+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and...

Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and...

Date post: 14-Mar-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 5 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
16
Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members: The Outgroup Covariation Effect Patricia W. Linville, Gregory W. Fischer, and Carolyn Yoon Duke University The authors show a new outgroup homogeneity bias--outgroup covariation. They investigated per- ceived covariation among the features describing group subtypes. Results support a familiarity co- variation effect. Those more familiar with a group perceive lower covariation among features of group members. Results also support an outgroup covariation effect. In cases in which people are less familiar with the outgroup, they perceive greater covariation among features of outgroup members. For age, in which young and old people were less familiar with the outgroup, both perceived greater covariation among features ofoutgroup subtypes. For occupation, in which undergraduates had less work experience than masters of business students, undergraduates perceived greater covariation among features of business subtypes. For gender, in which students were equally familiar with men and women, no covariation effect occurred. Familiarity mediated outgroup covariation effects. According to the outgroup homogeneity hypothesis, people view members of their outgroup as more homogeneous than members of their ingroup. The concept of homogeneity has been operationalized in various ways, including measures of overall intragroup similarity (Mackie & Asuncion, 1990; Park & Rothbart, 1982), number of subtypes (Brewer & Lui, 1984; Linville, 1982; Park, Ryan, & Judd, 1992), dimensional com- plexity (Linville, 1982; LinviUe & Jones, 1980), and the vari- ability of single features such as intelligence or likability. The predominant focus in this literature has been on these measures of single-feature variability, which include the range, standard deviation, perceived differentiation (PaL number of attribute levels, and stereotypicality of a feature (e.g., Jones, Wood, & Quattrone, 1981; Kraus, Ryan, Judd, Hastie, & Park, 1993; Linville, Fischer, & Salovey, 1989; Park & Judd, 1990; Park & Rothbart, 1982). The typical finding is that people perceive their outgroup to be less variable along single features than their ingroup, although an ingroup homogeneity effect may occur when social identity needs are strong (for reviews, see Brewer, 1993; Linville, in press; Linville & Fischer, 1993a; Messick & Mackie, 1989; Mullen & Hu, 1989; Ostrom & Sedikides, 1992; Patricia W. Linviile, Gregory W. Fischer, and Carolyn Yoon, Fuqua School of Business, Duke University. Carolyn Yoon is now at the Faculty of Management, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. This research was supported by research funds from the Fuqua School of Business. We thank the participants of the Duck Social Cog- nition Conference, June 1993, for their helpful feedback. We also thank Jennifer Escalas for her help in testing some of the participants. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Patri- cia W. LinviUe, Fuqua School of Business, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27708. Electronic mail may be sent via the Internet to [email protected]. Park, Judd, & Ryan, 1991; Sedikides & Ostrom, 1993; Simon, 1992). In this article we take a different approach, focusing on per- ceived covariation among the features that describe the mem- bers or subtypes of a social group. It is limiting to assume that people only think about group members in terms of single fea- tures. Instead, we assume that the mental representation of a social group includes dusters of features, each describing a spe- cific group member or subtype (Linville, Salovey, & Fischer, 1986). For example, a student's mental representation of other students might include subtypes such as, "Nerds are studious, unsociable, and dress without style" and "Party animals are so- ciable, not studious, and dress well" The feature patterns that comprise such a representation of a social group implicitly en- code knowledge regarding covariation among the features that describe group members. In this example, studiousness and so- ciability are negatively correlated attributes of college students. Research indicates that people are sensitive to covariation in- formation in social contexts (Chapman & Chapman, 1967; Crocker, 1981; Hamilton, 1979, 1981; Kelley, 1967; Lewicki, 1986a; Schneider, 1973). Perceived covariation plays a key role in social judgment, enabling people to infer missing features, to predict behaviors from their correlated traits, to infer motives and traits from behavior, and to form hypotheses regarding causal relations. This article investigates how group membership affects im- plicit perceptions of covariation among the features of group subtypes. In particular, are there ingroup--outgroup differences in perceived covariation? This question brings together two rel- atively separate areas of research, one concerning perceptions of covariation and the other concerning perceptions of group homogeneity. Our theoretical analysis of this question leads us to propose two hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1996, Vol. 70, No. 3, 421--436 Copyright 1996by the American PsychologicalAssociation, Inc. 0022-3514/96/$3.00 421
Transcript
Page 1: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members: The Outgroup Covariation Effect

Patricia W. Linville, Gregory W. Fischer, and Carolyn Yoon Duke University

The authors show a new outgroup homogeneity bias--outgroup covariation. They investigated per- ceived covariation among the features describing group subtypes. Results support a familiarity co- variation effect. Those more familiar with a group perceive lower covariation among features of group members. Results also support an outgroup covariation effect. In cases in which people are less familiar with the outgroup, they perceive greater covariation among features of outgroup members. For age, in which young and old people were less familiar with the outgroup, both perceived greater covariation among features ofoutgroup subtypes. For occupation, in which undergraduates had less work experience than masters of business students, undergraduates perceived greater covariation among features of business subtypes. For gender, in which students were equally familiar with men and women, no covariation effect occurred. Familiarity mediated outgroup covariation effects.

According to the outgroup homogeneity hypothesis, people view members of their outgroup as more homogeneous than members of their ingroup. The concept of homogeneity has been operationalized in various ways, including measures of overall intragroup similarity (Mackie & Asuncion, 1990; Park & Rothbart, 1982), number of subtypes (Brewer & Lui, 1984; Linville, 1982; Park, Ryan, & Judd, 1992), dimensional com- plexity (Linville, 1982; LinviUe & Jones, 1980), and the vari- ability of single features such as intelligence or likability. The predominant focus in this literature has been on these measures of single-feature variability, which include the range, standard deviation, perceived differentiation (PaL number of attribute levels, and stereotypicality of a feature (e.g., Jones, Wood, & Quattrone, 1981; Kraus, Ryan, Judd, Hastie, & Park, 1993; Linville, Fischer, & Salovey, 1989; Park & Judd, 1990; Park & Rothbart, 1982). The typical finding is that people perceive their outgroup to be less variable along single features than their ingroup, although an ingroup homogeneity effect may occur when social identity needs are strong (for reviews, see Brewer, 1993; Linville, in press; Linville & Fischer, 1993a; Messick & Mackie, 1989; Mullen & Hu, 1989; Ostrom & Sedikides, 1992;

Patricia W. Linviile, Gregory W. Fischer, and Carolyn Yoon, Fuqua School of Business, Duke University. Carolyn Yoon is now at the Faculty of Management, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

This research was supported by research funds from the Fuqua School of Business. We thank the participants of the Duck Social Cog- nition Conference, June 1993, for their helpful feedback. We also thank Jennifer Escalas for her help in testing some of the participants.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Patri- cia W. LinviUe, Fuqua School of Business, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27708. Electronic mail may be sent via the Internet to [email protected].

Park, Judd, & Ryan, 1991; Sedikides & Ostrom, 1993; Simon, 1992).

In this article we take a different approach, focusing on per- ceived covariation among the features that describe the mem- bers or subtypes of a social group. It is limiting to assume that people only think about group members in terms of single fea- tures. Instead, we assume that the mental representation of a social group includes dusters of features, each describing a spe- cific group member or subtype (Linville, Salovey, & Fischer, 1986). For example, a student's mental representation of other students might include subtypes such as, "Nerds are studious, unsociable, and dress without style" and "Party animals are so- ciable, not studious, and dress well" The feature patterns that comprise such a representation of a social group implicitly en- code knowledge regarding covariation among the features that describe group members. In this example, studiousness and so- ciability are negatively correlated attributes of college students. Research indicates that people are sensitive to covariation in- formation in social contexts (Chapman & Chapman, 1967; Crocker, 1981; Hamilton, 1979, 1981; Kelley, 1967; Lewicki, 1986a; Schneider, 1973). Perceived covariation plays a key role in social judgment, enabling people to infer missing features, to predict behaviors from their correlated traits, to infer motives and traits from behavior, and to form hypotheses regarding causal relations.

This article investigates how group membership affects im- plicit perceptions of covariation among the features of group subtypes. In particular, are there ingroup--outgroup differences in perceived covariation? This question brings together two rel- atively separate areas of research, one concerning perceptions of covariation and the other concerning perceptions of group homogeneity. Our theoretical analysis of this question leads us to propose two hypotheses.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1996, Vol. 70, No. 3, 421--436 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/96/$3.00

421

Page 2: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

422 LINVILLE, FISCHER, AND YOON

First, according to the familiarity covariation hypothesis, greater familiarity with the members of a social group leads to lower perceived covariation among the features describing group members. This hypothesis is based on the following theo- retical considerations. Research shows that people begin with default expectations that lead them to overestimate the covari- ation among the traits of group members, often by overestimat- ing the degree of evaluative, schematic, or semantic consistency among traits. However, greater familiarity with a group exposes perceivers to a wider variety of members, including more coun- terexamples or exemplars whose feature patterns violate these expectations of consistency. This weakens the covariation among features in the memory representation of group mem- bers and subtypes.

Second, according to the outgroup covariation hypothesis, be- cause people generally are less familiar with their outgroup than their ingroup, they tend to perceive greater covariation among the features of outgroup members. Familiarity should act as a mediator of the outgroup covariation effect. If familiarity is a mediator, then an important exception to the outgroup covari- ation effect is likely in cases in which people are as familiar with their outgroup as they are with their ingroup. Here, the ten- dency to perceive greater covariation among features of out- group members may be reduced, if present at all.

In this section, we briefly review research that provides the basis for our theoretical assumptions and hypotheses. We also describe an approach to measuring perceived covariation that reflects implicit knowledge ofcovariation among the features of group members.

Representation of Category Knowledge

A basic assumption of this research is that the memory struc- tures representing a social group include clusters of features, each describing an exemplar of the group (a specific member or subtype). This assumption is consistent with multiple exemplar and multitrace memory models of categorization that have been used widely in cognitive psychology (e.g., Hintzman, 1986; Kruschke, 1992; McClelland & Rumelhart, 1985; Merlin, 1989; Medin& Schaffer, 1978; Nosofsky, 1986; E. E. Smith & Medin, 1981 ) and that recently have been used to model social perception and categorization (e.g., Linville, in press; Linville & Fischer, 1993a; Linville et al., 1989; E. R. Smith & Zarate, 1990, 1992).

For our purposes, an exemplar memory representation of so- cial groups has three important properties. First, it represents both instances and abstracted subtypes of category members. For example, one's mental representation of older people might include exemplars of specific individuals, such as "Roger's grandfather is white-haired, miserly, unreliable, highly opinion- ated, and ignores his grandchildren," and exemplars of ab- stracted subtypes, such as "Kindly grandmothers are generous, dependable, wise, good cooks, and spoil their grandchildren." Although multiple-exemplar models are sometimes interpreted as encoding only specific instance information, E. E. Smith and Medin ( 1981; Medin, 1989 ) and other proponents of exemplar models explicitly assume that category exemplars include ab- stracted subtypes, often hierarchically organized (e.g., robins as an abstracted subtype of birds, birds as a subtype of animals,

and so forth). People also organize knowledge about social groups into subtypes (Brewer & Lui, 1984; Linville, 1982; Park et al., 1~992; Taylor, 1981; Weber & Crocker, 1983). The ability to represent both individual members and subtypes of social groups is an important advantage of exemplar models (Linville & Fischer, 1993a; Linville et al., 1986).

Second, the representation of each category member or subtype includes a list of features. In such a representation of a social group, the list of features describing each exemplar may include one or more labels encoding group membership and individual or subtype identity. It also may include features describing character- istics such as physical features, personality traits, intellectual skills, typical behaviors, and demographic characteristics. Mental repre- sentations of social groups may include other types of information as well, such as images (Brewer; 1988) or abstractions about group-level characteristics (Judd & Park, 1988; Park & Hastie, 1987). However, in this article, we focus on perceptions of the attributes of subtypes within a social group.

Third, multiple exemplar models implicitly represent covaria- tion among features (Linville et al., 1986, 1989; E. E. Smith & Merlin, 1981 ). For instance, the two exemplars of elderly people in the prior example sugg~ that generosity and dependability are positively associated among elderly people. However, this does not necessarily imply that people are aware of this covariation or that they can articulate it when asked (Lewicki, 1986b ). Rather, pat- tern-matching processes based on similarity to known exemplars implicitly exploit this covariation information when classifying new exemplars (Merlin & Schaffer, 1978) and making other judg- ments based on similarity to known exemplars.

Other types ofmultitrace memory models also have these prop- erties. For example, in a distributed memory representation of ~ e r a l and specific information (McClelland & Rumelhart, 1985), knowledge of the specific instances of a category is repre- sented by a set of individual memory traces that are superimposed on one another in a set of connection strengths among shared nodes in a memory structure. In this formulation, abstractions about category prototypes and subtypes emerge automatically from the superimposition of traces on one another. Knowledge of covariation is represented by the connection strengths among patterns of nodes representing different properties of category exemplars.

Perceived Covariation Among the Traits of People

What default expectations do people have about covafiation among the traits of other people? One important finding is that people overestimate covariation among the traits of others (see Crocker; 1981, and Nisbett & Ross, 1980, for reviews of covada- tion biases). In the extreme, an illusory correlation bias arises, in which people perceive strong correlations where none exist (Chapman & Chapman, 1967; Hamilton, 1981; Hamilton & Gifford, 1976). In other cases, people overestimate the magnitude of weak correlations, often by overestimating the degree of evalu- ative or semantic consistency among the Waits of other people. For example, D'Andrade (1974) found stronger correlations among retrospective trait judgments by participants than among concur- rent trait judgments made by trained observers. Berman and Kenny (1976) found bias in both retrospective and concurrent

Page 3: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

OUTGROUP COVARIATION 423

trait ratings, both in the direction of perceiving greater semantic consistency than was present in the stimuli.

The reasons why people ~ t e the degree of correlation among the traits of other people are complex. Several lines of re- search suggest that people are predisposed to anticipate relatively strong correlations among the features of other people. The first is cognitive consistency theory (e.g., Abelson & Rosenberg, 1958; Heidet; 1958), according to which people are predisposed to expect positive waits to co-occur with other Ix~sitive traits and negative traits to co-occur with other negative trai~. Such expectations may bias interpretations of ambiguous behavior as well as memories of past event~ Research suggests that the evaluative (good-had) di- mension tends to be most important in person perception (Rosenberg & Sedlak, 1972). Thus, expectations of eva/uat/ve con- sistency serve as an important default assumption in person perception.

Second, according to implicit personality theory (Bruner & Tag- iuri, 1954; Schneide~ 1973), people use naive theories regarding what kinds of traits tend to occur with one another to infer the presence of one trait from knowledge of the presence of others. Ex- pectations of semantic consistency provide one basis for making such inferences (Rosenberg & Sedlak, 1972). Beliefs about the co- variation of traits may depend more on the semantic similarity of traits than on actual coramtion among traits in individuals that one has observed (Berman & Kenny, 1976; D'Andrade, 1974 ). Because of the prominent role of the evaluative dimension of meaning, this often creates a tendency to perceive evaluatively consistent covaria- tion among traits. In sonae cases, however, nmve person schemas may lead to default expectations of negative (evaluatively inconsistent ) correlations. For example, Lewicki (1982) f o u ~ that active people are perceiv~ to be demanding (passive people to be not demanding), and warm people are ~ to be dependent (cold people to be independent ). Thus, implicit personality theory may lead to default expectations of either strong positive or negative correlations among features. In most cases, though, default expec- tations favor positive correlations. Such expectations have been shown to have a strong impact on perceived covariation (e.g., Bet- man & Kenny, 1976; Trolier & Hamilton, 1986).

Hnally, memory biases also contribute to erroneous perceptions of covariation. For example, people show better recall for distinctive stimuli. Hamilton and Gifford (1976) h y p o ~ that negative stimuli are more distinctive than positive ones, and that members of a minority group are more distinctive than members of a majority group. As predicted, people overattributed negative behaviors to membership in the minority group, even though the ratio of the positive to negative behaviors was identical in the two groups (i.e., there was no correlation).

In short, a variety of psychological processes bias our ixrceptions of covariation. The preceding discussion indicates that people fre- quently overestimate the degree of covariation among the features of other people, eslx~ially in the direction of evaluative consistency. These considerations regarding mental representation and per- ceived covariation lead us to propose two new h y p o ~

Hypotheses Regarding G r o u p Membership , Familiarity, and Perceived Covaria t ion

Familiarity Covariation Hypothesis

According to the familiarity covariation hypothesis, greater familiarity with a social group leads to lower perceived covaria-

tion among the features of group members. There are several reasons why this is likely to be the case. First, as noted, people tend to overestimate the degree of covariation among the traits of other people. However, as one becomes more familiar with a social group, one encounters a wider variety of group members displaying a greater variety of patterns of traits (Linville, 1982; Linville & Jones, 1980). This exposes one to numerous coun- terexamples to the default assumption that traits are strongly correlated among members of the group. Such variety and counterexamples weaken the perceived covariation among the features of category members and subtypes represented in long- term memory.

Second, according to statistical sampling theory, the larger the (random) sample of category exemplars to which one is ex- posed, the weaker the correlations among the features of these exemplars will tend to be. Specifically, the absolute expected value of each correlation is a function of the square root of n/ (n - 1 ), where n is the number of exemplars to which one has been exposed (Winkler & Hays, 1975 ). Thus, small sample cor- relations will be too large in absolute magnitude, overestimating the size of both positive and negative correlations. For instance, with a sample of only two exemplars from a group, interattri- bute correlations will be either + 1 or - 1 for all pairs of attri- butes on which the two exemplars differ. As the number of ex- emplars in the sample grows, however, correlations will become more moderate in absolute value and converge toward the IX~ ulation correlations.

Third, when familiarity with a group is low, a higher propor- tion of one's knowledge of the group may be based on second- hand exemplars, or socially conveyed stereotypes about sub- types of the group (Linville & Fischer, 1993a; Park & Hastie, 1987). We suggest that stereotypic exemplars, such as these, are likely to be biased by generalized expectations of semantic or evaluative consistency. Thus, impressions based on these exem- plars will tend to overestimate covariation among the features of group members. As familiarity with a group increases, however, these biased second-hand exemplars constitute a smaller por- tion of one's knowledge base, leading to lower perceived covari- ation among features.

Outgroup Covariation Hypothesis

According to the outgroup covariation hypothesis, because people tend to be less familiar with their outgroup, they tend to perceive greater covariation among the features of outgroup members. In most cases, membership in a group leads to famil- iarity with a greater number and variety of individuals and sub- types in the group (Linville et al., 1989). When this is the case, the familiarity covariation hypothesis implies that ingroup members will tend to perceive less covariation among the fea- tures of group members than will outgroup members. A poten- tial exception arises in cases in which ingroup members are not more familiar with the group. For example, college students are roughly as familiar with members of the opposite gender as they are with members of their own gender (Linville et al., 1989). In such cases, ingroup--outgroup differences in perceived covaria- tion may be relatively weak, if present at all. Clearly, however, differential familiarity is neither a necessary nor a sufficient pre- condition for ingroup--outgroup differences in perceived covari-

Page 4: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

424 LINVILLE, FISCHER, AND YOON

ation. Other factors also contribute to perceived covariation-- for example, motivational or cognitive biases and the actual de- gree ofcovariation among features.

Measur ing Perceived Covar ia t ion

Our use of the phrase perceived covariation is somewhat spe- cial because we do not assume that people are consciously aware of the covariation that they perceive. A correlation may be pres- ent in the feature patterns stored in memory, yet the perceiver may have no conscious awareness that such a correlation is pres- ent (Lewicki, 1986b). Our measures of perceived covariation are designed to tap implicit knowledge of covariation that is present in the memory representation of a group. These mea- sures may be contrasted with explicit measures of perceived co- variation, in which the perceiver makes a direct judgment re- garding the strength and direction ofcovariation.

To measure perceived covariation, we asked participants to generate subtypes within a social group, then to describe each subtype by rating it on a list of traits. We used these trait ratings to compute two measures of each participant's implicit knowl- edge of the covariation among the traits of group subtypes. The first was the average absolute correlation (Avg [ R [), calculated by taking the absolute value of the correlation between each pair of traits, then averaging across pairs of traits. This measure reflects covariation per se. The second was the average correla- tion (Avg R) among pairs of traits, calculated by averaging the signed correlations between all pairs of traits. If all traits are scaled so that higher levels are more favorable, then this mea- sure reflects the degree to which traits covary in an evaluatively consistent fashion. If people tend to perceive positive corre- lations among traits, these two measures will be positively cor- related with one another.

Overview of Studies

Experiment 1 tests the familiarity covariation hypothesis us- ing age-based groups. Greater familiarity with an age group will be associated with lower perceived covariation among the fea- tures of group members. It also tests the outgroup covariation hypothesis. Because people tend to have less contact with those of their age outgroup, they will perceive greater covariation among the features of outgroup members. Finally, familiarity will act as a mediator of the outgroup covariation effect.

Experiment 2 tests whether there are ingroup-outgroup differences in perceived covariation for gender-based groups. Because college students are highly familiar with those of both genders and roughly as familiar with those of the opposite gen- der as they are with those of their own, ingroup--outgroup differences in perceived covariation will be relatively weak, if present at all.

Experiment 3 tests the familiarity covariation hypothesis by comparing the perceptions of undergraduate business students (with little business experience) with those of master of busi- ness administration (MBA) students (with 3-4 years of full- time business experience). At the group level, because under- graduate business students are legs familiar than MBA students with people in business, undergraduates will perceive greater covariation among the features of business subtypes. At the in-

dividual level, greater work experience will be associated with lower perceived covariation among features of business sub- types. Finally, amount of work experience will act as a mediator of the MBA-undergraduate difference in perceived covariation.

Expe r imen t 1

Both college students and elderly people generally interact more with those of their own age group (Linville et al., 1989). Thus, the outgroup covariation hypothesis predicts that college students will perceive greater covariation among the features of older people and that older people will perceive greater covaria- tion among the features of young people. In addition, the famil- iarity covariation hypothesis predicts that greater familiarity with an age group will be associated with lower perceived co- variation among the features of group members. Finally, famil- iarity will act as a mediator of the outgroup covariation effect.

Method

Participants Forty-two older adults (aged 65-75 years) and 41 college-aged stu-

dents (aged 18-21 years) participated in a stud~'tn" perceiving others. Both groups were roughly half male and half female. Participants were run in small groups of approximately 3-5 people. We have found that running participants in small groups is critical for several reasons. First, the main task involves generating subtypes of people within a particular social group. The task permits participants to create differing numbers of subtypes. In larger groups, participants tend to stop forming subtypes when others around them stop, thus artificially reducing the variance of this measure. Second, clarification may be needed while performing the task, which is easier to provide in smaller groups.

Each participant generated subtypes of people of only one age group, resulting in a 2 X 2 (Subject Age X Target Age) between-subjects facto- rial design. This between-subjects design reduced demand characteris- tics in this situation• For example, there was no pressure to produce similar subtypes for both age groups to avoid appearing prejudiced.

Procedure Participants were informed that the study was investigating how peo-

ple perceive other people. They first completed the covariation task and then completed a set of familiarity measures. The general instructions mentioned the following: (a) They could take as much time as needed on each task, so people would probably finish at different times; (b) all of their responses were completely anonymous and confidential; and (c) there were no right or wrong answers, only their own perceptions. So the best way to help was to be as honest as they could in their responses.

Subtypes covariation task. In this task, participants used a list of features to construct patterns of features describing various subtypes of college-aged or older persons. A booklet contained an instruction sheet followed by 10 identical sheets. Each sheet had a heading "people 65-75 years old" or "college-aged people," followed by a list of 18 personality features, t Each feature had three levels (e.g., lazy, average, and

1 The attributes included the following: unsociable to sociable, boring to interesting, unhappy to happy, unattractive to attractive, passive to active, no sense of humor to sense of humor, physically inactive to ac- tive, aimless to purposeful, intense to easy-going, not intellectual to in- tellectual, dependent to independent, lazy to motivated, not frank to frank, cold to warm, demanding to not demanding, easily discouraged to persistent, lacks self-confidence to self-confident, and not expressive to expressive of feelings. These features were chosen based on ratings made by a separate sample of young and older participants. We used Park and Rothbart's (1982) criteria to include some positive and nega-

Page 5: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

OUTGROUP

motivated). The bottom of the page contained a space for a label for the subtype they were describing. We gave participants the following written task instructions:

In this task, we are interested in seeing what traits you think go together in describing different types of people. We'd like you to think about people whose age is between 65 and 75 [ college-aged people]. Your task is to use a list of personal characteristics to con- struct patterns that describe different types of people aged 65 to 75 [ college-aged people].

You will create these patterns using the sheets of paper in this book- let. The first characteristic on the sheet ranges from "unsociable" to "sociable"; the second from "boring" to "interesting"; and so forth. You should interpret the middle level of each characteristic as moderate or average with respect to that characteristic. You will use a different response sheet to describe each type of person that comes to mind. To begin, think of a particular type of person, then go down the list of traits on the response sheet and circle the level ofcach trait that best describes the type of person in question. Also, please do not circle more than one level of a trait for any single type of person. After you have finished circling the list of traits that describe a particular type of person, please write down a descriptive label or phrase that describes this type of person.

Any questions? If not, please go ahead and construct as many types of 65- to 75-year-old people [college-aged people] as come to mind (up to 10 types). If you are straining to think of another type, that is probably a good time to stop creating more types.

Familiarity. Participants answered the following questions: Ap- proximately how many people aged 65-75 [ college-aged people ] would you consider acquaintances of yours? Approximately how many people aged 65-75 [college-aged people] do you know well? Approximately how many hours a month do you spend interacting on a regular basis with people aged 65-75 [college-aged people]? Finally, participants were given 3 rain to list the names of specific people from that age group whom they knew personally.

Measures of Perceived Covariation

Our theoretical framework emphasizes knowledge ofcovariation that is implicit in the memory representation of a social group. Thus, we used two measures of perceived covariation that reflect such implicit knowledge. Both measures are based on the subtypes task just de- scribed. The first measure is the average absolute correlation between pairs of attributes, which we denote by Avg I RI. This measure reflects the magnitude of correlations, regardless of their sign. Computing this measure, for a single individual involves the following steps: 1. Rescale all features so that higher attribute values reflect greater PO- sitivity. If an individual does not mark a level of a feature, assign a value of "average;' the scale midpoint, under the assumption that this is the most likely default value for a missing feature. Discard any individual with a high proportion of missing feature values (e.g., 15% or more). (Filling in missing features did not affect our results. See the discussion of scale use in the Results section of Experiment 1 ).2 2. For each pair of features, X and Y, compute the product-moment correlation, r~, between the feature values of the n subtypes generated by the participant, l fa feature has 0 variance across subtypes, all corre-

tive features that were more characteristic of people 65-75 years old, some positive and negative features that were more characteristic of col- lege-aged persons, and some features that were equally characteristic of both age groups.

COVARIATION 4 2 5

Table 1 Familiarity With Members of Age Ingroups and Outgroups

Familiarity measure Ingroup Outgroup F

Names listed in 3 n~in 21.4 6.4 86.9*** Contact hours per month 103.4 1.6 241.6"** No. of acquaintances 102.0 6.4 180.4*** No. known well 28.9 2.5 118.1"**

Note. Table entries for familiarity measures are geometric means. ***p < .0001.

lations involving this feature are indeterminate, so delete such features from this and subsequent computations. 3. Compute the absolute value, I rx~l, ofcach oftbe correlations com- puted in Step 2. 4. Compute Avg I R I by averaging the I r~l values computed in Step 3 across all pairs of attributes. This measure can assume values between 0 and 1. It reflects the strength of perceived covariation regardless of direction. The measure is com- puted separately for each individual.

The second measure is the average correlation between pairs of attri- butes, which we denote by Avg R. It is calculated in exactly the same fashion except that Step 3 is omitted. The Avg R measure can assume both positive and negative values, with an upper bound of + 1 and a lower bound o f - 1.3 Recall that Step 1 involves scaling all attributes so that high scores are positive. Thus, this measure reflects the degree to which attributes are perceived to covary in an evaluatively consistent fashion--that is, the extent to which positive levels of one attribute are perceived to occur with positive levels of another, and negative levels of one with negative levels of another. This measure is also calculated separately for each individual.

Results

We focus on tests o f the Par t i c ipan t Age × Target Age interac- t ion because they reflect differences in rat ings o f the ingroup versus outgroup. There were no significant gender effects, so these are no t reported.

Ingroup Versus Outgroup Familiarity

Our predictions for this experiment assume that people are more famil iar wi th the i r age ingroup t h a n outgroup. To test this assumpt ion , we e x a m i n e d the results o f four famil iar i ty mea- sures. Inspect ion o f the d is t r ibut ions o f these famil iar i ty mea- sures revealed t ha t all four were strongly skewed to the right. To avoid giving u n d u e weight to outliers, as well as to create more normal ly d is t r ibuted dependen t variables, we pe r fo rmed all sta- tistical tests after applying log t r ans fo rmat ions to these famil- iari ty measures . Table 1 displays the geometr ic means o f these

2 In this experiment, 4 older participants were deleted because they had 15% or more missing trait ratings. Two oftbese participants were in the young target group and 2 in the older target group conditions. Be- cause other participants rarely failed to rate an attribute, filling in mid- values for missing features had a negligible effect on these measures.

3 With more than two attributes, the lower bound will be greater than - 1 because of the mathematical constraints on correlations. For in- stance, with three variables, it is impossible for all three pairwise corre- lations to assume values o f - 1.

Page 6: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

426 LINVILLE, FISCHER, AND YOON

measures (obtained by converting the means of the log mea- sures back into their original units). In each case, people re- ported greater familiarity with members of their own age group, multivariate F(4, 71 ) = 69.9, p < .00 I.

Three-minute listing of group members. In a 3-min period, people listed a greater number of members that they knew per- sonally when generating names ofingroup than outgroup mem- bers, F( 1, 74) = 86.9, p < .000 I. Young participants listed more young people (p < .0001 ), and older participants listed more older people (p < .0001 ).

Number of hours spent with group members. People esti- mated a greater number of hours per month spent interacting on a regular basis with ingroup than outgroup members, F( 1, 74) = 241.6, p < .0001. Young people interacted more with young people (p < .0001 ), and older people interacted more with older people (p < .0001 ).

Number of acquaintances and well-known group members. People estimated a greater number of acquaintances in the in- group than in the outgroup, F( 1,75 ) = 180.4, p < .0001. Young people estimated a greater number of young acquaintances (p < .0001 ), and older people estimated a greater number of older acquaintances (p < .0001 ). People also estimated a greater number of people they know well in the ingroup than in the outgroup, F( 1, 75) = 118.1, p < .0001. Young participants knew more young people well (p < .0001 ), and older partici- pants knew more older people well (p < .0001 ).

There were three main effects of less theoretical interest. Young participants estimated more acquaintances (p < .05) and more contact hours (p < .0001 ). Also, participants had more contact hours with young people (p < .0001 ).

Number of Subtypes

In the covariation task, both young and older people gener- ated slightly more subtypes for their ingroup than for their out- group (M--- 8.6 vs. 7.9, respectively), F ( I , 75) = 3.35, p = .07. Frequently mentioned subtypes of young persons included "politically active" "party animal" "sorority or fraternity types," "athletes," and "studious types?' Frequently mentioned subtypes of older people included "volunteers," "travelers," "athletic buffs," "grandmothers" "nursing home residents," and "conservatives" Number of subtypes generated was sig- nificantly positively correlated with two of the four familiarity measures: number of members listed in 3 min (r -- .22, p < .05) and number of members one knows well (r = .24, p < .05). Also, a main effect of less theoretical interest indicated that older people generated more subtypes (p < .001 ).

Outgroup Covariation Effect

According to the outgroup covariation hypothesis, people tend to perceive greater covariation among the features of out- group members. Results for both the Avg I R [ and Avg R mea- sures provided support for this hypothesis (see Figure 1 ).

Average absolu(e perceived correlations. For the Avg I R I mea- sure, reflecting covariation per se, people perceived more absolute covariation among features of the outgroup than the ingroup, F( 1, 75) = 6.69, p < .01. Young participants perceived more eovaria- tion among features of older people (p < .01 ),4 and older partici-

c 0 u

0 0 "0 =>

o

0.55

0.50 ¸

0.45

0.40

0.35

0.30

0.25

0.20

Y

J Ingroup Outg'roup

• Average IRI

Average R

Figure 1. PereeAved covariation among the features ofingroup and out- group members: Outgroup covariation effects for age in Experiment 1.

pants perceived more covariation among features of young people (p < .09). The outgroup covariation effect remained significant when controlling for the number of subtypes generated (p = .04) and the favorability of ratings (p < .01 ).

Average perceived correlations. Results for the Avg R measure, reflecting evaluative consistency, also supported the outgroup co- variation hypothesis (see Figure 1 ). People perceived more covari- ation among features of the outgroup than the ingroup, F( 1, 75) = 5.21, p < .02. Young participants perceived more covariation among features of older people (p < .001 ), and older participants perceived more covariation among features of young people (p < .40). The outgroup covariation effect remained significant when controlling for the number of subtypes generated (p = .01 ) and the favorability of ratings (p < .05).

In addition to these ingroup--outgroup effects, there were two main effects of less theoretical interest. Older participants per- ceived more covariation than did young participants (p < .05), and participants perceived more covariation among traits of older than younger people (p < .05).

We can rule out one potential artifactual cause of the out- group covariation effect. Suppose that participants were more likely to assign "average" ratings (the scale midpoint) or to leave features unspecified (which then are replaced by the scale midpoint) when describing outgroup subtypes. Could this ac- count for our findings? The answer is no on two grounds. First, such a response scale bias would lead to lower variance for out- group members and thus would tend to lead to lower, not higher, correlations among attributes for the outgroup. Second, in em- pirically comparing the use of scales, we found no significant

4 Because the familiarity covariation and the outgroup covariation hypotheses make theory-based, a priori, directional predictions, one- tailed tests are appropriate for testing planned contrasts based on these hypotheses (Winer, 1971 ),

Page 7: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

OUTGROUP COVARIATION 427

differences in the number of "average" ratings used in describ- ing ingroups versus outgroups.

Comparing absolute perceived covariation and perceived evalu- ative consistency. Scores for the Avg I RI measure were larger than fortheAvgR measure (see Figure 1), t(78) -- 12.7, p < .0001. This implies that people perceived some correlations among pairs of features to be negative or evaluatively inconsistent. If all pairwise correlations were positive, then Avg R = Avg I R 1. Nevertheless, the Avg I R I and Avg R measures ofcovariation were highly correlated across participants, r = .76, p < .0001.

Mediators of Perceived Covariation

Familiarity and perceived covariation. According to the fa- miliarity covariation hypothesis, greater familiarity with a group leads to lower perceived covariation among the features describing members of that group. To test this hypothesis, we conducted a set of regression analyses in which we used famil- iarity to predict Avg I R I and Avg R. We formed a familiarity index by summing standardized values of all four familiarity measures. (A principal-components analysis revealed only one significant familiarity factor, which accounted for 82% of the variance.) This familiarity index was significantly negatively re- lated to both the Avg I R[ (standardized ~ = -0 .28, p < .01, two-tailed) and Avg R (standardized B = -0 .25 , p < •05, two- tailed) measures of perceived covariation. Examination of the individual familiarity measures revealed significant standard- ized regression coefficients for three of the measures--number of names listed in 3 rain, number of contact hours, and number of acquaintances (B ranging from -0 .23 , p < .05, to -0 .31, p < .01 ). The fourth measuremnumber of group members that one knows well--was also significantly negatively related to Avg I R I (# = -0 .21, p < .05) but was weaker for Avg R (/~ = -0 .16) . The negative relationship between familiarity and perceived co- variation remained significant when we controlled for age- group effects---the standardized regression coefficients and sig- nificance levels were essentially unchanged. It also remained significant when we controlled for the number of subtypes gen- erated: for Avg I RI,/~ -- -0 .28 , p < .01; for Avg R,/~ = -0 .21, p < .05• In short, familiarity was negatively related to perceived covariation.

To test whether familiarity satisfied the other properties of a mediator of perceived covariation, we adopted the approach used by Park et al. ( 1992, pp. 558-560) in their tests of media- tors ofoutgroup homogeneity effects. First, a potential mediator should be related to the effect. As we showed earlier, participants were significantly more familiar with their ingroup. Further- more, as we have just seen, familiarity was significantly nega- tively related to perceived covariation. Thus, this criterion was also satisfied.

Finally, when differences in the mediating variable are con- trolled, the effect should be substantially weakened (Baron & Kenny, 1986; Judd & Kenny, 1981 ). In the present context, the outgroup covariation effect should be weaker when we control for differences in familiarity. Our results supported this predic- tion. Controlling for the familiarity index, the previously sig- nificant outgroup covariation effect became nonsignificant for both Avg I R[ and Avg R (see Table 2).5 In short, these results support the hypothesis that familiarity with a social group is a

Table 2 F Values for the Age Outgroup Covariation Effect Controlling for Potential Mediators

Perceived covariation measure

Covariate Average R Average I RI

No covariate 5.21 * 6.69** Familiarity index 1.16 0.64 No. of subtypes 5.84** 4.33*

Note. Table entries are F values for ingroup versus outgroup differ- ences in perceived covariation (outgroup covariation effect), first with no covariate then with familiarity and number of subtypes separately introduced as covariates. A weakened F value supports the hypothesis that the covariate acts as a mediator of the outgroup covariation effect. *p <.05. **p < .01.

mediator of perceived covariation among the features of group members.

Number of subtypes as a mediator. We also investigated the possibility that the number of subtypes generated acted as a me- diator of the outgroup covariation effect. Number of subtypes was significantly negatively correlated with the Avg ] R I mea- sure, reflecting covariation per se ( r = - .33 , p < .01 ), but not with the Avg R measure, reflecting evaluative consistency (r = • 1 l, ns). (Note that the latter correlation is positive, not nega- tive, as would be required by the mediator hypothesis.) Further- more, the outgroup covariation effect remained significant for both Avg I R [ and Avg R when controlling for number of sub- types, indicating that number of subtypes was not a mediator of the effect (see Table 2). In short, number of subtypes generated was significantly negatively related to Avg I R I but not to Avg R, and it was not a mediator of ingroup and outgroup differences for either covariation measure.

Ingroup Favorability

To measure the positivity of the subtypes generated, we formed a favorability index for each participant by first calculating the mean feature rating for each subtype created and then averaging these mean ratings across the subtypes. Results for this favorability index showed evidence ofingronp favoritism, F( 1, 75) = 8.45, p = .005. Older people generated more favorable old than young subtypes (M--- 2.36 vs. 2.24 on a 1 to 3 scale, p < .01 ), and young people generated more favorable young than old subtypes ( M = 2.23 vs. 2.14, p < .05). Controlling for number of subtypes, the ingroup favoritism effect remained significant, p < .01. Favorabil- ity of the subtypes generated for an age group was positively corre-

5 The three familiarity measures that were significantly related to per- ceived covariation--number of names listed in 3 rain, number of con- tact hours, and number of acquaintances--all showed strong media- tional patterns (F values for the outgroup covariation effect when con- trolling for familiarity ranged from 0.54 to 2.60, ns). The number of group members that one knows well, the weakest measure in the earlier regression analyses, showed a weaker mediation effect. Controlling for this measure, the outgroup covariation effect was weakened for Avg [ R ], F( 1, 74 ) = 3.01, n s, but not for the Avg R measure,/7( 1, 74) = 3.91, p < .05.

Page 8: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

428 LINVILLE, FISCHER, AND YOON

lated with the familiarity index for that age group, r = .32, ps < .01. In addition to the ingroup favoritism effect, an age-group main effect indicated that older people were more favorable in their ratings (p < .01 ).

Discussion

This study produced three main findings regarding perceived covariation among the features of age-group members. First, as predicted by the outgroup eovariation hypothesis, people per- ceived greater covariation among attributes of their age out- group. Significant effects occurred for both the Avg R measure (reflecting evaluative consistency) and the Avg [R[ measure (reflecting correlation per se). These effects remained signifi- cant even when controlling for favorability and number of sub- types. Second, as predicted by the familiarity covariatlon hy- pothesis, both the Avg R and Avg [ R I measures of perceived covariation were negatively related to familiarity. Our new mea- sure of familiarity, number of names listed in 3 min, was the strongest correlate ofAvg [RI and the second strongest of Avg R. This measure uses availability in memory as a proxy for fa- miliarity. Third, when we controlled for the effects of familiar- ity, a proposed mediator, the outgroup covariation effect be- came weak and statistically nonsignificant. Together, these three findings support the hypothesis that differential familiarity is a mediator of the outgroup covariation effect.

Expe r imen t 2

This study compares how men and women perceive covaria- tion among features characterizing men and women. If differ- ential familiarity is an important mediator of ingroup versus outgroup differences in perceived covariation, then gender- based groups may be a special case. Although gender is an im- portant and salient group membership variable among college students, differences in familiarity and contact are relatively small. College students report high and approximately equal levels of familiarity with people of both genders (Linville et al., 1989). Thus, differences in perceived covariation for gender may be relatively small, if present at all, compared with cases in which there are substantial differences in familiarity.

Method

Participants

Thirty male and 33 female undergraduates participated in a study on perceiving others. Students were run in small groups. Each student generated subtypes of people of only one gender, resulting in a 2 × 2 (Participant Gender × Target Gender) between-subjects factorial design.

Procedure

The cover story, general instructions, and covariation task were sim- ilar to those in Experiment 1. The booklet contained 15 sheets, each with a heading "male undergraduates [ female undergraduates]" and a list of 17 personality features, each with three levels (e.g., assertive, av- erage, and unassertive). The features were chosen from pretesting data using Park and Rothbart's (1982) criteria to include some features that were more characteristic of men, some that were more characteristic

of women, and some that were equally characteristic of both genders. 6 Students constructed subtypes of male and female undergraduates at their university by selecting appropriate levels of the 17 features. The bottom of the page contained a space for a label for the type of person they were describing. Finally, students completed familiarity measures in which they estimated the number of their male friends, close male friends, female friends, and close female friends.

Results

Ingroup Versus Outgroup Familiarity

Inspection of the distributions of the number of male friends and number of female friends revealed that both were highly skewed to the right. Thus, we used the same procedure as in Experiment 1. We performed all statistical tests with respect to log-transformed frequency estimates and converted back to geometric means to summarize data. There was a slight ten- dency to have more friends of one's own gender, but this ten- dency did not approach statistical significance: for number of friends, F( 1, 53) = 0.28, ns; for number of close friends, F( 1, 54) = 0.44, ns. 7 Men and women did not significantly differ in their number of male friends (M = 28.8 vs. 20.9, ns), close male friends (M = 4.6 vs. 3.6, ns), female friends (M = 23.8 vs. 27.3, ns), or close female friends (M = 3,3 vs. 5.3, ns). Moreover, 48% of the men and 38% of the women reported hav- ing as many or more friends of the opposite gender as of their own and 42% of the men and 41% of the women reported having as many or more close friends of the opposite gender. In short, these results suggest that college students have a relatively large number of friends of both genders and are almost as familiar with the opposite gender as they are with their own.

Number of Subtypes

In the covariation task, both men and women generated ap- proximately the same number of subtypes for their ingroup (M = 9.8 ) as their o utgroup (M = 10.0), F( 1, 59 ) = 0.16, n s. Men did not create more male than female subtypes (ns), and women did not create more female than male subtypes (ns). Students generated very similar subtypes when describing either men or women. Examples of frequently generated subtypes in- cluded "jock," "preppie," "pre-med," "radical," "party-goer," "gay," and "artsie or theater type?' Number of subtypes gener- ated was not significantly correlated with either familiarity measure (rs = .01 ).

6 The attributes included the following: unmotivated to motivated, unassertive to assertive, unhappy to happy, unsociable to sociable, im- mature to mature, unexpressive to expressive of feelings, unintelligent to intelligent, unattractive to attractive, academically irresponsible to responsible, boring to interesting, unpretentious to pretentious, no sense of humor to sense of humor, loud to quiet, unambitious to ambi- tious, not studious to studious, physically inactive to active, and super- ficial to genuine. Each attribute had three levels, with average as the middle level.

7 Performing statistical tests on nontransformed measures also pro- duced no ingroup-outgroup differences in either number of friends or close friends.

Page 9: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

OUTGROUP COVARIATION 429

Outgroup Covariation Effect

Average absolute perceived correlations. The Avg I R [ mea- sure, reflecting covariation among features per se, showed no ingroup versus outgroup differences, F( 1, 59) = 0.64, ns. In fact, perceived covariation wasalmost identical across both gen- der groups and not in the direction of greater outgroup covaria- tion (see Figure 2). Men did not perceive more covariation among features of men than women, F( 1, 28) = 1.52, ns; and women did not perceive more covariation among features of women than men, F( 1, 31 ) = 0.49, ns. The ingroup-outgroup effect remained nonsignificant when we controlled for number of subtypes generated and the favorability of the ratings, Fs < 1.

Average perceived correlations. Results for Avg R, reflecting evaluative consistency, also showed no evidence of greater out- group covariation, F( 1, 59) = 0.0, ns. Again, perceived covari- ation was almost identical across both gender groups, and the pattern of results was not even in the direction of greater out- group covariation (see Figure 2). Men did not perceive more covariation among features of men than women, F < 1; and women did not perceive more covariation among features of women than men, F < 1. When controlling for number of sub- types generated and the favorability of the ratings, the effect re- mained nonsignificant, F < 1.

Comparing average and absolute average perceived covaria- tion. As in Experiment 1, Avg I R I scores were larger than Avg R scores, and the difference was even larger than in Experiment 1, t(63) = 28.5, p < .0001. The correlation between the two measures was r = . 16, ns. Both findings indicate that students perceived many correlations among pairs of features to be neg- ative, thus evaluatively inconsistent, for both the ingroup and outgroup.

Perceived covariation among stereotypic versus nonstereo- typic features. As a check on the possibility that greater out- group covariation might occur only for items stereotypic for

o

0 0

g.

0.45 ̧

0.40

0.35,

0.30'

0.25'

0.20"

0.15"

0.10:

0.05

0.00

D 0

Ingroup Outgroup

l Average IRI

Average R

Figure 2. Perceived covariation among features ofingroup and outgroup members: Outgroup covariation effects for gender in Experiment 2.

gender, we repeated the analyses on an index of gender-stereo- typic features and an index of gender-nonstereotypic features according to criteria of Park and Rothbart (1982). The effects remained nonsignificant for both stereotypic as well as nonste- reotypic features.

In short, a variety of analyses revealed no significant in- group-outgroup differences in either perceived covariation or familiarity for gender groups among college students.

Ingroup Favoritism

Results for the favorability index showed no evidence of in- group favoritism, F < 1, even when controlling for the number of subtypes. Instead, both men and women were more favorable toward women, F( 1, 59) --- 4.52, p < .04.

Discussion

Ingroup-outgroup differences in perceived covariation should be relatively weak when familiarity with both groups is roughly equal. The present data supported this. Our college stu- dents were roughly equally familiar with students of both gen- ders, and male and female students perceived almost identical covariation among the features of their gender ingroup and out- group. This finding is consistent with the hypothesis that famil- iarity plays an important role in perceived covariation. In the absence of familiarity differences, differences in perceived co- variation are also weak. This finding for perceived covariation parallels earlier results regarding perceived single-feature vari- ability among men and women. These studies revealed either no outgroup homogeneity effect for gender (Linville et al., 1989; Lorenzi-Cioldi, Eagly, & Stewart, 1995) or a reliable but rela- tively small effect (Park & Judd, 1990).

Exper iment 3

This experiment investigates how amount of experience in a domain affects perceptions of covariation. The domain studied was types of people in business, and the measure of experience was years of work experience in business. At a group level, we compared the perceptions of MBA students with those of stu- dents in an undergraduate business course. Because MBA stu- dents have more years of business experience, they are likely to have encountered more varied types of people and roles in business settings. Thus, the familiarity covariation hypothesis predicts that they will generate subtypes of business people that display lower covariation among features than those generated by undergraduates. At the individual level, those with greater work experience will perceive lower covariation among business subtypes. This prediction involves a new operationalization of familiarity as years of experience in a social domain as opposed to self-reports of amount of contact with a group. In addition, it shifts the emphasis from ingroup versus outgroup member- ship to group and individual differences in expertise regarding a social group.

Method

Participants

Twenty-two undergraduates in a business course and 20 MBA stu- dents participated in a study on impressions of people. Students were

Page 10: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

430 LINVILLE, FISCHER, AND YOON

run in small groups of 3 to 5 people. Each student generated subtypes of people in business, resulting in a one-factor (high vs. low experience in business) between-subjects design. Standardized test scores for the two populations in¢lieate that they are similar in general intelligence.

Procedure

The general instructions were similar to those in Experiment 1. Stu- dents completed two tasks---a covariation task and then a measure of business experience.

Subtypes covariation task. In a task similar to the one in Experi- ment 1, students used a list of features to construct patterns describing various subtypes of people in business settings. A booklet contained 10 sheets, each with a list of 14 features) Each feature had three levels (e.g., disorganized, average, and organized). The features were chosen from a pretest with MBA and undergraduate business students to rep- resent a variety of attributes relevant in a business setting. Students pro- vided a label for each subtype they generated. The instructions began as follows (see Experiment 1 for the complete instructions for forming subtypes that followed this introduction):

We are interested in seeing what traits or characteristics you think go together in describing different types of people in a job setting. Because not everyone is alike, we are interested in the various ways that you think traits go together to describe various types of people in organizations. Your task is to use a list of characteristics to con- struct various patterns of characteristics, each pattern describing a different type of person.

We checked to verify that the subtypes generated did describe types of people encountered in business settings. Four students ( 1 MBA and 3 undergraduates) were dropped because they misinterpreted the sub- types task. They formed types of occupations (e.g., doctor, politician, kindergarten teacher, and actor) rather than types of people encoun- tered in work settings (see examples below).

Background sheet. Students listed their gender, age, nationality, col- lege major or MBA concentration, and work experience since high school graduation. There were no significant gender effects, so these are not reported.

Results

Number of Subtypes

Undergraduate and MBA students did not generate signifi- cantly different numbers of business subtypes (6.8 vs. 7.5), t (36) = 1.3, ns. Undergraduate business students and MBA students generated similar subtypes of people in business. Some of the frequently ment ioned subtypes by both groups included "leader" "9 to 5-ers," "office clown," "ambit ious CEOs," " team players," "floundering underachievers," "bossy bosses; ' "dele- gators," "smart slackers" and "fast-track high achievers." Number of subtypes was negatively correlated with the Avg I R I measure ( r = - . 4 2 ) but not with the Avg R measure ( r = .09 ). Thus, we used it as a covariate in all analyses of the Avg [ R I measure, and for consistency, we also included it in analyses of the AvgR measure (although it had no effect).

Group Differences in Work Experience and Perceived Covariation

In previous experiments, we measured familiarity with group members in terms of self-reported a m o u n t of contact with the

t -

o

.g O o

o eL

0.55"

0.50"

0.45'

0.40-

0.35.

0.30"

0.25.

0.20

J

j MI~A Undergrad

= Average IRI

Average R

Figure 3. Perceived covariation among features of types of business people by Inaster of business administration and undergraduate busi- ness students in Experiment 3.

group. In this experiment, we operationalized familiarity as amoun t of experience in business. This was defined at both a group level (MBA vs. undergraduate students) and individual level ( number of years of work experience).

Work experience. Inspection ofthe distribution of the num- ber of years of work experience revealed that it was skewed to the right. Thus, we followed the same procedure used in Exper- iments l and 2, performing statistical tests with respect to log- transformed work experience and using geometric means to summarize results. MBA students had significantly more work experience than did undergraduate business students (3.36 vs. 0.67 years), t (36) = 7.25, p < .0001. Thus, we can view the MBA versus undergraduate business student distinction as one of high versus low experience in business.

Average perceived correlations. Results for the Avg R mea- sure, reflecting evaluative consistency, supported the familiarity covariation hypothesis. As predicted, MBA students perceived lower covariation among features of subtypes of business people than did undergraduate business students, t (35) = 2.13, p = .02 (see Figure 3). The effect remained significant when we con- trolled for favorability of ratings as well as number of subtypes (p < .03).

Average absolute perceived correlations. Results for the Avg [ R [ measure, reflecting covariation per se, also supported the familiarity covariation hypothesis. MBA students perceived less

s The attributes included the following: low to high analytical skills, communicates poorly to well, low to high verbal skills, irresponsible to responsible, lazy to hard working, gets along poorly to well with others, disorganized to organized, lacks self-confidence to self-confident, inde- cisive to decisive, low to high leadership, impatient to patient, unimagi- native to imaginative, dependent to independent thinker, and easily dis- tracted to focused. Each attribute had three levels, with average as the middle level.

Page 11: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

OUTGROUP COVARIATION 431

absolute covariation among features of business people than did undergraduate business students, t ( 35) = 1.77, p = .04 (see Fig- ure 3). The results were essentially identical when controlling for the favorability of the ratings as well as the number of sub- types (p -- .05).

Comparing evaluative consistency versus absolute correla- tion. The correlation between the Avg R and Avg [ R I mea- sures across all participants was r = .58, p < .0001. As in both previous experiments, Avg I R I scores were higher than Avg R scores, t(38) = 8.97, p < .0001, implying that students per- ceived some pairs of features to be negatively correlated, thus evaluatively inconsistent.

Mediation o f Perceived Covariation

Work experience and perceived covariation. The analyses just reported indicated that group-level differences in the expe- rience of MBA and undergraduate business students were asso- ciated with different levels of perceived covariation, as predicted by the familiarity covariation hypothesis. This hypothesis also implies that individual differences in years of work experience should be related to perceived eovariation among the features of business subtypes. As predicted, number of years of work experience was negatively related to Avg R, reflecting evaluative consistency (8 = -0.32, p < .03), and to Avg I RI, reflecting covariation per se (# = -0.23, p = .06). The results were essen- tially identical when controlling for favorability as well as num- ber of subtypes: for Avg R, p = .02; for Avg I R I, P --- .06. Thus, we find clear support for the familiarity covariation hypothesis at the individual as well as group level.

Work experience as a mediator. As already shown, years of work experience meets two properties of a mediator of the MBA-undergraduate difference in perceived covariation. MBA students had significantly more work experience, and years of work experience was significantly negatively related to per- ceived covariation. As a final test of whether familiarity was a mediator of the group-level differences, we introduced years of work experience as a covariate in the group-level analyses. The mediation hypothesis implies that the group-level differences should become smaller and less significant when controlling for the mediating variable (Baron & Kenny, 1986; Judd & Kenny, 1981 ). As predicted, the MBA-undergraduate differences were smaller and nonsignificant when controlling for work experi- ence: For Avg R, controlling for work experience reduced the effect from t(35) = 2.13, p = .02, to t(34) = 0.92, ns; for Avg I RI, it reduced the effect from t(35) = 1.77, p = .04, to t(34) = 0.83, ns. The same pattern emerged when we controlled for favorability as well as number of subtypes when performing these tests, ts = 0.55 and 0.66, ns. In short, we found clear sup- port for the hypothesis that years of work experience was a me- diator of the MBA-undergraduate difference in perceived co- variation among the features of people in business.

Number of subtypes as a mediator We also investigated whether the number of subtypes ~naerated by students acted as a mediator of the MBA-undergraduate differences in perceived covariation. The results v~re generally negative. First, number of subtypes generated did not differ significantly between MBA stu- dents and undergraduate students and thus is unlikely to account for any differences between the two groups. Second, number of

subtypes was negatively related to Avg [RI (/~ -- -0.42, p < .01 ), but it was not significantly related to Avg R (8 = 0.10, p = .60). Most important, the MBA-undergraduate covariation effect was significant when controlling for number of subtypes (p -- .02 for Avg R; p = .04 for Avg I RI), indicating that number of subtypes was not a mediator of the covariation effect. In short, we found no support for the hypothesis that number of subtypes ~rnerated mediated the MBA-undergraduate difference.

Favorability

Results for the favorability index, reflecting the positivity of the subtypes generated, showed no differences for MBA (M = 2.24) versus undergraduate (M = 2.21 ) students, t(36) = 0.75, n s. The effect remained nonsignificant when controlling for the number of subtypes generated. There was no relationship be- tween favorability and number of years of work experience (r = - .Ol) .

Discussion

In this experiment, we used years of work experience to opera- tionalize familian'ty with the subtypes of people in business. This experiment provided both group-level (MBA vs. undergraduate students) and individual-level tests of the familiarity covariation hypothesis. At the group level, MBA students, who had more work experience than undergraduate business students, perceived less covariation among the features of business subtypes than did un- dergraduates. At the individual level, years of experience in busi- ness was negatively related to perceived covariation among the fea- tures of business subtypes. Furthermore, when years of experience was statistically controlled, there was no significant difference be- tween undergraduate and MBA students in perceived covariation. This pattern of results is consistent with the hypothesis that famil- iarity (experience) was an important mediator of the group-level differences in perceived covariation by MBA and undergraduate students. Thus, the familiarity covariation hypothesis was sup- ported even in a domain in which there is no clear ingroup--out- group distinction.

Genera l Discuss ion

Main Findings and Theoretical Implications

Three studies investigated implicit perceptions ofcovariation among the features describing subtypes of age, gender, and oc- cupational groups. The data provided strong support for the fa- miliarity covariation hypothesis. Those more familiar with an age group perceived lower covariation among the features of subtypes of that age group. Similarly, at an individual level, those with more work experience perceived lower covariation among the features of business subtypes. At a group level, MBA students, who have more work experience than undergraduates, perceived lower covariation among the features of business sub- types than did undergraduates. Furthermore, when we con- trolled for the level of individual work experience, the MBA- undergraduate difference became nonsignificant, as one would expect if work experience was mediating the MBA-undergrad- uate difference.

The data also provided strong support for the outgroup co-

Page 12: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

432 LINVILLE, FISCHER, AND YOON

variation hypothesis. In cases in which people are less familiar with the outgroup, they perceive greater covariation among the features of outgroup than ingroup members. For age groups, in which both young and older participants were less familiar with the outgroup, participants perceived greater covariation among the features of outgroup subtypes. This outgroup covariation effect became nonsignificant when we controlled for familiarity with the group, a finding consistent with the hypothesis that differential familiarity is a mediator of the effect. On the other hand, in the absence of a familiarity difference, ingroup--out- group differences in perceived covariation may be weaker, if present at all. For gender groups, in which men and women were roughly equally familiar with both genders, we found no sig- nificant differences in perceived covariation.

Finally, both the outgroup covariation and familiarity covari- ation hypotheses were supported using two conceptually different measures of perceived covariation--an absolute aver- age correlation measure (reflecting covariation per se) and an average correlation measure (reflecting evaluatively consistent covariation).

These findings regarding the link between familiarity and perceived covariation have parallels in research on perceived single-feature variability. For example, for Irish and American students, perceived variability of the outgroup was significantly positively correlated with both the number of outgroup mem- bers they had met (r = .49) and the number they knew well (r -- .40; Linville & Fischer, 1993a). Similarly, for Hindu and Muslim students, amount of contact with the outgroup was the best predictor of perceived outgroup variability (Islam & Hew- stone, 1993). Thus, it appears that those with greater contact or familiarity with a group perceive greater single-feature variabil- ity and lower covariation among features.

Why do greater familiarity and ingroup membership lead to lower perceived covariation? Previous research has shown that people tend to overestimate the degree of correlation among the features describing other people, in part because they overesti- mate the evaluative consistency among features. Thus, they are predisposed to overestimate the covariation among the features describing group subtypes. As they are exposed to more mem- bers of the group, however, they encounter more exemplars that violate their default expectations about feature correlations. With enough experience, these counterexamples weaken the perceived covariation among the features of group members and subtypes. Thus, greater familiarity leads to lower perceived covariation. Furthermore, because people are usually more fa- miliar with their ingroup, they tend to perceive less covariation among the features of ingroup members. This tendency will be weaker in eases in which people are not more familiar with their ingroup.

Clearly, however, differential familiarity is not a necessary precondition for differences in perceived covariation. Because other factors also contribute to perceived covariation, ingroup-- outgroup differences may occur in the absence of familiarity differences. Other possible mechanisms include differences be- tween groups in the actual level of covariation among features (Berman & Kenny, 1976; Nisbett & Ross, 1980 ), greater incen- tives to make distinctions among ingroup members (Linville, 1982; Linville et al., 1986, 1989), and greater attention to the

individuating features of ingroup members and common fea- tures ofoutgroup members (Park & Rothbart, 1982). By mak- ing stronger distinctions among ingroup members and by focus- ing more on their individuating features, perceivers may be more likely to encode feature patterns that violate a priori ex- pectations of evaluative consistency.

Measuring Perceived Covariation

Implicit Versus Explicit Measures

Our measures of perceived covariation do not assume that people are consciously aware of covariation among the features of group members. Rather, our measures are based on implicit knowledge of covariation present in the feature patterns that represent group subtypes in memory. This approach may be contrasted with explicit measures of covariation in which the perceiver is asked to make direct judgments regarding the strength and direction of covariation. For example, in prelimi- nary research on this topic, we experimented with a 2 × 2 boxes task in which people were asked to distribute 100 category members over a table defined by the four possible combinations of high and low values of various pairs of attributes (e.g., lazy vs. motivated × not intellectual vs. intellectual). We obtained results similar to those reported for the implicit measures used in this article, but the results were much weaker and less consis- tent across pairs of attributes.

Our implicit approach to measuring perceived covariation has at least two advantages. First, it may reflect more accurately the correlations present in the memory structures guiding social judgments rather than metabeliefs about the correlations among features. Lewicki (1982, 1986b) has shown that judg- ments are affected by knowledge of covariation that people are unable to articulate. Second, because it is not obvious what properties our measures tap, people cannot easily engage in self- presentational strategies that will make them appear in a more favorable light.

Average Versus Absolute Average Perceived Covariation

Our measures make the important distinction between aver- age absolute perceived covariation (measured by Avg I R I ) and average perceived covariation (measured by Avg R). The first measure reflects degree of covariation per se, whereas the sec- ond reflects the degree of evaluatively consistent perceived co- variation. This distinction provides a potential tool for distin- guishing among competing hypotheses regarding perceived co- variation biases. To our knowledge, no prior studies of perceived covariation among the features of social group mem- bers have made the distinction captured by our Avg R and Avg I R I measures.

Relations to Previous Measures and Findings

Several previous studies have used measures similar to ours in some respects but different in others. Judd and Lusk (1984) computed individual-level correlations among ratings of the features of rock-and-roll bands and sororities. From these inter- feature correlations, they computed a measure corresponding to our Avg I R [ measure of absolute perceived covariation but

Page 13: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

OUTGROUP COVARIATION 433

did not include a measure corresponding to our Avg R measure of evaluatively consistent covariation. Their study also differed from ours in that it did not involve participant-generated sub- types and did not examine ingroup--outgroup differences in perceived covariation.

Other studies have included measures of covariation in an ingroup-outgroup context, but, these studies differ from ours in other important respects. Linville and Jones (1980) computed group-level correlations among trait ratings of two hypothetical law school applicants. They found stronger absolute corre- lations among traits when the applicants were racial outgroup members. Although these results parallel ours, the Linville and Jones correlations were computed at a group level across many people who judged the attributes of two targets constructed by the experimenters. In our current research, correlations were calculated at the individual level over a larger number of targets generated by the individuals themselves. The presence of group- level correlations does not ensure that similar correlations are present at the level of individual knowledge structures. Further- more, Linville and Jones did not use a measure comparable to our Avg R measure of evaluatively consistent covariation.

An ingroup--outgroup study by Park et al. (1992) used a mea- sure of absolute perceived covariation as one component of an index measuring both redundancy of attributes and redun- dancy of subtypes. Their participants displayed a nonsignificant tendency to perceive greater redundancy among outgroup members. These results are qualitatively consistent with those reported here but weaker, possibly because their redundancy measure was a mixture of our Avg [ R I measure, which they used to measure attribute redundancy, and a second measure of redundancy of subtypes. In addition, their study did not include a measure corresponding to our Avg R measure and thus did not discriminate between covariation per se and evaluatively consistent covariation.

The present results are also related to but distinct from stud- ies showing that people perceive greater dimensional complex- ity (measured by the H statistic) in their age- and race-related ingroups (Linville, 1982; Linville & Jones, 1980). The H sta- tistic is based on a trait-sorting task in which people generate subtypes within a group and select a set of traits that describe each subtype. The H statistic reflects the number of indepen- dent binary dimensions underlying a person's trait sort (Attneave, 1959; Scott, 1969). The more underlying binary di- mensions present, the greater the complexity of the knowledge structure. High values of H occur when people use numerous traits in a relatively nonredundant fashion. Thus, the complex- ity statistic is similar in spirit to our present measures of per- ceived covariation. However, it differs from them in that our present measures concern covariation among observable fea- tures, not underlying dimensions. Also, our present measures reflect covariation per se, whereas H reflects covariation only indirectly (i.e., high redundancy results in fewer underlying in- dependent dimensions). Furthermore, H is not sensitive to the distinction between evaluatively consistent and inconsistent co- variation that is reflected in our Avg R and Avg I R I measures.

Finally, the current results are distinct in that they are the first to show a significant relationship between ingroup--out- group differences in perceived covariation and individual-level measures of familiarity with the ingroup and outgroup.

Measuring Familiarity

The construct of familiarity plays a key role in our theoretical formulation. Unfortunately, it is not obvious how to measure familiarity. If one's purpose is merely to determine whether people are more familiar with their ingroup, it may not be nee- essary to measure familiarity with a high degree of precision. However, if one's goal is to test whether familiarity is a mediator of other ingroup--outgroup differences (e.g., in perceived covariation), then the question of how one measures familiarity becomes more critical.

We used several different types of measures in the current experiments. In the study of gender groups, people estimated the number of friends and close friends of a gender. In the study of age groups, people estimated the number of acquaintances, number of close friends, and hours of contact with members of a group. These measures relate to judged frequency of exposure to group members. We also included a new measure of famil- i a r i ty - the number of known group members whose names were generated in a 3-rain listing task. This reflects the avail- ability of category exemplars in memory. This new measure was one of the strongest predictors of perceived covariation. Finally, in the study of perceived subtypes of people in business, people reported their years of business experience, a temporal duration measure of familiarity. Each of these measures is quantitative. Measures like these are different, and we believe more sensitive, than measures asking people to estimate their familiarity with a group on qualitative scales with endpoints such as highly famil- iar and not at all familiar This latter type of scale assumes that people define highly familiar the same way for different groups. However, people may redefine the scale endpoints for ingroups and outgroups. This is less likely with quantitative measures like those used here. Additional research is needed to determine which familiarity measures best reflect knowledge of social categories.

Implications for Social Categorization Models

People reliably use information about variability and inter- feature correlations when making categorization judgments (e.g., Hayes-Roth & Hayes-Roth, 1977; Medin & Schaffer, 1978 ). A pure prototype model (Posner & Keele, 1968 ) has no place in it for information regarding the variability of a feature or feature correlations within a category. It abstracts only a list of prototypic features representing the central tendency of the category on relevant features (e.g., professors are smart and curious). The category density model (Fried & Holyoak, 1984) is a more general abstraction model that abstracts the mean and variance of each feature. However, this model assumes that each feature is completely summarized by its mean and variance and thus fails to encode any information about covariation among features. In principle, one can imagine an even more general abstraction model in which on-line inferences are made regard- ing feature means, variances, and covariances. We call this the intuitive multivariate statistician model. A key feature of this and other property abstraction models is that they hold that in- formation about category properties (e.g., central tendency, variability, or covariation of features) is actively abstracted and updated on-line as an instance of the category is initially en-

Page 14: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

434 LINVILLE, FISCHER, AND YOON

countered. Thus, properties of a group such as feature variabil- ity are stored at the group level along with the group prototype. When a judgment about the group property is needed later, the precomputed abstraction can be retrieved without accessing group exemplars.

One impetus for turning to exemplar models in representing social groups is their natural ability to deal with people's knowl- edge of the variability and covariance within a group. In exemplar models such as the context model (Medin & Schaffer, 1978; No- sofsky, 1986; E. R. Smith & Zarate, 1992), MINERVA (Hintzman, 1986), PDIST (LinviUe et al., 1989), and ALCOVE (Kruschke, 1992), category knowledge consists of a set of exem- plars, and knowledge of category properties is implicitly stored in the features of category exemplars in memory. Judgments about category variability or covariation, like other category judgments, are made by activating a set of relevant exemplars in memory (Linville & Fischer, 1993a; Linville et al., 1986, 1989). PDIST is a computer model of how people generate perceived distributions of category features, which are the basis of group variability judg- ments (Linville et al., 1989).

One key issue that distinguishes between models is that exem- plar models hold that judgments are memory based (formed by retrieving exemplar information from memory), whereas prop- erty abstraction models hold that judgments are made and up- dated on-line (when stimuli are initially encountered) and then later recalled. It seems likely that some judgments of covariation are made in an explicit, on-line fashion, as suggested by abstrac- tion models. This seems especially likely when the perceiver has an explicit goal of estimating interfeature correlations and when the number of features of interest is small. Nevertheless, two types of considerations strongly support the notion that judgments of variability and covariation are memory based.

First, recent empirical research using a reaction time paradigm favors memory-based models over on-line property abstraction models (Mackie, Sherman, & Worth, 1993). In one experiment, judgments of a group's variability were made slower than a pre- sumed on-line judgment (liking) and at the same speed as a pre- sumed memory-based judgment (religiousness). In a second ex- periment, amount of similarity information recalled was signiti- cantly related to both the latency and extremity of recall judgments. They concluded from these findings that judgments of group variability are memory-based, not abstracted on-line.

Second, considerations of cognitive economy cast doubt on the assumption that all (or even most) knowledge of variability and covariation is actively abstracted on-line. For example, if a perceiver encodes only six features of each category member encountered, then to form on-line judgments about the central tendency, variability, and covariance structure of the category, the perceiver must actively update six means, six variances, and 15 correlations each time a new category member is encoun- tered. To make matters worse, an object of social perception is likely to be a member of multiple categories (e.g., female, Afri- can American, lawyer). To make matters worse still, there is evidence that on-line variability judgments are disrupted by competing on-line judgments (e.g., Mackie & Asuncion, 1990 ). In short, considerations of cognitive economy suggest that it is implausible to assume that people make the number of on-line inferences needed to acquire all knowledge of variation and co-

variation through active abstraction processes. By contrast, judgments of variability or covariation make relatively minimal demands on an exemplar process. The perceiver only needs to encode and store a list of features at the time when stimuli are encountered, then activate exemplars later when a judgment is needed. Because exemplar activation is a parallel and implicit process that need not be accessible to consciousness, a large number of exemplars can be activated without consuming time or cognitive resources (Hintzman, 1986). Because the judg- ment is focused on a few features of interest, the judgment pro- cess also poses a manageable cognitive burden. Furthermore, exemplar mechanisms for judging group membership implic- itly exploit information about variability and covariation with- out requiring any explicit judgments about these properties (E. E. Smith & Medin, 1981 ). In short, exemplar models pro- vide a cognitively feasible process for implicitly acquiring and using information about many properties of many features, whereas property abstraction models do not.

We do not suggest that all use of covariation information is memory based. However, the above considerations do suggest that exemplar memory processes play an important role in pro- cessing information about the variability and covariation of fea- tures describing group members. In principle, exemplar mem- ory processes can coexist with property abstractions that were made on-line.

Challenges for Future Research

This research demonstrates a new ingroup-outgroup bias-- the outgroup covariation effect. People tend to perceive greater covariation among the features of outgroup members. Further- more, these differences appear to be mediated in part by diffe r - ences in familiarity with group members. One challenge is to develop a converging set of methods for measuring familiarity with a social group. A second challenge is to identify other de- terminants of group differences in perceived covariation. We have suggested several, including greater incentives to make dis- tinctions among ingroup members and the greater tendency to encode individuating characteristics of ingroup members. A final challenge is to explore how group differences in perceived covariation affect other types of judgments about group mem- bers. Perceptions ofcovariation appear to influence the extrem- ity of evaluative judgments (Judd & Lusk, 1984; Linville, 1982; Linville & Fischer, 1993b, 1995) and may influence other types of judgment as well. The present findings raise a variety of in- teresting questions regarding the causes and consequences of perceived covariation among the features of social group members.

References

Abclson, R. P., & Roscnbcrg, M. J. (1958). Symbolic psycho-logic: A model of attitudinal cognition. BehavioralScience, 3, 1-13.

Attneave, E (1959). Applications of information theory to psychology. New York: Holt-Dryden.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator vari- able distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strate- gic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.

Page 15: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

OUTGROUP COVARIATION 435

Berman, J. S., R, Kenny, D. A. (1976). Correlational bias in observer ratings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 263-273.

Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In T. K. Srull & R. S. ~ e r (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 1-36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Brewer, M. B. (1993). Social identity, distinctiveness, and in-group ho- mogeneity. Social Cognition, 11, 150-164.

Brewer, M. B., & Lui, L. (1984). Categorization of the elderly by the elderly: Effects of pereeiver's category membership. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, I0, 585-595.

Bruner, J. S., & Tagiuri, R. (1954). The perception of people. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (VoL 2, pp. 634- 654). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. (1967). Genesis of popular but erro- neous psychodiagnostic observations. Journal of Abnormal Psychol- ogy, 72, 193-204.

Crocker, J. ( 1981 ). Judgment of covariation by social perceivers. Psy- chological Bulletin, 90, 272-292.

D'Andrade, R. G. (1974). Memory and the assessment of behavior. In H. Blalock (Ed.), Measurement in the social sciences (pp. 159-186 ). Chicago: Aldine.

Fried, L. S., & Holyoak, K. J. (1984). Induction of category distribu- tions: A framework for classification learning. Journal of Experimen- tal Psychology: Learning, Memory" and Cognition, 10, 234-257.

Hamilton, D. L. (1979). A cognitive-attributional analysis of stereo- typing. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy- chology(Vol. 12, pp. 53-84). New York: Academic Press.

Hamilton, D. L. ( 1981 ). Illusory correlation as a basis for stereotyping. In D. L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stereotyping and in- tergroup behavior (pp. 115-144). HiUsdale, N J: Erlbaum.

Hamilton, D. L., & Gifford, R. K. (1976). Illusory correlation in inter- personal perception: A cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 392-407.

Hayes-Roth, B., & Hayes-Roth, E (1977). Concept learning and the recognition and classification of exemplars. Journal of Verbal Learn- ing and Verbal Behavior, 16, 321-338.

Heider, E ( 1958 ). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

Hintzman, D. L. (1986). "Schema abstraction" in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 93, 411-428.

Islam, M. R., & Hewstone, M. (1993). Dimensions of contact as pre- dictors of intergroup anxiety, perceived out-group variability, and out-group attitude: An integrative model. Personality and Social Psy- chology Bulletin, 19, 700-710.

Jones, E. E., Wood, G. C., & Quattrone, G. A. ( 1981 ). Perceived vari- ability of personal characteristics in ingroups and outgroups: The role of knowledge and evaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Bul- letin, 7, 523-528.

Judd, C. M., & Kenny, D. A. ( 1981 ). Process analysis: Estimating me- diation in treatment evaluations. Evaluation Review, 5, 602-619.

Judd, C. M., & Lusk, C. M. (1984). Knowledge structures and evalua- tive judgments: Effects of structural variables on judgmental extrem- ity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology" 46, 1193-1207.

Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1988). Outgroup homogeneity: Judgments of variability at the individual and group levels. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology" 54, 778-788.

Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation ( Vol. 15, pp. 129- 238). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Kraus, S., Ryan, C. S., Judd, C. M., Hastie, R., & Park, B. (1993). Use of mental frequency distributions to represent variability among members of social categories. Social Cognition, 11, 22-43.

Kruschke, J. K. (1992). ALCOVE: An exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. PsychologicalReview, 99, 22-44.

Lewicki, P. (1982). Trait relationships: The nonconscious generaliza- tion of social experience. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 439-445.

Lewicki, P. (1986a). Nonconscious social information processing. Or- lando, FL: Academic Press.

Lewicki, P. (1986b). Processing information about covariation that cannot be articulated. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn- ing, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 135-146.

Linville, E W. (1982). The complexity-extremity effect and age-based stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psycholog2g, 42, 193- 211.

Linville, P. W. (in press). The heterogeneity of homogeneity. In J. Coo- per & J. Darley (Eds.), Interpersonal perception and social action. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Linville, P. W., & Fischer, G. W. (1993a). Exemplar and abstraction models of perceived group variability and stereotypicality. Social Cognition, 11, 92-125.

Linville, P. W., & Fischer, G. W. ( 1993b, June). Perceived variability, c~ariation, and extremity of judgment about group members. Paper presented at the Duck Conference on Social Cognition, Duck, NC.

Linville, P. W., & Fischer, G. W. ( 1995 ). Perceived variability, comri- ance, and extremity of judgments about group members. Unpublished manuscript, Duke University, Durham, NC.

LinviUe, P. W., Fischer, G. W., & Salovey, P. (1989). Perceived distribu- tions of the characteristics of ingroup and outgroup members: Em- pirical evidence and a computer simulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 165-188.

Linville, P. W., & Jones, E. E. (1980). Polarized appraisals of outgroup members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,, 38, 689-703.

Linville, P. W., Salovey, P., & Fischer, G. W. (1986). Stereotyping and perceived distributions of social characteristics: An application to in- group-outgroup perception. In J. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 165-208). New York: Ac- ademic Press.

Lorenzi-Cioldi, E, Eagiy, A. H., & Stewart, T. L. (1995). Homogeneity of gender groups in memory. Journal of Experimental SocialPsychol- ogy, 31, 193-217.

Mackie, D. M., & Asuncion, A. G. (1990). On-line and memory-based modification of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy" 59, 5-16.

Mackie, D. M., Sherman, J. W., & Worth, L. T. (1993). On-line and memory-based processes in group variability judgments. Social Cog- nition, 11, 44-69.

McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1985). Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 159-188.

Medin, D. L. (1989). Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist, 44, 1469-148 I.

Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Revie~, 85, 207-238.

Messick, D. M., & Mackie, D. M. (1989). Intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology" 40, 40-81.

Mullen, B., & Hu, L. (1989). Perceptions ofingroup and outgroup vari- ability: A meta-analytic integration. Basic and Applied Social Psy- chology" 10, 233-252.

Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference." Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification- categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 39-57.

Ostrom, T. M., & Sedikides, C. (1992). Outgroup homogeneity effects in natural and minimal groups. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 536- 552.

Park, B., & Hastie, R. (1987). Perception of variability in category de-

Page 16: Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and ...webuser.bus.umich.edu/yoonc/research/Papers/... · Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members:

436 LINVILLE, FISCHER, AND YOON

velopment: Instance- versus abstraction-based stereotypes. Journalof Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 621-635.

Park, B., & Judd, C. M. (1990). Measures and models of perceived group variability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,, 59, 173-191.

Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Ryan, C. S. ( 1991 ). Social categorization and the representation of variability information. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 211-245 ). Chichester, England: Wiley.

Park, B., & Rothbart, M. (1982). Perception ofoutgroup homogeneity and levels of social categorization: Memory for the subordinate attri- butes of ingroup and outgroup members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 1051-1068.

Park, B., Ryan, C. S., & Judd, C. M. (1992). Role of meaningful sub- groups in explaining differences in perceived variability for ingroups and outgroups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,, 63, 553-567.

Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 353-363.

Rosenberg, S., & Sedlak, A. (1972). Structural representations of im- plicit personality theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experi- mental socialpsychology ( Vo !. 6, pp. 235-297 ). New York: Academic Press.

Schneider, D. J. (1973). Implicit personality theory: A review. Psycho- logicalBulletin, 79, 294-309.

Scott, W. A. (1969). Structure of natural cognitions. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 12, 261-278.

Sedikides, C., & Ostrom, T. M. (1993). Perceptions of group variability:

Moving from an uncertain crawl to a purposeful stride. Social Cog- nition, 11, 165-174.

Simon, B. (1992). The perception ofingroup and outgroup homogene- ity: Re-introducing the intergroup context. In W. Stroebe & M. Hew- stone ( Eds. ), European review of social psychology ( Vol. 3, pp. I - 30 ). Chichester, England: Wiley.

Smith, E. E., & Medin, D. L. (1981). Categories and concepts. Cam- bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Smith, E. R., & Zarate, M. A. (1990). Exemplar and prototype use in social categorization. Social Cognition, 8, 243-262.

Smith, E. R., & Zarate, M. A. (1992). Exemplar-based model of social judgment. Psychological Review, 99, 3-21.

Taylor, S. E. (1981). A categorization approach to stereotyping. In D. L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stereotyping and in- tergroup behavior(pp. 88-114). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Trolier, T. K., & Hamilton, D. L. (1986). Variables influencing judg- ments of correlational relations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 879-888.

Weber, R., & Crocker, J. (1983). Cognitive processes in the revision of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,, 45, 961-977.

Winer, B. J. ( 1971 ). Statisticalprinciples in experimental design. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Winkler, R. L., & Hays, W. L. (1975). Statistics: Probability, inference, and decision (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Received May 23, 1994 Revision received August 20, 1995

Accepted August 29, 1995 •


Recommended