+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture€¦ · Musical-Space Synaesthesia:...

Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture€¦ · Musical-Space Synaesthesia:...

Date post: 30-Jun-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 17 times
Download: 1 times
Share this document with a friend
7
Multisensory Research 30 (2017) 279–285 brill.com/msr Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture Svetlana Rudenko 1,2,and Maria José de Córdoba Serrano 1 1 Faculty of Fine Arts, University of Granada, Spain 2 School of Computer Science and Statistics, Trinity College Dublin, College Green, Dublin, Ireland Received 1 October 2016; accepted 22 February 2017 Abstract I, Svetlana Rudenko, am a concert pianist. I am also a synaesthete and for me, sound is visual. It has shape. According to recent research (Akiva-Kabiri et al., 2014, pp. 17–29): “In musical-space synaesthesia, . . . unlike the vertical and horizontal representation of musical pitch tones in the gen- eral population, synaesthetes describe a linear diagonal organisation of pitch tones.” Different piano sounds have aroma and texture. The forms I have lived with since childhood are what I call ‘sound landscapes’. I am not alone. Composers such as Liszt, Scriabin, Gubaidulina and Messiaen, as well as artists such as de Córdoba Serrano and Ninghui Xiong, also experienced these impressions. This paper will explore synaesthetic experiences of musical texture, visualisation of sound and tactile sensations of musical texture as well as enhanced cross-modal associations. On the basis of my expe- riences, I argue that the visualisation of musical texture influences sound perception and even timing: phrases, dynamics and the whole interpretation. Keywords Musical-space synaesthesia, visualization, musical texture, archetype The collection of synaesthetic and cross-modal experiences here includes statements by musicologists and personal experiences of authors. For any- one who ever tried to play a musical instrument, it is obvious that music has the power to engage many different sensory systems and result in a range of sensory responses, mostly auditory. But for some of us, these responses also include the visual and tactile. * To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected] © Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2017 DOI:10.1163/22134808-00002562
Transcript
Page 1: Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture€¦ · Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture Svetlana Rudenko1,2,∗ and Maria José de Córdoba

Multisensory Research 30 (2017) 279–285 brill.com/msr

Musical-Space Synaesthesia:Visualisation of Musical Texture

Svetlana Rudenko 1,2,∗ and Maria José de Córdoba Serrano 1

1 Faculty of Fine Arts, University of Granada, Spain2 School of Computer Science and Statistics, Trinity College Dublin, College Green, Dublin,

Ireland

Received 1 October 2016; accepted 22 February 2017

AbstractI, Svetlana Rudenko, am a concert pianist. I am also a synaesthete and for me, sound is visual. Ithas shape. According to recent research (Akiva-Kabiri et al., 2014, pp. 17–29): “In musical-spacesynaesthesia, . . . unlike the vertical and horizontal representation of musical pitch tones in the gen-eral population, synaesthetes describe a linear diagonal organisation of pitch tones.” Different pianosounds have aroma and texture. The forms I have lived with since childhood are what I call ‘soundlandscapes’. I am not alone. Composers such as Liszt, Scriabin, Gubaidulina and Messiaen, as wellas artists such as de Córdoba Serrano and Ninghui Xiong, also experienced these impressions. Thispaper will explore synaesthetic experiences of musical texture, visualisation of sound and tactilesensations of musical texture as well as enhanced cross-modal associations. On the basis of my expe-riences, I argue that the visualisation of musical texture influences sound perception and even timing:phrases, dynamics and the whole interpretation.

KeywordsMusical-space synaesthesia, visualization, musical texture, archetype

The collection of synaesthetic and cross-modal experiences here includesstatements by musicologists and personal experiences of authors. For any-one who ever tried to play a musical instrument, it is obvious that music hasthe power to engage many different sensory systems and result in a range ofsensory responses, mostly auditory. But for some of us, these responses alsoinclude the visual and tactile.

* To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected]

© Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2017 DOI:10.1163/22134808-00002562

Page 2: Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture€¦ · Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture Svetlana Rudenko1,2,∗ and Maria José de Córdoba

280 S. Rudenko, M. J. de Córdoba Serrano / Multisensory Research 30 (2017) 279–285

I may feel the shape of the melody, the aroma of timbre or the taste or colourof harmony. I am aware of research in neuroscience which describes synaes-thesia as forming “links between different brain maps representing differentconcepts” (Ramachandran and Hubbard, 2001) and how “different peoplemight, quite without knowing it, subjectively perceive the world in very differ-ent ways” (Mitchell, 2010). For me as a teacher as well as a pianist, imagery inpiano pedagogy is a very important aspect of my practice and mostly manifestsin the impact of sound through touch where the musical instrument becomesan extension of my hands and feelings.

Musicologists have also documented their synaesthetic experiences. Forexample, Galeyev (2007) asserted that “synaesthesia is the essential com-ponent of musical thinking, first of all, music intended to evoke images”.Also Vuoskoski and Eerola (2015) stated that “music-induced visual im-agery refers to a process whereby a listener conjures up-either intentionally orunintentionally-visual images while listening to music.” Ward (2008, pp. 37,38) explored “Maurer’s idea that we all start life as synesthetes” and answeredthat “we cannot definitely conclude that the baby experiences synesthesia, butwe can conclude that there is a far greater mixing of the senses in infancy thanat other stages of life”. I believe this implies we all have access, on some level,to this form of thinking and that crossmodal correspondences can be used tohelp students practice music or art (Rudenko, 2015; Tilot, 2016). I believe thatsound can stimulate responses in other sensory modalities and be a creativesource in the arts.

Synaesthete artist Dr. José de Córdoba Serrano’s whose engraving ‘Soundsand Textures’ (see Fig. 1), stated that “It was in 1988, after being diagnosedwith otosclerosis, through a self-investigation on the transfer of sound to im-age I discovered the sensation. The sound had not only colors, smell or spatialposition but also texture. In most of my exhibitions, unlike other synaestheticartists who emphasize the color in their creations, in my prints I emphasize thesound texture. I hardly emphasize bright colors because in my perception, tex-tures, visual and tactile feel are predominant. As an artist, primarily a painter,engraving and visual artist, the ‘time’, as parameter, is also present, but thetexture is always marked by the rhythm of sound predominantes.” (CórdobaSerrano, 2015, p. 331).

According to Sean A. Day, Ph.D., there are more than 80 different typesof synaesthesia, suggesting there are many combinations of sensory experi-ences (Day, 2015, p. 47). My experience of music of composers followingvisual metaphors or symbolistic poetry, such as John Buckley’s composition‘The Silver Apples of the Moon, the Golden Apples of the Sun’ (which drawsits inspiration from W. B. Yeats’ poem ‘The Song of Wondering Aengus’),or young Russian composer Veronika Kuzmina’s ‘Musical Pictures to Ander-sen’s and Russian Fairy Tales’, is that the visual image evoked by the lyrics

Page 3: Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture€¦ · Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture Svetlana Rudenko1,2,∗ and Maria José de Córdoba

S. Rudenko, M. J. de Córdoba Serrano / Multisensory Research 30 (2017) 279–285 281

Figure 1. M. J. de Córdoba Serrano, Sounds and Textures.

dictates the sound and choice of the musical texture. For me, as a pianist, theselection of the repertoire which I particularly enjoy playing is based on themusical texture which gives me vivid imagery and sensation of ‘sound touch’in my hands.

Time-signature also gives me pulsations of musical texture and has a tactilesensation. I feel it in something I call ‘Cross Waves’ (see Fig. 2 for an illustra-tion). For example, in Rachmaninoff, Prelude No. 12, G sharp-minor, op. 32,the time-signature is 12/8 with four groups of six semiquavers figurations andI experience corresponding feelings in my hands. I ‘see’ the shape of melodyby the pitch and could relate my experience to what Akiva-Kabiri describedas “linear diagonal organisation of pitch tones” (Akiva-Kabiri et al., 2014).

Synaesthete composer Sofia Gubaidulina describes her similar experienceof the spatial presentation of sound as follows: “. . . I hear a huge, shape-less, multi-faceted sound, absolutely fascinating, with everything piled up

Page 4: Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture€¦ · Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture Svetlana Rudenko1,2,∗ and Maria José de Córdoba

282 S. Rudenko, M. J. de Córdoba Serrano / Multisensory Research 30 (2017) 279–285

Figure 2. S. Rudenko, Score + Art: Rachmaninoff, Prelude No. 12, G sharp-minor, op. 32.

together in a way you could never notate — something which exists outsidetime, . . . and I consider it a duty to transform it from vertical to horizontal.”(Kurtz, 2007, p. 223). In her late compositions she used the Fibonacci seriesto ‘balance musical form’, in much the way that other synaesthete composersorganise their compositional language in a way that is specific to an image(archetype in the case of Scriabin) that is audibly and visually recognisable inmusical texture. The following are examples of synaesthetic musical analysisbased on the extraction of archetypes/images, from Scriabin’s Désir, op. 57(Fig. 3).

1. Red: The Eternal Feminine, “calling for wake up”, “. . . illuminate yourself,hear your prophetic voice” (Smith, 2013, p. 54).

2. Blue: Masculine Principal, “. . . I do not see my path in the starred attire”(text from Mysterium (Morrison, 2002, p. 315)).

3. Brown: earthly chains (slurred couplets, author remark).

4. Vertical Green: The notion of mystic unity, ‘Mystic chord’ (Garcia’sarchetype, 2000).

Susanna Garcia described the six most common gesture archetypes in com-positions of Scriabin and pointed out that “Scriabin repeatedly conjoined cer-tain types of expressive language with specific musical gestures thus creatinga body of musical symbols consistent throughout . . . the late works” (Gar-cia, 2000). Miniature compositions on the piano were the laboratory work

Page 5: Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture€¦ · Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture Svetlana Rudenko1,2,∗ and Maria José de Córdoba

S. Rudenko, M. J. de Córdoba Serrano / Multisensory Research 30 (2017) 279–285 283

Figure 3. A. Scriabin’s Archetypes, Desir, op. 57.

for Scriabin in preparation for bigger orchestra compositions. He plannedhis final unfinished composition Mysterium as a multisensory drama and hisPrometheus: Poem of Fire, op. 60, as “an entirely new genre: a ‘symphony ofsound’ counterpointed by a ‘symphony of light’” (Gawboy and Townsend,2012). He took many ideas from the theosophy movement of theosophistleader Madame Blavatsky and her theory of colors.

For me, the visualisation of musical texture also changes the perception oftime. On the basis of my experience, I would argue that synaesthetes hear the

Page 6: Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture€¦ · Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture Svetlana Rudenko1,2,∗ and Maria José de Córdoba

284 S. Rudenko, M. J. de Córdoba Serrano / Multisensory Research 30 (2017) 279–285

pulsation of musical texture layers in multiple timelines. For example, John-son describes Oliver Messiaen’s texture as: “The effect is one of two strandsof music moving at different tempos, with the consequence that the functionsof tempo and duration overlap in much the same way as the functions of har-mony and timbre are made to overlap by use of added resonance” (Johnson,1989, p. 38). Anatole Leikin analyzed transcriptions of the piano rolls, whichScriabin recorded on the Hupfeld and Welte-Mignon reproducing pianos in1908 and 1910. Leikin (2011, p. 32) stated that “When Scriabin’s performingtempo fluctuates continuously and widely and yet the average tempo coincideswith the published metronome indication, it means that Scriabin keeps concur-rently, side by side, two timelines”. Leikin continues by stating that: “Thosewho heard the composer play noticed that even in the most intricate musicalfabric he separated the layers of texture so that all the voices were clearlyenunciated.” In this way Scriabin keeps what I call a ‘volumetric sculpture oflayers’.

In another document of synaesthetic experiences in music, Kievman (2012)outlined that: “Timbre, Klangtint, or tone-colouring was used with startlingoriginality by Richard Wagner during the 19th century, and raised to a sciencein the early/mid 20th century by Scriabin, Sibelius, Ives, Varese, Messiaen,Ligeti, Nono and several other composers.” Kievman refers to Hermann vonHelmholtz’s ‘On the Sensations of Tone’ and describes timbre as “wave form,amplitude, spectral envelope, the range between tonal and noise-like character,the time envelope in terms of rise, duration, and decay” (Kievman, 2012).

In conclusion, synaesthetic multisensory experiences of sound and musicaltexture remain an inspiration for creative practice and musical performance.Furthermore, synaesthetic experiences or volumetric visualisation of musi-cal texture, projected into a synesthetic ‘inner screen’ or ‘mind’s eye’ (Ward,2008, p. 14), change the perception of time and interpretation of musical el-ements. I believe these experiences could be used in pedagogy as a morecreative alternative tool, for example, to substitute the blind copying of aninstructors’ playing.

References

Akiva-Kabiri, L., Linkovski, O., Gertner, L. and Henik, A. (2014). Musical space synesthesia:automatic, explicit and conceptual connections between musical stimuli and space, Consc.Cogn. 28, 17–29.

Córdoba Serrano, M. J. (2015). Percentage of population who have possible synesthesiacolour/voice/texture, in Spanish-speaking countries, in: V International Conference: Synes-thesia: Science and Art, Alcala la Real de Jaen, Spain. DOI:10.13140/RG.2.1.2454.2884.

Day, S. A. (2015). Less common forms of synesthesia, in: V International Conference: Synes-thesia: Science and Art, Alcala la Real de Jaen, Spain, p. 47.

Page 7: Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture€¦ · Musical-Space Synaesthesia: Visualisation of Musical Texture Svetlana Rudenko1,2,∗ and Maria José de Córdoba

S. Rudenko, M. J. de Córdoba Serrano / Multisensory Research 30 (2017) 279–285 285

Galeyev, B. M. (2007). The nature and functions of synesthesia in music, Leonardo 40, 285–288.

Garcia, S. (2000). Scriabin’s symbolist plot archetype in the late piano sonatas, Nineteenth Cent.Music 23, 273–300.

Gawboy, A. M. and Townsend, J. (2012). Scriabin and the possible, MTO 18, 6–20.Johnson, R. S. (1989). Messiaen. Omnibus Press, London, UK.Kievman, C. (2012). Sound-Color and Visceral Perception: the Historical Ascendancy of Tim-

bre. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, USA.Kurtz, M. (2007). Sofia Gubaidulina, a Biography. Indiana University Press, Bloomington, IN,

USA.Leikin, A. (2011). The Performing Style of Alexander Scriabin. Ashgate Publishing Company,

Farnham, UK.Mitchell, K. (2010). http://www.wiringthebrain.com/2010/03/synesthesia-crossed-wires-or-

free.html. Accessed 25 March 2017.Morrison, S. (2002). Russian Opera and the Symbolist Movement. University of California

Press, Berkeley, CA, USA.Ramachandran, V. S. and Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Psychophysical investigations into the neural

basis of synaesthesia, Proc. Biol. Sci. 268, 979–983.Rudenko, S. (2015). Imagery in piano pedagogy: visualisation of musical texture in children’s

cycle musical toys by Sofia Gubaidulina, Royal Irish Academy of Music, Dublin, in: ICMEMInternational Conference on the Multimodal Experience of Music, University of Sheffield,UK, Abstract 9, p. 17.

Smith, K. M. (2013). Skryabin, Philosophy and the Music of Desire. Royal Musical AssociationMonographs, Vol. 19. Ashgate Publishing Company, Farnham, UK.

Tilot, A. (2016). Blended senses: understanding synaesthesia, musical textures bySvetlana Rudenko. http://www.scienceinschool.org/content/blended-senses-understanding-synaesthesia#. Accessed on March 25, 2017.

Vuoskoski, J. K. and Eerola, T. (2015). Extramusical information contributes to emotions in-duced by music, Psychol. Music 43, 262–274.

Ward, J. (2008). The Frog Who Croaked Blue: Synesthesia and the Mixing of the Senses. Rout-ledge, London, UK and New York, NY, USA.


Recommended