+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Paei Punye Kajian Konon

Paei Punye Kajian Konon

Date post: 07-Apr-2018
Category:
Upload: faiszie
View: 229 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 27

Transcript
  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    1/27

    Critical age

    Patkowsky found that learners under

    the age of fifteen achieved higher

    syntactic(connected with syntax)

    proficiency than those who were over

    the age of fifteen at the onset of

    exposure.

    The study sought to discern if learners

    who were exposed to second language

    learning before the age of 15 actually

    received higher syntactic proficiency

    than older learners.

    Johnson and Newport believe that after

    the age of six, the ability to learn a

    second language begins to decline.

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    2/27

    The results also show a correlation

    between the age of acquisition and

    the variance in the ultimate

    performance in adults.

    In the exercisehypothesis, it is believed

    that humans easily learn languages at

    an early age.

    This hypothesis states that even though

    humans have a capacity to learn

    languages early in life, they are unable

    to do so with the same outcome if

    learning a second language commences

    in the future

    Lennebergs original proposal of a

    critical period in language acquisition

    seemed to predict a rectangular

    function in the relationship between

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    3/27

    age of acquisition and ultimate

    performance. That is, Lenneberg

    hypothesized that normal language

    learning was possible between the

    period from infancy to puberty, with a

    loss of abilities after puberty (p. 95)

    In response to these and many other

    empirical studies, Long (1990), in his

    seminal paper, reviewed the second

    language research on agerelated

    differences. In this paper he draws

    several conclusions that are relevant to

    this topic: 1. Both the initial rate of

    acquisition and the ultimate level of

    attainment depend in part on the ageat which learning begins. 2. There are

    sensitive periods governing both first

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    4/27

    and second language development,

    during which both the acquisition of

    different linguistic domains is

    successful and after which it is

    incomplete. 3. The age-related loss of

    ability is cumulative, not a one-time

    event. 4. Deterioration in someindividuals begins as early as six.

    Starting after age six appears to make

    it impossible for many learners (and

    after age 12 for the remainder) to

    achieve native-like competence in

    phonology; starting later than the early

    teens, more precisely after age 15,

    seems to create the same problems inmorphology and syntax. (p. 274)

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    5/27

    there is an ideal 'window' of time to

    acquire language in a linguistically rich

    environment, after which this is no

    longer possible due to changes in

    the brain.

    Various ages have been proposed for

    the supposed end of the CPH; those

    that point to pre-adolescent ages such

    as 12 have been vulnerable to

    alternative theories which

    invoke psychological or social factors

    applying as children move

    into adolescence.

    The hypothesis was developed by Eric

    Lenneberg in his 1967 Biological

    Foundations of Language, which set the

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    6/27

    end of the critical period for native

    language acquisition at 12

    In SLA, a weaker version of the CPH

    emerged in the 1970s. This refers to

    a sensitive periodin which nativelike

    performance is unlikely but not ruled

    out.[2] The strongest evidence for the

    CPH is in the study of accent, where

    most older learners seem not to reach

    a native-like level.

    First language acquisition

    For more information, see: First

    language acquisition.

    Children without language

    The CPH as applied to first language

    acquisition proposes that a child

    deprived of exposure to natural

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    7/27

    language would fail to acquire it if

    exposure commenced only after the end

    of the critical period. Because testingsuch a theory would be unethical, in

    that it would involve isolating a child

    from the rest of the world for several

    years, researchers have gatheredevidence of the CPH from a few victims

    of child abuse. The most famous

    example is the case of Genie (a

    pseudonym), who was deprived of

    language until the age of 13. Over the

    following years of rehabilitation,

    improvement in her ability

    to communicate was noted, but during

    this time she did not develop thelanguage ability common to other

    children.[5] However, this case has been

    criticised as a firm example of the

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    8/27

    critical period in action, and data has

    not been gathered from Genie since the

    1970s.[6]

    Although there are several cases on

    record of deaf children being deprived

    of sign language, this could also count

    as abuse. One case in which no abusetook place is that of Chelsea, whose

    deafness was left undiagnosed until the

    age of 31. Once hearing aids had

    apparently restored her hearing to near-

    normal levels, she seemed to develop a

    large vocabulary while her phonology

    and syntax remained at a very low

    level.[7] The implications of this have

    been disputed, given the apparently

    unlikely circumstances of Chelsea's

    diagnosis.[8]

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    9/27

    Many researchers hold opposing beliefs

    about the Critical Period Hypothesis

    put forth by Lenneberg (1967). The

    hypothesis cited in Lightbown and

    Spada (1999) states that there is a

    specific and limited time period for

    language acquisition (p. 19).Lenneberg believed that the language

    acquisition device, like other biological

    functions, works successfully only when

    it is stimulated at the right time (p.19).

    2.2 The critical period hypothesis

    for language acquisition

    In the field of language acquisition,the hypothesis that there is a critical

    or

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    10/27

    sensitive period for language

    acquisition plays an important role.

    In the latefifties and sixties, Penfield and

    Roberts (1959) and Lenneberg

    (1967) were

    the first to propose that there was acritical period for language

    acquisition.

    They based this proposal on

    different types of evidence:

    - evidence from feral and abused

    children who grew up without being

    exposed to human language in

    childhood and who did not acquire

    language normally after they werefound

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    11/27

    - evidence from deaf children whose

    development in spoken language

    stopped after puberty- evidence that children with aphasia(

    recovered much better than adults

    with aphasia

    All this evidence comes from first

    language acquisition.

    2.2. Studies of a Human Brain and its

    Lateralization

    If a language develops with the

    increase of the brain cell connections,

    the development of language and brain

    should bear some relation to each

    other. Physiological functions of

    neuronal cells, circuits, or connections

    themselves have not been identified

    clearly so far, but some specified fields

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    12/27

    of linguistic functions have been

    reported. Geschwind (1980:62-63)

    claims:

    Although the content, that is, the

    specific language learned, is completely

    determined by the environment, the

    capacity to acquire language isbiologically determined. Indeed, one

    should not speak about a contrast

    between language and general

    cognitive ability.

    Earlier studies show that the brain

    seems to have special-purpose

    computers for limited functions, and

    there is at present no evidence of any

    all-purpose computer for any generalcognition. If a language is acquired in

    parallel with the development of the

    human brain as children grow, it is

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    13/27

    reasonable to postulate(to suggest /

    accept the existence,fact,or truth of

    something as a basis of reasoning ortheory)some language function in the

    brain. Consequently, the critical period

    for language learning is considered to

    be the biologically determined period

    in which the brain keeps its plasticityfor acquisition of any language.

    At first it was expected that the

    function of human language acquisition

    was clarified by exploring a specialstructure, which all other animal brains

    lack. The brain of human beings

    consists of a left hemisphere and a

    right hemisphere, and different

    functions are said to develop graduallyin different parts of the brain as

    children grow older. The parts of brain

    which control a language are placed in

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    14/27

    the left hemisphere after its

    lateralization(. One of the parts is called

    the Broca's area and the other part iscalled the Wernicke's area. The study

    of aphasia has revealed that the Broca's

    area controls spoken language. The

    Wernicke's area is, on the other hand,

    said to be the center of languageunderstanding.

    Lenneberg posits that the development

    of language is the result of brain

    maturation. Although bothhemispheres of the brain are equal at

    birth, the function of language

    gradually settles in the dominant left

    hemisphere of the brain after biological

    maturation or the critical period. Thatis, the critical period for language

    learning has been considered to agree

    with the period of the lateralization of

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    15/27

    brain. Research on people who have

    suffered brain damage also supplies the

    evidence for the lateralization ofbrains. Lenneberg viewed clinical data

    and claimed that children younger than

    nine years old had a higher incidence of

    right hemisphere lesions (damage

    caused by injury or illness) causingaphasia than adults (Villiers & Villiers

    1978:211). Another support of

    lateralization is an experimental report

    of speed and accuracy in language

    acquisition by Kimura (1973). He

    examined speech sounds heard in each

    ear. His result is that speech sound

    through the right ear is processed more

    quickly than through the left earbecause the left hemisphere is

    connected to a right ear.

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    16/27

    A brain is said to lose its plasticity after

    the lateralization, and some case

    studies are reported on the impairedbrains before and after the critical

    period. Adults who have suffered brain

    damage in their left hemisphere fail to

    recover their language if they don't

    recover in five months, but childrenshow an ability to recover over a longer

    period, and have sometimes made a

    full recovery if they were very young at

    the time of damage (Crystal 1987:263).

    Even total removal of the left

    hemisphere did not preclude children's

    reacquisition of language. Lenneberg's

    argument is based on this period of

    lateralization, of which completionmeans the end of the critical period.

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    17/27

    2.4. Criticism of the Critical Period

    Hypothesis

    2.4.1. Critical Period Hypothesis

    reexamined

    As medical science has progressed, some

    points in Lenneberg (1967) are under

    criticism. Firstly, his claim that the CPHcould be supported by the study of Down's

    syndrome cases is attacked. According to

    his argument, the development of children

    with Downfs syndrome is so slow that

    they pass their critical period for language

    learning. However, a recent survey

    disputes his claim because children with

    Down's syndrome have a build-in endpoint

    to their ability (Gleitman 1984).

    Secondly, the theory of brain's

    lateralization at the age of two, with the

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    18/27

    critical period set by Lenneberg (1967), is

    also criticized. He claimed that children

    before their critical period were lessseverely impaired by brain damage.

    However, Krashen (1973) reexamined the

    data used in Lenneberg (1967) and found

    all the cases of complete recovery from

    aphasia were under the age of five.

    Surprisingly, the number of cases of

    recovery at the age of more than five or

    over was nearly the same number as the

    adults'. Kinsbourne (1975) pointed out the

    difficulty in deciding whether only half of

    the hemisphere was injured or not.

    Another piece of counter evidence is seen

    inMacKain et. al. (1983). Their experiment

    with babies of six months or less shows

    that lateralization begins much earlier

    than two years old. If so, lateralization

    cannot be the evidence of the critical

    period. Reports of dichotic listening,

    experiments in which different stimuli are

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    19/27

    presented simultaneously to the two ears,

    also show that language functions are

    lateralized much earlier than the criticalperiod. Regrettably, no conclusive

    evidence for right ear advantage based on

    the lateralization has not been reported.

    Thirdly, the case studies of three

    linguistically isolated children have someproblems. Because the early research in

    Isabelle's case was not written by a

    specialist in language (Aitchson 1989:86),

    her speech may have been less sufficient

    than reported. There is also a report thatGenie's left hemisphere was atrophied

    because of the brain damage. This implies

    that she used only the right hemisphere of

    the brain, the part which is said to have

    little function in language processing.Curtiss, et. al. (1974) writes:

    The result of the dichotic tests using

    environmental sounds also show a left-ear

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    20/27

    advantage, but only to a degree found in

    normal subjects. This 'normal' result

    shows that Genie is not simply one ofthose rare individuals with reversed

    dominance, but instead is one in whom all

    auditory processing currently appears to

    be taking place in the right hemisphere

    (Curtiss, Fromkin, Krashen, Rigler, and

    Rigler 1974:542).

    Isabelle was deaf and Chelsea had hearing

    problems too (Aitchson 1989:87). A

    difference between deaf and normal

    children should have been taken intoconsideration in the case studies. Since no

    further case of feral children will be

    reported, the data have been reanalyzed.

    All of these criticisms of the past studies

    show how difficult it is to connect brainfunctions and language acquisition.

    Moreover, the different methods for the

    research on the CPH have also resulted in

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    21/27

    varieties of inconsistencies. For example,

    native speakers were asked to rate the

    performance or naturalness of theutterances of learners in one study (Scovel

    1981, cited in Ellis 1984:485), whereas

    grammatical judgments made by learners

    of different ages were examined in

    another study (Coppetiers 1987). Birdsong

    (1992) designed a grammaticality

    judgment test for twenty native English

    speakers who spoke French with native-

    like fluency in order to examine Long's

    question (1990:281) of 'whether the very

    best learners actually have native-like

    competence.'

    The critical period hypothesis (CPH) refers

    to a long-standing debate

    in linguistics and language acquisition over

    the extent to which the ability to

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    22/27

    acquire language is biologically linked

    to age. The hypothesis claims that there is

    an ideal 'window' of time to acquirelanguage in a linguistically rich

    environment, after which this is no longer

    possible due to changes in the brain. The

    hypothesis has been discussed in the

    context of both first (FLA) and second

    language acquisition (SLA), and is

    particularly controversial in the latter. In

    FLA, it seeks to explain the apparent

    absence of language in individuals whosechildhood exposure was very limited, and

    in SLA it is often invoked to explain

    variation in adults' performance in

    learning a second language, which is very

    often observed to fall short

    of nativelike attainment. Various ages

    have been proposed for the supposed end

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    23/27

    of the CPH; those that point to pre-

    adolescent ages such as 12 have been

    vulnerable to alternative theories whichinvoke psychological or social factors

    applying as children move

    into adolescence

    History

    The critical period hypothesis is associated

    with Wilder Penfield, whose 1956

    Vanuxem lectures at Princeton

    University formed the basis of his 1959

    work with Lamar Roberts, Speech andBrain Mechanisms. Penfield and Roberts

    explored the neuroscience of language,

    concluding that it was dominant in the left

    hemisphere of the brain on the basis of

    hundreds of case studies spanning manydecades. The review focussed on how

    individuals with brain damage evidenced

    atypical linguistic performance, rather

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    24/27

    than examining neurotypical cases of

    'normal' language acquisition, and the

    authors' conclusions were also based onthe prevailing tabula rasa view that

    children were born without any

    real innate language ability; however,

    linguistic "units", once "fixed", would

    affect later learning.[1]

    Their

    recommendations for language schooling

    recommended starting early in order to

    avoid fixed effects; though these claims

    did not form the core of the book, being

    confined to the last chapter, other

    researchers and popular opinion were

    much-influenced by them. The hypothesis

    was developed by Eric Lenneberg in his

    1967 Biological Foundations of Language,

    which set the end of the critical period for

    native language acquisition at 12. The

    hypothesis has been fiercely debated since

    then, and has continued to inform popular

    assumptions about the presumed

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    25/27

    (in)ability of adults to fluently learn a

    second language.

    In SLA, a weaker version of the CPHemerged in the 1970s. This refers to

    a sensitive periodin which nativelike

    performance is unlikely but not ruled

    out.[2]

    The strongest evidence for the CPH

    is in the study of accent, where most olderlearners seem not to reach a native-like

    level. This leads some researchers to apply

    the CPH only to second

    language phonology rather than all

    aspects of language; indeed, a CPH wasnot seriously considered for syntax until

    the 1990s, in research that remains a

    minority view.[3]

    However, under certain

    conditions, native-like accent has been

    observed, suggesting that accent in SLA isaffected by multiple factors, such

    as identity and motivation, rather than a

    biological constraint.

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    26/27

    Children without language

    The CPH as applied to first language

    acquisition proposes that a child deprivedof exposure to natural language would fail

    to acquire it if exposure commenced only

    after the end of the critical period.

    Because testing such a theory would be

    unethical, in that it would involve isolatinga child from the rest of the world for

    several years, researchers have gathered

    evidence of the CPH from a few victims

    of child abuse. The most famous example

    is the case of Genie (a pseudonym), whowas deprived of language until the age of

    13. Over the following years of

    rehabilitation, improvement in her ability

    to communicate was noted, but during

    this time she did not develop the languageability common to other

    children. However, this case has been

    criticised as a firm example of the critical

  • 8/6/2019 Paei Punye Kajian Konon

    27/27

    period in action, and data has not been

    gathered from Genie since the 1970s.

    Although there are several cases on recordof deaf children being deprived of sign

    language, this could also count as abuse.

    One case in which no abuse took place is

    that of Chelsea, whose deafness was left

    undiagnosed until the age of 31.Once hearing aids had apparently restored

    her hearing to near-normal levels, she

    seemed to develop a large vocabulary

    while her phonology and syntax remained

    at a very low level. The implications of thishave been disputed, given the apparently

    unlikely circumstances of Chelsea's

    diagnosis.


Recommended