+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Neuroscience Meets Social Psychology -...

Neuroscience Meets Social Psychology -...

Date post: 18-Feb-2018
Category:
Upload: haque
View: 219 times
Download: 2 times
Share this document with a friend
30
1 Neuroscience Meets Social Psychology: An Integrative Approach to Human Empathy and Prosocial Behavior Grit Hein 1 and Tania Singer 1 1 University of Zurich, Center for the Study of Social and Neural Systems Corresponding author: Grit Hein University of Zurich Center for the Study of Social and Neural Systems Blümlisalpstrasse 10 CH-8006 Zurich / Switzerland Tel: 0041 44 634 3741 E-Mail: [email protected]
Transcript

1

Neuroscience Meets Social Psychology:

An Integrative Approach to Human Empathy and Prosocial Behavior

Grit Hein1 and Tania Singer

1

1 University of Zurich, Center for the Study of Social and Neural Systems

Corresponding author:

Grit Hein

University of Zurich

Center for the Study of Social and Neural Systems

Blümlisalpstrasse 10

CH-8006 Zurich / Switzerland

Tel: 0041 44 634 3741

E-Mail: [email protected]

2

From the moment we are born, we interact with our social environment. Long-

term deprivation of social interaction causes uttermost despair and neural damage

(Innocenti, 2007), supporting the notion that human beings are innately social. The

question of how “peoples’ thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are influenced by the actual,

imagined, or implied presence of others” is the focus of research in social psychology

(Allport, 1985). The long and fruitful tradition of social psychological research has

produced a variety of empirical approaches and models concerning prosocial motives,

emotions and behavior, as outlined in the other chapters of this book.

The field of neuroscience has only recently become interested in studying the

affective and social brain. A new interdisciplinary field, social neuroscience, has emerged

from a union of classical cognitive neuroscience and social psychology. Recent

neuroscientific research has addressed classical social psychological issues such as

peoples’ ability to understand other people’s minds: their beliefs, intentions, and feelings.

In the first part of this chapter we summarize the social neuroscience perspective on

“understanding others” and describe some of the key research findings. The second part

of the chapter discusses approaches and results related to the study of empathy and

prosocial behavior in social and developmental psychology. The last part of the chapter

provides a preliminary integration of the social neuroscience and social psychology

perspectives and discusses implication for future research.

3

“Understanding Others” in Social Neuroscience

At first, social neuroscience focused mainly on the investigation of basic social

abilities (for a review and overviews, see Adolphs, 1999, 2003; Blakemore, Winston, &

Frith, 2004; Ochsner & Lieberman, 2001). Several functional neuroimaging studies, for

example, have investigated the neural correlates of attending, recognizing, and

remembering socially relevant stimuli such as the facial expressions of fear, attractive

faces, indicators of trustworthiness, racial identity, and faces of fair and unfair players in

a game (Hart et al., 2000; Morris et al., 1996; O’Doherty et al., 2003; Singer et al., 2004;

Winston et al., 2002).

More recently, social neuroscientists have begun to clarify the neural mechanisms

underlying our ability to understand other people’s beliefs and thoughts, an ability known

as having a “theory of mind” (e.g., Premack & Woodruff, 1978), “mentalizing” (Frith &

Frith, 2003), “mindreading” (Baron-Cohen, 1995), or “cognitive perspective taking”; and

our ability to share other people’s feelings, referred to as “empathy” (for a similar

distinction between cognitive and affective aspects of reactions to other people, see Blair,

2005; de Vignemont & Singer, 2006; Decety & Lamm, 2007; Gallese, 2003; Keysers &

Gazzola, 2007; Preston & de Waal, 2002; Singer, 2006). Even though mentalizing and

empathizing are often used in concert when we try to understand other people’s minds,

preliminary evidence from studies of patients with marked social deficits, such as autism

or psychopathy, suggest that mentalizing and empathizing are two distinct abilities that

rely on different neural circuitries (see also Figure 1; Blair, 2005; Singer, 2006). For

example, patients with autistic spectrum disorders often have deficits in cognitive

perspective taking, while psychopaths are very good at understanding other people’s

4

intentions and consequently at manipulating other people’s behavior. In contrast,

psychopaths lack empathy, which may be part of the reason for their antisocial behavior.

This dissociation points to an important difference between our ability to

mentalize and our ability to empathize. Whereas both abilities play an important role in

drawing inferences about other people’s cognitive and emotional states, it has been

suggested that empathy is not just a matter of cognition, but also has motivational,

emotional, and social aspects (for a similar argument, see de Vignemont & Singer, 2006).

De Vignemont and Singer (2006) describe empathy as follows: We “empathize” with

others when we have (a) an affective state (b) that is isomorphic to another person’s

affective state and (c) was elicited by observing or imagining another person’s affective

state, and (d) when we know that the other person’s affective state is the source of our

own affective state.

The first statement is important because it differentiates empathy from theory of

mind, cognitive perspective taking, and mentalizing. The term “mentalizing” connotes a

person’s ability to cognitively represent the mental states of others, including their

affective states, without necessarily becoming emotionally involved. The term

“empathizing” connotes the capacity to share other people’s feelings. Accordingly, when

one empathizes with another person who is in pain, one feels the other person’s pain in

one’s own body. In contrast, when one understands someone else’s thoughts, one does

not feel the thought of the other in one’s own body. There are no qualia attached to the

representation of the other person’s thoughts. This difference and its significance become

clearer when we consider psychopaths: They do not have an impaired ability to

understand other people’s wishes, beliefs, intentions, and desires, but they seem to lack

5

the embodied feeling of empathy, which allows non-psychopaths to anticipate and

appreciate others’ suffering, thereby often preventing them from harming others. Thus,

although psychopaths possess the ability to mentalize, they are not able to empathize (for

a similar argument, see Blair, 2005).

Part b of de Vignemont and Singer’s (2006) description of empathy is important

in distinguishing “empathy” from “sympathy” or “compassion” (see Eisenberg & Miller,

1987, for a similar distinction). In all three cases, we feel vicariously for the other person.

But when we “empathize,” we share the other person’s feelings; when we “sympathize”

or show “compassion” we do not necessarily share the same feeling. For example, to use

first-person language for a moment, when I empathize with a person who is sad, I feel sad

myself. When I sympathize with or feel compassion for a sad person, I feel pity, love, or

concern for the person but am not sad myself. Also, when I notice that someone is jealous

or envious of me, I can sympathize with or show compassion toward that person, but I am

not jealous or envious myself. Further, empathy is not necessarily linked to prosocial

motivation – that is, a wish to maximize the other person’s happiness or alleviate the

other person’s distress, whereas there is such a link from sympathy or compassion to

prosocial motivation and actions or action tendences. Empathy can be misused, for

example, by a torturer who empathizes to find his victim’s weakest point, but he is far

from showing compassion for the suffering person.

Finally, de Vignemont and Singer’s (2006) conception of empathy distinguishes

between “empathy” and what might be called emotional contagion. The latter refers to a

reaction in which one shares an emotion with another person without realizing that the

other person’s emotion was the trigger. For example, babies start crying when they hear

6

other babies crying, long before they develop a sense of a self separate from others.

Those reactions might be a precursor of the development of a capacity for empathy (see

Sagi & Hoffman, 1976), but they are not considered empathic responses per se, because

the babies are not aware that they are vicariously feeling another person’s distress.

This discussion emphasizes the difference between perspective taking, the cooler,

more cognitive apprehension of others’ emotions, and empathy, which is a warmer, more

embodied sharing of emotions perceived in another person. From the neuroscientific

perspective, this distinction is expected to be reflected in differences in the neural

systems involved in cognitive perspective taking and empathic sharing of emotions.

“Theory of Mind” Studies

Neuroimaging studies of cognitive perspective taking are often referred to as

“theory of mind” experiments. In most cases, they are conducted with healthy adults, who

are asked to understand the intentions, beliefs, and desires of a protagonist in a story or a

cartoon (for a review, see Gallagher & Frith, 2003). Theory of mind studies have

consistently revealed a neural network comprising the posterior superior temporal sulcus

(STS), extending into the temporoparietal junctions (TPJ), the medial prefrontal cortex

(mPFC), and sometimes the temporal poles (TP). A schematic representation of the

mentalizing brain network is shown in Figure 1 in green.

--------------------------------

Insert Figure 1 about here

--------------------------------

Interestingly, the mPFC is involved not only when people mentalize other

people’s thoughts, intentions, and beliefs, but also when people reflect on their own states

7

(Mitchell, Banaji, & Macrae, 2005a). Jason Mitchell (Mitchell et al., 2002, 2005b, 2006)

recently conducted a series of experiments on mentalizing, which revealed functional

differences between judging the mental states of similar and dissimilar others. A more

ventral part of the mPFC was recruited when participants made self-judgments or

judgments about people whom they perceived as being similar to themselves with respect

to appearance or political attitudes. By contrast, a more dorsal part of the mPFC showed

enhanced activation – close to the activation found in the mentalizing studies cited above

– when participants judged the mental states of people perceived as being dissimilar to

themselves. This suggests that we use two different strategies when inferring other

people’s mental states: With one strategy, we simulate the other person’s states on the

basis of knowledge we have about ourselves; with the other strategy, we infer the mental

states of the other on the basis of more abstract knowledge about the world.

Studies of Empathy

While theory of mind research focuses on complex inferences about abstract

mental states such as other people’s beliefs, another line of neuroscientific research has

focused on our ability to understand other people’s goals and intentions by neuronally

simulating them. This line of research is based on the ground-breaking discovery of

“mirror neurons” in monkey premotor cortex, which fired both when a monkey

performed hand movements itself and when it merely observed another monkey or a

human performing the same hand movements (Ferrari et al., 2003; Gallese et al., 1996;

Rizzolatti et al., 1996). Later on, a similar common coding of the perception and

generation of motor actions was demonstrated in the human brain, using imaging

techniques such as PET or fMRI (for a review, see Grezes & Decety, 2001). In line with

8

the monkey studies, the human studies revealed that the same circuitry was recruited

when participants merely observed another person performing an action and when they

performed the same action themselves.

The idea that perception-action links in the brain enable us to understand others, a

claim that was originally established in the motor domain, has recently been expanded to

feelings and sensations (de Vignemont & Singer, 2006; Decety & Jackson, 2004; Decety

& Lamm, 2006; Gallese, 2003; Preston & de Waal, 2002). Indeed, fMRI studies in

humans have provided evidence that such shared neural networks enable us to “share”

pain, touch, or disgust with another person merely by perceiving or imagining the other

person in a relevant situation, in the absence of any stimulation of our own body. For

example, a study by Wicker et al. (2003) showed that viewing pictures of disgusted faces

and smelling disgusting odors produced corresponding neural responses in viewers. Jabbi

et al. (2007) had participants watch video clips showing people sampling pleasant and

unpleasant tastes, and then experience the different tastes themselves. Both studies

(Wicker et al., 2003; Jabbi et al., 2007) found neural activation in anterior insula cortex

(see Figure 1), a brain region involved in processing disgust and taste, among other

sensations, when people passively watched disgust in another person and when they were

disgusted themselves.

The majority of studies on empathic brain responses have been conducted in the

domain of pain (Avenanti et al., 2005, 2006; Botvinick et al., 2005; Bufalari et al., 2007;

Cheng et al., 2007; Gu & Han, 2007; Moriguchi et al., 2007; Jackson et al., 2005, 2006;

Lamm, Batson, & Decety, 2007; Morrison et al., 2004, 2007; Morrison & Downing,

2007; Singer et al., 2004, 2006; Saarela et al., 2007). For example, in an early study,

9

Singer and colleagues (2004) recruited couples and measured empathy “in vivo” by

assessing brain activity in the female partner while painful stimulation was applied either

to her own or to her partner’s right hand via electrodes attached to the back of the hand.

The male partner was seated next to the MRI scanner and a mirror system allowed the

female partner to see her own as well as her partner’s hand lying on a tilted board in front

of her. Before the experiment started, the couples were allowed to engage in social

interaction to increase the feeling of being in a “real-life situation.” Differently colored

flashes of light on a screen behind the board pointed to either the male or the female

partner’s hand, indicating which of them would receive painful and which would receive

non-painful stimulation.

This procedure permitted the measurement of pain-related brain activation when

pain was applied to the scanned participant (felt pain) or to her partner (empathy for

pain). The results indicated that parts of the so-called “pain matrix” – bilateral anterior

insula (AI), rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), brainstem, and cerebellum – were

activated when participants experienced pain themselves as well as when they saw a

signal indicating that a loved one had experienced pain. These areas are involved in the

processing of the affective component of pain – that is, the degree to which the

subjectively felt pain is unpleasant. Thus, both the experience of one’s own pain and the

knowledge that a beloved partner is experiencing pain activate the same affective pain

circuits, suggesting that if a beloved partner suffers pain, our brains also cause us to

suffer from this pain.

Activation in this network was also observed when participants saw an unknown

but likeable person suffering pain (Singer et al., 2006) or watched videos showing body

10

parts in potentially painful situations (Jackson et al., 2005, 2006), painful facial

expressions (Lamm et al., 2007), or hands being pricked by needles (Morrison et al.,

2004; for a review, see de Vignemont & Singer, 2006).

Analyses of empathic brain responses obtained while participants were observing

other people suffering – be it their loved ones or people the participants liked (Singer et

al., 2004, 2006) – have revealed individual differences in activity in empathy-related

pain-sensitive areas (ACC and AI). According to the results of Singer and associates,

these differences co-vary with scores on the Empathic Concern Scale of the Interpersonal

Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1980) and the Balanced Emotional Empathy Scale (BEES;

Mehrabian & Epstein, 1972). The higher participants scored on these questionnaires, the

higher their activation in ACC and anterior insula. Interestingly, Jabbi et al. (2007)

observed similar correlations between IRI subscales and empathic brain responses in the

anterior insula among participants who had observed others tasting pleasant or unpleasant

drinks associated with facial expressions of joy or disgust.

Empathic brain responses are positively correlated not only with trait measures of

empathy, but also with unpleasantness ratings given online after each trial of an empathy-

inducing task (Jackson et al., 2005; Lamm et al., 2007; Saarela et al., 2007). Future

research will have to clarify how these individual differences in empathic brain responses

come about and whether they are able to explain individual differences in prosocial

behavior, two lines of research which have not yet been sufficiently addressed.

“Understanding Others” in Social Psychology

In social psychology, the term most commonly used in the context of

understanding others’ emotional states is empathy. Depending on the focus of research,

11

different definitions of empathy have been offered. Hoffman (1981, p. 44) proposed a

relatively broad definition: “an affective response appropriate to someone else’s situation

rather than one’s own.” He considered empathy to be the result of a developmental

sequence, beginning with babies crying when they hear another baby’s cry and arriving,

after considerable development, at a clear sense of others as distinct from the self.

Perceived distress in the other can elicit “empathic distress” or “sympathetic distress.”

The latter is the basis for altruistic motivation, which consists mainly of the urge to

relieve one’s own distress.

Eisenberg and her associates (see review in Eisenberg, Chapter 7, this volume)

distinguish between empathy and sympathy. Empathy is defined as “an affective state

that stems from the apprehension of another’s emotional state or condition, and that is

congruent with it” (Eisenberg & Miller, 1987, p. 91). Sympathy is defined as “an

emotional response stemming from another’s emotional state or condition that is not

identical to the other’s emotion, but consists of feelings of sorrow or concern for

another’s welfare” (p. 92). Empathy in its pure form is not other-oriented. With further

cognitive processing it can turn into sympathy, personal distress, or a combination of

both (Eisenberg, 2000). Prosocial behavior is negatively correlated with personal distress,

and positively associated with sympathy (Eisenberg & Miller, 1987). Sympathy can

derive from empathy, but also from cognitive perspective taking. In children, empathic

responding is observed in the second year of life (Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, & King,

1979; Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, Wagner, & Chapman, 1992). It correlates with

parents’ expressivity, mediated by the level of the child’s effortful control (Valiente et al.,

12

2004), and predicts measurable prosocial dispositions, which are stable across five years

in early adulthood (Eisenberg et al., 2002).

The term “empathic concern,” introduced by Batson (see review in Chapter 1, this

volume) is similar to Eisenberg’s definition of sympathy. Empathic concern is defined as

an other-oriented response congruent with the perceived distress experienced by another

person (e.g., Batson, Turk, Shaw, & Klein, 1995). It is elicited by adopting the other’s

perspective and requires valuing the other’s welfare (Batson, Eklund, Chermok, Hoyt, &

Ortiz, 2007). In many cases, similarity between the individual and the person in need

increases empathic concern (Batson, Duncan, Ackerman, Buckley, & Birch, 1981) but is

neither a necessary nor a sufficient condition (Batson, Lishner, Cook, & Sawyer, 2005).

Valuing the welfare of the other is also affected by similarity, and remains stable even

after situational empathy has declined (Batson et al., 1995).

In line with the results of Eisenberg and associates on children and young adults

(e.g., Eisenberg, & Miller, 1987), the findings of Batson and colleagues support the

assumption that feeling empathy for a person in need leads to increased helping, one kind

of prosocial behavior. Experiments have shown that most people are willing to receive

uncomfortable electrical shocks themselves in order to help another person in need, even

if they are offered an “easy escape” that would not require helping. Only people who

report a high level of personal distress instead of empathy prefer to escape rather than

help (Batson, 1991).

What is the difference between someone who is empathic and helps, and someone

who is distressed and opts to escape? One approach to answering this question is to

identify personality traits that make up a “prosocial personality” (Penner, Fritzsche,

13

Craiger, & Freifeld, 1995; Penner & Orom, Chapter 3, this volume). Penner and

associates have identified two major components of a prosocial personality. The first

concerns prosocial thoughts and feelings, such as a sense of responsibility and a tendency

to experience cognitive and affective empathy. A second factor concerns being helpful –

that is, perceiving oneself as someone who is willing and able to help. Both factors are

significantly associated with actual prosocial behavior, such as helping co-workers or

volunteering in the community (e.g., Penner, 2002; Penner, Dovidio, Piliavin, &

Schroeder, 2005).

Another framework for investigating individual differences in empathy and

prosocial behavior is attachment theory, proposed originally by Bowlby (1982) and

elaborated and experimentally tested by Mikulincer and Shaver (2007; also Chapters 4

and 13 in the present volume). According to this theory, human beings are innately

equipped with attachment and caregiving behavioral systems. The attachment system is

especially apparent during the first years of life, but has also proven to influence social

interactions across the life span (Fraley & Shaver, 2000; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007).

Individual differences in attachment style can be measured along two orthogonal

dimensions, attachment-related anxiety and avoidance (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998).

Relatively low scores on these dimensions indicate attachment security, which has been

associated with empathy in young children (2 to 3 years old; Kerstenbaum, Farber, &

Sroufe, 1989; van der Mark, van IJzendoorn, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2002) and in

adults (Mikulincer et al., 2001). Increasing security by experimental techniques such as

implicit and explicit priming has been shown to affect compassion and altruistic behavior

(Mikulincer, Shaver, Gillath, & Nitzberg, 2005), whereas attachment insecurity can

14

interfere with the natural tendency to help others in need (Gillath, Shaver, & Mikulincer,

2005; Feeney & Collins, 2001; also Collins, Chapter 18, this volume).

Besides the influences of individual differences, the relation between empathy and actual

prosocial behavior is influenced by social factors, such as whether the person in need is

seen as a member of one’s own or a different group (Dovidio, Gaertner, Johnson,

Ashburn-Nardo, & Spicer, Chapter 20, this volume). There is a large body of evidence

indicating strong favoritism toward members of one’s group (e.g., Hewstone, Rubin, &

Willis, 2002; Mullen, Brown, & Smith, 1992). Empathy is a better predictor of helping an

ingroup member, probably because empathic concern is facilitated by familiarity and

attachment, whereas prosocial behavior toward outgroup members is based on factors

such as attraction (Sturmer, Snyder, & Omoto, 2005). Taking the perspective of an

outgroup member (Finlay, & Stephan, 2000), however, or focusing on the person’s

feelings (Batson et al., 1997; Vescio, Sechrist, & Paolucci, 2003) reduces intergroup

prejudice and bias (Dovidio et al., 2004).

“Understanding Others” in Neuroscience and Social Psychology:

An Integrative Perspective

In both disciplines, empathy is seen as central for our ability to understand

another person’s emotional states, but does “empathy” have the same meaning in social

neuroscience and in social and developmental psychology? In some social psychological

definitions, empathy is viewed as an affective response that is congruent with (Eisenberg

& Miller, 1987) or appropriate for (Hoffman, 1981) another’s emotion. Other definitions

emphasize that empathy is related to adopting the other’s perspective (e.g., Batson et al.,

1995). While affective empathic responses and perspective taking are often linked, the

15

reviewed neuroscientific studies suggest a clear distinction between putting oneself into

another’s shoes (or mind: cognitive perspective taking) and sharing the other’s affective

state in an embodied manner (empathy). Accordingly, part of the conception of empathy

in social neuroscience is based on its demarcation from cognitive perspective taking

(Blair, 2005; de Vignemont & Singer, 2006; Decety & Lamm, 2007; Gallese, 2003;

Keysers & Gazzola, 2007; Singer, 2006). This distinction is in line with early social

psychological work, which also proposed a distinction between perspective taking and

empathic concern (e.g., Coke, Batson, & Mc Davis, 1978).

Although so far empathy research in the two disciplines has developed relatively

independently, there is consistency between measures of empathy in social psychology

and neuroscience. This is reflected by the covariation of individual scores on empathy

questionnaires such as the IRI (Davis, 1980) and the BEES (Mehrabian & Epstein, 1972)

with empathy-related activation of ACC and anterior insula (Singer et al., 2004, 2006).

Social neuroscience has taken only preliminary steps in investigating individual

differences in empathy-related brain activity. But it is already clear that the neural

empathy response is related to a participant’s affective ties to the other person (Singer et

al., 2004, 2006), the participant’s appraisal of whether the other person’s suffering is

justified (Lamm et al., 2007), prior experience with the situation (Cheng et al., 2007), and

the intensity of the inflicted pain (e.g., seeing a needle pricking vs. penetrating a muscle,

Avenanti et al., 2006).

It would now be worthwhile to investigate whether differences in prosocial

personality traits (Penner, 2002) or attachment styles (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007)

predict differences in activation of the brain network associated in social neuroscience

16

studies of empathy. There is already evidence that attachment-style differences in ability

to suppress emotionally painful thoughts modulate brain activity (Gillath, Bunge, Shaver,

Wendelken, & Mikulincer, 2005).

Another focus for future research would be possible links between empathic brain

responses, sympathy, and prosocial behavior. Based on current neuroscientific evidence it

is difficult to decide whether empathic brain responses reflect feeling like the other

person, in the sense of simulation the other’s feelings, or feeling for the other, in the

sense of sympathy. One way to disentangle the two would be to correlate empathic brain

responses with nuanced situational self-reports, as already done in social psychology (for

example, in distinguishing between empathy and personal distress). Moreover, social and

developmental psychologists have designed clever experimental paradigms to investigate

the links between empathy or sympathy and prosocial behavior, and some of these could

be adapted for neuroscience experiments.

Working from the other side of the disciplinary divide, social and developmental

psychologists might be able to avoid some difficulties in previous empathy research by

using neuroimaging methods. For example, in many behavioral empathy studies,

participants are repeatedly asked to report their feelings. This can be problematic,

because it creates strong demand characteristics (Eisenberg, & Lennon, 1983) and may

induce social desirability biases. Neuroimaging methods should be beneficial in such

cases, because empathy and related phenomena can be investigated without such explicit

instructions and self-observations (e.g., Singer et al., 2004, 2006). Moreover,

neuroimaging methods can help to disentangle separate psychological processes, which

may have similar behavioral correlates, yet be importantly distinct. For example,

17

emotionally empathizing with or cognitively taking the perspective of another person can

lead to the same behavioral outcome, but if they have different neural correlates they can

be more carefully distinguised. Another interesting contribution of neuroscience methods

is the quantification of changes in empathic response over the life span and as a function

of empathy training or other interventions. For example, assessing the neural plasticity in

empathic responses in children of different ages would add to our understanding of

developmental aspects of empathy, complementing the already existing behavioral data.

Last but not least, our brains do not exist in isolation and their functioning should

be modulated by the social factors and culture, on the “meso-level” of groups and the

“macro-level” of society (Penner et al., 2005). According to research in social

psychology, ingroup and intergroup processes have strong effects on empathy and

prosocial behavior (Dovidio et al., 2004), but such effects have not yet been explored

with neuroscientific methods. It would be interesting to investigate whether ingroup and

intergroup effects modulate empathy-related brain activity, opening yet another broad

avenue for future social neuroscience research.

-------------------------------

Insert Figure 2 about here

--------------------------------

18

Social neuroscience and social and developmental psychology have contributed

different pieces to the large, complex puzzle of empathy and prosocial behavior, as

visualized in Figure 2. So far, social neuroscience has elucidated how emotional

contagion and empathy are implemented in the human brain, but it has not yet explored

how empathy, sympathy, and empathic concern affect prosocial behavior. Empathy

research in social and developmental psychology starts there, providing knowledge and

measures related to connections between mental processes, individual differences, and

prosocial behavior. It will prove useful to have a more extensive dialogue between the

different kinds of researchers.

Conclusion

In this chapter, we have reviewed findings related to empathy and prosocial

behavior from social neuroscience and social and developmental psychology. In the

young field of social neuroscience, empathy research has focused on identifying brain

regions and mechanisms involved in empathy and perspective taking, which leaves a

variety of open questions concerning the sources of individual differences in empathic

brain responses, the development of the empathic brain, and the links between empathic

brain responses and actual helping behavior. Social and developmental psychologists

have acquired expertise in these domains. We are convinced that integrating and linking

the social neuroscience and the social and developmental psychology of empathy and

prosocial behavior will be fruitful for both fields and will improve our understanding of a

very important domain of human experience and behavior..

19

References

Adolphs, R. (1999). Social cognition and the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,

3, 469-479.

Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Reviews:

Neuroscience, 4, 165-178.

Allport, G. W. (1985). The historical background of social psychology. In G. Lindzey &

E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology. New York: McGraw Hill.

Avenanti, A., Bueti, D., Galati, G., & Aglioti, S. (2005). Transcranial magnetic

stimulation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for pain. Nature

Neuroscience, 8, 955-960.

Avenanti, A., Paluello, I. M., Bufalari, I., & Aglioti, S. M. (2006). Stimulus-driven

modulation of motor-evoked potentials during observation of others’ pain.

Neuroimage, 32, 316-324.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind.

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press..

Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruismc question: Toward a social-psychological answer.

Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Batson, C. D., Eklund, J. H., Chermok, V.L., Hoyt, J. L., & Ortiz, B. G. (2007). An

additional antecedent of empathic concern: Valuing the welfare of the person in

need. Journal of Personality and Social Research, 93, 65-74.

Batson, C. D., Lishner, D. A., Cook, J., & Sawyer, S. (2005). Similarity and nurturance:

Two possible sources of empathy for strangers. Basic and Applied Social

Psychology, 27, 15-25.

20

Batson, C. D., Polycarpou, M. P., Harmon-Jones, E., Imhoff, H. J., Mitchener, E. C.,

Bednar, L. L., Klein, T. R., & Highberger, L. (1997). Empathy and attitudes: Can

feeling for a member of a stigmatized group improve feelings toward the group?

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 108-118.

Batson, C. D., Turk, C. L., Shaw, L. L., & Klein, T. R. (1995).Information function of

empathic emotion: learning that we value the other’s welfare. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1042-1054.

Blair, R. J. (2005). Responding to the emotions of others: Dissociating forms of empathy

through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. Consciousness and

Cognition, 14, 698-718.

Blakemore, S. J., Winston, J., & Frith, U. (2004). Social cognitive neuroscience: Where

are we heading? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 216-222.

Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd

ed.). New York: Basic

Books.

Bufalari, I., Aprile, T., Avenanti, A., Di Russo, F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2007). Empathy for

pain and touch in the human somatosensory cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2553-

2561.

Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult

attachment: An integrative overview. In J.A. Simpson & W.S. Rholes (Eds.),

Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46-76). New York: Guilford Press.

Cheng, Y., Lin, C. P., Liu, H. L., Hsu, Y. Y., Lim, K. E., Hung, D., & Decety, J. (2007).

Expertise modulates the perception of pain in others. Current Biology, 17, 1708-

1713.

21

Coke, J. S., Batson, C. D., & McDavis, K. (1978). Empathic mediation of helping: a two-

stage model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 752-766.

Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy.

JSAS Catalogue of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10.

de Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when, and why?

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 435-441.

Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy.

Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71-100.

Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience.

The Scientific World Journal, 6, 1146-1163.

Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2007). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social

interaction: How low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition.

The Neuroscientist [doi:10.1177/1073858407304654].

Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual Review of

Psychology, 51, 665-697.

Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Cumberland, A., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., et al.

(2002). Prosocial development in early adulthood: A longitudinal study. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 993-1006.

Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and related capacities.

Psychological Bulletin, 94, 100-131.

Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P.A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related

behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 91-119.

22

Feeney, B. C., & Collins, N. L. (2001). Predictors of caregiving in adult intimate

relationships: an attachment theoretical perspective. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 80, 972-994.

Ferrari, P. F., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G., & Fogassi, L. (2003). Mirror neurons

responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the

monkey ventral premotor cortex. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 1703-

1714.

Finlay, K.A., & Stephan, W.G. (2000). Improving intergroup relations: the effects of

empathy on racial attitudes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 1720

1737.

Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing.

Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological

Sciences, 358, 459-473.

Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: theoretical

developments, emerging controversies, and unanswered questions. Review of

General Psychology, 4, 132-154.

Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of “theory of mind.” Trends

in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 77-83.

Gallese, V. (2003). The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: The quest for a

common mechanism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.

Series B, Biological Sciences, 358, 517-528.

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the

premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593-609.

23

Gillath, O., Bunger, S .A., Shaver, P. R., Wendelken, C., & Mikulincer, M. (2005).

Attachment-style differences in the ability to suppress negative thoughts:

Exploring the neural correlates. Neuroimage, 28, 835-847.

Gillath, O., Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer, M. (2005). An attachment-theoretical approach

to compassion and altruism. In P. Gilbert (Ed.), Compassion: its nature and use in

psychotherapy (pp. 121-147). London: Brunner-Routledge.

Grezes, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation,

observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-analysis. Human Brain

Mapping, 12, 1-19.

Gu, X., & Han, S. (2007). Attention and reality constraints on the neural processes of

empathy for pain. Neuroimage, 36, 256-267.

Hart, A. J., Whalen, P. J., Shin, L. M., McInerney, S. C., Fischer, H., & Rauch, S. L.

(2000). Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs. ingroup

face stimuli. Neuroreport, 11, 2351-2355.

Hewstone, M., Rubin, M., & Willis, H. (2002). Intergroup bias. Annual Review of

Psychology, 51, 575-604.

Hoffman, M. L. (1981). The development of empathy. In J. P. Rushton, & R. M.

Sorrentino (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior: Social, personality, and developmental

perspectives (pp. 41 – 63). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Innocenti, G. M. (2007). Subcortical regulation of cortical development: Some effects of

early, selective deprivations. Progress in Brain Research, 164, 23-37.

Jabbi, M., Swart, M., & Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and negative emotions

in the gustatory cortex. Neuroimage, 34, 1744-1753.

24

Jackson, P. L., Brunet, E., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2006). Empathy examined

through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you

feel pain. Neuropsychologia, 44, 752-761.

Jackson, P. L., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2005). How do we perceive the pain of

others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy. Neuroimage, 24,

771-779.

Kerstenbaum, R., Farber, E.A., & Sroufe, L.A. (1989). Individual differences in empathy

among preschoolers: relation to attachments history. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.),

Empathy and related emotional competence. New Directions for Child

Development, No. 44 (pp. 51-64). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2007). Integrating simulation and theory of mind: From self

to social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 194-196.

Keysers, C., Wicker, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J.-L., Forgassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2004). A

touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch.

Neuron, 42, 335-346.

Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of human empathy:

Effects of perspective taking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of Cognitive

Neuroscience, 19, 42-58.

Mehrabian, A., & Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of

Personality, 40, 525-543.

Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., Halevy, V., Avihou, N., Avidan, S., & Eshkoli, N. (2001).

Attachment theory and reactions to other’ needs: Evidence that activation of the

25

sense of attachment security promotes empathic responses. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 81, 1205-1224.

Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P.R., Gillath, O., & Nitzberg, R.A. (2005). Attachment,

caregiving, and altruism: Boosting attachment security increases compassion and

helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 817-839.

Mitchell, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & Macrae, C. N. (2005a). General and specific

contributions of the medial prefrontal cortex to knowledge about mental states.

Neuroimage, 28, 757-762.

Moriguchi, Y., Decety, J., Ohnishi, T., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto, K., Matsuda, H., &

Komaki, G. (2007). Empathy and judging other’s pain: An fMRI study of

alexithymia. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2223-2234.

Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., Rowland, D., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., &

Dolan, R. J. (1996). A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful

and happy facial expressions. Nature, 383, 812-815.

Morrison, I., Lloyd, D., di Pellegrino, G., & Roberts, N. (2004). Vicarious responses to

pain in anterior cingulate cortex: Is empathy a multisensory issue? Cognitive,

Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 270-278.

Morrison, I., Peelen, M. V., & Downing, P. E. (2007). The sight of others’ pain

modulates motor processing in human cingulate cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2214-

2222.

Mullen, B., Brown, R.J., & Smith, C. (1992). Ingroup bias as a function of salience,

relevance, and status: An integration? European Journal of Social Psychology,

22, 103-122.

26

O’Doherty, J., Winston, J., Critchley, H., Perrett, D., Burt, D. M., & Dolan, R. J. (2003).

Beauty in a smile: The role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness.

Neuropsychologia, 41, 147-155.

Ochsner, K. N., & Lieberman, M. D. (2001). The emergence of social cognitive

neuroscience. The American Psychologist, 56, 717-734.

Penner, L. A. (2002). The causes of sustained volunteerism: An interactionist perspective.

Journal of Social Issues, 58, 447 – 467.

Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., & Schroeder, D. A. (2005). Prosocial

behavior: Multilevel perspectives. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 365-392.

Penner, L. A., Fritzsche, B. A., Craiger, J. P., & Freifeld, T. R. (1995). Measuring the

prosocial personality. In J. Butcher, C. D. Spielberger (Eds.), Advances in

personality assessment (Vol. 10, pp. 147-163). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?

Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515-526.

Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases.

Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1-20.

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the

recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131-141.

Saarela, M. V., Hlushchuk, Y., Williams, A. C., Schürmann, M., Kalso, E., & Hari, R.

(2007). The compassionate brain: Humans detect intensity of pain from another’s

face. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 230-237.

Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in newborns. Developmental

Psychology, 12, 175-176.

27

Singer, T. (2006). The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading:

Review of literature and implications for future research. Neuroscience and

Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 855-863.

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D.

(2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of

others. Nature, 439, 466-469.

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004).

Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain.

Science, 303, 1157-1162.

Sturmer, S., Snyder, M, & Omoto, A. M. (2005). Prosocial emotions and helping: the

moderating role of group membership. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 88, 532-46.

Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Cumberland, A., & Losoya, S.

H. (2004). Prediction of children’s empathy-related responding from their

effortful control and parents’ expressivity. Developmental Psychology, 40, 911-

926.

Van der Mark, I. L., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2002).

Development of empathy in girls during the second year of life: Associations with

parenting, attachment, and temperament. Social Development, 11, 451-468.

Vescio, T. K., Sechrist, G. B., & Palolucci, M. P. (2003). Perspective taking and

prejudice reduction: The mediational role of empathy arousal and situational

attributions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 455-472.

28

Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003).

Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling

disgust. Neuron, 40, 655-664.

Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O’Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Automatic and

intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nature

Neuroscience, 5, 277-283.

Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R.A. (1979). Child rearing and children’s

prosocial initiations toward victims of distress. Child Development, 50, 319-330.

Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development

of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28, 126-136.

29

Note

We thank Daniel Batson for valuable comments on the manuscript. This work was

supported by the University of Zürich and the Society in Science: The Branco Weiss

Fellowship (G.H.). Correspondence should be addressed to [email protected].

30

Figure Captions

Figure 1. Brain networks involved in understanding others. Schematic representation of

the brain areas typically involved in theory of mind (green) and empathy (orange) tasks.

MFC = medial prefrontal cortex; ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; AI = anterior insula;

SII = secondary somatosensory cortex; TP = temporal poles; STS = superior temporal

sulcus; TPJ = temporo-parietal junction.

Figure 2. Summary of complementary foci of research on empathy and prosocial

behavior in social neuroscience and social and developmental psychology.


Recommended