+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of...

Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of...

Date post: 24-May-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 3 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
34
CELL BIOLOGY Copyright © 2017 The Authors, some rights reserved; exclusive licensee American Association for the Advancement of Science. No claim to original U.S. Government Works Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription factor HSF-1 to enhance the expression of molecular chaperones in C. elegans Felicia K. Ooi and Veena Prahlad* Learning, a process by which animals modify their behavior as a result of experience, enables organisms to synthe- size information from their surroundings to acquire resources and avoid danger. We showed that a previous en- counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen by increasing the heat shock factor 1 (HSF-1)dependent expression of genes encoding molecular chaperones. Experience-mediated enhancement of chaperone gene expression required serotonin, which primed HSF-1 to enhance the expression of molecular chaperone genes by promoting its localization to RNA polymerase IIenriched nuclear loci, even before transcription occurred. However, HSF-1dependent chaperone gene expression was stimulated only if and when animals encountered the pathogen. Thus, learning equips C. elegans to better survive environmental dangers by preemptively and specifically initiating transcriptional mechanisms through- out the whole organism that prepare the animal to respond rapidly to proteotoxic agents. These studies provide one plausible basis for the protective role of environmental enrichment in disease. INTRODUCTION The ability to accurately predict danger and implement appropriate protective responses is critical for survival. Many animals have neuro- nal circuits that detect unfavorable conditions and initiate an avoidance response. In addition, all cells have conserved mechanisms to repair and protect macromolecules from damage that occurs under adverse conditions. One such mechanism present in all cells to protect against protein damage is the heat shock response (14). The heat shock re- sponse is mediated by the transcription factor heat shock factor 1 (HSF1), which, in response to a variety of stressors, induces the expression of cytoprotective heat shock proteins (HSPs), which are molecular chap- erones that maintain protein stability and help degrade proteins that misfold and aggregate under stressful conditions (14). HSF1 activity is essential for all organisms to adapt to changing environments. Be- cause the heat shock response has been characterized predominantly in mammalian cells in culture and in unicellular organisms such as yeast, the activation of HSF1 has been considered an autonomous re- sponse of cells to protein damage caused by stressors (13). However, emerging evidence has shown that within a metazoan such as the nem- atode Caenorhabditis elegans HSF1 (HSF-1) and the cellular response to protein damage are not autonomously controlled by individual cells, but instead are under the regulation of the animalsnervous system (511). The biological role for such systemicrather than cell-autonomousregulation is unclear. We discovered that one mechanism by which the activity of C. elegans HSF-1 is regulated is through the neurosensory release of the bioamine serotonin [5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT)] (7). In vertebrates and invertebrates, serotonergic systems play a central role in neurophysiological processes underlying learning and memory, allowing animals to learn about threats in their environment and form memories that can be later recalled to modify behavior (1223). There- fore, we asked whether control by the serotonergic-based learning cir- cuitry allowed C. elegans to modulate HSF-1 activity in response to prior experience, so as to better combat threats in its environment. Here, we show that, in C. elegans, olfactory experience of specific odorants released by the toxic bacterium Pseudomonas aeruginosa PA14 primed HSF-1dependent transcription of cytoprotective hsp genes, such that the expression of these genes was enhanced if and when the animals subsequently encountered the pathogen. This priming re- quired 5-HT and appeared to be a consequence of the preemptive mo- bilization of HSF-1 to the vicinity of RNA polymerase II (pol II) in nuclei throughout the animal. Animals that cannot synthesize 5-HT were deficient in relocalizing HSF-1 in response to olfactory stimuli and did not show this learning-dependent enhancement of hsp ex- pression. Olfactory priming of HSF-1 was protective, allowing ani- mals that had previously experienced only the smell of P. aeruginosa to better respond to a subsequent exposure to the pathogen itself. We conclude that neuronal control over the HSF-1mediated defense mechanism of cells allows learning and memory to elicit anticipatory changes that promote the ability of cells to respond to stress, thus facilitating survival. RESULTS Olfactory exposure to odorants produced by the toxic bacterium P. aeruginosa PA14 accelerates the pathogen avoidance response of C. elegans To test whether prior experience primes animals to activate HSF-1, we set up a paradigm to train C. elegans to avoid sensory stimuli that are predictive of damage and asked whether pre-exposure to such stimuli affected HSF-1 activity. To do this, we exploited previous findings that C. elegans have an innate aversion to specific pathogens and display experience-dependent plasticity to avoid ingesting pathogenic bacteria such as P. aeruginosa strain PA14 (15, 24). Although C. elegans are typ- ically attracted to any novel bacterium, be it pathogenic bacteria such as PA14 or nonpathogenic Escherichia coli strains (25, 26), animals pre- viously exposed to a lawn of pathogenic PA14 will avoid PA14 lawns upon subsequent exposure. This learned avoidance behavior requires the olfactory nervous system and 5-HT (15, 24, 27, 28). We used this information to train animals to avoid PA14 using odor alone, thereby circumventing any physical damage that could be inflicted by actual Department of Biology, Aging Mind and Brain Initiative, 143 Biology Building East, 338 BBE, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA. *Corresponding author. Email: [email protected] SCIENCE SIGNALING | RESEARCH ARTICLE Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017 1 of 17 on October 17, 2017 http://stke.sciencemag.org/ Downloaded from
Transcript
Page 1: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

CELL B IOLOGY

Department of Biology, Aging Mind and Brain Initiative, 143 Biology Building East,338 BBE, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA.*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

Copyright © 2017

The Authors, some

rights reserved;

exclusive licensee

American Association

for the Advancement

of Science. No claim

to original U.S.

Government Works

Dow

nlo

Olfactory experience primes the heat shocktranscription factor HSF-1 to enhance the expressionof molecular chaperones in C. elegansFelicia K. Ooi and Veena Prahlad*

Learning, a process by which animals modify their behavior as a result of experience, enables organisms to synthe-size information from their surroundings to acquire resources and avoid danger. We showed that a previous en-counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to thepathogen by increasing the heat shock factor 1 (HSF-1)–dependent expression of genes encoding molecularchaperones. Experience-mediated enhancement of chaperone gene expression required serotonin, which primedHSF-1 to enhance the expression of molecular chaperone genes by promoting its localization to RNA polymeraseII–enriched nuclear loci, even before transcription occurred. However, HSF-1–dependent chaperone gene expressionwas stimulated only if and when animals encountered the pathogen. Thus, learning equips C. elegans to bettersurvive environmental dangers by preemptively and specifically initiating transcriptional mechanisms through-out the whole organism that prepare the animal to respond rapidly to proteotoxic agents. These studies provideone plausible basis for the protective role of environmental enrichment in disease.

ade

on O

ctober 17, 2017http://stke.sciencem

ag.org/d from

INTRODUCTIONThe ability to accurately predict danger and implement appropriateprotective responses is critical for survival. Many animals have neuro-nal circuits that detect unfavorable conditions and initiate an avoidanceresponse. In addition, all cells have conserved mechanisms to repairand protect macromolecules from damage that occurs under adverseconditions. One such mechanism present in all cells to protect againstprotein damage is the heat shock response (1–4). The heat shock re-sponse is mediated by the transcription factor heat shock factor 1 (HSF1),which, in response to a variety of stressors, induces the expression ofcytoprotective heat shock proteins (HSPs), which are molecular chap-erones that maintain protein stability and help degrade proteins thatmisfold and aggregate under stressful conditions (1–4). HSF1 activityis essential for all organisms to adapt to changing environments. Be-cause the heat shock response has been characterized predominantlyin mammalian cells in culture and in unicellular organisms such asyeast, the activation of HSF1 has been considered an autonomous re-sponse of cells to protein damage caused by stressors (1–3). However,emerging evidence has shown that within a metazoan such as the nem-atode Caenorhabditis elegans HSF1 (HSF-1) and the cellular response toprotein damage are not autonomously controlled by individual cells, butinstead are under the regulation of the animals’ nervous system (5–11).The biological role for such systemic—rather than cell-autonomous—regulation is unclear. We discovered that one mechanism by whichthe activity of C. elegansHSF-1 is regulated is through the neurosensoryrelease of the bioamine serotonin [5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT)] (7).In vertebrates and invertebrates, serotonergic systems play a centralrole in neurophysiological processes underlying learning and memory,allowing animals to learn about threats in their environment and formmemories that can be later recalled to modify behavior (12–23). There-fore, we asked whether control by the serotonergic-based learning cir-cuitry allowed C. elegans to modulate HSF-1 activity in response to priorexperience, so as to better combat threats in its environment.

Here, we show that, in C. elegans, olfactory experience of specificodorants released by the toxic bacterium Pseudomonas aeruginosaPA14 primed HSF-1–dependent transcription of cytoprotective hspgenes, such that the expression of these genes was enhanced if and whenthe animals subsequently encountered the pathogen. This priming re-quired 5-HT and appeared to be a consequence of the preemptive mo-bilization of HSF-1 to the vicinity of RNA polymerase II (pol II) innuclei throughout the animal. Animals that cannot synthesize 5-HTwere deficient in relocalizing HSF-1 in response to olfactory stimuliand did not show this learning-dependent enhancement of hsp ex-pression. Olfactory priming of HSF-1 was protective, allowing ani-mals that had previously experienced only the smell of P. aeruginosato better respond to a subsequent exposure to the pathogen itself. Weconclude that neuronal control over the HSF-1–mediated defensemechanism of cells allows learning and memory to elicit anticipatorychanges that promote the ability of cells to respond to stress, thusfacilitating survival.

RESULTSOlfactory exposure to odorants produced by the toxicbacterium P. aeruginosa PA14 accelerates the pathogenavoidance response of C. elegansTo test whether prior experience primes animals to activate HSF-1, weset up a paradigm to train C. elegans to avoid sensory stimuli that arepredictive of damage and asked whether pre-exposure to such stimuliaffected HSF-1 activity. To do this, we exploited previous findingsthat C. elegans have an innate aversion to specific pathogens and displayexperience-dependent plasticity to avoid ingesting pathogenic bacteriasuch as P. aeruginosa strain PA14 (15, 24). Although C. elegans are typ-ically attracted to any novel bacterium, be it pathogenic bacteria suchas PA14 or nonpathogenic Escherichia coli strains (25, 26), animals pre-viously exposed to a lawn of pathogenic PA14 will avoid PA14 lawnsupon subsequent exposure. This learned avoidance behavior requiresthe olfactory nervous system and 5-HT (15, 24, 27, 28). We used thisinformation to train animals to avoid PA14 using odor alone, therebycircumventing any physical damage that could be inflicted by actual

1 of 17

Page 2: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

exposure to the pathogen (Fig. 1A and fig. S1A). We then asked whetherolfactory training on the odor of this toxic bacterium could enhancethe animals’ avoidance behavior if they were to subsequently encounterthe pathogen. Animals were trained by exposing them to the odor ofPA14 cultures for 30 min. Controls were mock-trained by exposureto the odor of the standard nonpathogenic E. coli OP50 strain on

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

which the animals are typically raised. To assess whether olfactorypre-exposure was sufficient to elicit learned avoidance behavior, trainedand mock-trained animals were then immediately given a choice be-tween PA14 lawns and OP50 lawns. Behavioral preference was quan-tified by calculating a choice index (CI) for PA14, wherein a CI of 1.0indicates maximal preference and a CI of −1.0 indicates maximal

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Naive

OP50OP50

Olfactory pre-exposure(odor)

Positive choice indexindicates preference

for experimental bacteria

Negative choice indexindicates avoidance

of experimental bacteria

or

Choice index =# on experimental bacteria

- # on OP50Total #

5' 15' 30' 45' 1 h 1.5 h 2 h 2.5 h 3 h 3.5 h 4 h–1.0

–0.8

–0.6

–0.4

–0.2

0.0

0.2

0.4

Time

Ch

oic

e i

nd

ex

fo

r P

A1

4

OP50 odor (OP50 versus PA14)PA14 odor (OP50 versus PA14)

* ***

** ** ****

**

*

0 14 16 18 20 22 240

20

40

60

80

100

120

Time (hours)

Pe

rce

nt

su

rviv

al

Control RNAihsf-1 RNAi

P = 0

Odor only Odor + PA14 lawn

0

5

10

15

20

25F

old

ch

an

ge

of

hs

p-7

0

(F4

4E

5.4

/F4

4E

5.5

) m

RN

A

OP50PA14

***

n.s.

Odor only Odor + PA14 lawn

0

20

40

60

Fo

ld c

ha

ng

e o

f h

sp

-1

6.2

(Y4

6H

3A

.3)

mR

NA

OP50PA14

**

n.s.

Odor only Odor + PA14 lawn

0

10

20

30

40

50

Fo

ld c

ha

ng

e o

f h

sp

-1

6.4

1

(Y4

6H

3A

.2)

mR

NA

OP50PA14

**

n.s.

A B

C D

E F

Fig. 1. Olfactory learning enhances HSF-1 activation. (A) Schematic of olfactory training and subsequent choice assay. Animals were reared on OP50, and naïve animalsor animals pre-exposed to PA14 or OP50 odors were given a choice between PA14 lawns and OP50 lawns. The PA14 and OP50 lawns on the choice plate were 1 inch apartand grown as described in Materials and Methods. Animals were placed in the center, equidistant from both lawns, and the number of animals that migrated onto each lawnwas tracked over time. A choice index (CI) at each time point scored, over 4 hours was calculated as shown. (B) CI for PA14 of wild-type animals pre-exposed to the odor ofeither OP50 or PA14 and then offered the choice between OP50 and PA14 lawns. Preference was recorded at the times indicated on the x axis. n = 16 to 17 experimentsof 30 animals per condition. Student’s paired t test, *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01. (C) Survival of hsf-1 knockdown animals on PA14. n = 3 experiments of 50 animals per condition.Log-rank test, P = 0. See table S1. (D to F) hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5), hsp-16.2 (Y46H3A.3), and hsp-16.41 (Y46H3A.2) mRNA abundance measured by quantitative reversetranscription polymerase chain reaction (qRT-PCR) upon exposing animals that had been trained on OP50 or PA14 odor to a lawn of PA14. Values were normalized to wild-type animals pre-exposed to OP50 odor. n = 38 (D), n = 12 (E), and n = 10 (F) experiments of 30 animals per condition. Pairwise mean comparison from linear mixed modelanalysis, **P < 0.01 and ***P < 0.001. Data represent means ± SEM for (B), (D), (E), and (F). Data in (C) represent total animals across all experiments. n.s., not significant.

2 of 17

Page 3: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

aversion (Fig. 1A). Because of the variability inherent to behavioralassays, all avoidance assays were conducted and are represented aspairwise comparisons between control and experimental populationsof C. elegans evaluated in parallel. As previously reported (15), whenfaced with a choice between OP50 or PA14, naïve animals initiallypreferred the novel bacterium and accumulated on PA14 within 5 min(fig. S1B). However, after 45 min, the animals began to avoid PA14, andby 4 hours, 80% of the animals had left the lawn of PA14 and movedto the lawn of OP50 (fig. S1B). Animals pre-exposed to the OP50 odor(mock-trained, control animals) behaved like naïve animals and alsoinitially accumulated on PA14 and then began to leave the lawn by1 hour (Fig. 1B). In contrast, animals pre-exposed to the odor of PA14avoided the PA14 lawn significantly earlier and left within the first5 min (Fig. 1B). The avoidance of PA14 after pre-exposure to PA14odorants appeared to reflect an innate response of the animals to PA14.It was also not a simple consequence of adaptation to the smell. Thiswas inferred from the behavior of animals exposed for similar durationsto the odor of another novel, but nonpathogenic, bacterium, E. coliHT115. In this case, animals did not avoid HT115 when given a choicebetween HT115 and OP50 but remained on HT115 throughout theanalysis (fig. S1C). This enhanced avoidance response was also spe-cific to the pathogen in that animals responded to the pathogen whoseodorants they had previously experienced. Pre-exposure of animals tothe odor of PA14 did not trigger avoidance of another known C. eleganspathogen, Serratia marcescens strain DB11. Animals pre-exposed toPA14 or OP50 odorants behaved like naïve animals and remainedon DB11 throughout the analysis (fig. S1D). These data together pointto the existence of sophisticated mechanisms by which C. elegans dis-criminate between bacteria in their environment and show that priorexposure to odorants generated by a pathogen such as P. aeruginosa caninduce C. elegans to accelerate their avoidance of that specific pathogenupon subsequent encounter.

The HSF-1–dependent expression of HSP genes is enhancedby prior olfactory exposure to PA14 odorantsExposure to PA14 is toxic to C. elegans, causing increased proteindamage (29, 30) and ultimately death (31, 32). Consistent with this, sur-vival on PA14 was dependent on HSF-1. Knocking down hsf-1 by RNAinterference using standard methods for feeding double-strandedRNA (dsRNA) to C. elegans accelerated death upon PA14 exposure(Fig. 1C, table S1, and fig. S4A). To assess whether training on PA14odorants modulated the HSF-1–mediated transcriptional response, weplaced animals exposed to OP50 odorants or PA14 odorants on PA14lawns that covered the entire surface area that was available to the ani-mals. Under these conditions, HSF-1 was activated: All animals placedon PA14 lawns for only 10 min increased HSF-1–dependent expres-sion of one or both of the identical inducible hsp70 genes (F44E5.4and F44E5.5) and the small HSP genes (hsp-16.2 and hsp-16.41), asmeasured by qRT-PCR (Fig. 1, D to F, and fig. S1E). Pre-exposure tothe odor of PA14, however, enhanced this HSF-1–dependent transcrip-tional response (Fig. 1, D to F, and table S2). The amounts of all thesechaperone mRNAs were about twofold higher in animals that werepre-exposed to the odor of PA14 compared to control animals pre-exposed to the smell of OP50 (Fig. 1, D to F, and table S2). This suggestedthat HSF-1–mediated gene expression could be enhanced by prior ex-perience of signals that were predictive of danger. Pre-exposure to theodor of PA14 did not in itself induce expression of chaperone genes;animals exposed to the odor of PA14 had low basal chaperone expres-sion similar to control animals (Fig. 1, D to F, and table S2).

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

P. aeruginosa releases several molecules that alter the behavior ofother organisms. The “grape-like” odorant 2-aminoacetophenone(2AA) is one such compound synthesized relatively early in the growthcycle and is enriched when P. aeruginosa infects animal tissues, suchas the wounds of human burn victims or the lungs of patients withcystic fibrosis (33, 34). 2AA is responsible for the attractive behaviorof Drosophila melanogaster toward the pathogen (33, 35) as well asthe aversive behavior of vertebrate species such as birds and mice fromPseudomonas (36, 37). We tested whether this compound was, at leastin part, responsible for the learned enhanced aversion of C. elegansto PA14. Pre-exposure to 2AA mimicked the results observed in ourchoice assay (Fig. 2A), but 2AA did not in itself elicit an avoidanceresponse (fig. S2A). Animals that were pre-exposed to the smell of 2AAavoided PA14 lawns by 15 min compared to mock-trained, control ani-mals exposed to water as the odorant, who only began to avoid PA14lawns by 45 min (Fig. 2A). Pre-exposure to 2AA odorant also enhancedthe expression of hsp70 when animals were subsequently exposed toPA14 lawns, but the 2AA odorant itself did not induce hsp70 expres-sion (Fig. 2B and table S2). Moreover, the avoidance of 2AA did notappear to be due to its potential toxicity, and prolonged 2AA exposurehad no effect on the life span of animals, be it administered as an odoralone (Fig. 2C and table S3) or mixed into OP50 for direct contact andingestion (Fig. 2D and table S3). The enhancement of PA14-avoidancebehavior by 2AA also appeared to be somewhat specific, because pre-exposure to another volatile semiochemical secreted by Pseudomonas,N-3-oxododecanoyl homoserine lactone (38), did not affect subse-quent avoidance of PA14 lawns or enhance hsp gene expression (fig.S2, B and C, and table S2). Consistent with a role in signaling a poten-tial threat, pre-exposure to 2AA appeared to facilitate a mechanism bywhich animals activated HSF-1 only if they subsequently encounteredPA14. When C. elegans that were pre-exposed to 2AA odorant en-countered an OP50 lawn instead of a PA14 lawn, they did not activateHSF-1–dependent hsp gene expression (Fig. 2B and table S2). However,if animals did encounter PA14, pre-exposure to PA14 odorants con-ferred a consistent and significant survival advantage: 63% of theanimals pre-exposed to PA14 odor survived after 18 hours of PA14exposure, compared to 46% of control animals exposed to OP50 odor(Fig. 2E and table S4). The protection conferred by pre-exposure toPA14 was also stressor-specific, enhancing survival on PA14 but notupon prolonged heat stress (fig. S2D). These data together suggest thatthe prior experience of PA14 odor, mimicked in large part by theodorant 2AA, enhanced the organism’s ability to survive, not only byhastening the avoidance behavior of the animal from the pathogen butalso by enhancing the expression of cytoprotective HSF-1 transcrip-tional targets upon encounter with the pathogen itself. 2AA itself wasnot toxic or aversive but instead appeared to convey specific informa-tion regarding the bacterial environment that prepared C. elegans forsurvival on the pathogen and conferred in some unknown way a de-gree of specificity to the HSF-1 transcriptional response.

Serotonin is required for olfactory training toenhance HSF-1–dependent hsp gene expressionupon exposure to PA14In C. elegans and other organisms, the neuromodulator 5-HT mediateslearning (15, 21, 22, 26, 39–42). We therefore tested whether theenhanced behavioral and transcriptional response to PA14 that oc-curred after the pre-exposure of C. elegans to PA14-derived odors re-quired 5-HT. Compared to the 5 min needed for wild-type animalstrained on PA14 odor to avoid PA14 lawns, tph-1mutant animals that

3 of 17

Page 4: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

lack functional tryptophan hydroxylase (43), the rate-limiting enzymefor 5-HT synthesis, took 1 hour to avoid PA14 after pre-exposure tothe odor of PA14 (Fig. 3A and fig. S3A). This delay in avoidance wasdue to the lack of 5-HT, because even transient incubation with exog-enous 5-HT before training, which causes 5-HT to be loaded into se-rotonergic neurons (fig. S3B) (42), rescued the deficiency in the learnedresponse of tph-1 mutant animals. tph-1 mutant animals treated with5-HT and trained on PA14 odor acted like wild-type animals trainedon PA14 odor and avoided PA14 lawns within 5 min (Fig. 3B and fig.S3C, compare to Fig. 1A). 5-HT was not required for the detection or

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

the attractiveness of PA14, because tph-1 mutants were also firstattracted to, then subsequently avoided PA14 lawns. However, for rea-sons we do not understand, but which may be related to the role of5-HT in providing both excitatory and inhibitory inputs to modulateolfactory behavior (44, 45), control, mock-trained tph-1 animals lack-ing 5-HT avoided PA14 lawns earlier than wild-type control animals(fig. S3A, compare to Fig. 1A). This aberrant behavior of mock-trainedtph-1 animals was reversed with exogenous 5-HT treatment: 5-HT–treated tph-1 animals mock-trained on OP50 odor acted more likewild-type animals exposed to OP50 odor and avoided PA14 lawns

5' 15' 30' 45' 1 h 1.5 h 2 h 2.5 h 3 h 3.5 h 4 h–1.0

–0.8

–0.6

–0.4

–0.2

0.0

0.2

0.4

Time

Ch

oic

e i

nd

ex

fo

r P

A1

4

H2O odor (OP50 versus PA14)2AA odor (OP50 versus PA14)

*

*****

*****

**

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

35

Fo

ld c

ha

ng

e o

f h

sp

-7

0

(F4

4E

5.4

/F4

4E

5.5

) m

RN

A

H2O2AA

***

Odor only Odor +PA14 lawn

Odor +OP50 lawn

n.s.

0 5 10 15 20 250

20

40

60

80

100

120

Time (days)

Pe

rce

nt

su

rviv

al

H2O odor2AA odor

P = 0.1594

0 5 10 15 20 250

20

40

60

80

100

120

Time (days)

Pe

rce

nt

su

rviv

al

H2O ingestion2AA ingestion

P = 0.1214

or or

0 14 16 18 20 22 240

20

40

60

80

100

120

Time (hours)

Pe

rce

nt

su

rviv

al

OP50 odorPA14 odor

P = 0.000002

A B

C D

E Fig. 2. 2AA enhances olfactory avoidance behavior and HSF-1 activation. (A) CI ofwild-type animals for PA14 after pre-exposure to the odor of either water or 2AA andthen offered the choice between OP50 and PA14 lawns. Preference was recorded at thetimes indicated on the x axis. n = 10 experiments of 30 animals per condition. Student’st test, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, and ***P < 0.001. (B) hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) mRNA abun-dance measured by qRT-PCR in wild-type animals that were pre-exposed to the odor ofwater or 2AA and subsequently placed on a PA14 or OP50 lawn. Values are relative toanimals pre-exposed to water. n = 3 to 21 experiments of 30 animals per condition. Pair-wise mean comparison from linear mixed model analysis, ***P < 0.001. (C and D) Life-span curves of animals (C) continuously exposed to water (control) or 2AA odor, or (D) inphysical contact with water-treated (control) or 2AA-treated OP50. n = 3 experiments of50 animals per condition. Log-rank tests indicated no significant differences betweenexperimental groups. (E) Survival of wild-type animals on PA14 after pre-exposure toOP50 or PA14 odor. n = 8 experiments of 50 animals per condition. Log-rank test, P <0.001. Also see table S4. Data in (A) and (B) represent means ± SEM, and data in (C) to (E)represent total animals across all experiments.

4 of 17

Page 5: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

later, by 45 min (fig. S3, C and D). However, this rescue was variableand did not reach significance.

Consistent with the behavioral response, tph-1 mutant animalswere deficient in the enhanced HSF-1–dependent transcriptional re-sponse elicited by pre-exposure to PA14 odor (Fig. 3C and table S2),although 5-HT was not required for HSF-1 activation on PA14 per se.We inferred this from the observation that tph-1 mutant animals didinduce hsp70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) expression when exposed to PA14lawns as assessed by qRT-PCR (Fig. 3C and table S2). However, tph-1mutant animals pre-exposed to OP50 or PA14 odors both expressedsimilar amounts of hsp70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) mRNA upon subsequentencounter with PA14 lawns, and there was no increase in hsp70 ex-pression on the basis of prior olfactory experience (Fig. 3C and tableS2). We tested whether treatment with exogenous 5-HT could alsoreverse this defect. However, consistent with what we had previouslyobserved upon optogenetic activation of serotonergic neurons (7), ex-posure to exogenous 5-HT induced the expression of hsp70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) mRNA even without pre-exposure to PA14 odor (Fig. 3D).Although these data confirm the role of 5-HT in triggering HSF-1 activity,

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

such data suggest that the fine control over HSF-1–mediated gene expres-sion that occurs in the animal in response to physiological stimuli may bedue to a more stringent regulation of 5-HT release and availability.

Because 5-HT is synthesized only in neurons in C. elegans whereas5-HT receptors are expressed in multiple tissues (39, 43), we testedwhether 5-HT–dependent HSF-1 activation was restricted to neuronsor whether it occurred throughout the animal. To do this, we usedsingle-molecule fluorescence in situ hybridization (smFISH) to detecthsp70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) mRNA across the entire organism. smFISHindicated that exposure to PA14 induced F44E5.4/F44E5.5 mRNA inall tissue types including neurons, the intestine, and the germ line,and mRNA expression was enhanced in all these tissues in wild-typeanimals trained on PA14 odor (Fig. 4, A to F). Consistent with the whole-animal qRT-PCR data, tph-1 mutant animals also induced hsp70(F44E5.4/F44E5.5) mRNA when exposed to PA14, but the inductionof mRNA in tph-1 mutant animals remained the same irrespective ofprior olfactory training and was also similar to that in control, wild-type animals mock-trained with OP50 (Fig. 4, A to F). Together, thesestudies showed that both enhanced avoidance behavior and enhanced

5' 15' 30' 45' 1 h 1.5 h 2 h 2.5 h 3 h 3.5 h 4 h–1.0

–0.8

–0.6

–0.4

–0.2

0.0

0.2

0.4

Time

Ch

oic

e i

nd

ex

fo

r P

A1

4Wild type; PA14 odor (OP50 versus PA14)tph-1(mg280)II; PA14 odor (OP50 versus PA14)

* * **

5' 15' 30' 45' 1 h 1.5 h 2 h 2.5 h 3 h 3.5 h 4 h–1.0

–0.8

–0.6

–0.4

–0.2

0.0

0.2

0.4

Time

Ch

oic

e i

nd

ex

fo

r P

A1

4

tph-1(mg280)II; PA14 odor (OP50 versus PA14)tph-1(mg280)II + 5HT; PA14 odor (OP50 versus PA14)

* ***

**

***

* *

**

***

**

0

10

20

30

40

50

Fo

ld c

ha

ng

e o

f h

sp

-7

0

(F4

4E

5.4

/F4

4E

5.5

) m

RN

A

Wild typetph-1(mg280)II

**

OP50 OP50PA14 PA14Odor only Odor + PA14 lawn

n.s.

0

1

2

3

4

5

Fo

ld c

ha

ng

e o

f h

sp

-7

0

(F4

4E

5.4

/F4

4E

5.5

) m

RN

A

*

H2Otreatment

5-HTtreatment

A B

C D

Fig. 3. Serotonin is required for HSF-1 activation in response to olfactory learning. (A) CI for PA14 of wild-type and tph-1(mg280)II animals pre-exposed to the odor ofPA14 before being offered the choice between OP50 and PA14 lawns. Preference was recorded at the times indicated on the x axis. n = 3 to 4 experiments of 30 animals percondition. Student’s two-sample t test (unequal variance), *P < 0.05. (B) CI for PA14 of tph-1(mg280)II animals pre-exposed to the odor of PA14 compared to the choice indicesin tph-1(mg280)II animals treated with exogenous 5-HT and pre-exposed to PA14 odor before being offered the choice between OP50 and PA14 lawns. n = 4 experiments of30 animals per condition. Student’s paired t test, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, and ***P < 0.001. (C) hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) mRNA abundance measured by qRT-PCR upon PA14exposure in wild-type and tph-1(mg280)II animals pre-exposed to the odor of OP50 or PA14. Values are relative to wild-type animals pre-exposed to the odor of OP50. n =9 experiments of 30 animals per condition. Pairwise mean comparison from linear mixed model analysis, **P < 0.01 for wild type (odor + PA14 lawn). No significance fortph-1(mg280)II (odor + PA14 lawn). (D) hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) mRNA abundance measured by qRT-PCR after exposure of animals to exogenous 5-HT. Values arerelative to control water-treated animals. n = 7 experiments of 20 to 30 animals per condition. Student’s paired t test, *P < 0.05.

5 of 17

Page 6: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Wild type tph-1(mg280)II

OP

50 o

dor

+ P

A14

law

nP

A14

odo

r+

PA

14 la

wn

DAPIF44E5.4/F44E5.5

0

5

10

15

Nu

mb

er

of

hs

p-7

0

(F4

4E

5.4

/F4

4E

5.5

) fo

ci

Wild type; headtph-1(mg280)II; head

*

OP50 OP50PA14 PA14Odor only Odor + PA14 lawn

*

n.s.

Wild type tph-1(mg280)II

OP

50 o

dor

+ P

A14

law

nP

A14

odo

r+

PA

14 la

wn

DAPIF44E5.4/F44E5.5

0

5

10

15

20

25

Nu

mb

er

of

hs

p-7

0

(F4

4E

5.4

/F4

4E

5.5

) fo

ci

Wild type; intestinetph-1(mg280)II; intestine

*

OP50 OP50PA14 PA14Odor only Odor + PA14 lawn

n.s.

n.s.

DAPIF44E5.4/F44E5.5

Wild type tph-1(mg280)II

OP

50 o

dor

+ P

A14

law

nP

A14

odo

r+

PA

14 la

wn

DAPIF44E5.4/F44E5.5

0

5

10

15

20

25

Nu

mb

er

of

hs

p-7

0

(F4

4E

5.4

/F4

4E

5.5

) fo

ci

Wild type; germlinetph-1(mg280)II; germline

*

OP50 OP50PA14 PA14Odor only Odor + PA14 lawn

n.s.

n.s.

DAPIF44E5.4/F44E5.5

A D

B E

C F

Fig. 4. Serotonin-mediated learning activates HSF-1 throughout the animal. (A to C) smFISH confocal micrographs showing hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) mRNA and4′,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole (DAPI) in wild-type and tph-1(mg280)II animals pre-exposed to OP50 or PA14 odor and subsequently placed on a PA14 lawn. Images areprojected z-stack images of 10-mm sections across the (A) head, (B) intestine, and (C) germ line. Arrowheads indicate hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) mRNA foci. Scale bars, 10 mm.(D to F) Quantification of the number of hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) foci in projected images. n = 8 to 11 animals per tissue per genotype per condition, quantified from twoto three independent experiments. Student’s paired t test, *P < 0.05 for wild type (OP50 odor + PA14 lawn) compared to (PA14 odor + PA14 lawn). No significance fortph-1(mg280)II (OP50 odor + PA14 lawn) compared to (PA14 odor + PA14 lawn). Data represent means ± SEM.

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017 6 of 17

Page 7: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

HSF-1–dependent chaperone gene expression were mediated by the5-HT learning circuitry.

Olfactory training and serotonin cause HSF-1 to localizeto nuclear bodies and prime HSF-1–dependentgene expressionHow might olfactory learning enhance HSF-1 transcriptional activity?To answer this, we examined whether olfactory training modified anyof the steps known to accompany HSF-1 activation. HSF1-dependenttranscription of hsp genes is a multistep process that varies somewhatbetween species (1, 4, 46–51). In mammalian cells, HSF1-dependenthsp expression involves the conversion of HSF1 monomers to trimers,increased phosphorylation and other posttranslational modifications,acquisition of competence to bind heat shock elements (HSEs) in thepromoters of hsp genes, and recruitment of HSF1 to HSEs in a man-ner that depends on the chromatin landscape and transcriptional ma-chinery. We characterized these steps of HSF-1 activation in C. elegans(figs. S4 and S5, A and B). Consistent with its essential role in devel-opment (52), C. elegans HSF-1, as detected by an antibody specific forendogenous C. elegansHSF-1 (fig. S4, A and B) and by the localization ofgreen fluorescent protein (GFP)–tagged HSF-1, is constitutively presentin nuclei (fig. S4C) (7, 51, 53), is likely phosphorylated (fig. S4D), andappears to be present as a trimer (fig. S4E) even at ambient tempera-tures. Electrophoretic mobility shift assays (EMSAs) indicated that inaccordance with trimerization at ambient temperatures, C. elegansHSF-1can bind DNA containing canonical C. elegans HSEs in vitro (fig. S5,A and B). The ability of C. elegansHSF-1 to bind HSE-containing DNAin vitro does not change with stress-induced transcriptional activation(fig. S5, A and B). However, in agreement with the lack of expression ofinducible hsp genes at ambient temperatures in the absence of stress,C. elegans HSF-1 did not constitutively bind the hsp70 promoter regionin vivo as assayed by chromatin immunoprecipitation and qPCR (ChIP-qPCR) (fig. S5C). Instead, HSF-1 binding to the hsp70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5)promoter in vivo required a stressor such as heat shock, which causedtranscriptional activation and an about fivefold enrichment of HSF-1at the hsp70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) locus (fig. S5C).

Olfactory pre-exposure to PA14 odor or the PA14 odorant 2AA didnot enhance the ability of HSF-1 to bind DNA in vitro as determinedby EMSA (fig. S5, A and B), nor did it cause HSF-1 to bind hsp70 pro-moter regions in vivo as indicated by ChIP-qPCR (fig. S5C). HSF-1appeared to be phosphorylated upon exposure of animals to PA14lawns, as visualized by its retarded mobility in SDS–polyacrylamidegel electrophoresis (PAGE); however, pre-exposure to 2AA did not in-duce this posttranslational change, and HSF-1 in both control (watertrained) and 2AA-trained animals appeared identical by SDS-PAGEanalysis (fig. S5D). Exposure to 2AA odor or PA14 odor alone, how-ever, without exposing the animals to a lawn of PA14, caused a signif-icant fraction of HSF-1 to relocalize to punctate nuclear bodies (Fig. 5,A and B). This was similar to changes in HSF-1 localization that occurwhen HSF-1 is actively transcribing hsp genes in response to stressorssuch as increased temperatures (fig. S4C) (7, 51, 53) and PA14 lawns(Fig. 5A). Therefore, the formation of nuclear bodies upon 2AA expo-sure alone was surprising, because exposure to 2AA odor did not in-duce the transcription of hsp genes. The number of nuclei containingHSF-1 nuclear bodies after exposure of animals to 2AA averaged 8.7%,ranging from 3 to 42% among the germline nuclei, where it was easiestto visualize HSF-1 and was visible in 71% of animals scored (Fig. 5, Aand B). In comparison, only an average of 2.2% of germline nucleiranging between 0 and 10% (Fig. 5, A and B) of control animals ex-

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

posed to the odor of water showed any evidence of HSF-1 nuclearbodies. The relocalization of HSF-1 into nuclear bodies was reversible,and the numbers of nuclei with HSF-1 nuclear bodies in animals ex-posed to PA14 odorants diminished to control values (3.0%) after30-min recovery on OP50 and did not differ from that in control water-exposed animals (3.2%; Fig. 5, A and B).

The HSF-1–mediated transcriptional memory of pre-exposure toPA14 odors that resulted in enhanced HSF-1–dependent hsp gene ex-pression correlated with the presence of HSF-1 nuclear bodies (Fig. 5,A and C). Whereas animals exposed to the PA14 odorant showed thepresence of HSF-1 nuclear bodies and displayed enhanced expressionof hsp genes when placed on PA14 lawns, animals that were allowedto recover for 30 min on innocuous OP50 lawns after being trained onPA14 odorants no longer displayed enhanced hsp gene expression whenplaced on PA14 lawns (Fig. 5, A and C; compare with Figs. 1D and 2B).In further support of the role of HSF-1 nuclear bodies in the learning-dependent enhancement of HSF-1 transcription, tph-1 mutant animalsthat lacked 5-HT and were deficient in olfactory experience–mediatedincrease in hsp gene expression also had markedly fewer HSF-1 nuclearbodies upon olfactory training (Fig. 5, D and E). Together, these dataindicate that the priming of HSF-1 upon olfactory exposure to PA14odorants, which resulted in an enhancement of hsp gene expressionupon encounter with PA14 lawns, occurred through the mobilizationof HSF-1 to nuclear bodies throughout cells of the animal.

HSF-1 nuclear bodies colocalize with RNA pol IITranscription does not occur homogeneously throughout the nucleusbut rather occurs at specialized, discrete sites (54). We hypothesizedthat because animals that were trained on PA14-derived odors did notinduce hsp70 gene expression unless they encountered PA14 lawns,olfactory signaling may facilitate the association of HSF-1 with thetranscriptional machinery, thus preparing the animal for a transcrip-tional response if an actual encounter with the threat (PA14) were tooccur.We therefore investigated whether the location of HSF-1 nuclearbodies corresponded to known sites of enriched transcriptional activ-ity. In C. elegans, the hsp-16.2 promoter localizes to the nuclear porecomplex (NPC) after heat shock (54, 55). However, although we oc-casionally observed HSF-1 nuclear bodies in the vicinity of NPCs ingermline nuclei, they did not colocalize with NPCs under any of ourconditions (Fig. 6, A to C). On the other hand, more than half of theHSF-1 nuclear bodies (0.2 of 0.3 nuclear bodies per nucleus) that wereinduced by olfactory exposure to 2AA colocalized with RNA pol II(Fig. 6, D to F). The number of HSF-1 nuclear bodies that colocalizedwith pol II remained the same even when HSF-1 was actively involvedin pol II–dependent transcription of hsp genes, such as upon heatshock or when animals were exposed to PA14 lawns (Fig. 6, D to F).In comparison, few of the rare HSF-1 nuclear bodies visible in controlanimals colocalized with pol II (Fig. 6, D to F). Consistent with pre-vious reports (46, 56–58), pol II appeared to cluster in discrete nuclearregions even before 2AA exposure or heat shock (Fig. 6, D to F). Theformation of HSF-1 nuclear bodies, however, did not appear to requirepol II: RNA interference (RNAi)–induced knockdown of the largesubunit of RNA pol II (AMA-1) substantially decreased the amountsof pol II protein in oocyte nuclei (fig. S6A) but did not interfere with theheat shock–induced formation of HSF-1 nuclear bodies in oocytes (fig.S6B). We conclude from these studies that olfactory training with PA14odorants primed HSF-1 for promoting transcription by preemptivelyconcentrating it at nuclear loci in close proximity to RNA pol II. Al-though we do not yet understand the nature of these nuclear foci where

7 of 17

Page 8: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

HSF-1 and pol II were concentrated or the intracellular mechanisms bywhich this occurred, together, these data suggest that the ability of theserotonin-based learning circuitry to induce the colocalization of HSF-1with pol II in nuclei throughout the animal in response to only the odorof the pathogen could result in an enhanced transcriptional responseupon actual exposure to the pathogen (46, 47, 55–58).

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

HSF-1 is required for the learned avoidance behaviorof C. elegans toward PA14Not only was HSF-1 activity enhanced by aversive olfactory stimuli,HSF-1 appeared to be required for the behavioral avoidance of PA14.This was evidenced in choice assays where animals subject to RNAi-induced knockdown of hsf-1 were pre-exposed to the odor of PA14

Control

HSF-1::GFP

PA14 lawn

2AA odorH2O odor

H2O odor + recovery 2AA odor + recovery

Odor only Odor + PA14 lawn0

2

4

6

8

Fo

ld c

han

ge o

f h

sp

-70

(F44E

5.4

/F44E

5.5

) m

RN

A

H2O2AA

n.s.

+ 30-min recovery

Odor only Odor + 30-min recovery0

2

4

6

8

10

12

% n

ucle

i w

ith

HS

F-1

::G

FP

nu

cle

ar

bo

die

s

H2O2AA**

n.s.

2AA odorH2O odor

Wild type

tph-1(mg280)II

HSF-1

H2O odor 2AA odor0.0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

Nu

mb

er

of

HS

F-1

nu

cle

ar

bo

die

s

p

er

nu

cle

us

Wild typetph-1(mg280)II

*

*

A B

C

D E

Fig. 5. Olfactory learning primes HSF-1 through the formation of HSF-1 nuclear bodies. (A) HSF-1::GFP localization in germline nuclei of control animals atambient temperature, animals on a PA14 lawn, animals exposed to water or 2AA odor, and animals after 30 min of recovery after exposure to water or 2AA odor.Arrowheads indicate HSF-1 nuclear bodies. Scale bar, 5 mm. (B) Quantification of HSF-1 nuclear bodies under all conditions listed in (A). n = 33 to 39 nuclei per animal,17 to 25 animals per condition, across three to four independent experiments. Student’s two-sample t test (unequal variance), **P < 0.01 for odor only. No significancewas detected for (odor + 30-min recovery). (C) hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) mRNA abundance measured by qRT-PCR after exposure of animals to the odor of water or 2AAand allowed to recover for 30 min on OP50 lawn before being placed on PA14 lawns. n = 5 to 9 experiments of 30 animals per experiment. Pairwise mean comparisonfrom linear mixed model analysis. No significance was detected. (D) Confocal micrographs (individual z-sections) showing HSF-1 localization in germline nuclei of wild-type and tph-1(mg280)II animals exposed to water odor or exposed to 2AA odor alone before being placed on a PA14 lawn. Scale bar, 5 mm. (E) Quantification of HSF-1 nuclear bodies under all conditions shown in (D). n = 50 to 55 nuclei per animal, 8 to 18 animals per condition, across two to three independent experiments.Student’s two-sample t test (unequal variance), *P < 0.05.

8 of 17

Page 9: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

H2O odor 2AA odor Heat shock2AA odor + PA14 lawnH

2O odor + PA14 lawn

HS

F-1

::G

FP

Po

l II

DA

PI

2AA odorH2O odor

HS

F-1

NP

CD

AP

I

Heat shock

H2O odor 2AA odor Heat shock

0.0

0.2

0.4

0.6

Nu

mb

er

of

nu

cle

ar

bo

die

s

pe

r n

uc

leu

s

HSF-1 only

*

H2O odor 2AA odor Heat shock

–0.01

0.00

0.01

0.02

0.03

0.04

Nu

mb

er

of

nu

cle

ar

bo

die

s

pe

r n

uc

leu

s

HSF-1/NPCcolocalization

n.s.

0.0

0.5

1.0

1.5

Nu

mb

er

of

nu

cle

ar

bo

die

s

pe

r n

uc

leu

s

HSF-1 only

H2O 2AAOdor only Odor +

PA14 lawn

H2O 2AA Heatshock

*

*

n.s.

0.0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

Nu

mb

er

of

nu

cle

ar

bo

die

s

pe

r n

uc

leu

s

HSF-1/RNA pol IIcolocalization

H2O 2AAOdor only Odor +

PA14 lawn

H2O 2AA Heatshock

*

*n.s.

A B

C

D

E F

Fig. 6. Olfactory learning primes HSF-1 by increasing its association with RNA pol II. (A) Immunofluorescence confocal micrographs (individual z-sections)showing HSF-1, NPCs, and DAPI in germline nuclei of wild-type animals exposed to water odor, 2AA odor, or heat shock. Scale bar, 5 mm. (B) Quantification of numbersof total HSF-1 nuclear bodies per nucleus and (C) HSF-1 nuclear bodies per nucleus that colocalize with NPCs. n = 30 to 36 nuclei per animal, four to six animals percondition per experiment, two independent experiments. (B) Student’s two-sample t test (unequal variance), *P < 0.05 when water odor is compared to heat shock. (C)No significance between all conditions. (D) Immunofluorescence confocal micrographs (individual z-sections) of HSF-1, RNA pol II, and DAPI in germline nuclei ofdissected animals expressing HSF-1::GFP after exposure to water odor, 2AA odor, water odor + PA14 lawn, 2AA odor + PA14 lawn, or heat shock. Scale bar, 5 mm.(E and F) Quantification of numbers of (E) total HSF-1::GFP nuclear bodies per nucleus and (F) HSF-1 nuclear bodies per nucleus that colocalize with RNA pol II in (D). n =number of nuclear bodies per nucleus in 68 to 82 nuclei per animal, 8 to 12 animals per condition per experiment, three independent experiments. Student’s two-sample t test (unequal variance), *P < 0.05. (B, D, and F). Data represent means ± SEM.

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017 9 of 17

Page 10: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

and assessed for their behavioral avoidance of PA14 lawns (Fig. 7Aand fig. S7A). All animals for these experiments were grown on astrain of dsRNA-expressing bacteria used for inducing the knockdownof genes in C. elegans rather than on OP50. For this reason, the odorof control RNAi–expressing bacteria was used as the control to trainanimals, instead of the odor of OP50, and animals were given a choicebetween lawns of control RNAi–expressing bacteria and PA14. De-creasing the amounts of hsf-1 mRNA and protein abrogated the be-havioral plasticity observed upon exposure to PA14, and animals deficientfor hsf-1 remained equally distributed between the PA14 and lawns ofcontrol RNAi–expressing bacteria, indicating a deficiency in theiravoidance of PA14 (Fig. 7A and fig. S7A). Loss of hsf-1 slightly re-tarded motility, causing a delay of ~102 s for hsf-1 RNAi–treated

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

animals to traverse the 1-inch distance between the PA14 and OP50lawns as compared to wild-type animals (fig. S7B). However, this slightdecrease in motility rates could not account for the lack of avoidancebehavior of hsf-1 RNAi–treated animals, because they did not avoidPA14 even by 4 hours after the start of the choice assay. By this time,all wild-type animals raised on control RNAi, whether trained on PA14odor or the odor of control RNAi–expressing bacteria, had left the PA14lawns (Fig. 7A). It therefore appeared that 5-HT signaling was integrat-ing olfactory information and HSF-1 activation to flag a sensory stim-ulus as a threat, providing a basis for the coupling of the enhancedbehavioral aversion with the enhanced transcriptional response seenin our experiments. To test if this was the case, we stimulated 5-HTrelease using optogenetic methods while simultaneously exposing

10 of 17

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Choice betweenHT115 and PA14

ATR+

HT115

ATR–

HT115

or

Control RNAi hsf-1 RNAi–1.5

–1.0

–0.5

0.0

0.5

Time: 4 hours

Ch

oic

e i

nd

ex

fo

r P

A1

4

Odor of control RNAi bacteria(Control RNAi bacteria versus PA14)PA14 odor (control RNAi bacteria versus PA14)

*

5' 15' 30' 45' 1 h 1.5 h 2 h–0.5

0.0

0.5

1.0

Time

Ch

oic

e i

nd

ex

fo

r H

T1

15

ATR–; light (HT115 versus PA14)ATR+; light (HT115 versus PA14)

* *

*

*

A B

C

D No olfaction5-HT–dependentolfactory training

OP50

HSF-1 RNApol II

PA14 PA14

hsp-70 hsp-70

Avoidance behavior

Fig. 7. 5-HT signaling couples olfactory information with HSF-1 activation tomark sensory stimuli as threats. (A) CI for PA14 of wild-type animals subjected tocontrol RNAi (empty vector) or hsf-1 RNAi after being exposed to the odor of PA14 orto the odor of the control RNAi–expressing bacteria. Preference was recorded 4 hoursafter animals were exposed to the corresponding odors and placed on the choiceplates. n = 5 to 6 experiments of 30 animals per condition. Student’s two-samplet test (unequal variance), *P < 0.05. Data represent means ± SEM. (B) Schematic ofolfactory pre-exposure to HT115 odor in conjunction with optogenetic excitation ofserotonergic neurons followed by behavioral choice assay. ATR+ indicates the pres-ence of all-trans-retinal (ATR), which is required for the light-induced excitation ofchannelrhodopsin in the serotonergic neurons and subsequent release of 5-HT.ATR− indicates control, mock-excited animals. (C) CI for HT115 in animals ± ATR

after optogenetic excitation of serotonergic neurons. The choice offered was between HT115 and PA14 lawns. n = 6 experiments in triplicate of 10 animals percondition. Student’s paired t test, *P < 0.05. Data represent means ± SEM. (D) Model: 5-HT–dependent olfactory learning facilitates the association between RNApol II and HSF-1, resulting in enhanced avoidance behavior as well as enhanced transcription of HSF-1 targets in a stressor-specific manner.
Page 11: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

animals to the odor of the attractive E. coli HT115 (figs. S1C andS7C). We predicted that although HT115 does not activate HSF-1 orevoke an avoidance response on its own, optogenetically exciting se-rotonergic neurons so as to activate HSF-1 (7, 59) in the presence ofHT115 odor may change the valence of HT115 from that of attractionto one of aversion. Animals that were stimulated to increase serotoninrelease while experiencing the odor of HT115 now avoided HT115when given a choice between HT115 and PA14 (Fig. 7, B and C). Thisaversion was transient but lasted for as long as 45 min. Control ani-mals that were mock-stimulated by light did not change their be-havior and, as expected, were attracted to HT115 and repelled by PA14(Fig. 7, B and C). Thus, inducing 5-HT release during an olfactorystimulus was sufficient to associate olfactory information regardingodor with HSF-1 activation and trigger an aversive response ofC. elegans to danger.

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

DISCUSSIONIn summary, our data provide a mechanism whereby 5-HT–dependentlearning and HSF-1 activation are coupled to elicit behavioral avoidanceand transcription of cytoprotective chaperone genes under threat, thusenhancing the survival of the animal (Fig. 7D). Our data suggest that5-HT release from neurons needs to be reinforced by HSF-1 activationthroughout the animal to interpret a signal as aversive. Conversely,HSF-1 itself is activated by 5-HT release in a multistep process. Whetherthe cellular relocalization of HSF-1 in some way mediates learning, orwhether learning produces the HSF-1 relocalization is an intriguingquestion and remains to be answered. In our experiments, we showin some detail the interaction between neurosensory experience andHSF-1 in response to the odorants of the toxic bacterium Pseudomonas.However, similar responses could underlie the reaction of C. elegans toother stressors. The neuroethological significance of the response ofC. elegans to 2AA seen in our studies is unknown. Our data suggest that2AA acts as a kairomone (60), an interspecies chemical messenger thatappears to benefit the recipient more than it does the emitter. C. elegansis a bacterivore and, like related parasitic nematode species, relies onchemical cues to interpret the hostility or hospitality of its environment.However, the observation that 2AA alone does not elicit an aversiveresponse suggests that 2AA does not act as a danger pheromone. In-stead, we speculate that 2AA is akin to what a loud noise may signify toa human—a reason for investigation, to be coupled with an avoidanceresponse if confirmed to be associated with danger. 2AA is also se-creted by other known pathogens of C. elegans such as Burkholderiasp. and predators such as arthropods (61–63), perhaps accounting forthe ability of C. elegans to detect these dangers and to effectively mod-ulate its behavior and stress responsiveness accordingly.

Our data also suggest that neuronal control over HSF-1–dependenttranscription of chaperone genes in C. elegans is at least a two-step pro-cess. The first step, the reversible and anticipatory change in nuclearlocalization of HSF-1, which is mediated by neurons and 5-HT, pre-emptively promotes HSF-1 concentration at nuclear regions nearRNA pol II. This could conceivably prepare the chromatin and tran-scriptional machinery for transcription, were the stressor to materi-alize. Encounter with the actual stressor then enhances chaperonegene expression, potentially increasing the rate or amount of hsp genetranscription (Fig. 7D) (46, 47, 55–58). This subsequent, as-yet un-known signal “confirming” the threat appears to be regulated indepen-dently, is required for the actual transcription of hsp genes (Fig. 7D),and, we hypothesize, confers the specificity of the transcriptional re-

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

sponse to the stressor. The exact mechanism by which 5-HT–dependentlearning induces HSF-1 to organize into nuclear bodies and the natureof these structures and the genomic regions with which they are asso-ciated (64, 65) remain to be investigated. In Drosophila and mamma-lian cells, a fraction of RNA pol II is held paused at hsp loci until HSF-1binding initiates transcription and the release of pol II into the genebody (46, 48, 57, 66, 67). However, consistent with our data, in thesecells too HSF-1 binding alone is not the determining event for therelease of pol II pausing, because HSF-1 can bind hsp70 loci withoutinducing transcription (68).

The multistep activation of a fundamental cytoprotective responseto a threat raises intriguing questions. Given its extraordinarily benefi-cial roles in conferring stress resistance, why not simply activate HSF-1in anticipation, even upon the slightest hint of stress? We believethat the answer to this may lie in findings that high chaperone geneexpression disrupts basic functions of a cell such as growth, division,and secretory functions and increases susceptibility to transformation(69–71). In fact, it has been shown that the amount of chaperoneswithin cells of a multicellular organism is not maintained in excess,suggesting that excess chaperones are detrimental (72). We thereforehypothesize that for cells within a metazoan, activation of HSF-1 needsto be tightly controlled to occur only upon confirmation of danger, soas to prevent the possible disruption of tissue homeostasis. Organismssurvive a range of environmental fluctuations and have evolved to colo-nize a vast diversity of environmental niches despite the sensitivity ofprotein-based biological processes to environmental perturbations.We believe that our data begin to address one mechanism throughwhich such adaptation could occur.

MATERIALS AND METHODSC. elegans strainsThe following C. elegans strains were used. The following strains wereobtained from the Caenorhabditis Genetics Center (CGC): BristolN2 (wild type), MT15434 tph-1 (mg280) II, and AQ2050 lite-1(ce314);ljIs102 [tph-1;;ChR2::YFP;unc-122::gfp]. Generation of AM1061 unc-119(ed9)III,rmSi1[hsf-1p (4kb)::hsf-1(minigene)::gfp::3′UTR (hsf-1)+Cbrunc-119(+)] II; hsf-1(ok600) I is described in Tatum et al. (7).

Growth conditions of C. elegans and bacteriaAll strains were grown and maintained at 20°C. Ambient temperaturewas maintained at 20° to 22°C and carefully monitored throughoutthe experimental procedures. All animals included in the experi-ments, unless stated otherwise, were 1-day-old hermaphrodites thatwere staged as L4 animals 24 to 26 hours before the start of the ex-periment. Worms were grown and maintained at low densities un-der standard conditions in standard incubators (20°C), as previouslydescribed (7). Specifically, animals were fed with E. coli OP50 obtainedfrom the CGC that were seeded onto culture plates 2 days before use,and stock strains were maintained at low densities by passaging 8 or10 L4s onto nematode growth media (NGM) plates and, 4 days later,picking L4 animals onto fresh plates for experiments. The NGM plateswere standardized by pouring 8.9 ml of liquid NGM per plate thatyielded plates with an average weight of 13.5 ± 0.2 g. Any plates thatvaried from these measurements were discarded. The P. aeruginosastrain PA14 was obtained from the Yahr Laboratory (University ofIowa), and the S. marcescens strain DB11 was obtained from the CGC.Both PA14 and DB11 lawns were kept at 25°C for 2 days before usein experiments.

11 of 17

Page 12: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

RNA interferenceRNAi experiments were conducted using the standard feeding RNAimethod (73–75). Bacterial clones expressing the control (empty vector)construct and the dsRNA targeting most of the C. elegans genes wereobtained from the Ahringer RNAi library (73) now available throughSource Bioscience. (www.sourcebioscience.com/products/life-science-research/clones/rnai-resources/c-elegans-rnai-collection-ahringer/).The RNAi clones used in experiments were sequenced for verifica-tion before use. The pL4440 empty vector was used as control RNAi.RNAi-induced knockdown was conducted by feeding animals for 24hours (ama-1) or for over one generation, where second-generationanimals were born and raised on RNAi bacterial lawns (hsf-1). RNAi-mediated knockdown was confirmed by scoring for known knock-phenotypes of the animals subject to RNAi that have been reportedin genome-wide RNAi screens in C. elegans (slow and arrested larvalgrowth as well as larval arrest at 27°C for hsf-1 RNAi and second-generation embryonic lethality in the case of the ama-1 RNAi).Knockdown was further ascertained using either Western blots (HSF-1)or immunofluorescence (AMA-1) to verify a decrease in protein levels.

Olfactory pre-exposureBacterial cultures were grown in Luria broth to OD600 (optical densityat 600 nm) values of between 1.4 and 1.7, and the variation betweencultures within an experiment was kept to ±0.1. For pre-exposure tobacterial odors, experiments were carried out in a 25°C incubator, and750 ml of culture was placed in the lid of a 35 mm × 10 mm petri dish(catalog no. 10799-192, VWR International), which was then placedin the lid of an inverted standard NGM petri dish (fig. S1A). Becausethe plates were inverted, animals crawled on OP50 lawns on “top” ofthe plates, whereas the odorant remained at the “bottom,” undisturbed,and so, at no point, did animals come in contact with the odorant.L4 animals were picked onto these NGM plates on OP50 lawns onthe previous day and remained on their respective OP50 lawns dur-ing the course of the exposure to odor. We verified that no bacterialspores were transferred via this exposure by conducting the sameprocedure with an unseeded NGM plate and observing the plates overthe course of the next 2 days for bacterial growth. For “naïve” condi-tions, the animals were not given any odor before the start of the ex-periments. When the pre-exposure was to water or 2AA (catalog no.A38207, Sigma-Aldrich), 3 ml of water or 1 mM 2AA (kept at 37°C for5 min before use) was used in place of the bacterial culture, and exper-iments were carried out at room temperature ranging from 20° to22°C. When the pre-exposure was to ethanol or N-(3-oxododecanoyl)-L-homoserine (3OC12; catalog no. O9139, Sigma-Aldrich), 3 ml of0.2% ethanol or 10 mM 3OC12 was used and experiments were carriedout at room temperature. For experiments with a recovery condition,after the 30 min of olfaction, the plate containing the liquid odorantwas removed and animals were allowed to recover at room tempera-ture for 30 min before harvesting for subsequent experiments.

Bacterial lawn choice assaysL4s were harvested 24 to 26 hours before the start of the behavioralchoice experiments. Choice plates were seeded and grown at 25°C for2 days before use. For PA14 lawns, the duration of growth on theNGM plates before the behavioral assay was particularly important.Younger lawns elicited later avoidance behaviors. Bacterial culture(85 ml; OP50, PA14, HT115, or DB11) grown to OD600 values of be-tween 1.3 and 1.6 was used for seeding each lawn. The distance be-tween the lawns was 1 inch. After olfactory training (conducted as

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

described above), 30 1-day-old adult animals were transferred tothe middle of the choice plates at a point equidistant from the mid-dle of each lawn, and the behavior of the animals was observed atsaid frequencies for the next 4 hours at room temperature. The num-ber of animals present on bacterial lawns or off bacterial lawns wasrecorded, and the experimental bacteria CI was calculated using thefollowing equation:

ð# of animals on experimental bacteriaÞ � ð# of animals on control bacteriaÞ∑½ð# of animals on experimental bacteriaÞ þ ð# of animals on control bacteriaÞ�

Animals were considered to be on a lawn as long as they were phys-ically in the lawn, be it on the edge or in the middle of the lawn at thetime of observation at a maximum of 4.0× magnification.

Chemotaxis assaysChemotaxis between water and 10 mM 2AA was carried out at roomtemperature. Water (5 ml) and 2AA (5 ml) (premixed with 0.5 M so-dium azide; catalog no. S2002, Sigma-Aldrich—such that the final con-centration of 2AA was 10 mM and that of sodium azide was 0.25 M)were dropped onto an unseeded NGM plate. The two spots were1.5 inches apart from each other. The spots were air-dried for 5 min,and then the chemotaxis assay was carried out by placing 30 day1 worms (harvested as L4s the day before) at a point on the plate equi-distant from the two spots. Worms were counted as having made theirchoice only if they were immobile at the time points at which obser-vations were made. CI for 2AA was calculated as:

ð# of animals on 2AAÞ � ð# of animals on waterÞ∑½ð# of animals on 2AAÞ þ ð# of animals on waterÞ�

Exogenous serotonin (5-HT) treatmentExogenous serotonin treatment was modified from Jafari et al. (42).A serotonin solution (catalog no. 85036, Sigma-Aldrich) in sterile wa-ter was dropped onto the surface of OP50 bacterial lawns (such thatthe lawns were fully covered in serotonin) on NGM plates and driedfor ~2 hours at room temperature before use. For confirmation of se-rotonin uptake using immunofluorescence, serotonin concentrationsbetween 2 and 20 mM were used and day 1 animals were placed ontoserotonin-treated plates for between 30 min and 2 hours. For the ex-ogenous serotonin choice assays, a 2 mM serotonin solution was used,and L4s were then picked onto these plates and kept at 20°C for 24 to26 hours before experimenting with these day 1 adults the next day.Olfactory pre-exposure was carried out using OP50 and PA14 bac-terial cultures as described above, followed by choice assays as de-scribed above.

PA14 survival assaysSurvival assays were carried out on day 1 adult worms that had beenharvested as L4s the previous day (50 worms per plate). PA14 killingwas performed in liquid bacterial culture in six-well dishes (76). OP50liquid bacterial culture was used as a control. Both PA14 and OP50bacteria were grown to an OD600 range of 0.8 to 1.0. Olfactory pre-exposure was performed as described above using OP50 and PA14,and immediately after olfaction, worms were picked into liquid bacte-rial culture in the six-well dishes. Plates were covered loosely to allowfor air circulation, kept in a 25°C incubator set to 85 rpm, and scored

12 of 17

Page 13: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

periodically for survival by visualization under a microscope. Animalswere scored as dead if they were not moving in response to gentleswirling of the media and if there was no pharyngeal pumping. Atthe end of the experiment, death was confirmed by pipetting the ani-mals onto unseeded plates and lack of revival.

Longevity assaysEach experiment was carried out on 50 day 1 adults harvested asL4s the previous day. For longevity with olfaction, animals were pre-exposed to water and 2AA as described above and then transferredonto a new OP50-seeded NGM plate. Animals were transferred every2 days to avoid starvation, until the point where they were no longercapable of reproduction, typically at day 9. The 2AA liquid and waterwere refreshed on a daily basis. For longevity with ingestion, water and1 mM 2AA were dropped onto OP50 lawns on NGM plates and al-lowed to dry for 2 hours before use. Animals were transferred every2 days until day 9, and the water- and 2AA-treated plates were madefresh on the day of use. Animals were scored as dead if they were notmoving in response to tapping of the plate or a gentle touch on theNGM adjacent to the animal. Animals that died of internal hatchingwere discarded.

Thermotolerance assaysThese assays were carried out on day 1 adult worms that had beenharvested as L4s the previous day, with 20 worms per plate. Olfactorypre-exposure was performed as described above, and immediately af-ter olfaction, worms were subjected to an extended heat treatment(45 min) in a circulating water bath preheated to 37.5°C. After this heatexposure, the animals were allowed to recover for 16 hours at 20°C andwere then scored as live or dead the following day. The lack of pharyn-geal pumping and lack of response to gentle and harsh touch were thecriteria used for scoring an animal as dead.

RNA extraction and qRT-PCRSamples for RNA were day 1 adult worms that had been harvested asL4s the previous day, with 30 worms per plate. Olfactory pre-exposurewas performed as described above, and animals were either imme-diately harvested (olfaction only) or subjected to a PA14 lawn for10 min (olfaction + lawn) and then harvested. RNA extraction wasconducted according to previously published methods (6, 7). RNAsamples were harvested in 50 ml of Trizol (catalog no. 400753, LifeTechnologies) and snap-frozen immediately in liquid nitrogen. Thefollowing steps were carried out immediately after snap-freezing orsamples were stored at −80°C. Samples were thawed on ice and 200 mlof Trizol was added, followed by brief vortexing at room temperature.Samples were then vortexed at 4°C for at least 45 min to lyse wormscompletely. RNA was then purified as detailed in the manufacturer’sprotocol with appropriate volumes of reagents modified to 250 ml ofTrizol. RNA pellet was dissolved in 17 ml of ribonuclease (RNase)–freewater. RNA was treated with deoxyribonuclease using the TURBODNA-free Kit (catalog no. AM1907, Life Technologies) as per the man-ufacturer’s protocol. Complementary DNA (cDNA) was generated byusing the iScript cDNA Synthesis Kit (catalog no. 170-8891, Bio-Rad).RT-PCR was performed using LightCycler 480 SYBR Green I MasterMix (catalog no. 04887352001, Roche), in LightCycler 480 (Roche) ata 10-ml sample volume, in a 96-well white plate (catalog no. 04729692001,Roche). The relative amounts of hspmRNA were determined using theDDCt method for quantitation. act-1, syp-1, and/or pmp-3 mRNA wasused as internal controls. The use of syp-1, which is a germ line–expressed

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

gene, controlled for the variable numbers of embryos that were inthe animals when they were prepared for mRNA extraction. All re-lative changes of hsp mRNA were normalized to either that of thewild-type control or the control for each genotype (specified in figurelegends). DDCt values were obtained in triplicate for each sample(technical triplicates). Each experiment was then repeated a mini-mum of three times. For qPCR reactions, the amplification of asingle product with no primer dimers was confirmed by melt-curveanalysis performed at the end of the reaction. No–reverse transcriptasecontrols were included to exclude any possible genomic DNA am-plification. Primers were designed using Roche’s Universal ProbeLibrary Assay Design Center software and generated by IntegratedDNA Technologies. The primers used for the PCR analysis are shownin table S5.

Single-molecule fluorescence in situ hybridizationsmFISH probes were designed against F44E5.4/5 by using the StellarisFISH Probe Designer (Biosearch Technologies Inc.) available online atwww.biosearchtech/com/stellarisdesigner. The fixed worms were hy-bridized with the F44E5.4/5 Stellaris FISH Probe set labeled withCy5 dye (Biosearch Technologies Inc.), following the manufacturer’s in-structions available online at www.biosearchtech.com/stellarisprotocols.Ten to twenty 1-day-old adult wild-type or tph-1(mg280)II worms percondition (30′OP50 olfaction, 30′ PA14 olfaction, 30′OP50 olfaction +10′ PA14 lawn, and 30′ PA14 olfaction + 10′ PA14 lawn) were harvested,washed once in 1× RNase-free phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) (catalogno. AM9624, Ambion), fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde, and subsequent-ly washed in 70% ethanol at 4°C for about 24 hours to permeabilizethe animals. Samples were washed using Stellaris Wash Buffer A (cat-alog no. SMF-WA1-60, Biosearch Technologies Inc.), and then the hy-bridization solution (catalog no. SMF-HB1-10, Biosearch TechnologiesInc.) containing the probes was added. The samples were hybridizedat 37°C for 16 hours, after which they were washed three times withWash Buffer A and then incubated for 30 min in Wash Buffer A withDAPI. After DAPI staining, worms were washed with Wash Buffer B(catalog no. SMF-WB1-20, Biosearch Technologies Inc.) and mountedon slides in about 12 ml of Vectashield mounting medium (catalog no.H-1000, Vector Laboratories). Imaging of slides was performed usinga Leica TCS SPE Confocal Microscope (Leica) using a 63× oil objec-tive. LAS AF software (Leica) was used to obtain and view z-stacks,and quantification was conducted visually by counting the number ofF44E5.4/5 puncta present in nuclei in the head, intestine, and germline of each individual worm.

Western blottingFor all Western blot analyses, animals were day 1 adults. Acute heatshock was performed by wrapping NGM plates with parafilm andsealing plates in a zippered plastic bag. Plates were submerged in acirculating water bath set to 34°C for 10 min. For protein analysis,30 day 1 adult worms were collected into 18 ml of 1× PBS (pH 7.4), andthen 4× Laemmli sample buffer (catalog no.1610737, Bio-Rad) sup-plemented with 10% b-mercaptoethanol was added to each samplebefore boiling for 30 min. Whole-worm lysates were resolved on 8%SDS-PAGE gels and transferred onto nitrocellulose membrane (cata-log no. 1620115, Bio-Rad). Immunoblots were imaged using LI-COROdyssey Infrared Imaging System (LI-COR Biotechnology). Rabbitanti-HSF1 primary antibody (catalog no. HPA008888, Sigma-Aldrich)was used to detect HSF-1, whereas the mouse anti–a-tubulin primaryantibody (AA4.3), developed by C. Walsh, was obtained from the

13 of 17

Page 14: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank (DSHB), created by theNational Institute of Child Health and Human Development of theNational Institutes of Health (NIH), and maintained at the Depart-ment of Biology, University of Iowa. The following secondary antibodieswere used: Sheep anti-Mouse IgG (H&L) Antibody IRDye 800CW Con-jugated (catalog no. 610-631-002, Rockland Immunochemicals) andAlexa Fluor 680 goat anti-rabbit IgG (H + L) (catalog no. A21109, Mo-lecular Probes, Invitrogen). The LI-COR Image Studio software wasused to quantify protein levels in different samples, relative to a-tubulinlevels. Subsequent analysis of protein levels was calculated relative towild-type controls. For Phos-tag PAGE analysis, Phos-tag reagent wasobtained from Wako Pure Chemicals Industries Ltd. and the protocolwas provided here www.wako-chem.co.jp/english/labchem/journals/phos-tag_GB2013/pdf/Phos-tag.pdf. Our experiments used 25 mM finalconcentration of Phos-tag reagent in a 6% SDS-PAGE gel. Full-lengthrecombinant C. elegansHSF-1 protein was a gift of R. Morimoto (North-western University). For ethylene glycol bis(succinimidyl succinate) (EGS)cross-linking experiments, whole-worm lysate was prepared by washingworms off the plate in lysis buffer [10 mM Hepes (pH 7.4), 130 mMNaCl, 5 mMKCl, 1 mMEDTA, and 10% glycerol] supplemented with1 mM dithiothreitol (DTT), 0.2% NP-40, and protease inhibitor cocktail(catalog no. 87785, Thermo Fisher Scientific). Worms were lysed in aPrecellys 24 homogenizer (Bertin Corp.) with VK05 beads (Bertin Corp.),and cleared lysate was incubated at room temperature with 0, 0.1, or0.5 mM EGS (catalog no. 21565, Thermo Fisher Scientific) for 30 min.4× Laemmli sample buffer supplemented with 10% b-mercaptoethanolwas added and samples were boiled briefly for 5 min to quench re-actions. Samples were then resolved on a 6% SDS-PAGE gel, andWestern blot analysis for HSF-1 was carried out as described above.

Immunofluorescent staining of whole wormsand dissected gonadsAnti-serotonin and anti-HSF1 staining was performed following the pro-tocol developed by the Loer Lab (http://home.sandiego.edu/~cloer/loerlab/anti5htshort.html), and modifications were described in fullin Tatum et al. (7). Briefly, worms were picked into 500 ml of 1× PBS(pH 7.4), spun down quickly, and then fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde(catalog no. 15710, Electron Microscopy Sciences) in 1× PBS (pH 7.4)at 4°C for 18 hours. Worms were then incubated in b-mercaptoethanolsolution for 18 hours, followed by cuticle digestion using collagenasetype IV (catalog no. C5138, Sigma-Aldrich). For serotonin staining, theprimary antibody was 1:100 rabbit anti-serotonin as in the study byTatum et al. (7). Primary antibody used was 1:100 rabbit anti-HSF1antibody (Sigma-Aldrich), whereas the secondary antibody used was1:100 donkey anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 647 (catalog no. A-31573, LifeTechnologies). For staining of NPC proteins, we used 1:100 mouse anti-NPC (55) (Abcam, ab24609) and 1:100 rabbit anti-mouse Alexa Fluor488 (catalog no. A-11059, Life Technologies). Worms were incubatedwith DAPI in 0.1% bovine serum albumin (BSA)/1× PBS for 30 min atroom temperature and then washed and mounted on slides in about12 ml of Vectashield mounting medium (catalog no. H-1000, VectorLaboratories). Imaging of slides was performed using a Leica TCS SPEConfocal Microscope (Leica) using a 63× oil objective. LAS AF soft-ware (Leica) was used to obtain and view z-stacks. For gonad dissec-tions to stain for RNA pol II, day 1 adult AM1061 worms were pickedinto 15 ml of 1× PBS (pH 7.4) on a coverslip, and quickly dissected witha blade (product no. 4-311, Integra Miltex). A charged slide (SuperfrostPlus, catalog no. 12-550-15, Thermo Fisher Scientific) was then placedover the coverslip and immediately placed on a pre-chilled freezing

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

block on dry ice for at least 5 min. The coverslip was quickly removed,and the slides were fixed in 100% methanol (−20°C) for 1 min andthen fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde, 1× PBS (pH 7.4), 80 mM Hepes(pH 7.4), 1.6 mMMgSO4, and 0.8 mM EDTA for 30 min. After rinsingin 1× PBST (PBS with Tween 20), slides were blocked for 1 hour in 1×PBST with 1% BSA and then incubated overnight in 1:500 mouseanti-pol II (no. MMS-126R clone 8w16g, BioLegend) (77). The nextday, slides were washed and then incubated for 2 hours in 1:1000 don-key anti-mouse Cy3 (code no. 715-165-150, Jackson ImmunoResearchLaboratories) before they were washed and incubated in DAPI in 1×PBST and then mounted in 8 ml of Vectashield and imaged as describedabove. For quantification of nuclear bodies, the images were mergedwhen co-staining was carried out (HSF-1 and NPC or HSF-1::GFP andpol II), and discrete puncta ranging in size from about 400 to 550 nmwere counted first in the HSF-1 channel. We then determined whetherthese HSF-1 puncta colocalized with the puncta in the other channel.When HSF-1 immunostaining alone was carried out, only the HSF-1channel was used to quantify nuclear bodies.

Optogenetic activation of serotonergic neuronsin choice assaysTo make experimental ATR+ plates, a 100 mM ATR (product no.R2500, Sigma-Aldrich) stock dissolved in 100% ethanol was diluted toa final concentration of 0.4 mM into OP50 and 250 ml was seededonto a fresh NGM plate. Control (ATR−) plates for experiments wereseeded at the same time with the same OP50 culture but without ATR.Plates were kept in the dark and allowed to dry for a minimum of10 hours before use. Plates were never used later than 1 day after theywere seeded with ATR. The C. elegans strain used in this experimentwas AQ2050 (ijIs102; lite-1(ce314)). L4s were harvested onto ATR+and ATR− plates, and the experiment was carried out on day 1 adultworms that were transferred in sets of 10 worms per plate onto platescontaining 5 ml of either ATR+ or ATR− OP50 lawns. All plates werekept in the dark, and animals were allowed to acclimatize to roomtemperature (20° to 22°C) for at least 30 min before the start of theoptogenetic activation.

For the olfactory training, HT115 and PA14 bacterial cultures weregrown to an OD600 range of 1.4 to 1.7, and five 10-ml drops of culture(either HT115 or PA14) were placed around the small bacterial lawn.The animals were then immediately illuminated with blue light for5 min at a 6.3× magnification using an MZ10 F microscope (Leica)connected to an EL6000 light source (Leica) and subsequently trans-ferred onto choice plates containing HT115 and PA14 lawns. ATR−animals were treated similarly. During the process of olfactory trainingand optogenetic activation, animals that moved away from the cen-tral lawn were not used for the subsequent choice assay.

Scoring germline nuclei for HSF-1::GFP activationOlfaction was carried out as described above (see “Olfactory pre-exposure” section) using water and 2AA. The C. elegans strain usedwas AM1061 (rmSi1 II; hsf-1(ok600) I).Whole-worm live imaging wascarried out using a Zeiss Observer A1 inverted microscope, and ani-mals were immobilized in 25 mM levamisole on 2% agarose pads withcoverslips. Nuclei were scored for induction within 10 min after olfac-tion. Induction was assessed on the basis of the presence or absence ofHSF-1::GFP stress-induced nuclear puncta in the nuclei of germ cellslocated in the two gonads of C. elegans. Analysis of the HSF1::GFPpuncta was carried out by counting the number of nuclei showingdistinct puncta compared to the total number of nuclei present in a

14 of 17

Page 15: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

single focal plane. Images also included animals put on OP50 (control)or PA14 for 30 min on PA14 at 20°C.

Statistical analysis and N valuesFor qRT-PCR data expressed as fold change in mRNA levels relativeto control, a linear mixed model analysis for a randomized block de-sign was used to compare the different conditions in each experimen-tal data set. This was done to account for variation between differentbiological replicates, where treatment response was compared withinthe experiment. The data for this analysis were the response measureexpressed as a ratio of control (for example, OP50 or H2O odor only).Because the distribution of ratios was usually not normally distrib-uted, the natural log transformation was applied to the data to nor-malize the data distribution, with the log-transformed values used inthe analysis. Means in the log scale were then back-transformed to ob-tain geometric mean estimates in the original scale. The statisticalanalysis was performed using MIXED procedure in SAS (version 9.4).For all other data sets and where described (choice assays, smFISH/immunofluorescence/fluorescence quantification, Western blot quan-tification, and ChIP-qPCR analysis), the parametric Student’s t test(paired) was conducted to test for significance. For longevity/survivalassays, OASIS software available online (https://sbi.postech.ac.kr/oasis2/surv/) was used to calculate mean life span, and the log-ranktest was used to calculate statistical significance between the differentconditions.

For all data quantified and analyzed, N values represent at leastthree independent repeats conducted to generate the number in eachof the represented data points. For data that do not require quantifi-cation, the experiments were also conducted three or more than threetimes, and the data shown are representative data. N values for choiceassays are the numbers of independent repeats of the choice assay.N values for qRT-PCR data are the number of repeats of the mRNAmeasurements per sample. When assays are conducted to comparethe difference between different genotypes, only the number of pairedrepeats is considered to generate the N value. The N value in smFISHdata, or data regarding HSF-1 nuclear bodies, is the number of nucleicounted per animal (per field of observation) per genotype per treat-ment condition obtained from two to three independent repeats of theexperiment. Only N of paired experiments are used when genotypesare compared.

Chromatin immunoprecipitationPreparation of samples for ChIP was performed by modifying the pro-tocols previously described (51, 78). One hundred wild-type day 1 adultanimals per condition were collected from NGM plates, washed with1× PBS (pH 7.4), and cross-linked with 2% formaldehyde at roomtemperature for 10 min. Reactions were quenched with 250 mM tris(pH 7.4) at room temperature for 10 min and then washed three timesin 1× PBS with protease inhibitor cocktail and snap-frozen in liquidnitrogen. The worm pellet was resuspended in FA buffer [50 mMHepes (pH 7.4), 150 mM NaCl, 1 mM EDTA, 1% Triton X-100, and0.1% sodium deoxycholate], supplemented with protease inhibitorcocktail, lysed using a Precellys 24 homogenizer (Bertin Corp.), andthen sonicated in a Vibra-Cell Processor (Sonics & Materials Inc.).Pre-cleared lysate was then incubated overnight with 5 ml of rabbitanti–HSF-1 antibody (Sigma-Aldrich), and immunoprecipitation wasperformed with Protein A/G Magnetic Beads (catalog no. 88802, Pierce).qPCR analysis of DNA was performed using the reagents describedabove, and the primer sets syp-1 and hsp-70 (F44E5.4) were used to

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

respectively quantify nonspecific and specific binding of gene promo-ters to HSF-1. The primers used for PCR are shown in table S6. Theamplified qPCR products were run on agarose gels to verify that ChIPhad resulted in the amplification of the appropriate sized band.

Electrophoretic mobility shift assayHSE probe sequences were obtained from Silva et al. (79), and IR700-labeled oligos were obtained from Integrated DNA Technologies. Wormlysate was prepared by washing worms off in 1× PBS (pH 7.4) and im-mediately snap-freezing them in liquid nitrogen. Worm pellets werethawed on ice and lysed in a binding buffer [10 mM Hepes (pH 7.4),130 mMNaCl, 5 mMKCl, 1 mMEDTA, 0.2%NP-40, and 10% glycerol]supplemented with protease inhibitor cocktail and 1 mM DTT. Ly-sis was carried out using the Precellys 24 homogenizer (Bertin Corp.).EMSA binding reactions [lysate, poly(deoxyinosinic-deoxycytidylic)acid, and labeled IR700-HSE probes] were incubated at room temper-ature for 30 min, except for heat shock–binding reactions, with orwithout competition using unlabeled probes, which were performed at35°C for 30 min. Samples were then run out on a 6% acrylamide gelin 0.5× tris-borate EDTA, imaged using LI-COR Odyssey InfraredImaging System (LI-COR Biotechnology), and quantified using LI-CORImage Studio software. IR700 HSE (forward), taaattgtagaaggttctagaa-gatgccaga; IR700 HSE (reverse), tctggcatcttctagaaccttctacaattta.

Motility assaysFor motility assays, second-generation RNAi animals were used (referto “Growth conditions of C. elegans and bacteria” section). Animalswere harvested as L4s the day before the experiment. Day 1 adults weresingled onto a lawn of OP50, and a video of the animals’movement wascaptured at 0.8× magnification using a Leica MZ120 camera attachedto an upright microscope (Leica KL1500) for about 30 s. Videos wereanalyzed using ImageJ software to measure the distance traveled bythe animal, and from this, the velocity of the worm was calculated.The velocities were then used to calculate the time needed for eachanimal to travel 1 inch (the distance between bacterial lawns in thechoice assay as described above).

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALSwww.sciencesignaling.org/cgi/content/full/10/501/eaan4893/DC1Fig. S1. Design and specificity of olfactory pre-exposure and choice assay.Fig. S2. The compound 2AA made by PA14 specifically modulates olfactory avoidancebehavior and protects against PA14-induced death.Fig. S3. Serotonin is required for learning-mediated HSF-1 activation.Fig. S4. Characterization of C. elegans HSF-1.Fig. S5. Characterization of C. elegans HSF-1 after exposure to water odor and 2AA odor.Fig. S6. The formation of HSF-1 nuclear bodies does not require RNA pol II.Fig. S7. HSF-1 is required for olfactory learning.Table S1. Survival of animals on PA14 is dependent on HSF-1.Table S2. Statistical analyses.Table S3. 2AA does not appear to be toxic to C. elegans.Table S4. Pre-exposure to the odor of PA14 protects animals from subsequent exposure to PA14.Table S5. Primers used for qRT-PCR analysis.Table S6. Primers used for ChIP-PCR and ChIP-qPCR analysis.

REFERENCES AND NOTES1. J. Anckar, L. Sistonen, Regulation of HSF1 function in the heat stress response:

Implications in aging and disease. Annu. Rev. Biochem. 80, 1089–1115 (2011).2. S. Lindquist, The heat-shock response. Annu. Rev. Biochem. 55, 1151–1191 (1986).3. X.-D. Liu, D. J. Thiele, Oxidative stress induced heat shock factor phosphorylation and

HSF-dependent activation of yeast metallothionein gene transcription. Genes Dev. 10,592–603 (1996).

15 of 17

Page 16: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

4. R. I. Morimoto, Proteotoxic stress and inducible chaperone networks in neurodegenerativedisease and aging. Genes Dev. 22, 1427–1438 (2008).

5. V. Prahlad, T. Cornelius, R. I. Morimoto, Regulation of the cellular heat shock responsein Caenorhabditis elegans by thermosensory neurons. Science 320, 811–814 (2008).

6. V. Prahlad, R. I. Morimoto, Neuronal circuitry regulates the response of Caenorhabditis elegansto misfolded proteins. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 14204–14209 (2011).

7. M. C. Tatum, F. K. Ooi, M. R. Chikka, L. Chauve, L. A. Martinez-Velazquez,H. W. M. Steinbusch, R. I. Morimoto, V. Prahlad, Neuronal serotonin release triggersthe heat shock response in C. elegans in the absence of temperature increase. Curr. Biol.25, 163–174 (2015).

8. C. Kumsta, T.-T. Ching, M. Nishimura, A. E. Davis, S. Gelino, H. H. Catan, X. Yu, C.-C. Chu,B. Ong, S. H. Panowski, N. Baird, R. Bodmer, A.-L. Hsu, M. Hansen, Integrin-linked kinasemodulates longevity and thermotolerance in C. elegans through neuronal control of HSF-1.Aging Cell 13, 419–430 (2014).

9. P. van Oosten-Hawle, R. S. Porter, R. I. Morimoto, Regulation of organismal proteostasisby transcellular chaperone signaling. Cell 153, 1366–1378 (2013).

10. S. Wolff, J. S. Weissman, A. Dillin, Differential scales of protein quality control. Cell 157,52–64 (2014).

11. T. Takeuchi, M. Suzuki, N. Fujikake, H. A. Popiel, H. Kikuchi, S. Futaki, K. Wada, Y. Nagai,Intercellular chaperone transmission via exosomes contributes to maintenance ofprotein homeostasis at the organismal level. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, E2497–E2506(2015).

12. V. Castellucci, H. Pinsker, I. Kupfermann, E. R. Kandel, Neuronal mechanisms ofhabituation and dishabituation of the gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Science 167,1745–1748 (1970).

13. M. Brunelli, V. Castellucci, E. R. Kandel, Synaptic facilitation and behavioral sensitization inAplysia: Possible role of serotonin and cyclic AMP. Science 194, 1178–1181 (1976).

14. D. L. Glanzman, S. L. Mackey, R. D. Hawkins, A. M. Dyke, P. E. Lloyd, E. R. Kandel, Depletionof serotonin in the nervous system of Aplysia reduces the behavioral enhancementof gill withdrawal as well as the heterosynaptic facilitation produced by tail shock.J. Neurosci. 9, 4200–4213 (1989).

15. Y. Zhang, H. Lu, C. I. Bargmann, Pathogenic bacteria induce aversive olfactory learning inCaenorhabditis elegans. Nature 438, 179–184 (2005).

16. E. C. Azmitia, Serotonin and brain: Evolution, neuroplasticity, and homeostasis.Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 77, 31–56 (2007).

17. P. Fossat, J. Bacqué-Cazenave, P. De Deurwaerdère, D. Cattaert, J.-P. Delbecque,Serotonin, but not dopamine, controls the stress response and anxiety-like behavior inthe crayfish Procambarus clarkii. J. Exp. Biol. 218, 2745–2752 (2015).

18. R. K. Hukema, S. Rademakers, G. Jansen, Gustatory plasticity in C. elegans involvesintegration of negative cues and NaCl taste mediated by serotonin, dopamine, andglutamate. Learn. Mem. 15, 829–836 (2008).

19. R. Pocock, O. Hobert, Hypoxia activates a latent circuit for processing gustatoryinformation in C. elegans. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 610–614 (2010).

20. Y. Qin, X. Zhang, Y. Zhang, A neuronal signaling pathway of CaMKII and Gqa regulatesexperience-dependent transcription of tph-1. J. Neurosci. 33, 925–935 (2013).

21. D. Tsui, D. van der Kooy, Serotonin mediates a learned increase in attraction to highconcentrations of benzaldehyde in aged C. elegans. Learn. Mem. 15, 844–855 (2008).

22. F. Ballestriero, J. Nappi, G. Zampi, P. Bazzicalupo, E. Di Schiavi, S. Egan, Caenorhabditiselegans employs innate and learned aversion in response to bacterial toxic metabolitestambjamine and violacein. Sci. Rep. 6, 29284 (2016).

23. K. P. Curran, S. H. Chalasani, Serotonin circuits and anxiety: What can invertebrates teachus? Invert. Neurosci. 12, 81–92 (2012).

24. J. D. Meisel, O. Panda, P. Mahanti, F. C. Schroeder, D. H. Kim, Chemosensation of bacterialsecondary metabolites modulates neuroendocrine signaling and behavior of C. elegans. Cell159, 267–280 (2014).

25. B.-m. Song, L. Avery, Serotonin activates overall feeding by activating two separate neuralpathways in Caenorhabditis elegans. J. Neurosci. 32, 1920–1931 (2012).

26. B.-m. Song, S. Faumont, S. Lockery, L. Avery, Recognition of familiar food activatesfeeding via an endocrine serotonin signal in Caenorhabditis elegans. eLife 2, e00329(2013).

27. H.-i. Ha, M. Hendricks, Y. Shen, C. V. Gabel, C. Fang-Yen, Y. Qin, D. Colón-Ramos, K. Shen,A. D. T. Samuel, Y. Zhang, Functional organization of a neural network for aversiveolfactory learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. Neuron 68, 1173–1186 (2010).

28. G. Harris, Y. Shen, H. Ha, A. Donato, S. Wallis, X. Zhang, Y. Zhang, Dissecting the signalingmechanisms underlying recognition and preference of food odors. J. Neurosci. 34,9389–9403 (2014).

29. A. Mohri-Shiomi, D. A. Garsin, Insulin signaling and the heat shock response modulateprotein homeostasis in the Caenorhabditis elegans intestine during infection. J. Biol. Chem.283, 194–201 (2008).

30. V. Singh, A. Aballay, Heat-shock transcription factor (HSF)-1 pathway required forCaenorhabditis elegans immunity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 13092–13097(2006).

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

31. M.-W. Tan, F. M. Ausubel, Caenorhabditis elegans: A model genetic host to studyPseudomonas aeruginosa pathogenesis. Curr. Opin. Microbiol. 3, 29–34 (2000).

32. M.-W. Tan, S. Mahajan-Miklos, F. M. Ausubel, Killing of Caenorhabditis elegans byPseudomonas aeruginosa used to model mammalian bacterial pathogenesis. Proc. Natl.Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 715–720 (1999).

33. C. D. Cox, J. Parker, Use of 2-aminoacetophenone production in identification ofPseudomonas aeruginosa. J. Clin. Microbiol. 9, 479–484 (1979).

34. A. J. Scott-Thomas, M. Syhre, P. K. Pattemore, M. Epton, R. Laing, J. Pearson, S. T. Chambers,2-Aminoacetophenone as a potential breath biomarker for Pseudomonas aeruginosa inthe cystic fibrosis lung. BMC Pulm. Med. 10, 56 (2010).

35. S.-E. Kapsetaki, I. Tzelepis, K. Avgousti, I. Livadaras, N. Garantonakis, K. Varikou,Y. Apidianakis, The bacterial metabolite 2-aminoacetophenone promotes associationof pathogenic bacteria with flies. Nat. Commun. 5, 4401 (2014).

36. E. B. Spurr, R. E. R. Porter, C. Thomson, Palatability of bird repellents to Rattus norvegicus.Pest Manag. Sci. 57, 615–619 (2001).

37. J. R. Mason, D. L. Nolte, B. P. Bryant, Effectiveness of thirteen vertebrate repellents asrodent trigeminal stimulants. Physiol. Behav. 60, 1449–1452 (1996).

38. F. Massai, F. Imperi, S. Quattrucci, E. Zennaro, P. Visca, L. Leoni, A multitask biosensorfor micro-volumetric detection of N-3-oxo-dodecanoyl-homoserine lactone quorumsensing signal. Biosens. Bioelectron. 26, 3444–3449 (2011).

39. D. L. Chase, M. R. Koelle, Biogenic amine neurotransmitters in C. elegans.WormBook 2007,1–15 (2007).

40. H. A. Colbert, C. I. Bargmann, Environmental signals modulate olfactory acuity,discrimination, and memory in Caenorhabditis elegans. Learn. Mem. 4, 179–191 (1997).

41. O. Hobert, Behavioral plasticity in C. elegans: Paradigms, circuits, genes. J. Neurobiol. 54,203–223 (2003).

42. G. Jafari, Y. Xie, A. Kullyev, B. Liang, J. Y. Sze, Regulation of extrasynaptic 5-HT by serotoninreuptake transporter function in 5-HT-absorbing neurons underscores adaptationbehavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. J. Neurosci. 31, 8948–8957 (2011).

43. J. Y. Sze, M. Victor, C. Loer, Y. Shi, G. Ruvkun, Food and metabolic signalling defects in aCaenorhabditis elegans serotonin-synthesis mutant. Nature 403, 560–564 (2000).

44. A. M. Dacks, D. S. Green, C. M. Root, A. J. Nighorn, J. W. Wang, Serotonin modulatesolfactory processing in the antennal lobe of Drosophila. J. Neurogenet. 23, 366–377(2009).

45. G. Harris, A. Korchnak, P. Summers, V. Hapiak, W. J. Law, A. M. Stein, P. Komuniecki,R. Komuniecki, Dissecting the serotonergic food signal stimulating sensory-mediatedaversive behavior in C. elegans. PLOS ONE 6, e21897 (2011).

46. A. K. Boehm, A. Saunders, J. Werner, J. T. Lis, Transcription factor and polymeraserecruitment, modification, and movement on dhsp70 in vivo in the minutes followingheat shock. Mol. Cell. Biol. 23, 7628–7637 (2003).

47. M. J. Guertin, J. T. Lis, Chromatin landscape dictates HSF binding to target DNA elements.PLOS Genet. 6, e1001114 (2010).

48. S. J. Petesch, J. T. Lis, Activator-induced spread of poly(ADP-ribose) polymerase promotesnucleosome loss at Hsp70. Mol. Cell 45, 64–74 (2012).

49. S.-G. Ahn, P. C. C. Liu, K. Klyachko, R. I. Morimoto, D. J. Thiele, The loop domainof heat shock transcription factor 1 dictates DNA-binding specificity and responsesto heat stress. Genes Dev. 15, 2134–2145 (2001).

50. K. A. Morano, D. J. Thiele, Heat shock factor function and regulation in response tocellular stress, growth, and differentiation signals. Gene Exp. 7, 271–282 (1999).

51. J. Li, L. Chauve, G. Phelps, R. M. Brielmann, R. I. Morimoto, E2F coregulates an essentialHSF developmental program that is distinct from the heat-shock response. Genes Dev.30, 2062–2075 (2016).

52. Y. M. Hajdu-Cronin, W. J. Chen, P. W. Sternberg, The L-type cyclin CYL-1 and theheat-shock-factor HSF-1 are required for heat-shock-induced protein expression inCaenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 168, 1937–1949 (2004).

53. E. A. Morton, T. Lamitina, C. elegans HSF-1 is an essential nuclear protein that forms stressgranule-like structures following heat shock. Aging Cell 12, 112–120 (2013).

54. D. Egecioglu, J. H. Brickner, Gene positioning and expression. Curr. Opin. Cell Biol. 23,338–345 (2011).

55. S. Rohner, V. Kalck, X. Wang, K. Ikegami, J. D. Lieb, S. M. Gasser, P. Meister, Promoter- andRNA polymerase II–dependent hsp-16 gene association with nuclear pores inCaenorhabditis elegans. J. Cell Biol. 200, 589–604 (2013).

56. L. Becirovic, I. R. Brown, Targeting of heat shock protein HSPA6 (HSP70Bʹ) to theperiphery of nuclear speckles is disrupted by a transcription inhibitor following thermalstress in human neuronal cells. Neurochem. Res. 42, 406–414 (2017).

57. M. S. Buckley, J. T. Lis, Imaging RNA Polymerase II transcription sites in living cells.Curr. Opin. Genet. Dev. 25, 126–130 (2014).

58. A. Papantonis, P. R. Cook, Transcription factories: Genome organization and generegulation. Chem. Rev. 113, 8683–8705 (2013).

59. G. Gürel, M. A. Gustafson, J. S. Pepper, H. R. Horvitz, M. R. Koelle, Receptors and othersignaling proteins required for serotonin control of locomotion in Caenorhabditis elegans.Genetics 192, 1359–1371 (2012).

16 of 17

Page 17: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

SC I ENCE S I GNAL ING | R E S EARCH ART I C L E

http://stke.sciencemag

Dow

nloaded from

60. L. L. Fortes-Marco, E. E. Lanuza, F. F. Martinez-Garcia, Of pheromones and kairomones:What receptors mediate innate emotional responses? Anat. Rec. 296, 1346–1363(2013).

61. U. Groenhagen, R. Baumgartner, A. Bailly, A. Gardiner, L. Eberl, S. Schulz, L. Weisskopf,Production of bioactive volatiles by different Burkholderia ambifaria strains. J. Chem. Ecol.39, 892–906 (2013).

62. R. E. Page, M. S. Blum, H. M. Fales, o-Aminoacetophenone, a pheromone that repelshoneybees (Apis mellifera L.). Experientia 44, 270–271 (1988).

63. V. S. Cooper, W. A. Carlson, J. J. Lipuma, Susceptibility of Caenorhabditis elegansto Burkholderia infection depends on prior diet and secreted bacterial attractants.PLOS ONE 4, e7961 (2009).

64. C. Jolly, R. Morimoto, M. Robert-Nicoud, C. Vourc’h, HSF1 transcription factor concentratesin nuclear foci during heat shock: Relationship with transcription sites. J. Cell Sci. 110(Pt. 23), 2935–2941 (1997).

65. C. Jolly, Y. Usson, R. I. Morimoto, Rapid and reversible relocalization of heat shockfactor 1 within seconds to nuclear stress granules. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96,6769–6774 (1999).

66. F. M. Duarte, N. J. Fuda, D. B. Mahat, L. J. Core, M. J. Guertin, J. T. Lis, Transcriptionfactors GAF and HSF act at distinct regulatory steps to modulate stress-induced geneactivation. Genes Dev. 30, 1731–1746 (2016).

67. K. L. Zobeck, M. S. Buckley, W. R. Zipfel, J. T. Lis, Recruitment timing and dynamics oftranscription factors at the Hsp70 loci in living cells. Mol. Cell 40, 965–975 (2010).

68. D. A. Jurivich, C. Pachetti, L. Qiu, J. F. Welk, Salicylate triggers heat shock factor differentlythan heat. J. Biol. Chem. 270, 24489–24495 (1995).

69. J. H. Feder, J. M. Rossi, J. Solomon, N. Solomon, S. Lindquist, The consequences ofexpressing hsp70 in Drosophila cells at normal temperatures. Genes Dev. 6, 1402–1413(1992).

70. R. I. Morimoto, Regulation of the heat shock transcriptional response: Cross talkbetween a family of heat shock factors, molecular chaperones, and negative regulators.Genes Dev. 12, 3788–3796 (1998).

71. C. Dai, L. Whitesell, A. B. Rogers, S. Lindquist, Heat shock factor 1 is a powerfulmultifaceted modifier of carcinogenesis. Cell 130, 1005–1018 (2007).

72. T. Gidalevitz, A. Ben-Zvi, K. H. Ho, H. R. Brignull, R. I. Morimoto, Progressive disruption ofcellular protein folding in models of polyglutamine diseases. Science 311, 1471–1474 (2006).

73. R. S. Kamath, J. Ahringer, Genome-wide RNAi screening in Caenorhabditis elegans.Methods 30, 313–321 (2003).

74. J. Wang, M. M. Barr, RNA interference in Caenorhabditis elegans. Methods Enzymol. 392,36–55 (2005).

Ooi and Prahlad, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017) 17 October 2017

75. R. M. Alcazar, R. Lin, A. Z. Fire, Transmission dynamics of heritable silencing induced bydouble-stranded RNA in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 180, 1275–1288 (2008).

76. S. Garvis, A. Munder, G. Ball, S. de Bentzmann, L. Wiehlmann, J. J. Ewbank, B. Tümmler,A. Filloux, Caenorhabditis elegans semi-automated liquid screen reveals a specializedrole for the chemotaxis gene cheB2 in Pseudomonas aeruginosa virulence. PLOS Pathog. 5,e1000540 (2009).

77. L. R. Baugh, J. DeModena, P. W. Sternberg, RNA Pol II accumulates at promoters of growthgenes during developmental arrest. Science 324, 92–94 (2009).

78. A. Mukhopadhyay, B. Deplancke, A. J. M. Walhout, H. A. Tissenbaum, Chromatinimmunoprecipitation (ChIP) coupled to detection by quantitative real-time PCR tostudy transcription factor binding to DNA in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat. Protoc. 3,698–709 (2008).

79. M. C. Silva, M. D. Amaral, R. I. Morimoto, Neuronal reprograming of protein homeostasisby calcium-dependent regulation of the heat shock response. PLOS Genet. 9, e1003711(2013).

Acknowledgments: We thank past and current members of the V.P. laboratory for commentsand discussion and in particular M. Wheat, K. Dvorak, and C. Anbalagan for assistance with datageneration. We acknowledge S. Smolikove (University of Iowa) for sharing protocols andadvice, T. Yahr (University of Iowa) and A. Aballay (Duke University) for PA14 strains, I. Ruvinsky(Northwestern University) for helpful suggestions and advice, and CGC [funded by NIH Officeof Research Infrastructure Programs (P40 OD010440)] for worm and bacterial strains. Funding:F.K.O. was supported by V.P.’s grants, a graduate student fellowship from the DSHB, and ascholarship from Glenn/American Federation for Aging Research Foundation. V.P. is funded bythe NIH (R01 AG 050653) and the Lawrence Ellison Medical Foundation (AG-NS-1056-13).Author contributions: F.K.O. and V.P. designed the project and experiments, analyzed data,and wrote the manuscript. F.K.O performed experiments. Competing interests: The authorsdeclare that they have no competing interests. Data and materials availability: All wormstrains, material, and protocols will be made available on request.

Submitted 20 April 2017Accepted 20 September 2017Published 17 October 201710.1126/scisignal.aan4893

Citation: F. K. Ooi, V. Prahlad, Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription factorHSF-1 to enhance the expression of molecular chaperones in C. elegans. Sci. Signal. 10,eaan4893 (2017).

.or

17 of 17

on October 17, 2017

g/

Page 18: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

C. elegansexpression of molecular chaperones in Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription factor HSF-1 to enhance the

Felicia K. Ooi and Veena Prahlad

DOI: 10.1126/scisignal.aan4893 (501), eaan4893.10Sci. Signal. 

chaperone gene expression systemically in anticipation of a proteotoxic encounter.dependent−, olfactory learning can initiate HSF-1C. elegansprotein damage. Instead, the authors show that in

memory. The activation of HSF-1 and chaperone expression has been considered an autonomous reaction of cells toenriched for RNA polymerase II. These responses required serotonergic signaling, which is important for learning and thus increasing survival. Olfactory experience of the pathogen odor alone caused HSF-1 to accumulate at genomic locithis experience enhanced heat shock factor 1 (HSF-1) target gene expression when animals encountered the pathogen,

In addition,Caenorhabditis elegans.pathogenic bacterium enhanced the pathogen avoidance response of the nematode being prepared to fight damage is the next-best option. Ooi and Prahlad found that previous experience of the odor of a

The best way to prevent pathogen-induced cellular damage is to avoid becoming infected. If that is not possible,Learning the smell of danger

ARTICLE TOOLS http://stke.sciencemag.org/content/10/501/eaan4893

MATERIALSSUPPLEMENTARY http://stke.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2017/10/13/10.501.eaan4893.DC1

CONTENTRELATED

http://stke.sciencemag.org/content/sigtrans/10/495/eaam7703.fullhttp://science.sciencemag.org/content/sci/353/6294/aac4354.full

REFERENCES

http://stke.sciencemag.org/content/10/501/eaan4893#BIBLThis article cites 79 articles, 36 of which you can access for free

PERMISSIONS http://www.sciencemag.org/help/reprints-and-permissions

Terms of ServiceUse of this article is subject to the

registered trademark of AAAS.is aScience Signaling Association for the Advancement of Science. No claim to original U.S. Government Works. The title

York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. 2017 © The Authors, some rights reserved; exclusive licensee American (ISSN 1937-9145) is published by the American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 NewScience Signaling

on October 17, 2017

http://stke.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 19: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Supplementary Materials for

Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription factor HSF-1

to enhance the expression of molecular chaperones in C. elegans

Felicia K. Ooi and Veena Prahlad*

*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Published 17 October 2017, Sci. Signal. 10, eaan4893 (2017)

DOI: 10.1126/scisignal.aan4893

This PDF file includes:

Fig. S1. Design and specificity of olfactory pre-exposure and choice assay.

Fig. S2. The compound 2AA made by PA14 specifically modulates olfactory

avoidance behavior and protects against PA14-induced death.

Fig. S3. Serotonin is required for learning-mediated HSF-1 activation.

Fig. S4. Characterization of C. elegans HSF-1.

Fig. S5. Characterization of C. elegans HSF-1 after exposure to water odor and

2AA odor.

Fig. S6. The formation of HSF-1 nuclear bodies does not require RNA pol II.

Fig. S7. HSF-1 is required for olfactory learning.

Table S1. Survival of animals on PA14 is dependent on HSF-1.

Table S2. Statistical analyses.

Table S3. 2AA does not appear to be toxic to C. elegans.

Table S4. Pre-exposure to the odor of PA14 protects animals from subsequent

exposure to PA14.

Table S5. Primers used for qRT-PCR analysis.

Table S6. Primers used for ChIP-PCR and ChIP-qPCR analysis.

www.sciencesignaling.org/cgi/content/full/10/501/eaan4893/DC1

Page 20: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Fig. S1. Design and specificity of olfactory pre-exposure and choice assay.

(A) Schematic of olfactory pre-exposure and subsequent choice assay. Animals were pre-exposed to the

odor of PA14 or OP50 as shown. The PA14 and OP50 lawns on the choice plate were 1 inch apart and

grown as described in Materials and Methods. (B) Choice index (CI) for PA14 of naïve wild-type

animals. CI was recorded at the indicated times (x-axis). N = 6 experiments of 30 animals each. (C) CI

for HT115 of wild-type animals pre-exposed to OP50 or HT115 odor. N = 4 experiments of 30 animals

per condition. Student’s paired t-test. No significance. (D) CI for DB11 of wild-type animals that were

naïve, or pre-exposed to the odor of either OP50 or PA14. N = 3 experiments of 30 animals per condition.

Student’s t-test (pair-wise comparisons). No significance. (E) qRT-PCR analysis of hsp-70

(F44E5.4/F44E5.5) mRNA abundance in wild-type animals upon PA14 exposure. Animals were

subjected to control (empty vector) or hsf-1 RNAi knockdown using feeding RNAi (see Materials and

Methods). Western blots (see Fig S4A) were used to confirm RNA knockdown. mRNA values are

relative to control, untreated animals on control RNAi. N = 5 experiments of 30 animals per condition. A

linear mixed model analysis for a randomized block design, using log transformation to normalize data

was used. *p<0.05 (unadjusted).

Page 21: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Fig. S2. The compound 2AA made by PA14 specifically modulates olfactory avoidance behavior

and protects against PA14-induced death.

(A) Chemotactic indices of naïve, wild-type animals exposed to 2AA alone. Animals were placed

equidistant from a drop of water and a drop of 2AA, both which had been pre-absorbed into the plate.

The drops were 1.5 inches apart, and the numbers of animals that migrated towards each drop were

represented using a Choice Index for 2AA odorant calculated as described in Materials and Methods.

Animals retained a slightly positive chemotactic index across 120 mins. N = 6 experiments of 30 animals

per condition. (B) Choice index of wild-type animals for PA14 following pre-exposure to the odor of

either ethanol (vehicle) or N-3-oxododecanoyl homoserine lactone (3OC12-HSL; (38)). Preference was

recorded at the times indicated on the x-axis. N = 5 experiments of 30 animals per condition. Student’s t-

test, no significance. (C) hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) mRNA abundance measured by qRT-PCR in wild-

type animals that were pre-exposed to ethanol (vehicle) odor or 3OC12-HSL odor and subsequently

placed on a PA14 lawn. Values are relative to animals pre-exposed to ethanol. N = 5-8 experiments of 30

animals per condition. A linear mixed model analysis for a randomized block design, using log

transformation to normalize data was used. No significance. See Materials and Methods and Table S2 for

complete details. (D) Thermotolerance assay for wild-type animals that were naïve or pre-exposed to the

odor of OP50 or PA14. N = 3 experiments in triplicate with 20 animals each. Log-rank test. No

significance. (A, B) Data represent means ± S.E.M. Legends: pre-exposure conditions (choice).

Page 22: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Fig. S3. Serotonin is required for learning-mediated HSF-1 activation.

(A) Choice index for PA14 of tph-1 (mg280) II animals pre-exposed to the odor of OP50 and PA14.

Preference was recorded at times indicated (x-axis). N = 7-8 experiments of 30 animals per condition.

Student’s paired t-test.*p<0.05, **p<0.01. (B) Treatment of tph-1 (mg280) II animals with 2mM

exogenous 5-HT results in the loading of 5-HT into serotonergic neurons. Immunofluorescence

micrographs showing projected confocal images of heads of tph-1 (mg280) II animals stained with an

antibody recognizing 5-HT, showing (upper panel) the lack of 5-HT in their neurons, and (lower panel;

arrowhead) the presence of 5-HT in NSM neurons 30 minutes following 5-HT treatment. Scale bar, 5µm.

N=4-5 experiments of 10 animals per experiment. (C) Choice index for PA14 of tph-1 (mg280) II animals

rescued by treatment with 2mM exogenous 5-HT for 24 hours, pre-exposed to the odor of OP50 and

PA14. Preference was recorded at times indicated (x-axis). N = 4 experiments of 30 animals per

condition. Student’s paired t-test. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.01. (D) Choice index for PA14 of tph-1

(mg280) II animals pre-exposed to the odor of OP50, compared to the choice indices in tph-1 (mg280) II

animals rescued by treatment with 2mM exogenous 5-HT for 24 hours, and pre-exposed to OP50 odor.

Preference was recorded at times indicated (x-axis). N = 4 experiments of 30 animals per condition.

Student’s paired t-test. No significance. (A, C and D) Legends: pre-exposure conditions (choice).

Page 23: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Fig. S4. Characterization of C. elegans HSF-1.

(A) Representative Western blot showing HSF-1 protein abundance in wild-type animals subjected to

control (empty vector) RNAi, or animals subject to hsf-1 RNAi knockdown using feeding RNAi (see

Materials and Methods). Tubulin is a loading control. (B) Quantification of amounts of HSF-1 protein

relative to control. N = 3 experiments of 30 animals per condition. Student’s t-test.*p<0.05. (C)

Immunofluorescence micrographs of endogenous C. elegans HSF-1 and DAPI in germline nuclei of wild-

type animals at ambient temperatures or subjected to heat shock. Scale bar, 5µm. N=3 experiments of 20

Page 24: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

animals each. (D) Representative Western blot using HSF-1 antibody following phos-tag PAGE analysis

of full length recombinant, C. elegans HSF-1, and C. elegans lysate from wild-type animals under

ambient conditions. N=3 experiments of 30 animals per condition. Arrowhead indicates phosphorylated

HSF-1 in control animals under ambient conditions. (E) Representative Western blot using HSF-1

antibody on EGS-crosslinked C. elegans lysates. Cross-linking was conducted at ambient temperatures or

upon subjecting the lysates to heat shock (trimers at approx. 250kD and dimers at 150kD). N=3

experiments of 50 animals per condition.

Page 25: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Fig. S5. Characterization of C. elegans HSF-1 after exposure to water odor and 2AA odor.

(A) Electrophoretic mobility shift assay (EMSA) indicating in vitro binding of HSF-1 to DNA: control

lysate, heat-shocked lysate, heat-shocked lysate competed with cold probe, lysate from animals exposed

to water-odor and lysate from animals exposed to 2AA odor. (B) Quantification of relative intensity of the

shifted HSF-1 band in the EMSA, normalized to control lysate. N = 5 experiments of >200

animals/condition. Student’s paired t-test. No significance. (C) Chromatin immunoprecipitation-qPCR

(ChIP-qPCR) data showing occupancy of HSF-1 at the promoter of hsp70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) and syp-1

(control for specificity; not a target of HSF-1), represented as percent input. N = 5 experiments of 100

animals per condition. Student’s t-test *p<0.05 for Control compared to Heat shock. No significance for

Control compared to 2AA odor. (D) Representative Western blot using HSF-1 antibody on animals

exposed to the odor of water, 2AA odor, water odor followed by exposure to PA14 lawns, and 2AA odor

followed by exposure to PA14 lawns. N = 4 experiments of 30 animals per condition. Note that odorant-

exposure does not change the migration of HSF-1, but exposure to PA14 lawns does. (B and C) Data

represent means ± S.E.M.

Page 26: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Fig. S6. The formation of HSF-1 nuclear bodies does not require RNA pol II.

(A) Representative immunofluorescence micrographs of RNA Pol II and DAPI in oocyte nuclei of

animals subjected to control (empty vector) or Pol II (ama-1) RNAi knockdown using standard

procedures of feeding RNAi in C. elegans ( see Materials and Methods). Scale bar, 5µm. RNAi

knockdown was confirmed across 3 experiments of 10 animals each. (B) HSF-1::GFP localization in

oocyte nuclei of animals subjected to control or Pol II (ama-1) RNAi knockdown under ambient

conditions or upon heat shock. Scale bar, 10µm.

Page 27: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Fig. S7. HSF-1 is required for olfactory learning.

(A) Choice index for PA14 of wild-type animals subjected to hsf-1 RNAi and pre-exposed to the odor of

either control RNAi bacteria or PA14 odor. Preference was recorded at time indicated (x-axis). N = 6

experiments of 30 animals per condition. Student’s t-test, no significance. Also compare to Fig 1A,

showing the choice index of wild-type animals. (B) The motility of wild-type animals subjected to control

(empty vector) or hsf-1 RNAi knockdown measured as the time required to translocate over a distance of

1 inch (the distance between the OP50 and PA14 lawns in all experiments; y-axis). N=2 experiments of 5

animals per experiment. See Materials and Methods for the measurement of motility. Student’s t-test

*p<0.05. (C) Choice index for HT115 of naïve wild-type animals. Choice was between HT115 and PA14

lawns. N = 5 experiments of 30 animals/condition. Data represent means ± S.E.M. Legends: pre-exposure

conditions (choice).

Page 28: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Table S1. Survival of animals on PA14 is dependent on HSF-1.

Survival of control (empty vector-treated) wild-type animals or hsf-1 RNAi treated animals when exposed

to PA14. RNAi was conducted by feeding, using standard RNAi techniques for C. elegans. Mean lifespans

were calculated using OASIS software (https://sbi.postech.ac.kr/oasis2/surv/), and the log-rank test was

used to calculate statistical significance between the different conditions. p< 0.001. N=4 repeats of 50

animals each.

Experiment

ID

Condition # animals Mean

lifespan

(hours)

S.E. 95% C.I. p (Log-rank)

Fig. 1B (1) Control

RNAi

50 21.96 0.39 21.19 ~ 22.73 0

hsf-1 RNAi 50 14.36 0.17 14.03 ~ 14.69

Fig. 1B (2) Control

RNAi

50 21.76 0.42 20.94 ~ 22.58 8.00E-08

hsf-1 RNAi 50 16.24 0.54 15.19 ~ 17.29

Fig. 1B (3) Control

RNAi

50 20.32 0.4 19.54 ~ 21.10 0

hsf-1 RNAi 50 14.88 0.28 14.32 ~ 15.44

Fig. 1B (4) Control

RNAi

50 20.88 0.31 20.27 ~ 21.49 0

hsf-1 RNAi 50 14.76 0.29 14.19 ~ 15.33

Page 29: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Table S2. Statistical analyses.

qRT-PCR data expressed as fold change in mRNA levels relative to control was analyzed using a linear mixed model

analysis for a randomized block design to compare the different conditions in each experimental data set. This was done

to account for variation between different biological replicates, where treatment response was compared within the

experiment. The data for this analysis was the response measure expressed as a ratio of control (for example, OP50 or

H2O odor only). Since the distribution of ratios was usually not normally distributed, the natural log transformation was

applied to the data to normalize the data distribution, with the log transformed values used in the analysis. Means in the

log scale were then back-transformed to obtain geometric mean estimates in the original scale. The statistical analysis

was performed using MIXED procedure in SAS (version 9.4).

Figure 1C; hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5)

Treatment Mean SE Pairwise comparison

Comparison Mean Ratio (95% CI) p-value

PA14 odor 0.87 0.06 PA+lawn/PA odor 11.05 (7.20, 16.97) <0.0001

OP50 odor +

PA14 lawn 6.18 0.98 PA+lawn/OP+lawn 1.55 (1.24, 1.93) 0.0001

PA14 odor +

PA14 lawn 9.57 1.58 -- -- --

Figure 1D; hsp16.2 (Y46H3A.3)

Treatment Mean SE Pairwise comparison

Comparison Mean Ratio (95% CI) p-value

PA14 odor 1.13 0.22 PA+lawn/PA odor 24.24 (11.11, 52.90) <0.0001

OP50 odor +

PA14 lawn 12.79 5.30 PA+lawn/OP+lawn 2.15 (1.39 3.32) 0.002

PA14 odor +

PA14 lawn 27.47 9.20 -- -- --

Figure 1E; hsp16.41 (Y46H3A.2)

Treatment Mean SE Pairwise comparison

Comparison Mean Ratio (95% CI) p-value

PA14 odor 1.08 0.23 PA+lawn/PA odor 16.99 (7.01, 41.13) <0.0001

OP50 odor +

PA14 lawn 10.05 4.53 PA+lawn/OP+lawn 1.83 (1.26, 2.65) 0.004

PA14 odor +

PA14 lawn 18.38 7.92 -- -- --

Figure 2B; hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5)

Treatment Mean SE Pairwise comparison

Comparison Mean Ratio (95% CI) p-value

2AA odor 0.99 0.09 2AA+lawn/2AA

odor 13.63 (7.92, 23.44) <0.0001

H2O odor + PA14

lawn 7.03 1.27

2AA+lawn/ H2O

+lawn 1.92 (1.35, 2.73) 0.0006

2AA odor +

PA14 lawn 13.51 3.16 -- -- --

Page 30: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Figure 3C; hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5)

Genotype Treatment Mean SE Pairwise comparison

Comparison Mean Ratio (95% CI) p-value

N2

PA14 odor 0.80 0.15 PA+lawn/PA odor 22.32 (8.15, 61.10) <0.0001

OP50 odor +

PA14 lawn 8.10 3.00 PA+lawn/OP+lawn 2.20 (1.32, 3.66) 0.003

PA14 odor +

PA14 lawn 17.83 6.27 -- -- --

tph-

1(mg280)II

PA14 odor 0.67 0.12 PA+lawn/PA odor 6.21 (2.27, 17.01) 0.001

OP50 odor +

PA14 lawn 6.14 2.27 PA+lawn/OP+lawn 0.68 (0.41, 1.12) 0.164

PA14 odor +

PA14 lawn 4.16 1.46 PA+lawn: tph-1/N2 0.23 (0.10, 0.53) 0.003

*PA+lawn response relative to OP+lawn response differed significantly between tph-1 and N2 (0.68 vs. 2.20; p<0.0001),

with tph-1 having a significantly reduced response compared to N2

Figure 5C; hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5)

Treatment Mean SE Pairwise comparison

Comparison Mean Ratio (95% CI) p-value

2AA odor + rec 0.85 0.28 2AA+lawn/2AA

odor 5.09 (1.41, 18.40) 0.019

H2O odor + rec +

PA14 lawn 3.52 1.14

2AA+lawn/ H2O

+lawn 1.24 (0.34, 4.46) 0.863

2AA odor + rec +

PA14 lawn 4.34 1.41 -- -- --

Figure S1E; hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5)

Treatment Mean SE Pairwise comparison

Comparison Mean Ratio (95% CI) p-value

hsf-1 RNAi 4.20 1.30 hsf-1+lawn/hsf-1 10.68 (5.13, 22.38) <0.0001

Control RNAi +

PA14 lawn 85.39 26.47

hsf-1+lawn/

control+lawn 0.52 (0.25, 1.09)

unadjusted

p=0.047

hsf-1 RNAi +

PA14 lawn 44.61 13.89 -- -- --

Figure S2C; hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5)

Treatment Mean SE Pairwise comparison

Comparison Mean Ratio (95% CI) p-value

3OC12 odor 1.03 0.23 3OC12+lawn/

3OC12 odor 14.94 (7.68, 29.08) <0.0001

EtOH odor +

PA14 lawn 13.45 2.90

3OC12+lawn/

EtOH+lawn 1.14 (0.59, 2.22) 0.855

3OC12 odor +

PA14 lawn 15.32 3.29 -- -- --

Page 31: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Table S3. 2AA does not appear to be toxic to C. elegans.

The mean lifespans of wild-type animals exposed to the odor of water and 2AA, and animals allowed to be

in direct contact and ingest water and 2AA. Mean lifespans were calculated using OASIS software

(https://sbi.postech.ac.kr/oasis2/surv/), and the log-rank test was used to calculate statistical significance

between the different conditions. No significance. N=3 repeats for each condition of 50 animals each.

Experiment

ID

Condition # animals Mean

lifespan

(days)

S.E. 95% C.I. p (Log-rank)

Fig. 2C (1) H2O odor 50 11.04 0.53 10.00 ~ 12.08 0.8397 (n.s.)

2AA odor 50 11.06 0.72 9.64 ~ 12.48

Fig. 2C (2) H2O odor 50 11.17 0.67 9.87 ~ 12.48 0.3927 (n.s.)

2AA odor 50 12.04 0.76 10.54 ~ 13.53

Fig. 2C (3) H2O odor 50 11.12 0.6 9.95 ~ 12.30 0.3021 (n.s.)

2AA odor 50 11.88 0.62 10.67 ~ 13.09

Fig. 2D (1) OP50 50 12.25 0.64 11.00 ~ 13.50 0.0754 (n.s.)

OP50 +

2AA

50 10.97 0.49 10.00 ~ 11.94

Fig. 2D (2) OP50 50 11.4 0.6 10.22 ~ 12.58 0.77 (n.s.)

OP50 +

2AA

50 11.84 0.59 10.67 ~ 13.00

Fig. 2D (3) OP50 50 12.97 0.67 11.66 ~ 14.27 0.381 (n.s.)

OP50 +

2AA

50 12.56 0.58 11.42 ~ 13.70

Page 32: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Table S4. Pre-exposure to the odor of PA14 protects animals from subsequent exposure to PA14.

Survival of wild-type animals that were pre-exposed to OP50 or PA14 odor. N = 8 experiments of 50

animals per condition. Mean lifespans were calculated using OASIS software

(https://sbi.postech.ac.kr/oasis2/surv/), and the log-rank test was used to calculate statistical significance

between the different conditions. p value of independent repeats are shown. p value of aggregated data (see

Fig 2E) = 0.000002

Experiment

ID

Condition # animals Mean

lifespan

(hours)

S.E. 95% C.I. p (Log-rank)

Fig. 2E (1) OP50 odor 50 17.96 0.42 17.14 ~ 18.78 0.0194

PA14 odor 50 19.44 0.4 18.66 ~ 20.22

Fig. 2E (2) OP50 odor 50 19.4 0.49 18.43 ~ 20.37 0.0299

PA14 odor 50 20.8 0.44 19.93 ~ 21.67

Fig. 2E (3) OP50 odor 50 18.08 0.42 17.25 ~ 18.91 0.0039

PA14 odor 50 20.28 0.4 19.50 ~ 21.06

Fig. 2E (4) OP50 odor 50 19.2 0.39 18.43 ~ 19.97 0.0016

PA14 odor 50 21.16 0.41 20.36 ~ 21.96

Fig. 2E (5) OP50 odor 50 18.48 0.56 17.38 ~ 19.58 0.6516 (n.s.)

PA14 odor 50 19 0.57 17.89 ~ 20.11

Fig. 2E (6) OP50 odor 50 20.48 0.52 19.47 ~ 21.49 0.1718 (n.s.)

PA14 odor 50 21.32 0.47 20.41 ~ 22.23

Fig. 2E (7) OP50 odor 50 19.72 0.44 18.85 ~ 20.59 0.0402

PA14 odor 50 20.4 0.45 19.51 ~ 21.29

Fig. 2E (8) OP50 odor 50 20.56 0.46 19.66 ~ 21.46 0.6424 (n.s.)

PA14 odor 50 20.72 0.4 19.95 ~ 21.49

Page 33: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Table S5. Primers used for qRT-PCR analysis.

The sequences of the primers used in qRT-PCR experiments.

hsp70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) Forward 5’-TGC ACC AAT CTG GAC AAT CT-3’

hsp70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) Reverse 5’-TCC AGC AGT TCC AGG ATT TC-3’

Actin Forward 5’-ATC ACC GCT CTT GCC CCA TC-3’

Actin Reverse 5’-GGC CGG ACT CGT CGT ATT CTT G-3’

syp-1 Forward 5’-GAT GAA ATG ATA ATT CGC CAA GA-3’

syp-1 Reverse 5’- ACG CAA TCT TCC CTC ATT TG-3’

pmp-3 Forward 5’-TAG AGT CAA GGG TCG CAG TG-3’

pmp-3 Reverse 5’-ATC GGC ACC AAG GAA ACT GG-3’

hsp-16.2 (Y46H3A.3) Forward 5’-CGT TCC GTT TTT GGT GAT CT-3’

hsp-16.2 (Y46H3A.3) Reverse 5’-ACC TCA GAA GAC TCA GAT GGA GA-3’

hsp-16.41 (Y46H3A.2) Forward 5’-AAT TTT TCC GAT AAT ATT GGG GAG-3’

hsp-16.41 (Y46H3A.2) Reverse 5’-TTC TGG TTT GAA ATG AGA GAC ATC-3’

Page 34: Olfactory experience primes the heat shock transcription ... · counter with only the odor of pathogenic bacteria prepared Caenorhabditis elegans to survive exposure to the pathogen

Table S6. Primers used for ChIP-PCR and ChIP-qPCR analysis.

The sequences of the primers used in ChIP-PCR and ChIP-qPCR experiments.

hsp70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) Forward 5’-TGC ACC AAT CTG GAC AAT CT-3’

hsp70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) Reverse 5’-TCC AGC AGT TCC AGG ATT TC-3’

syp-1 Forward 5’-ACA CCA CTT ACC GCA GCT AC-3’

syp-1 Reverse 5’-CTT CCC TCC TCT CTT TCG GC-3’

hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5)_PCR Forward 5’-TTG AGA TCC TCG CCA ACT CG-3’

hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5)_PCR Reverse 5’-TGC ATC CTT TGT TGC TTG CC-3’

hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) promoter Set 1 Forward 5’-ATA CTA CCC GAA TCC CAG CC-3’

hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) promoter Set 1 Reverse 5’-GCA ACA GAG ACG CAG ATT GT-3’

hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) promoter Set 2 Forward 5’-GTC GGC CGT CTC TTT CTC TT-3’

hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) promoter Set 2 Reverse 5’-CCC GAA TCC CAG CCC TTT T-3’

hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) promoter Set 3 Forward 5’-TAA AAG GGC TGG GAT TCG GG-3’

hsp-70 (F44E5.4/F44E5.5) promoter Set 3 Reverse 5’-ACC GAG GTC GAT ACC AAT AGC-3’


Recommended