+ All Categories
Home > Documents > my.ilstu.edumy.ilstu.edu/~glcates/Interventions-Spelling.doc · Web view(aged 10-12 yrs), placed in...

my.ilstu.edumy.ilstu.edu/~glcates/Interventions-Spelling.doc · Web view(aged 10-12 yrs), placed in...

Date post: 21-May-2018
Category:
Upload: buitu
View: 215 times
Download: 2 times
Share this document with a friend
19
Pratt-Struthers, J., Struthers, T. B., & Williams, R. L. (1983). The effects of the Add-A-Word Spelling Program on spelling accuracy during creative writing. Education & Treatment of Children, 6(3), 277-283. The following is excerpted from PsychInfo. Abstract. Examined whether correct spelling by 9 learning disabled students (aged 10-12 yrs), placed in a special education resource room, could be increased in the context of creative writing through the use of C. A. McGuigan's (1975) add-a-word spelling program. Target words were those the Ss used frequently but consistently misspelled in their creative writing. Target words were placed on the add-a-word spelling program format using a multiple baseline across words. Interobserver checks were made for each misspelled word, and overall agreement was 100%. Ss' correct spelling of target words during their creative writing increased from 0% to a mean of over 90% after the words were introduced in the program. Results indicate generalization of accurately spelling words to creative writing assignments.
Transcript

Pratt-Struthers, J., Struthers, T. B., & Williams, R. L. (1983). The effects of the Add-A-Word Spelling Program on spelling accuracy during creative writing. Education & Treatment of Children, 6(3), 277-283.

The following is excerpted from PsychInfo.

Abstract. Examined whether correct spelling by 9 learning disabled students

(aged 10-12 yrs), placed in a special education resource room, could be increased in the

context of creative writing through the use of C. A. McGuigan's (1975) add-a-word

spelling program. Target words were those the Ss used frequently but consistently

misspelled in their creative writing. Target words were placed on the add-a-word spelling

program format using a multiple baseline across words. Interobserver checks were made

for each misspelled word, and overall agreement was 100%. Ss' correct spelling of target

words during their creative writing increased from 0% to a mean of over 90% after the

words were introduced in the program. Results indicate generalization of accurately

spelling words to creative writing assignments.

Stevens, K. B. & Schuster, J. W. (1987). Effects of a constant time delay procedure on the written spelling performance of a learning disabled student.

Learning Disability Quarterly, 10, 9-16.

The following is excerpted from Stevens & Schuster (1987).

Abstract. A constant time delay procedure was used to teach written spelling to

an 11-year-old, school-labeled LD youngster who had severe spelling deficits. The time

delay procedure is a near errorless instructional method that transfers stimulus control

from a controlling stimulus (a prompt that signals the correct response) to a new stimulus

(a target response). As a result of a 5-second constant delay procedure the student

acquired, maintained, and generalized 15 spelling words. The procedure is easy to

implement, requires little teacher preparation time, and results in a low student error rate.

In the absence of published time delay research with LD individuals, the results are

encouraging. Based on our findings, the time delay procedures is a viable alternative for

LD students who have not benefited from traditional instructional procedures. Since the

results of this study and those in the unpublished literature indicate that time delay

procedure is effective, further examination with other LD students is warranted.

Method. The constant time delay procedure is a near errorless instructional

method that transfers stimulus control from a controlling stimulus (a prompt that signals

the correct response) to a new stimulus (a target response). This is accomplished by

pairing the controlling stimulus with the new stimulus and systematically increasing the

amount of time between their presentations. The interval between the presentation of the

task request (new stimulus) and the teacher’s model/prompt (controlling stimulus) is

systematically increased until the student emits the correct response before the

controlling stimulus is presented. For example, the new stimulus (the task request, Spell

[target word]” is given to the student. Simultaneously, a printed model of the target word

(the controlling stimulus) is presented (zero-second delay) to allow the student to copy

the model. After the model has been presented at the zero-second delay for a

predetermined number of trials, the time between the task request and presentation of the

model is systematically increased. This gives the student an opportunity to respond

before the presentation of the model or to wait for the model if further prompting is

required.

Gordon, J., Vaughn, S., & Schumm, J. S. (1993). Spelling interventions: A review of literature and implications for instruction for students with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Practice, 8(3), 175-181.

The following is excerpted from Gordon, Vaughn & Schumm (1993).

Abstract. The purpose of this article is to summarize 17 spelling intervention

studies for students with learning disabilities and provide implications for improved

spelling instruction. Interventions are reviewed with respect to subjects, procedures, and

results. Discussion focuses on typical spelling instruction and suggested spelling

instruction for students with learning disabilities in the areas of error imitation and

modeling, unit size, modality, computer-assisted instruction, peer tutoring, and study

techniques.

17 Spelling Interventions

1. Individual instruction of four words in each condition on 4 consecutive days.

Bradley, L. (1981). The organization of motor patterns for spelling: An effective remedial

strategy for backwards readers. Developmental Medical Child Neurology, 23, 83-

91.

2. Group instruction for 3 days, including: oral spelling, individual word presentation,

worksheet practice, mastery training, corrective feedback, and distributed and cumulative

practice.

Bryant, N. D. Drabin, I. R., & Gettinger, M. (1981). Effects of varying unit size on

spelling achievement in learning disabled children. Journal of Learning

Disabilities, 14, 200-203.

3. Ten spelling words were taught in a 5 step study procedure during a 15-min training

session.

Frank, A. R., Wacker, D. P., Keith, T. Z., & Sagen, T. K. (1987). Effectiveness of a

spelling study package for learning disabled students. Learning Disabilities

Research, 2, 110-118.

4. Interevntion included a standard dictation test and imitation-modeling procedure to

100% mastery. A second list of words with the same orthographic features was

administered without instructions to promote generalization.

Gerber, M. M. (1986). Generalization of spelling strategies by learning disabled students

as a result of contingent imitation/modeling and mastery criteria. Journal of

Learning Disabilities, 19, 530-537.

5. Instruction included eight sessions over a 3-week period. Lessons for the experimental

group incorporated reduced unit size, distributed practice and review, and training for

transfer.

Gettinger, M., Bryant, N. D., & Gayne, H. R. (1982). Designing spelling instruction for

learning disabled students: An emphasis on unit size, distributed practice, and

training for transfer. Journal of Special Education, 16, 439-448.

6. Classwide peer tutoring.

Harper, G. F., Mallette, B., & Moore, J. (1991). Peer-mediated instruction: Teaching

spelling to primary school children with mild disabilities. Reading, Writing, and

Learning Disabilities, 7, 137-151.

7. Groups were directed-study, student-controlled, or teacher-monitored groups.

Harris, K. R., Graham, S., & Freeman, S. (1998). Effects of strategy training on

metamemory among learning disabled students. Exceptional Children, 54, 332-

338.

8. Spelling instruction was given for 30 minutes, 5 days per week. Twenty words were

taught weekly for a 4-week period. Instruction consisted of phonics drill, drills with

flashcards, and practice writing words on the chalkboard.

Kauffman, J. M., Hallahan, D. P., Haas, K., Brame, T., & Boren, R. (1978). Imitating

children’s errors to improve their spelling performance. Journal of Learning

Disabilities, 11, 217-222.

9. Subject received three 25-minute tutoring sessions each week with a time-delay

computer-assisted instruction (CAI) procedure.

Kinney, P. G., Stevens, K. B., & Schuster, J. W. (1988). The effects of CAI and time

delay: A systematic program for teaching spelling. Journal of Special Education

Technology, 9, 61-72.

10. Peer tutoring.

Mandoli, M., Mandoli, P., & McLaughlin, T. F. (1982). Effects of same-age peer tutoring

on the spelling performance of a mainstreamed elementary learning disabled

student. Learning Disability Quarterly, 5, 185-189.

11. The teachers used strategic procedures emphasizing awareness of word structure and

spelling strategies, combined with keyboard training in twice weekly, 45-minute sessions

for 3 months.

Margalit, M., & Roth, Y. B. (1989). Strategic keyboard training and spelling

improvement among children with learning disabilities and mental retardation.

Educational Psychology, 9, 321-329.

12. Intervention included a pretest, nine learning trials, and a test for transfer of learning.

Learning trials required the subject to say the words, and write them on a piece of paper.

A contingent imitation procedure was used if he misspelled a word.

Nulman, J. H., & Gerber, M. M. (1984). Improving spelling performance by imitating a

child’s errors. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 328-333.

13. 5-second constant time delay.

Stevens, K. B. & Schuster, J. W. (1987). Effects of a constant time delay procedure

on the written spelling performance of a learning disabled student.

Learning Disability Quarterly, 10, 9-16.

14. Subjects received spelling training under three experimental conditions that involved:

writing, sorting letter tiles, or typing on the computer.

Vaughn, S., Schumm, J., & Gordon, J. (1992). Early spelling acquisition: Does writing

really beat the computer? Learning Disability Quarterly, 15, 223-228.

15. Subjects received spelling training under three experimental conditions that involved:

writing, sorting letter tiles, or typing on the computer. Training procedures incorporated

components of best instructional practice, examined student performance over time, and

incorporated student interviews concerning condition preference.

Vaughn, S., Schumm, J., & Gordon, J. (1993). Which motoric condition is most effective

for teaching spelling to students with and without learning disabilities. Journal of

Learning Disabilities, 26, 191-197.

16. Computer assisted instruction – The Spelling Machine program

Watkins, M. W. (1989). Computerized drill-and-practice and academic attitudes of

learning disabled students. Journal of Special Education Technology, 9, 168-172.

was taught concurrently with a self-questioning strategy.

Wong, B. Y. L. (1986). A cognitive approach to teaching spelling. Exceptional Children,

53, 169-173.

Following reviewed by Karyn Erkfritz

17. Structural word analysis Spelling Development(Ehri, 2000)

1. Prealphabetic levela. Children may store salient visual stimuli for words (e.g. golden arches for

McDonald’s)b. Children may produce scribbles for writing but may lack letter details

2. Partial alphabetic levela. Know names/sounds of alphabet lettersb. Can read/write words at semiphonetic levelc. Basic and incomplete knowledged. Vowel and consonant sounds not present in letter names are less well-

knowne. Spelling may be inventive and correct spellings may be difficult because

they alck sufficient knowledge of the alphabetic systemf. Spellings are partial because it is still difficult to segment words into

phonemes and because full knowledge of phoneme-grapheme isn’t developed

3. Full alphabetic levela. Can segment words into phonemesb. Knowledge of conventional grapheme-phoneme correspondences

including vowelsc. Inventive spellings are more complete and are able to stretch out sounds in

words to spell them (may find that extra sounds are not symbolized in conventional spelling)

d. Understanding of the basic system that accounts for many letters in conventional spellings, easier to read/write

e. Have some words stored in memory in sufficient letter detail so that reading new words by analogy to familiar words is possible (e.g. rig, fig, big)

4. Consolidated alphabetic levela. Learning structure of larger units consisting of letter sequences that recur

across different words

i. Units may involve spellings of syllables, or parts of syllables, or affixes appearing at beginnings or endings of words

ii. Recurring blends of graphophonemic units become consolidated into larger units

b. Easier for students to decode and invent spellings of longer multisyllabic words.

*Correlations between reading and spelling .68 to .86

Prerequisite Skills(Ehri, 2000)

1. Knowledge of letters2. Ability to associate letters with sounds (e.g. t is “tee” This is referred to as

grapheme-phoneme knowledge. This is important for reading.3. Ability to associate sounds with letters (e.g. “buh” is b). This is referred to as

phoneme-grapheme knowledge. This is important for spelling.4. Knowledge of digraphs (e.g. “ch” or “ar”). These are two letters together that are

pronounced or read differently than their spelling suggests.

Teach Spelling RulesWhile this is not an empirically validated treatment, an appropriate intervention may be to teach a student the rules of spelling. A list of these rules can be found: http://www.dyslexia.org/spelling_rules.shtmlhttp://www.mc3.edu/aa/lal/workshops/wksp_spelling/spellingrules.html (has practice exercises)

Attack Strategy (for beginning spellers)(Ehri, 2000)Involves inventing spellings of words

1. Stretch out pronunciations2. Detect sound units3. Apply knowledge of alphabetic system to generate plausible letter sequences for

the sounds*Likelihood that spellers will generate correct letter sequences is limited

Cover-Copy-Compare (http://www.interventioncentral.org/htmdocs/interventions/ccc.shtml)

1. Provide the child with a list of words written on the left hand side of a page.2. The teacher gives the student an index card.3. The student is directed to look at each correct item (e.g., correctly spelled word,

computation problem with solution) on the left side of the page.4. The student is instructed to cover the correct model on the left side of the page

with an index card and to spell the word in the space on the right of the sheet. 5. The student then uncovers the correct answer on the left to check his or her work.6. If the student gets the word correct, they would give themselves +1

7. If the student gets the word incorrect, they would have to write the word 3 times and not earn +1

8. General Rule: a child has acquired the word if they get it correct in two consecutive trials

Constant Time Delay (Mushinski, & Stormont-Spurgin, 1995)1. Provide the student with a blank sheet of paper.2. The teacher would give the students the rules: “I will say a word, give it in a

sentence, and then say it again. You must give me your best answer. If you get it right, you will earn 1 point. If you get it wrong, you will have to practice writing it correctly 3 times and will earn no points. If you do not know a word, you can sit and wait and then I will show you how to spell it. If you wait, you will not get any points, but will only have to write it once.”

3. Wait 7 seconds before providing the student with the model. If they do not write anything or write an incorrect response, write the word on a note card and then have them copy it correctly three times (only if they missed it) or 1 time (if they waited for the model) on the lined paper.

*These interventions have been looked at with respect to students w/ LD:Error Imitation and Modeling (Gordon, Vaughn, & Schumm, 1993)This intervention is designed to directs students' attention to discrepancies between misspellings and correct spellings through imitation of errors prior to the presentation of models

When a child makes an error, use this corrective feedback procedure:1. Imitate the child’s error by rewriting it2. Then present a correct model (especially in the case of non-phonetically spelled

words)Considerations: Need a ‘sufficient’ number of trials

Peer Tutoring (Mandoli, Mandoli, & McLaughlin, 1982)1. Tutee meets 15 minutes a day with the peer tutor2. First 2 days, a modeling method was used, the adult presented the teaching

procedure and the peer tutor was encouraged to follow it.3. On Day 3, the peer tutor conducted the entire session under the guidance of an

adult representing a good example of academic behavior (The peer tutor should not require additional assistance beyond adult supervision in the room beyond Day 3)

4. The tutor read the words aloud, then asked the tutee to read the words5. Then the tutee was instructed to orally spell the words6. Flashcards and games were used to enhance the teaching process7. If the tutee got the words wrong, he had to write them out 10 times8. The tutor provided assistance, encouragement, helpful hints and praise to the

tutee.

Study Techniques (Gordon, Vaughn, & Schumm, 1993)1. Propose a word to learn2. Correctly write the word or display the word with letter tiles3. Name the word4. Write the word5. Name the word again, check accuracy, and continue steps 2-4 until mastery6. Practice the word in this way for 6 consecutive days

Variation of steps:1. Say the word2. Write and say the word3. Check the word4. Trace and say the word5. Write the word from memory and check6. If the word is incorrect, repeat steps the first 5 steps

Variation of steps:1. Do I know this word?2. How many syllables do I hear in this word? (Write down the number)3. I’ll spell out the word4. Do I have the right number of syllables down?5. Now, does it look right to me?6. When I finish spelling, I’ll tell myself I’ve worked hard.

*May want to package with include interspersal with known and unknown words, use of positive practice and reinforcement, and possible teacher supervision of study sessions

Directed Spelling Thinking Activity (Graham, Harris, Loynachan, 1996)This intervention is directed at improving spelling skills by contrasting spelling patterns

1. Choose 2 or more patterns,1 that the student has at least some knowledge of, but lack of understanding of its application

2. Contrast selected can include more than one spelling pattern and more than two patterns

a. E.g. Compare short vowel consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) with long vowel pattern marked by e (CVCe)

3. Once contrast in selected, words are selected that illustrate target patterns. One or two exceptions are chosen to illustrate the consistencies/inconsistencies of English orthography

4. Lists with 10-12 words are suggested with students w/ LD, “typical” students 16-20 words

5. All words should be familiar to students and selected so that they will generate a fairly even mix of correct/incorrect spellings.

6. Each word is written on a note card7. The teacher begins the lesson by administering a test of words for that lesson (no

mention that words are related to particular patterns)8. Discussion follows with students talking about why they spelled a word they way

they did

9. Then teacher presents correct spelling of the word, complimenting students on the reasonableness of their spellings and highlight the parts of the word that caused difficultly

10. The students proceed through the list this way and the teacher encourages comparison by asking how words are similar and different

11. Then the students sort the words into their respective categories. One word (on a card) is chosen to represent each category and for the exception words, a question mark is used.

12. The teacher randomly selects one of the remaining words and says it, and puts in under the appropriate category (a wall chart with pockets for holding word cards can facilitate process)

13. As the teacher continues through the words, the students decide where to place them

14. Then the students add words of their own to the categories w/the teacher providing feedback

15. In concluding the activity, the teacher solicits from the group an explicit statement concerning the orthographic principles underlying the contrasting patterns

Follow-up activities: looking for words in reading materials that fir the patterns, scanning their own writing for words that fit the patterns, sorting new words that correspond to the target patterns and developing their own pattern study book of words that fit the patterns

Multisensory Study Techniques for Memorizing Spellings of Words(Scott, 2000)*Once a multisensory study techniques has been taught, Harris, Graham, and Freeman (1988) found equal effectiveness for students with LD when practice was independent, teacher-directed on an as-needed basis, or entirely teacher prompted

5-Step study strategy (Graham & Freeman, 1986)1. Say the word.2. Write and say the word.3. Check the word.4. Trace and say the word.5. Write the word from memory and check spelling.

Horn Method 2 (Horn, 1954)1. Pronounce each word carefully.2. Look carefully at each part of the word as you pronounce it.3. Say the letters in sequence.4. Attempt to recall how the word looks, then spell the word.5. Check this spelling attempt to recall.6. Write the word.7. Check this spelling attempt.8. Repeat the above steps if necessary.

8-step method for pencil or computer practice(Berninger, Abbot, Rogan, Reed, Abbott, Brooks, Vaughan, & Graham, 1998a)

1. Look carefully at the word while tutor sweeps finger over and says it out loud.2. Watch and listen while tutor says sound corresponding to color-coded graphemes,

in a left to right fashion (for example, /b/ /o/ /t/ while point to “b,” “oa,” and “t.”)3. Name letters as tutor points to letters.4. Close eyes and picture the word in the “mind’s eye.”5. Keep eyes closed and spell the word out loud.6. With pencil, open eyes and write word or with computer, open eyes and point to

letters on an alphabetical grid; then tutor points to the letter on keyboard and child presses key.

7. Compare spelling to target.8. If incorrect, tutor points out where difference lies, then pervious steps are

repeated.

Summary/Other Resources (Gordon, Vaughn, & Schumm, 193)


Recommended