).-U)0)-1---NNNNNc')

ImageNumberFIG.2.(A)Thecoloroverlaysrepresentz-scoredeviationsofthe

firstGenerateconditionfromthefirstBaselinecondition.ROI1includesinfoldedcortexandtheanteriorinsula.(B)ThecoloroverlaysrepresentadirectcomparisonoftheGenerateandRepeatconditions.(CandD)TimecourseoftheactivationeffectforGenerate,Repeat,andMotormeasuredforROI1ofAforthefirstandsecondreplicationofeachcondition,respectively.(E)TimecourseoftheactivationeffectfortheaverageoftworeplicationsofListenandCovertmeasuredforROI1ofA.

wereaskedtogenerateverbsmentallytothepresentednounsbutnottorespondvocally.

DataAnalysis.MRIdatawereprocessedasdescribed(10).Toisolatetask-relatedintensitychanges(AS)thestaticbaselinewasremovedbysubtractingvoxelbyvoxelthemeanofthefivepre-taskimage(SO)fromall32imagesineach

-4-:--4 4-gelo-E.L.-.q544zz.-i

Neurobiology:McCarthyetal.

Page 14: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

Language Processing Relies on a Widely Distributed Network

•  Fedorenko and Thompson-Schill (in press, TiCS)

•  Comparison of language-related tasks to various

“non-linguistic” baselines reveals increased metabolic activity in a widely distributed network —not just ‘classical’ language areas

3  jan  2014   14  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

(printed  word  processing)  

Page 15: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

Coordinated Interaction Between Brain Areas •  Early neuroimaging work: Functional localization

–  Which brain regions show heightened metabolic activity when a certain cognitive function is (strongly) engaged?

•  More recent work: Functional connectivity –  What are interdependencies in metabolic or electrical activity

across brain regions?

•  Interdependencies are context-dependent –  Ex: Shifting task from judging

spelling vs. sound similarity of written words shifts interdependence between brain regions.

–  Red: stronger when judging spelling similarity

Bitan  et  al.,  2005,  Fig.  5      

3  jan  2014   15  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

inferior  frontal  gyrus  

intraparietal  sulcus  

lateral  temporal  cortex  

fusiform  gyrus  

Page 16: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

From nodes to networks •  These technological/methodological advances have fueled

nascent theoretical perspectives (Keating, Aronoff) Fedorenko and Thompson-Schill (in press)

•  Long-standing debate: What is the function of a brain region, domain-general or domain-specific?

•  Proposal: Re-focus on dynamic network structure –  How are domain-specific vs. -general regions coordinated

to accomplish current processing goals? t=1 t=2

t=1 t=2

a.

c.

b.

d.

Ex: Domain-general nodes (multi-colored) coordinate with two

distinct domain-specific networks

(green, pink nodes) depending on task

3  jan  2014   16  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  

Page 17: Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 - Linguistic Society … and the Brain, 1924-2014 Developments in Neurology/ Neuroscience, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics Lise%Menn,%University%of%Colorado%

Language and the Brain, 1924-2014 Linguistics: From Words to Multilayered Structures

Now using recorded observations to bring the conceptual structure of linguistics to bear on neurological data

Psycholinguistics: From Relaying Stored Auditory Images to Computation

Now using insights of the cognitive revolution for analysis of language behavior

Neurology/Neuroscience: From Autopsy to fMRI Now using methods that provide a picture of the dynamic, living

brain

Challenge: Need to create new theoretical frameworks to handle the torrent of data about activity in the brain and the cognitive

computations that underlie language use. 3  jan  2014   17  Menn  &  Goldrick,  LSA  1924-­‐2014  


Recommended