+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Neuropsychological sequelae of bacterial and viral meningitisNeuropsychological sequelae of...

Neuropsychological sequelae of bacterial and viral meningitisNeuropsychological sequelae of...

Date post: 15-Feb-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 15 times
Download: 1 times
Share this document with a friend
13
doi:10.1093/brain/awh711 Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 Neuropsychological sequelae of bacterial and viral meningitis H. Schmidt, 1 B. Heimann, 3 M. Djukic, 1 C. Mazurek, 1 C. Fels, 2 C.-W. Wallesch 3 and R. Nau 1 Georg August University Go ¨ ttingen 1 Department of Neurology, 2 Department of Neuroradiology, Robert-Koch-Street 40, D-37075 Go ¨ttingen and Otto von Guericke University of Magdeburg, 3 Department of Neurology, Magdeburg, Germany Correspondence to: H. Schmidt, University of Go ¨ ttingen, Department of Neurology, Robert-Koch-Street 40, 37099 Go ¨ ttingen, Germany E-mail: [email protected] Survivors of meningitis often complain about neurological and neuropsychological consequences. In this study, the extent of these sequelae was quantified and correlated to MRI findings. Neurological, neuropsychological and neuroradiological examinations were performed with adult patients younger than 70 years, 1–12 years after recovery from bacterial meningitis (BM; n = 59), or from viral meningitis (VM; n = 59). Patients with other potential causes for neuropsychological deficits (e.g. alcoholism) were carefully excluded. Patients were com- pared to 30 healthy subjects adjusted for age, gender and length of school education. With the exception of attention functions, both patient groups showed more frequently pathological results than the control group for all domains examined. Applying an overall cognitive sum score, patients after BM did not differ significantly in their performance from patients after VM. Separate analyses of various cognitive domains, however, revealed a higher rate of persistent disturbances in short-term and working memory after BM than after VM. Moreover, patients after BM exhibited greater impairment of executive functions. Associative learning of verbal material was also reduced. These deficits could not be ascribed to impaired alertness functions or decreased motivation in BM patients. Applying a logistic regression model, the neuropsychological outcome was related to the neurological outcome. Patients with a Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS) of <5 had more frequently impaired test results for non-verbal learning and memory. GOS was also correlated with performance in executive functions. Brain volume was lower and ventricular volume was higher in the bacterial than in the VM group, and cerebral volume and the amount of white matter lesions of patients after BM were negatively correlated with short-term and working memory. In conclusion, patients after both BM and VM with favourable outcome showed affected learning and memory functions. More patients after BM than after VM displayed pathological short-term and working memory. BM resulted in poorer performance in executive functions, language, short- term memory and verbal learning/memory tests. As a result of neurological and neuropsychological sequelae, BM with a GOS 4 led to decreased activities of daily living but only a minority of patients were disabled in a way that social functions were affected. The extent of neuropsychological sequelae of BM might have been over- estimated in earlier studies which often had not been controlled for comorbidity factors such as alcoholism. Keywords: bacterial meningitis; viral meningitis; neuropsychological sequelae; neurological sequelae; MRI Abbreviations: AAT = Aachen Aphasia Test; BM = bacterial meningitis; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test; GCS = Glasgow Coma Scale; GOS = Glasgow Outcome Scale; HAWIE-R = Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest fu ¨r Erwachsene-R; SD = standard deviation; SNRS = Scripps Neurological Rating Scale; VM = viral meningitis; VV = ventricular volume; WMS-R = Wechsler Memory Scale-R Received February 3, 2005. Revised October 20, 2005. Accepted November 3, 2005. Advance Access publication December 19, 2005 Introduction Survivors of bacterial meningitis (BM) often complain of neurological (Durand et al., 1993) and neuropsychological (Bohr et al., 1983) sequelae. The overall mortality rate can reach 20% (van de Beek et al., 2004). Cognitive deficiencies after BM in children (predominantly persistent difficulties in learning, deficits in short-term memory, behavioural prob- lems and poorer academic performance) (Grimwood et al., 2000) have been described. In adults, to our knowledge, only # The Author (2005). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: [email protected] by guest on October 6, 2016 http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ Downloaded from
Transcript

doi:10.1093/brain/awh711 Brain (2006), 129, 333–345

Neuropsychological sequelae of bacterial andviral meningitis

H. Schmidt,1 B. Heimann,3 M. Djukic,1 C. Mazurek,1 C. Fels,2 C.-W. Wallesch3 and R. Nau1

Georg August University Gottingen 1Department of Neurology, 2Department of Neuroradiology, Robert-Koch-Street 40,D-37075 Gottingen and Otto von Guericke University of Magdeburg, 3Department of Neurology, Magdeburg, Germany

Correspondence to: H. Schmidt, University of Gottingen, Department of Neurology, Robert-Koch-Street 40,37099 Gottingen, GermanyE-mail: [email protected]

Survivors of meningitis often complain about neurological and neuropsychological consequences. In this study,the extent of these sequelae was quantified and correlated to MRI findings. Neurological, neuropsychologicaland neuroradiological examinations were performedwith adult patients younger than 70 years, 1–12 years afterrecovery from bacterial meningitis (BM; n = 59), or from viral meningitis (VM; n = 59). Patients with otherpotential causes for neuropsychological deficits (e.g. alcoholism) were carefully excluded. Patients were com-pared to 30 healthy subjects adjusted for age, gender and length of school education. With the exception ofattention functions, both patient groups showedmore frequently pathological results than the control group forall domains examined. Applying an overall cognitive sum score, patients after BM did not differ significantly intheir performance from patients after VM. Separate analyses of various cognitive domains, however, revealed ahigher rate of persistent disturbances in short-term and working memory after BM than after VM. Moreover,patients after BM exhibited greater impairment of executive functions. Associative learning of verbal materialwas also reduced. These deficits could not be ascribed to impaired alertness functions or decreased motivationin BM patients. Applying a logistic regression model, the neuropsychological outcome was related to theneurological outcome. Patients with a Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS) of <5 had more frequently impairedtest results for non-verbal learning and memory. GOS was also correlated with performance in executivefunctions. Brain volume was lower and ventricular volumewas higher in the bacterial than in the VM group, andcerebral volume and the amount of white matter lesions of patients after BM were negatively correlated withshort-term and working memory. In conclusion, patients after both BM and VM with favourable outcomeshowed affected learning and memory functions. More patients after BM than after VM displayed pathologicalshort-term and working memory. BM resulted in poorer performance in executive functions, language, short-term memory and verbal learning/memory tests. As a result of neurological and neuropsychological sequelae,BMwith aGOS� 4 led to decreased activities of daily living but only aminority of patientswere disabled in awaythat social functions were affected. The extent of neuropsychological sequelae of BM might have been over-estimated in earlier studies which often had not been controlled for comorbidity factors such as alcoholism.

Keywords: bacterial meningitis; viral meningitis; neuropsychological sequelae; neurological sequelae; MRI

Abbreviations: AAT = Aachen Aphasia Test; BM = bacterial meningitis; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test;GCS = Glasgow Coma Scale; GOS = Glasgow Outcome Scale; HAWIE-R = Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest furErwachsene-R; SD = standard deviation; SNRS = Scripps Neurological Rating Scale; VM = viral meningitis;VV = ventricular volume; WMS-R = Wechsler Memory Scale-R

Received February 3, 2005. Revised October 20, 2005. Accepted November 3, 2005. Advance Access publication December 19, 2005

IntroductionSurvivors of bacterial meningitis (BM) often complain of

neurological (Durand et al., 1993) and neuropsychological

(Bohr et al., 1983) sequelae. The overall mortality rate can

reach 20% (van de Beek et al., 2004). Cognitive deficiencies

after BM in children (predominantly persistent difficulties in

learning, deficits in short-term memory, behavioural prob-

lems and poorer academic performance) (Grimwood et al.,

2000) have been described. In adults, to our knowledge, only

# The Author (2005). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: [email protected]

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

two systematical neuropsychological follow-up studies with

a sufficient number of participants have been published

(Merkelbach et al., 2000; van de Beek et al., 2002). Unfortu-

nately, both studies performed comparisons only with healthy

subjects, and the participating patients were not controlled for

alcoholism or any other substance abuse. Alcoholism, how-

ever, is a predisposing factor for bacterial infections (Huang

et al., 2002), especially due to streptococci (Siboni et al., 1989)

and Listeria monocytogenes (Siboni et al., 1989; Aladro Benito

et al., 1995). Without information about the cognitive status

before onset of BM, it is not possible to differentiate between

cognitive dysfunction caused by alcohol and that caused by

meningitis. A Dutch study group (van de Beek et al., 2002)

demonstrated that patients after Streptococcus pneumoniae

meningitis often suffered long-lasting cognitive impairment,

whereas patients who had survived meningococcal meningitis

were hardly affected. Secondary brain lesions caused by brain

oedema, increased intracranial pressure, vasospasm, vascu-

litis, cerebral venous thrombosis and primary brain cell

death due to neurotoxicity of bacterial products or pro-

inflammatory mediators (Bohr et al., 1983; Pfister et al.,

1993; Nau et al., 2004) are responsible for persisting deficits

after BM. Brain damage induced by BM could be demon-

strated in both post-mortem (Nau et al., 1999) and MRI

examinations (Free et al., 1996). Although MRI alterations

after BM such as global atrophy (Davidson and Steiner, 1985)

or focal hippocampal atrophy (Free et al., 1996) have been

reported, they have not been related to cognitive disturbances.

The present study was designed to examine the neuropsycho-

logical impact of BM in humans. Therefore, through the use

of strict selection criteria the study population was selected to

minimize concomitant disease which could potentially influ-

ence cognitive assessments.

MethodsPatientsAll files of patients who were coded as ‘suspected meningitis’ at

admission to the University Hospital, Gottingen, during the past

12 years were screened. The diagnosis of ‘suspected meningitis’

was made clinically when patients presented with headache, fever,

neck-stiffness, qualitative or quantitative disturbances of conscious-

ness, photophobia, vomiting or signs of systemic inflammation in the

blood tests. Only patients who had received a lumbar puncture and

with a sufficient clinical documentation were eligible for the inclu-

sion/exclusion algorithm of our study as described in Fig. 1A. The

study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of

Gottingen.

Control groupThirty healthy subjects without a medical history of neurological

disease selected to match the age, gender and length of school edu-

cation of the study population served as a neuropsychological control

group. In order to rule out neurological impairment, they underwent

a physical neurological examination which had to be normal. To

ensure the reliability of our statements on normal and pathological

results of the patient groups, the test results of the control group were

transformed into Z-values. They served as a standard to identify

pathological results in both patient groups.

Inclusion/exclusion criteriaPatients with definite bacteriological (positive culture result or

positive Gram stain) or �2 distinctive laboratory signs of bac-

terial infection of the CNS (CSF leucocyte count �1000/ml, CSF

lactate concentration �3.0 mmol/l, CSF protein concentration

�1000 mg/l) in addition to clinical signs of meningitis, were assigned

to the BM group. Patients with meningitis at admission were

assigned to the viral meningitis (VM) group if there were a CSF

pleocytosis <1000/l, CSF protein concentration <1000 mg/l and

CSF lactate <3.0 mmol/l. The microbiological examination of the

CSF which included microscopy and bacterial culture had to be

negative, and the plasma C-reactive protein concentration was

required to be�100 mg/l. Cut-off values were derived from previous

studies on the differential diagnosis of BM (Karandanis and

Shulman, 1976; Berg et al., 1982; Ilanson et al., 1993; Leib et al.,

1999). Patients eligible for the VM group who had received prior

antibiotic treatment were excluded in order to prevent contamina-

tion of this group with patients suffering from mild BM. Exclusion

criteria for both patient and control groups were as follows:

� age under 15 years� evidence for alcoholism (as gained by information from the

patients themselves, their relatives or by typical laboratory find-

ings)� any other addictive disorders� age over 70 years� poor skills in German (non-native German speakers were only

included if their German was fluent)� ambiguous clinical results concerning the differential diagnosis

(see above)� any known affective or other psychiatric disease� known neurological disorders potentially affecting the CNS� severe recent life events that might have interfered with neuropsy-

chological testing� known systemic neoplasms� use of sedatives or neuroleptic medication.

In Fig. 1A, the inclusion/exclusion tree of the study is depicted.

Fig. 1B and C show the composition of the bacterial and VM group,

respectively. After application of the exclusion/inclusion algorithm,

eligible patients were contacted by an informal letter describing the

project. If patients did not refuse to be approached, they were called

by telephone. A standardized telephone interview adapted from the

Malt questionnaire (Malt et al., 1989) was performed. Patients were

invited for a follow-up examination in our outpatients clinic. Fifty-

nine patients with BM and 59 with VM were recruited.

Physical conditionA physical and neurological examination was performed by two

physicians (M.D. and H.S.). Existing outcome scales for neurological

function after meningitis are brief for reasons of clinical feasibility

(Bohr et al., 1984, 1985). Therefore, we applied the Scripps Neuro-

logical Rating Scale (SNRS), originally developed for the evaluation

of multiple sclerosis patients (Sipe et al., 1984), which met our needs

for thorough documentation and reproducibility. In addition, the

Nottingham scale of activities of daily living (Nouri and Lincoln,

1987), quality of life scores/psychic well-being (SCL-90-R GSI, PSI)

(Olsen et al., 2004) were applied. We extracted Glasgow Coma Scale

334 Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 H. Schmidt et al.

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

(GCS) data from the patients’ files on admission (Teasdale and

Jennett, 1974). The Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS) (Jennett and

Bond, 1975) was scored at re-examination.

Neuropsychological evaluationThe neuropsychological test battery focused on attention, mnestic

and executive cognitive functions. General intelligence measure-

ments were omitted in favour of more concise testing in the

above-mentioned domains. Attention functions were measured

with the ‘Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprufung’ (TAP)

(Zimmermann and Fimm, 1994; Becker et al., 1996) focusing on

selective attention (TAP subtest ‘alertness’), stimulus selectivity

(TAP subtest ‘go-no-go’) and divided attention (TAP subtest

‘divided attention’). Short-term and working memory were assessed

with the German version of the ‘Wechsler Memory Scale-R’ (WMS-

R) [subtests digit span forward/backward (fw/bw); blockspan fw/bw;

logical memory (LM), part I] (Harting et al., 2000), and the first trial

of the ‘California Verbal Learning Test’ (CVLT) (Delis et al., 1988).

Verbal learning and long-term memory were tested with WMS-R

(subtests ‘verbal pair association; LM’, part II), CVLT (Delis et al.,

1988) and with the ‘Verbal Learning Test’ (VLT) (Sturm and

A

Fig. 1 Continued.

Neuropsychological sequelae and MRI changes after meningitis Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 335

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Willmes, 1999b). Non-verbal learning and memory were

measured with the WMS-R (subtest visual pair association), the

complex Rey figures test (Osterrieth, 1944), the ‘Non-Verbal Learn-

ing Test’ (NVLT) (Sturm and Willmes, 1999a) and ‘Lern- und

Gedachtnistest 3’ (LGT-3, subtest city map) (Baumler, 1974). Exec-

utive functions were examined by verbal fluency tasks

(‘Regensburger Wortflussigkeitstest’ [RWT, subtests lexical fluency

with and without alterations, subtests semantic (sem.) fluency with

and without alterations] (Aschenbrenner et al., 2000), figural fluency

(FF) tasks [Ruff’s FF test (Ruff, 1988)], a verbal concept formation

(CF) task (‘Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest fur Erwachsene-R’,

HAWIE-R (Tewes, 1991); subtest ‘Gemeinsamkeiten finden’; simil-

arities), CVLT clustering and a figural concept formation task

[Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, WCST (Nelson, 1976)]. In addition

to attention, mnestic and executive functions, we tested visuo-

constructive (HAWIE-R subtest Mosaic test and Rey complex figure

copy) and language functions [‘Aachen Aphasia Test’ (Huber et al.,

1983) AAT subtest Token test]. The following domains and cut-off

values for pathological results were defined (please see Appendix for

the listing of constitutive subtests): attention (pathological �2 sub-

tests below z = �1.5), executive functions (pathological: �2 subtests

below z = �1.5), short-term/working memory (pathological: �2

subtests below z = �1.5), verbal learning/verbal memory (patholo-

gical: �3 subtests below z = �1.5), non-verbal learning/non-verbal

memory (pathological: �2 subtests below Z = �1.5) and visuo-

constructive functions (pathological: � 1 subtest below Z = �1.5).

Psychosomatic/psychiatric evaluationBeck’s depression scale (Beck et al., 1961) and SCL-90R question-

naire (Olsen et al., 2004) were used to control for possible influences

exerted by mood or personality alterations on cognitive perform-

ance, general social, psychical and physical functions. These self-

assessment questionnaires were sent to the participant after the

first telephone contact, i.e. before clinical presentation, to allow

him/her to fill in the answers in a familiar environment.

Neuroradiological examinationImage acquisitionFor each patient group, a T1 volume of three-dimensional (3D)

gradient echo acquisitions with a slice thickness of 1.3 mm in

sagittal volume excitation was created (TR = 24.03 ms, TE = 6 ms,

flip angle = 30�, matrix 256 · 256) to provide the basis for exact

B C

Fig. 1 (A) Study inclusion/exclusion diagram. (B) Exclusion diagram for the BM group. (C) Exclusion diagram for the VM group.

336 Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 H. Schmidt et al.

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

structural differentiation. In addition, an image set of 20 transverse

images in fluid attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) technique (TR

6000 ms, TE 100 ms) was chosen to evaluate the frequency and

severity of white matter changes. The field of view for all MR studies

was adjusted parallel to the orbito-meatal line. All measurements

were performed on a 1.5 T Gyroscan Philips MRI scanner.

Image processingSagittal T1 scans were applied for the manual slice-by-slice outlining

of cerebrum and basal ganglia. The acquired areas were multiplied by

the slice thickness and the number of the slices. The volume of the

ventricles was measured using the same procedure. The investigator

performing the planimetry was a trained physician in clinical neuro-

logy (C.M.), blinded for the respective patients’ diagnosis. The soft-

ware Osiris, Version 4 was used (Ligier et al., 1994). The landmark

dividing the cerebrum and basal ganglia from the upper brainstem

was defined as a line from the superior border of the superior col-

liculus to the lower margin of the root of the mamillary bodies drawn

in the mid-sagittal plane (Edwards et al., 1999). ‘Ventricular volume’

(VV) was defined as the sum of both lateral ventricles and third

ventricle without the fourth ventricle. Relative VV was calculated

as the ratio of VV and cerebral volume. FLAIR sequences were used

for the neuroradiological rating of atrophy, white matter and cortical

lesions to categorize these into ‘normal’, ‘pathological probably due

to meningitis’ or ‘pathological probably due to other causes’. The

neuroradiological evaluation was carried out by a neuroradiologist

who also was blinded for the diagnosis (C.F.).

StatisticsData were described asmean6 standard deviations (SD). Statements

on pathological alterations were made cautiously. They were not only

based on significant group differences but required Z-values of less

than�1.5 (Spreen and Strauss, 1998; Arnaiz et al., 2004). The norm-

ative data for this cut-off value were derived from the control group

by Z-transformation of raw data. Whenever only VM and BM groups

were compared [e.g. GCS at admission, brain volume (BV), etc.] we

used t-tests for independent samples for normally distributed data,

and U-tests when Gaussian distribution was not present. When both

patients and control groups were compared, we used ANOVA or

ANOVA on ranks (Kruskal-Wallis H-test) followed by Bonferroni’s

or Dunn’s post hoc test for multiple testing, respectively. Frequencies

were compared with either x2- or Fisher’s exact-test in the case of

small group size.Multivariate analysis was performedwithMANOVA

testing. We used Pearson’s correlation coefficient for normally dis-

tributed parameters, and Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient for

data not being normally distributed. A logistic step-wise regression

model was used for the identification of both clinical and neuropsy-

chological outcome parameters. The calculations were performed

with SPSS 11.0 software (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

ResultsSociodemographic dataAccording to the study inclusion diagram (Fig. 1A), 250

patients with BM and 246 patients with VM were potential

candidates for this study. In the BM group, significantly more

patients had died or were not trackable than in the VM group

(n = 39BM versus n = 6VM; x2-test P < 0.01). As expected, in the

overall BM group, significantly more patients suffered from

alcoholism or intravenous drug abuse than in the VM group

(n = 11 versus n = 2; x2-test P < 0.01), and were therefore

excluded. The exclusion tree for the BM and VM group is

given in Fig. 1B and C. We recruited 59 individuals for the

BM group, 59 VM and 30 control subjects. Neither gender

distribution (m/f: 34/25BM, 34/25VM, m/f: 17/13CONTROL;

x2-test P = 0.99) nor age distribution at the time of this

investigation (44.6 6 14.9BM, 40.4 6 11.6VM, 46.3 6 13.1

CONTROL years; H-test P = 0.12) was different. The age of

VM and BM group members at hospital admission (38.6

6 15.4BM, 34.4 6 11.7VM years, t-test P = 0.14) and the

interval between meningitis and assessment (6.0 6 3.5BM,

6.3 6 5.1VM, t-test P = 0.93) were comparable between the

groups. The handedness did not differ between the groups

(x2-test P = 0.44). The duration of school education was

similar among the control, the VM and BM groups

(H-test P = 0.52). However, more patients after BM (13

out of 59) than after VM (4 out of 59) were either unemployed

or retired (Fisher’s exact test P = 0.03). In our control group, 4

out of 30 persons were retired but none because of medical

conditions.

Microbiological featuresTable 1 shows the distribution of causative bacteria in the

screened population and in the study sample.

Neurological examinationOn hospital admission, BM patients exhibited signific-

antly lower GCS scores than VM patients (13.1 6 2.7BM;

14.9VM 6 0.4 ; U-test P < 0.01). Only 1 VM patient had a

GCS of �12, none of the VM patients displayed a GCS < 9,

whereas 13 BM patients showed a GCS of �12 and 6 BM

patients showed a GCS of <9. Neurological deficits (other

than headache and neck stiffness on admission) that could be

safely ascribed to meningitis were observed in 48 BM patients.

At the time of this study, the frequency of neurological deficits

differed significantly between patients after bacterial and VM

(x2-test P < 0.01). Only in 11 BM versus 40 VM patients was

the neurological examination without abnormalities. The

SNRS was lower for BM patients than for VM patients

(91.0 6 12.4BM versus 96.1 6 13.6VM ; t-test P = 0.04).

The GOS on re-examination was significantly higher for

VM than for BM patients (x2-test P < 0.01). No one in the

VM group had a diminished GOS, while 11 patients in the

BM group had a GOS of <5.

Complications of BMCerebral complications of BM have been described in detail

(Pfister et al., 1993). In the BM patients studied here, signi-

ficant brain swelling was observed in 14 out of 59 (24%),

ischaemic cerebral lesions in 5 out of 59 (10%), signs for

small vessel vasculitis in 8.5%, impairment of CSF circulation

(mostly hydrocephalus aresorptivus), in 4 out of 59 (7%),

sinus thrombosis in 2 out of 59 (3%), cerebritis/abscess

Neuropsychological sequelae and MRI changes after meningitis Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 337

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

formation in 2 out of 59 (3%) and intracranial haemorrhage

in 1 out of 59 patients (1.7%). No such complications

occurred in the VM patients.

Neuroradiological evaluationPlanimetryBVs were significantly smaller in the BM than the VM group

(11146 113 cm3BM versus 11586 88 cm3

VM ; U-test P = 0.03).

Conversely, the VVs were larger after BM than after VM (226

26 cm3BM versus 11 6 9 cm3

VM; U-test P = 0.03).

Neuroradiological evaluationFor the blinded neuroradiologist, it was difficult to distinguish

white matter lesions after meningitis from those induced by

small-vessel atherosclerosis. In 12 out of 58 patients after BM,

lesions in the FLAIR scans were diagnosed as being probably

induced by meningitis. In another 25 patients, the radiologist

found alterations but was unable to classify them as specific

for BM. Nevertheless, in his final diagnosis, none of the VM

patients was classified as having ‘MRI lesions probably caused

by meningitis’ (x2-test P < 0.01). Except for the number of

white matter lesions and the extent of brain atrophy that were

negatively correlated to the performance in short-term mem-

ory (both P = 0.03), none of the other subtests was associated

to MRI alterations.

Psychopathological evaluationAll psychopathological data were derived from self-

assessment questionnaires which the patients had completed

before presenting themselves for re-examination.

DepressionThe Beck’s depression index did not differ between the three

groups, nor between BM and VM patients (4.96 5.6BM, 5.26

5.9VM, and 2.5 6 2.5CONTROL; H-test P = 0.26). According to

the given cut-off values for the Beck’s depression index (�11:

‘not depressive’), most of the patients considered themselves

to be ‘not depressive’. Differences in prevalence of depression

between the groups could be ruled out as a cause for differ-

ences in the neuropsychological test performance.

General physical, psychical functioning andpsychic well beingWe analysed results from the Nottingham ADL, and the scales

GSI and PST from the SCL-90-R questionnaire. Patients after

bacterial but not after VM were more likely to experience

difficulties in their daily global functioning compared to

healthy control persons. ANOVA and post hoc analysis

again displayed lower scores for BM patients as compared

both with the VM and the control group. The VM patients

did not differ from the control group in this respect

[83.3 6 9.4BM, 86.8 6 2.9VM (U-test P = 0.02), versus

87.1 6 2.5CONTROL; ANOVA P < 0.01]. GSI [Z-values in

mean �1.4 6 1.9BM, �1.9 6 3.0VM (U-test P < 0.01), versus

0.06 1.0CONTROL; ANOVA P < 0.01] and PST scores [�0.86

1.3BM, �1.1 6 1.7VM; (U-test P < 0.01)] were significantly

lower in both patient groups than in the control group (0.06

1.0CONTROL; ANOVA P < 0.01).

Cognitive evaluationFrequency of cognitive dysfunctionsThe relative frequencies of patients with impaired perform-

ances for each domain in each group are listed in Table 2.

Except for alertness functions, in all domains the patient

groups displayed significantly higher proportions of patho-

logical results than the healthy control group. In spite of

consistent differences in favour of the VM group, only the

domain of short-term and working memory was significantly

more often impaired in BM than in VM patients (Fisher’s

exact test P < 0.01) (Table 2).

Global cognitive sum scoreThe ‘global cognitive sum score’ was derived from the number

of subtests of pathological domains. Whenever the majority of

the domains were pathological the global cognitive score was

considered ‘pathological’. Calculating and comparing this

score, both patient groups showed significantly lower global

cognitive sum scores than the controls (ANOVA P � 0.01),

while the differences of the frequencies of pathological global

cognitive sum scores in the BM and the VM groups did not

reach statistical significance (x2-test P = 0.75).

Multivariate analysisAs an important pre-requisite for the comparison of both

patient groups, attention functions for VM, BM and control

groups were comparable (MANOVA P = 0.13). In all other

domains meningitis groups showed lower mean Z-values

than the control group. However, only in two neuropsycho-

logical tests in the BM group, the mean Z-values were below

�1.5 (Rey-figure copy and HAWIE-R similarities, Fig. 2).

Table 1 Distribution of causative bacteria in adultpatients with bacterial meningitis

Bacterium All BMpatients n (%)

Studypopulation n (%)

Streptococcus pneumoniae 64 (26) 16 (27)Neisseria meningitidis 27 (11) 16 (27)Staphylococcus aureus 18 (7) 1 (2)Streptococci 13 (5) 5 (8)Listeria monocytogenes 8 (3) 3 (5)Haemophilus influenzae 3 (1) 0 (0)Other 4 (2) 0 (0)Mycobacterium tuberculosis* 29 (12) 0 (0)†

Borrelia spp. 15 (6) 0 (0)†

Not identified 69 (29) 18 (31)Total 250 59

*Culture proven and clinically probable cases of tuberculousmeningitis. †Exclusion criterion.

338 Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 H. Schmidt et al.

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Analysing thedifferencesbetween thepatientgroups, the scores

for executive functions were significantly lower in the BM than

in the VM group (MANOVA P < 0.01). Mean Z-values for

visuo-constructive abilities were significantly lower in BM

patients (MANOVA P < 0.01). Short-term and working mem-

orywasmore impaired in BM than inVMpatients (MANOVA

P < 0.01). Concerning the language domain, BM but not VM

patients showed significantly lower Z-values in the AAT token

test levels 4 and 5 than healthy control subjects (�1.06 2.3BMversus �0.2 6 1.4VM; U-test P = 0.02 and 0.47, respectively;

MANOVA P < 0.01). Since the AAT token test (AAT 4 more

thanAAT 5) is dependent on short-term andworkingmemory

functions, the lower scores for language functions could be

an effect of the observed differences between VM and BM in

short-term/working-memory functions. The domains of

verbal learning/memory (MANOVA P < 0.01) but not of

non-verbal learning/memory (MANOVA P = 0.15) were sig-

nificantly different. When analysing the non-verbal recall

functions (Rey figure delayed recall (del. recall), city map

test and WMS-R visual pair association, part II) separately,

patients after BM performed significantly worse than after

VM (MANOVA P < 0.01) (Fig. 2). In addition to the frequen-

cies of impaired test results in the respective groups, Tables 2

and 3 provide a summary of MANOVA and post hoc testing in

the various domains.

Predictive factorsNeuropsychological outcomeWhen applying a logistic regression model, only the age at

re-examination was predictive for the neuropsychological

result. When controlling for age, the neurological status at

the time of re-examination expressed as SNRS was predictive

for an impaired neuropsychological outcome in patients after

BM. Taken separately in a univariate comparison, the only

clinical parameter that differed significantly for BM patients

with pathological versus those with a normal neuropsy-

chological outcome was the VV heralding brain atrophy

(U-test P = 0.04).

Social impactFour of 59 BM patients but none of the VM patients had to

retire as a consequence of the disease (Fisher’s exact test

P = 0.05). The retired BM patients were significantly older

than BM patients who were able to continue their work

(53.1 6 4.2 versus 37.6 6 15.4 years; U-test P = 0.04).

Three of 4 retired patients had an impaired GOS and

their SNRS was significantly lower than the SNRS of

non-retired BM patients (81.5 6 9.0 versus 91.7 6 12.4,

U-test P = 0.02).

Subgroup analysis for BMIn patients with a culture- or microscopically proven bacterial

aetiology of meningitis, the most frequent causative patho-

gens were S. pneumoniae and Neisseria meningitidis, both

n = 16. When comparing these two groups, we found char-

acteristic differences such as mean younger age at admission

(N. meningitidis versus S. pneumoniae 21.8 6 10.9 versus

42.36 11.2 years; t-test P < 0.01) and higher GOS at discharge

for N. meningitidis patients (Fisher’s exact test P = 0.02). All

patients afterN.meningitidismeningitis had a normal GOS on

discharge, whereas 10 out of 16 patients after S. pneumoniae

meningitis had a GOS of 4 (Fisher’s exact test P = 0.02). For

the following analysis of psychopathological and cognitive

function we took into account the significant difference in

age (this analysis used Z-values from the normative data).

Sociodemographic determinants such as educational system

or duration of education at the time of our study did not

differ. In the multivariate analysis of psychometric data,

neither the domain for physical and social functioning nor

the domain of psychic well being revealed differences between

patients after meningococcal and pneumococcal meningitis.

The comparison between meningococcal and pneumococcal

meningitis did not exhibit differences for the number of

cognitive domains (Fisher’s exact test P = 0.43), except for

the domain of verbal memory, in which patients after

N. meningitidis meningitis achieved lower Z-values

than patients after pneumococcal meningitis (MANOVA

P = 0.02). Comparing VVs, pneumococcal meningitis patients

had significantly higher volumes than patients after menin-

gococcal meningitis. Since both groups differed in ages, and

higher age was accompanied by physiological brain atrophy,

these differences could at least be in part the result of group-

based bias. MRI examinations of patients after pneumococcal

meningitis were more frequently classified as ‘pathological’

(12 versus 6) and ‘pathological probably due to meningitis’

(6 versus 0) than those of patients after meningococcal

meningitis (Fisher’s exact test P < 0.01).

Table 2 Frequencies of impaired domain functions for BM and VM patients

Domain BM (%)versus control VM (%)versus control Control group (%) P-valueBM versus VM

Attention 39.0n.s. 42.6n.s. 20.0 n.s.Executive functions 63.6* 48.3* 25.0 n.s.Short-term/working memory 58.6* 39.5n.s. 15.4 <0.01Verbal learning/memory 31.0n.s. 25.0n.s. 10.0 n.s.Non verbal learning/memory 21.1n.s. 13.3n.s. 6.7 n.s.Visuo-constructive functions 74.6* 59.0* 26.7 n.s.Global cognitive sum score 37.2* 15.2* 3.0 n.s.

All comparisons made with two sided Fisher’s exact test, Bonferroni corrected P-values; * denotes P < 0.05.

Neuropsychological sequelae and MRI changes after meningitis Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 339

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

DiscussionBM can cause persistent neurological (Durand et al., 1993)

and cognitive impairment, i.e. attention deficits, mnestic

problems, learning disabilities in children (D’Angio et al.,

1995; Hugosson et al., 1997; Grimwood et al., 2000) and

adults (Zahner et al., 1995; Merkelbach et al., 2000; van de

Beek et al., 2002). In comparison to healthy control persons,

Merkelbach et al. found a high proportion of persisting

Fig. 2 Overview of substantially different neuropsychological and MRI results for patients after BM and VM. Mean Z-values are given.Filled bars denominate results of patients after BM, open bars those after VM. Statistically significant differences are marked with*;please refer to Appendix for the explanations of the respective subdomains, and to Table 3 for exact means, standard deviations andrespective P-values.

340 Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 H. Schmidt et al.

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

cognitive deficits (over 70%) resembling subcortical

cognitive impairment which was not correlated to the

respective neurological outcome (Merkelbach et al., 2000).

Van de Beek et al. also reported cognitive slowing in patients

after BM (van de Beek et al., 2002), especially in patients after

S. pneumoniae meningitis, whereas patients after meningo-

coccal meningitis displayed almost normal test results in this

cohort.

Apart from attention functions, our reappraisal of cognitive

dysfunctions after long-term survival of bacterial and VM

revealed poorer cognitive functioning in both patient groups

in many subtests when compared to an age-adapted control

group of healthy adults. These impairments of cognitive func-

tions had substantial social consequences only for a minority

of BM patients. The Z-values given in the literature have been

obtained by testing subjects through only a single test, whereas

our array of tests usually took >3 h. For this study, as a

common ground for normative values, the results were inter-

preted in relation to Z-values determined by a group of

healthy adults who had undergone the same time-

consuming test procedure. In this way, we ruled out that

the differences between the normative data and the patient

groups were partly just a result of fatigue. Nevertheless, for the

evaluation of long-term sequelae in BM patients, it is probably

more important to look at the comparison with the age-

matched VM group having a comparable clinical history.

Serious life events such as meningitis can have a critical

long-term impact on psychic and mood stability (Twamley

et al., 2004; Bremner et al., 2004). Serious life events can induce

depression with its negative effects on cognition (Penick et al.,

1994). These direct and indirect cognitive disturbances are

difficult to distinguish from primary impairment caused by

the disease itself. Because the frequency of cognitive dysfunc-

tions (and MRI alterations) increases with age, we only

allowed patients up to 70 years to participate in this study.

Probably most important, we carefully excluded patients with

alcoholism (or other substance abuse), a frequent cause of

cognitive derangement (Goldstein et al., 2004). Alcohol abuse

impairs frontal executive functions (Dao-Castellana et al.,

1998) and affects memory functions (Parsons, 1994).

Alcoholism—with or without related medical diseases—is

one of the predisposing factors for streptococcal, including

pneumococcal diseases (Siboni et al., 1989; Huang et al.,

2002). Failure to exclude these patients can produce a bias

with respect to cognitive functions in patients after pneumo-

coccal meningitis and may be responsible for the high rate of

severe neuropsychological abnormalities in one previous

study (Merkelbach et al., 2000).

Although we controlled for the variable alcoholism by

studying the patients’ files for typical laboratory signs of alco-

holism, and by thoroughly interviewing the patients them-

selves and/or their relatives, our analysis may still have

underestimated the true rate of alcoholism, since the rate

of excluded patients in our VM group was a little lower

(2%) than the 3% reported for alcoholism in the German

population (Singer and Teyssen, 2001).

Our patient and control groups were well balanced with

respect to age, gender and education. The results showed all

groups to be homogeneous concerning their personality traits

and depression scores. Long-term psychological and cognitive

disturbances after VM without encephalitis have been repor-

ted (Hotopf et al., 1996; Lepow et al., 1962; Muller et al.,

1958). These disturbances have been described as subtle. In

this study, the neuropsychological outcome for VM patients

was significantly poorer than for the healthy control group.

Yet, most of the Z-values of the VM group were in mean

greater than �1.5. These Z-values greater than or equal to

�1.5 are in the lower normal range and might in part be a

result of having experienced a serious life disruption by VM.

This hypothesis is supported by the fact that the scores for

psychic well being for both VM and BM were significantly

lower than for the healthy control group.

We did not find a significant difference between BM and

VM patients with regard to a general cognitive sum score,

or when referring to the healthy control—or to the controls

from the literature as the reference. Not until separate cog-

nitive domains had been analysed in detail, was it possible to

determine valid differences between the BM and VM group.

BM patients more often exhibited pathological deficits in

short-term and working memory. The direct comparison

showed distinctively poorer results for executive tasks in

the BM patients, particularly when learning strategies and

control of mnestic interference were required. Patients after

BM made more mistakes in the AAT token test (domain

language) than those after VM and performed significantly

poorer in the domain of visuo-constructive functions. The

difference between the test performance of BM and VM

patients in the domains of short-term and working memory

was significant, irrespective of the mode of analysis [frequen-

cies of pathological test results (x2); mean Z-values 6 SD

(MANOVA)].

While the domains of verbal learning and non-verbal

learning/memory did not differ in total, especially in subtests

which required plan formation and learning strategies, we

saw significantly poorer performance of the BM group.

The differences in the visuo-constructive performance and

language domain can be specific but could also be in

part caused by executive deficits due to poorer concept

formation.

We ruled out with certainty deficits of attention as a cause

for these differences since the respective test results equalled

each other. In spite of these differences, only a small subgroup

of those BM patients surviving BM with a GOS �4

(thus qualifying them for our neuropsychological testing)

experienced socially significant impairment leading to retire-

ment or disability (4 out of 59 patients).

Except for one domain, in our culture proven cases, MAN-

OVA analysis did not show relevant differences between

patients after S. pneumoniae and N. meningitidis meningitis.

One explanation for this discrepancy between the findings

reported previously (van de Beek et al., 2002) and ours

probably was the fact that we rigorously excluded

Neuropsychological sequelae and MRI changes after meningitis Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 341

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

patients after S. pneumoniae meningitis with concomitant

conditions—such as alcoholism—having a negative impact

on cognitive functions.

For the interpretation of this study, the findings of van de

Beek et al. are very important, since these authors provided

age-corrected neuropsychological data presented as t-values.

Considering our definition of an impaired test performance,

the results of both studies are very similar. In van de Beek’s

neuropsychological investigation of BM patients there was no

item with a mean t-value of less than 40, i.e. none of the given

normalized domains displayed mean Z-value lower than �1.

Although there is no common cut-off Z-value for the defini-

tion of a pathological test result, Z-values below �1.5 are

considered as impaired test performance (Spreen and

Strauss, 1998; Arnaiz et al., 2004). For their cognitive sum

score, van de Beek et al. considered results below the fifth

Table 3 Pairwise comparisons of subtests (please refer to Appendix 1 for abbreviations)

Domain BM Z-values(mean 6 SD)

VM Z-values(mean 6 SD)

Control group raw values(Mean 6 SD raw values [Z-values*])

Comparison(P-value)

Executive functionsMANOVA <0.01Lexical verbal fluency �0.8 6 1.0† �0.7 6 0.9† 17.4 6 4.5 [0.6 6 0.81] n.s.Lexical verbal fluency; alternations �0.6 6 0.8† �0.5 6 0.8† 14.5 6 3.6 [0.3 6 1.0] n.sSemantic verbal fluency �0.9 6 1.0† �0.6 6 1.1† 25.4 6 5.1 [0.6 6 0.8] n.s.Semantic verbal fluency; alternations �0.6 6 0.9† 0.0 6 1.0† 14.9 6 3.3 [0.3 6 1.0] <0.01Figural fluency �1.1 6 1.0† �0.8 6 1.1 36.9 6 9.2 [0.6 6 1.1] n.s.Verbal concept formation �1.7 6 1.8† �1.2 6 1.9† 29.4 6 2.3 [1.3 6 1.1] n.s.WCST concepts �0.4 6 1.2 �0.2 6 1.1 4.8 6 1.7 [n.a.] n.s.WCST errors �0.2 6 1.0 �0.3 6 1.1 8.2 6 6.0 [n.a.] n.s.WCST perseverations �1.2 6 3.4 �0.4 6 1.1 2.4 6 2.7 [n.a.] n.s.§

CVLT semantic clustering �0.9 6 0.7 �0.4 6 1.0† 2.4 6 0.9 [0.03 6 1.1] 0.01Visuo-constructive functions

MANOVA <0.01HAWIE-R mosaic test �1.0 6 1.5† �0.6 6 1.4 35.3 6 7.3 [0.9 6 0.8] n.s.Rey figure copy �2.6 6 4.1† �1.1 6 2.7† 35.5 6 0.9 [0.9 6 0.4] 0.02§

LanguageAAT token test �1.0 6 2.3† �0.2 6 1.4 0.1 6 0.5 [n.a.] 0.01§

Short-term and working memoryMANOVA <0.01Digit span forward �0,7 6 1.2 �0.5 6 1.0† 7.0 6 2.4 [0.7 6 1.0] n.s.Digit span backward �0.6 6 0.8† �0.3 6 0.8 8.9 6 1.8 [0.1 6 1.4] 0.05Block span forward �0.9 6 1.0† �0.6 6 1.0† 8.6 6 1.9 [0.2 6 1.3] n.s.Block span backward �0.6 6 0.9† �0.2 6 0.9† 7.3 6 2.0 [�0.4 6 1.3] 0.03CVLT 1st trial �0.9 6 0.8† �0.6 6 0.8† 8.2 6 2.4 [�0.1 6 1.2] 0.03WMS logical memory part I �1.0 6 1.2† �0.8 6 1.1† 33.1 6 5.8 [1.0 6 0.9] n.s.

Verbal learning and memoryMANOVA <0.01CVLT trial 1–5 �1.2 6 1.1 �0.8 6 1.3 59.6 6 10.6 [0.4 6 1.3] n.sCVLT slope �0.1 6 1.1 �0.1 6 1.2 1.4 6 0.5 [0.3 6 1.0] n.s.CVLT free recall �1.1 6 1.1† �0.5 6 0.8† 12.4 6 2.8 [0.4 6 1.0] 0.01CVLT cued recall �1.0 6 1.1† �0.5 61.0† 13.2 6 2.6 [0.3 6 1.1] 0.02CVLT delayed free recall �0.9 6 1.2† �0.5 6 1.0† 13.0 6 2.6 [0.4 6 1.1] 0.03CVLT delayed cued recall �1.1 6 1.3† �1.1 6 1.3† 13.3 6 2.3 [0.2 6 1.1] 0.02CVLT recognition hits �0.9 6 1.4† �1.0 6 2.1† 15.4 6 1.0 [0.3 6 0.8] n.s.§

CVLT recognition errors �0.6 6 1.5 �0.2 6 1.7 0.6 6 1.1 [0.03 6 0.4] n.s.§

VLT difference right positive/false positive �0.4 6 1.0 �0.2 6 0.8 26.6 6 8.5 [0.0 6 1.1] n.s.WMS verbal association part I 0.2 6 2.1 �0.0 6 0.4 22.1 6 2.8 [0.5 6 0.8] n.s.§

WMS verbal association part II �1.4 6 2.1 �0.6 6 1.5† 7.7 6 0.5 [0.7 6 0.3] 0.02§

WMS logical memory part II �1.2 6 1.2† �0.9 6 1.2† 30.6 6 6.1 [1.1 6 0.8] n.s.Non-verbal learning and memory

MANOVA 0.15Rey figure delayed recall �1.1 6 1.3† �0.7 6 1.1† 63.4 6 16.8 [0.8 6 0.8] n.s.LGT city map �0.7 6 0.8† �0.6 6 0.8† 18.7 6 5.1 [0.1 6 1.0] n.s.NVLT difference right positive/false positive �0.4 6 1.0 �0.3 6 1.0 20.1 6 7.4 [�0.4 6 1.1] n.s.WMS visual association part I �0.1 6 1.1 0.2 6 1.0 17.0 6 3.1 [0.4 6 1.0] n.s.WMS visual association part II �1.0 6 2.0† �0.3 6 1.3 5.8 6 0.5 [0.7 6 0.3] <0.01§

Comparisons were performed with two-sided t-tests unless otherwise denoted. *Comparison of the control group versus normal valuesderived from the tests’ handbooks or from the literature. †Significantly different as compared with the healthy control group.n.a.: standardised Z-values not available in the literature. §Comparison with U-test.

342 Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 H. Schmidt et al.

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

percentile as pathological. Applying this cut-off value, 16% of

their study patients displayed a pathological cognitive sum

score. With the same definition of the cut-off value, only 10%

of our BM group would have been characterized as abnormal.

In contrast to a previous report (Merkelbach et al., 2000),

the neurological outcome at re-examination in our study

group was correlated to impaired overall cognitive abilities,

as well as to lower executive, non-verbal learning and non-

verbal memory functions.

As an expression of atrophy, cerebral volumes in BM

patients were smaller, VVs were higher and MRI scans

were more often classified as abnormal due to atrophy and/or

white matter lesions than in VM patients. For the entire

patient group, only dysfunction of short-term and working

memory but not a reduced global cognitive sum score or any

other domain of learning and memory was correlated with

lower cerebral volumes. At first glance, the lack of a tighter

correlation of BV with overall cognitive functions seems sur-

prising but corresponds well to the finding that the BV does

not clearly correlate with memory functions in healthy per-

sons (Torres et al., 1997).

In patients after VM without encephalitic signs, the

achieved Z-values were mostly lower than zero, i.e. poorer

than the results of our healthy control subjects (and poorer

than the control groups given in the literature and in the test

manuals). Whether this confirms that VMmay induce lasting,

cognitive limitations (Hotopf et al., 1996; Iushchuk et al.,

2001) or whether this might be a mere effect of the serious

life event these patients have experienced (resulting in

decreased psychic well-being) remains to be elucidated by

further studies.

In conclusion, the extent of neuropsychological impair-

ment after VM is measurable but probably has only limited

social impact for these patients. The damage of cognitive

functions by BM is more severe but might have been

overestimated in some previous studies due to the inclusion

of patients with causes of cognitive deficits other than

meningitis. Many adult long-term survivors of BM who

were able to present themselves for clinical re-evaluation,

showed unimpaired cognitive performance. Short-term/

working memory and executive functions are the domains

which are affected most frequently and most severely

after BM. The executive deficits have a secondary impact

on other neuropsychological functions (memory recall,

visuo-constructive functions and language). These neuropsy-

chological impairments are correlated to poor clinical out-

come. Examinations of short-term/working memory and of

executive functions can represent important end-points for

future outcome studies evaluating new treatment options for

BM in humans.

AcknowledgementThis study was supported by a grant of the Deutsche

Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG Na 165/5-1).

References

Aladro Benito Y, Perez Correa S, Elcuaz Romano R, Angel-MorenoMaroto A,

Granados Monzon R. [Listeriosis in the nonpregnant adult during an

epidemic outbreak on Grand Canary]. Rev Clin Esp 1995; 195: 154–9.

Arnaiz E, Almkvist O, Ivnik R, Tangalos L, Wahlund L, Winblad B, et al. Mild

cognitive impairment: a cross-national comparison. J Neurol Neurosurg

Psychiatry 2004; 75: 1275–80.

Aschenbrenner S, TuchaO, Lange K. RegensburgerWortflussigkeitstest. Lisse:

Swets und Zeitlinger Verlag; 2000.

Baumler G. Lern- und Gedachtnistest LGT-3. Gottingen: Hogrefe Verlag fur

Psychologie; 1974.

Beck A, Ward C, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for meas-

uring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1961; 4: 561–71.

Becker M, Sturm W, Willmes K, Zimmermann P. Normierungsstudie zur

Aufmerksamkeitstestbatterie (TAP) von Zimmermann und Fimm. Zeitschr

Neuropsychol 1996; 1: 3–15.

Berg B, Gardsell P, Skansberg P. Cerebrospinal fluid lactate in the diagnosis of

meningitis. Diagnostic value compared to standard biochemical methods.

Scand J Infect Dis 1982; 14: 111–5.

Bohr V, Hansen B, Kjersem H, Rasmussen N, Johnsen N, Kristensen H, et al.

Sequelae from bacterial meningitis and their relation to the clinical con-

dition during acute illness, based on 667 questionnaire returns. Part II of a

three part series. J Infect 1983; 7: 102–10.

Bohr V, Paulson O, Rasmussen N. Pneumococcal meningitis. Late

neurologic sequelae and features of prognostic impact. Arch Neurol 1984;

41: 1045–9.

Bohr V, Rasmussen N, Hansen B, Gade A, Kjersem H, Johnson N, et al.

Pneumococcal meningitis: an evaluation of prognostic factors in 164

cases based on mortality and on a study of lasting sequelae. J Infect

1985; 10: 143–57.

Bremner J, Vermetten E, Afzal N, Vythilingam M. Deficits in verbal

declarative memory function in women with childhood sexual abuse-

related posttraumatic stress disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 2004; 192: 643–9.

D’Angio C, Froehlke R, Plank G, Meehan D, Aguilar C, Lande M, et al. Long-

term outcome of Haemophilus influenzae meningitis in Navajo Indian

children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1995; 149: 1001–8.

Dao-CastellanaM, Samson Y, Legault F, Martinot J, AubinH, Crouzel C, et al.

Frontal dysfunction in neurologically normal chronic alcoholic subjects:

metabolic and neuropsychological findings. Psychol Med 1998; 28:

1039–48.

Davidson H, Steiner R. Magnetic resonance imaging in infections of the

central nervous system. Am J Neuroradiol 1985; 6: 499–504.

Delis D, Freeland J, Kramer J, Kaplan E. Integrating clinical assessment with

cognitive neuroscience: construct validation of the California verbal

learning test. J Consult Clin Psychol 1988; 56: 123–30.

DurandM,Calderwood S,WeberD,Miller S, Southwick F, Caviness V Jr, et al.

Acute bacterial meningitis in adults. A review of 493 episodes. N Engl J Med

1993; 328: 21–8.

Edwards S, Gong Q, Liu C, Zvartau M, Jaspan T, Roberts N, et al. Infra-

tentorial atrophy on magnetic resonance imaging and disability in multiple

sclerosis. Brain 1999; 122: 291–301.

Free S, Li L, Fish D, Shorvon S, Stevens J. Bilateral hippocampal volume loss in

patients with a history of encephalitis or meningitis. Epilepsia 1996; 37:

400–5.

Goldstein R, Leskovjan A, Hoff A, Hitzemann R, Bashan F, Khalsa S, et al.

Severity of neuropsychological impairment in cocaine and alcohol addic-

tion: association with metabolism in the prefrontal cortex. Neuropsycho-

logia 2004; 42: 1447–58.

Grimwood K, Anderson P, Anderson V, Tan L, Nolan T. Twelve year out-

comes following bacterial meningitis: further evidence for persisting effects.

Arch Dis Child 2000; 83: 111–6.

Harting C, Markowitsch H, Neufeld H, Calabrese P, Deisinger K, Kessler J.

Wechsler Gedachtnis Test - Revidierte Fassung (WMS-R). Bern: Huber;

2000.

Hotopf M, Noah N, Wessely S. Chronic fatigue and minor psychiatric mor-

bidity after viral meningitis: a controlled study. J Neurol Neurosurg

Psychiatry 1996; 60: 504–9.

Neuropsychological sequelae and MRI changes after meningitis Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 343

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Huang C, Lu C, Chang H, Lee P, Lin M, Chang W. Community-acquired

spontaneous bacterial meningitis in adult diabetic patients: an analysis of

clinical characteristics and prognostic factors. Infection 2002; 30: 346–50.

Huber W, Poeck K, Weniger D, Willmes K. Der Aachener Aphasietest.

Gottingen: Hogrefe; 1983.

Hugosson S, Carlsson E, Borg E, Brorson L, Langeroth G, Olcen P. Audi-

ovestibular and neuropsychological outcome of adults who had recovered

from childhood bacterial meningitis. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 1997;

42: 149–67.

Ilanson L, Axelsson G, Linne T, Aurelius E, Lindquist L. Serum C-reactive

protein in the differential diagnosis of acute meningitis. Scand J Infect Dis

1993; 25: 625–30.

Iushchuk ND, Krasnov VN, and Dovzhenko TV, Tarasova NIu. [Diagnosis

and approaches to therapy of emotional disorders in patients with

infectious diseases]. Ter Arkh 2001; 73: 52–5.

Jennett B, Bond M. Assessment of outcome after severe brain damage - a

practical scale. Lancet 1975; 1: 480–4.

Karandanis D, Shulman J. Recent survey of infectious meningitis in adults:

review of laboratory findings in bacterial, tuberculous, and aseptic men-

ingitis. South Med J 1976; 69: 449–57.

Leib S, Boscacci R, Gratzl O, Zimmerli W. Predictive value of cerebrospinal

fluid (CSF) lactate level versus CSF/blood glucose ratio for the diagnosis

of bacterial meningitis following neurosurgery. Clin Infect Dis 1999;

29: 69–74.

Lepow M, Coyne N, Thompson L. A clinical epidemiologic and laboratory

investigation of aseptic meningitis during the four year period 1955-

1958: II the clinical disease and its sequelae. N Engl J Med 1962; 266:

1188–93.

Ligier Y, Ratib O, Logeon M, Girard C. Osiris: a medical image-manipulation

system. MD Comput 1994; 11: 212–8.

Malt U, Blikra G, Holvig B. The late effect of accidental injury questionnaire

(LEAIQ). Acta Psychiatr Scand 1989; 80: 113–30.

Merkelbach S, Sittinger H, Schweizer I, Muller M. Cognitive outcome after

bacterial meningitis. Acta Neurol Scand 2000; 102: 118–23.

Muller R, Nylander I, Larsson L, Widen L, Frankenhauser M. Sequelae

of primary aseptic meningoencephalitis: a clinical, sociomedical,

electroencephalographic and psychological study. Acta Psychiatr

Neurol Scand 1958; 33 (Suppl. 126): 1–115.

Nau R, Soto A, BruckW. Apoptosis of neurons in the dentate gyrus in humans

suffering from bacterial meningitis. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1999; 58:

265–74.

Nau R, Gerber J, Bunkowski S, Bruck W. Axonal injury, a neglected

cause of CNS damage in bacterial meningitis. Neurology 2004; 62:

509–11.

NelsonH. Amodified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex

1976; 12: 313–24.

Nouri F, Lincoln N. An extended activities of daily living scale for stroke

patients. Clinical Rehabilitation 1987; 1: 301–5.

Olsen L, Mortensen E, Bech P. The SCL-90 and SCL-90R versions validated by

item responsemodels in a Danish community sample. Acta Psychiatr Scand

2004; 110: 225–9.

Osterrieth P. Le test de copie d’une figure complexe. Arch Psychol 1944; 30:

206–353.

Parsons O. Neuropsychological measures and event-related potentials in

alcoholics: interrelationships, long-term reliabilities, and prediction of

resumption of drinking. J Clin Psychol 1994; 50: 37–46.

Penick E, Powell B, Nickel E, Bingham S, Riesenmy K, Read M, et al.

Co-morbidity of lifetime psychiatric disorder among male alcoholic

patients. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1994; 48: 1289–93.

Pfister H, Feiden W, Einhaupl KM, Einhaupl E. Spectrum of complications

during bacterial meningitis in adults. Results of a prospective clinical study.

Arch Neurol 1993; 50: 575–81.

Ruff R. Ruff Figural Fluency Test. Administration Manual, San Diego,

CA, USA, 1988.

Siboni A, Solander F, Sondergaard O. [Serious infections in alcoholics. 1.

Bacteremia, lobar pneumococcal pneumonia and pneumococcal menin-

gitis in alcoholics 1974-1987]. Ugeskr Laeger 1989; 151: 374–6.

Singer M, Teyssen S. Alkoholassoziierte Organschaden [Alcohol associated

somatic hazards]. Dt Arzteblatt 2001; 98: 2109–20.

Sipe J, Knobler R, Braheny S, Rice G, Panitch H, Oldstone M. A neurologic

rating scale (NRS) for use in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 1984; 34:

1368–72.

Spreen O, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological tests. 2nd ed.

New York: Oxford University Press; 1998.

Sturm W, Willmes K. Nonverbaler Lerntest (NVLT). Gottingen: Hogrefe

Verlag fur Psychologie; 1999a.

SturmW,Willmes K. Verbaler Lerntest (VLT). Gottingen: Hogrefe Verlag fur

Psychologie; 1999b.

Teasdale G, Jennett B. Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness.

A practical scale. Lancet 1974; 2: 81–83.

Tewes R. Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest fur Erwachsene—Revision. Bern:

Huber; 1991.

Torres IJ, Flashman LA, O’Leary DS, Swayze V, Andreasen NC. Lack of an

association between delayed memory and hippocampal and temporal lobe

size in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Biol Psychiatry

1997; 42: 1087–96.

Twamley E, Hami S, SteinM. Neuropsychological function in college students

with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res 2004; 126:

265–74.

Van de Beek D, Schmand B, de Gans J, Weisfelt M, Vaessen H, Dankert J, et al.

Cognitive impairment in adults with good recovery after bacterial

meningitis. J Infect Dis 2002; 186: 1047–52.

van de Beek D, de Gans J, Spanjaard L, Weisfelt M, Reitsma J, Vermeulen M.

Clinical features and prognostic factors in adults with bacterial meningitis.

N Engl J Med 2004; 351: 1849–59.

Zahner B, Harrer M, Erbguth F, Stefan H, Neundorfer B. [Purulent

meningoencephalitis—studies of disease progression and prognosis].

Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr 1995; 63: 220–6.

Zimmermann P, Fimm B. Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprufung (TAP),

volume 1.02c. Herzogenrath: Psytest, Vera Fimm, Psychologische

Testsysteme; 1994.

Appendix: domainsAttention

� TAP tonic alertness: median reaction time, SD

reaction time.

� TAP phasic alertness: median reaction time, SD reaction

time.

� TAP stimulus selectivity: median reaction time, SD reaction

time, errors.

� TAP divided alertness: median reaction time, SD reaction

time, missing items.

Executive functions

� ‘Regensburger Wortflussigkeitstest’: Verbal lexical fluency

with and without alternations, sem. (category) fluency with

and without alternations.

� Ruff FF test.

� HAWIE-R ‘Gemeinsamkeiten finden’ (similarities): CF.

� WCST: concepts, errors, perseverations.

� CVLT: sem. clustering.

Short-term memory/working memory

� WMS-R digit span: fw, bw.

� WMS-R block span: fw, bw.

344 Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 H. Schmidt et al.

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

� CVLT: 1st trial.

� WMS-R LM: part I.

Verbal learning/memory

� CVLT: sum score of 1st and 5th trial, slope, free recall after

interference, cued recall after interference, free late recall

after interference, free cued recall after interference, correct

recognition hits, recognition errors.

� VLT: difference right-positive/false-positive.

� WMS-R verbal association (VeP) recognition: part I.

� WMS-R VeP recognition: part II.

� WMS-R LM: part II.

Non verbal learning/memory

� Rey figure: del. recall.

� LGT-3: city map test.

� NVLT: difference right-positive/false-positive.

� WMS-R: Visual association (VP): part I.

� WMS-R: VP: part II.

Visuo-constructive functions

� HAWIE-R: mosaic test.

� Rey figure: copy.

Language

� AAT: Token test 4 and 5.

Neuropsychological sequelae and MRI changes after meningitis Brain (2006), 129, 333–345 345

by guest on October 6, 2016

http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from


Recommended