+ All Categories
Home > Documents > M.S. Thesis of Mehmet Gültaş

M.S. Thesis of Mehmet Gültaş

Date post: 22-Mar-2022
Category:
Upload: others
View: 1 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
155
WORK DISCIPLINE COMPOUND PERSONALITY SCALE DEVELOPMENT WITH ITEM RESPONSE THEORY A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF SOCIAL SCIENCES OF MIDDLE EAST TECHNICAL UNIVERSITY BY MEHMET GÜLTAŞ IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE IN THE DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY SEPTEMBER 2014
Transcript

WORK DISCIPLINE COMPOUND PERSONALITY SCALE

DEVELOPMENT WITH ITEM RESPONSE THEORY

A THESIS SUBMITTED TO

THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF SOCIAL SCIENCES

OF

MIDDLE EAST TECHNICAL UNIVERSITY

BY

MEHMET GÜLTAŞ

IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS

FOR

THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE

IN

THE DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY

SEPTEMBER 2014

Approval of the Graduate School of Social Sciences

Prof. Dr. Meliha Altunışık

Director

I certify that this thesis satisfies all the requirements as a thesis for the degree of

Master of Science.

Prof. Dr. Tülin Gençöz

Head of Department

This is to certify that we have read this thesis and that in our opinion it is fully

adequate, in scope and quality, as a thesis for the degree of Master of Science.

Asst. Prof. Dr. Yonca Toker

Advisor

Examining Committee Members

Prof. Dr. Reyhan Bilgiç (METU, PSY)

Assoc. Prof. Dr. F. Pınar Acar (METU, BA)

Asst. Prof. Dr. Yonca Toker (METU, PSY)

iii

I hereby declare that all information in this document has been obtained and

presented in accordance with academic rules and ethical conduct. I also declare

that, as required by these rules and conduct, I have fully cited and referenced all

material and results that are not original to this work.

Name, Last name: Mehmet Gültaş

Signature:

iv

ABSTRACT

WORK DISCIPLINE COMPOUND PERSONALITY SCALE DEVELOPMENT

WITH ITEM RESPONSE THEORY

Gültaş, Mehmet

M.S., Department of Psychology

Supervisor: Assist. Prof. Dr. Yonca Toker

September 2014, 141 pages

The Big Five personality factors differentially relate to various organizational criteria.

The most notable characteristic of personality scales while used in prediction is that

they are criteria dependent. Nevertheless, even the predictive power of the most

promising of the factors (i.e., conscientiousness) varies across occupations and

situations. Increasing the specificity of a measurement instrument by designing the

instrument considering criterion-relevant facet-level behaviors has been shown to

improve prediction. In this study I developed a compound scale that uses facet level

constructs of the Five Factor Model (FFM). By using an empirical and rational match

between predictors and criteria, predictive power of the scale is expected to be better

than conventional FFM factors. In the development of this compound scale, first the

literature was reviewed to identify the personality facets that predict work

behavior/performance. Indicator items of these facets were obtained from the

International Personality Item Pool. Items were put to empirical analysis based on

Classical Test Theory (CTT) and Item Response Theory (IRT) in order to fine-tune the

instrument by identifying the best items. The compound scale correlation with task

performance was larger than the BFI factor compound. Herein, the proposed study will

contribute to the literature on facet-based compund scales and the IRT applications in

personality scale development.

Keywords: Personality, compound scale, job performance, item response theory,

situational strength

v

ÖZ

İŞ DİSİPLİNİ BİRLEŞİK KİŞİLİK ÖLÇEĞİNİN MADDE TEPKİ KURAMI İLE

GELİŞTİRİLMESİ

Gültaş, Mehmet

Yüksek Lisans, Psikoloji Bölümü

Tez Danışmanı: Yard. Doç. Dr. Yonca Toker

Eylül 2014, 141 sayfa

Büyük Beşli kişilik faktörleri farklı kurumsal sonuçlarla farklılaşan şekillerde

ilişkilidir. Bu kişilik faktörlerinden en önemlisi olan öz disiplin faktörünün bile

yordayıcılığı meslek ve durumlara göre farklılık gösterir. Büyük Beşli kişilik

faktörlerinin alt boyutlarından iş ile ilgili olanları bir araya getirerek tasarlanan

ölçeklerin yordayıcı gücünü arttırdığı yazında görülmektedir. Bu çalışmada Büyük

Beşli faktörlerinin görev performansı ile ilişkili olan alt boyutları birleştirilerek

bütünleşik bir kişilik ölçeği geliştirilmiştir. Bütünleşik kişilik ölçeğinin yordayıcı

gücünün Büyük Beşli faktörlerinden daha yüksek olacağı tahmin edilmektedir.

Bütünleşik kişilik ölçeğinin, üniversitede araştırma görevlilerinin beraber çalıştıkları

bir öğretim üyesi tarafından değerlendirimiş olan iş performanslarını istatistiksel

olarak anlamlı düzeyde yordadığı bulunmuştur. Oluşturulan ölçek, madde tepki kuramı

(MTK) uygulaması ile elde edilen madde ayırdedicilik parametresi ve madde bilgi

fonksiyonu kullanılarak, en iyi çalışan maddeler kullanılarak kısaltılmıştır. Böylelikle,

araştırma sınırlı sayıda görülen kişilik ölçeği geliştirilmesinde MTK kullanımı

yazınına da katkı sağlamıştır.

Anahtar Kelimeler: Kişilik, bütünleşik ölçek, iş performansı, madde tepki kuramı,

durumsal kuvvet

vi

to those who collect, create, and disseminate

knowledge

vii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to express my gratitude to my supervisor, Assit. Prof. Yonca Toker for

providing relentless efforts in guiding me during the thesis.

I also thank my thesis committee members Assoc. Prof. Pınar Acar and Prof. Reyhan

Bilgiç for their valuable contributions.

I would like to express my sincere appreciation to everyone who helped me collect

data in a short period of time; Assoc. Prof. Ahmet Uysal, Assist. Prof. Bahar Öz,

Assist. Prof. Banu Cingöz Ulu, Prof. Bengi Öner-Özkan, Assoc. Prof. Özlem Bozo,

and Prof. Reyhan Bilgiç.

My friends provided me with valuable gift of their times and support.

My family supported me in my entire life. Knowing that you are there for me cannot

be thanked or appreciated enough. I know that, but I thank you.

viii

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ABSTRACT ........................................................................................................... iv

ÖZ ...................................................................................................................... v

DEDICATION.........................................................................................................vi

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .................................................................................. vii

TABLE OF CONTENTS ..................................................................................... viii

LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................................. xi

LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................... xii

CHAPTER

1. INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................... 1

1.1 Overview ............................................................................................... 1

1.2 Correspondence between Predictor and Criteria ................................... 3

1.3 Rationale for Using Facet Level Predictors .......................................... 6

1.4 Work Discipline Compound Personality Scale ................................... 10

1.4.1 Conscientiousness and its Facets ..................................................... 11

1.4.2 Extraversion and its Facets .............................................................. 13

1.4.3 Openness to Experience and its Facets ........................................... 15

1.4.4 Agreeableness and its Facets ........................................................... 15

1.5 Effects of Situations and Situational Strength .................................... 20

1.6 Application of the Item Response Theory .......................................... 24

2. STUDY 1 METHOD ......................................................................................... 30

2.1 Overview ............................................................................................. 30

2.2 Participants and Procedure .................................................................. 30

2.3 Measures ............................................................................................. 31

2.3.1 The Big Five Inventory ................................................................... 31

ix

2.3.2 Development of the Compound Personality Scale .......................... 32

3. STUDY 1 RESULTS .......................................................................................... 34

3.1 Exploratory Factor Analyses ................................................................ 34

3.2 IRT Analyses ........................................................................................ 39

4. STUDY 2 METHOD and RESULTS ................................................................. 55

4.1 Participants and Procedure................................................................... 55

4.2 Measures .............................................................................................. 56

4.2.1 Situational Strength at Work Scale .................................................. 56

4.2.2 AEF .................................................................................................. 57

4.3 Results .................................................................................................. 58

4.3.1 Interrelations of Compound Scale and BFI Factors ......................... 59

4.3.2 Compound Personality Scale and Job Performance Relationship ... 63

4.4 Moderating Effects of Situational Strength ......................................... 66

4.5 The use of IRT Methodologies ............................................................ 69

5. DISCUSSION .................................................................................................... 70

5.1 The Use of Personality Facets ............................................................. 71

5.2 The Nature of Facet-Performance Associations in the Present Study . 73

5.3 Using IRT Methods in Personality Scale Refinement ......................... 75

5.4 Situational Strength as the Proposed Moderator of the Personality-

Performance Association ........................................................................................ 76

5.5 Practical Implications .......................................................................... 80

5.6 Limitations ........................................................................................... 81

5.7 Suggestions for Future Research ......................................................... 82

REFERENCES ....................................................................................................... 83

APPENDICES ....................................................................................................... 93

APPENDIX A: Compound Personality Measure Items of Study 1 .................. 93

APPENDIX B: Compound Personality Measure Items of Study 2 .................. 98

APPENDIX C: The Big Five Inventory (BFI) Items ...................................... 102

APPENDIX D: Assistant Evaluation Form ..................................................... 104

APPENDIX E: Situational Strength at Work (SSW) Scale ............................. 106

x

APPENDIX F: Research Assistant Informed Consent ................................... 108

APPENDIX G: Faculty Informed Consent .................................................... 109

APPENDIX H: Debriefing Form .................................................................... 110

APPENDIX I: Facet Item Information Function Plots .................................... 111

APPENDIX J: Study 1 Factor Analysis Pattern Matrix ................................. 123

APPENDIX K: Tukish Summary ................................................................... 126

APPENDIX L: Tez Fotokopisi İzin Formu .................................................... 141

xi

LIST OF TABLES

TABLES

Table 1 Correlations of Facets with Performance Dimensions ............................. 17

Table 2 Definitions of NEO Facets ....................................................................... 19

Table 3 Descriptive statistics for the 12-factor solution ........................................ 36

Table 4 1st Facet Self-Discipline ........................................................................... 44

Table 5 2nd Facet Excitement Seeking .................................................................. 46

Table 6 3rd Facet Altruism ..................................................................................... 47

Table 7 4th Facet Achievement Striving ................................................................ 48

Table 8 5th Facet Deliberation ............................................................................... 49

Table 9 6th Facet Assertiveness ............................................................................. 50

Table 10 7th Facet Compliance .............................................................................. 50

Table 11 8th Facet Order ........................................................................................ 51

Table 12 9th Facet Openness to Ideas .................................................................... 51

Table 13 10th Facet Assertiveness/Exhibition ....................................................... 52

Table 14 11th Facet Dutifulness ............................................................................. 53

Table 15 12th Facet Trust ....................................................................................... 54

Table 16 Descriptive Statistics of the Situational Strength Measure .................... 56

Table 17 Descriptive Statistics for the CTT and IRT Based Facets ...................... 61

Table 18 Correlations between the BFI Factors and Compound Scale ................. 62

Table 19 Compund Facet and BFI Correlations with Performance ...................... 64

Table 20 Hierarchical Regression Analysis Results .............................................. 66

Table 21 Situational Strength Hierarchical Regression ANOVA Results ............. 67

Table 22 Situational Strength Hierarchical Regression ........................................ 67

Table 23 Job Performance and IRT Based Scale Correlations .............................. 69

xii

LIST OF FIGURES

FIGURES

Figure 1 Item Response Function (IRT) with differing sample characteristics. ... 28

Figure 2 Parallel Analysis Real and Random Data Generated Eigenvalues ......... 35

Figure 3 IIF of the 11th Item of the Excitement Seeking Scale ............................ 42

Figure 4 IIF of the 8th Item of the Excitement Seeking Scale .............................. 43

1

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

1.1 Overview

In today’s world personality measures are enjoying quite a widespread acceptance and

use in organizational settings, even though this is not as much as cognitive ability tests

(Scroggins, Thomas, & Morris, 2009). But according to Hough and Oswald (2008),

research that is relating personality variables to organizational outcomes (either

objective measures or subjective evaluations) was on hold for almost two decades

during the 70s and 80s. Hough and Oswald connect this lack of interest to the

conclusions of Guion and Gottier’s (1965) review article. In their concluding

statements Guion and Gottier emphasized that “taken as a whole, there is no

generalizable evidence that personality measures can be recommended as good or

practical tools for employee selection” (p. 159). These negative remarks merged with

the attack of the work of Mischel (1968) on trait psychology almost stopped the

research that was investigating the relationship between personality and organizational

outcomes.

The interest to the use and research of personality testing with regard to organizational

criteria of interest exploded in the last two and a half decades (Hough & Oswald,

2008). This surge of interest for the research and application of personality test may

be attributed, in part, to the common conceptual ground that was afforded by the use

of the Five Factor Model (FFM; Costa & McCrae, 1985). In any scientific research

domain, without a reliable and stable conceptual ground, development is hardly

possible. FFM afforded researchers and practitioners to share a common language and

work on the same constructs. Even though previous sentence may sound like a cliché,

theoretical and practical benefits of a common understanding and common language

cannot be overemphasized.

According to Barrick and Mount (2012) there are still unresolved issues that need

research attention on the use of personality measures in selection contexts of

2

ogranizations. One of the issues that was mentioned is the discussion regarding the

utility of lower-level facets. Specifically, they emphasized that the issue of whether the

use of lower-order facets will lead to higher validity coefficients is waiting to be

resolved. Using higher level constructs (e.g. conscientiousness, emotional stability)

of the FFM generally yield corrected validities not exceeding .30s in predicting overall

job performance. For instance, in their meta-analysis of previously published meta-

analyses Barrick, Mount, and Judge (2001) found that, conscientiousness predicted

job-performance in different criteria (i.e. supervisor ratings, objective performance,

training performance, and teamwork) within the ranges of .23 to .31. All of these

correlations are estimated corrected correlations at the construct level, meaning that

these values are corrected for imperfect construct assessment. Without this correction,

validities ranged from .19 to .26.

One significant aspect of personality constructs is their criterion dependency, meaning

that, validities of FFM factors change significantly as a function of the criterion used.

Extraversion and openness to experience best predict training performance (ρ = .28

and ρ = .33, respectively), emotional stability and agreeableness best predict team

work (ρ = .22 and ρ = .34), and conscientiousness best predicts supervisor ratings (ρ

= .31) (Barrick et al., 2001). Meta-analytic studies have demonstrated that only

conscientiousness is a valid predictor of job performance across various jobs (Barrick

& Mount, 1991; Tett, Jackson, & Rothstein, 1991; Tett, Jackson, Rothstein, & Reddon,

1994).

As a result of these less than impressive true score correlations, not everybody was

convinced about the use of personality measures in applied settings (e.g. Guion, 1998).

Morgeson et al. (2007) voiced a dissenting opinion about the use and usability of self-

report personality measures in the employee selection contexts. While criticizing self-

report personality tests, they were setting aside faking as an issue, in order to

emphasize the low validities of personality scales observed in selection contexts.

According to their view, validities this low should make practitioners use personality

measures appended to cognitive ability tests. Another concern that was voiced was the

relevancy of items to the job at hand. What is meant by that is that personality measures

3

should be constructed to be clearly job related in face valid ways. The main aim of the

compound scale being developed in this study is, more or less, in line with this advice.

Even though the scale that is going to be developed will not be clearly job related, the

facets that will be included in the scale is going to be job related. In other words, the

personality facets that are rationally and/or empirically related and theoretically

aligned with the specified job at hand (i.e. research assistant) will be included in the

compound scale.

Before delving into the literature I would like to point out to the naming convention

that is going to be used within this paper. When describing the conceptual domain of

FFM, researchers often use facet, subfacet, subfactor, factor, dimension, sub-

dimension, domain, sub-domain, or other naming conventions interchangeably. In this

paper “factor” will always be used to mean one of the five factors of FFM (i.e.

conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, openness, and neuroticism) and facet

will always be used to indicate sub-dimensions of these factors. Having said that,

researchers are trying new conceptual models by merging facets of FFM or by

following other theoretical hunches (e.g. the Hogan Personality Inventory). While

describing these kind of conceptual subdivisions of FFM factors, I will refer to them

as sub-dimensions.

The literature is reviewed focusing on the factor and facet predictors of job

performance and the usefulness of combining facets to form compound personality

scales. In addition, situational characteristics defining strong or weak situations that

influence how much personality can be expressed on the job is reviewed as situational

strength has been proposed as a moderator of personality-criterion valities. Finally, as

the compound measure will be refined based on IRT applications, the theory and its

proposed uses for personality and attitude measurement are described.

1.2 Correspondence between Predictor and Criteria

Adoption of the Big Five as a common framework to study personality related

organizational outcomes significantly improved the acceptance of personality

measures as selection tools. Because of the structure and common language offered by

4

the FFM, researchers are able to collaborate and accumulate knowledge regarding job

performance and personality much more easily. But, as stated above, and summarized

in Ones, Dilchert, Viswesvaran, and Judge (2007), meta-analytic results show that the

effect sizes corrected for unreliability in the criterion and range restriction are small in

predicting overall job performance, (R = .27), and objective measures of performance

(R = .23) from personality factors.

In order to better predict job performance, some researchers point out the importance

of theoretical alignment between the criterion and predictor (Jenkins & Griffith, 2004).

They are indicating that in organizations, before using any content based test in other

assessment areas, such as job knowledge, job analyses is typically conducted or

existing job descriptions are examined in order to decide what kind of knowledge

domain should be covered in the test. Similarly, Jenkins and Griffith (2004) concluded

that, selecting sub-domains of personality (i.e. sub-domains of 16 Personality Factor

Questionnaire; 16PF) that are relevant to the job increases the predictive power of

personality tests. Besides various theoretical standpoints for the use of specific

behaviors, the predictive power of compound personality scales should be an incentive

for their use (Ones & Viswesvaran, 2001), as operational predictive validities of these

tests can be as high as .41 (Ones, Viswesvaran, & Schmidt, 1993).

In their meta-analysis Hogan and Holland (2003) also emphasized the importance of

having conceptually aligned personality predictor and job performance criteria. In this

study, prior to the meta-analysis, subject matter experts (SEMs) were used to assign

criteria to one of the seven sub-dimensions of the Hogan Personality Inventory (HPI)

(i.e. adjustment, ambition, sociability, likeability, prudence, intellectance, and school

success). For instance, ‘likeability’ was matched with ‘shows interpersonal skills’,

‘exhibits capacity to compromise’, ‘demonstrates tactfulness and sensitivity’, and

‘shares credit’ criteria. In addition to that, same set of criteria was assigned to one of

the two groups, namely ‘getting along’ (i.e. getting along with members of a group)

and ‘getting ahead’ (i.e. inclination for achievement and gaining status). These formed

groups of job performance dimensions later served as criteria in their meta-analysis.

5

Results of this study (Hogan & Holland, 2003) showed that the sub-dimensions of

adjustment, prudence, and likeability, but not sociability and intellectance, predicted

getting along criteria with true validities of .34, .31, and .23, respectively. Getting

ahead criteria were predicted by the ambition ( = .26), adjustment ( = .22), and

prudence ( = .20) scales but not sociability. More importantly, criteria that were

assigned to and labelled as the adjustment criteria set by SMEs was empirically

predicted by the adjustment sub-dimension ( = .43), the ambition criteria set was

predicted by the ambition sub-dimension ( = .35), and the prudence criteria set was

predicted by the prudence sub-dimension ( = .36). Effect size were higher thus these

results highlighted that alignment of predictors and criteria has utility in validity

studies. This may lead us to the conclusion that using personality constructs (in this

case HPI sub-dimensions) with a related job criterion rather than a more general

criterion can increase the predictive power of the measurement.

Jenkins and Griffith (2004) emphasized the relatedness of certain personality concepts

to specific occupations, following the advice about using personality based job

analysis for analyzing the content of the jobs and the selection of relevant personality

constructs for prediction. They compared the predictive power of two 16PF broad

dimensions (i.e. self-control and independence) with the Accountant Personality Fit

Scale (APFS). Contents of this scale (i.e. which sub-dimensions it encapsulates) was

determined by a personality based job analysis and it includes the 16PF sub-

dimensions of warmth, openness to change, emotional stability, and trusting. Results

of the study showed that APFS predicted job performance (i.e. a performance review

completed by an immediate supervisor) (r = .35, p < .05) while general dimensions of

self-control and independence did not.

The study conducted by Oh et al. (2008) developed compound personality scales

specifically geared towards identified criteria. The compound scale to predict

teamwork performance included items from the dimensions of coperation, goodwill,

carefulness, savvy, and stability and the highest uncorrected correlation of this scale

6

was observed with teamwork performance (r = .29). Another compound scale to assess

managerial potential most highly correlated with managerial performance (r = .30).

Job performance should be considered as a concept that has a multi-faceted nature,

where nature of the jobs and tasks tend to change across occupations and organizations

(Barrick & Mount, 1991). In order to match specificity on the criterion side of the

personality job performance relationship, specific aspects of job performance should

be matched with specific sub-dimensions of generic personality dimensions or specific

individual differences variables. This basic premise is also an incentive to use narrow

over broad personality constructs to maximize predictive validity (Tett & Christiansen,

2007; Christiansen & Tett, 2008). Facet level constructs of the FFM provide us the

concepts that are specific enough for a predictive instrument. Other side of the equation

(i.e. occupational and job related information) can be filled with taxonomies of job

performance (Johnson, 2003) or also by extant job descriptions that can be found in

the occupational information network (O*NET).

Applicants seem to perceive custom made compound personality scales more job-

related than omnibus measures of the FFM (Jenkins & Griffith, 2004). This kind of

face validity may motivate applicants to take personality measures more seriously.

Another aspect is that, extremely long measures can dissuade practitioners in

organizations from using personality scales as prediction tools. When validity results

are less than impressive, human resources personnel may be inclined to use more

practical tools within selection processes, rather than time consuming and long

personality measurement tools that appear as irrelevant to the jobs at hand. A

compound personality scale that includes only the facets matched to the focal criteria

would offer a shorter measure which still can assess all relevant dimensions of

behavior.

1.3 Rationale for Using Facet Level Predictors

There are two reasons, one rational and the other empirical, for using facets of FFM

for the prediction of behaviors or job performance. First, I will discuss empirical

evidence that can be found in the extant literature that scrutinized the relationships

7

between personality and job performance. Then I will discuss the conceptual

underpinnings that are indicating the use of facets.

Hurtz and Donovan (2000) discussed the merits of using FFM facet level constructs

rather than broad dimensions. Even though they noted that their meta-analysis revealed

low to moderate correlations between factor level personality dimensions and job

performance (i.e. operational validity between conscientiousness and overall job

performance was .22), one thing they wrote is that different personality constructs

seem to affect job performance in different jobs or in different job performance

dimensions. As a result of this, they indicated that these theoretically meaningful

relationships were not practically impressive but “the magnitude of these correlations

might be enhanced if the most relevant specific facets of these broad dimensions could

be specified” (p. 877). Their suggestion was the use of facets that can have “theoretical

links to the performance dimensions under investigation” (p. 877). Furthermore, they

suggested that in order to increase criterion-related validity these facets can be used

individually or in combination with other theoretically-relevant facets. They proposed

not only to form composites from within a broad Big Five factor, but also noted the

possible practical merits of grouping facets from across different factors of FFM.

There is a vast amount of literature regarding the use of sub-dimensions of factors to

predict various work related outcomes. In their empirical research, Moon, Hollenbeck,

Marinova, and Humphrey (2008) suggested a tripartite model (i.e. a model that has

three parts) of extraversion with surgency, sociability, and positive affectivity as the

sub-dimensions, to differentially predict organizational citizenship behavior (OCB).

They tested the relationships between extraversion, these sub-dimensions and OCB

using hierarchical regression analysis. Extraversion and sociability were found to be

unrelated to OCB. On the other hand, the sub-dimension of surgency was negatively

related with OCB behaviors (β = -.22, ΔR2 = .06), and individuals who had higher

positive emotion scores were more likely to engage in OCB (β = .19, ΔR2 = .06). They

noted that their results may be used to shed light on the contradictory findings on the

relationship between extraversion and OCBs. This may be taken as evidence that when

extraversion was used as a broad factor, in some situations its predictive validity

8

diminishes as well as its utility. The study of Moon et al. (2008) highlights another

reason to use specifically formed compound scales with facets instead of factors, as

facets within one factor may predict criteria in opposite directions.

Another study that indicates broad constructs' lack of predictive validity is of Mussel,

Winter, Gelleri, and Schuler (2011). The authors of this study focused on the facets of

openness to experience, rather than the broad factor itself, and highlighted that the non-

significant association that was found in the meta-analysis of Barrick et al. (2001) can

be ascribed to the wide scope and heterogeneity of the factor. Results of their research

indicated that sub-dimension level analyses revealed large differences in criterion and

construct-related validity across facets of openness to experience. Specifically, using

EFA and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) they formed two sub-dimensions of the

openness factor; one of which was ‘perceptual’ which includes the facets of openness

to fantasy, aesthetics, and feelings, and the other was ‘epistemic’ which includes the

facets of openness to actions, ideas, and values. They compared three different latent

path models in the prediction of job performance and academic performance. The first

model included the broad openness to experience factor as the predictor, the second

model included the two-factor solution that includes epistemic and perceptual sub-

dimensions, and the third model included the six-factor solution composed of six facets

of openness. The six-factor and two-factor path models predicting performance had

better fit indices than the one-factor openness to experience model. In the two-factor

model, only epistemic openness predicted job and academic performance, whereas

perceptual openness did not. Still, the authors noted that the six-facet model explained

more variance in job and academic performance over the rest of Big Five factors, than

the two-factor model. Facet-level correlation analyses indicated that, of the three

epistemic openness facets, it was openness to ideas which showed the highest range of

correlations with the performance indices of job and academic performance, peer

ratings, university achievement, ranging from .25 to .27. This was followed by

openness to actions and values. The epistemic factor correlations ranged from .27

to .30.

9

Regarding the use of specific facets, De Vries et al. (2011) found similar results using

facets of the six-dimensional personality model. Their study suggests that the

academic criterion of grade point average (GPA) and counterproductive academic

behavior (CAB) can be predicted by using the narrow facets of conscientiousness and

honesty-humility dimensions of HEXACO (Honesty-Humility, Emotionality,

eXtraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness to Experience). In

HEXACO the sub-dimensions of conscientiousness are organization, diligence,

perfectionism, and prudence and sub-dimensions of honesty-humility are sincerity,

fairness, greed avoidance, and modesty.

In order to compare the effectiveness of the dimensions of conscientiousness and

honesty-humility with the previously listed sub-dimensions, De Vries et al. (2011) used

hierarchical regression analyses. In the hierarchical regression analyses, GPA and CAB

at first regressed on gender, then on emotionality, extraversion, agreeableness, and

openness, and at the final step on conscientiousness and humility-honesty dimensions.

On a similar manner, the sub-dimensions (e.g. organization, diligence, etc.) were

entered in the final step of a separate hierarchical regression analyses. Results showed

that while dimensions were explaining 10% of GPA scores, sub-dimensions explained

17% of the variance of GPA, and this difference was significant. A similar pattern was

observed when CAB was taken as the dependent variable. While dimensions were

explaining 23% of the variance, sub-dimensions explained 35% of the variance. A

separate test indicated that the sub-dimensions are more strongly related to CAB than

the dimensions.

The value of sub-dimensions in the prediction of job performance is also handled in

one of the meta-analytic studies. Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, and Cortina (2006)

examined the prediction power of sub-dimensions above and beyond the

conscientiousness factor. They examined the predictive power of achievement,

dependability, order, and cautiousness above and beyond conscientiousness and also

the criterion related validity with different outcomes (i.e. overall job performance, task

performance, job dedication, interpersonal facilitation, counterproductive work

behaviors). They found variability across sub-dimensions while predicting

10

performance criteria. For instance, job dedication was predicted by dependability (

= .46) to a greater extent than cautiousness ( = .08) and overall job performance was

predicted by dependability ( = .25) differently than cautiousness ( = -.01).

In this study (Dudley et al., 2006) the incremental validity of sub-dimensions on top

of conscientiousness factor was also analyzed. Across different criteria, specific sub-

dimensions added incremental variance on top of conscientiousness. Specifically, in

explaining job dedication, sub-dimensions added 26%, in explaining

counterproductive work behaviors (CWBs) they added 14%, and in explaining overall

job performance added 4% of incremental variance. As a result they advised the

consideration of sub-dimensions in the prediction of job performance. Another

important contribution of this study is their indication of fluctuation of predictive

power of narrow traits in particular performance dimension or with regard to the

specific occupation at hand. Findings of this study is signaling that the qualities of

tasks that inherently vary across jobs, or the job criteria that were used have an

important influence on criterion validity strength of personality constructs.

Taken together, empirical studies highlighted using facet level personality constructs

to predict performance as a promising area to study. In the literature, even where no

relationships between factors and criteria are found, studies using facet level constructs

have found significant relationships between facets and the criteria of interest. Thus,

the work discipline compound scale will be formed based on the facets identified as

being associated with the generic academic or job performance dimensions.

1.4 Work Discipline Compound Personality Scale

In order to match FFM traits with job performance dimensions, first an empirical

findings based on the existing literature, and also a rational approach to link personality

facests to the dimensions of a job performance taxonomy is needed. The starting point

is identifying a job performance taxonomy. Johnson (2003) proposed an integrated

taxonomy of job performance dimensions. This classification system uses three levels

to organize performance dimensions, from broad to specific. Level one of the

taxonomy, which includes broader dimensions, consists of task performance,

11

citizenship performance and adaptive performance. I focused on the third level of this

taxonomy in which the dimensions of performance are more specific, and composed

of sub-dimensions, such as handling emergencies or crisis situations, engaging in self-

development, demonstrating effort.

Because of their inherent nature, some of these dimensions seem to be more related to

ability and job-specific knowledge such as technical proficiency, solving problems

creatively, and decision making/problem solving. On the other hand, dimensions like

conscientious initiative, maintaining personal discipline, engaging in self-

development reflect the non-ability behavioral tendencies of individuals and would be

predicted from personality.

Initially, these level three dimensions of this taxonomy were selected for a literature

search with an aim of identifying any relationships between these dimensions and the

FFM traits. Specifically, these dimensions are demonstrating effort, handling stress (in

other words, stress tolerance), taking initiative, learning work tasks, maintaining

personal discipline, adaptability, and engaging in self-development. Concepts that are

related to job performance and studied within the literature are also included in this

search. These concepts are work engagement, demonstrating flexibility, task

persistence, and procrastination. Compound scale will consist of the Big Five facets

that were found to be related to these constructs or conceptually related ones. Each Big

Five factor’s relation to job performance dimensions is presented separately.

1.4.1 Conscientiousness and its Facets

Conscientiousness relationship with job performance was empirically well

documented, and known to be the most consistent FFM factor across different jobs and

occupational groups (Barrick et al., 2001). So, facets of conscientiousness (i.e. self-

efficacy, orderliness, dutifulness, achievement striving, self-discipline, cautiousness)

are natural candidates for a composite scale that is going to be related with job

performance.

12

Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, and Crawford (2013) conducted a meta-analysis in

order to examine the relationship between the NEO facets and task performance.

Accordingly, all conscientiousness facets had meta-analytic uncorrected validities

ranging from .10 to .15. Similar line of evidence comes from the work of Dudley et al.

(2006). They found that two facets of conscientiousness, namely, achievement striving

(r = .13 and r = .20) and dependability (corresponding to the self-discipline facet of

NEO-PI-R) (r = .09 and r = .23) were more related to task performance and job

dedication, whereas cautiousness (r = .06 and r = .04) and order (r = .08 and r = .05)

had lower associations.

Lievens, Coetsier, De Fruyt, and De Maesener (2002) studied the relationships

between medical students’ personality characteristics and academic performance. In

their study, after the initial NEO-PI-R administration, they followed the end-of-year

academic performance of medical students. Only FFM factor that predicted academic

performance in each of the years was conscientiousness. Standardized regression

coefficients for the first, second, and third year were .24, .17, and .19, respectively.

When they examined the correlation between academic end-of-year performances,

only two of the facets, namely achievement striving (r = .15, r = .19, r = .15) and self-

discipline (r = .23, r = .24, r = .18) were significantly correlated with academic

performance in each of the years. Their results replicated those of Dudley et al. (2006)

in terms of showing the most highly associated facets. They also conducted a series of

t-tests to determine the differences between successful and unsuccessful students.

Even though only self-discipline, appeared as a variable that differentiated between

successful and unsuccessful students after a Bonferoni correction, dutifulness,

achievement striving positively, gregariousness and excitement seeking negatively

related to end-of-year academic success. Bonferonni procedure entails multiplying

alpha levels and in this research, this caused alpha levels of .0018 to reach significant

results. This correction when used with over twenty concurrent significant tests, causes

alpha levels lower than conventional p < .05. For this reason it was criticized as being

too stringent (Perneger, 1998). For this reason, findings that were significant for

13

dutifulness, achievement striving, gregariousness and excitement seeking should be

taken into account.

Bakker, Demerouti, and ten Brummelhuis (2012) found in their moderation analysis

that work engagement and conscientiousness interacted in relation to task

performance. Work engagement was related to task performance for only those who

scored high (β = .45) or average (β = .26) on conscientiousness.

With regard to effort as a criterion, Hough (1992) found criterion-related validities

with regard to achievement and dependability. Validities of effort for achievement and

dependability were .21 and .14. Procrastination as a criterion was also shown to be

related to low conscientiousness and all of its facets, with the self-discipline facet

accounting for most of the variance (Johnson & Bloom, 1995; Watson, 2001).

Finally, with regards to adaptability as the performance criterion, Shoss, Witt, and Vera

(2012) found that adaptive performance lead to task performance in high

organizational politics perceptions, only for those high in conscientiousness. Neal,

Yeo, Koy, and Xiao (2012) found that conscientiousness positively (.09, .12, .14)

related to individual, team, and organizational adaptability, respectively, but no facet-

level associations were offered.

Dudley et al., (2006) suggested the use of four specific narrow facets of

conscientiousness, namely, achievement striving, order, cautiousness, and

dependability. Dependability definition of these authors seems to match the self-

discipline facet, and cautiousness reflects the deliberation facet of NEO PI-R. In the

development of the initial version of the compound personality scale I will also add

dutifulness to this list as it has empirical support.

1.4.2 Extraversion and its Facets

The meta-analytic investigation of Judge et al. (2013) indicatet that the most highly

associated extraversion facets with task performance were activity (r = .10),

excitement seeking (r = .09), and assertiveness (r = .07). Even though this meta-

analysis suggested all positive associations with performance for these three facets,

14

several other research showed inverse associations for activity and excitement seeking.

For instance, a study which focused on the facets of extraversion in predicting job

dedication and task performance found excitement seeking to have the highest

associations, though they were negative correlations of -.31 and -.27 (Denis, Morin, &

Guindon, 2010). Similarly, a study by Detrick, Chibnall, and Luebbert (2004) which

investigated the predictive validity of FFM facet level personality constructs on

academic performance on a sample of police academy recruits using three consecutive

classes, showed that being low on the excitement seeking (β = -.29, p = .05) facet

significantly predicted better academic performance (where performance did not

include physical performance or firearms performance). A study by Chamorro-

Premuzic and Furnham (2003) also reported a negative association with the activity

facet and university course exam grades, from -.16 to -.27.

Evidence of the relationship between the excitement seeking facet of extraversion and

various other criteria can also be found in the literature. Pincombe, Luciano, Martin,

and Wright (2007) studied monozygotic, dizygotic and their nontwin siblings in order

to understand the relationship between facets of extraversion and IQ. As a result of

their research they found no significant correlation between extraversion score and IQ

scores. But, in their sample excitement seeking facet score was significantly and

negatively correlated with both of the IQ scores, namely verbal (r = -.15) and

performance (r = -.11).

Hallberg, Johansson, and Schaufeli (2007) studied the relationship between Type A

behavior and work engagement. Their conclusion was that, one of the aspects of Type

A behavior, specifically achievement striving, was related to work engagement.

Achievement striving was defined as including the adjectives of energetic, fast,

powerful, enterprising, enthusiastic, ambitious, individualistic, talkative, extraverted,

and strong. These elements are much more similar to the extraversion factor of the

FFM. Its correlation with work engagement was .36. Based on rational grounds, the

assertiveness facet of extraversion as defined in the NEO PI-R were decided to be

included in the compound measure. Fein and Klein (2011) argued that the facet of

15

assertiveness has self-regulatory elements helping individuals to allocate attention and

energy toward the pursuit of goals.

Taken together, assertivess is expected to correlate positively with performance,

whereas activity and excitement seeking are expected to correlate negatively. These

three facets were included in the compound personality measure.

1.4.3 Openness to Experience and its Facets

Regarding openness to experience, in their meta-analysis Judge et al. (2013) indicated

that openness to values (r = .12), ideas (r = .07), and actions (r = .07) were the most

highly correlated facets with task performnce.

As mentioned in an earlier section, Mussel et al. (2011) found that the facet of

openness to ideas was correlated with work-related criteria, such as job performance

(r = .26), academic performance (r = .27), and peer-ratings of job competence (r = .25).

They also divided openness to experience into two dimensions. One of the sub-

dimension was epistemic openness to experience which included openness to actions,

ideas, and values. The other one was named perceptual openness to experience, which

was composed of openness to fantasy, aesthetics, and feelings. Considering epistemic

and perceptual openness to experience, they show different validities for academic and

job performance as noted in the previous section, favoring the espitemic factor. In light

of these results, compound scale will include the openness to actions and ideas facets

of the openness to experience factor.

1.4.4 Agreeableness and its Facets

Rossier, Zecca, Stauffer, Maggiori, and Dauwalder (2012) found that agreeableness

can predict work engagement (r = .25), after controlling for gender and adaptability.

When agreeableness is considered, there is one meta-analytic study indicating facet

task performance associations (Judge et al., 2013). Accordingly, the facets of trust (r

= .10) and compliance (r = .09) were most highly associated with task performance.

Based on its rational appeal altruism was included.

16

All facet-performance associations are summarized in Table 1. As a result, in the work

discipline compound personality scale being developed to predict performance, five

facets from conscientiounsness (achievement striving, deliberation, dutifulness, order,

self-discipline), three facets from extraversion (assertiveness, excitement seeking,

activity), two facets from openness to experience (ideas and actions), and three facets

from agreeableness (compliance, altruism, and trust) will be included. Definitions of

these facets are presented in Table 2 that is following this paragraph.

17

Table 1 Correlations of Facets with Performance Dimensions

Factor Facet Performance index Effect Size Study

C Achievement striving Task performance ṝ = .15 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

Competence Task performance ṝ = .14 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

Deliberation Task performance ṝ = .10 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

Dutifulness Task performance ṝ = .12 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

Order Task performance ṝ = .10 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

Self-discipline Task performance ṝ = .13 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

Achievement striving Task performance ṝ = .13 Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, & Cortina (2006)

Dependability Task performance ṝ = .09 Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, & Cortina (2006)

Order Task performance ṝ = .08 Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, & Cortina (2006)

Cautiosness Task performance ṝ = .06 Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, & Cortina (2006)

Achievement striving Job dedication ṝ = .20 Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, & Cortina (2006)

Dependability Job dedication ṝ = .23 Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, & Cortina (2006)

Order Job dedication ṝ = .05 Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, & Cortina (2006)

Cautiosness Job dedication ṝ = .04 Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, & Cortina (2006)

Self-discipline Academic performance r = .18 to .23 Lievens, Coetsier, De Fruyt, & De Maesener (2002)

Achievement striving Academic performance r = .15 to .19 Lievens, Coetsier, De Fruyt, & De Maesener (2002)

Achievement striving Effort ratings r = .21 Hough (1992)

Dependability Effort ratings r = .14 Hough (1992)

Achievement motivation Motivation to learn r = .27 Colquitt, LePine, & Noe (2000)

All facets, mostly self-discipline Procrastination Johnson & Bloom (1995); Watson (2001)

E Activity Task Performance ṝ = .10 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

Assertiveness Task Performance ṝ = .07 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

Excitement Seeking Task Performance ṝ = .09 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

18

Table 1 (Continued)

Adjectives defining an assertive

Type A personality Work engagement r = .36 Hallberg, Johansson, & Schaufeli (2007)

Activity University course exam grades r = -.16 to -.27 Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham (2003)

Excitement seeking Job dedication r = -.31 Denis, Morin, & Guindon (2010)

Excitement seeking Task performance r = -.27 Denis, Morin, & Guindon (2010)

Excitement seeking Academic performance of

policemen β = -.29 Detrick, Chibnall, and Luebbert (2004)

O Openness to ideas Task Performance ṝ = .07 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

Openness to actions Task Performance ṝ = .07 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

Openness to values Task Performance ṝ = .12 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

Openness to ideas Job performance r = .26 Mussel, Winter, Gelléri, & Schuler (2011)

Openness to ideas Peer ratings of job performance r = .25 Mussel, Winter, Gelléri, & Schuler (2011)

Openness to ideas Academic performance r = .27 Mussel, Winter, Gelléri, & Schuler (2011)

Epistemic openness to

experience (ideas, actions,

values)

Job performance, peer ratings,

and academic performance r = .27 to .30 Mussel, Winter, Gelléri, & Schuler (2011)

A Trust Task Performance ṝ = .10 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013)

Compliance Task Performance ṝ = .09 Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford (2013) Note. C = Conscientiousnes, A = Agreeableness, O = Openness to experience, E = Extraversion, ṝ = uncorrected meta-analytic correlation coefficient

.

19

Table 2 Definitions of NEO Facets that were utilized in the Development of the Work

Discipline Compound Scale

Conscientiousness

Self-Discipline Ability to begin and carry out tasks, self-motivating;

persistent

Achievement Striving High aspirations and work hard to achieve goals; driven to

succeed

Dutifulness Governed by conscience; ethical; fulfill moral obligations

Order Neat, tidy, and well-organized; methodical

Deliberation Ability to think carefully before acting; cautious and

deliberate

Extraversion

Assertiveness Dominant, forceful, and socially able; take charge and

assume leadership

Activity Prefer fast-paced life; high energy level; vigorous

Excitement Seeking Crave excitement and stimulation; sensation seeking

Agreeableness

Altruism Active concern for others’ welfare; helpful, generous, and

considerate

Trust Belief that others are honest and well intentioned; not

skeptical

Compliance Cooperative; seek to inhibit aggression; forgiving; mild-

mannered

Openness

Openness to Ideas Intellectual curiosity; willingness to consider new ideas

Openness to Actions Willingness to try new activities; preference for variety to

the routine

Note. FFM facet definitions were taken from Judge, Rodell, Klinger, and Crawford (2013).

Further support for the combination of these scales, though not a precise match, can

be seen in the work of Fein and Klein (2011) where behavioral self-regulation was the

focal outcome, including goal level, feedback seeking, and performance meta-

cognition. Their compound measure included the same four conscientiousness facets,

the assertiveness and activity facets of extraversion, and the openness to ideas facet.

Self-regulatory outcomes were predicted with observed correlations from .16 to .35.

Other examples of compound scales in the literature developed to predict specific

criteria can be count in the work of Hogan and Hogan (1992) with their specific

20

occupational scales, Ones and Viswesvaran (2001) with their criterion-focused

occupational personality scales and Oh et al. (2008) with their criterion-focused

personality indices.

In this study it was hypothesized that composite factors within the compound scale

will predict task performance better than their respective broad FFM factors. And it

was hypothesized that composite factors of the compound scale will predict task

performance ratings above and over respective FFM factors. Thus:

Hypothesis 1: The compound personality scale that is going to be developed will

perform better in predicting task performance than the broad FFM factors.

Hypothesis 2: The compound personality scale will add incremental variance over the

broad FFM factors in the prediction of task performance.

The hypotheses that was just mentioned are investigating the influences of personality

on the job behavior of interest (i.e. task performance). Though, personality is an

important construct to understand and predict on-the-job-behaviors of individuals, in

the current level of our understanding it also cannot be able to explain quite a lot of

variance in the criteria that is used both in human resources and in work and

organizational psychology. One of the reasons may be the fact that individuals and

their relatively stable attibutes and behavioral consistencies are not in isolation.

1.5 Effects of Situations and Situational Strength

Situational strength, as aptly named, is related to the qualities of the environment or

situations referring to the extent the situations signal expectations about what is

appropriate behavior. Traces of the concept can be found in the interactionist tradition,

and the term “situational strength” as used by Meyer, Dalal, and Hermida (2010) was

first coined by Walter Mischel (1977). In terms of the development of the concept,

much of the theoretical and empirical work was done by Meyer in a series of recent

articles (Meyer & Dalal, 2009; Meyer et al., 2009; Meyer et al., 2010; Meyer et al.,

2014). . Meyer et al., (2010) defined it formally as “implicit or explicit cues provided

by external entities regarding the desirability of potential behaviors” (p. 122).

21

Literature that is related to the use of personality in personnel selection includes traces

of support for the use of situational strength. For instance, in their review, Barrick and

Mount (2012) gave theoretical support for taking account of situational strength when

assessing for personality in selection contexts. As a result of their meta-analytic study

on the associations between personality and behavioral outcomes, they commented

that:

these meta-analyses revealed stronger relationships between

personality and behavior when performance is more discretionary and

volitional (e.g., citizenship behavior, counterproductive work behavior,

and personal development or training success) than with behavior that

is more closely monitored and structured (e.g., task performance or

even overall job performance) (Barrick & Mount, 2012, p. 233)

Situational strength not only interacts with the personality-job performance

relationship, it was also mentioned in the context of ability and motivation expression

in work settings. Peters and O’Connor (1980) hypothesized about the influences of

situational constraints, and indicated that situational factors may be working as a

ceiling factor to high ability and/or high motivation individuals. A worker due to

her/his higher motivation or ability can be expected to perform better, may perform on

a level that is determined by situational constraints. Likewise, an organization member

that has lower expectations regarding his performance may not be affected by

performance inhibiting situational constraints.

According to Meyer and Dalal (2009) there are a lot of ad hoc conceptualizations that

researchers use from time to time to describe, conceptualize, and measure situational

characteristics. For instance, researchers used concepts such as climate strength,

accountability, situational ambiguity, stability, autonomy, and feedback in order to

describe innate qualities of working environments. Meyer and Dalal (2009) tried to

integrate these different conceptualizations by using a four-factor structure to describe

and include the entire conceptual domain of situations. These four factors are clarity,

consistency, constraints, and consequences. High levels of clarity, consistency, and

constraints, and a high possibility of severe consequences reflect strong situations in

22

which variance in behavioral expressions across individuals at the workplace is

constrained.

The definition of clarity was given by Meyer et al. (2010) as “the extent to which cues

regarding work-related responsibilities or requirements are available and easy to

understand” (p. 125). Clarity can be achieved by unambiguous information flow

concerning the expected behaviors from employees. Qualities of various information

sources within an organization determine the clarity of the work environment. Salient

norms, well-established procedures, clear instructions from supervisors (e.g., feedback

quantity and quality) can lead to an organizational environment high in clarity.

Consistency was defined as “the extent to which cues regarding work-related

responsibilities or requirements are compatible with each other” (p. 126, Meyer, Dalal,

& Hermida, 2010). Even though, consistency is related to clarity, it is different

conceptually. When different sources of information (e.g., regulatory legislations vs.

organizational policies, supervisor instructions vs. organizational norms) convey

different cues on the appropriateness of behaviors, it leads to inconsistent situations.

In such situations, personality differences are posited to influence behavior to a greater

extent than in situations where there are a lot of cues regarding which behaviors are

more appropriate.

The third factor, constraints, was defined as “the extent to which an individual’s

freedom of decision and action is limited by forces outside his or her control” (p.126,

Meyer et al., 2010). This concept is quite related to discretionary behaviors in work.

To the extent that a worker is able to decide which tasks to perform, and how and when

to perform them, level of constraints decreases. And finally, the factor of consequences

was defined as “the extent to which decisions or actions have important positive or

negative implications for any relevant person or entity” (p. 127, Meyer, Dalal, &

Hermida, 2010). For instance, work environments which have the quality of being

extremely dangerous, may have serious negative consequences for any employee.

Within this kind of environments (e.g., high-rise building exterior facet cleaning, jobs

related to nuclear waste management), even employees who are relatively lower on

conscientiousness, or lower on dutifulness (i.e., a facet of conscientiousness) can

23

behave conscientiously. Negative consequences which are inherent qualities of some

jobs lead to uniform behaviors that can attenuate predictive power of personality

differences.

These conceptual definitions of the situational strength were also put to empirical

scrutiny by Meyer et al. (2014). In their research they used two different samples

representing diverse job titles, by selecting heterogeneous samples in terms of jobs.

The results of two separate CFA analyses revealed that the four-factor structure of

situational strength, as opposed to three and two factor structures was a more plausible

conceptual framework.

In their meta-analysis Meyer et al. (2009) found stronger correlations between

conscientiousness and job performance in occupations that were classified as weak by

the help O*NET classification. Particularly, when overall job performance was

considered as a criterion, correlations between conscientiousness and job performance

scattered between the ranges of .09 and .23. Likewise, in an analyses when task

performance was selected as a criterion, conscientiousness performance relationship

ranged from .06 to .18. In all of these correlation ranges, stronger correlations were

noted in occupations that were categorized as weak.

In order to develop a measurement instrument, Meyer et al. (2014) conducted four

sequential studies and created a 28-item questionnaire, which is used to measure

situational strength in this study. In their research, Meyer et al. (2014) also studied the

moderating influence of situational strength on the personality-voluntary work

behavior relationship. As a result of a series of moderation tests, it was found that

situational strength and its four facets (i.e., clarity, consistency, constraints, and

consequences) moderated the relationship between conscientiousness and OCBs (βs

of interaction terms ranged between -.13 to .14).

Also, in this study (Meyer et al., 2014) most of the hypotheses on the moderating

effects of situational strength on the relationship between agreeableness and OCB, and

between conscientiousness and CWBs were also supported. Hierarchical regression

analyses results showed that the relation between agreeableness and OCB was

24

significantly moderated by global situational strength (β = -.08, ΔR2 = .006),

consistency (β = -.10, ΔR2 = .011), constraints (β = -.10, ΔR2 = .011) and consequences

(β = -.07, ΔR2 = .006), but not clarity (β = -.07, ns). Also, the relation between

conscientiousness and CWB was significantly moderated by global situational strength

(β = -.28, ΔR2 = .054), clarity (β = -.18, ΔR2 = .030), consistency (β = -.15, ΔR2 = .024),

constraints (β = -.25, ΔR2 = .070) and consequences (β = -.15, ΔR2 = .026).

As a result of these findings, I hypothesize that situational strength will moderate the

relationship between the compound personality scale and job performance.

Specifically:

Hypothesis 3: The relationship between the compound personality scale and job

performance criterion of interest will be higher in weaker situations compared to strong

situations.

1.6 Application of the Item Response Theory

Item Response Theory (IRT), which was also named as latent trait theory by Lord

(1980), was gradually developed during 1960s and 1970s and became the mainstream

paradigm on testing, especially in the area of cognitive and aptitude test applications.

Development of IRT was also related to some of the weaknesses of CTT, which are

lack of invariance of item parameters across different examinee groups, unsuitable

support from CTT to tailored testing (also called as adaptive testing), and also

problems in test equating.

Hambleton, Swaminathan, and Rogers (1991; Hambleton & Swaminathan, 1985)

succinctly provided the basic tenets of IRT. First one is that the performance (this is

usually taken as the probability of correct response) of a person when faced with a test

item can be explained by abilities or latent traits of that individual. The second one is

that this relationship between a person’s trait level and performance can be described

by using a monotonically increasing function that is called item characteristic function

(ICF) (aka. item characteristic curve; ICC).

25

In its core, IRT is a series of mathematical models that relate the properties of items

and persons to the probability of correctly responding. The focal attribute of persons

is their latent trait level (θ). The properties of items can be their difficulty levels,

discrimination levels, or guessing. IRT models can estimate all three with larger

samples, or just the difficulty parameter as in the Rasch model, or both the difficulty

and discrimination parameters (2-PL models). Defining an IRT model as a model that

is relating some item and person characteristics to correctly responding does not hold

in personality or attitude research, as personality measurements mostly use

polytomous items, such as Likert-type questions. These types of questions have

multiple response options (e.g. presenting a scale which has ‘strongly disagree’ on one

end, ‘strongly agree’ on the other end) and there are no right and wrong answers, so

responding correctly is not an option in these kinds of items. In the case of personality

or attitude research, another family of IRT models; polytomous IRT models are used,

derived from the original IRT models. These models relate the characteristics of people

and items not to correctly responding but to the probability of selecting a response

category on the Likert-type scale (Emretson & Reise, 2000).

In classical test theory (CTT) properties of items are not taken into explicit

consideration. Rather than explicitly modeling item properties, CTT is more interested

in the aggregate scores of the test takers. As aptly summarized by DeVellis (2003), as

CTT relates itself mostly with composite scores, on the other hand IRT’s primary focus

is on item characteristics. The composite score may mean a sum of correct responses

in an aptitude test, or the sum of Likert-type responses in a personality test. In the

formula of CTT the basic outcome of interest is the observed score. A test score (or

observed score; X) is considered to be composed of true score (T) and error score (E).

This does not mean that there are no indices regarding item properties in CTT. There

are also the indices of item difficulty and discrimination in CTT but these indices are

defined and estimated differently than their counterparts in IRT parameters.

The CTT version of difficulty is defined as “the proportion of examinees that give a

correct response to a test item” (Reckase, 2009). In a sample if the average ability of

participants is higher than the average ability level of the population, item difficulty

26

levels will be higher when computed from that sample. In CTT as the item difficulty

index increases, item difficulty decreases, just the opposite of the interpretation that is

observed with the IRT difficulty parameter. In IRT, difficulty is defined as the required

trait level to pass an item with a fifty per cent probability. As such, higher difficulty

parameters indicate more difficult items. In CTT, item discrimination refers to the

difference between the percentage of high achievers and the percentage of low

achievers who get the item right. Discrimination in CTT is expressed as a correlation

between scores on an item and scores on the total test (i.e. item total correlation). In

IRT, discrimination refers to the slope of the ICC at the point of the difficulty

parameter, and similarly refers to how much an item can differentiate between trait

levels. In polytomous models, item difficulty takes on the name of item location

parameter. A high location parameter means that few examinees are expected to

endorse the higher response categories of the rating scale. Item discrimination

indicates how quickly item scores change as a function of trait level. Item information

function (IIF) provide information as to which items yield the most information and

also provides the trait level where the item is most informative.

On the one hand, as CTT doesn’t explicitly take item properties into account, these

item indices estimated from a sample are heavily dependent on the characteristics of

the sample. Average ability levels and the range of ability scores in a sample have an

impact on the item difficulty and item discrimination indices (Hambleton &

Swaminathan, 1985). On the other hand, this is not the case in IRT, and as it can be

inferred from Figure 1 parameters defining item characteristics are not influenced by

sample characteristics in IRT. In the figure, ability distributions of two different

samples are represented by the curves under the x-axis. But, when a proper model is

fitted to an item, performance on the item that is described by IRT is not influenced by

the characteristics of the sample.

Determining items that function best as indicators of the construct at hand (i.e. latent

trait) is a vital task in psychological measurement. This task, selecting the best items

for a test, is probably at the very core of developing any sound psychological measure.

In CTT, each item is given the same weight. In a cognitive ability measure this means,

27

we are assuming that every item has the same difficulty and discrimination levels as

the others, while this is clearly not the case. In IRT terminology, this means that while

scoring the participants, item characteristics aren’t taken into account. However, in

every testing situation some items are more reflective of the construct being measured

and some items do not reflect the construct being measured as good as the others.

With regard to this issue IRT offers a solution by explicitly taking into account the

characteristics of the items (see Equation 1) into the model estimated. Given below is

the formula of a two-parametric model (aka. two parametric logistic model; 2PL;

Embretson & Reise, 2000, p. 70).

(1)

In this formula, the probability of answering an item correctly (i.e. P(θ)) in a given

ability or trait level (i.e. θ) is modeled based on item discrimination that is denoted by

a, and item difficulty (i.e. also called location when IRT applied in measurement of

personality or aptitude) that is denoted by b. Once these item difficulty and

discrimination parameters are estimated for every item, it can be possible to detect in

which ranges of the trait an item best discriminates among people.

28

Figure 1 Item Response Function (IRT) with differing sample characteristics

(Hambleton et al., 1991, p. 8).

Item response theory has been enjoying popularity and widespread use in measurement

of aptitude, achievement, and proficiency for quite some time, but the approach has

been underutilized in the area of personality measurement. Reise and Henson (2003)

lamented the fact that unlike cognitive ability testing, the personality assessment

development efforts have ignored improvements in measurement theory and thus a lot

of personality measures have been developed without utilizing IRT parameters.

Using IRT and estimated difficulty and discrimination parameters, one can find in

which response ranges an item functions effectively (Weiner & Greene, 2008). For

instance, Gray-Little, Williams, and Hancock (as cited in Reise & Henson, 2003)

analyzed a measure using IRT that is known for its internal consistency reliability, the

29

Rosenberg Self-Esteem scale. Their analysis showed a high internal consistency

estimate, but also showed the poor differentiation potential of the scale among high

self-esteem examinees. If one is going to observe self-esteem change over time or try

to differentiate among high self-esteem individuals, one needs to be aware of this

shortcoming of the scale. As self-esteem scores of participants change in a positive

manner, the probability of finding significant associations with another construct will

probably decrease since true effects size will not be captured as variance at the upper

ends of the scale would be missed.

In another study (Hafsteinsson, Donovan, & Breland, 2007) goal orientation scales

were compared and contrasted based on IRT parameters. Each scale was analyzed and

items with the highest discrimination and information indices were retained. It

appeared that some items were not able to differentiate between goal orientation trait

levels, rendering the use of them questionable.

In this study, an IRT model fitted to the data will be used as a guide to select personality

items more discriminating between latent trait holders, especially at the higher trait

levels. Individuals with higher work discipline trait levels would be better identified

by a personality scale that is refined in terms of its item discrimination and information

parameters. Such a scale may offer higher validities in the prediction of performance.

In order to examine this possibility, IRT-based scale will be used. As a research

question, predictive validities of the IRT-based scale and CTT-based scale will be

compared to delineate differences of validities in predicting task performance.

30

CHAPTER 2

STUDY 1 METHOD

2.1 Overview

The basic aims of Study 1 were to: 1) develop a scale measuring the lower order facets

of FFM factors to predict work performance, 2) collect data in order to refine the scale

according to its psychometric properties based on: a) CTT, and b) IRT procedures.

Items of the facets which formed the conceptual framework of the compound

personality measure were compiled from an internet source; the International

Personality Item Pool (IPIP) web site (Goldberg et al., 2006). This web platform went

online in 1996 and includes over 2000 items regarding different personality

dimensions and from different personality measures and scales with free access to

researchers.

Using the data that was collected in Study 1, the psychometric characteristics of the

measure were first evaluated, and if necessary, modified according to exploratory

factor analysis (EFA) and internal consistency reliability analysis. The refined

instrument with 124 items based on CTT was administered to participants in Study 2.

2.2 Participants and Procedure

In order to conduct CTT-based psychometric analyses, determine the factor structure

of the personality measure and also estimate the needed parameters of the IRT analysis,

the first wave of data collection was conducted mostly in Middle East Technical

University (METU). Online questionnaires of the study were disseminated by using

an online questionnaire service provider, surveymonkey.com. In exchange for bonus

points or to participate in a lottery that gives the participants a chance to win a small

tablet computer, students filled out the initial compound personality measure

questionnaire online which took approximately 20 minutes of their time.

A total of 496 participant responded to the online questionnaire. Twenty-six

participants who had missing values more than 10% of the items, 32 of the participants

31

that answered wrong option in keyed questions, and 15 of the participants who have

completed questionnaires in less than 13 minutes were screened out from the analysis.

As a result, responses of 423 people who participated in study one were analyzed.

In terms of gender distribution, 62% of participants were female (N = 261) and 38%

were male (N = 161). Mean age of the participants was 22.2, and age of the participants

ranged from 17 to 68. But, as participants were mainly university students, the data

were skewed in terms of age. Median age was 23 and, 42.5% of participants were

between ages of 20 and 21. Of the participants who shared their education status, 91%

(N = 384) were undergraduate students, 5% (N = 23) were M.S. students, and 2% (N

= 9) were Ph.D. students.

In terms of faculty distribution, 52% (n = 220) of the participants were from the faculty

of arts and sciences, 24.3% (n = 103) were from the faculty of engineering, 12.3% (n

= 52) were from the faculty of economic and administrative sciences, 6.6% (n = 28)

were from the faculty of education, 2.6% (n = 11) were from the faculty of architecture,

and some of the participants (n = 9) did not disclose their faculty.

2.3 Measures

2.3.1 The Big Five Inventory

As a measure of broad Five Factor Model (FFM) dimensions, Turkish adapted version

(Sümer & Sümer, 2002) of the Big Five Inventory (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991)

was used. This measure is composed of 44 questions. Openness to experience scale

consists of 10 items, each of the Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness scales of the

inventory has 9 items, and each of the Extraversion and Neuroticism scales has 8 items.

Participants responded to each item of the inventory on a scale ranging from 1

(disagree strongly) to 6 (agree strongly). Even though the original BFI has a 5-point

scale, in order to avoid central tendency bias, in this study a 6-point Likert-type scale

was used.

Reliability of BFI was widely studied and accepted. Benet-Martinez and John (1998)

developed and tested a Spanish version of BFI on three different samples and reported

32

their reliability estimates across samples. In their English speaking sample estimated

reliability ranged from .73 and .80 with a mean reliability estimate of .78. In their

Spanish speaking sample and across all samples reliability estimates ranged from .69

and .77 with a mean reliability score of .74. Sümer, Lajunen, and Özkan (2005)

reported reliability values ranging from .64 to .77 for a Turkish sample. In a cross-

cultural study that was led by Schmit et al. (2007) the internal reliabilities of the factors

ranged from .70 to .79 on a sample of 17408 participants which represents 56 nations.

All of these reliability estimates reported by various empirical studies indicate high

internal reliability of the BFI.

In the current study, the Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities of the BFI ranged from .77

to .86. Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Openness

to experience scales had reliability scores of .77, .85, .86, .82, and .84, respectively.

2.3.2 Development of the Compound Personality Scale

In order to gather items to form the compound personality scale, relevant IPIP facet

and factor items were examined one by one. As an initial step 621 items were identified

that were thought to reflect the sub-dimensions of selected FFM facets (i.e.

achievement striving, dependability, order, self-discipline, dutifulness, assertiveness,

activity, excitement seeking, trust, altruism, compliance, openness to actions, and

openness to ideas). Secondly, which items to retain in data collection was decided

based on a deductive process as described below.

2.3.2.1 Item Elimination Procedure

Items were eliminated based on the close scrutiny of two industrial/ organizational

psychologists (the author and the supervisor of the study), on grounds related to item

“redundancy,” “ambiguity,” “conceptual equivalence,” “population specificity,” and

“conceptual irrelevance”. At first items that are coming from different scales that were

exactly the same or very similar were eliminated. At the second step, items that are too

vague, ambiguous, and hard to apprehend were eliminated, such as ‘I am always

prepared’, ‘Am ready to do battle for a cause’, or ‘Act according to my conscience’.

At the third step of the screening procedure, items that are not same but conceptually

33

very similar were deleted from the item pool. Items such as ‘Enjoy examining myself

and my life’ and ‘Try to examine myself objectively’ were deemed similar, or ‘Lack

the talent for influencing people’ and ‘Seek to influence others’ were deemed similar.

At the fourth step of the procedure, items that may be irrelevant to the general

population were deleted, such as ‘Know the answers to many questions’, or ‘Am nice

to store clerks’. As a final step, using the facet definitions on Table 2, conceptually

irrelevant items that were not thought to pertain to the definition of the relevant facet

were deleted. A total of 467 items were eliminated this way as they were deemed

redundant.

Remaining items were translated by the author from English to Turkish. Translated

items were later back translated to English by a bilingual speaker. After the back-

translation, original English items and back translated English items were compared

by another bilingual speaker. Items that were evaluated as similar to original item after

the back-translation process were kept, other were rephrased to keep to the original

meaning. A total of 150 items passed previously mentioned item elimination steps and

were disseminated online. These items can be seen in Appendix A.

34

CHAPTER 3

STUDY 1 RESULTS

After the collection of the data, an EFA was conducted first to determine the factor

structure of the initial questionnaire. Following the EFA, a series of IRT analyses were

done to estimate item discrimination and location parameter of the items, and to obtain

the IIF.

3.1 Exploratory Factor Analyses

Determining the number of factors to extract in an EFA is one of the most important

aspects of the factor analysis procedure. There are traditional ways to determine the

number of factors to extract in the analysis, such as the eigenvalues-greater-than-one

rule or the inflection point in the scree plot of eigenvalues. However, since these

decision rules are quite subjective and regarded as problematic and other solutions

were promoted by O’Connor (2000) as giving optimal solutions regarding the number

of factors to extract. These were parallel analysis and Velicer’s minimum average

partial (MAP). Of the two of these method offered, parallel analysis was used to

delineate the number of factors to extract.

In parallel analysis, eigenvalues from random data sets are compared with the

eigenvalues calculated from the actual data set. As a result of this comparison, number

of factors to retain is determined. Computed data sets that are produced by random

number generation algorithm, are similar to the actual one in terms of number of

participants and variables. The researcher doing the analysis determines the number of

random data sets to be calculated, samples sizes between 1000 and 5000 was

considered sufficient (O’Connor, 2000). After the computation of random samples,

95th percentile of distributions of eigenvalues compared with the eigenvalue that is

computed from the actual data. As long as the eigenvalue that is coming from the actual

sample is larger than the 95th percentile of eigenvalue distribution, it is retained as a

factor during the factor analysis procedure.

35

In order to determine the number of factors to retain in the compound personality scale

IBM SPSS syntax of O’Connor (2000) was used. In order to determine number of

factors to extract 500, 1000, and 5000 samples that are completely random and also

that were permutations of the raw study data created by the syntax. Eigenvalues on the

95th percentile of these random samples were compared with the eigenvalues from the

raw data. In all of these comparisons a 14-factor solution was suggested by the parallel

analysis (see Figure 2).

Figure 2 Parallel Analysis Real and Random Data Generated Eigenvalues

Using IBM SPSS 20, Principal Axis Factoring (PAF) with direct oblimin rotation was

employed to determine the factor structure of the scale. As the results of the parallel

analyses indicated a 14-factor solution, in the analysis14 factors were extracted.

Results of this analysis did not lead to interpretable results. A 13-factor was also

extracted, leading to more interpretable results. In order to reach an acceptable factor

solution, items with poor factor loadings were deleted. In addition to that, one of the

36

factors formed with only three items without any item that had a loading higher

than .40 was discarded, leading to a 12-factor solution.

Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy of the factor analysis

was .88. And also none of the KMO values of the individual items were below .55.

This is an index of appropriateness of sample size for the analysis, which can take on

values that range from 0 to 1. Values between .80 and .90 are considered good

(Hutcheson & Sofroniou, 1999). KMO value that was obtained as a result of the EFA

indicates that the sample size is adequate to produce a population correlation matrix.

The other index that was used to evaluate the factorability of the data was Bartlett’s

(1950) test of sphericity, which was found to be significant (χ2(11175) = 36719.69, p

< .001), which is indicating that variance the covariance matrix is proportional to the

identity matrix.

In total, factors explained 43.82% of the variance. Names were assigned to factors by

key indicator items of the factors. Factor names, means, standard deviations, and

reliability values (Cronbach’s alpha) of the factors formed are shown in Table 2. The

FA table with items and loadings based on the pattern matrix is given in Appendix J.

Table 3 Descriptive statistics for the 12-factor solution of the Compound Personality

Measure

Facet Name Corresponding FFM Factor Mean SD α

1 Self-discipline C 3.89 0.79 .95

2 Excitement seeking E 4.28 0.70 .80

3 Altruism A 4.64 0.63 .85

4 Achievement striving C 4.09 0.72 .73

5 Deliberation C 4.54 0.65 .81

6 Assertiveness E 4.45 0.75 .73

7 Compliance A 3.80 0.93 .72

8 Order C 4.09 1.19 .82

9 Openness to ideas O 4.40 0.71 .81

10 Assertiveness/Exhibition E 3.97 0.82 .82

11 Dutifulness C 4.45 0.75 .77

12 Trust A 3.74 0.89 .78

Note. Scale values ranges between 1 and 6. SD = Standard deviation α = Cronbach’s alpha, C =

Conscientiousness, E = Extraversion, A = Agreeableness, O = Openness to Experience, N = Neuroticism.

37

First factor was named as self-discipline facet. Marker items of this factor are ‘I don’t

put my mind on the task at hand’ (Elimdeki işe odaklanmakta zorlanırım), ‘I don’t see

things through’ (Bazı işlerin sonunu getiremem), ‘I quickly lose interest in the tasks I

start’ (Başladığım işlere ilgimi çabuk kaybederim), ‘I stop when work becomes too

difficult’ (İş çok zorlaştığında devam etmem), and ‘I don’t get sidetracked when I

work’ (Çalışırken başka şeylere kaymam) and this facet has thirty four items in it.

These items clearly represent the description written for self-discipline in Judge et al.

(2013) and that was also reproduced in Table 1.

Dimension that formed the second factor was named as excitement seeking and

consists of twelve items. Marker items for this factor are ‘I seek adventure’ (Macera

ararım) and ‘I am willing to try anything once’ (Her şeyi bir kere denemeye

gönüllüyüm). Definition given for this facet in Judge et al. (2013) is “crave for

excitement and stimulation; sensation seeking” (p. 877).

Third factor was named as altruism. Marker items for this dimension are ‘I take no

time for others’ (Diğer insanlar için vakit ayırmam), ‘I can’t be bothered with other’s

needs’ (Başkalarının ihtiyaçlarıyla uğraşamam), ‘I like to be of service to others’

(Diğer insanlara yardımımın dokunmasını severim), and ‘I am indifferent to the

feelings of others’ (Diğer insanların duygularına kayıtsızımdır). The factor included

thirteen items.

Fourth factor was named as achievement striving facet of conscientiousness. Seven

items loaded under this factor. Markers of this factor are ‘I stick to the rules’ (Kurallara

bağlı kalırım), ‘I respect authority’ (Otoriteye saygı duyarım), ‘I want to be the very

best’ (En iyisi olmak isterim), and ‘I try to outdo others’ (Performansımla diğerlerini

geçmeye çalışırım).

Fifth factor was named as deliberation, which is also a facet of conscientiousness factor

of NEO. It had ten items. ‘I act quickly without thinking’ (Düşünmeden aceleyle

harekete geçerim), ‘I am a firm beliver in thinking things through’ (Bir işe başlarken

enine boyuna düşünmenin gerekliliğine inanırım), ‘I make decisions only after I have

all the facts’ (Ancak bütün bilgileri değerlendirdikten sonra bir karar veririm), and ‘I

38

weigh the pros and cons’ (Durumların eksi ve artı yönlerini tartarım) are the marker

items for this factor.

Sixth factor was named as assertiveness, which is a facet of the agreeableness factor

of NEO. There are five items within this factor and ‘I am not afraid of providing

criticism’ (Eleştirilerimi dile getirmekten çekinmem), ‘I say what I think’

(Düşündüğümü söylerim), and ‘I stick up for myself when necessary’ (Gerektiğinde

kendimi savunurum) are the highest loading items of this factor.

Seventh factor was named after the compliance dimension of agreeableness. Five items

were loaded under this facet. Marker items for this dimension are ‘I have a sharp

tongue’ (Sivri bir dilim vardır) and ‘I contradict others’ (Diğer insanlarla zıtlaşırım).

Eighth factor was named as order, as it is parallel to the definition of the NEO factor

conscientiousness facet of order. There are four items within this facet. ‘I am not

bothered by messy people’ (Dağınık insanlar beni rahatsız etmez), and ‘I am not

bothered by disorder’ (Düzensizlik beni rahatsız etmez) are the items which have

highest loadings.

Ninth factor was named as openness to ideas, after the facet with the same name from

the NEO factor of openness. There are twelve items in this factor. Items that have the

highest loadings for this facet are ‘I love to read challenging material’ (Beni zorlayan

metinleri okumayı severim), ‘I avoid philosophical discussions’ (Felsefi tartışmalardan

kaçınırım), ‘I avoid difficult reading material’ (Ağır okumalardan uzak dururum), and

‘I am not interested in abstract ideas’ (Soyut fikirler ilgimi çekmez).

Tenth factor was named as assertiveness/exhibition. This factor has eight items in it. ‘I

am good at making impromptu speeches’ (Doğaçlama konuşmalar yapmakta

iyiyimdir), ‘I can express myself easily’ (Kendimi rahatça ifade edebilirim), ‘I don’t

like to draw attention to myself’ (Dikkati üzerime çekmeyi sevmem), and ‘I find it

difficult to manage others’ (Diğer insanları idare etmek bana zor gelir) are the markers.

39

The name that was given to the eleventh factor is dutifulness. This factor has eight

items. Items with the highest loadings in order are ‘I misrepresent the facts when

necessary’ (Gerektiğinde gerçekleri saptırırım), ‘I cannot imagine lying or cheating’

(Yalan söylemeyi veya hile yapmayı düşünemem bile), ‘I lie to get myself out of

trouble’ (Başımı beladan kurtarmak için yalan söylerim), and ‘I cheat to get ahead’

(Öne geçmek için hile yapabilirim).

The last factor was named after the trust facet of agreeableness. In this factor there are

six items. Highest loaded items within this factor are ‘I believe that others have good

intentions’ (Diğer insanların niyetlerinin iyi olduğuna inanıyorum), ‘Believe that

people are basically moral’ (İnsanların temelde dürüst olduğuna inanıyorum), and ‘I

trust what people say’ (İnsanların söylediklerine güvenirim).

As a result of the PFA, of the total 150 items, 124 items were retained. As mentioned

before three items that loaded under a factor with very low loadings were omitted.

Furthermore, 23 items that did not have loadings higher than .30 or had high cross-

loadings across factors were also omitted from further analyses. The following IRT

analyses are based on the remaining 124 items.

3.2 IRT Analyses

IRT analyses of the data were conducted in order to find category threshold and

discrimination parameters, by a package of R statistical software environment (R Core

Team, 2014) that was named as mirt (an acronym for multidimensional item response

theory) (Chalmers, 2012). Model estimation in the mirt package is done by an

expectation maximization (EM) algorithm. This package, as it was first developed in

2012, can be considered as quite a new software, and it was used in literature to

delineate the latent dimensional structure of a German vocational interest inventory,

the General Interest Structure Test (AIST-R) (Wetzel & Hell, 2014). Feuerstahler and

Waller (2014) also used the mirt package to study the fit of a 4-parameter IRT model

to Multiphasic Personality Inventory-Adolescent form.

40

As noted in Embretson and Reise (2000) assumptions of unidimensionality and local

independence are important for estimating sound parameters from IRT models.

Unidimensionality was said to be satisfied when “a single latent trait variable is

sufficient to explain the common variance among item responses” (p. 227). Local

independence is a related concept to unidimensionality. Local independence is said to

be achieved “when the abilities influencing test performance are held constant,

examinees’ responses to any pair of items are statistically independent” (p. 10,

Hambleton et al., 1991). With regard to the aforementioned assumptions, there are no

hard-and-fast rules to determine unidimensionality as it was defined in the IRT context

(Chernyshenko, Stark, Chan, Drasgow, & Williams, 2001). But, as it was indicated by

Drasgow and Parsons, IRT model parameter estimations can withhold small violations

of these assumptions (as cited in Embretson & Reise, 2000). This is especially the case

when the latent trait dimensions are quite correlated (e.g. in personality research) or

the other dimension that is violating the unidimensionality is relatively small. In this

study to assume a certain degree of unidimensionality, factors that were obtained with

the previously mentioned factor analysis were used as separate units of analysis. This

method to assure unidimensionality was also used by Yang and Kao (2014) and also

in Edelen and Reeve (2007).

The Graded Response Model (GRM) was used in order to estimate the item difficulty

and discrimination parameters (Samejima, 1969). In GRM discrimination parameters

vary across items. Furthermore, each item has a set of category threshold parameters.

The formula is given in Equation 2.

Equation 2. Samejima’s Graded Response Model (Samejima, 1969)

Pix(θ) = exp[αi(θ – bi)] / 1 + exp[αi(θ – bi)] (2)

Where:

θ: Latent trait level

Pix(θ): probability of an examinee’s raw item response (x) falling in or above

a given category threshold (i.e. 5 thresholds for a 6-point scale) conditional

on trait level.

41

αi: item discrimination parameter

bi: item location parameter

For each factor of the work discipline category threshold parameters, discrimination

parameters, operating characteristic curves (OCC), and IIF were obtained. Parameters

for each item can be seen in tables. The criteria for selecting items were three-fold: 1)

Attention was paid for the item to have a relatively high level of discrimination index,

2) IIF would make a peak that surpassed the value of 0.5 (Allen, Weissman, Hellwig,

MacCann, & Roberts, 2014), rather than being flat, 3) the ratio of chi-square to degrees

of freedom would be less than 3 (Hafsteinsson et al., 2007). The third criterion was

satisfied for all items, thus item elimination proceeded based on the first two. As an

example, item3 of the self-discipline facet had a discrimination value of 2.05, which

was relatively higher amongst others in the scale and the IIF peaked at 1.25. This item

was retained. On the other hand, item11 of the same scale had a discrimination value

of 0.99, and the IIF was relatively flat, reaching .32.

Similar to this comparison, item 11 (“Macera ararım”) and 8 (“Alışıldık yöntemlere

bağlıyımdır”) of the excitement seeking scale can be examined side-by-side. IIF of the

11th item can be seen on Figure 3 and IIF of the 8th item can be seen on Figure 4. In

both figures the vertical axis of the graph represents the information item gives and

the horizontal axis represent trait level continuum. Eleventh item of the excitement

seeking scale has a discrimination value of 1.57, and in IIF graph its information

approaches to 0.8 (see Figure 3). In comparison to that 8th item of the excitement

seeking scale has a discrimination parameter of .97 and information value that can be

shown by Figure 4 slightly surpasses .25 (see Figure 4). Using these criteria and IFF

graphs, 8th item of the scale of extcitement seeking removed from and 11th item kept

in the IRT-based scale. Using similar comparisons within the scales (i.e., self-

discipline, excitement seeking, etc.) across items, decisions to keep and to remove

items made.

42

Figure 3 IIF of the 11th Item of the Excitement Seeking Scale

In the self-discipline facet items 4, 10, 11, 22, 24, and 33 were removed as their

information curve didn’t reach beyond .50 in their respective IIF plots (see Table 4).

All of the IIF plots of the items that are mentioned here can be found in Appendix I.

In excitement seeking facet items 1, 2, 3, 6, 7, and 8 were removed (see Table 5). In

altruism facet after examining Table 6 and checking the IFF plots items 1, 2, 3, 4, 6,

and 13 were eliminated. With regard to achievement striving facet, 1, 2, 5, 6, and 7

were eliminated (see Table 7). In deliberation facets there were ten items. After IRT

analysis, items 4 and 6 were eliminated after examination of the IFFs (see Table 8). In

assertiveness facet items 1 and 3 were removed from IRT compound measure (see

Table 9). In compliance facet items 4 and 5 were removed (see Table 10). In order facet

none of the items were removed, as both of their discrimination parameters and IIF

were above the criterion threshold that were determined before (see Table 11). In

openness to ideas facet, of the initial twelve items five of the items were eliminated

(items 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 7, and 10) (see Table 12). In assertiveness/exhibition facet two

43

items were removed by IRT analysis (items 3 and 5) (see Table 13). Of the eight items

in the dutifulness facet four of the eight items were removed (items 1, 4, 6, and 8) (seet

Table 14). And in the final scale of trust two of the 5 items were removed (see Table

15).

Figure 4 IIF of the 8th Item of the Excitement Seeking Scale

As a result, 28 items were retained in self-discipline, seven in excitement seeking

(which also included items from openness to actions), seven in altruism, two in

achievement striving, eight in deliberateness, three in assertiveness, three in

compliance, all four in order, five in openness to ideas, six in assertiveness/ exhibition,

four in dutifulness, and three in trust. Thus, the CTT-based 124 item measure was

further reduced to 80 items.

44

Table 4 1st Facet Self-Discipline: IRT Item Parameters, Chi-squares, Chi-square/df ratios, and Decisions to Retain

Label a c1 c2 c3 c4 c5 χ2 df χ2/df Decision

1 Bir işi bitirmeden bırakmam 1.88 5.44 3.39 1.78 -0.04 -2.52 105.09 86 1.22 Kept

2 Zamanımı boşa harcarım 1.64 3.71 1.44 -0.27 -1.62 -3.83 105.99 95 1.12 Kept

3 Sıkı çalışırım 2.05 5.02 2.71 1.11 -1.01 -3.41 118.13 80 1.48 Kept

4 Zorlu görevleri kabul ederim 1.30 4.73 3.48 1.71 0.09 -2.18 93.18 87 1.07 Removed

5 Önümdeki engellere rağmen işleri bitiririm 2.11 8.42 5.02 2.85 0.27 -3.51 59.52 52 1.14 Kept

6 Çalışmaya başlamak için birisinin beni itmesi gerekir 1.71 3.85 2.05 0.42 -1.01 -2.70 130.54 103 1.27 Kept

7 Çalışırken başka şeylere kaymam 1.51 2.83 0.72 -0.75 -2.31 -4.53 103.53 93 1.11 Kept

8 Vermem gereken kararları ertelerim 1.95 4.50 1.77 -0.26 -1.64 -3.97 120.29 86 1.40 Kept

9 Görevlerime bütün kalbimle atılırım 1.64 6.68 3.98 1.98 -0.04 -2.54 85.86 74 1.16 Kept

10 İşleri ağırdan almayı severim 1.23 3.62 1.94 0.52 -1.01 -2.55 128.56 102 1.26 Removed

11 Keyfimi kaçıracak işleri ertelerim 0.99 2.28 0.15 -1.58 -2.82 -4.94 89.79 84 1.07 Removed

12 Benden beklenilenden daha fazlasını yaparım 1.41 5.07 3.41 1.64 -0.39 -3.04 101.73 76 1.34 Kept

13 Planlar yapar ve bunlara bağlı kalırım 1.74 5.13 3.50 1.57 -0.68 -3.15 83.19 71 1.17 Kept

14 İşimde çok titizimdir 1.44 6.34 3.58 1.95 0.16 -2.06 94.32 79 1.19 Kept

15 Başladığım/üstlendiğim işlerin sonunu getiririm 2.22 7.29 5.97 3.74 1.27 -2.29 43.28 47 0.92 Kept

16 Başarılı olmak için motivasyonum yüksek değildir 1.95 4.21 2.34 0.69 -0.98 -3.30 91.37 89 1.03 Kept

17 İş çok zorlaştığında devam etmem 1.79 5.39 3.31 1.35 -0.51 -2.79 101.27 78 1.30 Kept

18 İşlerimden kaytarırım 2.23 6.18 3.81 1.50 -0.42 -2.92 93.97 75 1.25 Kept

19 İşlerime vakit kaybetmeden başlarım 1.67 4.15 1.79 0.25 -1.42 -4.08 98.95 93 1.06 Kept

20 Sorumluluklardan kaçınırım 2.08 5.84 3.67 1.93 0.14 -2.12 76.27 83 0.92 Kept

21 İşleri son dakikada yaparım 1.68 3.14 1.48 -0.09 -1.40 -3.35 111.90 104 1.08 Kept

22 Sıkıldığım anda yapmakta olduğum işi bırakırım 1.07 2.61 0.95 -0.64 -1.69 -3.40 142.80 104 1.37 Removed

45

Table 4 (Continued)

23 Durumu kurtaracak kadar iş yaparım 1.45 4.37 1.97 0.24 -1.16 -3.04 105.29 97 1.09 Kept

24 Hayatımın yavaş bir temposu var 1.11 3.95 1.57 0.32 -0.93 -2.35 127.07 115 1.10 Removed

25 Başladığım işlere ilgimi çabuk kaybederim 1.87 4.05 2.42 0.50 -1.21 -3.32 108.27 86 1.26 Kept

26 Kolayca pes ederim 2.14 5.71 4.07 1.77 -0.06 -2.45 103.72 76 1.36 Kept

27 İşime pek fazla zaman ve çaba harcamam 2.38 7.46 4.68 2.43 0.34 -2.62 78.34 67 1.17 Kept

28 Kolay olan yolu seçerim 1.53 3.93 1.81 0.04 -1.17 -3.13 115.23 98 1.18 Kept

29 İşlerimi bir plana göre yaparım 1.45 4.21 3.09 1.33 -0.52 -3.08 97.66 84 1.16 Kept

30 Elimdeki işe odaklanmakta zorlanırım 2.17 5.33 2.60 0.28 -1.52 -4.10 90.12 75 1.20 Kept

31 Herşey mükemmel olana kadar devam ederim 1.37 4.12 2.62 1.11 -0.74 -2.75 118.92 87 1.37 Kept

32 Bazı işlerin sonunu getiremem 1.97 5.00 2.04 -0.13 -1.74 -4.03 92.22 83 1.11 Kept

33 İşi eğlencenin önüne koyarım 1.09 3.24 1.83 0.26 -1.13 -2.94 117.78 100 1.18 Removed

34 Günlük işlerimi vakit kaybetmeden bitiririm 1.56 4.25 2.26 0.33 -1.38 -3.70 87.72 89 0.99 Kept

Note. a = discrimination parameter, c = category treshold parameters

46

Table 5 2nd Facet Excitement Seeking: IRT Item Parameters, Chi-squares, Chi-square/df ratios, and Decisions to Retain

Label a c1 c2 c3 c4 c5 χ2 df χ2/df Decision

1 Her zaman hareket halindeyim 0.78 3.99 2.01 0.57 -0.69 -2.56 75.49 81 0.93 Removed

2 Yeni yerleri gezip görmekten keyif alırım 1.21 5.62 4.13 3.06 2.05 0.16 76.76 53 1.45 Removed

3 İlgi duyduğum birçok değişik şey vardır 1.37 6.96 4.11 2.37 0.85 -1.50 72.45 56 1.29 Removed

4 Yeni şeylere başlamaktan keyif alırım 2.27 7.63 5.29 3.74 1.74 -1.45 33.39 44 0.76 Kept

5 Değişikliklerden hoşlanmam 1.23 3.74 2.04 0.67 -0.47 -2.13 77.49 79 0.98 Kept

6 Yeni yemekler denemekten hoşlanmam 0.87 2.91 1.85 1.00 0.16 -1.19 108.14 97 1.11 Removed

7 Asla paraşüt veya bungee jumping yapmaya gitmem 0.93 2.23 1.31 0.87 -0.04 -1.25 86.69 100 0.87 Removed

8 Alışıldık yöntemlere bağlıyımdır 0.95 4.01 1.58 -0.09 -1.69 -3.23 61.41 67 0.92 Removed

9 Çeşitliliği sıradan işlere tercih ederim 1.23 4.90 3.35 2.37 0.78 -1.81 79.80 64 1.25 Kept

10 Her şeyi bir kere denemeye gönüllüyüm 1.48 4.19 2.91 1.54 -0.23 -2.43 77.21 69 1.12 Kept

11 Macera ararım 1.57 3.89 2.64 1.14 -0.48 -2.73 90.45 73 1.24 Kept

12 Risk alırım 1.28 4.44 2.73 1.07 -0.80 -2.99 62.52 67 0.93 Kept

47

Table 6 3rd Facet Altruism: IRT Item Parameters, Chi-squares, Chi-square/df ratios, and Decisions to Retain

Label a c1 c2 c3 c4 c5 χ2 df χ2/df Decision

1 Rekabetten ziyade işbirliğine değer veririm 0.98 4.86 3.09 2.11 0.67 -1.66 70.52 68 1.04 Removed

2 Diğer insanlarla işbirliği yapabilirim 1.17 5.28 3.17 1.34 -1.75 - 56.06 49 1.14 Removed

3 Memnun edilmesi kolay bir insanım 1.00 3.48 1.90 1.10 -0.03 -2.13 99.90 81 1.23 Removed

4 Diğer insanların görüşlerini göz ardı ederim 1.11 5.02 3.80 2.11 0.48 -1.79 43.51 60 0.73 Removed

5 Kendimden başkalarını da umursarım 2.10 5.96 4.99 4.10 2.35 -1.38 66.08 40 1.65 Kept

6 Eski tanıdıklarımla bağlantımı koparmam 0.93 3.06 1.87 0.67 -0.73 -2.65 84.41 84 1.00 Removed

7 Ortamın huzurunu korumaya çalışırım 1.38 5.26 4.02 3.22 1.25 -1.51 47.97 48 1.00 Kept

8 Diğer insanlara yardımımın dokunmasını severim 2.09 6.60 6.08 5.03 3.01 -0.17 34.58 31 1.12 Kept

9 Başkalarının ihtiyaçlarıyla uğraşamam 2.05 7.70 5.08 3.27 0.96 -1.71 59.25 46 1.29 Kept

10 Diğer insanların duygularına kayıtsızımdır 2.13 7.17 5.29 3.39 1.42 -1.27 54.32 43 1.26 Kept

11 Diğer insanlar için vakit ayırmam 2.29 5.79 3.72 1.61 -1.64 - 45.61 41 1.11 Kept

12 İnsanlara içtenlikle karşılandıklarını hissettiririm 1.69 4.94 4.20 2.85 1.02 -1.95 60.99 53 1.15 Kept

13 Diğer insanların çıkarlarını göz önüne alırım 1.19 4.24 3.00 1.65 -0.66 -3.57 79.15 60 1.32 Removed

48

Table 7 4th Facet Achievement Striving: IRT Item Parameters, Chi-squares, Chi-square/df ratios, and Decisions to Retain

Label a c1 c2 c3 c4 c5 χ2 df χ2/df Decision

1 Otoriteye saygı duyarım 1.24 3.07 1.96 0.47 -1.04 -3.39 51.50 57 0.90 Removed

2 Kendim ve diğerleri için yüksek standartlar belirlerim 0.80 4.73 3.31 1.93 0.57 -1.64 50.12 50 1.00 Removed

3 İşleri kuralına göre yaparım 1.56 5.54 4.40 2.36 0.18 -2.92 65.25 38 1.72 Kept

4 Kurallara bağlı kalırım 2.07 5.05 3.62 1.80 -0.67 -4.37 49.54 39 1.27 Kept

5 Performansımla diğerlerini geçmeye çalışırım 1.05 3.59 2.16 0.98 -0.43 -2.29 53.77 59 0.91 Removed

6 En iyisi olmak isterim 1.07 3.95 2.86 1.64 0.23 -1.34 52.33 54 0.97 Removed

7 Otoriteye direnirim 0.99 3.11 1.24 -0.48 -2.05 -3.71 65.94 56 1.18 Removed

49

Table 8 5th Facet Deliberation: IRT Item Parameters, Chi-squares, Chi-square/df ratios, and Decisions to Retain

Label a c1 c2 c3 c4 c5 χ2 df χ2/df Decision

1 Ancak bütün bilgileri değerlendirdikten sonra bir karar veririm 1.37 6.19 3.81 2.25 0.51 -1.94 43.68 49 0.89 Kept

2 Bir işe başlarken enine boyuna düşünmenin gerekliliğine inanırım 1.98 6.26 5.23 3.27 1.42 -1.64 37.90 38 1.00 Kept

3 Kararlarımı verirken farklı olasılıkları düşünmem 1.30 6.13 3.61 2.64 0.89 -1.32 51.46 50 1.03 Kept

4 Herhangi bir konuyu derinlemesine irdelemem 0.99 5.81 3.54 2.07 0.34 -1.69 60.92 54 1.13 Removed

5 Sonradan üzülmektense önceden tedbirimi almak gerektiğine inanırım 1.50 5.23 4.55 2.76 0.90 -1.43 49.12 43 1.14 Kept

6 Acele etmemeye özen gösteririm 0.57 3.39 1.73 0.35 -0.96 -3.19 64.36 68 0.95 Removed

7 Düşünmeden aceleyle harekete geçerim 1.76 5.25 3.22 1.42 -0.55 -2.93 58.08 52 1.12 Kept

8 Yaptığım işleri kontrol ederim 1.48 5.88 3.36 1.15 -1.88 - 43.32 40 1.08 Kept

9 Durumların eksi ve artı yönlerini tartarım 2.03 6.71 5.75 4.25 1.43 -1.84 32.96 31 1.06 Kept

10 Davranışlarımın sonuçlarını düşünmeden hareket ederim 1.56 5.01 3.41 1.70 0.39 -1.78 49.01 52 0.94 Kept

50

Table 9 6th Facet Assertiveness: IRT Item Parameters, Chi-squares, Chi-square/df ratios, and Decisions to Retain

Label a c1 c2 c3 c4 c5 χ2 df χ2/df Decision

1 Görüşlerimi kendime saklarım 1.21 3.85 1.90 0.25 -1.10 -3.25 23.16 30 0.77 Removed

2 Gerektiğinde kendimi savunurum 1.36 6.27 5.51 3.79 1.84 -0.96 24.62 22 1.12 Kept

3 Bana patronluk taslanmasına ses çıkarmam 0.98 6.47 4.49 2.37 0.75 -0.97 19.68 24 0.82 Removed

4 Eleştirilerimi dile getirmekten çekinmem 3.43 8.18 5.47 3.38 -0.02 -4.25 27.54 19 1.45 Kept

5 Düşündüğümü söylerim 1.81 5.86 3.50 2.46 -0.07 -3.16 19.98 23 0.87 Kept

Table 10 7th Facet Compliance: IRT Item Parameters, Chi-squares, Chi-square/df ratios, and Decisions to Retain

Label a c1 c2 c3 c4 c5 χ2 df χ2/df Decision

1 İnsanlarla zıtlaşmaktan hoşlanmam 1.47 4.48 2.72 1.42 -0.03 -2.36 51.04 44 1.16 Kept

2 Sivri bir dilim vardır 1.31 2.96 1.21 -0.07 -1.17 -2.73 54.66 47 1.16 Kept

3 Diğer insanlarla zıtlaşırım 3.28 6.66 4.08 1.31 -0.95 -4.18 30.43 34 0.89 Kept

4 Bana yapılan haksızlıkları kimsenin yanına bırakmam 0.84 2.08 0.56 -0.63 -2.05 -3.48 54.03 49 1.10 Removed

5 İnsanlara bağırırım 1.24 4.07 2.73 0.97 -0.20 -1.61 50.30 47 1.07 Removed

51

Table 11 8th Facet Order: IRT Item Parameters, Chi-squares, Chi-square/df ratios, and Decisions to Retain

Label a c1 c2 c3 c4 c5 χ2 df χ2/df Decision

1 Düzensizlik beni rahatsız etmez 2.71 5.10 2.63 1.21 -0.14 -2.29 28.93 28 1.03 Kept

2 Dağınık insanlar beni rahatsız etmez 2.96 5.54 2.41 1.00 -0.43 -2.66 21.12 25 0.84 Kept

3 Ortalığı toparlamayı severim 1.84 3.97 2.74 1.64 -0.07 -2.45 35.40 31 1.14 Kept

4 Çoğunlukla eşyaları olması gereken yere geri koymayı unuturum 1.79 4.39 2.39 1.18 -0.15 -2.16 20.81 30 0.69 Kept

Table 12 9th Facet Openness to Ideas: IRT Item Parameters, Chi-squares, Chi-square/df ratios, and Decisions to Retain

Label a c1 c2 c3 c4 c5 χ2 df χ2/df Decision

1 Derin düşüncelere dalmayı severim 0.69 4.63 3.49 1.99 0.72 -0.93 87.12 71 1.23 Removed

2 Beni düşünmeye sevk eden filmleri severim 1.25 6.03 3.90 2.82 1.46 -0.52 60.84 58 1.05 Removed

3 Bilgi dağarcığımı genişletmek isterim 1.13 6.61 4.77 4.13 2.53 -0.08 39.16 42 0.93 Removed

4 Ağır okumalardan uzak dururum 0.97 2.92 1.33 0.16 -0.83 -2.50 117.28 96 1.22 Removed

5 Karmaşık problemler üzerinde çalışmayı severim 1.01 3.78 2.55 1.22 -0.06 -1.98 101.22 85 1.19 Removed

6 Kuramsal tartışmalar ilgimi çekmez 1.36 3.61 1.93 0.88 -0.49 -2.04 89.08 86 1.04 Kept

7 Detayları nadiren fark ederim 1.06 4.45 3.30 1.73 0.48 -1.50 68.56 68 1.01 Removed

8 Beni zorlayan metinleri okumayı severim 1.43 3.37 1.65 0.43 -0.85 -2.95 88.41 85 1.04 Kept

9 Soyut fikirler ilgimi çekmez 2.44 6.32 4.60 2.95 0.92 -1.55 59.86 58 1.03 Kept

10 Zengin bir kelime haznesine sahibimdir 0.81 3.99 2.53 1.12 -0.39 -2.20 80.36 88 0.91 Removed

11 Soyut fikirleri anlamakta zorlanırım 1.48 5.12 3.49 1.80 0.35 -1.62 56.53 71 0.80 Kept

12 Felsefi tartışmalardan kaçınırım 2.32 5.17 3.52 1.73 0.18 -1.95 81.06 67 1.21 Kept

52

Table 13 10th Facet Assertiveness/Exhibition: IRT Item Parameters, Chi-squares, Chi-square/df ratios, and Decisions to Retain

Label a c1 c2 c3 c4 c5 χ2 df χ2/df Decision

1 Diğer insanları bir işi yapmaya ikna edebilirim 1.61 5.61 3.99 2.36 0.14 -3.12 42.18 44 0.96 Kept

2 Doğaçlama konuşmalar yapmakta iyiyimdir 1.85 3.97 2.57 0.95 -0.50 -2.79 89.10 60 1.48 Kept

3 Diğer insanları idare etmek bana zor gelir 1.23 3.76 1.70 0.22 -0.96 -3.26 65.28 64 1.02 Removed

4 Diğer insanların öncülük etmesini beklerim 1.62 5.27 2.80 0.80 -0.96 -3.04 69.20 51 1.36 Kept

5 Dikkati üzerime çekmeyi sevmem 1.15 2.58 0.89 -0.38 -1.61 -3.01 69.17 75 0.92 Removed

6 Kendimi kolayca öne çıkartabilirim 1.89 4.20 2.84 0.72 -1.00 -3.61 46.34 58 0.80 Kept

7 Kendimi rahatça ifade edebilirim 1.84 5.80 3.95 2.37 0.32 -2.79 59.95 47 1.28 Kept

8 Kararların alınmasını başkalarına bırakırım 1.49 6.03 3.37 1.36 -0.25 -2.35 46.88 54 0.87 Kept

53

Table 14 11th Facet Dutifulness: IRT Item Parameters, Chi-squares, Chi-square/df ratios, and Decisions to Retain

Label a c1 c2 c3 c4 c5 χ2 df χ2/df Decision

1 Benim olmayan eşyaları asla almam 0.73 4.06 2.61 1.64 0.77 -0.86 64.42 60 1.07 Removed

2 Öne geçmek için hile yapabilirim 2.45 6.94 5.30 3.20 1.32 -0.85 47.38 36 1.32 Kept

3 Başımı beladan kurtarmak için yalan söylerim 1.96 4.49 2.67 1.00 -0.80 -3.12 35.26 49 0.72 Kept

4 Güvenin temelinin dürüstlük olduğuna inanırım 0.95 6.47 4.64 3.58 2.14 -0.08 28.65 35 0.82 Removed

5 Yalan söylemeyi veya hile yapmayı düşünemem bile 1.69 4.02 2.07 0.99 -0.13 -2.27 67.34 58 1.16 Kept

6 Kendi işlerimi başkalarına yaptırmaya çalışırım 1.27 5.44 4.30 2.68 0.82 -0.86 58.78 43 1.37 Removed

7 Gerektiğinde gerçekleri saptırırım 1.97 6.08 3.87 1.90 0.37 -1.93 43.95 44 1.00 Kept

8 Kuralların etrafından nasıl dolanacağımı bilirim 0.66 3.66 1.35 -0.33 -1.54 -3.35 76.28 51 1.50 Removed

54

Table 15 12th Facet Trust: IRT Item Parameters, Chi-squares, Chi-square/df ratios, and Decisions to Retain

Label a c1 c2 c3 c4 c5 χ2 df χ2/df Decision

1 Başkalarının gizli niyetleri olduğundan şüphelenirim 1.22 3.01 1.04 -0.56 -1.77 -3.42 47.73 56 0.85 Removed

2 Diğer insanların niyetlerinin iyi olduğuna inanıyorum 4.09 7.97 4.79 1.06 -2.70 -7.73 26.23 30 0.87 Kept

3 İnsanların söylediklerine güvenirim 2.75 6.32 3.70 1.64 -1.09 -5.78 36.30 32 1.13 Kept

4 İnsanların temelde dürüst olduğuna inanıyorum 3.07 5.09 2.97 0.62 -1.99 -5.56 48.65 40 1.22 Kept

5 Her şeyin iyi neticeleneceğini düşünüyorum 0.80 3.64 1.78 0.75 -0.90 -2.82 54.56 59 0.92 Removed

6 Kin tutarım 0.73 3.03 1.54 0.44 -0.34 -1.42 86.52 68 1.27 Removed

55

CHAPTER 4

STUDY 2 METHOD and RESULTS

Study 2 was conducted to delineate the criterion-related validity pattern between

personality characteristics and task performance. In addition, the moderating effects

of situational strength on the job performance and personality relationship were

analyzed. In this study data were gathered from the faculties and research assistants

(RAs) which are in a work relationship with these faculties regularly or for the last two

academic semesters. Details regarding the participants and measures are as follows.

4.1 Participants and Procedure

Data were collected from research assistants and faculties of METU, Ankara,

Hacettepe, Atılım, İstanbul Technical University (İTÜ), Ege, Anadolu, Osmangazi,

and Namık Kemal University. All of the RAs were given monetary incentive in order

for them to participate in the study. Faculties that evaluated the job performance of

RAs were not given any incentive to participate. During the initial contact, all RAs

were made aware that they were going to participate in a lottery that gave them a

chance to win a tablet computer. But as data collection continued, this incentive was

proven to be ineffective. So, in addition to a chance to participate in a lottery,

participants (i.e. RAs) were given monetary incentive of 40 TL.

Mean age of RAs that participated in the study was 27.83. On average they are on their

4.49 year of their graduate study. Their departments spanned psychology (21%),

mechanical (9%), electrical and electronical engineering (8%), biosystems engineering

(8%), industrial engineering (5%), computer engineering (5%), metallurgical and

materials engineering (5%), and field crops engineering (5%). Faculty members were

asked about their departments and the period of time they worked with the RA. Mean

of the semesters worked together was 4.75, and median semester of working together

was 4.

56

4.2 Measures

In study 2, RAs’ personality data were collected with the use of the compound

personality measure developed in study 1 and also with the BFI personality inventory.

In addition, RAs filled the Situational Strength at Work (SSW) scale to evaluate their

work environment’s situational strength characteristics. Also, job performance data of

RAs were collected from their immediate supervisors by using the Assistant

Evaluation Form (AEF). Detailed information of these scales can be found below.

4.2.1 Situational Strength at Work Scale

The Situational Strength at Work (SSW) scale developed by Meyer et al., (2014) is

composed of four sub-scales, which are clarity, consistency, constraints, and

consequences. Items are rated on a 6-point Likert-type scale, with one referring to

‘does not define my job at all’ and six referring to ‘defines my job completely’. In all

of the items, higher values indicate stronger situations. An example item from clarity

sub-dimension is “Necessary detailed information is provided about how to do work.”

(Bir çalışanın işini düzgün bir şekilde nasıl yapabileceğine dair gereken detaylı bilgi

sağlanır). The Turkish version of the scale, which was translated by the authors and

backtranslated and corrected by bilingual speakers, can be seen in Appendix E.

Meyer et al. (2014) reported Cronbach’s alpha values for clarity, consistency,

constraints, and consequences as .95, .90, .89, and .86, respectively. In this study

Cronbach’s alpha reliability values are .91 for clarity, .85 for consistency, .87 for

constraints, and .78 for consequences (see Table 15).

Table 16 Descriptive Statistics of the Situational Strength Measure

Dimension Mean SD Min Max Skewness # of Items α

Clarity 4.19 0.98 1.60 6.00 -0.50 5.00 0.91

Consistency 3.98 0.96 1.14 5.14 -0.29 5.00 0.85

Constraints 3.34 0.94 1.43 5.14 0.00 7.00 0.87

Consequences 3.42 0.83 1.20 6.00 0.00 7.00 0.78

Note. N = 61.

57

4.2.2 AEF

In order to measure job performance, AEF (Oz, 2003) was used. This scale was

specifically developed for an immediate supervisor (a faculty member) to rate the job

performance of teaching assistants (TA). In the original AEF form observers provide

ratings of performance on a 5-point Likert-type scale. In this study a 6-point Likert-

type scale was used ranging from ‘does not describe at all’ to ‘describes completely’.

There are two dimensions within this scale which are parallel with the job performance

literature. These dimensions are contextual and task performance. Sixteen of the 25

items in the AEF are measuring task performance and the remaining nine items are

measuring contextual performance. Task performance are composed of questions such

as ‘Is not prepared for the tasks as she/he leaves it to the last-minute’ (Son ana bıraktığı

için, kendisinden istenilen hazırlığı tam olarak yapamamak) and ‘Investigates different

sources while carrying out an assignment’ (İşini çeşitli kaynaklardan araştırmalar

yaparak yapmak). Contextual performance marker questions are ‘Volunteers for extra

duties’ (Extra görevler için gönüllü olmak) and ‘Helps a coworker who is short in time

although it is not her/his duty’ (Kendi işi olmadığı halde, çok yoğun olduğu için sıkışan

bir çalışma arkadaşına işlerini yapmasında yardımcı olmak). Oz (2003) reported

reliability coefficients for the task and contextual performance dimensions as .73

and .74, respectively. In this study, observed reliability coefficients of AEF for task,

and contextual were .91, and .67, respectively. Mean, minimum and maximum values,

skewness statistics of the AEF can be found in Table 16.

58

4.3 Results

Results of Study 2 are presented within three sections. In the first section

intercorrelations among the personality facets and BFI factors are given and significant

patterns highlighted. In the following section, results regarding the compound

personality scale and job performance are presented based on both CTT and IRT

approaches. In the final part of the results, analyses regarding the moderating effects

of the situational strength construct are presented.

Data were checked for univariate outliers and no trace of any univariate outlier was

detected. Besides that, missing values within the data set were also examined. In none

of the variables, missing values exceeded 3% of the respective variable. Considering

this low level of missing values, mean replacement was employed.

Means, standard deviations, skewness and reliability values of the scales used in Study

2 can be seen in Table 16. Personality variables were normally distributed with two

exceptions. Deliberation facet had a skewness value higher than one in both CTT and

IRT composite measures, and assertiveness had a skewness value higher than one in

the IRT composite scale. Severe range restriction was observed with the performance

variables as can be seen from Table 16. Even though their skewness values did not

indicate any diversion from normality, minimum values on a 6-point scale for task and

contextual performances were 3.56 and 3.44, respectively. Histograms revealed these

variables to be normally distributed within their range of responses.

Reliability values of the scales were mostly higher than .70. BFI factors’ Cronbach’s

alpha values ranged from .73 to .87. In terms of facet scales that were formed with

CTT methods, achievement striving and assertiveness had the lowest two values, .63

and .66, respectively. Remaning scales’ reliability values ranged from .72 to .96. In

terms of the facet scales that were formed with the use of IRT methods reliability

values ranged from .69 to .95. Altruism facet had the lowest and self-discipline had

the highest reliability values (see Table 16).

59

4.3.1 Interrelations of Compound Scale and BFI Factors

In Table 17 facets that were formed after the CTT analyses were placed on the vertical

axis (i.e. facets listed from top to bottom) and facets that were created by using IRT

methods were placed on the horizontal axis (i.e. from left to right).

Considering the CTT based facets developed for the compound scale, the

conscientiousness facets of self-discipline (r = .86, p < .01), achievement striving (r

= .27, p < .05), deliberation (r =.49, p < .01), and order (r = .51, p < .01) were most

highly correlated with the BFI conscientiousness factor. Highest correlation of

dutifulness was with agreeableness (r = .45, p < .01) but it was also correlated highly

with conscientiousness (r = .41, p < .01). In terms of the IRT based facets, self-

discipline, deliberation, order, and dutifulness had the highest significant correlations

with conscientiousness, and achievement striving both with conscientiousness and

agreeableness.

In the CTT-based facets, the agreeableness facets of altruism, compliance, and trust

were most highly correlated with the BFI agreeableness factor (rs ranged from .57

to .68). Notably, altruism also correlated with conscientiousness as highly as with

agreeableness (both r = .57, p < .01). In terms of IRT facets, altruism correlated with

conscientiousness and agreeableness (rs .54 and .39, respectively). Compliance and

trust had the highest correlations with agreeableness (rs .61 and .65, respectively).

When extraversion facets were taken into consideration, excitement seeking, which

also included items related to openness to actions, most highly correlated with

openness to experience and extraversion (rs .60 and .50, respectively). Assertiveness

was negatively correlated with agreeableness (r = -.30, p < .05) and positively

correlated with extraversion (r = .28, p < .05). Assertiveness/exhibition was highly

correlated with extraversion (r = .59, p < .01) and had an inverse association with

agreeablesness (r = -.23, ns) just like assertiveness. When these facets were examined

in IRT based compound scale, excitement seeking was significantly correlated with

openness to experience and extraversion, by .68 and .53, respectively. Assertiveness

60

and assertiveness/exhibition had the highest correlations with extraversion (rs .40

and .61, respectively), as expected.

Openness to ideas facet was most highly correlated with the openness to experience

factor in both the CTT based (r = .67, p < .01) and IRT based scales (r = .63, p < .01).

Correlations between IRT based facets and their respective CTT based facets, given in

the lower right section of Table 17, were high as expected. Only the correlations of

altruism (r = .87, p < .01) and achievement striving (r = .70, p < .01) were below .90.

61

Table 17 Descriptive Statistics for the CTT and IRT Based Facets, BFI factors, and Performance Dimensions

CTT Based Statistics IRT Based Statistics

Facet Name Mean SD Min. Max. Skewnes

s # of Items α

Mea

n SD Min Max Skewness # of Items α

Co

mp

ou

nd

Sca

le

Self-discipline 4.26 .71 2.74 5.82 -.09 34 .96 4.35 0.73 2.79 5.89 -.15 28 .95

Excitement seeking 4.25 .60 2.92 5.33 -.34 12 .75 4.31 0.66 2.86 5.71 -.24 7 .74

Altruism 4.83 .46 3.77 5.85 -.05 13 .74 5.01 0.53 3.86 6.00 -.17 7 .69

Achievement striving 4.43 .59 3.14 6.00 .29 7 .63 4.70 0.79 2.00 6.00 -.71 2 .80

Deliberation 4.81 .68 2.22 6.00 -1.11 10 .79 4.82 0.69 2.13 6.00 -1.20 8 .86

Assertiveness 4.20 1.15 2.00 5.60 -.22 5 .66 4.55 0.66 2.00 5.67 -1.13 3 .72

Compliance 4.45 .63 2.00 5.80 -.13 5 .76 4.40 1.05 2.00 6.00 -.53 3 .73

Order 4.85 .65 1.75 6.00 -.99 4 .79 4.20 1.15 1.75 6.00 -.22 4 .79

Openness to ideas 4.05 .90 3.08 5.58 -.72 12 .79 4.18 0.95 2.00 5.80 -.12 5 .77

Assertiveness/Exhibition 4.99 .54 2.00 5.50 -.34 8 .72 4.36 0.64 2.33 5.50 -.65 6 .73

Dutifulness 5.08 .63 2.63 5.88 -.64 8 .77 4.92 0.92 2.00 6.00 -.94 4 .81

Trust 4.84 .58 1.60 5.40 -.05 5 .84 4.17 0.96 1.33 5.67 -.85 3 .87

Compound Composite 3.83 .30 3.13 4.50 -.30 88 3.77 .42 2.98 4.81 -.10 59

BF

I

Agreeableness 4.57 .61 3.00 5.56 -.58 9 .73

Conscientiousness 4.37 .70 2.78 5.89 -.09 9 .81

Extraversion 4.02 .86 1.75 5.50 -.34 8 .87

Neuroticism 3.20 .82 1.25 5.25 .24 8 .80

Openness to Experience 4.38 .63 2.80 5.60 -.14 10 .77

BFI Composite 3.63 .36 2.70 4.37 -.42 36

Per

for-

ma

nce

Task Performance 5.08 .63 3.56 6.00 -.64 16 .91

Contextual Performance 4.84 .58 3.44 6.00 -.05 9 .67

Note. SD = Standard deviation. α = Cronbach’s alpha. Standard error of skewness is .306. In the compound composite the facets of excitement seeking, openness to ideas, and

assertiveness/exhibition were reverse scored due to their inverse association with performance. The facets of assertiveness, altruism, order and trust were not included. In the composite

BFI score the factors of extraversion and openness were reverse scored. Neuroticism was not included to BFI composite score.

62

Table 18 Correlations between the BFI Factors and Compound Scale Facets

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17

1 Agreeableness .41** .02 .39** .44** .36** -.19 .61** .43** .12 -.20 .42** .65**

2 Conscientiousness .44** .86** .21 .54** .34** .45** .22 .20 .51** .20 .24 .45** .21

3 Extraversion .05 .30* .33** .53** .07 .01 .11 .40** -.23 .12 .11 .61** .11 -.01

4 Neuroticism -.42** -.32* -.01 -.38** -.08 -.16 .08 -.45** -.11 -.42** -.14 -.16 -.09 -.32* -.18

5 Opennesss to

Experience .08 .20 .37** -.07 .28* .68** .28* -.10 .05 .17 -.24 -.01 .63** .39** .16 -.06

6 Self-discipline .40** .86** .36** -.36** .29* 1**

7 Excitement Seeking -.01 .13 .50** -.09 .60** .23 .94**

8 Altruism .57** .57** .17 -.28* .27* .53** .16 .87**

9 Achievement Striving 0.21 .27* .08 .17 .11 .24 -.23 .14 .70**

10 Deliberation .34** .49** .12 -.43** .08 .39** -.09 .40** .11 .99**

11 Assertiveness -.30* .12 .28* -.13 .16 .28* .36** .03 .02 .44** .85**

12 Compliance .66** .15 -.25* -.41** -.22 .11 -.19 .26* .08 .34** -.35** .95**

13 Order .43** .51** .12 -.14 -.01 .42** .03 .32* .20 .09 -.04 .26* 1**

14 Openness to Ideas .09 .22 .20 -.12 .67** .23 .39** .28* -.13 .30* .30* -.19 .04 .93**

15 Assertiveness/Exhibitio

n -.23 .21 .59** -.05 .33** .36** .41** -.00 .18 .11 .46** -.42** -.05 .41** .95**

16 Dutifulness .45** .41** .07 -0.23 .16 .43** .11 .47** .07 .54** 0.22 .33** .26* .20 -.01 .94**

17 Trust .68** .18 -.08 -.30* -.05 .19 -.10 .28* .41** .05 -.41** .50** .20 -.07 -.20 .15 .94**

Note. ** p < .01, * p < .05. Facets that were formed after IRT analyses were presented in the horizontal axis and facets that were the result of CTT analyses were put on the vertical

axis. Figures that was presented in the lower right corner of the table are the correlations between CTT and IRT based compound scales.

63

4.3.2 Compound Personality Scale and Job Performance Relationship

Correlations between task performance, compound scale facets and the BFI factors can

be seen in Table 18. In this sample of 61 participants, the only facets that were

statistically significantly correlated with task performance were the CTT-based

excitement seeking scale (r = -.26, p < .05) and both the CTT and IRT-based openness

to ideas scales (r = -.26 and r = -.32, p < .05). Even though the relationship between

excitement seeking and task performance was in the expected direction as indicated

by the literature, openness to ideas facet’s correlation with task performance was not.

Besides these associations, there were no significant correlations between any of the

compound facets and task performance.

In order to create CTT based and IRT based compound scales, facets that were

correlated with task performance higher than .10 were taken. The deliberation facet’s

correlation with task performance was reduced to .09 after IRT-based elimination,

nonetheless it was still included in the compound scale based on rational grounds.

Given this selection criterion, facets of assertiveness, altruism, trust, order, and

dutifulness were taken out of the compound scale. Of these, altruism did not have any

empirical support in the literature as an associate of task performance. Order and trust

were not replicated across studies. Present study results further justified the exclusion

of these scales from the compound scale. Even though assertiveness and dutifulness

were found to be related to task performance, both CTT and IRT-based scales did not

produce consistent results with each other thus they were also omitted. In addition to

that, facets of excitement seeking, openness to ideas, and assertiveness/exhibition were

reverse coded as they inversely associated with task performance (see Table 18).

To create a BFI composite score, factor scores were aggregated and averaged. As

neuroticism had a nill association with task performance, and also as the compound

scale that was formed with the use of facets did not contain any neuroticism facets, the

neuroticism factor scores were not included in the BFI composite score calculation.

Factors of extraversion and openness to experience were reverse coded as they are

negatively related to task performance.

64

The first hypothesis of the study was that the compound personality scale developed

would perform better in predicting task performance than the broad FFM factors. In

order to test this hypothesis, CTT-based and IRT-based compound scale correlations

with task performance was compared to that of the BFI composite.

Table 19 Compund Facet and BFI Correlations with Task and Contextual

Performance

Task Performance Contextual Performance

CTT based IRT based CTT based IRT based

Self-discipline .21 .21 .03 .02

Excitement seeking -.26* -.23 -.01 .06

Altruism .04 .00 .03 .08

Achievement striving .15 .22 .11 -.02

Deliberation .10 .09 -.08 -.11

Assertiveness -.08 .02 -.05 -.02

Compliance .13 .13 -.07 -.09

Order .02 .02 -.06 -.06

Openness to ideas -.26* -.32* -.05 -.06

Assertiveness/Exhibition -.15 -.12 .10 .14

Dutifulness .06 .08 -.08 -.02

Trust .00 .04 -.19 -.14

Compound Total .36** .36** - -

BFI Conscientiousness .28* - .04 -

BFI Agreeableness -.04 - -.19 -

BFI Extraversion -.09 - .00 -

BFI Neuroticsm .00 - .05 -

BFI Openness -.19 - .07 -

BFI Composite .25 - - -

Note. ** p < .01, * p < .05. N = 61. CTT = Classical Test Theory, IRT = Item Response Theory. In the compound

composite the facets of excitement seeking, openness to ideas, and assertiveness/exhibition were reverse scored

due to their inverse associations with performance. The compound composite was formed with the facets that had

correlations higher than .10 with task performance, excluding the facets of assertiveness, altruism, order,

dutifulness, and trust. In the BFI composite the factors of extraversion and openness were reverse scored.

Neuroticism was not included.

Results of the correlation analysis showed that the compound scale that was created

using facets had a significant moderate correlation with task performance (r = .36, p

< .01). The magnitude of association was the same for both the CTT and IRT-based

compound scales. The correlation of the BFI composite with task performance was

less than moderate and not significant (r = .25, ns). In addition to that, the correlations

between task performance and compound scale composites were higher than the

65

correlation between task performance and the BFI conscientiousness factor (r = .28, p

< .05) (see Table 18). These findings indicated that the first hypothesis of the study

was supported.

In order to test the incremental validity gains that can be acquired by the use of the

compound scale on top of the BFI composite, a hierarchical regression analysis was

conducted. In this analysis, the BFI composite was entered into the regression analysis

in the first step. In the second step the IRT-based compound scale score was entered

into the analysis (see Table 19). The model with the BFI composite (β = .25, p = .05)

in the first step was marginally significant (F(1, 59) = 3.92, p = .05) and explained 6%

of variance in the task performance criterion. The inclusion of the compound scale

score in the second step explained around 7% incremental variance over the BFI

composite (ΔR2 = .065, ΔF(1, 58) = 4.33, p = .04). In this model, the BFI composite

score was not significant (β = -.02, ns) whereas the compound composite score was

significant (β = .37, p < .05). This analysis was conducted to reach a resolution

regarding the second hypothesis of the study, which posited that the compound

personality scale will add incremental variance over the broad Big Five factors. The

second hypothesis of the study was supported.

Even though the compound personality scale was not developed with a focus of

predicting contextual performance, this dimension was also included in the study for

exploratory purposes. Except for achievement striving (r = .11) and trust (r = -.19), no

other facet had an association that exceed .10, thus no composite was formed to

analyze associations with contextual performance.

66

Table 20 Hierarchical Regression Analysis Results

Model R2 ΔR2 ΔF(df) p β t p

Step 1 .062 .062 3.92(1, 59) .052

BFI Composite .25 1.98 .052

Step 2 .127 .065 4.33(1, 58) .042

BFI Composite -.02 -0.13 .90

Compound Composite .37 2.08 .04

Note. Compound composite based on the scale with 59 items refined after the IRT analysis. The BFI composite is

based on the four factors except for Neuroticism, with 36 items.

4.4 Moderating Effects of Situational Strength

The third hypothesis of this study was regarding situational strength’s moderation

effect. It was stated that the relationship between the compound personality scale and

task performance would be higher in weaker situations compared to stronger

situations. In order to test this hypothesis a series of moderated regression analyses

were conducted. In these analyses centered scores of the respective situational strength

scale (i.e. clarity, consistency, constraints, and consequences) and IRT-based

compound personality scale were entered in the first step of the regression analysis. In

the second step, an interaction term that was the product of the aforementioned

variables was entered into the regression.

In all of the regression analyses, the models were significant at both steps (see Table

20) being driven by the effect of the IRT based compound personality scale which was

significant (see Table 21). Interaction terms did not add any significant incremental

variance in any of the regressions with the four situational strength moderator factors

(Table 21). Thus, based on statistical grounds with a sample of 61 individuals, the third

hypothesis of this study was not supported.

67

Table 21 Situational Strength Hierarchical Regression ANOVA Results

Moderator Model SS df MS F p

Clarity 1 3.56 2 1.78 5.16 .01

2 3.97 3 1.32 3.85 .01

Consistency 1 3.00 2 1.50 4.23 .02

2 3.37 3 1.12 3.17 .03

Constraints 1 3.11 2 1.55 4.42 .02

2 3.12 3 1.04 2.90 .04

Consequence 1 3.60 2 1.80 5.23 .01

2 3.60 3 1.20 3.43 .02

Note. SS = Sum of squared errors, df = degrees of freedom, MS = Mean squared errors.

Table 22 Situational Strength Hierarchical Regression Second Step Coefficients

Clarity

Variable β t p

Constant 64.41 .00

Clarity Centered -.16 -1.27 .21

IRT Composite Centered .40 3.19 .00

Interaction Term .13

ΔR2 .02

ΔF(df) 1.20

p .28

Consistency

Variable β t p

Constant 58.47 .00

Consistency Centered .05 0.33 .74

IRT Composite Centered .39 2.75 .01

Interaction Term .15 1.02 .31

ΔR2 .02

ΔF(df) 1.05

p .31

Constraints

Variable β t p

Constant 64.433 .00

Constraints Centered .07 0.510 .61

IRT Composite Centered .37 2.866 .01

Interaction Term .02 0.140 .89

ΔR2 .00

ΔF(df) .02

p .89

Consequences

Variable β t p

Constant 65.18 .00

Consequences Centered -.17 -1.31 .20

IRT Composite Centered .40 3.14 .00

Interaction Term .01 0.10 .92

ΔR2 .00

ΔF(df) .01

p .92

Note. β = Standardized regression coefficients, ΔR2 = R square change, ΔF(df) = F change.

68

Nonetheless, to see the pattern of the correlations on different levels of clarity,

consistency, constraints, and consequences a series of correlation analyses were

conducted. Participants were divided into three groups by using the 33rd and 66th

percentiles in order to roughly equate group sizes. Exact group sizes of each group,

correlations between job performance and the personality composite and significance

levels can be seen in Table 22.

Even though the interaction terms were not significant with a sample of 61, noteworthy

correlational patterns emerged across the low-, medium-, and high-strength groups for

the factors of clarity, consistency, and consequences. For consequences, the highest

personality-performance correlation was observed in the low-strength group (r = .48,

p < .05); correlations were relatively smaller for the medium- (r = .38, ns) and high-

situational strength (r = .32, ns) groups, as expected. As the job context was perceived

to have potentially more critical consequences (stronger situation) personality was less

associated with performance. For clarity, consistency, and constraints contrary to

expectations, the highest personality-performance correlations were observed for the

high-situational strength group (r = .58, p < .01; r = .40, p < .05; r = .43, p < .05,

respectively). For clarity, correlations got smaller as the situation’s strength decreased;

with a moderate correlation for the medium-strength group (r = .38, ns) and a small

correlation in the low-strength group (r = .13, ns). Contrary to expectations, as the

situation got stronger, personality was more associated with performance ratings. In

general as situational strength increased, the correlations between task performance

and personality reached significance. The only exception to this rule was that of the

pattern observed for the consequences dimension. These contrary findings for the most

part didn’t support the third hypothesis of the study.

69

Table 23 Job Performance and IRT Based Scale Correlation at Different Situational

Strength Levels

Clarity Consistency

Group N r p Group N r p

Low 21 .13 .57 Low 20 .35 .13

Medium 17 .38 .14 Medium 16 .36 .17

High 23 .58** .004 High 25 .40* .045

Constraints Consequences

Group N r p Group N r p

Low 22 .35 .11 Low 20 .48* .03

Medium 16 .23 .40 Medium 22 .38 .09

High 23 .43* .04 High 19 .32 .18

Note. N = Number of participants in the group.

4.5 The use of IRT Methodologies

In this study IRT models were used to obtain item discrimination parameters. These

parameters were used to select items that are more discriminating in terms of the trait

of interest. As a research question, criterion related predictive power of the facet scales

that was developed with the use of item discrimination parameters compared with the

facet scales that was developed with CTT-based facet scales. As can be seen from the

Table 19, the correlations of both CTT-based and IRT-based scales with task

performance were the same (rs = .36, p < .05). This indicated that the predictive

validity of the scales was not influenced by the method employed in their development.

Though, the length of the scale that was developed with the use of IRT methods on top

of CTT methods is shorter than the one that was developed with the use of CTT

methods.

70

CHAPTER 5

DISCUSSION

The main purpose of this study was to develop a compound personality scale named

as the work discipline scale, by combining the criterion-related FFM facets, and

investigating its predictive validity with task performance as the focal outcome. While

doing so, the study took a rational-empirical approach. Personality facets relevant to

performance were first identified based on the literature. Focal performance outcomes

included operationalizations that would be common to most jobs. These were task

performance and factors that would lead to better task performance such as

demonstrating effort, maintaining personal discipline, taking initiative, job dedication,

work engagement, engaging in self-development, motivation, procrastinating, and

adaptability. Many of these dimensions were based on the model of job performance

by Johnson (2003) others (e.g. procrastination) were included by the researchers.

These are consistent with the dimensions of performance common to all jobs (job

specific task proficiency, maintaining personal discipline, and demonstrating effort) as

proposed by the model of Campbell, McCloy, Oppler, and Sager (1993). However,

predicting contextual performance or counterproductive work performance was not

included in the focus of the present study. Similarly, job or position specific factors

such as managerial performance, teamwork performance, creativity were not a focus

of the literature search.

Facets relevant to the aforementioned task and related performance dimensions were

put together by identifying their corresponding constructs in the IPIP. Identification of

the constructs to be utilized constituted the rational approach of developing the

compound scale. As part of the empirical approach, both CTT and IRT methodologies

were utilized for scale refinement at the item level, using a large university student

sample. Moreover, correlations between compound scale facets and task performance

ratings of employed graduate assistants at universities were used to further refine the

compound scale to obtain the best predictive power.

71

Furthermore, in the present study, the strength of the situation as defined by clarity,

consistency, constraint, and consequences were taken into account when analyzing the

work discipline compound scale and task performance relationship as personality-

performance associations have been shown to be moderated by situational strength

(Beaty, Cleveland, & Murphy, 2001; Meyer et al., 2009; Meyer et al., 2014). Results

are discussed regarding the use of facets, IRT methodology, and the hypothesized

moderating role of situational strength. A discussion on the practical contributions and

limitations of the study, and and future research suggestions follows.

5.1 The Use of Personality Facets

Several empirical studies suggested the use of compound personality scales in

predicting various performance criteria, such as task performance, CWB, managerial

performance, and team work (e.g., Fine & Klein, 2011; Oh et al., 2008; Ones &

Viswesvaran, 2001; Ones et al., 1993). As emphasized by Ashton, Jackson, Paunonen,

Helmes, and Rothstein (1995) theoretical benefits that are provided by the FFM may

not provide us with the specificity that is needed to predict outcomes in applied

settings. By just focusing on factor level personality constructs of the FFM and

overlooking facets may lead us to miss important real world relationships. As

empirical studies in the literature indicated that facet-preformance level associations

were driving personality performance relationships (De Vries et al., 2011; Dudley et

al., 2006; Hurt & Donovan, 2000; Mussel et al., 2011; Moon et al., 2008) facets were

combined to form the compound scale and to study the scale’s relationship with

performance in this study.

It was hypothesized that the compound personality scale would perform better in

predicting task performance than the broad FFM factors. Correlation analyses were

conducted to compare the predictive validity of the compound scale that was

developed by the use of facets with that developed by the use of factors. While the

compound scale that was formed by FFM factors had a non-significant correlation with

task performance in a sample of 61 participants, the work discipline compound scale

that was developed by the use of facets had a moderate significant correlation with

72

task performance. This finding is consistent with the other empirical findings in the

literature indicating higher performance associations for facets or facet compounds

(e.g., Fein & Klein, 2011; Oh et al., 2008).

Furthermore, it was hypothesized that the compound personality scale will add

incremental variance over the broad FFM factor composite in the prediction of task

performance. Results of hierarchical regression analysis showed that the compound

scale did really add incremental variance on top of the 4-factor BFI compound score.

The variance explained by the BFI compound score was 6% while the incremental

variance that was explained by the compound scale score was 7%. This finding

contributes to the literature indicating that facet-level information adds specificity and

information above broad factors (e.g., De Vries et al., 2011; Dudley et al., 2006;

Mussel et al., 2011). These findings supported the first two hypotheses.

As indicated by Judge et al. (2013) even though there were meta-analyses on the

subject of personality-job performance relationship, studies on which facets were the

best predictors of personality were scattered and not as numerous as studies that

included factor-performance associations. Judge et al. provided a meta-analysis on

such facet-performance associations based on the NEO facets. The most important

contritubion of the present study is that when it comes to prediction of task

performance, facets can be an important tool, more so than factors themselves or factor

composites. Not all reserachers may have access to the NEO-PI-R, nonetheless the

present study offers the potential usefulness of utilizing relevant IPIP facets.

According to the results of the present study, in predicting task performance,

conscientiousness facets of self-discipline, achievement striving, and deliberation;

extraversion facets of excitement seeking and assertiveness/exhibition; the

agreeableness facet of compliance; and the openness to experience facet of openness

to ideas were empirically useful. Two of these facets, however, yielded associations

with task performance that were counter expectations, which are discussed next.

73

5.2 The Nature of Facet-Performance Associations in the Present Study

Results of the study indicated that facets of self-discipline, excitement seeking,

achievement striving, deliberation, compliance, order, and dutifulness were related to

the task performance in the expected direction, even though, with the exeption of the

facet of excitement seeking, these correlations did not reach significance in a sample

of 61 participants. In addition, openness to ideas and assertiveness/exhibition facets’

correlations with task performance were negative and the correlation of openness to

ideas with task performance was found to be significant.

As self-discipline, achievement striving, deliberation, and dutifulness are all under the

factor of conscientiousness, their positive correlations with task performance were in

the expected direction (e.g., Judge et al., 2013; Dudley et al., 2006). As a facet of

agreeableness factor, positive correlation between compliance and task performance

was also in the expected direction (e.g., Judge et al., 2013).

The results of the study also indicated a negative and significant relationship between

openness to ideas and task performance. This correlation can be considered as the most

unexpected one, especially for a study that is studying research assistants and task

performance in the university environment. In addition, the factor of openness to

experience was generally found to be least predictive of the Big Five factors (Griffin

& Hesketh, 2004). Though openness to ideas was found to be positively correlated

with both task and overall performance (Judge et al., 2013; Blake, Potter, & Slimak,

1993; Chen, Gully, Whiteman, & Kilcullen, 2000). Moderate negative correlation that

was found is unexpected considering this fact.

Compliance facet’s relationship with task performance is also higher than expected.

Assertiveness/Exhibition dimension’s correlation with task performance was among

the highest ones, but negative just like openness to ideas, even though it was not

significant. These pattern of correlations can be interpreted in the light of the power

distance and uncertainty avoidance dimensions of Hofstede (1980a). He developed a

framework that has four dimensions and was based on the data that was collected from

all 40 countries in the 1960s and 1970s. These dimensions are individualism-

74

collectivism, power distance, uncertainity avoidance, and masculinity-femininity.

Countries were placed along these dimensions to describe their cultural value. These

dimensions have been widely accepted since their introduction. Their influence on the

reseach and understanding of culture is hard to overestimate (Taras, Kirkman, & Steel,

2010).

Power distance dimension is basically related to the distribution of power,

subordination, and independence in interpersonal relationships. It was formally

defined as “the extent to which a society accepts the fact that power in institutions and

organizations is distributed unequally” (Hofstede, 1980b, p. 45). In a practical and

work related sense, it is related to the extent to which a subordinates’ expression of

disagreement with her/his supervisor is expected. Uncertainty avoidance as defined in

the Hofstede’s cultural value dimensions is related to “the extent to which a society

feels threatened by uncertain and ambiguous situations and tries to avoid these

situations by providing greater career stability, establishing more formal rules, not

tolerating deviant ideas and behaviors, and believing in absolute truths and the

attainment of expertise” (Hofstede, 1980b, p. 45).

In the dimensions of power distance and uncertainty avoidance Turkey was found to

be standing in the high levels (Daller & Yıldız, 2006; Hofstede, 1980a). In our culture,

a behavioral style that has higher compliance, lower assertiveness/exhibition and

openness to ideas may be viewed as the appropriate way of behaving according to the

superiors that have completed the performance evaluation forms. As emphasized in

the prior paragraph, Hofstede’s power distance dimension is specifically related to the

expession of disagreeament, and in a culture where power distance is high, expression

of disagreeament is not the expected and, probably, accepted way of behaving. This

may lead to the conclusion that conformance is valued more and thus more related to

positive evaluations regarding task performance.

In cultures that are high in uncertainty avoidance, as stated in the definition, there is

not much tolerance to ideas that are deviating from the norms. When an environment

that is characterized by high power distance and high uncertainty avoidance is

75

considered, it is not too unconcievable that a subordinate (i.e. research assistant) who

has higher conformity, and also lower assertiveness/exhibition and openness to ideas

receives higher performance evaluations from a superordinate (i.e. faculties). The

pattern of relationships among conformity, openness to ideas,

assertiveneess/exhibition, and task performance may have emerged as the result of a

cultural environment that has high power distance and high uncertainty avoidance.

Interrelations across these facets are also in line with these patterns of relationships.

Assertiveness/exhibition was found to be significantly and positively correlated with

openness to ideas (r = .41, p < .01), and negatively with compliance (r = -.42, p < .01).

And openness to ideas had a nonsignificant correlation with compliance (r = -19, ns).

Higher compliance seems to be related to lower openness to ideas and

assertiveness/exhibition.

5.3 Using IRT Methods in Personality Scale Refinement

As emphasized by the personality researchers (Chiesi, Galli, Primi, Borgi, & Bonacchi,

2013) the use of IRT models in studying personality is behind that in studying ability,

proficiency, and achievement measurement. The research that was carried out in this

study is also an attempt to encourage researchers to explore the IRT models and to use

them in personality research.

As the item characteristics that were estimated with the use of IRT models are not

sample dependent these parameters can be considered as invariant across different

samples (Sodano & Tracey, 2011). This property is an advantage in situations when

the parameters of the model are wished to be generalized, and in most studies this is

the case.

Studies identified in the literature that used the IRT methods with personality

assessment have done so to evaluate and delineate the psychometric procedures of

existing measures and to produce shorter versions of them (Betancourt, Yang, Bolton,

& Normand, 2014; Hays, Morales, & Reise, 2000). To that end they are examining the

item and test properties by using the parameters that were estimated. While this

76

practice is useful and informative as it gives cues to how to best use scales, this line of

researh is not especially useful for better predicting criteria of importance, such as task

performance. In the previous studies, predictive utilities of IRT methodologies mostly

went unstudied.

In the development of the compound personality scale in the present study IRT

methodologies were used on top of CTT methodologies to improve predictive power.

It was expected that, by identifying the most discriminating items that also provide the

most information, the predictive validity of the compound scale would be enhanced.

As it turns out, while the IRT methodology did not result in any improvement in the

predictive power, it helped shortening the scale length. The compound scale that was

formed with the CTT was composed of 88 items while the IRT-based compound scale

was shortened by one-thirds, leaving 59 items. Thus, the present study contributed to

the scarce literature on IRT applications to shorten scale length. The present study went

further by showing that validity is preserved after shortening the scale by IRT, an

important finding that was not studied previously.

5.4 Situational Strength as the Proposed Moderator of the Personality-

Performance Association

In addition to these research goals of developing a compound scale to predict task

performance and refining the scale with IRT methods, moderating influences of the

environmental variables as conceptualized by situational strength (Meyer et al., 2010)

were also studied. For the most part the hypothesized moderating effects of situational

strength were not supported statistically in the sample of 61 participants. Specifically,

the interaction terms used to test the moderation hypotheses were not significant. As

testing for interaction effects require relatively larger sample sizes especially for small

effects (Aguinis, Beaty, Boik, & Pierce, 2005; Morris, Sherman, & Mansfield, 1986),

a decision was made to follow an approach similar to that taken by Beaty et al. (2001)

in their study of the moderating effect of situational strength on personality-contextual

performance. Since in that study they had 58 participants, in addition to statistically

testing for interaction effects, they divided participants into two groups of situational

77

strength; below average strength (weak situations) and above average strength (strong

situations). Their study did not reveal statistically significant moderating effects either,

however within group analyses indicated that the correlations were higher and

significant in the weak situation group than the strong situation group.

In the present study, groups varying on situational strength were formed based on the

33rd and 66th percentiles. Accordingly, at different levels (low, medium, high) of the

situational strength, the correlations between the compound scale and task

performance were changing from nonsignificance to significance or vice versa. In an

unparallel manner to the study hypothesis, on the sub-dimensions of clarity,

consistency, and constraints, significant correlations between task performance and the

compound scale were observed in the group scoring highest on situational strength. In

other words, in situations evaluated as more clear, or consistent, or constraining; the

relationship between task performance and the compound scale was larger and

significant. The interaction of work discipline compound personality scale with clarity

and consistency had 2% incremental variance, which was comparable to the

incremental variance in other situational strength studies (e.g., Beaty et al., 2001).

Nevertheless, the trend of associations across groups that are counter expectations are

puzzling in light of the literature findings. Specifically, relationships are just the

opposite of what was found in Meyer et al. (2014) in which the relationship between

criterion (i.e. OCB) and personality were significant such that as the strength of the

situation increased the relationship between personality and OCB decreased. Results,

especially for the clarity dimension, indicates a linear trend of increasing associations

as the situations get stronger. The trend for consistency or constraints was not that clear

and could more easily fluctuate as samples get bigger. The trend for consequences was

in the expected direction; the work discipline personality scale was more highly

associated with task performance as employees perceived their environment to have

more important consequences for stakeholders. Thus, especially the trend observed

with clarity is worth discussing.

Clarity refers to the explicitness of expectations, rules and regulations in a work

environment. Items include how clear performance expectations, responsibilities, and

78

regulations as to how to best do tasks are on the job. It could be argued that graduate

assistants who receive more explicit guides about performance expectations would

behave based on their self-concepts. That is, an assistant who has a positive self-

concept with regards to succeeding (e.g., when I work hard I succeed), would be more

expected to set a goal for achieving expectations, and thus would utilize personality

towards achieving the goals, whereas an assistant who has a relatively lower

achievement-related personality would not put effort in clearly identified

“challenging” tasks.

One possible explanation for the emerged pattern of unexpected correlations can be

found in Christiansen and Tett (2008). In this article they are highlighting the influence

of other situational cues with regard to trait activation. They proposed that not just the

cues from the tasks but also cues from social environment and organization level can

influence trait job performance relationship. And, according to their view, by just

studying task level situational variables we cannot solve the puzzle of the trait-job

performance relationship. In addition to their first suggestion of trait activation cues

from different levels of the environment, Christiansen and Tett (2008) also call for the

need of differentiation between trait expressing behaviors in work environments and

performance evaluations of these behaviors. Value system of the judges of

performance (i.e. supervisors) should also be taken into consideration to better

understand the relationship between trait expression and job performance.

In this research, information regarding the properties of the tasks of research assistants

in terms of situational strength were collected with the use of SSAW scale. This scale

basically collected information on the tasks of research asisstants. But, the scale did

not collect any information regarding the social and organizational cues that might

trigger trait behavior or that might effect the judgements of the judges (i.e. faculties)

of job performance. So, there might be other social and organizational variables that

were not accounted for by the situational strength measure of the study which could

have caused the observed pattern of personality and job performance relationship at

different levels of the situational strength.

79

The possible explanation finds support in the achievement motivation literature. When

goals are perceived to be challenging, achievement motivated people go for the goal

whereas people with fear of failure try to avoid the goal (e.g., Dweck, 1986; James &

Mazerolle, 2002). Even though participants in the current study were not asked how

challenging they perceived their duties to be, it could be argued that a graduate

assistant’s job is a transition from being a student with no responsibilities towards

others to being responsible for other students’ or faculties’ work. This possible

explanation could be studied in future research.

Goal seeting theory (Locke & Latham, 2002) can also provide some explanation

regarding the results. Besides stating that most difficult goal produce highest levels of

effort, this theory showed that setting specific goals increases performance. When

compared to situations in which workers are expected to do their best, specific and

difficult goals lead to higher performance. Do-your-best kind of expectations have no

clear external reference and this lack of external reference leads to idiosyncracy in

determining goals.

Strong situations in terms of clarity are, in a sense, ones with specific goals. Thus, one

could also argue that, in strong situations people are inclined to working towards the

goal more so than low clarity situations, and what makes a difference in strong

situations could be personality. For instance, the more conscientious and less active a

graduate assistant is, the more effort put toward the specific goal. In situations where

goals are not clear, the even highly conscientious individual may not be sure of what

to do.

Finally, since task performance is based on supervisor ratings in this study, it could be

argued that in a context with clearer expectations, supervisors may be more easily

differentiating between the performances of graduate assistants and this differentiation

would be due to the correct reasons more related to the behaviors (personality)

displayed on the job rather than factors such as likeability or possible halo effects.

80

5.5 Practical Implications

The use of facet level personality constructs that can be extracted from the FFM can

be considered as practically useful. By selecting just the relevant facets from the FFM,

questionnaires can be shortened and made more relevant to the job at hand. On top of

the requirement of completing lots of questions, omnibus scales may include questions

that are very irrelevant to the position that must be filled with an appropriate candidate.

Also, researchers can utilize the IPIP item pool to develop facet scales that can be used

to predict various criteria of interest. The present study offers one such compound scale

to predict task performance.

Besides that, the development of scales using IRT based approaches should be

highlighted. The IRT analyses proved useful for reducing the number of items by

preserving validity. Gaining same magnitudes of predictive power with less questions

should also be considered as a practical a gain.

Testing times can be quite limited in selection contexts of organizations. Especially in

mass selection situations this is quite barely seen. In addition to that, job openings that

must be filled with appropriate candidates in short times necessitates shorter

procedures. Besides, situations that necessitates the elimination of high number of

applicants or situations that necessitates quick action and procedures, a short test is in

itself much more practical and adds quality to the selection procedure itself. As

indicated by the literature (Ashton, 1998) shortening of a test length may allow user

of the test to include other relevant tests at hand, such as cognitive ability, without

spending too much time. This argument also holds for scientific investigations.

In this research, data were collected from different sources. Specifically, predictor

varibles of compound personality scale and BFI scores were collected from research

assistants. Task performance evaluations of the research assistants were collected from

the faculties that worked with these research assistants for at least half-a-year. This can

be considered as a strength of the study, as most of the studies in the literature use the

same participants as the source of both predictor and criterion data.

81

5.6 Limitations

Sample size that was reached in Study 1 was just close to being optimal. In order to

reach results that can represent population correlation matrix, it was generally advised

to use sample sizes higher than 500. That being said, a sample size of 423 is not entirely

inappropriate; especially KMO values and Bartlett’s test indicated this fact. IRT

analysis was also conducted using this data. Other researchers have also applied IRT

analyses to samples under five hundered participants (e.g., Chiesi et al., 2013). So,

sample size should not be considered as too problematic.

A specific limitation of the present investigation is the sample size of Study 2. A power

analysis for the correlation analyses of the study showed that the power was .89.

Results of the analysis also showed that for this correlation, with the classical

significance level of .05, in order to reach a power of .95 a sample size of 78 is

necessary (Cohen, 1988). But, conventionally power of .80 was considered as

statistically powerful (Mazen, Hemmasi, & Lewis, 1987). However, power was

insufficient for testing for the interaction effects. The power for an effect of .15 with a

sample of 61 is .32.

Sample characteristics can also be considered as a limitation of this research. As the

nature of the job, all of the participants in this sample were pursuing a Ph.D. or M.S.

degree. Besides that almost half of the sample consisted of engineers. Sample

participants were highly qualified and this quality of the sample makes it hard to make

generalizations with regard to the results. Replication of the study with different jobs

would be beneficial. Currently, the results of the study could be generalized to graduate

assistants.

After the development of the shortened version of the compound personality scale, in

an independent sample its criterion-related validity was tested. Even though the scale

that was shortened performed well under the criterion-related validation context, its

properties (i.e. item parameter estimated of IRT) should be checked in an independent

sample as advised in the literature (Edelen & Reeve, 2007). As sample size was not

82

sufficient in Study 2, estimated parameters’ volatility should be checked in another

large sample.

Finally, the data to test the hypotheses were collected from Turkey. At this time, the

predictive power of the compound scale developed to predict task performance cannot

be generalized to other cultures. This argument especially holds for the assertiveness

and openness to ideas facets, as discussed earlier.

5.7 Suggestions for Future Research

Although the way IRT methods and parameters were used in this study was beneficial

in terms of shortening the compound personality measure, using the IRT methods are

best suited for the adaptive tests, especially computerized adaptive tests. To harness

the real potential of the IRT methods, real time estimation of the parameters from the

IRT is a must. By using computerized adaptive testing, real benefits can be realized.

Even further shortening of personality scales and improved participant trait estimates

can be obtained by using IRT with the infrastructure that can be afforded by computers.

Even though the main focus of the literature that uses facets is on predicting task

performance, there are organizational outcomes besides task performance. In order to

predict contextual performance and CWBs facet level constructs may be used. As these

constructs are considered as more related to volition than task performance, studying

the relationship between facets and contextual performance and CWB may be more

fruitful than using facets for the prediction of task performance.

83

REFERENCES

Allen, V. D., Weissman, A., Hellwig, S., MacCann, C., Roberts, R. D. (2014).

Development of the situational test of emotional understanding – brief

(STEU-B) using item response theory. Personality and Invidivual

Differences, 65, 3-7. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.051

Ashton, M. C. (1998). Personality and job performance: The importance of narrow

traits. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 289-303.

Ashton, M. C., Jackson, D. N., Paunonen, S. V., Helmes, E., & Rothstein, M. G.

(1995). The criterion validity of broad factor scales versus specific facet

scales. Journal of Research in Personality, 29, 432-442. doi:

10.1006/jrpe.1995.1025

Aguinis, H., Beaty, J. C., Boik, R. J., & Pierce, C. A. (2005). Effect size and power in

assessing moderating effects of categorical variables using multiple

regression: A 30-year review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 94-107.

Bakker, A. B., Demerouti, E., & ten Brummelhuis, L. L. (2012). Work engagement,

performance, and active learning: The role of conscientiousness. Journal of

Vocational Behavior, 80, 555-564.

Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimensions and

job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1-26.

Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (2012). Nature and use of personality in selection. In

N. Schmitt (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of assessment and selection (pp. 225-

251). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Personality and performance at

the beginning of the new millennium: What do we know and where do we go

next? International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 9, 9-30.

Betancourt, T. S., Yang, F., Bolton, P., & Normand, S. (2014). Developing an African

youth psychosocial assessment: An application of item response theory.

84

International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 23, 142-160. doi:

10.1002/mpr.1420

Blake, R. J., Potter III, E. H., & Slimak, R. E. (1993). Validation of the structural

scales of the CPI for predicting the performance of junior officers in the U.S.

Coast Guard. Journal of Business and Psychology, 7, 431-448. doi:

10.1007/BF01013757

Chalmers, R. P. (2012). mirt: A multidimensional item response theory package for

the R environment. Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 1-29.

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2003). Personality traits and academic

examination of performance. European Journal of Personality, 17, 237-250.

doi: 10.1002/per.473

Chen, G., Gully, S. M., Whiteman, J., & Kilcullen, R. N. (2000). Examination of

relationships among trait-like individual differences, state-like individual

differences, and learning performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85,

835– 847. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.85.6.835

Chernyshenko, O. S., Stark, S., Chan, K., Drasgow, F., & Williams, B. (2001). Fitting

item response theory models to two personality inventories: Issues and

insights. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 36, 523-562.

Chiesi, F., Galli, S., Primi, C., Borgi, P. I., & Bonacchi, A. (2013). The accuracy of

the life orientation test-revised (LOT-R) in measuring dispositional optimism:

Evidence from item response theory analyses. Journal of Personality

Assessment, 95, 523-529. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2013.781029

Christiansen, N. D., & Tett, R. P. (2008). Toward a better understanding of the role of

situations in linking personality, work behavior, and job performance.

Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 1, 312-316.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale,

NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

85

Daller, H., & Yıldız, C. (2006). Power distance at work: The cases of Turkey,

successor states of the formet Soviet Union and Western Europe. Journal of

Politeness Research, 2, 35-53.

Denis, P. L., Morin, D., & Guindon, C. (2010). Exploring the capacity of NEO PI-R

facets to predict job performance in two French-Canadian samples.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 18, 201-207.

Detrick, P., Chibnall, J. T., & Luebbert, M. C. (2004). The revised NEO personality

inventory as predictor of police academy performance. Criminal Justice and

Behavior, 31, 676-694. doi: 10.1177/0093854804268751

DeVellis, R. F. (2003) Scale development: Theory and Applications. Thousand Oaks,

CA: Sage Publications.

De Vries, A., De Vries, R. E., & Born, M. PH. (2011). Broad versus narrow traits:

Conscientious and honesty-humility as predictors of academic criteria.

European Journal of Personality, 25, 336-348. doi: 10.1002/per.795

Dudley, N. M., Orvis, K. A., Lebiecki, J. E., & Cortina, J. M. (2006). A meta-analytic

investigation of conscientiousness in the prediction of job performance:

Examining the intercorrelations and the incremental validity of narrow traits.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 40-57. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.1.40

Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American

Psychologist, 41, 1040-1048.

Edelen, M. O., & Reeve, B. B. (2007). Applying item response theory modeling to

questionnaire development, evaluation, and refinement. Quality of Life

Research, 16, 5-18. doi: 10.1007/s11136-007-9198-0

Embretson, S. E., & Reise, S. P. (2000). Item response theory for psychologists.

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Fein, E. C., & Klein, H. C. (2011). Personality predictors of behavioral self-

regulation: Linking behavioral self-regulation to five-factor model factors,

86

facets, and a compound trait. International Journal of Selection and

Assessment, 19, 132-144. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2389.2011.00541.x

Feuerstahler, L. M., & Waller, N. G. (2014). Estimation of the 4-parameter model

with marginal maximum likelihood. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 49,

285. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2014.912889

Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C.

R., & Gough, H. C. (2006). The International Personality Item Pool and the

future of public-domain personality measures. Journal of Research in

Personality, 40, 84-96. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007

Griffin, B., & Hesketh, B. (2004). Why openness to experience is not a good

predictor of job performance. International Journal of Selection and

Placement, 12, 243-251. doi: 10.1300/J013v34n01_04

Guion, R. M. (1998). Assessment, measurement, and prediction for personnel

decisions. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Guion, R. M., & Gottier, R. F. (1965). Validity of personality measures in personnel

selection. Personnel Psychology, 18, 135-164.

Hafsteinsson, L. G., Donovan, J. J., & Breland, B. T. (2007). An item response theory

examination of two popular goal orientation measures. Educational and

Psychological Measurement, 67, 719-739. doi: 10.1177/0013164406299101

Hallberg, U. E., Johansson, G., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2007). Type A behavior and work

situation: Associations with burnout and work engagement. Scandinavian

Journal of Psychology, 48, 135-142.

Hambleton, R. K., & Swaminathan, H. (1985). Item response theory: Principles and

applications. New York, NY: Springer Science+Business Media.

Hambleton, R. K., Swaminathan, H., & Rogers, H. J. (1991). Fundamentals of item

response theory. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

87

Hays, R. D., Morales, L. S., & Reise, S. P. (2000). Item response theory and healty

outcomes measurement in the 21th century. Med Care, 38.

Hofstede, G. (1980a). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-

related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Hofstede, G. (1980b). Motivation, leadership, and organization: Do American

theories apply abroad? Organizational Dynamics, 9, 42–63.

Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (1992). Hogan Personality Inventory Manual. Tulsa: Ok:

Hogan Assessment Systems.

Hogan, J. H., & Holland, B. (2003). Using theory to evaluate personality and job-

performance relations: A socioanalytic perspective. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 88, 100-112. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.1.100

Hough, L. M. (1992). The “Big Five” personality variables – construct confusion:

Description versus prediction. Human Performance, 5, 139-155.

Hough, L. M., & Oswald, F. L. (2008). Personality testing and industrial-

organizational psychology: Reflections, progress, and prospects. Industrial

and Organizational Psychology, 1, 272-290. doi: 10.1111/j.1754-

9434.2008.00057.x

Hurtz, G. M., & Donovan, J. J. (2000). Personality and job performance: The big five

revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 869-879. doi: 10.1037/0021-

9010.85.6.869

Hutcheson, G. D., & Sofroniou, N. (1999). The multivariate social scientist:

Introductory statistics using generalized linear models. Thousand Oaks, CA:

Sage Publications.

James, L. R., & Mazerolle, M. D. (2002). Personality in work organizations.

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

88

Jenkins, M., & Griffith, R. (2004). Using personality constructs to predict

performance: Narrow or broad bandwidth. Journal of Business and

Psychology, 19, 255-269.

John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five Inventory -

Versions 4a and 54. Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley,

Institute of Personality and Social Research.

Johnson, J. W. (2003). Toward a better understanding of the relationship between

personality and individual job performance. In M. R. Barrick, & A. M. Ryan

(Eds.) Personality and work: Reconsidering the role of personality in

organizations (pp. 83-120). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Johnson, J. L., & Bloom, A. M. (1995). An analysis of the contribution of the five

factors of personality to variance in academic procrastination. Personality and

Individual Differences, 18, 127-133. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)00109-6

Lievens, F., Coetsier, P., De Fruyt, F, & De Maesener, J. (2002). Medical students’

personality characteristics and academic performance: A five-factor model

perspective. Medical Education, 36, 1050-1056. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-

2923.2002.01328.x

Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal

setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57,

705-717. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.9.705

Mazen, A. M. M., Hemmasi, M., & Lewis, M. F. (1987). Assessment of statistical

power in contemporary strategy research. Strategic Management Journal, 8,

403-410.

Meyer, R. D., & Dalal, R. S. (2009). Situational strength as a means of

conceptualizing context. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 2, 99-102.

doi: 10.1111/j.1754-9434.2008.01114.x

Meyer, R. D., Dalal, R. S., & Bonaccio, S. (2009). A meta-analytic investigation into

the moderating effects of situational strength on the conscientiousness-

89

performance relationship. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30, 1077-1102.

doi: 10.1002/job.602

Meyer, R. D., Dalal, R. S., & Hermida, R. (2010). A review and synthesis of

situational strength in the organizational sciences. Journal of Management,

36, 121-140. doi: 10.1177/0149206309349309

Meyer, R. D., Dalal, R. S., José, I. J., Hermida, R., Chen, T. R., Vega, R. P., Brooks,

C. K., & Khare, V. P. (2014). Measuring job-related situational strength and

assessing its interactive effects with personality on voluntary work behavior.

Journal of Management, 40, 1010-1041. doi: 10.1177/0149206311425613

Moon, H., Hollenbeck, J. R., Marinova, S., & Humphrey, S. E. (2008). Beneath the

surface: Uncovering the relationship between extraversion and organizational

citizenship behavior through a facet approach. International Journal of

Selection and Assessment, 16, 143-154.

Morgeson, F. P., Campion, M. A., Dipboye, R. L., Hollenbeck, J. R., Murphy, K., &

Schmitt, N. (2007). Reconsidering the use of personality tests in personality

selection contexts. Personnel Psychology, 60, 683-729.

Morris, J. H., Sherman, J. D., & Mansfield, E. R. (1986). Failures to detect

moderating effects with ordinary least squares-moderated multiple regression:

Some reasons and a remedy. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 282-288.

Mussel, P., Winter, C., Gelléri, P., and Schuler, H. (2011). Explicating the openness to

experience construct and its subdimensions and facets in a work setting.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 19, 145-156.

Neal, A., Yeo, G., Koy, A., & Xiao, T. (2012). Predicting the form and direction of

work role performance from the Big 5 model of personality traits. Journal of

Organizational Behavior, 33, 175-192.

Oh, I., Toker, Y., Ferreter, J., Whitman, D., McKinniss, T., Casillas, A., & Robbins, S.

(2008, April). Development of the WorkKeys Talent Assessment scales and

indices. Poster presented at the Annual Conference of the Society for

Industrial and Organizational Psychology, San Francisco.

90

Ones, D. S., Dilchert, S., Viswesvaran, C., & Judge, T. A. (2007). In support of

personality assessment in organizational settings. Personnel Psychology, 995-

1027. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2007.00099.x

Ones, D. S., & Viswesvaran, C. (2001). Integrity tests and other criterion-focused

occupational personality scales (COPS) used in personnel selection.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 9, 31-39.

Ones, D. S., Viswesvaran, C., & Schmidt, F. L. (1993). Comprehensive meta-analysis

of integrity test validities: Finding and implications for personnel selection

and theories of performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 679-703.

Oz, B. (2003). Dispositional affectivity and job performance: Mediating effects of

job satisfaction. (Unpublished master’s thesis). Orta Doğu Teknik

Üniversitesi, Ankara.

Pincombe, J. L., Luciano, M., Martin, N. G., & Wright, M. J. (2007). Heritability of

NEO PI-R extraversion facets and their relationship with IQ. Twin Research

and Human Genetics, 10, 462-469. doi: 10.1375/twin.10.3.462

R Core Team (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R

Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL http://www.R-

project.org/.

Reckase, M. D. (2009). Multidimensional item response theory. New York, NY:

Springer Science+Business Media.

Reise, S. P., & Henson, J. M. (2003). A discussion of modern versus traditional

psychometrics as applied to personality assessment scales. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 81, 93-103.

Rossier, J., Zecca, G., Stauffer, S. D., Maggiori, C., & Dauwalder, J. (2012). Career

adapt-abilities scale in a French-speaking Swiss sample: Psychometric

properties and relationships to personality and work engagement. Journal of

Vocational Behavior, 80, 734-743.

91

Samejima, F. (1969). Estimation of latent ability using a response pattern of graded

scores. Psychometrika, Monograph Supplement No. 17.

Scroggins, W. A., Thomas, S. L., & Morris, J. A. (2009). Psychological testing in

personnel selection, part III: The resurgence of personality testing. Public

Personnel Management, 38, 67-77.

Shoss, M. K., Witt, L. A., & Vera, D. (2012). When does adaptive performance lead

to higher task performance? Journal of Organizational Behavior, 33, 910-924.

Sodano, S. M., & Tracey, T. J. G. (2011). A brief inventory of interpersonal

problems-circumplex using nonparametric item response theory: Introducing

the IIP-C-IRT. Journal of Personality Assessment, 93, 62-75. doi:

10.1080/00223891.2010.528482

Sümer, N, Lajunen, T., & Özkan, T. (2005). Big Five personality traits as the distal

predictors of road accident involvement. In G. Underwood (Ed.) Traffic and

Transportation Psychology (pp. 215-227). Oxford: United Kingdom.

Sümer, N., & Sümer, H. C. (2002). Adaptation of BFI in a Turkish sample.

Unpublished manuscript.

Taras, V., Kirkman, B. L., & Steel, P. (2010). Examining the impact of Culture’s

Consequences: A three-decade, multilevel, meta-analytic review of Hoftede’s

cultural value dimensions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 405-439. doi:

10.1037/a0018938

Tett, R. P., & Christiansen, N. D. (2008). Personality tests at the crossroads: A

response to Morgeson, Campion, Dipboye, Hollenbeck, Murphy, and Schmitt

(2007). Personnel Psychology, 2007, 60, 967-993. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-

6570.2007.00098.x

Tett, R. P., & Christiansen, N. D. (2008). Personality assessment in organizations. In

G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The SAGE Handbook of

Personality Theory and Assessment: Volume 1 – Personality Theories and

Models (pp. 720-742). Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications.

92

Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., & Rothstein, M. (1991). Personality measures as

predictors of job performance: A meta-analytic review. Personnel Psychology,

44, 703-742.

Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., Rothstein, M., & Reddon, J. R. (1994). Meta-analysis of

personality-job performance relations: A reply to Ones, Mount, Barrick, and

Hunter (1994). Personnel Psychology, 47, 157-172.

Watson, D. C. (2001). Procrastination and the five-factor model: A facet level

analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 149-158.

Weiner, I. B., & Greene, R. L. (2008). Handbook of personality assessment.

Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Wetzel, E., & Hell, B. (2014). Multidimensional item response theory models in

vocational interest measurement: An illustration using the AIST-R. Journal of

Psychoeducational Measurement, 32, 342-355. doi:

10.1177/0734282913508244

93

APPENDICES

APPENDIX A: Compound Personality Measure Items of Study 1

Aşağıda sizi kısmen tanımlayan ya da pek tanımlayamayan bir takım özellikler sunulmaktadır. Örneğin,

başkaları ile zaman geçirmekten hoşlanan birisi olduğunuzu düşünüyor musunuz? Lütfen aşağıda

verilen özelliklerin sizi ne oranda yansıttığını ya da yansıtmadığını belirtmek için kendinizi

tanımladığınızı düşündüğünüz sayıyı işaretleyin.

Hiç

Tan

ımla

mıy

or

Tan

ımla

mıy

or

Pek

Tan

ımla

mıy

or

Bir

az

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Tam

amen

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Doğrudan hedefe yönelirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sıkı çalışırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Planlarımı eyleme dönüştürürüm. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Görevlerime bütün kalbimle atılırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Benden beklenilenden daha fazlasını yaparım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kendim ve diğerleri için yüksek standartlar belirlerim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Başarılı olmak için motivasyonum yüksek değildir. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Durumu kurtaracak kadar iş yaparım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşime pek fazla zaman ve çaba harcamam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

En iyisi olmak isterim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Her şey mükemmel olana kadar devam ederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Performansımla diğerlerini geçmeye çalışırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşi eğlencenin önüne koyarım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşimin hayatımın önemli bir parçası olmadığını

hissediyorum. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kolay olan yolu seçerim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Çaba göstermeye zahmet etmem. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İş çok zorlaştığında devam etmem. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Zorlu görevleri kabul ederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Hayatımın yavaş bir temposu var. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Her zaman yoğunumdur. 1 2 3 4 5 6

94

Her zaman hareket halindeyim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Boş zamanlarımda birçok iş yaparım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Birçok işi aynı anda idare edebilirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşleri ağırdan almayı severim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Acele etmemeye özen gösteririm. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşlerin kendi hızında ilerlemesine izin veririm. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğerlerinin neye ihtiyacı olduğunu öngörürüm. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kendimden başkalarını da umursarım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Herkes için söyleyecek iyi bir söz bulabilirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanların duygularına kayıtsızımdır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanlar için vakit ayırmam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İnsanlara içtenlikle karşılandıklarını hissettiririm. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanlara yardımımın dokunmasını severim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Ortamın huzurunu korumaya çalışırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Başkalarının ihtiyaçlarıyla uğraşamam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanların görüşlerini göz ardı ederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanların çıkarlarını göz önüne alırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Eski tanıdıklarımla bağlantımı koparmam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sorumluluğun bende olmasını isterim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanları bir işi yapmaya ikna edebilirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanları etkileyerek yönlendirmeye çalışırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kontrolü ele almayı severim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanların öncülük etmesini beklerim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Dikkati üzerime çekmeyi sevmem. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Görüşlerimi kendime saklarım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kendimi kolayca öne çıkartabilirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Gerektiğimde kendimi savunurum. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bana patronluk taslanmasına ses çıkarmam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kendimi rahatça ifade edebilirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kararların alınmasını başkalarına bırakırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Düşündüğümü söylerim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Eleştirilerimi dile getirmekten çekinmem. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Gerektiğinde sert tedbirler alabilirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Doğaçlama konuşmalar yapmakta iyiyimdir. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanları idare etmek bana zor gelir. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Memnun edilmesi kolay bir insanım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

95

İnsanlarla karşı karşıya gelmekten hoşlanmam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Israrcı görünmekten hoşlanmam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sivri bir dilim vardır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanlarla zıtlaşırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İnsanlara bağırırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kin tutarım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bana yapılan haksızlıkları kimsenin yanına bırakmam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Rekabetten ziyade işbirliğine değer veririm. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanlarla işbirliği yapabilirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kelimelerimi özenle seçerim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Ani kararlar veririm. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sık sık son dakika planları yaparım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Düşünmeden aceleyle harekete geçerim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Yaptığım işleri kontrol ederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Hataları fark ederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Detayları nadiren fark ederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Davranışlarımın sonuçlarını düşünmeden hareket

ederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Planlar yapmaktansa anı yaşamayı tercih ederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Durumların eksi ve artı yönlerini tartarım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Ancak bütün bilgileri değerlendirdikten sonra bir karar

veririm. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir işe başlarken enine boyuna düşünmenin

gerekliliğine inanırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kararlarımı verirken farklı olasılıkları düşünmem. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sahip olduğumdan daha fazla para harcarım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sonradan üzülmektense önceden tedbirimi almak

gerektiğine inanırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşimde çok titizimdir. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kendi işlerimi başkalarına yaptırmaya çalışırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Gerektiğinde gerçekleri saptırırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşleri kuralına göre yaparım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Otoriteye saygı duyarım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Otoriteye direnirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kuralların etrafından nasıl dolanacağımı bilirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kurallara bağlı kalırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sorumluluklardan kaçınırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Benim olmayan eşyaları asla almam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

96

Öne geçmek için hile yapabilirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Başımı beladan kurtarmak için yalan söylerim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Güvenin temelinin dürüstlük olduğuna inanırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sözümü tutacağım konusunda bana güvenilebilir. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Yalan söylemeyi veya hile yapmayı düşünemem bile. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Asla paraşüt veya bungee jumping yapmaya gitmem. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Tehlikeli durumlardan sakınırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Her şeyi bir kere denemeye gönüllüyüm. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Macera ararım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Risk alırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Çeşitliliği sıradan işlere tercih ederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Yeni yerleri gezip görmekten keyif alırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İlgi duyduğum birçok değişik şey vardır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Yeni şeylere başlamaktan keyif alırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bildiğim işlere devam etmeyi tercih ederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Değişikliklerden hoşlanmam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Yeni yemekler denemekten hoşlanmam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Alışıldık yöntemlere bağlıyımdır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Karmaşık problemler üzerinde çalışmayı severim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Beni zorlayan metinleri okumayı severim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Zengin bir kelime haznesine sahibimdir. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Soyut fikirler ilgimi çekmez. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Felsefi tartışmalardan kaçınırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Soyut fikirleri anlamakta zorlanırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kuramsal tartışmalar ilgimi çekmez. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Ağır okumalardan uzak dururum. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Derin düşüncelere dalmayı severim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Beni düşünmeye sevk eden filmleri severim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Herhangi bir konuyu derinlemesine irdelemem. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Öğrenmeyi sevmem. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bilgi dağarcığımı genişletmek isterim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Durmadan bir şeyleri kaybederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Ortalığı toparlamayı severim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşlerimi bir plana göre yaparım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Çoğunlukla eşyaları olması gereken yere geri koymayı

unuturum. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Dağınık insanlar beni rahatsız etmez. 1 2 3 4 5 6

97

Düzensizlik beni rahatsız etmez. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir işi bitirmeden bırakmam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Önümdeki engellere rağmen işleri bitiririm. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Çalışırken başka şeylere kaymam. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Keyfimi kaçıracak işleri ertelerim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Planlar yapar ve bunlara bağlı kalırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Başladığım/üstlendiğim işlerin sonunu getiririm. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşlerimden kaytarırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşleri son dakikada yaparım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sıkıldığım anda yapmakta olduğum işi bırakırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Başladığım işlere ilgimi çabuk kaybederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kolayca pes ederim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Elimdeki işe odaklanmakta zorlanırım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bazı işlerin sonunu getiremem. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Günlük işlerimi vakit kaybetmeden bitiririm. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşlerime vakit kaybetmeden başlarım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Zamanımı boşa harcarım. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Çalışmaya başlamak için birisinin beni itmesi gerekir. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Vermem gereken kararları ertelerim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanların niyetlerinin iyi olduğuna inanıyorum. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İnsanların söylediklerine güvenirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İnsanların temelde dürüst olduğuna inanıyorum. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Her şeyin iyi neticeleneceğini düşünüyorum. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Başkalarının gizli niyetleri olduğundan şüphelenirim. 1 2 3 4 5 6

98

APPENDIX B: Compound Personality Measure Items of Study 2

Aşağıda sizi kısmen tanımlayan ya da pek tanımlayamayan bir takım özellikler sunulmaktadır.

Örneğin, başkaları ile zaman geçirmekten hoşlanan birisi olduğunuzu düşünüyor musunuz? Lütfen

aşağıda verilen özelliklerin sizi ne oranda yansıttığını ya da yansıtmadığını belirtmek için kendinizi

tanımladığınızı düşündüğünüz sayıyı yuvarlak içerisine alınız.

Hiç

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Pek

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Bir

az T

anım

lıy

or

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Tam

amen

tan

ımlı

yor

Her zaman hareket halindeyim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Doğaçlama konuşmalar yapmakta iyiyimdir 1 2 3 4 5 6

Rekabetten ziyade işbirliğine değer veririm 1 2 3 4 5 6

Zamanımı boşa harcarım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Ancak bütün bilgileri değerlendirdikten sonra bir karar veririm 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanları bir işi yapmaya ikna edebilirim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Benim olmayan eşyaları asla almam 1 2 3 4 5 6

Yeni yerleri gezip görmekten keyif alırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Derin düşüncelere dalmayı severim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir işi bitirmeden bırakmam 1 2 3 4 5 6

Başkalarının gizli niyetleri olduğundan şüphelenirim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sıkı çalışırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanları idare etmek bana zor gelir 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanlarla işbirliği yapabilirim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Zorlu görevleri kabul ederim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir işe başlarken enine boyuna düşünmenin gerekliliğine inanırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Öne geçmek için hile yapabilirim 1 2 3 4 5 6

İlgi duyduğum birçok değişik şey vardır 1 2 3 4 5 6

Beni düşünmeye sevk eden filmleri severim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Önümdeki engellere rağmen işleri bitiririm 1 2 3 4 5 6

Memnun edilmesi kolay bir insanım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kararlarımı verirken farklı olasılıkları düşünmem 1 2 3 4 5 6

Çalışmaya başlamak için birisinin beni itmesi gerekir 1 2 3 4 5 6

Başımı beladan kurtarmak için yalan söylerim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Yeni şeylere başlamaktan keyif alırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

99

Hiç

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Pek

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Bir

az T

anım

lıy

or

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Tam

amen

tan

ımlı

yor

İşleri ağırdan almayı severim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Herhangi bir konuyu derinlemesine irdelemem 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanların görüşlerini göz ardı ederim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Görevlerime bütün kalbimle atılırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanların öncülük etmesini beklerim 1 2 3 4 5 6

İnsanlarla zıtlaşmaktan hoşlanmam 1 2 3 4 5 6

Vermem gereken kararları ertelerim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Güvenin temelinin dürüstlük olduğuna inanırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanların niyetlerinin iyi olduğuna inanıyorum 1 2 3 4 5 6

Benden beklenilenden daha fazlasını yaparım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Dikkati üzerime çekmeyi sevmem 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sonradan üzülmektense önceden tedbirimi almak gerektiğine inanırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Keyfimi kaçıracak işleri ertelerim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Otoriteye saygı duyarım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Değişikliklerden hoşlanmam 1 2 3 4 5 6

Planlar yapar ve bunlara bağlı kalırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

İnsanların söylediklerine güvenirim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kendim ve diğerleri için yüksek standartlar belirlerim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Acele etmemeye özen gösteririm 1 2 3 4 5 6

Görüşlerimi kendime saklarım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sivri bir dilim vardır 1 2 3 4 5 6

Düşünmeden aceleyle harekete geçerim 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşimde çok titizimdir 1 2 3 4 5 6

Yalan söylemeyi veya hile yapmayı düşünemem bile 1 2 3 4 5 6

Yeni yemekler denemekten hoşlanmam 1 2 3 4 5 6

Başladığım/üstlendiğim işlerin sonunu getiririm 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bilgi dağarcığımı genişletmek isterim 1 2 3 4 5 6

İnsanların temelde dürüst olduğuna inanıyorum 1 2 3 4 5 6

Başarılı olmak için motivasyonum yüksek değildir 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kendimi kolayca öne çıkartabilirim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanlarla zıtlaşırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Asla paraşüt veya bungee jumping yapmaya gitmem 1 2 3 4 5 6

İş çok zorlaştığında devam etmem 1 2 3 4 5 6

Yaptığım işleri kontrol ederim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kendi işlerimi başkalarına yaptırmaya çalışırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Alışıldık yöntemlere bağlıyımdır 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşlerimden kaytarırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

100

Hiç

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Pek

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Bir

az T

anım

lıy

or

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Tam

amen

tan

ımlı

yor

Her şeyin iyi neticeleneceğini düşünüyorum 1 2 3 4 5 6

Düzensizlik beni rahatsız etmez 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşlerime vakit kaybetmeden başlarım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Ağır okumalardan uzak dururum 1 2 3 4 5 6

Çeşitliliği sıradan işlere tercih ederim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sorumluluklardan kaçınırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Durumların eksi ve artı yönlerini tartarım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bana yapılan haksızlıkları kimsenin yanına bırakmam 1 2 3 4 5 6

Çalışırken başka şeylere kaymam 1 2 3 4 5 6

Gerektiğimde kendimi savunurum 1 2 3 4 5 6

Gerektiğinde gerçekleri saptırırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Karmaşık problemler üzerinde çalışmayı severim 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşleri son dakikada yaparım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Ortamın huzurunu korumaya çalışırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

İnsanlara bağırırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sıkıldığım anda yapmakta olduğum işi bırakırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Dağınık insanlar beni rahatsız etmez 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kuramsal tartışmalar ilgimi çekmez 1 2 3 4 5 6

Her şeyi bir kere denemeye gönüllüyüm 1 2 3 4 5 6

Hayatımın yavaş bir temposu var 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşleri kuralına göre yaparım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kin tutarım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Durumu kurtaracak kadar iş yaparım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kendimden başkalarını da umursarım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bana patronluk taslanmasına ses çıkarmam 1 2 3 4 5 6

Eski tanıdıklarımla bağlantımı koparmam 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kendimi rahatça ifade edebilirim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Detayları nadiren fark ederim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kurallara bağlı kalırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Macera ararım 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşime pek fazla zaman ve çaba harcamam 1 2 3 4 5 6

Beni zorlayan metinleri okumayı severim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanlara yardımımın dokunmasını severim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Başladığım işlere ilgimi çabuk kaybederim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Soyut fikirler ilgimi çekmez 1 2 3 4 5 6

Ortalığı toparlamayı severim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kolayca pes ederim 1 2 3 4 5 6

101

Hiç

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Pek

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Bir

az T

anım

lıy

or

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Tam

amen

tan

ımlı

yor

Başkalarının ihtiyaçlarıyla uğraşamam 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kararların alınmasını başkalarına bırakırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Eleştirilerimi dile getirmekten çekinmem 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kolay olan yolu seçerim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Davranışlarımın sonuçlarını düşünmeden hareket ederim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Zengin bir kelime haznesine sahibimdir 1 2 3 4 5 6

Performansımla diğerlerini geçmeye çalışırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanların duygularına kayıtsızımdır 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşlerimi bir plana göre yaparım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Düşündüğümü söylerim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kuralların etrafından nasıl dolanacağımı bilirim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Elimdeki işe odaklanmakta zorlanırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Çoğunlukla eşyaları olması gereken yere geri koymayı unuturum 1 2 3 4 5 6

En iyisi olmak isterim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Soyut fikirleri anlamakta zorlanırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Herşey mükemmel olana kadar devam ederim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanlar için vakit ayırmam 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bazı işlerin sonunu getiremem 1 2 3 4 5 6

Otoriteye direnirim 1 2 3 4 5 6

Felsefi tartışmalardan kaçınırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşi eğlencenin önüne koyarım 1 2 3 4 5 6

İnsanlara içtenlikle karşılandıklarını hissettiririm 1 2 3 4 5 6

Risk alırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

Günlük işlerimi vakit kaybetmeden bitiririm 1 2 3 4 5 6

Diğer insanların çıkarlarını göz önüne alırım 1 2 3 4 5 6

102

APPENDIX C: The Big Five Inventory (BFI) Items

Aşağıda sizi kısmen tanımlayan (ya da pek tanımlayamayan) bir takım özellikler sunulmaktadır.

Örneğin, başkaları ile zaman geçirmekten hoşlanan birisi olduğunuzu düşünüyor musunuz? Lütfen

aşağıda verilen özelliklerin sizi ne oranda yansıttığını ya da yansıtmadığını belirtmek için sizi en iyi

tanımlayan rakamı her bir özelliğin yanına yazınız.

Kendimi .................................... biri olarak görüyorum

____ 1. Konuşkan

____ 2. Başkalarında hata arayan

____ 3. İşini tam yapan

____ 4. Bunalımlı, melankolik

____ 5. Orijinal, yeni görüşler ortaya koyan

____ 6. Ketum/vakur

____ 7. Yardımsever ve çıkarcı olmayan

____ 8. Biraz umursamaz

____ 9. Rahat, stresle kolay baş eden

____ 10. Çok değişik konuları merak eden

____ 11. Enerji dolu

____ 12. Başkalarıyla sürekli didişen

____ 13. Güvenilir bir çalışan

____ 14. Gergin olabilen

____ 15. Maharetli, derin düşünen

____ 16. Heyecan yaratabilen

____ 17. Affedici bir yapıya sahip

____ 18. Dağınık olma eğiliminde

____ 19. Çok endişelenen

____ 20. Hayal gücü yüksek

____ 21. Sessiz bir yapıda

____ 22. Genellikle başkalarına güvenen

____ 23. Tembel olma eğiliminde olan

____ 24. Duygusal olarak dengeli, kolayca keyfi kaçmayan

____ 25. Keşfeden, icat eden

____ 26. Atılgan bir kişiliğe sahip

____ 27. Soğuk ve mesafeli olabilen

____ 28. Görevi tamamlanıncaya kadar sebat edebilen

____ 29. Dakikası dakikasına uymayan

____ 30. Sanata ve estetik değerlere önem veren

____ 31. Bazen utangaç, çekingen olan

____ 32. Hemen hemen herkese karşı saygılı ve nazik olan

____ 33. İşleri verimli yapan

103

____ 34. Gergin ortamlarda sakin kalabilen

____ 35. Rutin işleri yapmayı tercih eden

____ 36. Sosyal, girişken

____ 37. Bazen başkalarına kaba davranabilen

____ 38. Planlar yapan ve bunları takip eden

____ 39. Kolayca sinirlenen

____ 40. Düşünmeyi seven, fikirler geliştirebilen

____ 41. Sanata ilgisi çok az olan

____ 42. Başkalarıyla işbirliği yapmayı seven

____ 43. Kolaylıkla dikkati dağılan

____ 44. Sanat, müzik ve edebiyatta çok bilgili

Lütfen kontrol ediniz: Bütün ifadelerin önüne bir rakam yazdınız mı?

104

APPENDIX D: Assistant Evaluation Form

Değerlendirme Formu

Aşağıda, birlikte çalıştığınız bir araştırma görevlisinin iş yerindeki davranışlarını betimleyen cümleler

listelenmiştir. Lütfen listelenen bu davranış veya özelliklerin, araştırma görevlinizin iş yerindeki

davranışlarını ne ölçüde yansıttığını belirtmek için ilgili sayıyı daire içerisine alınız. Aşağıdaki yer alan

bir cümle, değerlendirdiğiniz araştırma görevlisinin davranışı ile birebir örtüşüyorsa “Tamamen

Tanımlıyor” anlamına gelecek şekilde 6 rakamını daire içerisine alınız. Eğer, ilgili cümle araştırma

görevlinizin davranışı ile tamamen ilgisiz ise “Hiç Tanımlamıyor” anlamına gelen 1 rakamını daire

içerisine alınız. Özetle, ilgili cümlenin araştırma görevlisinin davranışını yansıtma derecesine bağlı

olarak 1 ile 6 arasındaki sayılardan bir tanesini işaretleyiniz.

Hiç

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Pek

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Bir

az T

anım

lıy

or

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Tam

amen

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Çalışılan konu ile ilgili elindeki bilgileri birlikte çalıştığı

arkadaşlarıyla paylaşmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Yapması gereken bir işi unutmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşini özenerek ve hevesle yapmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kısa sürede yapabileceği bir işi uzun zamana yaymak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Zorunlu olmadığı halde, önemli olabileceğini düşündüğü bir

şeyi/konuyu araştırmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Daha önce kimsenin yapmayı kabul etmediği bir işi kabul etmek 1 2 3 4 5 6

Son ana bıraktığı için, kendisinden istenilen hazırlığı tam olarak

yapamamak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kendi işi olmadığı halde, çok yoğun olduğu için sıkışan bir

çalışma arkadaşına işlerini yapmasında yardımcı olmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Ekstra görevler için gönüllü olmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Önceliği kendi işlerine vererek öğretim elemanlarının işlerinin

beklenenden geç bitmesine sebep olmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Aynı anda birden fazla iş aldığı durumlarda bütün işleri

zamanında bitirebilmek 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sevdiğim iş/sevmediğim iş ayrımı yapmadan verilen tüm işleri

özenle yapmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

105

Hiç

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Pek

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Bir

az T

anım

lıy

or

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Tam

amen

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Kimseye haber vermeden/yerine kimseyi ayarlamadan görevini

yerine getirmemek 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşini yaparken kullanması gereken araçların (laboratuar

malzemesi, video, tepegöz, vb.) nasıl kullanılması gerektiği

hakkında bilgi sahibi olmak ve bunları etkili bir şekilde

kullanabilmek

1 2 3 4 5 6

Düzenlenen ofis saatlerinde ve dersle ilgili faaliyetlere ayrılan

zamanda yerinde olmamak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sınırlarını bilmeyerek diğer çalışma arkadaşlarının sorumluluk

alanlarına girmek 1 2 3 4 5 6

Görev alanında sayılabilecek ama kendisinden talep edilmeyen,

beklenmeyen ve yapmasa da hiçbir şekilde hesabı sorulmayacak

bir işi üstüne almak

1 2 3 4 5 6

Karşılaştığı sorunlara çeşitli çözüm yolları bulmak için çalışmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşini çeşitli kaynaklardan araştırmalar yaparak yapmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Literatürde bulunan bilgileri kendi kendine derleyebilmek,

analiz edebilmek ve analiz sonucunda çözüm üretebilmek 1 2 3 4 5 6

Grup çalışması sırasında fazla hırslı ve iddialı davranmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Öğretim elemanı-öğrenci arasındaki diyaloğun artmasına

yardımcı olmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Birlikte çalıştığı grubun hızına uyum sağlayamayarak işlerin

sürekli aksamasına neden olmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

Acilen çıkan bir işi, o anda yaptığı diğer işlerini organize ederek

yapmak 1 2 3 4 5 6

İşi ile ilgili aniden çıkan bir karışıklık veya sorunda hangi

değişiklik veya düzenlemelerin hangi yöntemlerle yapılacağına

karar verip, gerekli uygulamaları yapabilmek

1 2 3 4 5 6

106

APPENDIX E: Situational Strength at Work (SSW) Scale

Aşağıda işinizi tanımlayan bir takım ifadeler listelenmiştir. İfadenin işinizin özelliklerini ne

derecede yansıttığını belirlemek için lütfen aşağıdaki ölçeği kullanın. Örneğin, eğer işinizde

‘Sorumluluklarınızın açıkça tanımlandığını’ düşünüyorsanız, bu ifadeye ne ölçüde katıldığınıza bağlı

olarak, bu ifadenin yanında yer alan sayılardan ‘Biraz tanımlıyor’, ‘Tanımlıyor’ veya ‘Tamamen

tanımlıyor’ seçeneğine denk gelenini yuvarlık içine alınız. ‘Sorumluluklarınızın açıkça tanımlandığını’

ifadesine katılmıyorsanız, ne derece katılmadığınıza göre, ‘Hiç tanımlamıyor’, ‘Tanımlamıyor’ veya

‘Pek tanımlamıyor’ şıklarından bir tanesini daire içerisine alınız.

Hiç

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Pek

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Bir

az T

anım

lıyo

r

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Tam

amen

tan

ımlı

yo

r

İş ile ilgili sorumluluklar hakkında detaylı bilgi sağlanır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Çalışanların başarılı olmaları için yapmaları gerekenlerle ilgili

net bilgi sağlanır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir çalışanın işini düzgün bir şekilde nasıl yapabileceğine dair

gereken detaylı bilgi sağlanır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Hangi görevlerin tamamlanması gerektiği ile ilgili net bilgi

sağlanır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir çalışana kendisinden ne beklenildiği eksiksiz ve açık olarak

söylenir. 1 2 3 4 5 6

İş ile ilgili farklı bilgi kaynakları her zaman birbirleriyle

tutarlıdır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir çalışanın sorumlulukları birbiriyle uyumludur. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Prosedürler/iş süreçleri zaman içerisinde tutarlılık gösterir. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Üstlerin (yönetici, amir, süpervizör) talimatları ile kurum

politikaları birbiri ile uyumludur. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Resmi olmayan yönlendirmeler genellikle kurum politikaları ile

uyumludur. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Denetlenmeden, yönlendirme almadan önemli kararlar alma

imkânı vardır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir çalışanın görevlerini, önceliklerini veya hedeflerini

belirleme özgürlüğü vardır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Çok katı teslim tarihlerine/iş bitirme tarihlerine uyma

zorunluluğu vardır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

107

Hiç

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Pek

Ta

nım

lam

ıyo

r

Bir

az T

anım

lıyo

r

Tan

ımlı

yo

r

Tam

amen

tan

ımlı

yo

r

Bir çalışanın kendi kararlarını vermesi engellenir. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Kısıtlamalar bir çalışanın görevlerini kendi bildiği yöntemle

yapmasının önüne geçer. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir çalışanın karar verme özgürlüğü diğerleri tarafından

sınırlandırılır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Uygulanan yöntemler bir çalışanın kendi bildiği gibi

çalışmasına engel olur. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Çalışanlar, diğer çalışanların iş sonuçlarından önemli derecede

sorumludur. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir çalışanın görevini kesinlik ve doğruluk ile yapması çok

önemlidir. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Sıklıkla koruyucu veya güvenlik ekipmanı ile çalışması

gereklidir. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir çalışanın kararlarının diğer insanlar için son derece önemli

sonuçları vardır. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir çalışanın yapabileceği bir hata diğer insanlar için çok ciddi

sonuçlar doğurur. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir çalışan düşük performans gösterdiğinde diğer insanları zarar

görme riskine sokar. 1 2 3 4 5 6

Bir çalışan işinde kendisinden beklenileni vermediğinde bunun

olumsuz sonuçları olur. 1 2 3 4 5 6

108

APPENDIX F: Research Assistant Informed Consent

Değerli Katılımcı,

Bu çalışma Orta Doğu Teknik Üniversitesi (ODTÜ) Psikoloji Bölümü’nde yüksek lisans eğitimi

alan Mehmet Gültaş tarafından, Yard. Doç. Dr. Yonca Toker gözetmenliğinde yürütülen yüksek lisans

tezinin bir parçası olarak uygulanmaktadır. Çalışmanın amacı kişiliğe ait özellikler ve iş ortamında yer

alan belirli değişkenlerin iş performansı ile ilişkisini incelemektir. Anketimizde doğru/yanlış ya da

iyi/kötü cevap yoktur, vereceğiniz yanıtlardaki samimiyetiniz bilimsel bilginin doğruluğuna, çalışmanın

da geçerli ve güvenilir olmasına katkı sağlayacaktır.

Katılmayı kabul ettiğiniz takdirde, cevaplaması yaklaşık 20 dakika sürecek bir anketi

doldurmanız istenecektir. Ayrıca, beraber çalıştığınız bir öğretim üyesinin iş davranışlarınızı

değerlendirmek üzere 5 dakikalık kısa bir soru formunu cevaplaması istenecektir. Katılımınız

karşılığında 1 tablet bilgisayar kazanmak üzere 150 civarı katılımcının olacağı bir çekilişe katılma hakkı

kazanmış olacaksınız. Bu çekiliş sonuçları en geç 2014 yılı sonunda belirlenecek ve kazanan katılımcı

e-posta ile bilgilendirilecektir. Buna ek olarak, biraz önce bahsedilen her iki anket de doldurulduğunda

katılım ücreti olarak 40 TL banka hesabınıza gönderilecektir.

Anketler genel olarak kişisel rahatsızlık verecek soruları içermemektedir. Ancak, katılım

sırasında sorulardan ya da herhangi başka bir nedenden ötürü kendinizi rahatsız hissederseniz anketi

cevaplamayı yarıda bırakabilirsiniz. Böyle bir durumda çalışmayı uygulayan kişiye, devam etmek

istemediğinizi söylemeniz yeterli olacaktır. Toplanan bütün bilgiler ODTÜ’de güvenli bir şekilde

korunacaktır. Kişisel bilgilerinizin güvenliği, bilgiler saklanırken, bilgilerin isminiz yerine

oluşturacağınız bir rumuz kullanılarak korunacaktır. Katılımınız için teşekkür ederiz.

Çalışma hakkında daha fazla bilgi almak için ODTÜ Psikoloji Bölümü’nden Yard. Doç. Dr.

Yonca Toker (e-posta: [email protected]) veya Arş. Gör. Mehmet Gültaş (e-posta:

[email protected]) ile iletişime geçebilirsiniz.

Bu çalışmaya tamamen gönüllü olarak katılıyorum ve istediğim zaman yarıda kesip

çıkabileceğimi biliyorum. Verdiğim bilgilerin bilimsel amaçlı yayımlarda kullanılmasını kabul

ediyorum.

Adı Soyadı Tarih İmza

....... / ....... / .......

e-posta: ……………………………………………………………………………..

109

APPENDIX G: Faculty Informed Consent

Değerli Katılımcı,

Bu çalışma Orta Doğu Teknik Üniversitesi (ODTÜ) Psikoloji Bölümü’nde yüksek lisans eğitimi

alan Mehmet Gültaş tarafından, Yard. Doç. Dr. Yonca Toker gözetmenliğinde yürütülen yüksek lisans

tezinin bir parçası olarak uygulanmaktadır. Çalışmanın amacı kişiliğe ait özellikler ve iş ortamında yer

alan belirli değişkenlerin iş performansı ile ilişkisini incelemektir. Anketimizde doğru/yanlış ya da

iyi/kötü cevap yoktur, vereceğiniz yanıtlardaki samimiyetiniz bilimsel bilginin doğruluğuna, çalışmanın

da geçerli ve güvenilir olmasına katkı sağlayacaktır.

Katılmayı kabul ettiğiniz takdirde, cevaplaması yaklaşık 5 dakika sürecek bir soru formunu

doldurmanız istenecektir. Bu soru formunda sizinle birlikte çalışan bir araştırma görevlisinin iş ile ilgili

davranışlarını değerlendirmeniz istenmektedir. Değerlendirmeleriniz değerlendirdiğiniz kişi ile

paylaşılmayacaktır. Bu nedenle sorulara içtenlikle cevap vermeniz çalışmamızda sağlıklı veriler elde

edilmesi açısından önem taşımaktadır. Verdiğiniz cevapların araştırmacı tarafından araştırma

görevlisinin verdiği cevaplarla eşleştirilebilmesi için araştırma görevlisine ait bir rumuz kullanılacaktır.

Sizin kişisel bilgileriniz istenmemektedir.

Anketler genel olarak kişisel rahatsızlık verecek soruları içermemektedir. Ancak, katılım

sırasında sorulardan ya da herhangi başka bir nedenden ötürü kendinizi rahatsız hissederseniz anketi

cevaplamayı yarıda bırakabilirsiniz. Böyle bir durumda çalışmayı uygulayan kişiye, devam etmek

istemediğinizi söylemeniz yeterli olacaktır. Toplanan bütün bilgiler ODTÜ’de güvenli bir şekilde

korunacaktır.

Çalışma hakkında daha fazla bilgi almak için ODTÜ Psikoloji Bölümü’nden Yard. Doç. Dr.

Yonca Toker (e-posta: [email protected]) veya Arş. Gör. Mehmet Gültaş (e-posta:

[email protected]) ile iletişime geçebilirsiniz.

Bu çalışmaya tamamen gönüllü olarak katılıyorum ve istediğim zaman yarıda kesip

çıkabileceğimi biliyorum. Verdiğim bilgilerin bilimsel amaçlı yayımlarda kullanılmasını kabul

ediyorum.

Adı Soyadı Tarih İmza

....... / ....... / .......

e-posta: ……………………………………………………………………………..

110

APPENDIX H: Debriefing Form

Bu araştırmanın temel amacı iş performansını yordayan bir ölçek oluşturmaktır. Psikoloji

yazınına göre kişiliğin iş performansını yordayıcı gücü (prediction power) bulunmaktadır. Yazında

geçerliliği gösterilmiş olan Beş Faktör Kişilik Modeli’nin bazı alt boyutlarının iş performansını daha

iyi yordayabileceğine dair destek mevcuttur. Araştırma görevlilerinin işlerinin içeriği ve niteliği de

dikkate alınarak Beş Faktör modelindeki kişilik tiplerinin ilgili alt boyutları seçilmiş ve bir ölçek

oluşturulmuştur. Bu çalışma ile bu ölçeğin geçerliliği ve güvenilirliği incelenmektedir. Ayrıca kişilik

ölçeğinin performansı yordama gücünün durumsal kuvveti düşük olan iş ortamlarında, durumsal

kuvveti yüksek olan iş ortamlarına kıyasla daha yüksek olacağı sayıltısı test edilmektedir. Örneğin, bir

çalışanın işlerini kendi istediği biçimde yapabilmesi durumsal kuvveti düşük olan bir iş ortamına,

işlerini yaparken çok kesin ve net yönergeler alması ise durumsal kuvveti yüksek olan bir iş ortamına

örnektir. Yazına göre, durumsal kuvveti düşük olan ortamlarda kişilik ve performans arasındaki ilişki

daha yüksektir. Bu bulgunun yeni oluşturulan kişilik ölçeği ile de tekrar etmesi beklenmektedir.

Çalışma ile ilgili bulguların en geç 2014 Aralık ayında edinilmesi beklenmektedir. Çalışma

hakkında daha fazla bilgi almak için ODTÜ Psikoloji Bölümü’nden Yard. Doç. Dr. Yonca Toker (e-

posta: [email protected]) veya Arş. Gör. Mehmet Gültaş (e-posta: [email protected]) ile iletişime

geçebilirsiniz. Çalışmamıza sağladığınız değerli katkılar için çok teşekkür ederiz.

Arş. Gör. Mehmet Gültaş

111

APPENDIX I: Facet Item Information Function Plots

Self-Discipline Scale IIF Plots

112

Excitement Seeking Scale IIF Plots

113

Altruism Scale IIF Plots

114

Achievement Striving Scale IIF Plots

115

Deliberation Scale IIF Plots

116

Assertiveness Scale IIF Plots

117

Compliance Scale IIF Plots

118

Order Scale IIF Plots

119

Openness to Ideas Scale IIF Plots

120

Assertiveness/Exhibition Scale IIF Plots

121

Dutifulness Scale IIF Plots

122

Trust Scale IIF Plots

123

APPENDIX J: Study 1 Factor Analysis Pattern Matrix

Factors

Question 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

bke147selfdi .67

bke153selfdi .67

bke125selfdi .65

bke72achstr .60

bke34selfdi .60

bke103selfdi .55

bke10selfdi .54

bke130selfdi .54

bke35selfdi .52

bke131achstr .50

bke23selfdi .50

bke71achstr .49

bke13achstr .49

bke132achstr .49

bke111achstr .49

bke81selfdi .48

bke87dutifu .48

bke46selfdi .47

bke24selfdi .47

bke156selfdi .44

bke102selfdi .44 .34

bke18achstr .43

bke58selfdi .42

bke48achstr .42

bke83selfdi .41 .32

bke154achstr .41

bke69selfdi .41

bke14activi .40 -.33

bke37achstr .39

bke11selfdi .36

bke149achstr .35

bke112activi .34

bke38activi .34

bke26activi .34

bke146order .32

bke65dutifu .32

bke109achstr

bke92assert

bke121extsee .64

bke106extsee .55

bke32otoact -.53

bke21otoact -.49

bke56otoact -.44

bke78extsee .42

bke86otoact -.40

bke67otoact -.40

bke158extsee .38

bke8otoact -.38

bke79otoact -.34

bke2activi -.34

bke108delibe .33

124

Factors

Question 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

bke25achstr

bke150altrui .63

bke134altrui .55

bke127altrui .51

bke140altrui .51

bke17compli .49

bke5compli .47

bke113altrui .47

bke155altrui .46

bke28compli .45

bke116altrui .43

bke157altrui .41

bke36altrui .36

bke115altrui .32

bke94altrui

bke44otoact

bke120dutifu .60

bke54dutifu .56

bke148achstr .52

bke139achstr .52

bke151dutifu .45 -.31

bke60achstr .40

bke99extsee .33 .39

bke107dutifu .39

bke93activi

bke133altrui

bke64delibe .63

bke19delibe .63

bke6delibe .58

bke88delibe .56

bke30delibe .48

bke61activi .46

bke137delibe .44

bke53delibe .41

bke41delibe .33 .35

bke52delibe .34

bke33otoide .31

bke76delibe .31

bke29delibe

bke1achstr

bke136assert .69

bke141assert .56

bke95assert .51

bke62assert .45

bke114assert .35

bke63compli -.60

bke75compli -.54

bke40compli -.43

bke89compli -.40

bke96compli -.37

bke104order .76

bke84order .75

bke129order .70

125

Factors

Question 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

bke145order .69

bke124order

bke73activi

bke45otoide

bke91achstr

bke122otoide .70

bke152otoide .67

bke85otoide .60

bke128otoide .58

bke105otoide .53

bke144otoide .46

bke22otoide .44

bke100otoide .38

bke68otoide .34

bke138otoide .33

bke9otoide .33

bke118delibe .32

bke4assert .55

bke117assert .47

bke50assert .46

bke16assert .45

bke74assert .45

bke3assert .40

bke39assert .40

bke135assert .36

bke98dutifu .60

bke66dutifu .59

bke31dutifu .59

bke20dutifu .58

bke77dutifu .38

bke7dutifu .31

bke143dutifu .31

bke43dutifu .31

bke55dutifu

bke42delibe

bke97delibe

bke27assert

bke90assert

bke15assert

bke47trust -.84

bke70trust -.81

bke59trust -.76

bke12trust -.47

bke82trust -.36

bke110compli -.31

bke51compli

Note: Numbers at the header of the table as follows: 1 = self-discipline, 2 = excitement seeking, 3 = altruism, 4 =

achievement striving, 5 = deliberation, 6 = assertiveness, 7 = compliance, 8 = order, 9 = openness to ideas, 10 =

assertiveness/exhibition, 11 = dutifulness, 12 = trust.

126

APPENDIX K: Tukish Summary

Günümüzde, genel yetenek testleri kadar olmasa da, kişilik ölçekleri organizasyonların

seçme süreçlerinde ciddi bir oranda kabul görmektedir (Scroggins, Thomasve Morris,

2009). Ancak, 1980’ler öncesinde durum farklıydı. Guion ve Gottier’in (1965) kişilik

ölçekleri ve iş ile ilgili değişkenler arasında herhangi bir ilişkinin olmadığını iddia

eden makalesi bu durumun nedenlerinden biri olarak gösterilebilir.

Kişilik özelliklerinin son otuz senelik süreçte iş ile ilgili bağımsız değişkenler ile

ilişkilendirilerek yapılan çalışmalarda hızlı bir artış yaşanmıştır (Hough ve Oswald,

2008). Bu hızlı ilgi artışının nedenlerinden biri, kişilik özelliklerini beş farklı boyut

kullanarak sınıflandıran Beş Faktör Modeli’dir (BFM; Costa ve McCrae, 1985). Buna

karşın, Barrick ve Mount’un (2012) da belirttiği gibi alan içerisinde hala

çözümlenmesi gereken yönler vardır. Bunlardan önemli bir tanesi de BFM’de

belirtilmiş olan faktörlerin altında yer alan ve alt boyutlar olarak isimlendirilen kişilik

özellikleri kavramlarının, iş performansını ve diğer kurumsal değişkenleri yordayıcı

gücünün anlaşılmasıdır.

Dışa dönüklük ve gelişime açıklık boyutları eğitim performansını istatistiki olarak

anlamlı bir şekilde yordar (sırasıyla, ρ = .28 and ρ = .33), duygusal denge ve yumuşak

başlılık boyutları takım çalışması ile ilgili anlamlı şekilde yordar (ρ = .22 and ρ = .34).

Öz denetim boyutu ise amirlerin yaptığı iş performansı değerlendirmelerini

yordamakta kullanılabiliyor (ρ = .31) (Barrick ve arkadaşları, 2001). Bu belirtilen

korrelasyon değerlerine uygulanan istatistiksel düzeltmeler hesaba katılmadığında ise,

kişilik özellikleri ve bu belirtilen iş ile ilgili değişkenler arasındaki ilişki .19 ve .23

arasında değişiyor.

Yordayıcı Değişkenlerle Sonuç Değişkenleri Arasında Uyum

Yukarıda da ifade edildiği gibi BFM’nin kabul edilmesi ile elde edilen ortak dil

sayesinde, araştırmacılar iş performansı ve kişilik arasındaki ilişkiye dair daha fazla

işbirliği yapma imkânına kavuştular. Buna karşılık, Ones, Dilchert, Viswesvaran ve

Judge’ın (2007) araştırmasında da vurguladığı gibi, istatistiki düzeltmeler yapıldıktan

127

sonra kişilik özellikleri ile genel iş performansı (R = .27) ve iş performansına dair

nesnel ölçümler (R = .23) arasındaki ilişkiler çok yüksek değildir.

İş performansının daha yüksek bir korrelasyon ile tahmin edilebilmesi için bazı

araştırmacılar (Jenkins ve Griffith, 2004) yordanan ve yordayan değişkenler arasında

kuramsal bir örtüşme olması gerekliliğine işaret ediyorlar. Hogan ve Holland (2003)

çalışmalarında yordayan kişilik özellikleri ile yordanan iş performansı arasında

kavramsal bir paralelliğin olması gerekliliğini göstermiştir. Araştırma yapılan konu

hakkında uzmanlığı bulunan kişilerden bağımlı değişkenleri Hogan Kişilik

Envanteri’nin yedi boyutundan bir tanesine ataması istenmiştir. Bu çalışma sonucunda

bağımlı değişken olarak uyumluluk kişilik boyutunun altında sınıflandırılan bağımsız

değişken iş performans özellikleri, uyumluluk kişilik özelliği boyutu tarafından

istatistiki olarak anlamlı bir şekilde yordanmıştır ( = .43). Kısacası, yordanan iş

performansı boyutlarının kişilik özellikleri ile ilişkisi incelenirken, seçilen iş

performansı ile kullanılacak kişilik özelliği değişkenlerinin kavramsal olarak ilişkili

olması gerekir.

Alt Boyut Düzeyinde Kestirici Değişkenler Kullanılmasının Mantıksal Temeli

Hurtz ve Donovan (2000) BFM’nin ana boyutlarından ziyade alt boyutlarının

kullanılmasının getireceği faydaları değerlendirmişlerdir. Dikkat çektikleri nokta,

farklı kişilik faktörlerinin, farklı performans boyutları veya farklı işlerle farklı

şekillerde etkileşime girdiği gerçeğidir. Çalışmalarının sonucunda ortaya çıkan netice,

tahmin edilmeye çalışılan performans boyutu ile alakalı kişilik alt boyutlarının

kullanılması gerektiğidir. Moon, Hollenbeck, Marinova, and Humphrey (2008) dışa

dönüklük faktörünün alt boyutlarını kullaranak kurumsal vatandaşlık davranışını

açıklamaya çalıştıkları bir çalışma yapmışlardır. Dışa dönüklük faktörü ve kendi

ürettikleri alt boyutların kurumsal vatandaşlık davranışı ile ilişkilerin incelendi.

Hiyerarşik regresyon analizi kullanarak yaptıkları karşılaştırmalarda, dışa dönüklük

faktörü ile sosyallik alt boyutunun kurumsal vatandaşlık davranışı ile ilişkili

çıkmazken, tepkisellik ve olumlu duygular alt boyutları kurumsal vatandaşlık

davranışı ile ilişkili çıkmıştır. Bu araştırmanın sonuçları da dışa dönüklük faktörü tek

başına alındığında yordama gücünün olmayabileceğini gösteren kanıtlar olarak

128

değerlendirilebilir. Bir bütün olarak ele alındığında, görgül çalışmalar BFM’nin alt

boyutları kullanılarak araştırma yapılmasının görev performansının tahmini açısından

umut vaadeden bir alan olduğuna işaret etmektedir.

İş Disiplini Bütünleşik Kişilik Ölçeği

Çalışmada kullanılacak BFM alt boyutlarının belirlenmesi için yazın içerisinde iş

performansı ve benzer kurumsal bağımlı değişkenlerle ilişki gösteren alt boyutlar

belirlenmiştir. Bu yazın araştırması sırasında Johnson (2003) tarafından kavramsal

olarak çerçevesi çizilen iş performansı boyutları kullanılmıştır. Belirlenen iş

performansı boyutlarını bağımlı değişken olarak kullanan çalışmalar araştırılmış,

böylelikle seçilecek kişilik alt boyutları belirlenmiştir. İş performansı boyutları

belirlenirken kişisel girişkenlik, kişisel çabayı devam ettirebilme veya kişisel gelişime

önem verme gibi kişilik ile daha fazla ilişki göstereceği değerlendirilen iş performansı

boyutları seçilmiştir.

Öz Denetim ve Alt Boyutları

BFM faktörlerinden öz denetim, farklı işler ve farklı mesleklerle en fazla ilişki

gösteren faktör olarak ortaya çıkmaktadır (Barrick ve arkadaşları, 2001). Dolayısıyla,

bu faktör ve alt boyutları iş ile ilgili değişkenlerin tahmin edilmesinde ilk akla gelen

faktördür. Dudley, Orvis, Lebiecki, and Cortina (2006) öz disiplin faktörü alt

boyutlarından başarı için çabalamak (r = .13 ve r = .20) ve güvenilirlik (r = .09 ve r

= .23) alt boyutlarının görev performansı ve işe adanmışlık ile ilişkili olduklarını

bulmuşlardır. Dudley ve arkadaşları (2006) öz denetim faktörünün belirli alt

boyutlarının kullanılmasını tavsiye etmiştir. Bunlar başarılı için çabalamak, düzen,

tedbirlilik ve güvenilirlik alt boyutlarıdır. Bu alt boyutlara ek olarak yazında

yordayıcılığı gösterilen görev bilinci alt boyutu da çalışmada kullanılacak alt boyutlar

arasına dahil edilmiştir.

Dışa Dönüklük ve Alt Boyutları

Judge ve arkadaşları (2013) araştırmasın dışa dönüklük alt boyutları ile görev

performansı arasında pozitif ilişki buldu. Hareketlilik, heyecan arama ve ısrarcılık

görev performansını yordayan alt boyutlar olarak belirlendi. Bu çalışmada hareketlilik

129

ve heyecan arama alt boyutları görev performansı ile pozitif yönde ilişkili çıkmış

olsalar da, yazında genellikle bu alt boyutlar ve görev performansı arasında negatif

yönlü ilişkiler bulunmuştur. Denis, Morin ve Guindon (2010) heyecan arama ve görev

performansı arasında anlamlı negatif ilişki (r = -.27) rapor etmişlerdir.

Yazın dikkate alındığında ısrarcılık alt boyutunu görev performansı ile olumlu,

hareketlilik ve heyecan arama alt boyutlarının ise iş performansı ile olumsuz bir ilişki

göstermesi beklenmektedir. Bu nedenle bu alt boyutlar oluşturulacak bütünleşik kişilik

ölçeği içerisine dahil edileceklerdir.

Gelişime Açıklık ve Alt Boyutları

Judge ve arkadaşları (2013) tarafından yapılan araştırmanın sonuçları değerlere,

fikirlere ve eylemlere açıklık alt boyutlarının görev performansı ile ilişkili olduğunu

göstermiştir. Mussel ve arkadaşları (2011) fikirlere açıklık alt boyutunun iş

performansı, akademik performans ve iş arkadaşları tarafından yapılan iş yetkinliği

değerlendirmeleri ile ilişkili olduğunu bulmuştur. Bu bulgular ışığında, oluşturulacak

olan bütünleşik kişilik ölçeği içerisine eylemlere ve fikirlere açıklık alt boyutlarının

dahil edilmesine karar verilmiştir.

Uyumluluk ve Alt Boyutları

Judge ve arkadaşları (2013) çalışması, güven ve boyun eğme alt boyutlarının görev

performansı ile en yüksek ilişkisini göstermektedir. Ek olarak yardımseverlik alt

boyutunun da bütünleşik kişilik ölçeği içerisinde yer almasının uygun olacağı

düşünülmüştür. Uyumluluk faktörünün boyun eğme, yardımseverlik ve güven alt

boyutlarının BKÖ’de yer alması gerektiğine karar verilmiştir.

Sonuç olarak, görev performansını yordamak için geliştirilecek BKÖ içerisinde öz

disiplin faktörü içerisinden beş alt boyut (başarı için çabalama, tedbirlilik, görev

bilinci, düzenlilik ve öz disiplin), dışa dönüklük faktörü içerisinden üç alt boyut

(ısrarcılık, heyecan arama, hareketlilik), gelişime açıklık faktörü içerisinden iki alt

boyut (fikirlere ve hareketlere açıklık) ve uyumluluk faktörü içerisinden üç alt boyut

(boyun eğme, yardımseverlik, güven) dahil edilmiştir.

130

Çalışmada geliştirilecek BKÖ’nün ilgili BFM faktörlerinden daha iyi bir yordayıcı

geçerlilik değeri elde edeceği beklenmektedir. Aşağıda belirtilen sayıltılar bu durumu

test etmek için öne sürülmüşlerdir.

Sayıltı 1: Geliştirilecek olan bütünleşik kişilik ölçeği görev performansını, BFM

faktörlerinden daha kuvvetli bir şekilde yordayacaktır.

Sayıltı 2: Geliştirilecek bütünleşik kişilik ölçeği, görev performansında BFM

faktörlerinin açıkladığından daha fazla varyans açıklayacaktır.

Durumların ve Durumsal Sıkılığın Etkileri

Durumsal kuvvet çevrenin veya durumların nitelikleri ile ilgili bir kavramsal

çerçevedir. Çevrenin veya durumların uygun olan davranışlar hakkında ne ölçüde

işaretler verdiğini betimlemek için geliştirilmiştir. Bu kavramın bu çalışmada

kullanıldığı biçimi ile kavramsallaştırılması ve kuramsal bir yapıya kavuşturulması

Meyer, Dalal ve Hermida (2010) tarafından yapılmıştır. Genel olarak, kavramın Meyer

ve arkadaşları tarafından yayımlanan bir seri araştırma raporu ve makale ile kuramsal

ve görgül altyapısı oluşturulmuştur (Meyer ve Dalal, 2009; Meyer, Dalal ve Bonaccio,

2009; Meyer, Dalal ve Hermida, 2010; Meyer ve arkadaşları, 2014).

Meyer ve arkadaşları (2014) durumsal sıkılığı dört alt boyut içerecek şekilde

kavramsallaştırdılar. Bunlar açıklık, tutarlılık, kısıtlamalar ve sonuçlar alt boyutlarıdır.

Açıklık bir iş yerinde iş ile ilgili sorumluluk ve yükümlülüklere dair bilgilerin ne kadar

ulaşılabilir ve anlaşılması kolay olduğu ile alakalıdır. Tutarlılık bir iş yerinde iş ile

ilgili sorumluluk ve yükümlülüklere ilişkin bilgilerin birbiriyle ne düzeyde uyumlu

olduğu ile ilgili bir alt boyuttur. Kısıtlamalar alt boyutu bir iş yerinde çalışanın karar

alma ve harekete geçme serbestliğinin ne ölçüde kendi dışındaki faktörler tarafından

sınırlandırıldığını açıklamak için kullanılmaktadır. Dördüncü alt boyut olan sonuçlar

ise çalışanın davranışlarının veya kararlarının, ilgili herhangi bir kişi veya kurum için

ne ölçüde olumlu veya olumsuz sonuçları olduğuna dair ölçüm yapmak amacıyla

tasarlanmış bir alt boyuttur. Meyer ve arkadaşları (2009) yaptıkları meta-analiz

çalışmasında öz disiplin ve iş performansı arasında O*NET üzerinde durumsal sıkılık

ölçütlerinde zayıf olarak nitelendirilen işlerde anlamlı ilişki buldular. Bu çalışmada, iş

131

performansı bir bağımlı değişken olarak ele alınmıştır ve öz disiplin iş performansı

arasındaki ilişki kuvvetinin .09 ve .23 arasında değişiklik gösterdiği bulunmuştur. Bu

bulgular ışında, durumsal sıkılığın bütünleşik kişilik ölçeği ile etkileşime gireceği ve

iş performansı üzerinde düzenleyici etkisi olacağı düşünülmektedir. Daha açık bir

ifade ile:

Sayıltı 3: Bütünleşik kişilik ölçeği ve iş performansı arasındaki ilişkinin düşük

durumsal sıkılık gösteren ortamlarda daha yüksek çıkacağı beklenmektedir.

Madde Tepki Kuramı Uygulaması

Madde tepki kuramı (MTK) klasik test kuramının (KTK), madde parametrelerinin

veya kişi ölçek puanlarının değişmezliği gibi eksikliklerini gidermek için geliştirilmiş

bir kuram olarak değerlendirilebilir. Temelinde, KTK’dan farklı olarak test

katılımcılarının skorlarının toplamı ile değil de, her bir maddenin katılımcıların

yetenekleri ile ne şekilde ilişkilendirilebileceği ile ilgilenir. Temelinde MTK

maddelerin ve kişilerin özelliklerinin doğru cevap veya istenilen yönde cevap

verilmesi ile ilişkilendirildiği modellerden oluşmuştur. Modellerin, kişilerin cevap

örüntülerine oturtulması işlemi sonrasında, maddeler için zorluk, ayırt edicilik veya

tahmin etme gibi farklı parametreler hesaplanır. Kısacası, KTK’da madde özellikleri

dikkate alınmazken, MTK’da madde özellikleri de model oluşturma sürecinde hesaba

katılır. Madde tepki kuramı kişilik ölçeklerinin geliştirilmesinde sıklıkla kullanılmasa

bile genel yetenek ve başarı testlerinin geliştirilmesinde yoğun bir şekilde

kullanılmaktadır. Bu çalışmada MTK modellerinden elde edilecek ayırt edicilik

parametreleri kullanılarak, ayırt edicilikleri yüksek maddeler havuzu oluşturulacaktır.

Bu yöntemle seçilecek maddelerin oluşturulacak BKÖ’nün yordama gücüne ne ölçüde

katkı yapacağı çalışma sırasında incelenecektir.

ÇALIŞMA 1 YÖNTEM

Çalışma 1’in temel amaçları: 1) iş performansını tahmin etmek için BFM’nin alt

boyutlarını ölçen bir ölçek geliştirmek ve 2) geliştirilen ölçeğin psikometrik

özelliklerini KTK ve MTK yöntemleri kullanarak geliştirmek için veri toplamaktır.

132

Yöntem

Bütünleşik kişilik ölçeğinin kavramsal çerçevesini oluşturan alt boyutların maddeleri

Uluslararası Kişilik Maddeleri Havuzu’ndan (International Personality Item Pool;

IPIP) derlenmiştir. Bu internet platformu 1996 yılında çevrimiçi hizmete girmiştir ve

farklı kişilik boyutlarını kapsayan 2400 civarında maddeye ev sahipliği yapmaktadır.

Çalışma 1 ile toplanan veriler kullanılarak, ölçeğin psikometrik özellikleri

incelenmiştir. Gerekli görüldüğü durumlarda açımlayıcı faktör analizi (AFA) ve iç

geçerlilik katsayıları kullanılarak bazı maddeler elenmiştir.

Katılımcılar ve Süreç

KTK ile ilişkili analizlerin yapılması için veri toplama işlemi daha çok Orta Doğu

Teknik Üniversitesi (ODTÜ) öğrencileri kullanılarak yapılmıştır. Toplamda 423

katılımcının verileri istatistik analizlerinde kullanılmıştır. Çalışma

katılımcılarının %62’si kadındır. Ortalama yaş 22.2, ortanca yaş 23 olarak

hesaplanmıştır.

Ölçekler

BFM’nin boyutlarını ölçmek için Sümer ve Sümer (2002) tarafından Türkçe’ye

uyarlanan Beş Faktör Envanteri (BFE; Big Five Inventory, BFI) (John, Donahueve

Kentle, 1991) kullanılmıştır. Toplam 44 sorudan oluşan bu envanter dışadönüklük,

yumuşak başlılık, öz denetim, duygusal denge ve gelişime açıklık boyutlarını ölmek

için kullanılmaktadır.

Öncelikle, bütünleşik kişilik ölçeğinin (BKÖ) geliştirilmesi için IPIP üzerinde bulunan

alt boyut ve boyut maddeleri teker teker incelenmiştir. İlk aşamada seçilen BFM alt

boyutlarını (bunlar başarı için çalışma, güvenilirlik, düzen, öz disiplin, görev bilinci,

ısrarcılık, hareketlilik, heyecan arama, güven, yardımseverlik, boyun eğme, harekete

açıklık ve fikirlere açıklıktır) anlamsal olarak yansıttığı düşünülen 621 madde

belirlenmiştir. Seçilen maddeler arasından aşağıda açıklanan indirgeyici bir süreç

sonucunda ölçeği oluşturacak maddeler belirlenmiştir.

Belirtilen maddeler, iki endüstri ve örgüt psikoloğu tarafından ‘tekrar etme’,

‘belirsizlik’, ‘kavramsal eşdeğerlilik’, ‘örnekleme bağlılık’ veya ‘kavramsal

133

alakasızlık’ kıstasları kullanılarak bir eleme sürecinden geçirilmiştir. Bu seçim

kıstasları ışığında 467 madde elenmiştir. Kalan maddeler yazar tarafından

İngilizce’den Türkçe’ye çevrilmiştir. Türkçe’ye çevrilen bu maddeler her iki dili

anadili olarak kullanan biri tarafından İngilizce’ye geri çevrilmiştir. Bu geri çevrilen

maddeler ilk biçimleri ile karşılaştırılmış ve geri çevirme sürecinden sonra anlamının

bozulmadığına karar verilen maddeler seçilerek 150 maddelik ölçek oluşturulmuştur.

Bu 150 madde Ek A’da görülebilir.

ÇALIŞMA 1’İN SONUÇLARI

Veri toplama aşamasından sonra oluşturulan ilk ölçeğin yapısının belirlenmesi için

açımlayıcı faktör analizi (AFA) yapılmıştır. AFA tamamlandıktan sonra da ölçek

üzerinde MTK çözümlemeleri yapılmış her bir madde için madde ayırdedicilik ve

kategori eşik parametreleri elde edilmiştir. Bunlara ek olarak bu parametreler

kullanılarak her maddenin madde bilgi fonksiyonu (MBF) grafiği oluşturulmuştur.

AFA

AFA’nın en önemli yönünü, analiz sonucunda ortaya çıkacak faktör sayısına karar

vermek oluşturmaktadır. Bu çalışmada O’Connor (2000) tarafından öne sürülen

paralel analizi yöntemi kullanılarak ortaya çıkarılması gereken faktör sayısı

belirlenmiştir. Paralel analiz sonuçları, eldeki veriler kullanılarak on dört tane faktörün

oluşturulmasının uygun olacağını işaret etmiştir.

AFA IBM SPSS 22 kullanılarak yapılmıştır. Her ne kadar, analiz ondört faktöre

zorlanmış olsa da, sonuçlar değerlendirildiğinde on iki faktörün ortaya çıktığı

görülmüştür. Faktör analizi sonucunda elde edilen faktörler toplam

varyansın %43.82’sini açıklamıştır. Elde edilen faktörler, faktöre en fazla yüklenen

maddelerin anlamları dikkate alınarak isimlendirilmiştir. Faktör analizi sonucunda

elde edilen yüklemeler Ek J’de yer alan tabloda görülebilir.

Birinci faktör öz disiplin, ikinci faktör heyecan arama, üçüncü faktör yardımseverlik,

dördüncü faktör başarmak için çabalama, beşinci faktör ihtiyatlılık, altıncı faktör

ısrarcılık, yedinci faktör uyumluluk, sekizinci faktör düzenlilik, dokuzuncu faktör

134

fikirlere açıklık, onuncu faktör ısrarcılık/gösterişçilik, on birinci faktör görev bağlılığı

ve on ikinci faktör güven olarak isimlendirilmiştir. Birinci faktörden başlayarak,

isimlendirilen faktörlerin madde sayıları sırasıyla 34, 12, 13, 7, 10, 5, 5, 4, 12, 8, 8 ve

6 olarak ortaya çıkmıştır. Toplamda 150 olan maddeden, AFA sonucunda 124 tanesi

elde tutulmuştur. Bu sayıya, analiz sonucunda .30 üzerinde yüklemesi olmayan

maddeler, birden fazla faktöre benzer şekilde yüklenen 23 madde elenerek ve ayrıca,

bir faktöre çok düşük şekilde yüklenen üç madde de analizlerden çıkartılarak

ulaşılmıştır.

MTK Çözümlemeleri

Araştırmanın MTK çözümlemeleri R istatistik ortamı (R Core Team, 2014) için

hazırlanan mirt paketi kullanılarak yapılmıştır (Chalmers, 2012). Maddelerin

parametrelerinin hesaplanmasında Samejima (1969) tarafından geliştirilen aşamalı

tepki modeli (Graded response model; GRM) kullanılmıştır. Bu model ile madde ayırt

edicilikleri, kategori eşik parametreleri hesaplanılabilmekte ve elde edilen bu

parametrelerden madde bilgi fonksiyonlarının grafikleri çizilebilmektedir. Elde edilen

parametreler ve maddeler ile ilgili çeşitli endeksler dikkate alınarak açımlayıcı faktör

analizinden sonra bazı maddeler atılarak yeni bir ölçek oluşturulmuştur. Madde

seçerken maddelerin görece yüksek ayırdedicilik endeksine sahip olması, madde bilgi

fonksiyon grafiğinin 0.5 değerini geçmesi ve chi-kare’nin bağımsızlık derecesine

oranının 3’ü geçmemesi gibi ön kabulleri karşılamasına dikkat edilmiştir. Belirtilen

ölçütler dikkate alınarak, öz disiplin’de yirmi sekiz, heyecan aramada yedi,

yardımseverlikte yedi, başarı için çabalamada iki, ihtiyatlılıkta sekiz, ısrarcılıkta üç,

boyun eğmede üç, düzenlilikte dört, fikirlere açıklıkta beş, ısrarcılık/gösterişçilikte

altı, göreve bağlılıkta dört ve güven alt boyutunda üç madde seçilmiştir. Böylece, KTK

kullanılarak madde sayısı 124’e ve MTK kullanılarak madde sayısı 80’e indirilmiştir.

ÇALIŞMA 2 YÖNTEM VE SONUÇLARI

Çalışma 2’nin amacı BKÖ ile görev performansı arasındaki ilişkiyi incelemektir. Buna

ek olarak durumsal sıkılığın görev performansı ve kişilik arasındaki ilişkiyi

135

yönlendirip yönlendirmediği de incelemektir. Bu çalışmada kullanılan yöntem ve

katılımcılar aşağıda detaylı olarak anlatılmaktadır.

Katılımcılar ve Süreç

Bu çalışmada veriler ODTÜ, Ankara, Hacettepe, Atılım, İstanbul Teknik (İTÜ), Ege,

Anadolu, Osmangazi, Bilecik ve Namık Kemal Üniversitesi’nde çalışan araştırma

görevlileri (AG) ve öğretim üyelerinden (ÖÜ) toplanmıştır. Katılan AG’ler çok temel

demografik bilgilere ek larak, Çalışma 1’de geliştirilen BKÖ’yü ve BBE haricinde,

detayları aşağıda verilen İşte Durumsal Sıkılık Ölçeği’ni doldurmuşlardır. AG’lerin en

azından bir dönem birlikte çalıştığı ÖÜ’ler de Araştırma Görevlisi Değerlendirme

Formu’nu doldurmuşlardır. Çalışmaya katılan AG’ler katılımlarının karşılığı olarak,

katılımcılar arasında yapılacak bir çekiliş ile verilecek tablet bilgisayar kazanma şansı

ve 40 TL almışlardır. Veri toplanan AG’lerin ortalama yaşı 27.8’dir ve ortalama olarak

lisansütü öğretimlerinin 4.5’inci senesinde bulunmaktadırlar.

Ölçekler

Çalışma 2’de kişilik özellikleri ile ilgili veriler Çalışma 1’de geliştirilen BKÖ ve BBE

kullanılarak toplanmıştır. Bunlara ek olarak AG’ler durumsal sıkılık değerlendirmesi

yapmak için İşte Durumsal Sıkılık Ölçeği’ni doldurmuşlardır.

İşte Durumsul Sıkılık Ölçeği Meyer ve arkadaşlarının (2014) geliştirdiği ve durumsal

sıkılığın dört alt boyutunu (açıklık, tutarlılık, kısıtlamalar ve sonuçlar) ölçen bir

ölçektir. Bu çalışma sırasında 6 maddeli Likert-tipi bir ölçek olarak kullanılmıştır. Bu

ölçek Ek E’de bulunabilir. Bu çalışma sonucunda açıklık, tutarlılık, kısıtlamalar ve

sonuçlar alt ölçekleri için hesaplanan iç geçerlilik katsayıları sırasıyla .91, .85, .87

ve .86 olarak hesaplanmıştır.

Araştırma görevlisi değerlendirme formu Öz (2003) tarafından, araştırma

görevlilerinin iş performansı değerlendirmelerinin yapılması amacıyla

oluşturulmuştur. Bu çalışmada 6 seviyeli ölçek haline getirilip kullanılmıştır. Bu ölçek

oplamda 25 sorudur ve iki alt ölçek içermektedir. Bir alt ölçek bağlamsal performansı,

diğer ölçek ise iş performansını ölçmektedir. Bu çalışmada, bağlamsal ve iş perfomansı

alt ölçeklerinin iç geçerlilik katsayıları .67 ve .91 olarak hesaplanmıştır.

136

Sonuçlar

KTK ile oluşturulan öz disiplin (r = .86, p < .01), başarı için mücadele (r = .27, p

< .05), ihtiyatlılık (r =.49, p < .01) ve düzenlilik (r = .51, p < .01) en yüksek olarak

BFE öz disiplin faktörü ile ilişkili bulunmuştur. Görev bilinci hem uyumluluk (r = .45,

p < .01) hem de öz disiplin (r = .41, p < .01) faktörleri ile istatistiki olarak anlamlı

ilişkiler göstermiştir.

Bu çalışmaya katılan 61 katılımcının verileri kullanılarak yapılan korelasyon analizleri

sonucunda, iş performansı ile istatistiki olarak anlamlı ilişkiye giren alt boyut

ölçekleri, KTK ile hazırlanan heyecan arama (r = -.26, p < .05) ve KTK ve MTK ile

hazırlanan fikirlere açıklık (r = -.26 and r = -.32, p < .05) alt boyut ölçekleridir.

Heyecan arama alt boyutu ile iş performansı arasındaki ilişki beklenildiği gibi ters

yönlü olarak bulunmuştur. Ancak, fikirlere açıklık ve iş performansı arasındaki ilişki

beklenildiği gibi çıkmamış ve ters yönlü olduğu görülmüştür.

KTK ve MTK ile oluşturulmuş BKÖ’leri geliştirmek için alt boyut ölçeklerinden

görev performansı ile ilişkisi .10 üzerinde olanlar seçilmiştir. Bu ölçek puanlarının

ortalaması alınarak bütünleşik ölçek puanları oluşturulmuştur. Bu ölçüt dikkate

alınarak yapılan seçim sonunda ısrarcılık, yardımseverlik, güven, düzen ve görev

bilinci alt ölçekleri BKÖ’nün dışına çıkarılmıştır. Heyecan arama, fikirlere açıklık ve

ısrarcılık/gösterişlilik alt boyutlarının toplam puanları ters çevrilerek ölçeğe

eklenmiştir.

Çalışmanın birinci sayıltısını test etmek için KTK yöntemleri ve MTK yöntemleri ile

hazırlanan BKÖ’lerin görev performansı ile ilişkisi karşılaştırılmıştır. Her iki kuram

temel alınarak oluşturulan bütünleşik ölçeklerin görev performansı ile aynı büyüklükte

ve istatistiki olarak anlamlı ilişki kurduğu görülmüştür (r = .36, p < .01). BBE

faktörleri ile oluşturulan bütünleşik ölçeğin görev performansı ile istatistiki olarak

anlamlı olmayan aynı yönlü ilişkisi gözlemlenmiştir (r = .25). Bunun yanında, BBE

öz disiplin faktörü de görev performansı ile istatistiki olarak anlamlı bir pozitif ilişki

göstermiştir (r = .28, p < .05). Bu sonuçlar çalışmanın birinci sayıltısının

doğrulandığına işaret etmektedir.

137

Araştırmanın ikinci sayıltısını test etmek için hiyerarşik regresyon analizi yapılmıştır.

Bu analizin amacı çalışmada geliştirilen bütünleşik ölçeğinin BBE faktörleri

kullanılarak oluşturulan bütünleşik ölçekten fazla varyans açıklayıp açıklamayacağını

ortaya çıkartmaktır. Analizin birinci adımında BBE bütünleşik ölçeği, ikinci

adımından da MTK kullanılarak geliştirilen BKÖ analize dahil edilmiştir. BBE (β

= .25, p = .05) girilerek oluşturulan modelin (F(1, 59) = 3.92, p = .05) sınırda anlamlığa

sahip olduğu görülmüştür. İkinci adımda MTK ile hazırlanan BKÖ’nün (ΔR2 = .065,

ΔF(1, 58) = 4.33, p = .04) eklendiği modelin anlamlı olduğu ve birinci adıma ek

olarak %7 fazladan varyans açıkladığı görülmüştür. Bu bulgular ışığında, araştırmanın

ikinci sayıltısının desteklendiği kabul edilebilir.

Durumsal Sıkılığın Etkileri

Durumsal sıkılığın, kişilik özellikleri ve görev performansı arasındaki ilişkiyi nasıl

etkilediğini test etmek amacıyla bir dizi regresyon analizi yapılmıştır. Bu analizler,

durumsal sıkılık değişkenlerinin kişilik ile görev performansı arasındaki ilişkiyi

istatistiki olarak anlamlı bir şekilde etkilemediğini göstermiştir. Bu analize ek olarak

katılımcılar, durumsal sıkılık alt boyutlarındaki skorları dikkate alınarak, düşük, orta

ve yüksek durumsal sıkılık gruplarına ayrılmış ve bu gruplar içerisinde kişilik

özelliklerinin görev performansı ile ilişkisinin ne yönde olduğu incelenmiştir. Bu

inceleme sonunda, sonuçlar alt boyutunun düşük durumsal sıkılık grubunda kişilik ve

görev performansı arasında anlamlı ve pozitif yönlü bir ilişki bulunduğu görülmüştür

(r = .48, p < .05). Açıklık, tutarlılık ve kısıtlamalar alt boyutlarında ise, beklentinin

aksine, kişilik ile görev performansı arasındaki ilişki yüksek durumsal sıkılık grubu

içerisinde istatistiki olarak anlamlıdır (sırasıyla, r = .58, p < .01; r = .40, p < .05; r

= .43, p < .05).

MTK Yöntemlerinin Kullanılması

Bu çalışmada madde ayırt edicilik parametlerinin hesaplanması için MTK modelleri

kullanılmıştır. Bu parametrelerden, ilgilenilen kişilik özelliklerini en fazla ayırt eden

maddelerin seçimi için yararlanılmıştır. Bir araştırma sorusu olarak, KTK ve MTK

kullanılarak oluşturulan BKÖ’nün tahmin etme kuvvetleri karşılaştırılmıştır. Yapılan

istatistik analizleri göstermiştir ki, her iki yaklaşım kullanılarak oluşturulan bütünleşik

138

ölçek ve sadece KTK kullanılarak oluşturulan BKÖ’nün iş performansı ile olan

korelasyonları aynıdır (r = .36, p < .05).

TARTIŞMA

Kişilik Alt Boyutlarının Kullanılması

Çalışmanın sayıltılarından bir tanesi BKÖ ile görev performansının BFM

faktörlerinden daha iyi bir şekilde tahmin edilebileceğiydi. BFM faktörlerinden

oluşturulan bütünleşik ölçek görev performansını anlamlı bir şekilde yordamazken, bu

çalışma ile oluşturulan BKÖ görev performansını istatistiki olarak anlamlı bir biçimde

yordadı. Ayrıca, yapılan regresyon analizleri çalışmada oluşturulan BKÖ’nün, BFM

faktörlerinden oluşturulan bütünleşik ölçeğin açıkladığı varyanstan da fazla varyans

açıkladığını göstermiştir. Bu da alt boyut seviyesindeki kişilik değişkenlerinin BFM

faktörlerinin açıkladığından daha fazla varyans açıklayabileceğine işaret etmektedir.

Bu çalışmanın en önemli katkısı, görev performansının tahmin edilmesinde alt boyut

seviyesinde kişilik özelliklerinin, faktörlerin kendisinden veya faktörler kullanılarak

oluşturulan bütünleşik ölçeklerden daha faydalı olabileceğini ortaya çıkarmış

olmasıdır. Buna ek olarak, ticari kişilik ölçeklerine erişimi olmayan kişi ve kurumların

da, internet üzerinde sunulan maddeleri kullanarak ölçekler oluşturabileceği ve bunları

seçme süreçlerinde kullanabileceği gösterilmiştir.

Çalışmada Ortaya Çıkan İş Performansı ve Alt Boyutlar Arasındaki İlişki

Örüntüsü

Fikirlere açıklık alt boyutu ile görev performansı arasındaki ilişki yazın ışığında

beklenmeyen bir bulgudur. Boyun eğme alt boyutununda görev performansı ile ilişkisi

beklenilenden yüksektir. Israrcılık/gösteriş alt boyutunun ilişkisi de her ne kadar farklı

yönde olsa ve istatistiksel olarak anlamlı olmasa da beklenmeyen bir bulgudur. Bu

bulgular Hofstede’in (1980a) kültürel değer boyutları ışığında değerlendirilebilir.

Türkiye güç mesafesi ve belirsizlikten kaçınma boyutlarında yüksek seviyelerde yer

almaktadır (Daller ve Yıldız, 2006; Hofstede, 1980a). Türk kültürünün içersinde yer

alan üniversitelerimizde boyun eğen ve çok fazla ısrarcılık göstermeyen ve aynı

zamanda yeni fikirlere açıklık konusunda da fazla istekli olmayan AG’lerin amirleri

139

konumundaki ÖÜ’ler tarafından daha olumlu olarak değerlendirilmiş olma olasılığı

vardır ve bulgular bu şekilde açıklanabilir.

Kişilik Ölçeğinin Geliştirilmesinde Madde Tepki Kuramı’nın Kullanımı

MTK modelleri kişilik ölçümünde yeterince yaygın kullanılmamaktadır (Chiesi, Galli,

Primi, Borgive Bonacchi, 2013) ve bu aşartırma bu yönde atılmış bir adım olarak kabul

edilebilir. Hesaplanan MTK model parametreleri örneklemlerden bağımsız olduğu için

oluşturulan ölçeklerin genellenebilirliği açısından fayda sağlayabilirler. MTK

modelleri sadece ölçeklerde kullanılan maddelerin incelenmesi için değil, bu

çalışmada uygulandığı gibi, aynı zamanda incelenen maddeler kullanılarak ölçekler

geliştirilmesi için de fayda sağlayabilir.

Kişilik İş Performansı İlişkisinde Bir Ara Değişken Olarak Durumsal Sıkılık

Bu çalışmada, durumsal sıkılığın kişilik özellikleri ve görev performansı ilişkisine

etkisi regresyon analizleri kullanılarak test edilmiştir. Regresyon analizleri sonucunda

durumsal sıkılığın herhangi bir anlamlı etkisi ortaya çıkartılamamıştır. Bu bulgu elde

edildikten sonra, katılımcılar durumsal sıkılık düzeylerine göre düşük, orta ve yüksek

olarak ayrılmış ve bu grupların içerisinde kişilik özellikleri ve görev performansı

arasındaki ilişki incelenmiştir. Bu inceleme sonucunda, farklı gruplarda aradaki

ilişkinin farklılaştığı görülmüştür. Sonuçlar alt boyutunda ilişki beklenildiği yöndedir.

Bu gruplar incelendiğinde, açıklık alt boyutunun kişilik ve görev performansı ilişkisine

yaptığı etki tartışılmaya değerdir. Özellikle açıklık boyutunda, durumsal sıkılık

arttıkça artan aynı yönlü bir ilişki göze çarpmaktadır. Lock ve Latham (2002)

tarafından geliştirilen hedef belirleme kuramı (HBK) bu duruma bir açıklama

sağlayabilir. Daha açık ortamlar hedeflerin daha belirgin olduğu ortamlar yaratmış

olabilir. Böyle durumlarda kişilik özelliği olarak çalışma eğilimi olan kişiler hedefleri

benimseyerek daha fazla çalışmış ve ÖÜ’ler tarafından daha yüksek performans

gösterdikleri şeklinde değerlendirilmiş olabilirler.

Uygulamaya Yönelik Öneriler

Alt boyut seviyesinde kişilik değişkenlerinin kullanılması, seçme süreçlerinde

kullanılan kişilik ölçeklerinin kısaltılmasına imkân sağlayabilir. Sadece iş ile ilgili

140

kişilik alt boyutları kullanılarak oluşturulacak kişilik ölçekleri doğal olarak bütün

kişilik alt boyutlarını kullanarak geliştirilen envanterlerden daha kısa olabilir. Buna ek

olarak, MTK yöntemlerinin de ölçeklerin kısaltılmasına imkân verebileceği bu

araştırmada gösterilmiştir. Ölçeklerin uygulama süresinin kısa olması, uygulayıcıların

kullanılan ölçekleri benimsemesine ve kişilik ölçeklerinin daha fazla kullanılmasına

yardımcı olabilir.

Çalışmanın Kısıtlılıkları

Çalışma 1 için toplamda 423 kişilik bir örneklem grubu kullanılmıştır. Bu sayı madde

tepki kuramı çözümlemelerinin daha sağlıklı yapılabilmesi için daha fazla olabilirdi.

Çalışma 2’de kullanılan 61 kişilik örneklem grubu da çalışmada elde edilen ilişkilerin

örneklem dışına genellenmesini zora soktuğundan, örneklemdeki katılımcı sayısının

arttırılması araştırma sonuçlarının tekrarlanabilirliği açısından faydalı olacaktır.

Ayrıca, Çalışma 2’de kullanılan AG örnekleminin çok eğitimli olduğu da, sonuçların

değerlendirilmesi sırasında göz önünde bulundurulmalıdır.

İleride Yapılacak Araştırmalar için Öneriler

Bu çalışmada MTK yöntemlerinin ve parametrelerinin kullanımı oluşturulan

bütünleşik kişilik ölçeğinin kısaltılması açısından yardımcı olmuştur. Ancak, MTK

yöntemleri ile elde edilen parametrelerin gerçekten verimli olacağı temel alan

bilgisayarlı testlerdir. Bu nedenle kişilik testlerinin bilgisayarlı uyarlanabilen testler

haline getirilerek kullanılması da çalışılması gereken bir diğer araştırma alanıdır.

BFM’nin alt boyutlarının iş performansı dışında kullanım alanları da göz ardı

edilmemelidir. Bağlamsal performans ve üretkenliğe zarar veren davranışların, alt

boyutlar kullanılarak kestirimine dair araştırmalar da kurumlar açısında önemli

faydalar doğuracak ilişkilerin ortaya çıkarılmasına katkı sağlayabilir.

141

APPENDIX L: Tez Fotokopisi İzin Formu

ENSTİTÜ

Fen Bilimleri Enstitüsü

Sosyal Bilimler Enstitüsü

Uygulamalı Matematik Enstitüsü

Enformatik Enstitüsü

Deniz Bilimleri Enstitüsü

YAZARIN

Soyadı : Gültaş

Adı : Mehmet

Bölümü : Psikoloji

TEZİN ADI (İngilizce) : Work discipline compound personality scale

development with item response theory.

TEZİN TÜRÜ : Yüksek Lisans Doktora

1. Tezimin tamamından kaynak gösterilmek şartıyla fotokopi alınabilir.

2. Tezimin içindekiler sayfası, özet, indeks sayfalarından ve/veya bir

bölümünden kaynak gösterilmek şartıyla fotokopi alınabilir.

3. Tezimden bir bir (1) yıl süreyle fotokopi alınamaz.

TEZİN KÜTÜPHANEYE TESLİM TARİHİ:

x

x

X

x

X

x


Recommended