+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning...

Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning...

Date post: 18-Dec-2016
Category:
Upload: gina
View: 213 times
Download: 1 times
Share this document with a friend
21
This article was downloaded by: [University of Birmingham] On: 19 September 2013, At: 12:47 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Developmental Neuropsychology Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hdvn20 Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls Margaret Semrud-Clikeman Ph.D. a , Jenifer Walkowiak b , Alison Wilkinson c & Gina Christopher d a Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan b Children's National Medical Center, Washington, DC c Children's Medical Center Dallas, Dallas, Texas d Department of Educational Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas Published online: 16 Aug 2010. To cite this article: Margaret Semrud-Clikeman Ph.D. , Jenifer Walkowiak , Alison Wilkinson & Gina Christopher (2010) Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls, Developmental Neuropsychology, 35:5, 582-600, DOI: 10.1080/87565641.2010.494747 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2010.494747 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Transcript
Page 1: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

This article was downloaded by: [University of Birmingham]On: 19 September 2013, At: 12:47Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Developmental NeuropsychologyPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hdvn20

Neuropsychological Differences AmongChildren With Asperger Syndrome,Nonverbal Learning Disabilities,Attention Deficit Disorder, and ControlsMargaret Semrud-Clikeman Ph.D. a , Jenifer Walkowiak b , AlisonWilkinson c & Gina Christopher da Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, Michigan StateUniversity, East Lansing, Michiganb Children's National Medical Center, Washington, DCc Children's Medical Center Dallas, Dallas, Texasd Department of Educational Psychology, University of Texas atAustin, Austin, TexasPublished online: 16 Aug 2010.

To cite this article: Margaret Semrud-Clikeman Ph.D. , Jenifer Walkowiak , Alison Wilkinson& Gina Christopher (2010) Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With AspergerSyndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls, DevelopmentalNeuropsychology, 35:5, 582-600, DOI: 10.1080/87565641.2010.494747

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2010.494747

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as tothe accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinionsand views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Contentshould not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever orhowsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arisingout of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &

Page 2: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 3: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

Neuropsychological Differences Among ChildrenWith Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning

Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

Margaret Semrud-ClikemanDepartments of Psychology and Psychiatry, Michigan State University,

East Lansing, Michigan

Jenifer WalkowiakChildren’s National Medical Center, Washington, DC

Alison WilkinsonChildren’s Medical Center Dallas, Dallas, Texas

Gina ChristopherDepartment of Educational Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas

Confusion is present as to possible diagnostic differences between Asperger syndrome (AS) and Non-verbal learning disabilities (NLD) and the relation of these disorders to attentional difficulties.Three-hundred and forty-five children participated in this study in 5 groups; NLD, AS, attention defi-cit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Combined type, ADHD: Inattentive type, and controls. The NLDgroup showed particular difficulty on visual-spatial, visual-motor, and fluid reasoning measures com-pared to the other groups. There was also a significant verbal-performance IQ split in this group re-lated to difficulty in social functioning. This study extends the findings from previous studies and ex-tends these findings to differences between AS and NLD groups.

Previous studies have evaluated the neuropsychological functioning of children with nonverballearning disabilities (NLD) (2000), Asperger disorder (AS) (Ozonoff & Griffith, 2000), or atten-tion deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Nigg, Blaskey, Huang-Pollock, & Rappley, 2002)Few studies have compared the neuropsychological functioning of children across these diagno-ses in the domains of intelligence, achievement, motor, perception, and fluid reasoning. The pur-pose of this study was to evaluate possible differences among these groups on measures of verbalability, fluid reasoning, and visual-motor skills.

DEVELOPMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 35(5), 582–600Copyright © 2010 Taylor & Francis Group, LLCISSN: 8756-5641 print / 1532-6942 onlineDOI: 10.1080/875656412010494747

Correspondence should be addressed to Margaret Semrud-Clikeman, Ph.D., Michigan State University, PsychologyBuilding, 321 A West Fee Hall, East Lansing, MI 48824. E-mail: [email protected]

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 4: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

NONVERBAL LEARNING DISABILITY

The original concept of nonverbal learning disabilities was introduced by Johnson and Myklebust(1967) and included children who had difficulty with social perception. Subsequently Rourke andcolleagues later suggested that the combined deficits of visual-spatial learning, tactile and motorskills, and mathematics make up a syndrome they termed a nonverbal learning disability (NLD)(Rourke, 1989; Rourke & Tsatsanis, 1996). An area of controversy in the area of NLD has beenthe definition of this disorder. Many studies utilize an approach where the child must meet a selec-tion of symptoms from an array of possibilities in order to qualify for a diagnosis of NLD. Al-though the NLD subtype continues to be refined, it is currently characterized by three broad areasof dysfunction including motoric skills, visual/spatial organizational skills, and social abilities(Semrud-Clikeman, 2007).

Children with NLD often show excellent single word reading skills and spelling ability buthave difficulty with inferential thinking, complex reading comprehension, and mathematics(Semrud-Clikeman & Glass, 2008). Executive functioning for these children generally may beproblematic as they often have difficulty in understanding situations that involve cause–effectreasoning, generating solutions to problems, and learning new material that is complex or novel(Rourke, 1995; Semrud-Clikeman, 2003).

Many clinicians and researchers suggest that children with NLD have difficulty in correctlyperceiving social relations and with social judgment (Semrud-Clikeman, 2007). Some have sug-gested that these social difficulties are secondary to difficulties with visual-spatial development(Rourke, 2000) while others suggest that there are significant perceptual problems that contributeto problems with understanding facial expressions, voice intonation, and the speaker’s intent(Forrest, 2007; Guli, Wilkinson, & Semrud-Clikeman, 2008; Pennington, 1991, 2008; Semrud-Clikeman, 2007). It is unclear from the empirical literature whether this hypothesis is accurate andfurther research that evaluates whether these perceptual and visual-spatial deficits underlie the so-cial-emotional functioning of children with NLD is needed.

ASPERGER SYNDROME

A defining feature of AS is difficulty with social relationships. Particular difficulties are present innonverbal behaviors (eye contact, facial expression, and body gestures), successful peer relation-ships, and with social reciprocity. Children with AS often have interests that are circumscribedand intense and may be inflexible in their adherence to routines. Neuropsychologically, childrenwith AS show strong verbal skills, poor visual-spatial ability, and problems with executive func-tioning (Klin, Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Rourke, 1995). As some researchers have questioned thefindings of visual-spatial difficulties in AS (Edgin & Pennington, 2005), this area of inquiry re-mains open.

While diagnostic criteria for AS is contained within the DSM-IV-TR (American PsychiatricAssociation, 2000) structure, there is no commensurate manual for a diagnosis of NLD. There areseveral similarities between AS and NLD. In both disorders, problems with social communicationand reciprocity, nonverbal communication, pragmatic language, and visual-spatial skills appearto be present (Gunter, Ghaziuddin, & Ellis, 2002; Voeller, 1995). Stronger verbal compared toperformance skills on cognitive testing were found for children with AS or NLD (Gillberg &

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES 583

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 5: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

Billstedt, 2000; Klin et al., 1995). It is important to note that the many of the symptoms of NLDmay be present in children with AS. However, differences between children with AS and thosewith NLD include the presence of stereotyped and restricted patterns of interest and the need toadhere to routines present in children with AS but not NLD (Semrud-Clikeman, 2007).

ADHD, AS, AND NLD

Of interest for this study is the co-occurrence of ADHD in children with AS and NLD (Gillberg &Billstedt, 2000). Studies have found a high co-occurrence of ADHD in a sample of children withAS with 33% to 50% of the samples showing significant difficulties in inattention and 7% show-ing problems with overactivity (Gadow, DeVincent, & Pomeroy, 2006; Ghaziuddin, Weidmer-Mikhail, & Ghaziuddin, 1998; Leyfer et al., 2006; Nyden, Gillberg, Hjelmquist, & Heiman,1999). Similar to children with AS those with NLD also show a tendency to have attentional diffi-culties. Children with NLD are often identified as having problems with attention and tend to bediagnosed with ADHD: Predominately Inattentive type (ADHD–PI) (Semrud-Clikeman, 2007).Some have suggested that the attentional problems found in children with NLD are due to visualperceptual and tactile difficulties (Rourke, 2000) while others suggest these difficulties are sepa-rate but that visual attentional issues may be related to problems with social functioning (Corbett& Constantine, 2006; Fine, Semrud-Clikeman, Butcher, & Walkowiak, 2008). Rourke (2000)suggested that deficits in visual perceptual skills may be related to attentional difficulty seen inmany of these children rather than a true diagnosis of ADHD. Thus, these visual attentional issuesmay have a differential effect on social functioning for children with NLD apart from the difficul-ties found for children with ADHD; an area of inquiry that requires further research.

Given the hypothesis that children with AS or NLD may have attentional differences, it wasfelt important to control for significant attentional problems for these children on the neuro-psychological measures in this study. For this reason we included a group of children withADHD–PI and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Combined type (ADHD–C) to attemptto control the effect attentional difficulties have on the ability to complete tasks that require execu-tive functions. The main hypotheses of this study were that children with AS and NLD wouldshow significant problems on measures of executive functioning, visual perception, and perfor-mance abilities compared to the children with ADHD or the control group. It was also hypothe-sized that children with NLD would show a difference in Verbal-Performance IQ skills comparedto the other four groups as suggested by the literature. Moreover, it was hypothesized that childrenwith NLD would show right-sided difficulties on measures of motor dexterity while these prob-lems would not be present in the children with AS, ADHD–C, ADHD–PI, or controls.

METHOD

Participants

Participants were children referred by parents, teachers, psychologists, psychiatrists, pediatricians,and community organizations to a large university for participation in a larger eight-year study ex-amining the neuropsychological functioning of children with developmental disorders. The finalsample included 345 children ranging in age from 9.1 years to 16.5 years. There were 242 males and

584 SEMRUD-CLIKEMAN, WALKOWIAK, WILKINSON, CHRISTOPHER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 6: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

103 females in the sample with 18% of the sample self-identified as a minority (Hispanic, AfricanAmerican, and Asian). There were five groups of children identified for the purpose of this study;control (N = 113), AS (N = 50), NLD (N = 26), ADHD–C (N = 76), and ADHD–PI (N = 80). Re-flecting the general rates of AS and ADHD comorbidity 27 of the AS group also had a secondary di-agnosis of ADHD; 14 with ADHD–C and 13 with ADHD–PI. For the NLD group 22 also had a sec-ondary diagnosis of ADHD; 6 with ADHD–C and 16 with ADHD–PI.

Doctoral-level graduate students trained in administering a comprehensive neuropsycho-logical battery individually assessed the participants. These graduate students were blind to the di-agnosis of the child. Diagnoses were determined by consensus of two independent sources; li-censed psychologists within the community or university, and advanced doctoral students.Participants for whom a diagnosis was not unanimous were not included in the study (N = 5).Exclusionary criteria included a history of a reading disability, seizure disorder, progressive neu-rological problems, traumatic brain injury, or any other serious medical condition. Those withcomorbid psychopathology were also excluded from the sample, including participants with se-vere mood or conduct disorders. Only children with a FSIQ above 80 were included in the sample.

Children with a diagnosis of ADHD were diagnosed by either private practitioners (psychia-trists or psychologists) or through the neuropsychology clinic at the university. Our confirmationof a diagnosis for ADHD required a T-score of 65 or higher on the Behavior Assessment Scale forChildren–2 (BASC–2)(Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004) inattention and/or hyperactivity scale aswell as meeting criteria for DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) diagnosis ofADHD using a parent semi-structured interview. Children in the control group had no history oflearning, behavioral, or attentional difficulties both by parent and teacher report and an interviewcompleted prior to participation. No child in this group met criteria for AS, NLD, or reading dis-ability.

The Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADI–R) (Le Couteur, Lord, & Rutter, 2003) or the Autism Di-agnostic Observational System (ADOS)(Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 1999) were administered tochildren in the AS group through a private practitioner to determine the presence of autistism spectrumdisorders (ASD). These results were not generally available for the study from these private practitio-ners. No child met the criteria for autism using the ADI–R. The children were diagnosed with ASpartly based on the results of one of these measures with the diagnosis of AS confirmed usingDSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) criteria for this study. An AS screener basedon DSM-IV-TR Asperger syndrome criteria developed at the clinic and completed by the child’s pri-mary caretaker was used for tertiary confirmation. The cutoff score on this measure was 6 as definedby DSM-IV-TR criteria. There were three children excluded from the study who did not meet all ofthese criteria. Of the children with AS approximately 25% would also have met criteria for NLD.

An independent licensed psychologist evaluated approximately half of the sample of childrendiagnosed with a nonverbal learning disability. This diagnosis was confirmed through review ofprevious testing if current or through an updated assessment. Agreement among the two first au-thors and any previous diagnosis was required for inclusion in the study. There were six childrenwho were found to not qualify under the following criteria or who met criteria for significant de-pression or anxiety disorder diagnoses. Consistent with the recommendations from research crite-ria (Rourke & Tsatsanis, 1996), children with NLD qualified for this group if they met all of thefollowing criteria: (1)Scores 1 standard deviation below average on the parent form of the SocialSkills Rating Scale (SSRS) (Gresham & Elliott, 1990); (2) Math calculation skills below the 15thpercentile on the math calculation subtest of the Woodcock-Johnson Achievement Test III

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES 585

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 7: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

(WJ-Ach III) (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001b); (3) Average scores on the letter-wordidentification subtest from the WJ-Ach III (Woodcock et al., 2001b); (4) Average estimated ver-bal IQ of at least 85 standard score points on the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence(WASI) (Psychological Corporation, 1999); (5) Scores at least one standard deviation below aver-age on the Visual Motor-Integration Test (VMI) (Beery, Buktenica, & Beery, 2006); and (6) Be-low average scores on the Purdue Pegboard (Tiffin, 1968). We decided to use a more conservativeapproach to diagnosis and to view the disorder as a syndrome which incorporated the main areasof visual-motor skills, motor skills, social functioning, and good verbal ability.

It is important to mention here that children with AS may have many symptoms that are consistentwith a diagnosis of NLD. For the current study, however, children with AS were selected apart fromsimilarities with the NLD group when one of more of the following symptoms were found during theparent interview: a stereotyped and restricted pattern of interests, inflexible adherence to nonfunc-tional routines, stereotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms, preoccupation with parts of objects, anda lack of spontaneous seeking to share enjoyment, interests, or achievements with others. The childrenin the AS group also were not required to show the same symptoms as detailed above.

Inclusionary Instruments

WASI (Psychological Corporation, 1999). The WASI is an abbreviated scale of intelli-gence with measures of similarities, vocabulary, block design, and matrix reasoning. As discussedearlier, there are research findings that indicate the performance IQ may be impacted in childrenwith NLD. For this reason, the VIQ was used as an estimate of ability.

WJ-Ach III (Woodcock et al., 2001b). Three subtests from the WJ-Ach III were adminis-tered; letter-word identification, calculation, and mathematics reasoning.

VMI (Beery et al., 2006). The VMI requires the child to copy increasingly more difficultgeometric figures within a grid. It provides a measure of visual-motor skills.

Purdue Pegboard (Tiffin, 1968). The Purdue Pegboard requires the child to place pegs inholes as quickly as possible first with the dominant hand, then the nondominant hand, and then bothhands together for 30 seconds each. This measure has been found to be a good measure of manual dex-terity but not necessarily useful for determining lateralization (Reddon, Gill, Gauk, & Maerz, 1988).

The Structured Interview for Diagnostic Assessment of Children (SIDAC) (Puig-Antich& Chambers, 1978). The SIDAC is a formal interview based on DSM-IV diagnoses, modi-fied and updated from the Kiddie-Schedule of Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (K-SADS)developed by Puig-Antich and Chambers (1978). The ADHD portion of the SIDAC interviewwas used in this study to determine the presence of ADHD symptoms with severity based on thenumber of symptoms reported. Symptoms related to inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivitywere included. This measure was also used as partial confirmation of parent-reported diagnosis ofADHD, as noted earlier.

Behavior Assessment System for Children–2 (BASC–2) (Reynolds & Kamphaus,2004). Two subtests from the BASC–2 parent rating scale were used as a measure of attentionand hyperactivity. The BASC–2 is an omnibus behavioral rating scale that provides normativedata for specific behaviors. It has excellent psychometric properties.

586 SEMRUD-CLIKEMAN, WALKOWIAK, WILKINSON, CHRISTOPHER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 8: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

Social Skills Rating Scale (SSRS) (Gresham & Elliott, 1990). The SSRS is a behav-ioral rating scale completed by the child’s main caretaker. It measures total social skills through aLikert scale of three options (never, sometimes, and very often).

Study Measures

Woodcock-Johnson Cognitive Battery III (WJ-Cog III) (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather,2001a). Two subtests that make up the Fluid Reasoning index were utilized from the WJ-CogIII; analysis/synthesis and concept formation. These tests require the child to look at a problemand utilize the information present to solve the item. Analysis and synthesis requires the child tosolve picture equations while concept formation requires cognitive flexibility and skill in evaluat-ing problems to determine what is similar between the stimuli.

Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure (Osterreith, 1944). The Rey-Osterreith complex figuretest requires the child to copy a complex figure as accurately as possible and is a measure of per-ceptual organization (Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, 2004). The scoring system used is the one de-veloped by Osterreith and translated by Corwin and Bylsma (1993) involving the widely used18-item, 36-point scoring system.

Judgment of Line Orientation (JLO) (Benton, Sivan, Hamsher, Varney, & Spreen,2004). The JLO is a measure of spatial ability without a motor component. The child is asked tolook at an array of lines and then is shown two lines. He/she is then asked to match the two lines tothe array.

Grooved Pegboard (Klove, 1963). The grooved pegboard measures complex finger co-ordination. It consists of a board with slotted holes angled in various directions. Each peg has agroove and only fits in one way in each hole. The child uses first the dominant hand and then thenondominant hand to place the pegs as quickly as possible. It has been recommended as a goodmeasure of lateralized impairment (Lezak et al., 2004)

Finger Tapping Test (Reitan & Wolfson, 1985). The Finger Tapping test requires thechild to tap a key as quickly as possibly first with the dominant hand and then with thenondominant.

Analyses

Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with multiple post-hoc comparisons was used todetermine whether there were group differences based on age or verbal IQ in the sample. The gen-eral linear model (GLM) with post-hoc comparisons was used to evaluate group differencesamong the behavioral, cognitive, and neuropsychological measures.

RESULTS

Table 1 presents the group means and standard deviations for the sample age, estimated Verbal IQmeasure, and group selection measures. There were no significant differences among groups for

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES 587

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 9: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

588

TA

BLE

1S

igni

fican

tGro

upD

iffer

ence

sfo

rG

roup

Sel

ectio

nM

easu

res;

Mea

nsan

dS

tand

ard

Dev

iatio

ns

Var

iabl

e

Con

trol

(N=

113)

AS2

(N=

50)

NL

D3

(N=

26)

AD

HD

-C4

(N=

76)

AD

HD

-PI5

(N=

80)

Gro

upSi

gnif

ican

ceM

ean

SD1

Mea

nSD

Mea

nSD

Mea

nSD

Mea

nSD

Age

10.4

25.1

10.3

28.8

11.3

26.1

1024

.410

.825

.4N

.S.6

AS

scre

ener

.91.

410

.22.

35.

32.

35.

73.

75.

63.

2A

S>

C(p

<.0

001)

,NL

D(p

<.0

001)

,AD

HD

-C(p

<.0

001)

,AD

HD

-PI(

p<

.000

1)C

<N

VL

D,A

DH

D-C

,AD

HD

-PI

(p<

.000

1)

SID

AC

Tot

al7

2.7

310

.64.

110

.23.

414

.32.

410

.75.

2A

S,N

LD

,AD

HD

-PI

>C

(p<

.000

1)A

DH

D-C

>A

ll(p

<.0

001)

BA

SC-h

yper

846

.911

.164

.113

.864

.312

.372

11.9

55.4

10.8

AD

HD

-C>

AD

HD

-PI,

AS,

NL

D(p

<.0

001)

C<

AS,

NL

D,A

DH

D-C

,AD

HD

-PI(

p<

.000

1)

BA

SC-a

ttn9

49.7

9.3

64.7

1165

.78.

967

.98.

267

.77.

6C

<A

ll(p

<.0

001)

SSR

S1053

10.8

37.1

11.2

38.7

8.5

408.

744

.110

.9A

S,N

LD

<A

DH

D-C

(p<

.022

)A

DH

D-P

I>A

S(p

<.0

001)

,NL

D(p

=.0

22),

AD

HD

-C(p

=.0

14)

C<

All

(p<

.000

1)

VIQ

1111

1.9

14.3

107.

317

.610

9.7

16.7

110

16.6

106.

111

.7N

.S.

Rea

ding

106.

412

.310

7.8

1710

2.2

15.2

106.

713

.210

8.25

14.6

N.S

.

Mat

hem

atic

sC

alcu

latio

n11

2.5

1597

.117

.490

.819

.710

1.1

17.9

100

20.7

NL

D<

AD

HD

-C(p

=.0

07),

AD

HD

-PI

(p=

.04)

C>

All

(p<

.000

1)

VM

I1210

1.3

12.5

101

1787

.511

.796

.615

.597

8.7

NL

D<

All

(p<

.02)

Purd

ueR

H13

.04

1.04

–1.1

4a1.

8–1

.33

1.5

–.68

1.3

–.61

1.2

C<

All

(p<

.03)

Purd

ueL

H14

–.24

1.3

–1.3

1.7

–.98

1.2

–.44

1.2

–.42

1.4

AS

<C

(p=

.001

),A

DH

D-C

(p=

.01)

,A

DH

D-P

I(p

=.0

1)N

LD

<C

(p=

.04)

Purd

ueB

oth

Han

ds–.

111.

04–.

971.

9–1

.29

1.4

–.45

1.2

–.57

1.2

NL

D,A

S<

C(p

<.0

01),

NL

D<

AD

HD

-C(p

=.0

3)

1 Sta

ndar

dde

viat

ion;

2 Asp

erge

rSy

ndro

me;

3 Non

verb

alL

earn

ing

Dis

abili

ty;4 A

ttent

ion

defi

cith

yper

activ

itydi

sord

er–C

ombi

ned

type

;5 Atte

ntio

nde

fici

thyp

erac

tivity

diso

rder

—Pr

edom

inat

ely

Inat

tent

ive

Typ

e;6 N

otsi

gnif

ican

t;7 S

truc

ture

din

terv

iew

;8 Soc

ialS

kills

Rat

ing

Scal

e;9 B

ehav

iorA

sses

smen

tSys

tem

forC

hild

ren–

Hyp

erac

tivity

scal

e;10B

eh

av

ior

Assessm

en

tS

yste

mfo

rC

hil

dren

–A

tten

tio

nscale

;11V

erb

al

IQ;

12V

isu

al-

Mo

tor

Inte

grati

on

Test;

13R

igh

th

an

d;

14L

eft

han

d.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 10: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

age (p = .18). As expected due to selection criteria, there was not a significant difference in esti-mated Verbal IQ among the groups (p = .085, partial eta squared = .024).

Analyses of Group Selection Measures

Membership in groups was partially determined by scores on selected psychological measures.Preliminary analyses were conducted to determine whether the expected group differences werepresent. These results are provided in Table 1.

Visual-Spatial Measures. Scores from the VMI were evaluated using a one way ANOVA.A significant group difference was present for the VMI (F(4,290) = 5.68, p < .0001, partial etasquared = .07). Post-hoc comparisons found that the NLD group differed from all of the othergroups (p < .02). These findings were expected for the NLD group as it was a variable involved inthe selection for the group.

A preliminary analysis of the right (RH), left (LH), and both hands results of the Purdue Peg-board found a significant main group effect difference for all conditions (F(12, 418) = 2.45, p =.004, partial eta squared = .058). Follow-up analyses found that there were significant group dif-ferences for all conditions (p < .008). Post-hoc analysis found that Control group scored betterwith the RH compared to the clinical groups (p < .03). Among the clinical groups there were nodifferences for the RH. For the LH, the AS and NLD groups scored significantly worse than thecontrol group but not from each other (p = .44). This finding was predicted by the selection criteriafor the NLD group. This finding was not predicted for the AS group. For the both hands condition,the AS and NLD groups were significantly less successful than the control group (p < .001) but didnot differ from each other. The NLD group also scored worse compared to the ADHD–C group(p = .03).

Behavioral Measures

On the semi-structured interview (SIDAC) a significant main effect for group was found (F(4,337) = 124.6, p < .0001) for total symptoms of attention/impulsivity/hyperactivity. Post-hoc com-parisons found that the control group scored significantly lower than all clinical groups (p <.0001). Clinical group comparisons found the ADHD–C group to score lower than all other clini-cal groups (p < .0001). There were no significant differences between the AS, NLD, or ADHD–PIgroups.

A preliminary analysis of the hyperactivity and attention scales of the BASC–2 was completedusing a 2 × 5 MANOVA. As expected significant findings were present for the main effect forgroup (F(8, 656) = 47.2, p < .0001, partial eta squared = .37). Follow-up analyses found signifi-cant group differences for attention (p < .0001) and hyperactivity (p < .0001). For the hyperactiv-ity measure, post-hoc analyses found that the ADHD–C group differed significantly from all ofthe other groups (p = .005). When the clinical groups were compared, the ADHD–C showed sig-nificantly higher scores on the hyperactivity measure from all of the other clinical groups (p <.0001) while the AS, NLD, and ADHD–PI groups did not differ from each other. For the attentionmeasure all clinical groups were less successful than the control group (p < .0001). No significantdifferences were found among the clinical groups on the attention measure.

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES 589

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 11: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

Social Skills Selection Measure

On the SSRS there was a significant main effect for group (F(4, 329) = 30.2, p < .0001)with the control group scoring significantly higher than all of the clinical groups (p < .0001).There were significant differences between the NLD and ADHD–C group (p = .022) with theNLD group scoring significantly worse than the ADHD–C group. There was no significantdifference between the NLD and AS groups (p = .54). The ADHD–PI group was rated betteron this measure compared to the ADHD–C (p = .014), AS (p < .0001) and NLD (p = .022)groups.

For the AS screener an ANOVA found a significant group difference (p < .0001). Post-hocanalyses found that all of the clinical groups scored higher than the control group (p < .0001).Clinical group comparisons found that the AS group scored higher than the NLD and ADHDgroups (p < .0001). No difference was found among the NLD and ADHD groups (p > .05). Table 1provides these findings

Analysis of Measures of Interest

Evaluation of the estimated verbal and performance IQs using a 2 (verbal, performance) × 5(group) MANOVA found a significant main effect for group (F (8, 678) = 5.769, p < .0001, partialeta = .064). Table 2 provides these results. Follow-up analysis found no significant difference forthe verbal estimated IQ among groups (p = .085, partial eta = .024) with a significant differencepresent for the performance estimated IQ among groups (p < .0001, partial eta = .095). Post-hocanalysis found that the NLD group scored significantly worse than the control group (p < .0001),AS group (p = .001), and both ADHD groups (p < .0001). The AS group scored worse comparedto the control group (p = .018) but not the ADHD groups.

To evaluate whether the difference between verbal and performance estimated IQ differedamong the groups, a one way ANOVA was conducted. A significant group difference was found(F(4, 337) = 6.8, p < .0001, partial eta squared = .075). Post hoc comparisons indicated significantdifferences among the NLD group and all of the other groups with the NLD group showing thelargest split (p < .006). No other group differences were found.

A significant main effect for group was found for academic achievement (F(12, 804) = 6.97, p< .0001, partial eta squared = .084). (See Table 2.) Follow-up analysis found no significant differ-ence for reading recognition among the groups (p = .16) while mathematics calculation (p <.0001) and mathematics reasoning (p < .0001) were significant. Post-hoc analysis found signifi-cant differences among all of the clinical groups and the control group for mathematics calcula-tion (p < .0001) with the control group scoring better. Analysis of the clinical groups found thatthe NLD group performed significantly worse than the ADHD–C (p = .007) and ADHD–PI group(p = .04) on mathematics calculation but not the AS group (p = .16). The ADHD groups did notdiffer from each other (p = .39). The finding of differences for the NLD was not unexpected as itwas one of the criteria for selection for that group. For mathematics reasoning, the control groupperformed significantly better than all of the clinical groups (p < .04). Among the clinical groupsthe NLD group was less successful than the ADHD–C group (p < .0001) and the ADHD–PI group(p = .001) but not the AS group (p = .1). The ADHD groups did not differ from each other signifi-cantly (p = .84).

590 SEMRUD-CLIKEMAN, WALKOWIAK, WILKINSON, CHRISTOPHER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 12: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

591

TA

BLE

2S

igni

fican

tGro

upD

iffer

ence

son

Neu

rops

ycho

logi

calM

easu

res:

Mea

nsan

dS

tand

ard

Dev

iatio

ns

Con

trol

(N=

113)

AS2

(N=

50)

NL

D3

(N=

26)

AD

HD

-C4

(N=

76)

AD

HD

-PI5

(N=

80)

Mea

sure

Mea

nSD

1M

ean

SDM

ean

SDM

ean

SDM

ean

SDG

roup

Sign

ific

ance

PIQ

611

0.4

17.4

103.

918

.990

.918

.110

9.1

12.7

109.

514

.6A

S<

C(p

=.0

18)

NL

D<

C.A

DH

Dgr

oups

(p<

.000

1)N

LD

<A

S(p

<.0

01)

V-P

7Sp

lit1.

519

.23.

418

.418

.819

.8.7

116

–2.5

14.7

NL

D>

All

(p<

.006

)

Mat

hem

atic

sR

easo

ning

114.

013

.510

3.0

18.7

97.4

19.1

108.

316

.410

9.1

12.3

NV

LD

<A

DH

D-C

(p<

.000

1),A

DH

D-P

I(p

=.0

01)

C>

All

clin

ical

grou

ps(p

<.0

4)

Flui

dR

easo

ning

112.

613

.710

0.4

23.1

96.7

15.1

108

14.7

107.

113

.4A

S,N

LD

<C

(p<

.000

1)A

S<

AD

HD

-C(p

=.0

1),A

DH

D-P

I(p

=.0

3)N

LD

<A

DH

D-C

(p.0

03),

AD

HD

-PI

(p=

.006

)

Ana

lysi

s-Sy

nthe

sis

113.

813

.610

4.1

12.5

98.5

13.6

108.

516

.610

5.9

17.6

AS,

NL

D,A

DH

D-P

I<

C(p

<.0

3)

Con

cept

Form

atio

n11

0.1

13.7

102.

517

.996

.416

.810

5.5

18.1

106.

113

.4N

LD

<C

(p<

.000

1)A

S<

C(p

<.0

05)

NL

D<

AD

HD

-C,A

DH

D-P

I(p

<.0

1)

Rey O

ster

reith

–.16

.9–.

71.8

5–1

.72.

5–.

25.9

–.14

1.4

NL

D<

All

(p<

.01)

JLO

8–.

141.

2–.

081.

6–1

.81.

9–.

131.

5–.

31.

2N

LD

<A

ll(p

<.0

01)

Gro

oved

Peg

RH

9–.

821.

5–1

.64

2.6

–4.5

6.9

–.9

1.5

–.96

1.9

NL

D<

C(p

=.0

01),

AD

HD

-C(p

=.0

08),

AD

HD

-PI

(p=

.004

)

Gro

oved

Peg

LH

10–.

591.

3–2

.42.

8–2

.93.

5–1

.21.

6–1

.11.

8A

S,N

LD

<C

(p=

.01)

Tap

ping

RH

–.26

1.7

–.8

1.9

–1.4

1.7

.31.

2–.

21.

3N

.S.11

Tap

ping

LH

.21.

3–.

71.

7–.

8.9

5.3

41.

1–.

071

N.S

.

1 Sta

ndar

dde

viat

ion;

2 Asp

erge

rSy

ndro

me;

3 Non

verb

alL

earn

ing

Dis

abili

ty;4 A

ttent

ion

defi

cith

yper

activ

itydi

sord

er–C

ombi

ned

type

;5 Atte

ntio

nde

fici

thyp

erac

tivity

diso

rder

–Pre

dom

inat

ely

Inat

tent

ive

Typ

e;6 P

erfo

rman

ceIQ

;7 Ver

bal-

Perf

orm

ance

;8 Jud

gmen

tof

Lin

eO

rien

tatio

n;9 R

ight

hand

;10L

efth

and;

11N

otsi

gnif

ican

t.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 13: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

Fluid Reasoning Cluster

A significant main effect for group was found on the fluid reasoning measures (F(4, 305) = 8.28, p< .0001, partial eta squared = .1). (See Table 2.) Post-hoc analyses found that the NLD (p < .0001),AS (p < .0001), and ADHD–PI (p = .026) performed significantly worse than the control group onthis measure. Within the clinical groups the NLD group performed significantly worse than theADHD–C (p = .003) and ADHD–PI (p = .006) groups but not the AS group (p = .35). In addition,the AS group differed significantly from the ADHD–C (p = .010) and ADHD–PI (p = .03) groups.The ADHD groups did not differ from each other (p = .72).

When the individual subtests from the fluid reasoning cluster were evaluated a significant dif-ference was found for the analysis/synthesis subtest (p < .0001) and for the concept formationsubtest (p = .001). Post-hoc comparisons for the analysis/synthesis subtest found that the AS,NLD, and ADHD–PI all performed significantly worse than the controls (p < .03) while theADHD–C group did not differ significantly from the control group (p = .06). The NLD groupscored more poorly compared to the ADHD–C group (p = .01) with no other clinical group differ-ences found. On the concept formation subtest there was a significant difference between the AS(p = .005) and NLD (p < .0001) groups and the control group. The NLD group scored significantlymore poorly compared to the ADHD–C (p = .01) and ADHD–PI (p = .01) but not from the ASgroup (p = .12).

Visual-Spatial Abilities

To evaluate group differences on visual-motor and visual-spatial skills a 2 (JLO, Rey-Osterreith)× 5 (group) MANOVA was conducted. A significant main effect for group was present (F(8, 308)= 3.9, p < .0001, partial eta squared = .1). Follow-up analyses found that there was a group differ-ence for the Rey-Osterreith (p = .001) and the JLO (p < .0001). Post-hoc analysis found that theNLD group performed significantly worse than the control (p < .0001), AS (p = .01), and bothADHD groups (p < .0001) on the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure. There was no significant differ-ence among the other groups. Similarly for the JLO the NLD group performed significantly worsethan all of the groups (p < .001) with no differences found among the other groups. These findingsindicate significantly poorer performance on all measures of visual-organization and visual-spa-tial skills as predicted. To evaluate the relation among the level of social skill development andthese visual-spatial abilities, a correlation was performed. There was a significant overall correla-tion between the JLO and the SSRS social skills rating (p = .013). Similarly there was a significantoverall correlation between the Rey-Osterreith and the SSRS (p = .017).

Motor Skills

To evaluate complex motor skills a 4 (Grooved Pegboard RH, Grooved Pegboard LH, TappingRH, Tapping LH) × 5 (group) MANOVA found a significant main effect for group (F(16, 336) =2.56, p = .001, partial eta squared = .08). Follow-up analyses found significant differences presentfor the Grooved Pegboard RH (p = .002), Grooved Pegboard LH (p = .002), Tapping RH (p = .05),and Tapping LH (p =.02).

Post-hoc analyses found that for the Grooved Pegboard RH the NLD group was significantlyslower than the control group (p = .001) with no significant difference found among the ADHD

592 SEMRUD-CLIKEMAN, WALKOWIAK, WILKINSON, CHRISTOPHER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 14: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

groups, AS group, and the control group (p > .3). The NLD group performed significantly worsethan the ADHD–C (p = .008) and ADHD–PI group (p = .004) with no significance differencefrom the AS group (p = .056). For the Grooved Pegboard LH a significant differences was presentfor the AS (p = .01) and NLD (p = .01) groups compared to the control group.

Post-hoc analysis found that for Tapping LH the AS (p = .009) and NLD (p = .027) groups per-formed significantly worse than the control group. No significant differences were found betweenthe ADHD groups and the control group (p > .4). The AS group performed significantly worsethan the ADHD–C group (p = .01) but not the ADHD–PI group (p = .1) or the NLD group (p = .9).The NLD group performed significantly worse than the ADHD–C group (p = .03) but not theADHD–PI group (.13). No difference was present between the ADHD groups (p = .31).

DISCUSSION

One of the main aims of this study was to determine whether there are neuropsychological differ-ences among children with NLD, AS, or ADHD. A main research question was to explore skillson measures of visual-spatial, fluid reasoning, and motor skills compared to children with AS orADHD. It was hypothesized those children with ADHD–C and ADHD–PI would not show prob-lems in these areas.

Cognitive Differences

The hypothesis that the NLD group would score more poorly on the Performance IQ measurecompared to the other groups was confirmed. The AS group scored more poorly than the controlgroup but not the ADHD groups and there was no difference among the ADHD groups and thecontrol groups on this measure. The NLD group also scored more poorly than the AS group on thePerformance IQ of the WASI. In addition the Verbal-Performance IQ split was found to be signif-icantly larger only for the NLD group. No significant difference between verbal and performanceabilities was present among the other groups. These findings are consistent with those from the lit-erature suggesting the V-P split in children with NLD.

Our study did not select subjects based on this split. We utilized this procedure in order to de-termine whether this split is indeed present in this population. In the NLD sample 74% of the indi-viduals had a V > P split of more than 15 standard score points and 40% had a split of 25 standardscore points or higher (range from 15 to 55). This finding was different for the AS group where63% showed verbal and performance IQs within 15 of each other. However, 37% showed VIQ >PIQ in the AS group.

This finding indicates that while children with NLD are more likely to show a VIQ > PIQ split;there is a sizable minority of children who do not. While this study does find that most childrenwith NLD in our sample did show such a split, this finding is not sufficient to provide a distinctionbetween a diagnosis of NLD and that of AS. Other researchers have reached a similar conclusionwhen looking at children with NLD (Pelletier, Ahmad, & Rourke, 2001).

Conversely, for the AS group there is a sizable minority of children who also show a VIQ >PIQ split. Recently, Black, Wallace, Sokoloff, and Kenworthy (2009) found that children with au-tism with a discrepantly high verbal or performance IQ showed significantly more difficultieswith social functioning. Others have found that when the nonverbal IQ was higher than the verbal

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES 593

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 15: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

IQ more social difficulties were present (Joseph, Tager-Flusberg, & Lord, 2002). In the currentstudy a discrepancy between verbal and performance IQ in children with AS was also found withhigher verbal scores related to fewer social difficulties. A future study that evaluated children withNLD may evaluate whether less discrepant measures of ability are related to better outcomes orseverity of the disorder.

Academic findings were also similar to previous studies in that none of the children demon-strated a reading disability. Given that mathematics calculation skills were part of the inclusionarycriteria for our NLD group, it was not surprising that our NLD group scored more poorly on thissubtest (Drummond, Ahmad, & Rourke, 2005; Rourke, Ahmad, Collins, Hayman-Abello, &Warriner, 2002). This finding was also present for the mathematics reasoning subtest and is con-sistent with previous research studies (Pelletier et al., 2001; Strang & Rourke, 1985).

Findings from the measure of fluid reasoning indicated that the control group scored betterthan all of the clinical groups. Of more interest was the finding that both the NLD and AS groupsperformed significantly more poorly compared to the ADHD groups and did not differ from eachother on this measure. Behavioral difficulties are often present for children with NLD or AS whenthey are faced with a novel situation. It may well be that the deficit in fluid reasoning compromisestheir already limited ability to function in unfamiliar or complex situations. Previous studies havesuggested difficulties in concept-formation and novel problem-solving skills (Rourke & Tsatsanis,2000). Other researchers have begun to suggest that the difficulty some children with NLD havein social functioning is due to problems with executive functioning (Forrest, 2007).

Some studies have found that children with ASD may show good to excellent abilities on ameasure of matrix reasoning (Dawson, Soulieres, Gernsbacher, & Mottron, 2007; Hayashi, Kato,Igarashi, & Kashima, 2008). The Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices Test (Raven, Raven, &Court, 1993) requires the child to solve geometric designs by supplying the missing element bychoosing one of 6 to 8 alternatives that best match the design. This test differs from the WJ-Cog IIImeasures utilized in the current study. The fluid reasoning portion of the WJ-Cog III requires notonly solving of geometric designs but also the use of categorical reasoning, inductive and deduc-tive logic, and the ability to shift set (Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006). In contrast the Raven’shas been found to be a unidimensional estimate of reasoning ability (Raven, Raven, & Court,2000). The relation of the WJ-Cog III fluid reasoning test to social functioning is likely strongerthan that found for the Raven’s because of the multidimensional nature of social reasoning.

Our findings of difficulty with fluid reasoning for both NLD and AS groups support the sug-gestion that these difficulties complicate the child’s ability to solve novel, visually complex prob-lems. These difficulties may contribute to problems with perspective-taking as well as with theability to adapt to changing environmental demands. Children with ADHD were not found to havesignificant difficulties on this task. This finding suggests that problems encoding visual stimulifor children with AS and NLD contributes to social problems while for children with ADHD so-cial difficulties may not be due to inaccurate perception but rather to impulsivity and inattention(Fine et al., 2008). Further study is necessary to untangle the relation of executive functioning andattention to social competence.

Denckla (2000) suggests that there is a “cognitive overlap zone” of executive functions in NLDand ADHD due to overlapping neural regions. Neuroimaging provides some support for this the-ory. Structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have found differencesin right hemispheric frontal and striatal networks as well as in white matter structures (i.e., corpuscallosum) in children with ADHD (Castellanos et al., 1994; Castellanos et al., 1996; Giedd et al.,

594 SEMRUD-CLIKEMAN, WALKOWIAK, WILKINSON, CHRISTOPHER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 16: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

1994; Hynd et al., 1993; Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2000). Similarly, Rourke (1995) suggested thatdifficulty was present in the right-hemispheric white matter in NLD. Differences have also beenfound in the smaller volume of the corpus callosum for children with AS despite control for whitematter volume (Chung, Dalton, Alexander, & Davidson, 2004), higher volumes of white matter(Filipek, 1999), and increased volume in the caudate regions (Sears et al., 1999). While the emerg-ing MRI research in AS suggests white matter involvement in the disorder, the findings are notconclusive. Prospective research in imaging for children with NLD has not been published butfindings from existing neurological cases does suggest involvement of the right hemisphere andpossibly of white matter (Voeller, 1995). Further study is needed to determine the validity of theright hemispheric white matter hypothesis in NLD.

Visual-spatial differences. The NLD group showed significantly more difficulty com-pared to the other groups on the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure test. Impairment on this test isthought to reflect problems with planning, organization, and visual-spatial reasoning as well as vi-sual-motor skills. These findings are consistent with previous studies for children with NLD(Wilkinson & Semrud-Clikeman, 2008; Woods, Weinborn, Ball, Tiller-Nevin, & Pickett, 2000;Worling, Humphries, & Tannock, 1999). For the children with AS no significant differences werefound compared to the other three groups. This finding is consistent with that of Edgin and Pen-nington (2005) who found few problems in spatial cognition in children with AS while othershave found problems with visual organization (Kenworthy et al., 2005). These findings indicatedthat copying a complex figure may be an area of specific difficulty for children with NLD. Re-viewing the individual scores supports this hypothesis to some extent. In the NLD sample 63%showed scores in the below average range while for the AS sample 39% scored in the below aver-age range.

Similarly the JLO, a measure of spatial reasoning without a motor component, was found todiffer among the groups. The NLD group scored more poorly compared to the other groups.Children with AS were found to show, as a group, average to above average performance on theJLO (79% of the sample scored within the average to above average range). These visual-spatialmeasures were among the few that differentiated children with NLD from those with AS.

Motor. The literature suggests that a right hemispheric deficit is present in children withNLD and as such more difficulty would be seen with the left hand than the right hand measures. Ithas also been hypothesized that children with NLD show bilateral difficulties in fine motor skills(Rourke, 1995). We found only partial support for this hypothesis. While our findings indicatedthat the NLD and AS groups performed more poorly on a measure of complex fine motor func-tioning compared to controls with the left hand and bilaterally for the NLD group, these measuresdid not differ significantly from the scores obtained by the ADHD groups and as such may not bespecific to AS or NLD.

Limitations of the Study

There are several limitations to this study. One limitation was the lower number of children with adiagnosis of NLD. It required several years to find sufficient numbers of children who met criteriafor NLD who did not also meet criteria for AS. The sample for this study did not include childrenwith genetic or other disorders that have been associated with NLD (velocardiofacial syndrome,Williams syndrome) and these results may not be generalizable to groups of children with medical

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES 595

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 17: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

and/or genetic disorders. An additional issue is the possible confound between the visual fluid rea-soning tests of the WJ-Cog III and the visual-perceptual difficulties found in the children withNLD and many of those with a diagnosis of AS. These results should be interpreted in light ofthese problems and our findings are consistent with previous research, also using visual tasks thatfind problems with concept formation and executive functioning (Fisher, DeLuca, & Rourke,1997; Sikora, Haley, Edwards, & Butler, 2002)

Another limitation of the study was that we were not able to obtain the scores from the ADOSor ADI–R. These measures were obtained in a private clinic setting with the results were not avail-able to our study. In the future selection of subjects using these measures would provide for betterdiagnostics and the ability to evaluate performance based on number of symptoms reported on theADI–R.

Finally, our sample of children with NLD did show significant difficulties on the estimatedperformance test from the WASI. It was elected not to use covariate procedures to control for thisgroup difference. There are several articles that question the use of covariate procedures to equategroups (Adams, Brown, & Grant, 1985; Miller & Chapman, 2001) while others suggest thatANCOVAs are appropriate when evaluating treatment decisions (van Breukelen, 2006). In thecase of this study there were no differences in verbal estimated IQ—one of the selection criteria.We elected not to covary for Full Scale estimated IQ which did significantly differ among thegroups due to the group differences on performance estimated IQ given the concerns voiced inthese various articles.

Conclusion

Taken together, these findings indicate that the theoretical model suggested by Rourke (1988)provides a backdrop for our understanding of NLD. The performance deficits seen on the motortasks also suggest bilateral involvement of the brain with the NLD group scoring more poorly onboth right and left hands compared to the other groups. In contrast the AS group only showed dif-ferences with the LH. These findings do suggest difficulties in motor coordination for the NLDand AS groups with poorer performance present for the right hemisphere than for the left. Somehave suggested that children with NLD may appear to be clumsy and uncoordinated (Rourke,1988), while others have found a wide variation in bimanual motor coordination in children withAS (Gunter et al., 2002). Our study suggests that there is a wide variation in these skills for bothgroups and problems with coordination may not be an area specifically different for children withNLD (Wilkinson & Semrud-Clikeman, 2008).

Findings of differences in visual-spatial skills were strongly present for children with NLDeven when motor disability was not present. The finding of differences in visual-spatial skills andperformance abilities implicate the posterior regions of the brain. These difficulties, however,were present only for the NLD group. No difference was found among the AS, ADHD, or controlgroups on visual-spatial ability. This finding suggests that there may be differences between theNLD and AS groups in these areas; an issue that has been the subject of debate in the literature. Itmay well be that the discriminating difference between NLD and AS is in visual-spatial ability,thus it is strongly recommended that these measures to be included in the neuropsychological bat-tery when evaluating these children. A comparison between children with NLD and those diag-nosed with pervasive developmental disorder, not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS) has not beenconducted to determine what commonality may be present. One issue with such a comparison is

596 SEMRUD-CLIKEMAN, WALKOWIAK, WILKINSON, CHRISTOPHER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 18: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

the lack of definition for the diagnosis of PDD-NOS; basically this diagnosis is provided when thechild does not meet criteria for any other diagnosis in the autistic spectrum but has significant so-cial difficulty.

The difference between verbal and performance abilities was found specifically for childrenwith NLD even though the selection criteria had not required such a split. These findings supportclinical reports of larger than expected Verbal-Performance IQ splits in children with NLD. It isimportant to note that a sizable minority of children with AS also showed such a split; thus, thesplit alone is not sufficient for discriminating between NLD and AS for diagnostic purposes. Withlarger numbers in our study these findings may have been more robust. It is difficult, however, torecruit sufficient numbers of well-diagnosed NLD children as it required 7 years of recruiting toobtain our sample size in a large metropolitan region.

While attention is an important issue for children with AS or NLD, it was not strongly related toour measures. One of the issues for our study was attempting to control for attentional problems fre-quently found in children with AS or NLD for which we utilized ADHD–C and ADHD–PI groups.In the future, other studies may wish to contrast children with AS and comorbid ADHD symptoms,those with AS without ADHD symptomatology, as well as that combination for children with NLD.We cannot rule out that attentional issues may have played a role in some of our findings.

REFERENCES

Adams, K. M., Brown, G. G., & Grant, I. (1985). Analysis of covariance as a remedy for demographic mismatch of re-search subject groups: Some sobering simulations. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7, 445–464.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.).Washington, DC: Author.

Beery, K. E., Buktenica, N. A., & Beery, N. A. (2006). Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration-5. San Antonio:Pearson.

Benton, A. L., Sivan, A. B., Hamsher, K., Varney, N. R., & Spreen, O. (2004). Contributions to neuropsychological as-sessment: A clinical manual (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

Black, D. O., Wallace, G. L., Sokoloff, J. L., & Kenworthy, L. (2009). Brief report: IQ split predicts social symptoms andcommunication abilities in high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Develop-mental Disorders, 39, 1613–1619.

Castellanos, F. X., Giedd, J. N., Eckburg, W. L., Marsh, A. C., Kaysen, D., Hamburger, S. D., et al. (1994). Quantitativemorphology of the caudate nucleus in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry,151(1212), 1791–1796.

Castellanos, F. X., Giedd, J. N., Marsh, W. L., Hamburger, S. D., Vaiturzis, A. C., & Dickstein, D. P. (1996). Quantitativebrain magnetic resonance imaging in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53(7),607–616.

Chung, M. K., Dalton, K. M., Alexander, A. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2004). Less white matter concentration in autism: 2Dvoxel-based morphometry. NeuroImage, 23(1), 242–251.

Corbett, B. A., & Constantine, L. J. (2006). Autism and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Assessing attention andresponse control with the integrated visual and auditory continuous performance test. Child Neuropsychology, 12,335–348.

Corwin, J., & Bylsma, F. W. (1993). The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7(9–15).Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Gernsbacher, M. A., & Mottron, L. (2007). The level and nature of autistic intelligence. Psycho-

logical Science, 18, 657–662.Denckla, M. B. (2000). Learning disabilities and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: Overlap with executive

dysfunction. In T. E. Brown (Ed.), Attention-deficit disorders and comorbidities in children, adolescents, and adults(pp. 297–318). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES 597

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 19: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

Drummond, C. R., Ahmad, S. A., & Rourke, B. P. (2005). Rules for the classification of younger children with nonverballearning disabilities and basic phonological processing disabilities. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 171–182.

Edgin, J. O., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Spatial cognition in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Superior, impaired, or just in-tact? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 729–745.

Filipek, P. A. (1999). Neuroimaging in the developmental disorders: The state of the science. The Journal of Child Psy-chology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(1), 113–128.

Fine, J. G., Semrud-Clikeman, M., Butcher, B., & Walkowiak, J. (2008). Brief report: Attention effect on a measure of so-cial perception Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1797–1802.

Fisher, N. J., DeLuca, J. W., & Rourke, B. P. (1997). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and Halstead Category Test perfor-mances of children and adolescents who exhibit the syndrome of Nonverbal Learning Disabilities. Child Neuro-psychology, 3, 61–70.

Forrest, B. (2007). Diagnosing and treating right hemisphere disorders. In S. J. Hunter & J. Donders (Eds.), Pediatricneuropsychological intervention (pp. 175–192). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Gadow, K. D., DeVincent, C. J., & Pomeroy, J. (2006). ADHD symptom subtypes in children with pervasive developmen-tal disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(2), 271–283.

Ghaziuddin, M., Weidmer-Mikhail, E., & Ghaziuddin, N. (1998). Comorbidity of Asperger syndrome: A preliminary re-port. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 42(4), 279–283.

Giedd, J. N., Castellanos, F. X., Casey, B. J., Kozuch, P., King, A. C., Hamburger, S. D., et al. (1994). Quantitative mor-phology of the corpus callosum in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151(5),665–669.

Gillberg, C., & Billstedt, E. (2000). Autism and Asperger syndrome: Coexistence with other clinical disorders. ActaPsychiatrica Scandinavica, 102(5), 321–330.

Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social Skills Rating System. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc.Guli, L. A., Wilkinson, A., & Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2008). Social Competence Intervention Program. Champaign, IL:

Research Press.Gunter, H. L., Ghaziuddin, M., & Ellis, H. (2002). Asperger syndrome: Tests of right hemisphere functioning and

interhemispheric communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(4), 263–281.Hayashi, M., Kato, M., Igarashi, K., & Kashima, H. (2008). Superior fluid intelligence in children with Asperger’s disor-

der. Brain and Cognition, 66, 306–310.Hynd, G. W., Hern, K. L., Novey, E. S., Eliopulos, D., Marshall, R., Gonzalez, J. J., et al. (1993). Attention deficit-hyper-

activity disorder and asymmetry of the caudate nucleus. Journal of Child Neurology, 8(4), 339–340.Johnson, D. J., & Myklebust, H. R. (1967). Learning disablities: Educational principles and practices. New York: Grune

& Stratton.Joseph, R. M., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Lord, C. (2002). Cognitive profiles and social-communicative functioning in chil-

dren with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 807–821.Kenworthy, L. E., Black, D. O., Wallace, G. L., Ahluvalia, T., Wagner, A. E., & Sirian, L. M. (2005). Disorganization: The

forgotten executive dysfunction in High-Functioning Autism (HFA) Spectrum Disorders. Developmental Neuro-psychology, 28, 809–827.

Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Rourke, B. P. (1995). Validity and neuropsychological characterization ofAsperger syndrome: Convergence with nonverbal Learning Disabilities syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology andPsychiatry, 36(1127–1140).

Klove, H. (1963). Clinical neuropsychology. In F. M. Forster (Ed.), The medical clinics of North America (pp.1647–1658). New York: Saunders.

Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psycho-logical Services.

Leyfer, O. T., Folstein, S. E., Bacalman, S., Davis, N. O., Dinh, E., Morgan, J., et al. (2006). Comorbid psychiatric disor-ders in children with Autism: Interview development and rates of disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-orders, V36(7), 849–861.

Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (Fourth ed.). New York: OxfordUniversity Press.

Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Los Angeles, CA:Western Psychological Services.

Miller, G. M., & Chapman, J. P. (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110,40–48.

598 SEMRUD-CLIKEMAN, WALKOWIAK, WILKINSON, CHRISTOPHER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 20: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

Nigg, J. T., Blaskey, L. G., Huang-Pollock, C. L., & Rappley, M. D. (2002). Neuropsychological executive functions andDSM-IV ADHD subtypes. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 59–66.

Nyden, A., Gillberg, C., Hjelmquist, E., & Heiman, M. (1999). Executive function/attention deficits in boys with Aspergersyndrome, attention disorder and reading/writing disorder. Autism, 3(3), 213–228.

Osterreith, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une figure complexe. Archives de Psychologie, 30(206), 356.Ozonoff, S., & Griffith, E. M. (2000). Neuropsychological function and the external validity of Asperger syndrome. In A.

Klin, F. R. Volkmar, & S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome (pp. 72–96). New York: Guilford Press.Pelletier, P. M., Ahmad, S. A., & Rourke, B. P. (2001). Classification rules for basic phonological processing disabilities

and nonverbal learning disabilities: Formulation and external validity. Child Neuropsychology, 7(2), 84–98.Pennington, B. F. (1991). Diagnosing learning disorders: A neuropsychological framework. New York: The Guilford

Press.Pennington, B. F. (2008). Diagnosing learning disorders: A neuropsychological framework (2nd ed.). New York:

Guilford.Psychological Corporation. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment,

Inc.Puig-Antich, J., & Chambers, W. J. (1978). The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age

Children (K-SADS). New York: New York Psychiatric Press.Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1993). Raven manual: Section 1: General overview. Oxford, England: Psycholo-

gists Press.Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2000). Raven manual research supplement 3: American norms, neuro-

psychological applications. Oxford, England: Oxford Psychologists Press, Ltd.Reddon, J. R., Gill, D. M., Gauk, S. E., & Maerz, M. D. (1988). Purdue pegboard: Test-retest estimates. Perceptual and

Motor Skills, 66, 503–506.Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Interpretation.

Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2004). Behavior Assessment System for Children-2. Circle Pines, MN: Pearson As-

sessments.Rourke, B. P. (1988). The syndrome of nonverbal learning disabilities: Developmental manifestations in neurological dis-

ease and dysfunction. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 4, 293–330.Rourke, B. P. (1989). Nonverbal learning disabilities: The syndrome and the model. New York: Guilford Press.Rourke, B. P. (1995). The NLD syndrome and the white matter model. In B. P. Rourke (Ed.), Syndrome of nonverbal

learning disabilities: Neurodevelopmental manifestations (pp. 1–26). New York: Guilford Press.Rourke, B. P. (2000). Neuropsychological and psychosocial subtyping: A review of investigations within the University

of Windsor laboratory. Canadian Psychology, 41(1), 34–51.Rourke, B. P., Ahmad, S. A., Collins, D. W., Hayman-Abello, B. A., & Warriner, E. M. (2002). Child clinical/pediatric

neuropsychology: Some recent advances. Review of Psychology, 53, 309–339.Rourke, B. P., & Tsatsanis, K. D. (1996). Syndrome of nonverbal learning disabilities: Psycholinguistic assets and deficits.

Topics in Language Disorders, 16(2), 30–44.Rourke, B. P., & Tsatsanis, K. D. (2000). Nonverbal learning disabilities. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar, & S. S. Sparrow

(Eds.), Asperger syndrome (pp. 231–253). New York: The Guilford Press.Sears, L. L., Vest, C., Mohamed, S., Bailey, J., Ranson, B. J., & Piven, J. (1999). An MRI study of the basal ganglia in au-

tism. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 23, 613–624.Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2003). Executive functions and social communication disorders. Perspectives, 29, 20–22.Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2007). Social competence in children. New York: Springer.Semrud-Clikeman, M., & Glass, K. L. (2008). Comprehension of humor in children with Nonverbal Learning Disabilities,

Verbal Learning Disabilities and without Learning Disabilities. Annals of Dyslexia, 58, 163–180.Semrud-Clikeman, M., Steingard, R., Filipek, P. A., Bekken, K., Biederman, J., & Renshaw, P. F. (2000). Neuroanatomi-

cal-neuropsychological correlates of ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39,477–484.

Sikora, D. M., Haley, P., Edwards, J., & Butler, R. W. (2002). Tower of London test performance in children with poorarithmetic skills. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 243–254.

Strang, J. D., & Rourke, B. P. (1985). Arithmetic disability subtypes: The neruopsychological significant of specific arith-metical impairment in childhood. In B. P. Rourke (Ed.), Neuropsychology of learning disabilities: Essentials of subtypeanalysis (pp. 167–183). New York: Guilford Press.

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES 599

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13

Page 21: Neuropsychological Differences Among Children With Asperger Syndrome, Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Attention Deficit Disorder, and Controls

Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests (Third ed.). New York:Oxford University Press.

Tiffin, J. (1968). Purdue Pegboard: Examiner Manual. Chicago: Science Research Associates.van Breukelen, G. J. P. (2006). ANCOVA versus change from baseline had more power in randomized studies and more

bias in nonrandomized studies. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 59, 920–925.Voeller, K. K. (1995). Clinical neurological aspects of the right hemisphere deficit syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology,

10, 16–22.Wilkinson, A., & Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2008). Motor speed in children and adolescents with nonverbal learning disabil-

ities. Paper presented at the International Neuropsychological Society, February 2008, Waikoloa, HI.Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001a). Woodcock-Johnson III Test of Cognitive Abilities. Rolling

Meadows, IL: Riverside Publishing.Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001b). Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement. Itasca, IL: River-

side.Woods, S. P., Weinborn, M., Ball, J. D., Tiller-Nevin, S., & Pickett, T. C. (2000). Periventricular leukomalacia (PVL): An

identical twin case study illustration of white matter dysfunction and nonverbal learning disability (NLD). ChildNeuropsychology, 6, 274–285.

Worling, D. E., Humphries, T., & Tannock, R. (1999). Spatial and emotional aspects of language influencing in nonverballearning disabilities. Brain and Language, 70, 220–239.

600 SEMRUD-CLIKEMAN, WALKOWIAK, WILKINSON, CHRISTOPHER

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f B

irm

ingh

am]

at 1

2:47

19

Sept

embe

r 20

13


Recommended