+ All Categories
Home > Documents > ORGAHOLEPTIC TESTS DEVELOPED FOR MEASURIIG ... - Meat …

ORGAHOLEPTIC TESTS DEVELOPED FOR MEASURIIG ... - Meat …

Date post: 30-Dec-2021
Category:
Upload: others
View: 1 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
6
111. ORGAHOLEPTIC TESTS DEVELOPED FOR MEASURIIG THE PALATABlLlTY OF NEAT BELLE LOWE IOWA STATE COLLEGE No one realizes better than the author, that an adequate presentation of scoring cannot be presented in 20 minutes. several others are omitted, cussed rully. promote discussion from this group. For each point that is mentioned, In addition, the points presented cannot be dis- It is hoped, however, that the material that is presented will The Necessity for Organoleptic Scores Objective tests are excellent in fields of research in whioh perform- ance can be determined by objective tests. There are no objective tests for evaluating certain factors upon which acceptability depends in rating the vari- ations between samples of meat or other foods. In addition, it seems unlikely that usable, valid objective tests for evauating characteristics such a8 odor and taste of meat will be developed within a short time. Hence, it seems wise to use all objective tests that give u s e m information in evaluating scoring data and to spend time in elucidation of the complicating factors in organolep- t i c scoring. and the development of score cards. Food technologists, however, realize there is still much to do along these lines. ized in order to better compare results from different laboratories. Much thought has been put upon the selection of scoring panels In addition tests need to be standard- One of the major difficulties in organoleptic tests is the separation of the palatability factors. they start scoring. fairly evaluate the flavor of a product if the texture is unacceptable or vice versa. are four taste perceptions, bitter, sour, salt, and sweet. that all four tabtee are intimately connected and that the presence of any one taste affects and my modify the other three tastes (Moncrieff, 1946. p. 12). Students constantly comment on this point when It ta,kes mental discipline and training to separate and Even one palatability factor, that of taste, may be complicated. There We are further told Crocker in a study published in 1948 states that raw beef when cold The taste is ' sweetish, salty, and generally bloodlike has only a weak odor. When the beef is warmed, considerable odor develops. like a mixture of amines, including piperdine. cating the presence of sulfur compounds. sistent, and chiefly sweet and salty. Crocker found most of the meat flavor came from the meat fiber, an optimum i n 3 to 3.5 hours of boiling, then with longer cooking the flavor decreased. Crocker concluded that cooked-beef flavor is quite complicated chemically, and consists more of odor than taste. several kinds including a low simple form, one of the piperdine type, and possible indole were present. volatile acids. The only elements of true taste noted were sweetness and saltiness, no doubt owing t o blood salts. tracts as well a8 in cooked fiber. Many of these constituents are associated in our minds &from our chemistry days as unpleasant odors. Yet the small con- centrations present and the combination bring about the fragrant, appetizing It is alkaline and me118 It is also somewhat eggy, indi- The t a s t e is mild, pleasant, per- With longer cooking the flavor increased, reaching Hydrogen sulfide, amines of The vegetable type odors imply the presence of Astringency was noted in beef ex-
Transcript
Page 1: ORGAHOLEPTIC TESTS DEVELOPED FOR MEASURIIG ... - Meat …

111.

O R G A H O L E P T I C TESTS D E V E L O P E D FOR M E A S U R I I G T H E P A L A T A B l L l T Y O F NEAT

B E L L E LOWE I O W A S T A T E COLLEGE

No one rea l izes be t te r than the author, t ha t an adequate presentation of scoring cannot be presented i n 20 minutes. several others a re omitted, cussed rul ly . promote discussion from t h i s group.

For each point t ha t i s mentioned, I n addition, the points presented cannot be dis-

It i s hoped, however, that the material t h a t i s presented w i l l

The Necessity f o r Organoleptic Scores

Objective t e s t s a r e excellent i n f i e lds o f research i n whioh perform- ance can be determined by objective t e s t s . There a re no objective t e s t s for evaluating cer ta in factors upon which acceptabili ty depends i n ra t ing the var i - a t ions between samples o f meat o r other foods. I n addition, it seems unlikely that usable, val id objective tests fo r evauat ing character is t ics such a8 odor and taste of meat w i l l be developed within a short time. Hence, it seems wise t o use a l l objective t e s t s t ha t give u s e m information i n evaluating scoring data and t o spend time in elucidation of the complicating factors i n organolep- t i c scoring. and the development of score cards. Food technologists, however, rea l ize there i s s t i l l much t o do along these l i nes . ized i n order t o be t te r compare r e su l t s from different laboratories.

Much thought has been put upon the selection of scoring panels

I n addition t e s t s need t o be standard-

One of the major d i f f i cu l t i e s in organoleptic t e s t s i s the separation of the pa la tab i l i ty factors . they s t a r t scoring. f a i r l y evaluate the flavor of a product i f the texture i s unacceptable or vice versa. a r e four t a s t e perceptions, b i t t e r , sour, salt, and sweet. that a l l four tabtee a re intimately connected and tha t the presence of any one taste a f f ec t s and m y modify the other three t a s t e s (Moncrieff, 1946. p. 12 ) .

Students constantly comment on t h i s point when It ta,kes mental discipl ine and t ra in ing t o separate and

Even one pa la tab i l i ty factor, tha t of t a s t e , may be complicated. There We a re fur ther to ld

Crocker i n a study published i n 1948 s t a t e s t h a t raw beef when cold The taste is ' sweetish, sa l ty , and generally bloodlike has only a weak odor.

When the beef i s warmed, considerable odor develops. l i k e a mixture of amines, including piperdine. cating the presence of sulfur compounds. s i s ten t , and chiefly sweet and sa l ty . Crocker found most of the meat flavor came from the meat f ibe r , an optimum i n 3 t o 3.5 hours of boiling, then with longer cooking the flavor decreased. Crocker concluded tha t cooked-beef f lavor i s quite complicated chemically, and consists more of odor than taste. several kinds including a low simple form, one of t h e piperdine type, and possible indole were present. vo la t i l e acids. The only elements of t rue t a s t e noted were sweetness and sal t iness , no doubt owing t o blood salts. t r a c t s as w e l l a8 in cooked fiber. Many of these consti tuents a re associated i n our minds &from our chemistry days as unpleasant odors. Yet the s m a l l con- centrations present and the combination bring about the fragrant, appetizing

It i s alkal ine and me118 It i s also somewhat eggy, indi-

The t a s t e i s mild, pleasant, per-

With longer cooking the flavor increased, reaching

Hydrogen sulfide, amines of

The vegetable type odors imply the presence of

Astringency was noted i n beef ex-

Page 2: ORGAHOLEPTIC TESTS DEVELOPED FOR MEASURIIG ... - Meat …

112.

odor of cooked meat, surface of the meat during Prying, roasting, etc. differed from beef i n cer ta in flavor character is t ics .

None of Crocker's work included flavor developed on the Lamb, pork, and chicken all

Enough of Crocker'a r e su l t s have been included t o indicate that scoring meat flavor i s not a simple process, and that the flavor i s developed t o an optimum during cooking, then decrease8 with longer cooking. Crocker (1948) found that the flavor was increased by cooking, it follows that the character is t ic f lavor of meat is a breakdown product by cooking or i s re- leased from meat upon cooking. 1949) indicates the flavoring property i n chicken meat i s a weak acid produced by cooking.

Since

Work at the university of California (Bouthilet,

The Score Card

The score card should be prepared with care and above everything should be simple. A very complicated score card only frustrates the scorer and a l l too often ends with no s ignif icant variations having been found. of factors t o be scored should be kept as smll as possible i n re la t ion t o the information sought i n the study. may be aroma, flavor i n i t s wider sense, tenderness, and juiciness. Flavor of fat and texture may be added, although some cuts of cer ta in meats such as veal have l i t t l e f a t for acoring.

The number

If four pa la tab i l i ty factors a re used, they

I f flavor i s t o be divided in to in tens i ty and des i rab i l i ty , then t h i s i s d i f f i c u l t scoring and a t l e a s t two or three pa la tab i l i ty factors should be eliminated (such a s texture and tenderness or juiciness) t o give the scorer time t o concentrate on the points being scored.

I t is unfortunate from some standpoints t h a t numerical ra t ings must be assigned to pala tab i l i ty factors for acceptabili ty i s not basical ly a math- ematical f'unction. A score of 4 does not imply that the sample i s twice as palatable a8 one with a score of 2, nor that it i s two-thirds as acceptable as a sample with a score of 6. means of evaluating the results' s t a t i s t i c a l l y and f o r making comparisons with other laboratories.

But, numerical ra t ings a re pract ical ly our only

Nothing i s gained by using a scoring system that has a range of num- bers beyond the number of gradations of quali ty that can be recognized, On the other hand using a range of numbers smaller than the range of qua l i t i es t ha t can be recognized is a lao disadvantageous. use only the numerical ra t ings between the top and bottom scores a range of 0 t o 5 w i t h 1 point variation i s less than the gradations of quali ty factors t h a t can be recognized,

Since there i s a tendency to

A range of qua l i t i es l e easier t o remember i f descriptive terms are assigned t o each numerical ra t ing. used, 10 might be assigned the word perfect for a description, 9 excellent, 8 very good, 7 high average, 5 average, 4 low average, 3 f a i r , 2 poor, 1 very poor, and 0 unacceptable, negative rat ings, t ha t can be recognized, but tenderness, i f based upon the number of chews t o masticate a standard s ize sample t o a determined end point, has a wide range of tenderness variations.

For example, i f scores of 0 t o 10 a r e

Varying degrees of inedib i l i ty can be assigned Some pa la t ab i l i t y factors have a limited range of qua l i t i es

Page 3: ORGAHOLEPTIC TESTS DEVELOPED FOR MEASURIIG ... - Meat …

113.

The Scoring Panel

Variation i n organoleptic scoring i s admitted. Even a person who i s ordinar i ly an almost perfect scorer may be thrown off base when he has a head cold. scores for a panel. Probably four scorers i s the minimum number. However, a s t i l l smaller panel composed of members who have high sens i t i v i ty and a b i l i t y t o d i f f e ren t i a t e perceptions may be preferable t o a la rge group w i t h less sens i t iv i ty . One of the basic tene ts of the s t a t i s t i c a l treatment of the scoring data i s that a la rger number of individuals a r e needed fo r the panel when t h e var ia t ion i n scoring i s great. A l a rger number of scorers will a l s o tend t o rule out the experimental e r ror of da i ly individual var ia t ion because of physical o r mental conditions.

Hence, no hard and fast rule can be made concerning t h e numbers o f

A scorer should have a high degree of in tegr i ty , be able t o concen- t ra te and develop an objective and sc i en t i f i c a t t i t ude towards scoring meat or other foods, and above a l l be wi l l ing t o spend t i m e and e f f o r t needed for a falr evaluation of the problem.

If the members of the panel are t o be selected on t h e i r sens i t iv i ty of organoleptic ratings, the concenms a t present seems t o favor using the food t o be scored as a basis of selection instead o f d i lu t e salt solutions, sugar solution, e tc . I n panel t a s t ing it is the a b i l i t y of the scorer t o detect a flavor i n a mixture of flavors tha t i s important.

Probably the most common tes t used a t present f o r t he selection of a scoring panel i n t he t r iangular one. samples of meat. s ize . The scorer i s t o l d tha t two samples are duplicates and asked t o pick out t he duplicates. It i s advantageous t o ask upon what character is t ica , such as aroma, t a s t e , a f t e r taste, texture, tenderness, and/or juiciness the decision i s made, of using a l l pa l a t ab i l i t y factors . prospective scorer can be cal led t o his at tent ion in the preliminary t ra ining. I n the t r iangular t es t a person has t o pick the duplicates 5 times out of 7 fo r the results t o be s ignif icant . t he samples and who cannot duplicate their judgments i n picking duplicate samples should not be placed on t h e scoring panel.

Prospective scorers are given three The samples a re cut from a par t icu lar muscle, a l l t he same

Some persons w i l l t r y t o make the select ion on taste alone instead This lack of observation on the pa r t of the

Persons who cannot detect differences i n

Members of a panel should be given preliminary t ra in ing i n a6 wide var ia t ion i n qua l i t i e s of meat as possible. be greater than w i l l be m e t i n the study.

This var ia t ion should preferably

Preparation of and Scoring of the Sample

The sample should be presented t o the scorer i n an a t t r a c t i v e manner. A l l samples are coded, but coding does not mean t h a t the sample should be placed over a l a rge red o r black pencilled number or presented i n such a manner t h a t the scorer i s adversely affected. t o the appearance of the sample.

The scorer 's f irst reaction i s usually

Each scorer is given a sample, a8 nearly as possible, from the same anatomical location of each cut. This i s important for d i f fe ren t p a r t s of t he cu t may vary i n composition of f ibe r and connective t i s sue . center s l i c e has not been subjected t o heat during cooking as long as a s l i c e close t o the ex ter ior of t h e cut, par t icu lar ly i n meat cooked rare o r medium done.

I n addition, a

Page 4: ORGAHOLEPTIC TESTS DEVELOPED FOR MEASURIIG ... - Meat …

114.

The temperature of t he neat sample should be about the same for each Odor i s more intense when the meat scoring, or the scorer may be handicapped.

temperature i s high. (b i t te r , sour, salt, and meet ) (Moncrieff, 1946, Chapter 5) i s doubled on rais- ing the temperature from 10' t o 20%. from 2OoC t o 3@C, then falls off a s the temperature i s raised from 30' t o 40' C. I n h i s summary Moncrieff (p 125) give8 the optimum temperature for taste different ia t ion a8 200 to 4OoC.

In addition, the a b i l i t y t o d i f fe ren t ia te the four tastes

Acuteness of taste then i s f a i r l y steady

Sl ices of meat a re cut acros8 the f iber and should be of uniform thickness. i t s r e l a t ive tenderness, the meat should be s l iced on 8 s l i c ing machine. gives sections of the f ibe r s and connective tissue with as nearly uniform length as possible.

Preferably, from the standpoint of chewing the sample t o determine This

Samples should be large enough for the scorer t o have more than one Crocker thinks it i s preferable to w a i t long enough between bites for taste.

the sa l iva to c lear the mouth rather than eat other food or drink water o r beverage between samples. It should a l s o be indicated t h a t i f another food o r a drink is used between samples, it should also be used before s t a t i n g t o score the first sample.

Moncrieff (1946) s t a t e s that the sweet t a s t e is most eas i ly perceived a t the t i p of the tongue, the b i t t e r a t the back, the sour a t the edge, and the salt a t both the t i p and edge, t r ibu t ion of the t a s t e buds. I n 80me areas two or a l l of the t a s t e s can be distinguished and there is an area a l i t t l e l ee s than the size of a dime back of the tongue t i p i n which no taste i s perceived. Other areas i n which t a s t e buds a re located i n addition t o the dorsal par t of the tongue, a r e the sof t palate, the ep ig lo t t i s , and the beginning of the esophagus.

Crocker (1945) gives a somewhat d i f fe ren t dis-

It i a not necessary for us t o decide whether Moncrieff o r Crocker i s r ight . It i s indicated though that i n the preliminary t ra in ing the pros- pective scorer should not swallow the sample too hast i ly , but should develop a def in i te procedure of ro l l i ng the sample over the tongue between chews. addition, t o the t a s t e buds being located i n different areas, some t a s t e perceptions a re ident i f ied more slowly than others. The four t a s t e s i n order of ease of detection a re b i t t e r , sour, salt, and sweet, It is the combined result of a l l of these t a s t e and odor perceptions t h a t determine the re la t ive pa l a t ab i l i t y of two or more samples of meat. A good scorer develops a pro- cedure t o obtain as much information a b u t each sample as possible,

I n

The scorer should not be interrupted during scoring. be done i n a place f r ee of odors of other materials and foods.

Scoring should

Each ecorer, after the preliminary training, works independently.

There i s no def in i te ru le i n deciding upon the number of samples that can be ecored a t one t i m e . The senses of t a s t e and smell fatigue easi ly , hence the number should not be large. The number of samples that can be scored a t one t i m e i s a l so re la ted t o the number of pa la tab i l i ty factors t o be scored. If the score card i s complicated two samples &re suf f ic ien t for scoring. only pa r t of the pa l a t ab i l i t y factors are t o be rated, then four o r more samples may be scored a t one t i m e .

I f

Page 5: ORGAHOLEPTIC TESTS DEVELOPED FOR MEASURIIG ... - Meat …

115,

Subjective and Objective Tests

Subjective tenderness and juiciness of t he meat ra t ings may be com- pared with objective shear force (pounde or ounces t o shear a cylinder of a de f in i t e s ize) and press f luid. by the Carver prees of the Minnesota pressometer. t h e number of chews t o masticate a standard s i z e sample t o a predetermined end point gives a very easy way of evaluating comparative tenderness i n d i f - ferent samples. Do not be surprised i f objective tests and subjective tests do not always agree. For example, there are some meats, which with the first b i t e l o se a great deal of juice. After a f e w chews, t h e meat may become dry i n the mouth, The sample may be ra ted dry on an organoleptic rating, yet give a high percentage of press f lu id .

The amount of press f l u i d can be determined We have found t h a t counting

They may be measuring d i f fe ren t things.

Evaluation of Tests

Usually the study i s not complete unti l the scoring data have been analyzed by some standard s t a t i s t i c a l methods t o t e s t the significance of t he data.

L i te ra ture Cited

Bouthilet , R . J . , 1949. A note on the flavor constituent of poultry meat, Food Tech, 3: 118

Crocker, E.C., 1945. Flavor, McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc. 172 pp.

Crocker, E.C., 1948. Flavor of meat, Food Research 13: 179-183

Moncrieff, R.W., 1946. The chemical senses, John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 424 pp.

CHAIRMAN BUTLER: Thank you, Miss Lowe.

The discussion w i l l be directed by V . K. Johnson of Illinois.

PROF, JOHNSON: A r e there any questions t h a t you would l i k e t o d i r ec t t o Miss Lowe?

PROF. WANDERSTOCK: Miss Lowe, do you think t h a t it i s advisable, i n s e t t i ng up a pa la t ab i l i t y panel f o r a meat study, t o have your committee get together and have a trial run?

MISS LOWE: Oh, yes, that i s a preliminary t ra in ing , par t icu lar ly i f they have not been judging, o r i f you have been judging for t h i r t y years, like I have, and you had a vacation during the summer, you need t o refresh before you go in .

PROF. JOHNSON: Anybody else?

PROF, KLINE: Miss Lowe, would you use the pa l a t ab i l i t y committee a f t e r they have eaten, o r just before they have eaten, o r does tha t seem t o make much difference?

Page 6: ORGAHOLEPTIC TESTS DEVELOPED FOR MEASURIIG ... - Meat …

116.

MISS LOWE: Most of ours we use a t the time that it i s very easy; usually about eleven o'clock, because the meat w i l l come out about that time. I rather gather, i f I remember Moncrieffrightly, tha t that i s a pret ty good t i m e t o score,

I n the af'ternoon, we usually score at about four o'clock. some t h a t advocate ea r l i e r than that. t e s t , a f t e r we s ta r ted our co-operative meat proJects in 1926, that they sug- gested scoring a t 9:30 a.m. but I think it has not been borne out altogether that t h a t i s the best t i m e t o do it.

There m e I remember in our first pa la tab i l i ty

PROF. JOHNSON: Any other questione?

If not, I w i l l turn the meeting back t o Chairman Butler.

CHAIRMAN BVILER: Thank you, M r . Johnson,

Prof. Bratzler has been s t a l l i ng me off about two days on some ques- t ions I have been asking about tenderness of meat, and we w i l l now have a dis- cussion by him on "Determining the Tenderness of Meat by the Use of the Warner- Bratzler Method".

# # #


Recommended