+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Personality Genetics and Health in...

Personality Genetics and Health in...

Date post: 31-May-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 3 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
170
Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit Turner Nelson B.A. and B.Sc., University of Regina, 2007 Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science in the Department of Biomedical Physiology and Kinesiology Faculty of Science Jessica Marit Turner Nelson 2015 SIMON FRASER UNIVERSITY Fall 2015
Transcript
Page 1: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

Personality Genetics and Health in

Super-Seniors

by

Jessica Marit Turner Nelson

B.A. and B.Sc., University of Regina, 2007

Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the

Requirements for the Degree of

Master of Science

in the

Department of Biomedical Physiology and Kinesiology

Faculty of Science

Jessica Marit Turner Nelson 2015

SIMON FRASER UNIVERSITY

Fall 2015

Page 2: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

ii

Approval

Name: Jessica Marit Turner Nelson

Degree: Master of Science

Title: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors

Examining Committee: Chair: Dr. Parveen Bawa Professor

Dr. Angela Brooks-Wilson Senior Supervisor Professor

Dr. Andrew Wister Supervisor Professor

Dr. Robert Holt External Examiner Professor Molecular Biology and Biochemistry Simon Fraser University

Date Defended/Approved: December 17th, 2015

Page 3: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

iii

Ethics Statement

Page 4: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

iv

Abstract

Healthy aging is a complex phenotype, and genetic factors that contribute to long term

good health are not well understood. Longevity and health are associated with lifestyle

choices. Behaviour is governed by personality; therefore, variation in personality-related

genes may affect healthy aging. Five candidate genes involved in the physiology of

personality and personality disorders were identified from the literature: COMT, DRD4,

MAOA, SLC6A4, and TH. Single nucleotide polymorphisms and variable number of

random repeat polymorphisms were genotyped in DNA from 493 European-ancestry

healthy oldest-old and 431 European-ancestry middle-aged controls. Tests for allelic

associations were conducted, with stratification by sex. No associations remained

significant after correction for multiple tests. Variants tested in these candidate genes were

not associated with long-term good health in this sample. Either genetic variation in these

genes does not influence healthy aging, or true effects exist that are too small to be

detected in this study.

Keywords: Healthy Aging; Personality; Oldest-old; Genetic association; Longevity; Candidate Gene Design

Page 5: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

v

Dedication

For my own super grandparents, for teaching me how

to bring integrity, dedication and joy to everything I do.

Page 6: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

vi

Acknowledgements

Firstly, I would like to acknowledge my supervisor, Dr. Angela Brooks-Wilson, for this

wonderful opportunity. I appreciate all your guidance and counsel, and in particular, your

constant upbeat encouragement. I have learned so much from you throughout this

experience, related to this endeavor and beyond. To my committee member, Dr. Andrew

Wister, thank you for all thoughtful advice and efforts in helping to make this project a

success.

I would also like to extend thanks to my lab, the Cancer Genetics group. In particular

Stephen Leach for reacquainting me with the lab and helping with technical difficulties,

Ruth Thomas for seamlessly knowing all the facts in the Super-Senior Study, and

Samantha Jones for editing and assistance in the lab. My thanks to the many students

and volunteers in the lab that have helped to support my learning. I’m additionally grateful

for the statistical assistance that Andy Leung has provided.

I would also like to acknowledge the contribution of the Super-Seniors and controls who

volunteered for the Super-Senior Study, without them this work would not have been

possible.

To my parents, family and friends, thank you for your endless support and understanding

throughout this project. Finally, my eternal gratitude to Shawn Williams and Kelly

Santbergen for seeing this through with me from beginning to end.

Page 7: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

vii

Table of Contents

Approval .......................................................................................................................... ii Ethics Statement ............................................................................................................ iii Abstract .......................................................................................................................... iv Dedication ....................................................................................................................... v Acknowledgements ........................................................................................................ vi Table of Contents .......................................................................................................... vii List of Tables .................................................................................................................. ix List of Figures.................................................................................................................. x List of Acronyms ............................................................................................................. xi

Chapter 1. Background ............................................................................................. 1 1.1. Aging ...................................................................................................................... 1

1.1.1. The Aging Population ................................................................................ 1 1.1.2. Limitation in the Aging Field ....................................................................... 2 1.1.3. Chronic Disease and Disability .................................................................. 2 1.1.4. Behavioural Impact on Health .................................................................... 5

1.2. Personality .............................................................................................................. 6 1.2.1. Historical Perspectives on Personality ....................................................... 6 1.2.2. The Big Five .............................................................................................. 7 1.2.3. Heritability of Personality ........................................................................... 9 1.2.4. Stability of Personality ............................................................................. 10 1.2.5. Personality Gender Differences ............................................................... 11 1.2.6. Personality Profile of the Very Old and Very Healthy ............................... 11 1.2.7. Limitation in the Personality Field ............................................................ 12 1.2.8. The Impact of Personality on Behaviour .................................................. 14

1.3. Biochemistry and Physiology of Personality .......................................................... 15 1.3.1. Age Related Changes.............................................................................. 15 1.3.2. Personality Domains and Relevant Structures ......................................... 16

Extraversion ........................................................................................................... 16 Neuroticism ............................................................................................................ 18 Conscientiousness ................................................................................................. 20 Agreeableness ....................................................................................................... 20 Openness ............................................................................................................... 21

1.3.3. Neurotransmitters and Personality ........................................................... 21 1.4. The Genome and Genetic Variation ...................................................................... 23

1.4.1. Basic Structures ...................................................................................... 23 1.4.2. Microsatellites and Minisatellites .............................................................. 25 1.4.3. Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs) ............................................... 27

1.5. Candidate Genes .................................................................................................. 28 1.5.1. Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) ................................................... 28 1.5.2. Dopamine Receptor D4 Gene (DRD4) ..................................................... 30 1.5.3. Monoamine Oxidase A (MAOA) ............................................................... 31 1.5.4. Sodium Chloride Dependent Transporter (SLC6A4) ................................ 34 1.5.5. Tyrosine Hydroxylase (TH) ...................................................................... 37

1.6. Thesis Objective ................................................................................................... 39

Page 8: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

viii

Chapter 2. Methods.................................................................................................. 40 2.1. Study Participants ................................................................................................. 40 2.2. Literature Search and Gene Selection .................................................................. 40 2.3. SNP Selection ...................................................................................................... 42 2.4. SNP Genotyping ................................................................................................... 42 2.5. SNP Analysis ........................................................................................................ 42 2.6. VNTR Genotyping ................................................................................................ 43 2.7. VNTR Analysis ..................................................................................................... 45

Chapter 3. Results ................................................................................................... 46 3.1. Literature Search and Gene Selection Results ..................................................... 46 3.2. SNP Selection Results .......................................................................................... 50 3.3. SNP Genotyping Results ...................................................................................... 52 3.4. SNP Quality Control Results ................................................................................. 52 3.5. VNTR Genotyping Results .................................................................................... 54 3.6. VNTR Quality Control Results .............................................................................. 60 3.7. Association Results .............................................................................................. 61

Chapter 4. Discussion ............................................................................................. 69 4.1. Interpretation of Results ........................................................................................ 69

4.1.1. Relevant Association Studies .................................................................. 71 4.1.2. Potential Causes of Variation in Published Association Studies for

Candidate Genes ..................................................................................... 73 4.2. Limitations of Design ............................................................................................ 76

4.2.1. Candidate Gene Selection ....................................................................... 76 4.2.2. Marker Selection ...................................................................................... 77 4.2.3. Power and Rejection of the Null Hypothesis ............................................ 78 4.2.4. Control Group .......................................................................................... 80 4.2.5. Sample Size ............................................................................................ 81 4.2.6. Lack of Personality Testing ...................................................................... 81

4.3. Recommendations ................................................................................................ 82 4.3.1. Candidate Gene Based Design ............................................................... 82 4.3.2. Super-Senior Study Design ..................................................................... 87

4.4. Future Developments ........................................................................................... 88 4.4.1. Towards Integrative Approaches ............................................................. 88 4.4.2. Personality and Personalized Medicine Interventions for Longevity ......... 89

References 91

Page 9: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

ix

List of Tables

Table 2.1. Summary of program choice for genes and markers .............................. 43

Table 2.2. Primers and PCR conditions used for amplification of VNTRs ................ 44

Table 3.1. Summary of potential candidate genes found through literature search, including polymorphisms and results of association studies ................................................................................................... 47

Table 3.2. SNPs Selected to Represent the Five Final Candidate Genes ............... 51

Table 3.3. Average VNTR allele sizes and standard deviations .............................. 60

Table 3.4. Results from Pearson Chi-Square tests of SNP and Score Tests of VNTRs, for the Combined Sample and when Stratified by Sex .......... 62

Table 3.5. Adjusted p-values Using False Discovery Rate (FDR)* .......................... 67

Page 10: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

x

List of Figures

Figure 1.1. General Gene Structure ......................................................................... 24

Figure 1.2. Genomic structure of COMT................................................................... 28

Figure 1.3. Genomic structure of DRD4 ................................................................... 30

Figure 1.4. Genomic structure of MAOA................................................................... 31

Figure 1.5. Genomic structure of SLC6A4 ................................................................ 34

Figure 1.6. Genomic structure of TH ........................................................................ 37

Figure 3.1. Quality control for SNPs and VNTRs ...................................................... 53

Figure 3.2. CEPH Family #1341 showing Mendelian Segregation of the DRD4 VNTR ..................................................................................................... 54

Figure 3.3. DRD4 VNTR allele bins .......................................................................... 55

Figure 3.4. Examples of common calling problems in GeneMapper v5 .................... 57

Figure 3.5. Bin sets used in GeneMapper v5 to call each VNTR .............................. 59

Figure 4.1. Candidate gene network ........................................................................ 84

Figure 4.2. Serotonin receptors and associated BDNF, TPH1 and TPH2 network .................................................................................................. 85

Page 11: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

xi

List of Acronyms

5-HTT 5-hydroxytryptamine Transporter

ADHD Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

BC British Columbia

BLSA Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging

CEPH Centre d’Etude du Polymorphisme Humain

CEU Northern European Utah Population

CMMT Centre for Molecular Medicine and Therapeutics

COMT Catechol-O-Methyltransferase

DRD4 Dopamine Receptor D4

DSM-V Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Version 5

FBST Full Bayesian Significance Test

FDR False Discovery Rate

GWAS Genome Wide Association Studies

HWE Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium

HDL High-Density Lipoprotein

MAOA Monoamine Oxidase A

MAOAH Monoamine Oxidase A High Activity

MAOAL Monoamine Oxidase A Low Activity

MSGSC Michael Smith Genome Sciences Centre

NEO-FFI NEO-Five Factor Inventory

OCD Obsessive Compulsive Disorder

PCR Polymerase Chain Reaction

PHAC Public Health Agency of Canada

SES Socio-Economical Status

SLC6A4 Sodium Chloride Dependent Transporter

SNP Single Nucleotide Polymorphism

SVS8 Golden Helix SVS suite 8

TCI Temperament and Character Inventory

TH Tyrosine Hydroxylase

TPQ Tri-Personality Questionnaire

UN United Nations

Page 12: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

xii

VNTR Variable Number Tandem Repeat

Page 13: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

1

Chapter 1. Background

1.1. Aging

1.1.1. The Aging Population

Canada’s population is aging. In 1960, Canadians over 65 years in age constituted only

8% of the population. By 2009 that rose to 14%, and by 2036 Statistics Canada predicts they will

comprise 23%-25% [1]. The two main driving factors behind this growth are reduction in total

fertility rates and increase in life expectancy [2].

The total fertility rate, which is defined as the average number of children a woman would

bear if she survived through the end of her reproductive age span [3], has been declining globally

for a number of reasons. Increased education and reduced child mortality are the major

contributing factors to the decline [4], while other social programs such as family planning play a

smaller role [4]. The United Nations (UN) has projected that the global fertility rate will decline

from the current rate of 2.5 children per woman to 2.2 by 2045-2050 [2].

Increased life expectancy is caused by two factors. First, there has been an increased life

expectancy at birth which has added to the potential number of people in the aging population.

This was not initially intuitive as an element of increased life expectancy as there was an increase

in children and a reduction in the proportion of older adults in the population [2]. Second, there is

an increase in the proportion of older adults as late life survival continues to rise [2]. The UN World

Aging report predicts that life expectancy will rise to 83 years of age by 2045-2050 from the current

78 years in developed regions [2]. This trend is also seen in less developed regions with the

current expectancy at 68 years and future expectancy at 75 years by 2045-2050 [2]. As our

population ages it is important to define what aging is.

Page 14: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

2

1.1.2. Limitation in the Aging Field

While intuitively the definition of aging seems clear, there is no consensus definition and

researchers often use their own inclusion criteria for their studies. Criteria such as the age senior

status is achieved, who is the oldest-old, what defines healthy seniors, and which co-morbidities

are allowable, are some common measures that can be very different between studies. Depp and

Jeste looked at the definitions used in aging studies and found little agreement on what successful

aging is and that definitions used by researchers were often driven by their specific research

question [5]. This lack of defined characteristics for seniors is problematic, and is a potential

source of discrepancy between studies, since slightly different definitions will produce different

results that are not comparable or reproducible [6].

A further problem in the field is the lack of available controls for the successful aging

subjects. Ideal controls for this group usually do not escape the high mortality rates of this cohort

or are difficult to enroll due to disability or illness. As such, aging studies use a variety of

populations for their control group which also makes comparisons between studies hard to

evaluate [6]. Despite problems with definitions and controls, studying the very old and very healthy

can give researchers valuable insight into the development and management of chronic disease

and disability.

1.1.3. Chronic Disease and Disability

While the increase in life expectancy has been a remarkable feat for science and social

programs alike, it has created problems in caring for an aging population. Disability and chronic

disease are barriers in the aging population that cause early unemployment, poverty, increased

health care costs, stress and resource demands on family members, and decreased quality of

life. A meta-analysis from Marengoni et al. showed that “major consequences of multimorbidity

are disability and functional decline, poor quality of life, and high health care costs” [7].

Unsurprisingly, research has consistently linked chronic disease in elderly with poor quality of life,

such that the greater the number of chronic diseases, the poorer the reported quality of life [8].

Additional research by Campolina et al. found that the more years elderly spent free of chronic

disease, the more years they gained living free of disability and the greater their life expectancy

[9].

Page 15: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

3

Chronic disease and disability are common in the senior population. The Chief Public

Health Officer’s Report on the State of Public Health in Canada 2010 found that 89% of Canadian

seniors (defined as Canadians over the age of 65) had at least one chronic condition [10]. Seniors

with more than one chronic condition were further divided into younger (65-79) and older seniors

(80+), with disease prevalence of 25% and 37%, respectively [10]. The likelihood of developing a

chronic disease also increased with age; 71% of seniors aged 65 to 74 reported having at least

one chronic condition, increasing to 80% in the 75 to 84 cohort [11]. Notably, increases in chronic

conditions are not substantial in seniors over 85 years of age [11].

There are many types of chronic conditions in the senior population; arthritis, heart

disease, diabetes, cancer, dementia and pulmonary disease are the most common complaints.

Arthritis is the most prevalent, although it does not increase the overall mortality in the group. In

2006, 23% of seniors were living with some form of heart disease, 21% had diabetes and 56%

had novel developments of cancer [10].

More alarming still are reports that indicate diabetes is often under-diagnosed in the senior

population; Franse et al. suggests that a third of seniors with diabetes remain undiagnosed, with

Leong et al. suggesting that number is closer to 40% [12,13]. Furthermore, the annual cost of

health care per patient increases as diabetic patients age. Patients under 65 years of age have

an annual cost of $4,267 CDN, however that cost doubles in a senior population, to $8,902 CDN

[14]. This increase in cost is largely the consequence of worsening kidney function and poor

glycemic control [14].

Dementia is steadily increasing in the senior population. The Alzheimer Society of Canada

recorded 103,728 new dementia cases for seniors in 2008 and projected that to increase 2.5

times by 2038 [15], with Alzheimer disease accounting for 50% of new dementia cases [15].

Annual costs per patient in Canada vary from $4,164 CDN for individuals with mild Alzheimer

disease to $48,756 CDN for those with severe diagnoses [16].

Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease (COPD) is the ninth leading cause of death

globally [17] with a 27% risk of developing COPD by the time an individual is 80 [18]. This is a

decrease from its position as fourth leading cause in 2008 [19]. Maleki-Yazdi et al. reported an

annual cost per patient for COPD of $4,147 CDN [20].

Page 16: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

4

Cancer is the major cause of death for Canadians. The Canadian Cancer Society reports

that cancer death accounted for 30% of the total recorded deaths in Canada for 2011 and are

projecting 196,900 new cases in 2015 [21]. Moreover, 89% of these new cases are predicted to

affect Canadians over the age of 50. The Public Health Agency of Canada (PHAC) reported

malignant cancers to be the 8th leading direct cost to the Canadian health care system for 2008,

with hospital care, physician and drug costs totaling 3.8 billion dollars [22]. Accounting for indirect

costs, dollars lost due to illness, injury and premature death, the cost rose to 4.4 billion [22].

Cardiovascular disease, which encompasses heart and cerebovascular disease, was the

second major cause of death for Canadians in 2011, accounting for 25.5% of total recorded

deaths [23]. Heart disease was reported to affect 22% of seniors over the age of 75 and that

percentage rose to 27.5% in seniors over the age of 85. Additionally, 7% of Canadians over the

age of 75 reported living with the effects of stroke [24]. The cost of cardiovascular disease is

profound. The PHAC listed it as the most directly burdensome disease on the heath care system,

with direct costs of 11.7 billion dollars in 2008 [22].

The cost of these chronic conditions is substantial. The Certified General Accountants

Association of Canada estimates that by 2050 public health care costs will consume 10.9% of our

country’s gross domestic product. This is a marked increase from the 6.8% spent in 2007 [25].

Further, this increase in health care costs is underestimated, as it does not account for all the

informal care delivered by family members [26]. The Alzheimer Society of Canada reported

opportunity costs (the wage caregivers could have made in the workforce) from informal

caregiving in 2008 to be close to $5 billion with projections of that number increasing to $55 billion

by 2038 [15]. The Canadian Institute for Health Information reported in 2011 that seniors with

more than three chronic conditions had three times as many health care visits compared with

those under three visits [11]. Multiple chronic conditions in seniors are expensive, not only to the

public health care system but also to family members.

Keeping the senior population free of morbidities such as dementia, diabetes,

cardiovascular disease and cancer would greatly reduce this financial burden, as people free of

comorbidities are not only less burdensome on the health care system but are also more

productive members of society and have a better quality of life [27]. Regrettably, factors behind

why one individual ages with few comorbidities and another with many are not fully understood.

Behavioural research is one of the few areas where research has made gains into correlations

between healthy behaviours and longevity.

Page 17: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

5

1.1.4. Behavioural Impact on Health

Unsurprisingly, certain behaviours such as abstaining from smoking, maintaining weight

in the healthy range, and being physically active [28], have been associated with healthy aging.

Factors that influence health are known as determinants of health. These determinants can be

used as potential predictors for individuals and help identify preventative measures to correct

unhealthy behaviours.

Determinants of health are often used to look at the general health of a population of

interest. For example, one study found that the best determinants for general Canadian self-

reported health were socioeconomic status, marriage status and unmet healthcare needs [29].

Determinants from one population; however, do not always apply to another population.

Denton and Walters found that high income, working full-time, caring for family, and social

support were stronger predictors of general health in women compared to men [30]. This

emphasizes the need to look at a specific population for health determinants, and the need to

have a well defined group.

For a summary of determinants of healthy aging, Depp and Jeste conducted a meta-

analysis and found that healthy aging was most strongly correlated with being free of disability or

having good functional status. Other good determinants that were correlated were arthritis status,

hearing problems, type of daily activities, diabetes and smoking status. Physical exercise, systolic

blood pressure, self reported health, depression history and global cognitive functioning, were

found to only be moderate predictors of healthy aging. Interestingly, some of the best accepted

predictors of health, such as socioeconomic status, education, marriage status and ethnicity, were

found to be limited in their correlation to healthy aging [5].

In subsequent years, studies have reproduced the correlation with smoking status,

arthritis, and physical activity [31]. McKee and Schuz highlighted the importance of physical

activity and smoking status as determinants of healthy aging. They further proposed that

perception of control is an important moderator of health determinants, giving an example of self-

efficacy moderating the correlation between physical activity and health [32].

There have been few studies looking at gender differences in determinants of healthy

aging; however, Yates et al. followed a group of male, young-senior physicians and found that

Page 18: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

6

there were three strong predictors of longevity in this group. If sedentary lifestyle, obesity and

diabetes were present, there was only a 14% chance that a subject would reach 90 years of age.

In relation to the meta-analysis results of Depp and Jeste, Yates et al. showed that the chance of

longevity was reduced with each subsequent diagnosis of a chronic condition, such that one

condition decreased the odds to 67%, but four conditions decreased it to 12% [33].

Plainly, there are predictors of healthy aging. Physical activity, absence of chronic disease

and disability, smoking status, and arthritis are determinants that continually show up in the

literature. With the exception of arthritis, the remaining three determinants are modifiable; they

can be changed or prevented through behavioural modifications. Of particular interest is why

some people naturally abstain from smoking, exercise regularly and take preventative measures

against disease and others do not.

These healthy behaviours have been shown to be associated with personality. Individuals

who are high in extraversion, conscientiousness and emotional stability are better able to adhere

to exercise behaviours, and individuals high in conscientiousness are less likely to take part in

risky health behaviours [34,35]. Personality acts as a modifier of behaviour and is itself an

important predictor of health.

1.2. Personality

1.2.1. Historical Perspectives on Personality

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) defines personality

as “enduring patterns of perceiving, relating to, and thinking about the environment and oneself

that are exhibited in a wide range of social and personal contexts” [36]. Personality is not an

objective physical trait that is easily measured. Measurements can only be done indirectly through

either covert or overt observations, and/or reports on an individual’s complex behaviour [37]. As

such, there are many personality theories, resulting from the variety of schools of thought within

the psychology field; however, the focus of this project will be on the prevailing trait theory of

personality.

Lewis Goldberg’s condensed history of trait theory attributes Sir Francis Galton in 1884

as one of the first scientists to categorize personality. He formed the Lexical Hypothesis, which

Page 19: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

7

postulates that descriptive individual differences are encoded in our natural language. This would

later be refined to a set number of personality-related terms by scientists during the 1930’s, most

notably Gordon Allport, Henry Odber and later in the early 1960’s by Warren Norman [38]. During

the 1960s Trait Theory lost popularity in favour of the then fashionable Behaviorism model [39].

Trait theory, however, would regain a place in personality theory and become the dominant model

for personality theorist. Within trait theory there were two camps of theorist, those who thought

that there were a few broad traits and those who thought there were many narrowly specified

traits. Time and consensus has merged these two camps into a hierarchy model, where there are

broad domains that are constructed from more specialized traits [40].

Currently there are three models that have been widely adopted and validated through

research. Eysenck’s model uses three traits to summarize personality (neuroticism, extraversion,

and psychoticism). This model focuses on the biological basis of personality [41]. The second

model is Cloninger's, consisting of four temperaments (harm avoidance, novelty seeking, reward

dependence and persistence) and three character traits (self-directedness, cooperativeness, and

self-transcendence). This model takes into account both biological and sociocultural influences

[41]. Lastly there is Costa and McCrae's model which is a taxonomy of traits with no particular

original inclination of biological or sociocultural influences [41]. This model does assume that

personality is a strictly intrinsic process, and is not overly influenced by external forces [42]. The

model is called the big five and will be the model of focus for this project.

The big five model was chosen over Eysenck's and Cloninger's due to its prevalence in

the literature and the previous use of the NEO-Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) with a small

subset of participants in a related research project in our laboratory. There is little difference in

the overall traits captured by the various theories [43–46]. Bouchard and Loehlin describe the

various theories as “dividing the same pie in different ways”, for example impulsivity is listed under

neuroticism in the big five but is listed as a facet of psychoticism in Eysenck’s model [40].

1.2.2. The Big Five

The big five is divided into five factors or domains, each with a list of traits or facets. Factors

can be thought of as a continuum with extreme phenotypes at each end and average personality

somewhere in the middle. The five factors are extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness,

neuroticism and openness [38]. Each factor is briefly explained, with its opposite in parentheses,

and associations with general health.

Page 20: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

8

Extraversion (Introversion) is the tendency towards positive mood, sociability and activity

[47]. The facets measured are warmth, gregariousness, assertiveness, activity, excitement-

seeking, and positive emotions [48]. Many studies have been conducted on extraversion and its

role in health. Low levels of extraversion have been associated with mothers more likely to have

a caesarean section and experience complications during labour, and depression (Johnston &

Brown, 2013). High extraversion has been associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

(ADHD) [51], bipolar disorder [52], and better general health perceptions and physical functioning

[53].

Neuroticism (Emotional Stability) is characterized by a tendency to experience negative

emotions, become easily overwhelmed by stressful events and have difficulty controlling impulses

[50]. It is the only factor that is oriented towards the more negative spectrum in the naming

scheme; recent literature refers to emotional stability. This project will maintain the neuroticism

label to stay consistent with the literature. The facets in this factor include anxiousness, angry

hostility, depressiveness, self-consciousness, impulsivity, and vulnerability [48]. Neuroticism is

another factor that has been highly researched. High neuroticism has been associated biologically

with high levels of interleukin 6 (stimulates the immune response) [54], poor antibody response

[55] and low cortisol response [55,56]. High impulsivity in particular has been associated to low

high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol and high triglycerides [54]. Many mental health studies

have reported high neuroticism as risk factor, including substance abuse , panic disorder,

generalized anxiety disorder, phobias, obsessive compulsive disorder, uni-polar disorder, post-

traumatic stress disorder [57], depression [50,53], Alzheimer disease [58], low subjective physical

and mental health [50,53], and ADHD [51,59]. Further, high neuroticism has been associated with

higher reports of tremors, breathlessness, constipation, skin trouble and strokes [53].

Conscientiousness (Undirectedness) is defined as task and goal directed, planful, follows

social norms and rules and can delay gratification [60]. Facets in this factor are competence,

order, dutifulness, achievement, self-discipline and deliberation [48]. Interestingly,

conscientiousness seems to act as an antagonist to neuroticism. Physiologically, high

conscientiousness has been associated with low levels of interleukin 6 [54,61], low HDL

cholesterol and low triglycerides [62], the opposite profile of high neuroticism. Low

conscientiousness is also associated with increased substance abuse, uni-polar disorder, post-

traumatic disorder, panic disorder, phobia, generalized anxiety disorder [57], depression [53],

Alzheimer disease [58] and ADHD [51,59]. Individuals high in conscientiousness are associated

Page 21: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

9

with better subjective physical and mental health [50], and men in particular report less depression

and better general health [53].

Openness to Experience (Closed to Experience) is defined by Chapman as having

“cognitive and behavioural flexibility, urbane or cultured tendencies, and attunement to internal

and external events and experiences” [47]. Openness to experience will be shortened to

openness for the remainder of this project. Facets consist of fantasy, esthetics, feelings, actions,

ideas, and values [48]. Intelligence is often ascribed as being a result of openness given its

association with cognitive engagement, flexibility and maintenance [58]. Costa argues that

openness is not simply a measure of intelligence given that joint factor analysis shows intelligence

and openness are separate factors. Openness is oriented to intellectual curiosity but also

culturally oriented with aesthetical sensitivities and liberal value systems [63]. Low openness is

associated with Alzheimer disease [58], depression [50], and the facets of feelings, values and

actions with Schizophrenia [52]. High openness is associated with increased creativity in ADHD

patients [51], high cortisol activity [56], low levels of interleukin 6 [61], and men reporting less

vitality but women reporting better general health perceptions and physical health, with less pain

and more vitality [53].

Agreeableness (Antagonism) is an inclination towards maintaining interpersonal harmony

[47]. This factor’s facets are trust, straightforwardness, altruism, compliance, modesty, and

tender-mindedness [48]. Chapman argues that agreeableness in itself is not a great predictor, but

it’s combinations with other factors can produce good predictors [47]. For example, hostility is a

common trait that is the combination of low agreeableness and high neuroticism and has been

associated with poor health trajectories in male veterans [64]. There have been some positive

associations found at the factor level, including a relationship between high agreeableness and

high cortical activity [56], and women reporting better physical and mental health, fewer medical

problems and less visits to general practitioners [53].

1.2.3. Heritability of Personality

Monozygotic and dizygotic twin studies summarized by Bouchard found that heritability

was estimated to be between 40-60% and displayed an additive model [40], though multiple

personality scales were used. To further break down the heritability of specific factors, Bae et al.

show in the Long Life Family Study that openness was the most heritable personality trait (49%),

followed by extraversion (32%), conscientiousness (30%), neuroticism (25%), and finally

Page 22: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

10

agreeableness (18%) [65]. Notably, agreeableness is the least heritable trait, possibly explaining

its low predictive power. Further, Pilia et al. found in their family study that genetic effects

explained 19% of the variance in personality traits [66].

Givens et al. took the traits further and demonstrated parallel personality profiles in

offspring of long lived individuals. They found that children of centenarians displayed lower levels

of neuroticism and higher extraversion, much like the profile of their parents, when compared to

a normative population [67]. Not only are domains heritable, but there is evidence that specific

combinations could be inherited together.

Heritability links personality to genetic factors, conservatively estimated at around 40%.

Yet, there is still a large amount of personality that must therefore be explained through other

mechanisms such as environmental influences. Given these other factors could be influencing

personality the stability or personality must be addressed.

1.2.4. Stability of Personality

By its definition, personality is considered to be relatively stable. Drastic alteration of

personality from moment to moment is rare. There are, however, biological pressures that do

cause changes in personality. Chapman et al. noted that personality can change over the course

of a long period through intentional interventions or as a by-product of aging [47]. In a meta-

analysis performed by Roberts and Viechtbauer, they found that there was increasing positive

gains in personality throughout the lifetime, with exceptions of openness and the extraversion

facets contributing to social vitality, which decreased in old age [68]. These changes, however,

were small (at most 1 SD for the whole lifetime) and domains of conscientiousness and

agreeableness suffered from cohort standing [68]; where there are baseline differences between

the cohorts, in this case due to generational differences [68]. The report found that the biggest

changes in their comparison of cohorts occurred in early adulthood, from age 20-40, well below

our study population.

Aging is a biological occurrence that can cause minor shifts in personality. Personality is

not considered stable until adulthood, where it starts to stabilize in individuals around the age of

30 [66,69,70]. There is some debate as to the reasons for this stability; some argue that it is the

reflection of increasingly stable environmental factors such as mature romantic relationships and

occupation [42]. While personality is relatively stable, it does not mean that it is immutable.

Page 23: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

11

Terracciano et al. found that while personality was stable, the stability coefficients for traits rarely

reached 1.0, which would indicate constant stability [66]. Indeed, population studies have shown

there are consistent predictable changes to personality as individuals age, such that typically

there is a decrease in extraversion, neuroticism and openness with an increase in

conscientiousness and agreeableness [71,72].

Personality can be a flexible characteristic but there are limitations on the amount of

natural change that can occur. Roberts proposed that personality has set points that people vary

around throughout their lifetime [73], and that change is limited. Roberts further found in a meta-

analysis of longitudinal studies using a test-retest correlation coefficient that personality was more

stable with advancing age [70]. While personality is not a fixed phenotype throughout the lifespan,

there is enough evidence to suggest that personality in adulthood is relatively stable and that

changes that do occur are mild. Aging is a biological force that affects personality and should be

given consideration and accounted for when necessary. Other biological factors to consider are

the affects of sex and gender.

1.2.5. Personality Gender Differences

As hinted in the description of the factors, there are clear sex and gender differences in

personality as with most other biological systems. In psychology, gender is used to describe an

individual’s perception of being male or female from their social and cultural context [74]. Sex is

used in biology and other sciences to describe the physical manifestation of being male or female

[74]. Psychology typically measures gender and biology usually measures sex.

For trait theory, studies have generally found women to score higher in the agreeableness

and neuroticism factors than men [75–78]. Men have consistently scored higher in openness

[75,76,78]. These differences between genders will be taken into account during analysis.

1.2.6. Personality Profile of the Very Old and Very Healthy

Investigations into longevity and health have revealed a particular big five personality

profile for healthy seniors, such that healthy long-lived seniors demonstrate high

conscientiousness, extraversion, openness and low neuroticism. Conscientiousness is the largest

predictive factor for longevity, with most studies reporting a protective effect [79–83]. Wilson et al

Page 24: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

12

in particular reported that risk of death was halved in subjects reporting high conscientiousness

when compared to those with low conscientiousness [82].

Another major predictor was neuroticism. High neuroticism was consistently shown to be

detrimental to longevity [79,81,82,84]. Specifically, Shipley et al. found that individuals high in

neuroticism had a 12% increase in the risk of death from cardiovascular disease [85]. Duberstein

et al. found that high neuroticism was associated with poorer perceived health and found that this

perception became more pronounced with age [86], possibly indicating a mental health

mechanism for the decreased mortality in individuals with high neuroticism.

Additionally, extraversion was positively associated with longevity [79–81,84,86], as was

openness [80,81,86]. Terracciano in particular found that for every standard deviation gain in

either conscientiousness, emotional stability or the extraversion facet activity, there was an

increase in average life span of 2 to 3 years [79]. Agreeableness was not found to be a strong

predictor of longevity. Decidedly there is a distinctive personality profile for the very old and very

healthy.

1.2.7. Limitation in the Personality Field

Personality is not a physical trait, and we rely on indirect measurements as mentioned

previously. The predominant measurement used is the self-reported inventory, which is not

without drawbacks. It relies on subjects to complete the inventories honestly, and cannot account

for their biases or perceptions of themselves. There are, however, many other metrics that may

be used. Inventories completed by someone who knows the subject, behavioural measurements

(how frequently a particular behaviour is done), response tests (how an individual responds to a

specific stimuli), nonverbal tests (for example, report which pictures best describe you),

physiological measures (such as stress tests), or tests of ability (resisting temptation, recognizing

facial responses), are all options that could be used in conjunction with the self-reported inventory

to establish a more precise personality profile [47]. Ease and costs are factors that prevent

additional metrics from being used.

Selection of the proper inventory is also a consistent issue in personality. While inventories

are very similar, as previously discussed, some tend to show more positive results depending on

what relationships are under study. Studies using the big five model‘s NEO-PR-I inventory, for

example, consistently report positive associations with the HTTLPR gene when compared to

Page 25: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

13

those that employ Eysenck's Tri-Personality Questionnaire (TPQ) inventory [87]. While it is difficult

to establish what this means, it does indicate that proper selection of which inventory to use is

important. While the big five model’s inventories seem to be used most often for personality

related to biology there could be other models that better describe personality.

In recent years, there has been an emergence of a modified big five model. Current

personality models have a distinct lack of facets or domains that would properly predict or describe

less desirable behaviour patterns. The 6-domain HEXACO theory proposes that there is another

domain called Honesty-Humility which captures ethical behaviour. This model rearranges the big

five so that the opposite of honesty-humility is referred to as the dark triad and looks to capture

psychoticism, neuroticism and Machiavellian behaviours [88]. An individual measurement on the

honesty-humility to dark triad scale has so far shown to be a good predictor of sex, power and

money behaviours [89], and it will be interesting to see what associations it may yield. While this

model and its inventories still need to be validated, it’s important to remember that while current

tools used in personality may be validated and reliable, they may not capture all aspects of

personality and in the future there could be much better measures of personality available.

Noticeably in the studies discussed so far, associations have typically been made with the

domains of personality. This is characteristic in personality association studies but presents a

problem regarding the specificity of associations [47]. Rarely are facets analyzed for their

relationships to health, behaviours or even genetics; yet, the intrinsic processes that govern facets

could potentially be vastly different. It does not seem plausible that biological processes regulating

anxiety would also control impulsivity; however, these are both facets of neuroticism. In general,

it seems that specific facets are more useful in measuring specific measures of health, like blood

pressure, whereas domains may be best suited for measurements of general overall health, like

subject health reports and disease status [90]. Additionally, there are personality profiles,

characteristic mixes of domains, which are rarely evaluated.

Kinnunen et al. found that certain personality profiles were associated with subjective

reported health and level of symptoms experienced. Those with resilient profiles (high

extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness with low neuroticism) were found to report

better health, fewer symptoms and low psychological distress when compared to other profiles.

Individuals with high extraversion and neuroticism , referred to as the over-controlled profile,

reported the worst subjective health and the most symptoms [91]. These profiles are useful

Page 26: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

14

predictors that should be evaluated more often, as they could strengthen personality’s ability to

be used as a health predictor.

1.2.8. The Impact of Personality on Behaviour

Personality can be just as effective in predicting health outcomes as the standard socio-

economical status (SES) or IQ scores. Roberts et al. found in their meta-analysis that

conscientiousness, extraversion, neuroticism and hostility were stronger predictors of mortality

when compared to SES; they noted there were mixed results for agreeableness and openness

[92]. They also found that conscientiousness, agreeableness, and neuroticism were better

predictors of divorce, and personality was similar in ability to predict occupation [92]. Weiss et al.

used a senior sample to further refine these relationships to facets showing that survival was

associated with higher impulsiveness (neuroticism), straightforwardness (agreeableness), and

most strongly with self-discipline (conscientiousness) [93]. Here, high conscientiousness and high

neuroticism stand out as good predictors of mortality.

Personality is able to predict health outcomes because it is associated with decision styles

and behaviour choices. Flynn et al. reviewed personality and decision styles in a senior population

and found that seniors who scored high in conscientiousness and openness but low in neuroticism

and agreeableness were associated with having the most active decision style, deliberative

autonomist [94]. Further, the deliberative autonomists were more likely to be female and

nondeliberative delegators were the least active in decision making and showed an opposite

personality profile [94]. Personality affects decision styles, which in turn affects behaviour choices.

As highlighted in the description of personality domains, personality is associated with

healthy behaviours. Young male militants who engaged in wellness behaviours, accident control

and less frequent risk taking were associated with higher reported conscientiousness and

agreeableness scores and lower neuroticism scores [95]. A meta-analysis of conscientiousness

by Bogg et al. showed that conscientiousness could predict a variety of health behaviours.

Predictive power of conscientiousness from strongest to weakest behaviour correlation are drug

behaviours, risky driving, excessive alcohol use, violence, tobacco use, risky sex, unhealthy

eating, suicide, and activity [60].

It has been proposed that individuals high in conscientiousness are better able to predict

and plan for future negative events, which gives them an advantage for preventing the escalation

Page 27: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

15

of stressful situations and enhances their coping [61]. Extraversion’s optimism facet has also been

proposed to encourage healthy behaviours by guiding individuals towards adaptive coping

whereby strategies to eliminate, reduce or manage stress are utilized more frequently [96].

Personality domains are important predictors of behaviours and decision styles. If personality

affects behaviours and there is evidence of genetic heritability (as previously discussed) than

there must be a modulating biological component to personality.

1.3. Biochemistry and Physiology of Personality

Linking decision making and complex health behaviours to physiological processes is still

a developing field but there has been progress that is of interest to this project. The focus of this

section will be on specific neuro-physiology and the biochemistry that controls behavioural

processes in relation to personality and aging.

1.3.1. Age Related Changes

Aging results in many structural changes throughout the brain. In general there is a loss

of brain mass replaced by an increasing amount of cerebrospinal fluid [97]. The diminishing mass

represents both grey matter (brain tissue consisting of glial, neurons and vasculature responsible

for processing and cognition) and white matter (the myelinated axons of neuronal cells

responsible for transmission of signals from one area to another in the brain).

Sex differences, in relation to the proportion of white and grey matter, persist in aging.

Lemaitre et al. found that the rate of loss for grey and white matter was the same for men and

women but still found classic dimorphism of tissues where men showed larger compartmental

volumes with more white matter and cerebrospinal fluid while women showed larger grey matter

volumes [98]. This sexual dimorphism is also seen with cognitive performance tests, where the

elderly display the same pattern of women performing better in psychomotor speed, verbal

learning and memory task and men score higher on visuoconstruction and visual perception tasks

[99].

Page 28: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

16

1.3.2. Personality Domains and Relevant Structures

There are few studies that examine the relationship between physiological structure or

processes and all the personality domains. Instead, studies often look at one or two specific

personality domains and their potential influence on a specific structure or activation of certain

area. Each domain is summarized below. Neuroticism and Extraversion are the most well

researched domains and have a substantially greater amount of literature and support for specific

associations with cognitive functions and structures.

Extraversion

Many structures have been identified as being associated with extraversion. Generally,

there is greater cortical volume in the prefrontal and frontal cortex with higher scores of

extraversion. Specifically, volumes of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [100], medial orbitofrontal

cortex [101], temporal lobe [100], and the mid and superior frontal cortexes [100] are all positively

associated with higher extraversion. Functionally, extraverts display increased glucose

metabolism activity in the orbitofrontal cortex [102], the right putamen [103], and the middle

temporal gyrus [103], compared to introverts. Together these regions create planned activity,

including working memory [104], evaluation of outcomes [105], decision making [105], learning

[106], and facial attraction [107].

Interestingly, Bjornebekk et al., found that there was a negative association between

extraversion and inferior frontal gyrus volume, with the excitement seeking facet as the major

contributor to the association [108]. This relationship was previously established by Blankstein et

al. but only found in females [109]. Bjornebekk proposed this is related to the uninhibited pattern

of speech that is demonstrated by some extraverts, as this area is important for inhibition of

activities such as risky behaviours [108].

Anterior cingulate volume was also positively associated with extraversion [100].

Connectivity to this area was also found to have a positive association with extraversion,

particularly the facets warmth, gregariousness and positive emotions [110]. Increased activation

of the anterior cingulate has been positively associated with extraversion when comparing positive

to neutral stimulation, with facet analysis revealing that excitement seeking and warmth

contributed the most to the activation [110]. The anterior cingulate also plays a role in learning

and reward dependence [111].

Page 29: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

17

Extraversion has also been connected to the limbic system which collective manages

intrinsic drives and emotions. Cohen et al. looked at the strength of white matter connectivity

between the limbic system and striatum with varying levels of extraversion [112]. They found that

there was a positive association between the strength of connectivity and extraversion; particular

fiber tracks in the hippocampus, amygdala and ventral striatum were predicted by the novelty

seeking facet and tracks between the prefrontal cortex and striatum were predicted by the reward

dependence facet [112]. This could indicate a stronger or faster information relay to these areas

for extraverts.

The orbitofrontal cortex is a structure that is very involved in extraversion and its role in

the reward system. The reward system is a dopamergenic system that is key for cognitive control

and learning [113]. It controls the ability to observe some event or action in the environment and

make a prediction of a potential reward or outcome using negative or positive feedback [114]. The

signal begins in the ventral tegemental area of the brain, within the midbrain, which projects to

the ventral striatum’s nucleus accumbens or to the prefrontal cortex [115]. This pathway is called

the mesolimbic dopamine pathway and contains 80% of the brain’s dopamine [116]. The system

can be broken down into two phases, anticipation and receipt or outcome [114].

The nucleus accumbens is associated with anticipation of rewards. The prefrontal cortex,

in particular the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, has been associated with reward outcome [117].

While there are no major healthy aging changes in the system for reaction time, accuracy or task

performance, Vink et al. found there was a difference in activity for the ventral striatum (where the

nucleus accumbens lies) activation [114]. They found the area of reward anticipation was less

active for their older subjects, perhaps the results of learned patterns over the lifetime.

Deckersbach et al. has proposed that the orbitofrontal cortex guides motivation behaviour

and decision making, so that individuals high in orbitofrontal metabolism/activation may value

more external positive rewards than lower metabolizers [102]. Hooker et al. hypothesized that

extraverts have greater sensitivity to the type of reward or the value placed on the object instead

of a constant higher sensitivity to rewards [118]. As extraversion is related to reward-dependence,

there is a strong connection to dopamine function [118,119].

Page 30: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

18

Neuroticism

Due to its links with anxiety and depression, neuroticism has been extensively researched

for a physiological origin. Findings include a few positive associations between neuroticism and

grey matter volume, notably visual stream areas [100], ventral anterior cingulate cortex [109] ,

mid cingulate cortex [101] and the cerebellum [101]. The best understood connection is to the

anterior cingulate cortex, which will be discussed further below.

Many structures have been negativity associated with higher neuroticism scores. The

orbitofrontal cortex [100,120,121], ventrolateral prefrontal cortex [120], dorsolateral prefrontal

cortex [100,120], dorsomedial prefrontal cortex [101], precentral gyrus [101], medial temporal lobe

[101], middle frontal gyrus [121], superior frontal gyrus [121,122], inferior frontal gyrus [122], and

general cortical thickness [120,122] were all areas identified as having reduced volume.

The middle frontal gyrus is important in top-down control of executive and attentional

processes and is specifically associated with the impulsivity facet of neuroticism [121]. It is a

proposed mediator of impulsivity [121], which is generated in the orbitofrontal cortex [105]. The

facet vulnerability is negatively correlated with anterior cingulate cortex volume [108]. Many

specific areas are seemingly affected by neuroticism, but there is strong evidence of lower total

volume associated with higher neuroticism scores.

Many studies have linked higher neuroticism with lower total grey matter volume

[108,120,123]. Specific facets such as anxiety and self-conscientiousness have been found to be

the largest contributors [123], and depression, anxiety and vulnerability to stressors have been

found to contribute to lower gray matter volume, particularly in the frontotemporal region [108].

White matter is also reported as being affected by higher neuroticism [108,123], Jackson

et al. saw that there was a large white matter decline in an aging brain for subjects reporting

higher neuroticism scores when compared to their low scoring equivalents [120]. Xu et al. further

found that there were specific white matter tracks that showed worse integrity in high neuroticism

individuals [124]. Tracks running from the anterior cingulum and uncinate fasciculus connect the

amygdala to the medial and lateral prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate and the orbitofrontal

cortex [124]. The amygdala is an important structure for emotional regulation, self-regulation and

self-referential processes. It is interconnected to the anterior cingulate cortex and many areas in

Page 31: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

19

the prefrontal cortex [125]. It is often the target structure in studies about anxiety, as dysfunction

is related to unregulated emotional states.

Neuroticism is well known for its connection to amygdala activation [110,118,126]. Ormel

et al. have summarized the findings of the positive association between neuroticism and

amygdala activity, describing a negative coupling between the left amygdala and the anterior

cingulate cortex and a positive coupling between the right amygdala and the medial prefrontal

cortex [125]. They hypothesized that it causes reduced control over evaluating negative stimuli

but increased self-referential evaluation, such that there is a hyper arousal state in the amygdala

that is not regulated by the anterior cortex or the prefrontal cortex [125]. This leads to an amygdala

that is on high alert, constantly looking for potential threats, or sustained vigilant monitoring

behaviour [125].

Many other studies support the hypothesis of increased anxiety. High neuroticism has also

been linked to higher activity in the insular cortex, which is important for homeostasis as it

regulates blood pressure, heart rate, respiration and gastrointestinal activity [102]. Feinstein et al.

looked at the anterior insular cortex function between neurotic and non-neurotic subjects, finding

that there was increased activation for the neurotic individuals in the action selection phase of a

decision-making task where the outcome was certain [127]. This could mean there is increased

anxiety around everyday low risk decisions in individuals suffering from anxiety. The anterior

insula may also play a role in the development and maintenance of anxiety and its activation

during a task where the outcome may reveal the increased anxiety felt by subjects who score

highly on neuroticism scales. Deckersbach et al. also investigated this region, looking at resting

regional cerebral glucose metabolism rates in the insular cortex and found there was a negative

correlation to neuroticism [102]. They hypothesized that this lower baseline metabolism caused

subjects to be more sensitive to signal changes in this region causing the increased activation

and ultimately anxiety [102].

The anterior cingulate cortex is another structure connected to neuroticism. It can be

divided into two sections, dorsal and ventral. The dorsal section is cognitively specialized for

recognizing errors, problem-solving and adapting to changing conditions [111]. It is connected to

the prefrontal cortex and monitors performance and rewards. The ventral section is specialized

towards emotional processes, and receives projections from the amygdala [111].The anterior

cingulate and amygdala function as a circuit that is regulated by serotonin, with the ventral anterior

cingulate showing positive coupling and the dorsal anterior cingulate showing negative coupling

Page 32: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

20

with the amygdala [128]. This relationship could explain why there is sometimes difficultly problem

solving as anxiety builds.

There is also evidence of stronger coupling between the amygdala and ventral anterior

cingulate when neurotic subjects processed an emotional conflict [110]. The authors noted that

this was primarily driven by anxiety related facets of neuroticism, such that as situations become

more stressful there could be more processing through emotional processes, resulting in

increased anxiety, anger or depression [110].

Conscientiousness

Conscientiousness has been positively linked to a variety of structural changes in the

orbitofrontal prefrontal cortex [100,120], dorsomedial prefrontal cortex [100], premotor cortex

[100], putamen [100], middle frontal gyrus [101], frontopolar cortex [100], and lateral prefrontal

cortex [101] and negatively linked to the superior temporal gyrus and the supramarginal gyrus

areas [108]. Given the definition of individuals high in conscientiousness, it is not surprising to find

that these areas have a function in control. Where there could be heightened function in decision

making [105,129], moral reasoning and nonsocial semantic processing [130], learning [106,131],

planning and voluntary movement [100,132], cognitive and behavioural regulation [133], hand

manipulation [131], and assigning movement to specific environmental cues [131], there is a

potential for lessened capability in the processing of auditory stimuli and social cognition [134],

and lower ability to make social judgments due to poor regulation of the self-other distinction [135].

Higher conscientiousness is good for planning and learning but seems to decrease certain social

processes.

Agreeableness

Agreeableness has been positively linked to the volume of brain structures including the

lateral orbitofrontal cortex [100], cingulate cortex [101], and the fusiform gyrus [101]. Intriguingly,

Rankin et al. also found a positive relationship between agreeableness and the orbitofrontal

cortex; however, they found there was a difference between the right and left volumes, where the

right half was positively associated and the left was negatively associated [136]. They have

suggested that this split is caused by the right half playing a larger role in social functioning and

the left half looking after self-interests [136].

Page 33: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

21

Negative associations have also been found with the volumes of dorsomedial prefrontal

cortex [100], superior temporal sulcus [101] and the superior temporal gyrus [101]. These

structures may have functions in social cognition [130], interpreting the gaze and emotionally body

language of another person [137], and auditory processing and processing social cognition [134].

Openness

Openness is a difficult domain to study due to its association with intelligence. Often

measurements are for IQ or other tests of intelligence that are not specifically targeted towards

the personality domain. For example, when investigating the integrity of white matter tracks, there

is an association between higher openness and better integrity of the track near the dorsolateral

prefrontal cortex [124]. There is a general global increase in integrity, however, for subjects high

in openness that could be due to increased intelligence which would universally raise the integrity

of the white matter [124]. Further, specific facets have been associated with certain types of

intelligence and greater activation in the prefrontal cortex. Ideas and values were found to be

positively associated with fluid general cognitive ability, which is a measure of raw cognitive ability

[138]. Crystallized general cognitive ability, a measure of acquired knowledge, was found to be

positively associated with all facets of openness except for action [138]. DeYoung argues that

openness is related to dopamine through its activation of the prefrontal cortex [138].

Other areas that have shown a positive association with openness include the frontopolar

cortex [100], vs. negative associations with the lateral parietal cortex [122], fronto insular cortex

[100] and the medial orbitofrontal cortex [100,122]. The frontopolar cortex is involved with decision

making but functions to suspend certain tasks while other, more immediate plans, are

implemented [129]. This could function to protect long term plans from being overwhelmed by day

to day tasks, an important skill for intelligent animals to have. Some studies suggest this area has

a function in creativity [139] which would further highlight its connection to the openness domain.

The lateral parietal cortex, fronto-insular cortex and the medial orbitofrontal cortex studied are

potentially involved in episodic memory [140], homeostasis [102], and cautionary or inhibitory

responses which activate to social, aversive physical and fear of loss [100].

1.3.3. Neurotransmitters and Personality

Early research into the biology of affective disorders (now commonly referred to as mood

disorders such as bipolar disorder or depression) shed light on the connection between

Page 34: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

22

personality and neurotransmitters, particularly how the catecholamine group of neurotransmitters

are linked to depression through pharmacological inhibition or activation [141]. Monoamines that

have received the most attention in such studies are the catecholamines, dopamine and

norepinephrine, and the indol amine, serotonin [142].

Dopamine has a strong connection to promotion of positive and negative incentives [143].

These connections to positive and negative incentives are linked to the extraversion facet of the

NEO-FFI through the reward system already discussed in the physiology section. Dopamine is

generally found in the corpus striatum with lower concentrations throughout the brain [142]. The

corpus striatum relays information from the cerebral cortex, thalamus, and substantia nigra to the

basal ganglia, which controls movement. The system is referred to as the meso-cortical pathway

[144]. The striatum helps regulate the motivations of the higher functions of the cerebral cortex

and the lower functions from the mid-brain [145].

Dopamine concentrations in the brain experience age related changes. Increasing age

shows a lower activation of dopamine pathways in many parts of the brain, in addition to a more

reduced activation of the meso-cortical pathway [144]. Low dopamine levels have been strongly

linked to the development of Parkinson’s disease [143].

Norepinephrine is associated with attentional processes, in particular selective attention,

and acts as serotonin's antagonist [143]. It is a critical neurotransmitter for inhibiting distracting

stimuli. Due to its relationship with attention, norepinephrine has been linked in some studies with

the extraversion and agreeableness factors [146]. Impaired norepinephrine receptors cause

symptoms similar to attention deficient disorder and increased receptor function improves the

prefrontal cortex regulation of behaviour and enhances attention [147]. Norepinephrine is found

throughout the brain with particularly high concentrations in the hypothalamus, which is primarily

involved in managing homeostasis [142].

Like norepinephrine, serotonin is found throughout the brain with the highest

concentrations in the hypothalamus and throughout the rest of the limbic system [142]. The limbic

system is involved in memory, emotion, learning and motivation. Serotonin is an important

mediator of the sleep/wake/arousal cycle, circadian variation, and sensory stimulation [148].

Introduction of serotonin causes a reduction of motivated and emotional behaviours such as

feeding, play and sexual behaviours, but a promotion of sleep [143]. Serotonin has been studied

extensively for its connection with impulsivity and aggression, which are facets of neuroticism in

Page 35: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

23

the NEO-FFI. An inverse relationship has been found, such that low serotonin results in increased

aggression and high levels of serotonin can produce deceased aggression and increased

cooperativeness in individuals [149]. Low serotonin levels have also been linked to increased

impulsivity [149]. At the other end of the spectrum, high serotonin levels have been associated

with obsessive compulsive disorder, often characterized by a low level of impulsivity [149].

In connection with aging, dopamine and serotonin are two neurotransmitters that have

been implicated in cognitive decline in aging brains [150]. Monoamine oxidase function is another

chemical suspect in cognitive decline. Its presence increases with age, causing more free radicals

to be liberated from its catabolic activity. Anti-oxidation reserves become shallow in late life,

leaving free radicals to potentially harm synaptic pathways [150].

Personality has physiological origins, through neurological systems such as the dopamine

reward system, processes that regulate anxiety and impose potential structural changes in the

prefrontal cortex. These origins can be further reduced to look at their genetic basis.

1.4. The Genome and Genetic Variation

1.4.1. Basic Structures

The genome is an all encompassing term for the heritable genetic information found in a

cell. Human genetic information is organized into 46 chromosomes, a set of 23 from each parent.

At their simplest, chromosomes are two complimentary strands of deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA)

strings, with each nucleic acid representing a base pair (bp). There are approximately 3 billion

base pairs in the human genome [151], so the strands must be condensed into nucleosomes (a

histone and bound DNA), then into another helical structure called a solenoid, to fit within the

nucleus [152]. This packaging is dynamic and can become decondensed to expose the double

strand structure and the genes that are encoded on it.

A gene is a portion of a DNA strand that codes for a transcript of ribonucleic acid (RNA)

[152]. There are approximately 20-25,000 genes in the human genome, which is sparse

compared to the total size [153]. The RNA transcripts have a variety of functions and can also

experience post-transcriptional changes to further enhance their function. Messenger RNA

(mRNA), goes on to be translated into a shorter polypeptide chain, which can undergo further

Page 36: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

24

alterations to become a functional protein [152]. The transcript is translated through a redundant

coding system, such that every three nucleic acids specify a particular amino acid [152].

Genes are further organized into functional regions (Figure 1). The promoter is located at

the 5 prime end and promotes the initiation of transcription through the recruitment of

transcriptional factors. The gene also contains start and stop signals for transcription, and

untranslated sequences [152]. The 5 prime untranslated region contains a signal to start

transcription [152]. The 3 prime untranslated region contains a signal to stop transcription and

add multiple adenosine residues to the end of the transcript, creating a poly(A) tail [152]. The

transcript contains both coding and non-coding sequences. The coding sequences are referred

to as exons and the non-coding as introns [152]. Introns are removed post-transcription through

a process known as splicing [152].

Figure 1.1. General Gene Structure Promoter region (in purple) is of variable length and distance from the 5 prime untranslated region, depending on the gene.

Genes differ slightly between individuals, through small variations in the DNA sequence,

referred to as alleles [152]. These alleles can have a variety of functional effects in a gene and

can contribute to the organism’s phenotype. So while genetic material is nearly identical between

two people, these small variations are capable of coding an amazing diversity of phenotypes in

the human species.

Page 37: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

25

1.4.2. Microsatellites and Minisatellites

Microsatellites and minisatellites are referred to as repeating elements. They both consist

of 2-200 tandem repeats of a number of nucleotides [152]. Because of this variation in the repeat

number, these satellites are multi-allelic due to the different lengths of alleles. Microsatellites are

characterized as smaller repeats, between 2 to 4 nucleotides and are sometimes referred to as

simple sequence repeats (SSRs) [152]. Minisatellites are often referred to as variable number

tandem repeats (VNTR), and consist of repeats of 10 to 100 nucleotides [152].

These strings of repeats are common in the genome [154] and have a nonrandom

distribution [155]. They are theorized to arise from slippage and unequal cross over events (where

the chromosome pairs break and recombine) during replication [154]. The resulting expansion or

shortening of areas in the genome has an important evolutionary role in adding to the genetic

complexity of the genome. Slippage events favour growth where point mutations can break down

the repeating segment [155]. Fondon et al. explain that slippage mutations are readily reversible

unlike point mutations [156], and can have an evolutionary advantage.

Repeating strings are important structures for creating genetic variation and are influential

mechanisms for adaptive evolution. Variations in the length of the base unit, and the purity of the

repeats help with specific site adjustments to affect the mutation rate or the effect of the mutation

itself [157]. King has famously named this phenomenon the “genetic tuning knob” (analogous to

those of a stringed instrument), since they are able to influence gene activity and aid in quick and

efficient evolutionary adaptation [158]. Microsatellites and Minisatellites can have an impact gene

activity, through evident mechanisms such as when they are inserted in the coding region, and

through less apparent means.

VNTRs in the exonic, coding region, could result in the loss or gain of function, a frameshift

mutation or an expanded mRNA [155]. Fortunately frameshift is not a common mutation in VNTR

expansion/reduction. Wren et al. used a human complimentary DNA (DNA synthesized from

mRNA) database to predict the occurrence of potential frameshifting repeating elements in coding

regions. They found that 92% of the repeating elements were in multiples of three, which would

protect against frameshift mutations [159]. Interestingly, some detrimental exonic expansions

have neurological phenotypes [155], such as spinobulbar muscular atrophy and Huntington’s

disease. Li et al. proposed this was due to instability in the protein product of the affected gene

[155].

Page 38: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

26

VNTRs are more common in the untranslated regions of genes [159]. The 3 prime

untranslated region can cause transcriptional slippage, reducing expression, or result in an

expanded mRNA, while the 5 prime region can affect recruitment of both transcriptional and

translational machinery, affecting expression [155].

Impressively, intronic (non-coding region) VNTRs display a wide variety of effects on

genes. VNTRs can affect the efficiency of gene expression through two mechanisms: the binding

of transcriptional factors and alteration of splicing sites. A VNTR in intron 2 of the SLC6A4 gene

regulates transcription by binding transcriptional factor YB-1 [160]. YB-1 is a transcriptional

regulator that has been shown to have both repression and activation properties [161], but shows

activation in the SLC6A4 gene [160]. VNTRs can alter the spliceosome configuration or efficiency

of splicing. SSRs with a GGG sequence ( three guanines in tandem) have been implicated as

enhancers on the 5 prime end of an intron, as a place for splicing factors to bind, affecting

spliceosome assembly [162].

Splicing alterations caused by mini and micro-satellites can also influence which type of

mRNA is produced from a gene. Li et al. summarized the role of an SSR acting as an intronic

enhancer in CFTR. They show that shorter repeats significantly increased the skipping of exon 9,

and proposed that SSRs have functional significance in tissue specific exon inclusion [155].

Intronic VNTRs can also function as small regulatory RNAs. The eNOS gene has an

intronic VNTR that produces a small intronic repeat RNA (sir-RNA) that regulates expression of

the gene through negative feedback [163]. The greater the expression of the gene the more

sirRNA is produced, which in turn decreases expression of the gene. sirRNA is thought to affect

transcription by modifying methylation status, histone acetylation or by affecting splicing of the

mRNA [163,164].

VNTRs in imprinted regions have been implicated in affecting phenotype. The insulin

gene, INS, is within an imprinted region and has a VNTR upstream of it [164]. There are three

common alleles for the INS VNTR, alleles I, II, III [164]. Allele III is associated with an increased

risk of diabetes, with the risk being equivalent for the I/III, II/III, III/III genotypes. It was found,

however, that if the I allele of an individual with the I/III genotype came from the father, it conferred

a protective effective against diabetes [164]. Further, allele III inherited from the father was shown

to have increased risk of obesity and worse diabetic outcomes [164].

Page 39: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

27

Microsatellites and minisatellites can exert effects by many different mechanisms, from

direct insertion into the coding region, affecting transcriptional machinery, recruitment of

transcription factors, functioning as feedback mechanisms, alterations to the splice sites and even

amplifying the affects of parental origin imprints. Due to the wealth of potentially functional

capabilities of VNTRs and SSRs, any VNTRs found in candidate genes will be genotyped and

analyzed.

Microsatellites and minisatellites are currently not often used in looking at genetic

variations. More popular is the use of SNPs to genotype genes of interest; SNPs will also be used

in this project.

1.4.3. Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs)

SNPs are single nucleotide differences that are found throughout the genome and are

fairly uniformly distributed [152]. The ubiquity of SNPs has allowed creation of dense genetic

maps across the genome. Correlations between the genotypes of different SNPs have been

established in the form of a linkage disequilibrium map [152].

Linkage disequilibrium occurs when there is preferential association between two markers,

such that certain alleles are seen together more often than expected by chance [152]. If crossover

events (where chromosome pairs break and recombine their DNA strands) occurred completely

at random, we would expect to see to see a random distribution of crossing over events across

the genome, and alleles would be assorted randomly from each other. This is not the case. There

are stretches of DNA that show regions of preserved allele combinations across the genome

[152]. When two markers are in high linkage disequilibrium, for example if SNP #1’s ‘A’ allele is

usually found with SNP #2’s ‘C’ allele, then SNP #2 can be ‘tagged’ by genotyping SNP #1. SNP

tagging allows researchers to use fewer SNPs than the total number, to represent the genetic

variation across a gene.

This project uses SNPs and VNTRs as genetic polymorphisms for study. SNPs will be

used to tag across each candidate gene, and VNTRs will be genotyped when appropriate.

Variations in genes related to personality phenotypes are summarized in the next section.

Page 40: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

28

1.5. Candidate Genes

Candidate genes were selected based on a literature review described in the methods

section. Five genes were chosen based on their prevalence in the literature: COMT, DRD4,

MAOA, SLC6A4, and TH. Each gene is briefly overviewed for functional importance to personality

and notable polymorphisms.

1.5.1. Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT)

Figure 1.2. Genomic structure of COMT Showing selected SNPs, including the missense rs4680 SNP located in exon 3.

The COMT gene is located on chromosome 22q11.21 [165]. The gene contains 6 exons,

with exon 1 and 2 as non-coding exons in a transcript variant [166]; see figure 1.2. It encodes an

enzyme that inactivates catechols, which include dopamine, norepinephrine (formally called

noradrenaline), epinephrine, and L-dopa (a precursor of dopamine) [167]. The COMT protein

catalyzes the inactivation by transferring a methyl group of S-adenosylmethionine to the hydroxyl

group on the catechols [167].

Tenhunen et al. found that exon 3 distinguishes the production of two variant transcripts,

a short 1.3kb and a longer 1.5kb. There are two promoters and two transcription initiating codons.

The proximal promoter is located between the two translational initiation codons, approximately

200bp up stream of the 1.5kb transcript’s ATG start codon [166]. The proximal promoter produces

the 1.3kb transcript and the more distal promoter produces the 1.5kb transcript [166]. The two

transcripts code for two variations of the COMT protein, a soluble form and a membrane-bound

form [168]. The 1.3kb codes for the soluble or S-COMT protein and the 1.5kb codes for the

Page 41: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

29

membrane-bound MB-COMT protein. The variants are tissue specific and differentiated by the

presence of a hydrophobic amino acid extension in the MB-COMT protein [166]. MB-COMT was

also shown to have a lower Vmax, a measure of the rate of a reaction, with the same substrates

when compared to S-COMT [169]. However MB-COMT has a higher affinity for dopamine

compared to S-COMT [170].

The COMT enzyme competes with the Monoamine oxidase (MAOA) enzyme to inactivate

catechols. Rivett et al. studied the location and substrate affinity of both COMT and MAOA

enzymes [171]. They found that dopamine is preferentially inactivated by MAOA and

norepinephrine is metabolized by both COMT and MAOA [171]. In addition they showed that

COMT consistently had a lower affinity for the same substrates as MAOA and that the presence

of either COMT or MAOA was tissue dependent [171].

Matsumoto et al. determined that the highest levels of COMT mRNA are in the prefrontal

cortex, and that 70% of that COMT mRNA coded for MB-COMT protein [172], suggestive of

function in that location. They also found levels of COMT mRNA in structures important for the

dopamine reward system, including the striatum and the midbrain’s substantia nigra and ventral

tegmental area, although at much lower levels [172].

Matsumoto el al hypothesized that COMT may function as the main dopamine regulator

in the prefrontal cortex, since there is little recycling of dopamine there [170]. This is supported

by studies with Comt knockout mice, which show an increase in prefrontal cortex dopamine [172].

COMT encodes a protein that has a clear physiological effect and variations in this gene could

plausibly alter an organism’s phenotype and, in particular, alter personality through affecting the

prefrontal cortex.

One polymorphism that has undergone extensive research is the rs4680 SNP, which

corresponds to a methionine or valine amino acid in the protein. Biochemically the valine variant

increases protein activity by 38% [173], possibly due to valine being a more hydrophobic amino

acid than methionine, which helps to stabilize the surface of the protein [173]. The valine variant

has been found to be more thermostable compared to the methionine variant [169], supporting

the claim that the valine variant is more stable generally. Homozygous methionine individuals

were associated with improved performance on a sorting task used to measure prefrontal

cognitive function [174,175], indicating that lower activity of the COMT enzyme may help with

higher cognitive function resulting from more available dopamine.

Page 42: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

30

Estrogen is a regulator of COMT and females are known to have consistently lower COMT

protein blood levels. Chen et al. also found their female participants on average had lower levels

of COMT activity [173]. As such, it is particularly important to conduct sex specific analysis in this

gene.

1.5.2. Dopamine Receptor D4 Gene (DRD4)

Figure 1.3. Genomic structure of DRD4 Showing a functional VNTR located in exon 3.

DRD4 is located on chromosome 11p15.5 [176]. The gene contains four exons and codes

for a 387 amino acid protein that has 7 transmembrane domains, an n-linked glycoslytion site and

several phosporylation sites [177], see figure 1.3.

The DRD4 receptor belongs to a family of D2-like receptors [178]. The receptor is activated

by dopamine and inhibits the activity of adenylyl cyclase, which catalyzes the conversion of

adenosine triphosphate to 3 prime, 5 prime cyclic AMP [178]. This receptor works to inhibit

dopamine signaling [178]. The receptor is located throughout the central nervous system, and is

highly concentrated in the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, amygdala, hypothalamus, and meso-

limbic pathways (midbrain to striatum) [179], structures implicated in the dopamine reward

system.

DRD4 has a highly researched VNTR in exon 3. The tandem repeat is 48bp and codes

for the third cytoplasmic loop of the protein [180]. There can be 2-11 repeats of the 48bp repeat

but the majority of alleles are the 2 (2R), 4 (4R) and 7 (7R) repeats [180]. The 4R allele is most

frequent, at a 64.3% global allele frequency [181]. The 7R allele has a frequency of 20.6%,

Page 43: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

31

although this is higher in an American population at 48.3%; the 2R allele has the lowest frequency

at 8.2%, but reaches 18.1% in an Asian population [181]. Other alleles were found to vary widely

between populations [181]. The 4R allele is considered the oldest allele as it is present in 36

studied populations [181]. It is thought that the 7R allele is relatively new as evidenced by to its

strong flanking linkage disequilibrium [182].

The 7R allele produces a functionally diminished receptor for adenylyl cyclase binding

[178]. The 2R and 4R alleles have receptor activity twice that of 7R [178]. Though not statistically

significant, there were possible small differences between the 2R and 4R alleles’ protein activity

[178]. Schoots et al. have suggested that there is also reduced expression of the gene with the

7R allele, potentially caused by an unstable RNA or translational inefficiencies [183]. The bulk of

the research however, has been on the functionality of the encoded receptor.

Adding to the complexity, dopamine can function as a chaperone to improperly folding

DRD4 receptors [184]. Van Craenenbroeck et al. illustrated that the 2R, 4R and 7R alleles have

different sensitivities to this chaperone effect [184]. A continuous relationship was found such that

the 7R experienced twice the up-regulating effect of the chaperone when compared to the 2R

allele [184]. The 4R allele was not significantly different from either and fell between the two in

terms of increased cellular response of the DRD4 receptor produced [184]. The researchers

hypothesized that the 2R allele experienced less up-regulation because the protein efficiently

folds on its own or it is too rigid and doesn’t allow for chaperone assistance [184]. This study

supports the notion that the decreased function of the 7R allele is the result of a mis-folded protein.

1.5.3. Monoamine Oxidase A (MAOA)

Figure 1.4. Genomic structure of MAOA Showing a functional VNTR 1.2kb upstream from the coding region.

Page 44: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

32

Located on chromosome Xp11.23, MAOA has 15 exons and spans 60kb [185]; see figure

1.4. There is a promoter with two 90bp repeats [186]. Whether the gene escapes x-inactivation

or not has been a topic of debate, however, recent consensus is that x-inactivation does affect

this gene [187–189].

MAOA catalyzes oxidative deamination, which is the removal of an amine group, from

various biogenic amines, with the production of hydrogen peroxide [190]. There are two forms of

the monoamine oxidase, A and B, and each has different affinities for different substrates [190].

MAOA prefers serotonin but has an affinity to norepinephrine, dopamine and an inhibitor named

clorgyline [191]. The B form mainly catalyzes phenylethylamine, benzylamine and deprenyl [190].

While both forms are found throughout the brain, MAOA is mostly found in the dopamine and

norepinephrine pathways and in a specialized nucleus, called locus coeruleus, in the pons region

[190]. Schildkraut summarized that when MAO inhibitors are used there is a global increase of

norepinephrine in the brain [141], lending support that MAO is important in norepinephrine

pathways.

Studies by Buckholtz et al. found that MAOA dysregulation is linked to a particular

personality profile, such that individuals showed enhanced reactivity to threat cues (harm

avoidance), increased tendency to experience anger, frustration, and bitterness (anger hostility),

and reduced sensitivity to cues that elicit and maintain prosocial behaviour (reward dependence)

[192]. Harm avoidance is noted to have associations with serotonin functions and reward

dependence with norepinephrine [192]. Given the traits found in this study, there is evidence for

neuroticism being influenced by the MAOA gene and physiologically linked to serotonin and

norepinephrine.

MAOA has a functional VNTR about 1.2kb upstream from the coding region that affects

transcriptional activity. The tandem repeat is 30bp and repeats 2, 3, 3.5, 4, or 5 times [191]. There

are two categories that the repeats are typically used; Sabol et al. found in vitro that the 3.5R and

4R are transcribed approximately 2-10 more times than the 3R or 5R depending on the

combination of alleles [191]. The 3.5R and 4R are grouped as high functioning and the 3R and

5R are grouped as low functioning [191]. This was supported by Denney et al. who found similar

transcription rates [193]. In further support of this categorization, Wu et al. tested if MAOA affected

serotonin concentration in the pineal glands of Alzheimer’s patients (serotonin is a precursor of

melatonin, which is reduced in Alzheimer’s patients) [194]. While they did not find a difference

between the melatonin between their two groups they did find that 3.5R and 4R alleles ultimately

Page 45: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

33

showed higher MAOA expression in the cells [194]. The VNTR alleles can therefore be reduced

to two categories, high (MAOAH) and low (MAOAL) functioning.

The MAOAH alleles have been linked to depression [195–197]. Dannolowski et al. looked

at MAOA alleles and the coupling of the amygdala with the prefrontal cortex in depressive and

non-depressive brains (the amygdala generates the affect and the prefrontal cortex regulates the

emotion [125]). Dannolowski et al. found that 3.5R and 4R alleles were associated with a reduced

connectivity and that if a carrier was depressed the symptoms were amplified [197]. In connection

to this, Meyer et al. found that MAOA expression was increased by approximately 34% through

the brains of depressed individuals when compared to healthy matched controls [196]. The high

functioning allele does seem to play a role in major depression, by possibly reducing the MAOA

substrate serotonin too drastically.

The MAOAL allele is highly associated with aggression in males [192,195,198]. Early

studies in Maoa knockout mice found that the knockout males became hyper-aggressive [199].

Huang et al. went on to show that MAOAL men had a higher risk of impulsivity if there was a

history of childhood abuse [198], hinting at the possibility of epigenetic effects. Buckholtz et al.

were able to deduce a cognitive consequence by looking at the structural and functional effects

of the MAOAL allele. They found that MAOAL males had decreased anterior cingulate activation

and ventral prefrontal engagement [192]. In particular the perigenual anterior cingulate was

affected which regulates negative amygdala function. They further suggested that MAOAL males

compensate by engaging the ventro-medial prefrontal cortex [192]. Where the MAOAH allele

reduces substrate levels too much, it seems as though the MAOAL allele does not reduce them

enough resulting in an over activated state.

Nishioka summarized in a meta-analysis the various associations for each allele, finding

hypoactive behaviours for MAOAH, such as depression, anxiety and neuroticism [195]. For

MAOAL, they found hyperactive behaviours like impulsivity, aggression, personality conduct

disorder and ADHD [195].

Page 46: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

34

1.5.4. Sodium Chloride Dependent Transporter (SLC6A4)

Figure 1.5. Genomic structure of SLC6A4 Showing two VNTRs, HTTPLR in the promoter region and STin2 in intron 2.

The sodium chloride dependent transporter gene is located on chromosome 17q11.2,

contains 14 exons and is approximately 31kb in size [200]; see figure 1.5. The gene codes for 5-

hydroxytryptamine transporter (5-HTT), or the serotonin transporter [201]. The sodium/chloride

dependent transporter clears serotonin from the synaptic gap after an action potential in neurons

of the central and peripheral nervous systems [201]. This reabsorbed serotonin is either recycled

or degraded [202]. Changes to 5-HTT can therefore result in prolonged or shortened serotonin

presence in the gap.

There can be a variety of consequences depending on what type of serotonin receptor is

present on the post-synaptic neuron. There are 7 families of serotonin receptors (5-HT1, 5-HT2

…, 5-HT7), with 15 subfamilies [202]. The first 3 families are expressed in the brain [203], and are

therefore of interest to this project. The 5-HT1 family is an inhibitor of adenylyl cyclase, with both

subfamilies usually found on post-synaptic neurons [202]. The 5-HT2 family binds ligands that

increase the hydrolysis of inositol phosphates which in turn increases the cytosolic calcium

concentration [203]. 5-HT3 family receptors bind ligands that control cation channels [202]. Low

5-HTT activity can cause reduced receptor binding in 5-HT1 but increased binding in 5-HT2 and

5-HT3 receptors [202], causing an overall excitatory effect.

In a review conducted by Lesch et al., functional importance of the serotonin transporter

was highlighted in its role as a drug target for serotonin-reuptake-inhibitors [204]. SLC6A4

Page 47: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

35

regulates serotonin and deviation in its ability to regulate serotonin in the gap could have

implications for neuronal development [204], since serotonin plays a key role in the differentiation

of serotonergic and glutamatergic neurons [202]. Lesch et al. highlighted research areas of

depression, anxiety, stress response and aggression in connection to SLC6A4 dysregulation

[204].

SLC6A4 is another gene that can be regulated by estrogen levels. McEwn et al. found that

estrogen treatments in women caused a decrease in SLC6A4 mRNA expression [205].

Stratification of sexes in analysis is of importance for this gene.

There are 2 VNTRs located in SLC6A4, the 5HTTLPR VNTR, located approximately 1Kb

upstream from the start codon in the promoter region [206], and the STin2 VNTR located in the

second intron [200]. Lesch et al. characterized the function of the 22bp 5HTTLPR VNTR in a

predominantly male population [206] . The VNTR has two common alleles, the 14 repeat and the

16 repeat, called the short ‘S’ and long ‘L’ alleles, respectively [206]. Lesch et al. found that the L

allele had a frequency of 57% and the S of 43% (Lesch et al., 1996). When the gene was inserted

into lymphoblast cell line, there was steady expression of the L allele, which produced mRNA

amounts 1.4 times higher than the S allele (Lesch et al., 1996). Cells expressing the L allele bound

30-40% more serotonin than the S allele expressing cells (Lesch et al., 1996).

They concluded that the L allele is more highly transcribed, such that it behaves in a

dominant fashion (Lesch et al., 1996). They confirmed the higher transcriptional activity in the L/L

genotypes in a repeated experiment but found S/L and S/S were equivalent [204], which was

confirmed by the Paaver et al. lab [207] This led them to hypothesize the S allele functioned in a

dominant manner. Bradley et al., however, found that there were differences in transcriptional

rates between the three genotypes, hypothesized that the pattern could occasionally appear

dominant due to the influence of another transcriptional regulatory element, although they did not

name any specific ones [208].

The S allele has been implicated in the activation of the amygdala [209,210], the structure

responsible for processing emotions [125]. The right amygdala shows stronger activation in

individuals carrying the S allele [209]; these individuals had greater positive coupling between the

amygdala and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (which processes risk and fear and inhibition of

emotion) [210]. S-bearing individuals also showed lower grey matter in the anterior cingulate

cortex (emotional processing) and amygdala, which resulted in reduced coupling between the

Page 48: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

36

areas [210]. The amygdala is regulated by the anterior cingulate cortex via a negative feedback

loop [210]. Reduced coupling could result in over-activation of the amygdala [210].

Given the relationship of the HTTLPR VNTR to amygdala, neuroticism has been

investigated for association with this polymorphism. Pezawas et al. found that 30% of the variance

in harm avoidance scores could be predicted by how well the amygdala and anterior cingulate

cortex were connected, notably the effect was more pronounced in males [210]. Harm avoidance,

a facet of neuroticism, is positively associated with the S allele. This association with neuroticism

was also found in other studies [211], with anxiety, angry hostility, depression and impulsiveness

as other prominent facets [206].

Conscientiousness has been associated with the S allele, but only in women with no men

showing the affect [211]. The S association with neuroticism is not consistently found but may be

due to mixed sex populations, as positive associations are often in all male or predominantly male

study populations [206,211].

The second VNTR, STin2, has a 16 or 17bp repeating unit. There are three common

alleles, the 9 repeat (9R), the 10 repeat (10R) and the 12 repeat (12R) [212]. The 9R is rare in

the European population with a frequency of 1-3% [213]. The VNTR is a transcriptional enhancer

and binds the YB-1 transcriptional factor [160]. The YB-1 protein has many functions which

include DNA replication and repair, transcription, pre-mRNA splicing and mRNA translation [214].

Higher expression of SLC6A4 has been found with the 12R when compared to the 10R

allele [204,215]. The 10R allele has been found to have lower expression, and one study has

found genotypes, 10R/10R and 10R/12R to be functionally equivalent, hypothesizing that the 10R

alleles acts in a dominant manner [216]. The 9R STin2 allele is not as well researched due to its

low frequency, and therefore has not been functionally characterized, although it may be

associated with unipolar disorder [217].

The 12R allele is notably associated with schizophrenia; Fan et al. conducted a meta-

analysis on SLC6A4 and found a positive association with the presence of the 12R allele and

schizophrenia [218]. While STin2 itself has not been associated with personality, the combined

effect of both HTTLPR and STin2 has. Kazantseva et al. found that S and 12R carriers were

associated with lower sociability related traits, such as extraversion and novelty seeking [219].

This S12R group was additionally associated with increased harm avoidance (Cloninger’s

Page 49: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

37

neuroticism [41]) [219]. Upon sex stratification, they found that females who were carriers for both

L and 10R showed lower neuroticism [219].

Ali et al. looked at the contribution of both HTTLPR and STin2 VNTRs to the expression

of SLC6A4. They found no difference in the activity of STin2 12R alone, but an 8 fold increase in

expression with the HTTLPR S allele alone, and reported an impressive 17 fold increase when

both 12R and S allele were present [220]. This relationship could help explain inconsistent results

from studies looking at single VNTRs in the SLC6A4 gene.

1.5.5. Tyrosine Hydroxylase (TH)

Figure 1.6. Genomic structure of TH There is a VNTR located in intron 1 and a missense SNP, rs6356, in exon 2.

The tyrosine hydroxylase gene is located on chromosome 11p15.5 [221], has 14 exons

and spans approximately 8.5 kb [222], see figure 1.6. TH produces the tyrosine hydroxylase

protein that catalyzes the rate limiting conversion of L-tyrosine to 3,4-dihydroxy-L-phenylalanine,

or DOPA, which is a precursor in the synthesis of dopamine and norepinephrine [223]. TH activity

is found in the adrenal glands and brain stem [223].

TH expresses 4 different kinds of mRNA, which differ by the inclusion or exclusion of exon

1 and 2 [222]. TH expressed in the brain have exon 2 deleted and are active in the brain stem

[222,223]. The deletion of exon 2 occurs through a hairpin mechanism, where intron 1 and intron

2 have complementary sequences and interact to leave exon 2 at the top of the hairpin and

excluded from transcription [222].

Page 50: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

38

A microsatellite located in intron 1, with a four bp (TCAT) tandem that repeats usually six

to ten times [224], has been researched for potential functional effects. Uniquely, the 10R allele

of this microsatellite has two versions; a perfect (10R) and an imperfect (10iR) repeat [225]. The

10iR allele is missing an adenine in the seventh copy and this variation is more common in the

Caucasian population; the 10R allele is rare [225]. Many studies do not distinguish between the

two alleles, although there seems to be little difference in their overall functional effects [226,227].

The 6R and 7R alleles are thought to be the ancestral alleles, with other longer alleles arising

later [228].

The repeat region’s sequence is very similar to the TRE sequence which recruits

transcriptional factors like the Fos-Jun complex [226], ZNF191 [227,229] and HBP1 [229]. Meloni

et al. found that when the 10R and 10iR alleles were placed upstream from a promoter in vitro,

the expression levels were approximately 9 times greater than when the alleles were absent [226].

Albanese et al. showed that, in vitro, the ZNF191 and HBP1 transcriptional factors exerted

silencing effects on the TH gene, and silencing increased with increasing repeat length [229].

Meloni et al. further refined this relationship to show that ZNF191 can only bind at one site on the

shorter repeats but can bind at two sites in repeats greater than 8R [227].

The extra inhibition could be due to longer repeats causing increased transcription. Wei

et al. found that longer repeats caused increases in serum norepinephrine and serum

homovanillic acid, the end product of dopamine degradation [230]. Specifically, they found the

9R/9R genotype produced the highest norepinephrine serum levels and 10R/10R produced the

highest homovanillic acid serum levels [230]. Zhang et al. also found that longer repeats were

associated with higher levels of norepinephrine [228]. The 10iR allele showed increased basal

and stress-induced heart rates and increased norepinephrine renal excretion when compared to

the 6R allele [228].

Numerous studies have linked alleles of this microsatellite to disorders. The general trend

is that shorter alleles offer protective effects and longer alleles are implicated in disorders. The

8R-10R have been associated with schizophrenia [227,231], suicide attempt [232] and bipolar

disorder [227]. The 7R allele has been associated with protective effects against smoking [233].

The microsatellite has also been associated with aging and personality. De Benedicts et

al. found that male centenarians were less likely to carry a longer allele, although they classified

8R as a short allele [234]. Persson et al. found that the 8R allele was associated with higher

Page 51: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

39

scores on the neuroticism scale, and had significant associations in the angry hostility and

vulnerability facets [235]. The 8R-10R alleles for this VNTR may generate increased

norepinephrine levels in the brain from over active TH production, which may have potential

detrimental effects.

1.6. Thesis Objective

Healthy aging is a complex phenotype with many contributing factors. Here we

concentrate on the genetic contributions to the role of personality in achieving a long and healthy

life. Personality is a predictive pattern of behaviour, attitude and emotional responses that can be

used as a predictor of health. Personality is a heritable trait that is governed by neuro-chemical

and physiological processes that ultimately affects behaviour and decision styles in individuals.

Genes that encode neurotransmitters, their receptors or affect processes in their metabolism are

candidates for study on the effects of personality influencing behaviour and lifestyle. The objective

of this project is to determine if genetic variation in personality-related genes is associated with a

healthy aging phenotype, specifically whether genetic variants in genes involved in

neurotransmission or that underlie personality disorders influence healthy aging.

Page 52: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

40

Chapter 2. Methods

2.1. Study Participants

The joint Clinical Research Ethics Board of the British Columbia Cancer Agency and the

University of British Columbia approved this study and all subjects were enrolled through signed

informed consent.

The Super-Seniors study recruited cases and controls between January 2004 and August

2007 in the Greater Vancouver Lower Mainland, British Columbia (BC). Inclusion criteria for

Super-Senior participants were that they be 85 years of age or older at the time of enrollment and

self-report that they have never been diagnosed with Alzheimer disease, diabetes, cardiovascular

disease, cancer or major pulmonary disease. Subjects were identified through the use of Ministry

of Health lists, Insurance Corporation of British Columbia (if they renewed their driver’s license in

the past three years) and as volunteers after press coverage. Controls are a population-based

group from the same geographic area, ranging from 41 to 54 years of age. They were identified

solely from Ministry of Health lists, and were not selected for health or disease status. The

response rate in both groups was approximately 60%.

DNA was purified from subjects’ blood samples using the PureGene DNA isolation kit,

following the manufacturer's instructions. An interviewer visited each Super-Senior at his or her

home and administered questionnaires regarding overall wellness (health, mental, physical and

occupational). A total of 462 (67.3% female) European-ancestry Super-Seniors and 418 (60.0%

females) European-ancestry controls have been genotyped for this study (880 samples, 63.9%

female). European ancestry was defined as having four grandparents of European ethnicity

though self-reporting. Individuals who were unsure of one or more of their grandparent’s ethnicity

have been excluded from this analysis.

2.2. Literature Search and Gene Selection

A literature search was conducted for gene selection. Categories for the literature search

included, researching a longevity personality profile, genes involved in personality, genes involved

personality disorders, genes involved in major psychiatric disorders, and genes identified in

Page 53: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

41

genome wide association studies (GWAS) of personality traits. Web of Science was used to

search for the following criteria. The ‘very old and very healthy’ personality profile was researched

by searching for search terms “personality”, “aging”, “healthy aging”, “oldest-old” and “longevity”.

Searches were then conducted for genes related to personality traits associated with very old

healthy individuals. Search terms included “association”, “polymorphism”, “conscientiousness”,

“openness”, “emotional stability”, “neuroticism”, “extraversion”, “extroversion” and “gene*”.

Potential candidate genes of interest could also be associated with maladaptive

personality processes; therefore personality disorders were identified through the American

Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 5th edition (DSM-V). Distinguishing

characteristics of personality disorders are “enduring pattern[s] of inner experience and behaviour

that deviates markedly from the expectations of the individual’s character” [36] and are only

classified when the traits are “inflexible, maladaptive, persist[ent], and cause significant functional

impairment or subjective distress” [36]. Search terms used in Web of Science to identify genes

were “paranoid personality disorder”, “schizoid personality disorder”, “schizotypal personality

disorder”, “antisocial disorder”, “borderline personality disorder”, “histrionic personality disorder”.

“narcisisti*”, “avoidant personality disorder”, “dependent personality disorder”, “obsessive

compulsive personality disorder”, “association”, “polymorphism”, and “gene*”.

Major psychiatric illnesses including depression, schizophrenia, obsessive compulsive

disorder, and anxiety disorder were consequently searched, as the DSM-V notes that these can

be difficult to distinguish when making a diagnosis of a personality disorder. Many personality

disorders can be thought of as on a spectrum with major mental illness, such as avoidant

personality disorder and general anxiety [36]. Searches for major mental health illnesses, such

as schizophrenia or depression, produce an enormous amount of results (over 1000), so searches

were limited by the search term “personality”. A Web of Science search was conducted for the

following terms “depression”, ‘schizophrenia”, “obsessive compulsive disorder”, “anxiety

disorder”, “bipolar disorder”, “association”, “polymorphism”, and “gene*”.

GWAS studies were looked at for potential genes of interest. Search terms in Web of

Science used were “genome wide association studies”, “GWAS”, “personality”, and “personality

disorder”. The candidate list was then refined based on biological plausibility and frequency of

the gene in relation to the search criteria. Searches in Web of Science were conducted to expand

association studies and identify SNPs for each candidate gene. Genes with low search yields and

Page 54: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

42

no associations found were dropped from the list, such that the genes with a higher frequency of

published associations were included in the candidates selected.

2.3. SNP Selection

SNP Selection was completed using three methods. First, HapMap’s Tagger program

version 3, release 2, Northern European Utah population (CEU) was used to choose tagSNPs for

autosomal genes. For X-linked genes, version 2, release 21, CEU data was used. Selection

criteria for the tagSNPs were a minor allele frequency of at least 10%, an r2 of 0.8, and a minimum

distance of 300 bp between SNPs.

In addition to tagSNPs, SNPs that were found to be associated with a personality trait in

more than one paper during the literature search were considered. Inclusion criteria consisted of

the SNP being positively associated with a personality trait in more than one paper and having a

minor allele frequency of at least10%. Lastly, SNPs that reached genome wide significance and

had a minor allele frequency of at least 10% in the GWAS studies were also included.

2.4. SNP Genotyping

SNP genotyping was done at the McGill University and Génome Québec Innovation

Centre (Montreal) using the Sequenom MassArray Method. The Sequenom MassArray is being

utilized for its multiplexing ability and minimal assay setup costs. The technology measures the

mass of extended primers to genotype each single nucleotide allele [236]. Plates were prepared

in accordance to the Innovation Centre’s protocol of 30uL of 40 ng/uL of template submitted for

880 samples. Ten plates, each containing 96 samples, control samples, or blanks were shipped

on dry ice to the Innovation Centre. The SNPs were randomly split into two sets as the submission

of 44 SNPs exceeds the number of markers that can be genotyped in one set (34). Genotyping

results were downloaded through the Centre’s online system.

2.5. SNP Analysis

SNP filtering and analysis was conducted in Golden Helix SVS suite 8 (SVS8), with

planned sex stratification. The following filtering criteria were applied to the genotyped SNPs.

Page 55: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

43

First, SNPs that failed genotyping were removed from the data set. Second, samples were filtered

for those with low genotype call rates (below 90%), outliers in the autosomal heterozygosity rate,

and presumed duplicate samples with identical genotypes. Lastly, SNPs were filtered for low call

rates (below 95%), low minor allele frequency (below 1%), and Hardy Weinberg Equilibrium

(HWE) failure in the control group.

Uncorrected Pearson Chi-squares were conducted in SVS8 for autosomal candidate

genes and for the female subgroup for X-linked makers. Male subjects were considered

hemizygous at X-linked genes, contributing only one allele to non-autosomal allele counts.

Hemizygote data cannot be analyzed in SVS8, therefore X-linked markers for the total study

sample and the male subgroup were analyzed in R Studio v 0.98.1103 with R v 3.2.2. Table 2.1

summarizes which programs were used for analysis. The false rate of discovery procedure (FDR),

Benjamini-Hochberg [237], was used for the multiple testing corrections in R. The p-value cutoff

was set to 0.05.

Table 2.1. Summary of program choice for genes and markers

Marker Group Autosomal genes Non-Autosomal genes

SNP Total Sample SVS8 R

Female SVS8 SVS8

Male SVS8 R

VNTR Total Sample R R

Female R R

Male R R

2.6. VNTR Genotyping

Polymerase Chain Reaction (PCR) amplification was conducted at the Michael Smith

Genome Sciences Centre (MSGSC). PCR Primer pairs flanking VNTRs of interest were chosen

from the literature and are summarized in Table 2.2. Primers were optimized initially without dyes

(for reasons related to cost), and Mendelian segregation checked using reference families from

Centre d’Etude du Polymorphisme Humain (CEPH). PCR products were run on a 2.5% agarose

gel for separation of the fragment sizes and visualization. Primer pairs showing the expected

sized PCR products were ordered with dye attached and checked again. Four different fluorescent

Page 56: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

44

dyes are used to differentiate fragments produced using different primer pairs; this allowed

pooling of all PCR products from all VNTRs of each subject for fragment size analysis. Table 2.2

lists the optimized PCR conditions and dyes used. Each of the 5 VNTRs was PCR amplified

separately in the 880 samples, in ten 96-well plates (50 plates total). PCR products were checked

on an agarose gel to confirm successful amplification. Plates were then pooled by sample, and

the 10 plates of pooled samples were transported to the Centre for Molecular Medicine and

Therapeutics (CMMT), Vancouver for fragment size analysis.

Table 2.2. Primers and PCR conditions used for amplification of VNTRs

Gene Dye PCR Protocol Forward Reverse Reference

MAOA NED 94°C 2 m denaturation, followed by 30 cycles of 94°C 30s, 58°C 5s, 68°C 1 m and 68°C for 5m extension

5’- ACA GCC TGA CCG TGG AGA AG -3’

5’- GAA CGG ACG CTC CAT TCG GA -3’

[238]

DRD4 6-FAM

94°C 2 m denaturation, followed by 35 cycles of 94°C 30s, 62°C 5s, 68°C 1 m and 68°C for 5m extension

5′-GCG ACT ACG TGG TCT ACT CG-3′

5′-AGG ACC CTC ATG GCC TTG-3′ [239]

HTTLPR NED 94°C 2 m denaturation, followed by 30 cycles of 94°C 30s, 60°C 5s, 68°C 1 m and 68°C for 5m extension

5’-GGC GTT GCC GCT CTG AAT GC-3’

5’-GAG GGA CTG AGC TGG ACA ACC AC-3’

[240]

SIN2 HEX 94°C 2 m denaturation, followed by 35 cycles of 94°C 30s, 52°C 15s, 68°C 1 m and 68°C for 5m extension

5’-GGT CAG TAT CAC AGG CTG CGA GTA G-3’

5’-TGT TCC TAG TCT TAC GCC AGT GAA-3’

[241]

TH 6-FAM

94°C 2 m denaturation, followed by 35 cycles of 94°C 30s, 61°C1 5s, 68°C 1 m and 68°C for 5m extension

5’-CAG CTG CCC TAG TCA GCA C-3’

5’-GCT TCC GAG TGC AGG TCA CA-3’

[224]

An Applied Biosystems 3130 Genetic Analyzer was used to detect peak sizes of the PCR

products via fluorescence-based capillary electrophoresis. One µL of pooled PCR product was

loaded into the analyzer, which automates the processes of polymer loading, sample injection,

separation, detection and fragment size analysis.

Results from the ten plates were loaded into GeneMapper V5 and alleles were called

based on the following criteria: peak heights greater or equal to 150 for the NED and HEX dyes

and 50 for the 6-FAM dyes, peaks twice as high as the average baseline noise, and peaks in the

Page 57: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

45

expected size ranges (as specified by the user). GeneMapper’s standard settings for peak quality

were used.

2.7. VNTR Analysis

No samples were excluded based on call rates, as VNTR data was genotyped until

completion. Additional sample quality checks were not performed on VNTR data, as there were

too few markers to test for identical genotypes or to test for excess heterozygosity. Markers were

checked for low call rates and HWE. HWE was checked using the full Bayesian significance test

(FBST), which is adapted for HWE by Lauretto et al for multiallelic data [242]. HWE was tested

only in controls and for X-linked genes only in female controls. Before HWE testing in the X-linked

genes, allele frequencies were compared between males and females with a Fisher’s Chi-square

test.

Rare alleles were of interest and were not excluded. Instead, rare alleles with frequency

less than 10% were grouped into a “rare alleles” category, for analysis of each VNTR.

Allele frequency data was used for Chi Squared analyses. Males were assigned a status

of hemizygous for their X-linked VNTR allele. Analysis was run in R Studio v 0.98.1103 with R v

3.2.2. As the data is multiallelic, Galta et al’s score test was used [243]. The score test functions

much like a Pearson’s chi-square, however, it gives more weight to common makers. The score

test was used as it has greater power in moderate sample sizes to detect true associations when

the effect of the association does not strongly affect the phenotype. The p-value cutoff was set to

0.05.

Page 58: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

46

Chapter 3. Results

3.1. Literature Search and Gene Selection Results

The literature search was conducted in five parts: 1) personality profile of the very old and

very healthy, 2) genes involved in personality, 3) genes involved in personality disorders as

defined by the DSM-V, 4) genes involved in major psychiatric disorders, 5) genes identified as

associated with personality in GWAS studies.

1) Search results revealed specific personality traits for the very old [79,81,84,244–247]

and very healthy [248], where longevity is associated with individuals high in conscientiousness,

extraversion and openness, and low in neuroticism. 2) Searches for genes involved with

personality resulted in a total of 42 candidate genes. 3) Search criteria for genes involved in

personality disorders found eight additional genes. 4) No additional genes were found by looking

at major psychiatric disorders with the previously described criteria. 5) No genes were identified

in the GWAS literature searches; however, three SNPs associated with personality were located

in close proximity to known genes, RAS1A and KATNAL2.

The candidate list was refined to 32 genes based on biological plausibility and frequency

of the gene in prior searches (see Table 3.1). Twenty one candidate genes with low literature

yields were dropped and six more were dropped for having largely negative association results.

This resulted in five genes, DRD4, TH, COMT, MAOA, and SLC6A4.

Page 59: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

47

Table 3.1. Summary of potential candidate genes found through literature search, including polymorphisms and results of association studies

Number of Papers that show (Yes) or do not show (No) association

Gene Polymorphism Yes No Reference

1 5HT2A rs6313 3 1 [249–252]

rs6311 2 1 [241,253,254]

rs4941573 1 0 [249]

2 ADRA2 rs1800544 3 2 [255–259]

rs1800545 1 1 [256,259]

rs7682295 1 0 [260]

rs521674 1 0 [261]

rs602618 1 0 [261]

rs583668 1 0 [256]

rs553668 1 0 [256,259]

3 BDNF rs6265 8 2 [262–271]

rs11030102 1 0 [272]

4 COMT rs4680 13 15 [146,241,255,262,273–296]

rs4818 1 2 [274,284,297]

rs4633 1 1 [283,284]

rs737866 1 1 [276,298]

rs737865 1 0 [283]

rs9332377 1 0 [283]

rs6269 1 0 [284]

rs165599 0 4 [262,276,283,287]

rs5993883 0 2 [276,283]

rs4646312 0 2 [276,299]

5 DAT/

SLC6A3

VNTR (3 prime end) 12 4 [146,239,241,250,255,277,300–309]

rs27072 2 1 [304,305,310]

rs6347 1 3 [255,299,310,311]

rs403636 0 2 [301,310]

VNTR (intron 8) 0 1 [304]

6 DRD2 rs1800497 16 7 [239,273,296,305,308,312–328]

rs6277 4 2 [280,296,300,314,320,329]

rs1799732 4 2 [280,314,320,330–332]

Page 60: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

48

Number of Papers that show (Yes) or do not show (No) association

Gene Polymorphism Yes No Reference

rs1800479 1 1 [277,333]

rs1079597 1 0 [334]

rs6276 1 0 [320]

rs1076560 1 0 [335]

7 DRD4 rs1800955 9 7 [179,278,279,296,332,334,336–345]

VNTR 8 8 [179,239,275,277,301,302,328,337,341,342,345–350]

rs936461 2 0 [328,336]

rs747302 1 6 [179,334,336–338,343,345]

rs3758653 1 4 [276,299,311,336,351]

rs916457 1 1 [336,351]

rs7124601 1 0 [351]

rs916455 0 3 [299,336,340]

rs752306 0 2 [351,352]

8 MAOA VNTR 13 8 [146,195,238,241,275,277,289,318,323,353–364]

rs6323 1 2 [354,365,366]

rs979606 1 0 [367]

9 NET1 rs998424 2 3 [255,274,368–370]

rs2242447 2 2 [274,297,368,369]

rs3785157 2 1 [255,276,369]

rs3785143 1 5 [261,276,368,369,371,372]

rs36009 1 2 [260,276,368]

rs11568324 1 1 [368,372]

rs5558 1 1 [369,373]

rs36020 1 0 [261]

rs36029 1 0 [261]

rs28386840 1 0 [368]

10 SERT/

SLC6A4

5-HTTLPR VNTR 26 18 [146,240,252,267–269,282,289,304,321,325,374–406]

STin2 VNTR 7 10 [240,241,277,304,370,378,381,382,387,389,394,395,397,399,400,407,408]

rs25531 2 2 [240,280,313,385]

rs140700 1 5 [276,391,395,409–411]

Page 61: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

49

Number of Papers that show (Yes) or do not show (No) association

Gene Polymorphism Yes No Reference

rs6354 1 4 [240,276,391,395,409]

rs2020942 1 3 [240,382,395,410]

rs140701 1 3 [240,382,395,397]

rs1042173 1 3 [240,276,382,410]

rs2020936 1 2 [276,391,397]

rs4583306 1 2 [276,382,410]

rs3794808 1 3 [240,276,382,397]

rs16965628 1 1 [240,410]

rs6355 1 1 [240,391]

rs4325622 1 1 [240,382]

rs25532 1 0 [240]

11 TH TCAT VNTR 3 3 [235,348,412–415]

rs6356 2 3 [387,416–419]

rs10770141 1 0 [420]

12 5HT1A rs6295 1 2 [275,421,422]

rs1800044 1 0 [373]

13 5HT1B rs6296 2 3 [383,422–425]

14 5HTR2C rs6318 2 0 [407,426]

rs3813928 1 0 [407]

rs3813929 1 0 [407]

rs518147 1 0 [407]

15 ALDH2 rs671 4 0 [317,356,358,427]

16 ANK3 rs10994336 1 0 [428]

17 CACNAC1 rs1006737 1 0 [428]

18 CRHR1 rs110402 1 0 [429]

19 DARPP-32 rs907094 1 0 [430]

20 DRD3 Ba1I 4 1 [404,431–434]

rs6280 2 1 [264,292,435]

21 GABRA2 rs279871 1 0 [436]

rs79867 1 0 [436]

22 GABRA6 rs3219151 1 0 [267]

23 NOS1 VNTR 1 0 [437]

rs7298903 1 0 [438]

Page 62: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

50

Number of Papers that show (Yes) or do not show (No) association

Gene Polymorphism Yes No Reference

24 NRG1 rs3924999 1 0 [439]

rs10503929 0 1 [439]

25 NTRK2 NR 84515449 1 0 [440]

rs993315 1 0 [440]

rs10780691 1 0 [440]

rs7170215 1 0 [272]

rs11073742 1 0 [272]

26 OPRK1 36 G>T 1 0 [441]

27 OPRM1 Asn40Asp 0 1 [442]

28 p250GAP rs2298599 1 0 [443]

29 SNAP-25 rs1051312 1 0 [444]

rs3746544 1 0 [444]

30 TPH1 rs1800532 0 2 [381,445]

31 TPH2 rs4570625 1 1 [426,446]

rs10784941 1 0 [426]

rs2171363 1 0 [426]

32 ZNF804A rs1344706 2 0 [447,448]

rs7597593 1 0 [448]

3.2. SNP Selection Results

Table 2.2 shows a list of the tagSNPs selected. From HapMap’s Tagger program, 29 SNPs

were chosen to represent the five final candidate genes. From the literature, two more SNPs were

added and two others were already included among the tagSNPs. Three SNPs were found in the

GWAS searches and were included in the list. In total, 34 SNPs were chosen.

Page 63: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

51

Table 3.2. SNPs Selected to Represent the Five Final Candidate Genes

Gene (CHR) SNP MAF (%) Method of Selection SNP Changes and Genotyping Outcome

RAS1A (5) rs1477268 22 GWAS

rs2032794 16 GWAS

KATNAL2 (18) rs2576037 43 GWAS

COMT (22) rs2020917 23 Tagger

rs740601 38 Tagger

rs5748489 33 Tagger

rs933271 39 Tagger

rs4680 39 Tagger/Literature search

rs9332377 16 Tagger

rs5993883 48 Tagger

rs4646316 25 Tagger

rs174696 44 Tagger

rs165815 34 Tagger

DRD4 (11) rs11246226 48 Tagger

rs11246228 40 Tagger

rs3758653 24 Tagger

rs1800955 37 Literature search Failed validation

MAOA (X) rs3027456 26 Tagger

rs3027450 11 Tagger

rs5905512 49 Tagger Failed call rate

rs1799836 43 Tagger

SERT (17) rs6354 21 Tagger

rs9303628 36 Tagger

SERT (17) rs140701 49 Tagger

rs7214248 29 Tagger

rs6505165 48 Tagger

rs4251417 11 Tagger

rs25531 11 Literature search Failed design

TH (11) rs7483056 48 Tagger

rs10840491 15 Tagger

rs6356 42 Tagger/Literature search

Page 64: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

52

Gene (CHR) SNP MAF (%) Method of Selection SNP Changes and Genotyping Outcome

rs10840489 15 Tagger Failed design, replaced with rs1544325

rs10743152 30 Tagger

rs11042978 49 Tagger

3.3. SNP Genotyping Results

At the Innovation Centre, the 34 SNPs were tested in silico for their ability to support SNP

assay development. They were done in two batches, starting with an initial set of 30 SNPs. In

this set, SLC6A4 rs25531 and COMT rs5748489 failed design and were removed from the list.

COMT rs5748489 was replaced with a SNP in high linkage disequilibrium (LD) with it, rs1544325.

No SNP in high LD with SLC6A4 rs25531 was available. This set of 29 SNPs was then physically

validated, through testing of the SNP panel on one plate of study samples. Two markers were

identified as problematic; DRD4 rs1800955 failed testing, and COMT rs4680 was judged as ‘might

fail in production’. Since no SNPs were in linkage disequilibrium with these, they were left

unchanged in the hope that the assays would perform better than predicted. A higher

concentration of oligonucletides was added into the pool to assist reaction with rs4680. After

genotyping of these SNPs in study samples, four SNPs did not have a call rate of at least 95%

(TH rs11042978, 94%; KATNAL2 rs2576037, 93%; COMT rs933271, 94%; COMT rs4680, 91%)

and DRD4 rs1800955 failed to genotype as predicted. The four SNPs were submitted again in

the second set. No SNPs in the second set failed design, validation or production, or failed to

meet the call rate cutoff.

3.4. SNP Quality Control Results

The two datasets received from the Innovation Centre were merged into one list of 32

successfully genotyped SNPs, then filtered for quality. Figure 3.1 displays a flowchart of data

quality checks. Blank and control samples were checked for no genotype and identical

genotypes, respectively, before removal from the dataset. Twenty-two samples that did not meet

a genotype call rate of greater than 90% were excluded (12 Super-Senior and 10 control

samples). No samples were removed for excess heterozygosity or for having unexpected identical

Page 65: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

53

genotypes. One SNP, MAOA rs5905512, did not meet a minimum SNP call rate of 95% (94%)

and was excluded. A total of 858 samples (62.6% female) and 31 SNPs remained after data

quality control.

Figure 3.1. Quality control for SNPs and VNTRs

Page 66: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

54

* No samples were dropped from VNTR for low call rates as any failed samples were repeated.

3.5. VNTR Genotyping Results

Upon PCR amplification, the fragment sizes that define VNTR alleles showed Mendelian

segregation in CEPH family samples, as exemplified in figure 3.2. Mendelian segregation was

tested and confirmed in one family for each VNTR; in each case allele segregation was

Mendelian.

Figure 3.2. CEPH Family #1341 showing Mendelian Segregation of the DRD4 VNTR

GeneMapper software converts fragment sizes for each VNTR allele in each sample to

genotypes based on size ranges (bins) input by the user. An example of the allele bins for DRD4

is shown in figure 3.3. GeneMapper was run iteratively three times, the first time with initial bin

estimates, the second involved refinement of the bins, manual selection of the alleles and

identifying missing data, and the third included repeated samples and resulted in a complete

VNTR genotypes dataset.

Page 67: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

55

Figure 3.3. DRD4 VNTR allele bins This individual is homozygous for the 4R allele. Bins are represented as grey columns; their widths reflect expected peak size ranges. Approximate peak sizes and bin names are shown. Peaks that fall in these size ranges, and meet minimum peak height and noise to signal ratios are called as VNTR alleles.

The first GeneMapper run showed one sample failing to size and two samples were

missing. There were 1999 results requiring visual check or for which the allele required manual

calling. The first run only included two bins for the two most frequent alleles of each VNTR;

additional allele bins were calculated and manually added into the program. The alleles of each

VNTR marker were named according to the number of repeats of each VNTR, in a manner

consistent with the literature.

GeneMapper was run a second time, after which 551 alleles were uncalled and 882 alleles

required a visual check. The majority of these non-calls and checks were due to peaks that did

Page 68: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

56

not meet the calling criteria. Examples of reasons for failed calling and manual selection of peaks

are described below.

Most failed calls resulted from peaks failing the peak to noise ratio cutoffs, peaks too small

to detect, or too many apparent peaks due to spectral pull-up. Spectral pull-up occurs when the

signal from one dye is very strong and causes the other dyes in the same lane to show additional,

false, peaks the same size as the peaks of the first dye (Applied Biosystems 2011). The majority

of alleles needing a visual check were due to samples with uneven concentrations of the

fragments in the pooled sample. If one or two of the fragments in the pooled sample was at a

higher concentration than the rest, its peak was much stronger compared to the other fragment

peaks. GeneMapper flags such samples for a visual confirmation, as the smaller peaks meet the

minimum height requirement but fail signal to noise ratio. Alternatively these strong signals

occasionally caused spectral pull-up. Spectral pull-up was easily identified, however, as it

coincided with expected intervals of the size marker dye, or with other very strong fragments. See

figure 3.4, A and C for examples of common GeneMapper genotype calling issues.

When having to manually select a peak, the following set of rules was implemented: 1) for

a genotype to be considered bi-allelic, both peaks needed to be of the same relative strength. In

figure 3.4 A there are two peaks of equal strength at 7R and 9R so there are two alleles. In figure

3.4 B the apparent allele at 9R is much smaller than the one called at 12R; it is not a real allele

as the peaks are not of similar strengths. 2) Clusters of peaks, caused by excessive noise or

spectral pull-up, would occasionally be greater than the set minimum peak height and cause an

allele to be falsely called at that site. These were flagged for visual inspection, after which the

largest peaks were left and smaller ones deleted (figure 3.4 B). 3) Peaks caused by spectral pull-

up were also flagged for inspection and deleted (figure 3.4 C). 4) Weak peaks not meeting the

minimum height were called only if there were one or two peaks at least twice the height of the

noise (figure 3.4 D). 5) If there were more than two peaks at the same height and no visible

spectral pull-up, all peaks were deleted and the sample was considered failed.

Page 69: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

57

Figure 3.4. Examples of common calling problems in GeneMapper v5 The x axis indicates peak size in bp; the y axis indicates the strength of the peak signal. A) Strong signal requiring a visual check due to high concentration of sample. Here TH peaks at 7R and 9R are very strong causing spectral pull-up. B) Deletion of a false peak based on strength of signal. The double 9R peak for SLC64A’s STin2 VNTR is much smaller that the peak at the 12R position. Because true peaks are of a comparable size, the apparent 9R peak was not called as an allele. C) A false peak due to spectral pull-up at a nearby marker site. MAOA shows a real peak at 4R but the strong red signal to the left is caused by marker dye pull up at the nearby 300bp site. D) A weak peak that did not meet the minimum height. This DRD4 peak almost meets the criteria at the 4R site and requires a visual check, the height of the peak is double that of the noise; manual calling of the peak was needed.

After manual selection, 171 fragments could not be called and were identified as missing

data. There were 6 samples that failed in all VNTRs (166, 469, 470, 473, 481, C541). Inter-plate

variability for allele calls was calculated for quality control, using the mean of the allele sizes and

identifying any alleles outside 1 standard deviation. Allele averages were checked to ensure the

alleles had the size differences observed in the literature.

Page 70: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

58

Of note, samples on plates 5 and 6 displayed consistently 1-3 base pairs larger allele sizes

when compared to allele sizes determined from other plates, frequently showing alleles greater

than 1 standard deviation from the average. Samples showing these increased sizes were

checked visually again and showed a consistent pattern across all VNTR alleles. Figure 3.5 best

illustrates this for HTTLPR, showing some fragments to the right side of the bins. Other plates did

not show alleles deviating more than 1. Plates 5 and 6 were the first two plates to be sent to

CMMT, and it is likely that this shift is caused by a calibration problem. Upon visual check of the

gels, the fragment sizes for these plates were within the expected ranges; therefore the alleles

called in GeneMapper are correct. Alleles for plates 5 and 6 were left as they were originally

called.

A total of 239 failed samples were re-plated and run again on the PCR. These samples

were sent, unpooled, to CMMT for fragment analysis. Data from these samples were added to

the dataset in GeneMapper upon completion for the final allele calling run. All samples had two

alleles called for each of the five VNTRs, with the exception of two DRD4 samples, consistent

with the high heterozygosity generally seen in VNTRs. The final bin sets used in GeneMapper are

displayed in figure 3.5; average allele sizes with standard deviation given in table 3.3.

Page 71: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

59

Figure 3.5. Bin sets used in GeneMapper v5 to call each VNTR

Bins are shown as grey columns, with fragment sizes (bp) along the x axis and peak strength along the y axis. See table 3.3 for average peak size.

Page 72: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

60

Table 3.3. Average VNTR allele sizes and standard deviations

VNTR DRD4 HTTLPR MAOA STin2 TH

Allele Avg bp

Size (SD)

Allele Avg bp

Size (SD)

Allele Avg bp

Size (SD)

Allele Avg bp

Size (SD)

Allele Avg bp

Size (SD)

2R 369.77 (0.46)

S 483.09 (0.73)

2R 287.47 (0.18)

9R 250.98 (0.16)

5R 237.33 (0.12)

3R 416.67 (0.64)

L 525.77 (0.90)

3R 316.95 (0.27)

10R 268.33 (0.18)

6R 241.31 (0.15)

4R 464.58 (0.64)

17R 546.52 (-)

3.5R 334.83 (0.39)

12R 301.72 (0.15)

7R 245.31 (0.13)

5R 512.66 (0.52)

4R 346.70 (0.29)

13R 319.29 (-)

8R 249.33 (0.13)

6R 560.24 (0.39)

5R 376.48 (0.13)

9R 253.36 (0.14)

7R 607.96 (0.46)

10R 256.38 (0.24)

8R 655.80 (0.44)

9R 703.45 (-)

10R 749.31 (-)

11R 796.58 (-)

3.6. VNTR Quality Control Results

Results of quality control checks can be seen in figure 3.1. All 880 samples (63.9% female)

were genotyped with the exception of two DRD4 VNTR samples. Male and female frequencies

did not differ in MAOA (p-value 0.91), and no deviation from HWE was detected in the female

control group (p-values: DRD4 1.00; HTTLPR 0.80; MAOA 0.59; STin2 0.99; TH 0.62)

Rare alleles were identified in the data but at very low counts. Analyzing low-count alleles

would not be meaningful, so low count alleles were grouped together until the ‘rare alleles’

category was sufficiently large enough to support analysis. Grouping and analysis was performed

Page 73: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

61

through a statistical code in R, created by Andy Leung. Groups required a minimum of five

samples, starting with the smallest counts, adding additional allele groups until a count of at least

five was achieved for both Super-Senior group and the controls.

3.7. Association Results

SNP data included 858 samples, of 450 Super-Seniors and 408 controls. Stratification

produced a female group with 305 Super-Senior and 246 controls, and a male group with 145

Super-Seniors and 162 controls. The VNTR data maintained the original sample size of 880, with

462 Super-Seniors and 418 controls. Once stratified, there were 311 Super-Senior females, 251

female controls, 151 male Super-Seniors and 167 male controls.

Table 3.4 shows the results of the uncorrected association tests. The following rare allele

groups were created for the VNTR data. The DRD4 VNTR had low counts for the 6R, 8R, 9R,

10R and 11R alleles, they were combined into one rare allele group for the combined and female

analysis. The males had fewer counts and did not show alleles for 10R or 11R. The rare allele

DRD4 VNTR group for the male-only analysis contained 3R, 5R, 6R and 8R.

MAOA’s rare allele group contained 2R and 5R for the combined and female groups, and

3R, 3.5R, 5R with no 2Rs available for the males. SLC6A4’s HTTLPR did not require a rare allele

group but STin2 did. Rare alleles for Stin2 for combined and females included 9R and 13R and

the males did not require one although they had zero counts for 13R. Lastly TH’s VNTR’s 5R and

8R alleles were pooled into the rare allele group for all three analyses.

Prior to multiple testing corrections there are three SNPs of interest, showing p-values

around the 0.05 threshold. COMT rs174696 in the combined sample shows a weak association

with an odds ratio of 1.30, CI 1.03-1.65. Upon stratification by sex, males show two very weak

associations; COMT rs933271 (OR 1.46, CI 0.98-2.19) and to the DRD4 VNTR rare allele group

(OR 1.96, CI 1.00-3.98); although these do not meet significance cut offs.

Once the FDR correction is applied these associations are not statistically significant (table

3.5). No associations were found between variants in these genes between the Super-Seniors

and the control group.

Page 74: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

62

Table 3.4. Results from Pearson Chi-Square tests of SNP and Score Tests of VNTRs, for the Combined Sample and when Stratified by Sex

Combined Female Male

Gene Marker Allele Super-Senior Control Allele Super-Senior Control Allele Super-Senior Control

COMT rs1544325 T 406 (45.1) 367 (45) T 271 (44.4) 225 (45.7) T 135 (46.6) 142 (43.8)

C 494 (54.9) 449 (55) C 339 (55.6) 267 (54.3) C 155 (53.4) 182 (56.2)

p-value 0.96 0.66 0.50

COMT rs165815 G 130 (14.4) 112 (13.7) G 85 (13.9) 67 (13.6) G 45 (15.5) 45 (13.9)

A 770 (85.6) 704 (86.3) A 525 (86.1) 425 (86.4) A 245 (84.5) 279 (86.1)

p-value 0.67 0.88 0.57

COMT rs174696 C 211 (23.5) 156 (19.1) C 140 (23.1) 93 (18.9) C 71 (24.5) 63 (19.4)

T 685 (76.5) 660 (80.9) T 466 (76.9) 399 (81.1) T 219 (75.5) 261 (80.6)

p-value 0.03* 0.09 0.13

COMT rs2020917 T 243 (27) 240 (29.4) T 156 (25.6) 141 (28.7) T 87 (30) 99 (30.6)

C 657 (73) 576 (70.6) C 454 (74.4) 351 (71.3) C 203 (70) 225 (69.4)

p-value 0.27 0.25 0.88

COMT rs4646316 T 225 (25) 210 (25.7) T 160 (26.2) 130 (26.4) T 65 (22.4) 80 (24.7)

C 675 (75) 606 (74.3) C 450 (73.8) 362 (73.6) C 225 (77.6) 244 (75.3)

p-value 0.73 0.94 0.51

COMT rs4680 C 442 (49.2) 399 (48.9) T 301 (49.5) 248 (50.4) C 135 (46.6) 155 (47.8)

T 456 (50.8) 417 (51.1) C 307 (50.5) 244 (49.6) T 155 (53.4) 169 (52.2)

p-value 0.89 0.77 0.75

COMT rs5993883 T 436 (48.7) 396 (48.9) T 291 (48) 241 (49.4) T 145 (50) 155 (48.1)

G 460 (51.3) 414 (51.1) G 315 (52) 247 (50.6) G 145 (50) 167 (51.9)

p-value 0.92 0.65 0.65

COMT rs740601 C 360 (40) 343 (42) C 250 (41) 214 (43.5) C 110 (37.9) 129 (39.8)

A 540 (60) 473 (58) A 360 (59) 278 (56.5) A 180 (62.1) 195 (60.2)

Page 75: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

63

Combined Female Male

Gene Marker Allele Super-Senior Control Allele Super-Senior Control Allele Super-Senior Control

COMT rs740601 p-value 0.39 0.40 0.63

COMT rs9332377 A 129 (14.4) 139 (17) A 88 (14.5) 83 (16.9) A 41 (14.2) 56 (17.3)

G 765 (85.6) 677 (83) G 518 (85.5) 409 (83.1) G 247 (85.8) 268 (82.7)

p-value 0.14 0.29 0.30

COMT rs933271 C 210 (25) 194 (24.3) C 162 (27.9) 115 (23.9) C 48 (18.5) 79 (24.8)

T 630 (75) 606 (75.8) T 418 (72.1) 367 (76.1) T 212 (81.5) 239 (75.2)

p-value 0.72 0.13 0.07**

DRD4 rs11246226 C 410 (45.9) 382 (47.4) C 288 (47.7) 235 (48.4) C 122 (42.1) 147 (45.9)

A 484 (54.1) 424 (52.6) A 316 (52.3) 251 (51.6) A 168 (57.9) 173 (54.1)

p-value 0.53 0.83 0.34

DRD4 rs11246228 C 412 (45.8) 365 (44.7) C 270 (44.3) 212 (43.1) C 142 (49) 153 (47.2)

T 488 (54.2) 451 (55.3) T 340 (55.7) 280 (56.9) T 148 (51) 171 (52.8)

p-value 0.66 0.70 0.67

DRD4 rs3758653 G 168 (18.9) 146 (18.1) G 110 (18.3) 88 (18) G 58 (20.1) 58 (18.1)

A 722 (81.1) 662 (81.9) A 492 (81.7) 400 (82) A 230 (79.9) 262 (81.9)

p-value 0.67 0.92 0.53

DRD4 VNTR rare 11 (1.2) 12 (1.4) rare 10 (1.6) 8 (1.6) rare 24 (7.9) 14 (4.2)

2R 78 (8.5) 58 (7) 2R 54 (8.7) 31 (6.2) 2R 24 (7.9) 27 (8.1)

3R 45 (4.9) 33 (4) 3R 24 (3.9) 24 (4.8) 4R 206 (68.2) 222 (66.5)

4R 618 (67) 560 (67.1) 4R 412 (66.5) 338 (67.6) 7R 48 (15.9) 71 (21.3)

5R 9 (1) 10 (1.2) 5R 7 (1.1) 9 (1.8)

7R 161 (17.5) 161 (19.3) 7R 113 (18.2) 90 (18)

p-value 0.63 0.52 0.06**

KATNAL2 rs2576037 T 377 (42.9) 351 (43.1) T 248 (41.9) 216 (44.1) T 129 (45.1) 135 (41.7)

C 501 (57.1) 463 (56.9) C 344 (58.1) 274 (55.9) C 157 (54.9) 189 (58.3)

Page 76: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

64

Combined Female Male

Gene Marker Allele Super-Senior Control Allele Super-Senior Control Allele Super-Senior Control

KATNAL2 rs2576037 p-value 0.94 0.47 0.39

MAOA rs3027456 C 219 (28.9) 199 (30.9) C 180 (29.8) 150 (31) C 37 (25) 48 (30.4)

T 539 (71.1) 445 (69.1) T 424 (70.2) 334 (69) T 111 (75) 110 (69.6)

p-value 0.41 0.67 0.29

MAOA rs3027450 C 186 (24.1) 174 (26) C 149 (24.4) 124 (25.2) C 34 (22.5) 46 (27.4)

T 585 (75.9) 496 (74) T 461 (75.6) 368 (74.8) T 117 (77.5) 122 (72.6)

p-value 0.42 0.77 0.32

MAOA rs1799836 C 359 (46.7) 315 (47.2) C 289 (47.5) 234 (47.8) C 64 (42.7) 74 (44.3)

T 409 (53.3) 352 (52.8) T 319 (52.5) 256 (52.2) T 86 (57.3) 93 (55.7)

p-value 0.86 0.94 0.77

MAOA VNTR rare 10 (1.3) 14 (2.1) rare 9 (1.4) 12 (2.4) rare 61 (40.4) 63 (37.7)

3R 288 (37.3) 223 (33.3) 3R 231 (37.1) 164 (32.7) 4R 90 (59.6) 104 (62.3)

3.5R 14 (1.8) 13 (1.9) 3.5R 11 (1.8) 11 (2.2)

4R 461 (59.6) 419 (62.6) 4R 371 (59.6) 315 (62.7)

p-value 0.31 0.31 0.67

RAS1A rs2032794 C 200 (22.3) 177 (21.8) C 131 (21.6) 101 (20.6) C 69 (23.8) 76 (23.6)

T 696 (77.7) 635 (78.2) T 475 (78.4) 389 (79.4) T 221 (76.2) 246 (76.4)

p-value 0.79 0.69 0.96

RAS1A rs1477268 C 202 (22.5) 180 (22.1) C 131 (21.6) 103 (20.9) C 71 (24.5) 77 (23.8)

T 694 (77.5) 636 (77.9) T 475 (78.4) 389 (79.1) T 219 (75.5) 247 (76.2)

p-value 0.81 0.78 0.84

SLC6A4 HTTLPR L 564 (61.1) 512 (61.2) L 389 (62.5) 309 (61.6) L 175 (58.1) 203 (60.8)

S 359 (38.9) 324 (38.8) S 233 (37.5) 193 (38.4) S 126 (41.9) 131 (39.2)

p-value 0.96 0.76 0.53

SLC6A4 rs140701 T 348 (38.7) 304 (37.3) T 227 (37.2) 186 (37.8) T 121 (41.7) 118 (36.4)

Page 77: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

65

Combined Female Male

Gene Marker Allele Super-Senior Control Allele Super-Senior Control Allele Super-Senior Control

SLC6A4 rs140701 C 552 (61.3) 512 (62.7) C 383 (62.8) 306 (62.2) C 169 (58.3) 206 (63.6)

p-value 0.55 0.84 0.18

SLC6A4 rs4251417 A 93 (10.3) 87 (10.7) A 71 (11.6) 52 (10.6) A 22 (7.6) 35 (10.8)

G 807 (89.7) 729 (89.3) G 539 (88.4) 440 (89.4) G 268 (92.4) 289 (89.2)

p-value 0.82 0.57 0.17

SLC6A4 rs6354 C 190 (21.1) 176 (21.6) C 131 (21.5) 105 (21.3) C 59 (20.3) 71 (21.9)

A 710 (78.9) 640 (78.4) A 479 (78.5) 387 (78.7) A 231 (79.7) 253 (78.1)

p-value 0.82 0.96 0.63

SLC6A4 rs6505165 G 422 (47) 392 (48.2) G 278 (45.7) 241 (49) G 144 (49.7) 151 (46.9)

A 476 (53) 422 (51.8) A 330 (54.3) 251 (51) A 146 (50.3) 171 (53.1)

p-value 0.63 0.28 0.49

SLC6A4 rs7214248 A 325 (36.2) 309 (38) A 217 (35.7) 195 (39.6) A 108 (37.2) 114 (35.4)

G 573 (63.8) 505 (62) G 391 (64.3) 297 (60.4) G 182 (62.8) 208 (64.6)

p-value 0.45 0.18 0.64

SLC6A4 rs9303628 G 399 (44.3) 361 (44.2) G 263 (43.1) 217 (44.1) G 136 (46.9) 144 (44.4)

A 501 (55.7) 455 (55.8) A 347 (56.9) 275 (55.9) A 154 (53.1) 180 (55.6)

p-value 0.97 0.74 0.54

SLC6A4 STin2 rare 13 (1.4) 15 (1.8) rare 7 (1.1) 5 (1) 9R 6 (2) 10 (3)

10R 346 (37.4) 326 (39) 10R 239 (38.4) 199 (39.6) 10R 107 (35.4) 127 (38)

12R 565 (61.1) 495 (59.2) 12R 376 (60.5) 298 (59.4) 12R 189 (62.6) 197 (59)

p-value 0.61 0.93 0.53

TH rs10743152 T 320 (35.6) 300 (36.9) T 214 (35.2) 176 (35.9) T 106 (36.6) 124 (38.5)

C 578 (64.4) 512 (63.1) C 394 (64.8) 314 (64.1) C 184 (63.4) 198 (61.5)

p-value 0.57 0.80 0.62

TH rs10840489 T 124 (13.8) 121 (14.8) T 87 (14.3) 84 (17.1) T 37 (12.8) 37 (11.4)

Page 78: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

66

Combined Female Male

Gene Marker Allele Super-Senior Control Allele Super-Senior Control Allele Super-Senior Control

TH rs10840489 C 776 (86.2) 695 (85.2) C 523 (85.7) 408 (82.9) C 253 (87.2) 287 (88.6)

p-value 0.53 0.20 0.61

TH rs10840491 A 116 (12.9) 118 (14.5) A 83 (13.6) 81 (16.5) A 33 (11.4) 37 (11.5)

G 784 (87.1) 696 (85.5) G 527 (86.4) 411 (83.5) G 257 (88.6) 285 (88.5)

p-value 0.33 0.19 0.97

TH rs11042978 A 463 (51.4) 392 (48) A 313 (51.3) 232 (47.2) C 140 (48.3) 164 (50.6)

C 437 (48.6) 424 (52) C 297 (48.7) 260 (52.8) A 150 (51.7) 160 (49.4)

p-value 0.16 0.17 0.56

TH rs6356 T 326 (36.7) 289 (35.6) T 216 (35.8) 175 (35.7) T 110 (38.7) 114 (35.4)

C 562 (63.3) 523 (64.4) C 388 (64.2) 315 (64.3) C 174 (61.3) 208 (64.6)

p-value 0.63 0.99 0.40

TH rs7483056 C 424 (47.1) 378 (46.3) C 291 (47.7) 234 (47.6) C 133 (45.9) 144 (44.4)

T 476 (52.9) 438 (53.7) T 319 (52.3) 258 (52.4) T 157 (54.1) 180 (55.6)

p-value 0.74 0.96 0.72

TH VNTR rare 86 (9.3) 85 (10.2) rare 57 (9.2) 48 (9.6) rare 29 (9.6) 37 (11.1)

6R 201 (21.8) 207 (24.8) 6R 138 (22.2) 133 (26.5) 6R 63 (20.9) 74 (22.2)

7R 183 (19.8) 145 (17.3) 7R 125 (20.1) 84 (16.7) 7R 58 (19.2) 61 (18.3)

9R 134 (14.5) 126 (15.1) 9R 83 (13.3) 82 (16.3) 9R 51 (16.9) 44 (13.2)

10R 320 (34.6) 273 (32.7) 10R 219 (35.2) 155 (30.9) 10R 101 (33.4) 118 (35.3)

p-value 0.41 0.13 0.67

* Uncorrected p-values below 0.05

** Uncorrected p-values above 0.05 but below 0.10

Page 79: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

67

Table 3.5. Adjusted p-values Using False Discovery Rate (FDR)*

Combined Female Male

Gene Marker Origina

l Adjuste

d

Original

Adjusted

Origina

l Adjuste

d

COMT rs1544325 0.955 0.969 0.665 0.987 0.498 0.837

rs165815 0.669 0.969 0.880 0.987 0.569 0.837

rs174696 0.026 0.923 0.091 0.987 0.131 0.837

rs2020917 0.267 0.969 0.251 0.987 0.881 0.933

rs4646316 0.727 0.969 0.942 0.987 0.507 0.837

rs4680 0.894 0.969 0.767 0.987 0.750 0.864

rs5993883 0.925 0.969 0.653 0.987 0.645 0.837

rs740601 0.392 0.969 0.401 0.987 0.633 0.837

rs9332377 0.139 0.969 0.286 0.987 0.303 0.837

rs933271 0.725 0.969 0.132 0.987 0.065 0.837

DRD4 rs11246226

0.527 0.969 0.825 0.987 0.337 0.837

rs11246228

0.663 0.969 0.696 0.987 0.666 0.837

rs3758653 0.669 0.969 0.919 0.987 0.528 0.837

VNTR 0.626 0.969 0.523 0.987 0.062 0.837

KATNAL2

rs2576037 0.940 0.969 0.469 0.987 0.392 0.837

MAOA rs3027456 0.413 0.969 0.671 0.987 0.294 0.837

rs3027450 0.420 0.969 0.766 0.987 0.317 0.837

rs1799836 0.855 0.969 0.942 0.987 0.768 0.864

VNTR 0.306 0.969 0.307 0.987 0.674 0.837

RAS1A rs2032794 0.795 0.969 0.686 0.987 0.956 0.966

rs1477268 0.809 0.969 0.784 0.987 0.836 0.912

SLC6A4 HTTLPR 0.959 0.969 0.762 0.987 0.529 0.837

rs140701 0.547 0.969 0.840 0.987 0.178 0.837

rs4251417 0.825 0.969 0.575 0.987 0.170 0.837

rs6354 0.817 0.969 0.957 0.987 0.635 0.837

rs6505165 0.630 0.969 0.281 0.987 0.495 0.837

rs7214248 0.449 0.969 0.179 0.987 0.637 0.837

rs9303628 0.969 0.969 0.742 0.987 0.543 0.837

STin2 0.607 0.969 0.932 0.987 0.531 0.837

TH rs10743152

0.573 0.969 0.804 0.987 0.618 0.837

Page 80: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

68

rs10840489

0.534 0.969 0.200 0.987 0.611 0.837

rs10840491

0.333 0.969 0.185 0.987 0.966 0.966

TH rs11042978

0.159 0.969 0.170 0.987 0.562 0.837

rs6356 0.631 0.969 0.987 0.987 0.397 0.837

rs7483056 0.744 0.969 0.962 0.987 0.725 0.864

VNTR 0.414 0.969 0.134 0.987 0.673 0.837

* Corrected for 36 tests

Page 81: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

69

Chapter 4. Discussion

4.1. Interpretation of Results

After multiple test corrections, no associations were found between allele

frequencies in five personality related genes (COMT, DRD4, MAOA, SLC6A4, TH) for the

Super-Seniors versus the middle-aged control group. There was one significant

association before corrections in the COMT SNP rs174696. Additionally, there were two

associations in the male group, COMT SNP rs933271 and the DRD4 VNTR rare alleles,

with p-values less than 0.10 but did not meet significance.

Associations with the COMT SNP rs174696 have been reported before. The C/C

genotype was associated with increased novelty seeking scores using Cloninger’s TCI in

a heroin dependent Chinese study group [449]. Novelty seeking is comparable to the big

five’s excitement-seeking. The Super-Seniors showed increased minor allele frequency

(C) at this marker before correction, possibly connecting novelty seeking to dopamine

systems as previously discussed in the gene’s background. The SNP is located mid intron

5 but there could be potential functional effects through the modification of a splicing site

or recruitment of transcriptional factors; however, this has not been reported in the

literature.

The COMT SNP rs933271 observed in the male group has not been well studied.

It has been associated with impulsivity in a haplotype report [450], but there is little other

research for association studies with personality. The DRD4 VNTR alleles that were

grouped together as rare alleles for analysis (3R, 5R, 6R and 8R) are understudied alleles.

There are very few publications that focus on alleles outside of the common 2R, 4R and

7R, most likely due to small sample sizes for the rare alleles. Recently, however, Hasler

et al. found the 6R allele to be associated with an adult ADHD phenotype [451]. The 6R

allele has also been associated in intellectual disabilities of unknown etiology [452]. As

discussed previously, the DRD4 VNTR causes functional changes to the dopamine

receptor, with the 7R allele showing diminished function [178], but these rare alleles have

not been investigated for their specific functionalities.

Page 82: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

70

It was not surprising to see COMT as an association; COMT was a very good

candidate for potential difference between Super-Seniors and controls given the MB-

COMT’s regulation of dopamine [170] in the prefrontal cortex [170,172]. As dopamine is

integral to the reward-system [113,114,118,119], COMT should be an excellent candidate

for studies looking at associations with extraversion.

The combined sample was well powered (0.8) to detect an OR of 1.5. The p-value

threshold was set at 0.05, meaning there is one expected false positive result for every 20

tests performed. For the 36 tests conducted, one association was found before

corrections. The COMT SNP rs174696 was likely a false positive and was appropriately

corrected in the FDR calculation. The stratified male group consisted of 145 Super-

Seniors and 162 controls for a total power of 0.39 to detect an OR of 1.5 [453]. There is

only a 40% chance of finding a real effect in this sub-group. There were two notable weak

associations that did not meet the significance cutoff and were corrected for by the FDR

calculation.

One possibility is that there is truly an association between the Super-Senior

phenotype and the candidate genes, but we have failed to detect it due to power or multiple

testing adjustments. This is a valid concern for the female and male subgroups given their

respective powers are 0.62 and 0.39, however, the combined sample was sufficiently

powered to detect an effect of modest magnitude (OR 1.5). It is also possible that the

effect size, or contribution, of these candidate genes is less than the moderate OR of 1.5.

GWAS studies have reported associations to range from small [454] to modest [65,455].

If the candidate genes only have a small effect size the current design is not powered to

detect it. Alternative, the FDR adjustment could have eliminated true associations.

While these results are in agreement with other healthy aging and personality

studies (DRD4 [456], SLC6A4 [457,458]), other studies have been published supporting

associations (COMT [132], SLC64A [398], TH [234], RAS1A and KATNAL2 [454]). To our

knowledge this is the first study looking at MAOA associations within the scope of healthy

aging and personality.

Page 83: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

71

4.1.1. Relevant Association Studies

COMT’s SNP rs4680 G allele was recently found to be associated with lower

neuroticism scores using 616 unrelated healthy young-seniors [132]. The sample was

Caucasian, 56% female, with an average age of 69.26 (SD 9.7) [132]. Subjects were

labeled as healthy if they passed dementia related criteria looking for depression and

cognitive impairments [132]. The study tested SNP rs4680 and scores from the NEO-FFI

[132]. It was well powered for their sample size at the domain level, and shows good

evidence for a true association in this sample.

DRD4 has been tested for personality associations in a healthy young-senior

population [456]. Vandenberg et al. looked at novelty seeking, measured by the NEO-PI-

R, and the 7R allele in DRD4’s VNTR [456]. Using the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of

Aging (BLSA) cohort, they compared 100 subjects with the highest and 100 subjects with

the lowest novelty seeking scores [456], finding no associations. The average age of the

subject was 61.3 years and the sample was 56% male, no other characteristics were given

in the paper [456]. Given they were only testing for one allele and used extreme

phenotypes, their design was sufficient to detect an association if there was one.

SLC6A4’s HTTLPR VNTR has varying associations even within studies.

Terracciano et al. looked for associations between the VNTR and eight SNPs around that

region with personality in two sample groups [457]. The first group consisted of 3913

Italians from the SardiNIA cohort who completed the NEO-PI-R [457]. This first group had

an average age of 42.5 (SD 16.7) with 57% of the subjects being female [457]. There were

no associations found between personality and the nine markers [457]. Additionally, they

also used 548 mostly Caucasian individuals from the BLSA cohort, which averaged 52.9

years of age (SD 12.5), 51% being female [457]. In this sample they saw an association

between the VNTR S/L genotype and lower neuroticism scores [457]. The association

seen in the BLSA sample is likely a false positive given the lower sample size and inability

to establish an allelic affect.

Brummet et al. had previously looked at 103 depressed and 99 non-depressed

geriatric, mostly Caucasian, individuals for associations between depression, HTTLPR

alleles, and personality [458]. The sample was 63.9% female, personality was measured

Page 84: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

72

with the NEO-PI-R, and criteria for consideration of geriatric status were not defined in the

paper [458]. No associations between depression status and personality were found [458],

but they did find that the S allele was associated with lower neuroticism in males [458].

Results are questionable given the low sample size and the division of the study sample

to look for alternative associations, likely there is no association for HTTLPR and

personality.

Gondo et al. published a study looking for associations between HTTLPR alleles

and personality in a centenarian sample [398]. The study contained 265 Japanese

centenarians who were measured for personality with the NEO-FFI and 1377 younger

controls with no personality measures [398]. The centenarian’s average age was 101.2

(SD 1.8) and 78% were female [398]. The younger cohort was derived through published

HTTLPR allele frequencies in other studies with Japanese samples of varying

characteristics [398]. They did not find an association between HTTLPR and personality

scores in the centenarians but did find that their centenarians more frequently carried the

L allele compared to the younger control group [398]. This study supports SLC6A4’s

association with longevity but fails to make a connection to personality. The population is

also not comparable to our Caucasian sample making similar associations unlikely.

TH has been associated to aging in a study conducted by De Benedict et al. [234].

The study looked at genotypic frequencies in a group of 196 Italian centenarians (73%

female) and 358 Italian controls (55% female) aged 10 to 85 year [234]. The TH VNTR

alleles were reduced to a long (9, 10 and 10i repeats) and short (6, 7 and 8 repeats)

version for analysis [234]. The group found that male centenarians carried the L/L

genotype less frequently than the control group [234], supporting the genes involvement

with longevity. The study, however, is not well powered to find associations in the male

group as there were only 53 male centenarians in the study.

RASA1 SNPs rs1477268 and rs2032794 and KATNAL2 SNP rs2576037 were

markers identified in de Moor et al.’s 2012 personality GWAS [454]. Personality was

measured with a variety of NEO questionnaires in the pooled study sample and the design

used 17375 middle aged samples in the discovery phase and 3294 middle aged samples

in the replication phase [454]. The number of SNPs genotyped varied depending on

Page 85: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

73

sample population but ranged from 6K-1M SNPs [454]. The RASA1 SNPs were found to

be associated with increased openness scores and the KATNAL2 SNP rs2576037 was

found to be associated to increased conscientiousness scores, although neither of these

associations were found in the replication sample [454]. This study was not concerned

with longevity, despite using longevity sample groups such as BLSA, and the results from

a middle aged sample may not apply to other age groups.

Amongst the candidate genes and SNPs selected for study, there is a lot of

variation in the published associations with personality and longevity. This variation makes

it difficult to determine if true relationships exist. Data in this study supports the conclusion

that there is no association between the candidate genes selected and the Super-Senior

phenotype but that does not exclude these genes from being associated in personality or

other healthy aging studies that study related but different phenotypes.

4.1.2. Potential Causes of Variation in Published Association Studies for Candidate Genes

Personality and aging are complex phenotypes, reasonably affected by many

small common contributing polymorphisms throughout the genome to produce the

phenotype. The expected effect size for any given marker could be quite small and difficult

to detect without a large sample size [459]. The study of psychiatric disorders, such as

schizophrenia and bi-polar disorder, represent related complex phenotypes. Identifying

genes in these disorders has been difficult due to imprecise clinical definitions of the

phenotype, allelic heterogeneity, epistasis (modifier genes) and interactions from non-

gene factors (i.e. environmental factors) [460]; issues that parallel problems within healthy

aging and personality research.

As previously discussed, healthy aging can vary considerably between studies in

its definition [5]. This study’s definition of aging is not exactly the same as that of previous

studies where significant associations were found [132,398,454], therefore making it

difficult to predict if previous associations would be observed in the Super-Senior sample.

Further, the unique features and regulatory elements of the candidate genes may

contribute to finding associations in some studies and none in others. SLC64A will be used

Page 86: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

74

as an example to illustrate mechanisms by which allelic heterogeneity and epistasis may

be at play in inconsistencies in establishing association.

The well studied SLC6A4 gene has many variants. The SLC6A4 VNTR, HTTLPR,

has two alleles, S and L, but these have been found to have many SNP variants

superimposed upon these size alleles. Nakamura et al. genotyped CEPH control and

Japanese samples finding there were 4 variants of the S allele and 6 of the L allele [376].

Investigations into allelic frequency differences between the Japanese and Caucasian

groups showed Caucasian samples displaying only two of the six L alleles and the

Japanese samples having a greater range of variants [376]. Variations of the SNPs on the

haplotypes of the S and L alleles could be contributing to association inconsistency

between personality studies.

The SLC6A4 has another polymorphism in the HTTLPR VNTR. Within the L allele,

an A/G SNP causes a functional change [461]. When G is present in the L allele, there is

a similar rate of transcription as the lower transcribed S allele [461]. An obsessive

compulsive disorder (OCD) study looked at allelic frequencies within different populations

and found that the L(A) allele is more frequent in Caucasian OCD [461]. Here the L(A)

allele is over expressing, causing more transporter proteins to be produced, clearing

serotonin from the synaptic gap too quickly. Very few studies test for this SNP, which could

add to inconsistencies between studies.

SLC6A4 has a well established epistasis effect with brain-derived neurotrophic

factor (BDNF) in emotional regulation. Individuals carrying the S or functional equivalent

L(G) allele and were homozygous for the Val allele in the BDNF gene are associated with

increased cognitive reactivity (a term for dysfunctional thinking where mood changes

rapidly from neutral or positive to negative) [462], and reduced volumes in the anterior

cortex (regulator of emotion) [463]. If the Met allele in BDNF was present, it showed a

protective effect, with subjects showing less cognitive reactivity [462] and smaller

reductions in the anterior cortex compared to the L allele [463]. BDNF interacts with

SLC6A4 to regulate emotional stability and could be adding to inconsistent associations

between SLC6A4 and observed personality.

Page 87: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

75

Epidemiological studies have reported associations between personality and

longevity [79,80,82,86]. Twin studies have shown personality and healthy aging to be

approximately 40% [40,65] and 25% [464] heritable, respectively. There is evidence of a

genetic factor but the effect may not be captured at an allelic level, which would explain

why this study could not find an association. Epigenetic factors are known to impact many

of the candidate genes and could explain why allelic associations were not seen.

Epigenetics is a dynamic modification of the DNA or histones to regulate expression [460],

and can result in short term or long term modifications [465]. Epigenetics has been

implicated in learning and memory [466,467] and four of the candidate genes have been

investigated for an epigenetic role in neurological functions.

For example, the COMT methylation site within the promoter region has shown

different methylation patterns in monozygotic twins [468]. The methylation of SLC6A4 is

implicated in the stress response, such that increased methylation results in increased

stress [469,470]. In human neural stem cells, methylation of TH was found to reduce its

expression [471]. Finally, bipolar patients had increased methylation on their MAOA and

COMT genes [472]. It is worth noting that there is regional tissue specificity for methylation

[473–475] and blood or buccal swab samples used in these experiments likely do not

represent the true methylation patterns in the brain. These epigenetic effects could be

causing false positives in personality association studies; where by chance, the genotype

of affected individuals at a gene appears associated, but the true effect is caused or

regulated by an epigenetic mechanism, complicating the search for true associations.

There are many scenarios that could explain the variation of reported associations

with the chosen candidate genes. Our failure to discover associations in personality-

related genes may be attributed to false positives in the literature [476], misguiding the

selection of candidate genes. The correct conclusion to draw is that there is likely no

association between the allelic frequencies of the Super-Seniors and the younger control

groups for these selected candidate genes. There are of course limitations to the design

of this study that limit its level of certainty.

Page 88: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

76

4.2. Limitations of Design

4.2.1. Candidate Gene Selection

Candidate gene designs are good to use in smaller sample sizes, as they require

fewer tests [477], however, they rely on current biological knowledge to select the

candidate genes [477]. Reliance on established knowledge can be difficult if the

phenotype is unique, and genetically heterogeneous.

Variability in associations between studies can be caused by differences in design,

particularly the definition of study populations and definition of the studied phenotype

[477,478]. This study’s population is uniquely defined compared to other healthy aging

studies in terms of age and health status [65,84,398]. The age classification begins at 85

and contains exclusion criteria for five specific chronic conditions (dementia, cancer,

diabetes, major pulmonary disease, and cardiovascular disease). This definition is

selected based on the top five chronic diseases and cutoffs for average life expectancy.

As this definition is unique, candidate gene selection relies on the use of results of studies

of similar phenotypes to ask related questions which may not have the same underlying

biological or genetic factors.

Genetic heterogeneity of complex traits could be a complication for determining

genetic contributions. The etiology of many psychiatric disorders, including personality

disorders is known to be variable [478]. It also stands to reason that personality could

develop through many different biological pathways, which would result in many genotypic

profiles producing a commonly observed phenotype. Limitations in our current knowledge

regarding the internal production of personality make this problem difficult to assess. Well

defined phenotypes and strict enrolment criteria for certain biological pathways can help

control for this heterogeneity [477], but such information is not currently available for aging

or personality. The uncertain biological origination of personality and aging make it difficult

to rely on associations seen in other studies for gene selection.

Page 89: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

77

4.2.2. Marker Selection

Many genetic study designs, in general, suffer from an over-reliance on linkage

disequilibrium. Linkage disequilibrium between two markers can vary across populations

[477] and the markers used can have different allele frequencies [479], although we try to

control for this by restricting the study population to one ethnicity. To illustrate, a marker

randomly drawn from a population of interest is in high linkage disequilibrium with the true

allele, the variant affecting the phenotype. In one study an association is found between

the marker and the phenotype of interest. When repeating the experiment, sampling from

the same study population, an effect was not found due to the sample by chance having

less linkage disequilibrium between the marker and the true variant. In this example the

difference in linkage disequilibrium between the marker and true allele can cause variation

in the observed association [476]. Differences in allele frequencies of the markers can

also affect the association [479].

When the frequency of the marker and true allele are similar there is greater power

to detect an association [479]. A marker with the same minor allelic frequency (30%) as

the true allele in high linkage disequilibrium, is more likely to show an association

compared to another marker, also in high linkage disequilibrium but with a different allele

frequency (60%) [479]. In complex traits, it is hypothesized that the underlying genetic

variants are very common (MAF >10%) [479], and this difference in allele frequencies

between sample population could account for variation in association findings. The current

design relies on using tagSNPs to represent areas of high linkage disequilibrium to detect

association within that area; however, this design has not accounted for the potential

variation in linkage disequilibrium or allelic frequencies other than selecting markers using

Caucasian HapMap data for the Caucasian study population.

The HapMap data has potential problems in admixed populations [480] but works

very well when the correct reference is matched to the study population. Studies looking

at the variance between linkage disequilibrium blocks in the CEU HapMap data and

European populations have found the blocks to be almost identical [481] and tagSNPs

were able to capture a high amount of variance in the regions tested [481,482]. Matching

the correct HapMap reference population to the study population is an important

Page 90: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

78

consideration to avoid problems in varying linkage disequilibrium blocks and allelic

frequencies.

VNTR data is not commonly used as it once had been in candidate studies. This

is due to the innovation in SNP genotyping technology that has made SNP markers a

more cost effective strategy and has allowed studies, such as GWAS designs, to look at

the whole genome for associations [459]. VNTR genotyping has remained a time

consuming process. Given the volume of samples and procedures needed, there are

many opportunities for human error. To reduce human errors during genotyping, plate

maps were checked twice, plates were prepared using two researchers, and pooling of

the VNTRs was checked by two researchers. Prior to performing wet laboratory

genotyping primers and primers with dye were optimized before use in samples. The

VNTR PCR fragments were verified for predicted size, and blank and repeat samples were

present on every plate sent for VNTR fragment analysis. Additionally, there is no database

or structured nomenclature to help researchers identify previous studies, alleles, imperfect

copies (such as the 10iR VNTR in TH [225]), or help score the repeats [459]. The

conducted literature search was done to ensure information regarding alternate names,

alleles, rare alleles and methods to score the alleles were known prior to study

commencement. While known beforehand, the current design was not able to differentiate

between TH’s 10R and 10iR repeats [225] and had to be combined during allele calling

with GeneMapper.

4.2.3. Power and Rejection of the Null Hypothesis

Candidate gene designs may ultimately have poor reproducibility because they

consistently fail to exclude chance as an explanation for weak associations [476]. Savitz

and Ramesar have summarized reasons behind the many false positives in candidate

gene studies, citing a failure to correct for multiple testing, arbitrary grouping of alleles,

ethnic stratification, lack of sex stratification, and poor sample size as factors behind

irreproducible results [483]. Calhoun et al. further criticized candidate studies for stratifying

or examining haplotypes only when there is failure to produce an association at the SNP

level to increase the likelihood of finding an association [476]. The current design attempts

to reconcile these concerns.

Page 91: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

79

Sample size in the combined group met power requirements and multiple

corrections were applied. Importantly, stratification was done by sex given many of the

candidate genes showed differences between females and males (COMT [173], MAOA

[192,199] and SLC6A4 [219]). The subgroups created were not well powered, however,

and the validity of the sex-specific results is limited. The current study design avoids using

potentially biased grouping of multi allelic data together and instead attempts to analyze

each allele. There were, however, many small counts of rare alleles that had to be binned

together for analysis purposes. The inclusion of subjects in the design requires all four

grandparents of the subject to be of European descent, so that there is ethnic stratification

at the design level, minimizing confounding due to population stratification.

Candidate study designs, like many other designs, suffer from publication bias

[484]. Studies showing associations are more likely to be found in subgroups (smaller

sample populations that have less power compared to the total group) and in markers of

interest found without a prior hypothesis as to which alleles are more likely to be

associated based on biological function [484]. The current design’s candidate genes were

chosen through a literature search and suffer from bias induced by possible unpublished

studies showing no associations. As this is an acknowledged risk, biological plausibility

for function of candidate genes in personality was also a criterion for selection. Prior

knowledge of problems with power in stratified groups ensures the potential association

is recorded and explored, but that the loss of power is acknowledged.

There is also loss of power from having no a prior hypothesis regarding which allele

is likely to associate to the Super-Senior status, as testing one allele against the rest

increases the allele count as opposed to testing all alleles against each other. The

literature reviewed for the candidate genes was heavily focused on alleles associated with

personality dysfunctions. For example, the 7R allele in DRD4 is well researched for its

association with impulsivity and aggression [195], and many study designs collapse alleles

to “7R” and “non-7R”. Difficulty arises in making predictions for functionality in the Super-

Seniors as there are no studies tailored to the action of these markers in the very old and

very healthy. This is primarily a problem in the multi-allelic VNTRs. As there are no

established alleles for the Super-Senior phenotype, an a priori hypothesis for functional

allele could not be established.

Page 92: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

80

4.2.4. Control Group

The control group used in this study is a younger cohort, which have not been

selected for disease status, to represent the population. They range in age at enrolment

from 41-54 and reside in the Greater Vancouver Lower Mainland, BC. This is not the ideal

control group for the Super-Seniors, whose age range is 85 and over; however, alternative

control groups for the Super-Seniors are not available. Ideal candidates are those born in

the same era, who have succumbed to disease and are already deceased. Long lived

individuals who have been diagnosed with one of the mentioned morbidities are not

appropriate either, as they have achieved longevity but not the comparable health status

as the Super-Seniors. If the purpose of the Super-Senior Study was to investigate the

genetics of a specific disease, this group would be appropriate. They would, however,

confound studies of longevity. The study design relies on using a known imperfect control

group that differs in environmental generational effects.

As the use of a younger cohort is common in the genetics of aging, Lewis et al.

conducted a meta-analysis of longevity studies looking at the validity of two assumptions

[485]. First, that the initial relative allelic frequencies between the cases and controls were

similar [485]. Secondly, that the risk of mortality and genotype did not depend the year of

birth [485]. The meta-analysis concluded that these two assumptions were generally

invalid for many studies but suggested some control measures [485]. Allelic differences

can arise from gene flow, or migration in and out of an area that alters the allelic frequency.

This can cause different allelic frequencies between decades and is a problem even after

ethnic stratification [485], as individuals of European ancestry can come from a variety of

regions and countries [485]. Choosing a stable population in terms of exposures and gene

flow would maintain the integrity of these assumptions [485] and can be controlled for by

classifying ethnicity by place of birth. The current design’s more stringent criteria require

European ancestry for all four grandparents to be classified as being of European descent.

Selection bias for the control groups is a possible issue in the study design.

Controls were not ascertained based on health status as they are to represent a random

sample from the population. Controls were contacted only through Ministry of Health lists

with a 60% response rate. There could be differences between the controls that responded

and those that did not, known as volunteer bias. Volunteers are known to be different from

Page 93: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

81

the population in terms of intelligence [486], wellbeing [487] and personality [488,489].

There is also evidence that volunteers have reduced mortality [490], which could be

confounding for this study if there are a greater number of controls that will reach Super-

Senior status than predicted. This would decrease the power of the current design by

lessening the magnitude of the effect size. Nevertheless, volunteer bias would be present

in both the Super-Seniors and controls which may help mitigate the overall bias.

Misclassification of exposure, the labeling of a non-Super-Senior as a Super-

Senior vice versa [476], is present in this study. Some members of the control group, on

the order of a few percent, could become Super-Seniors later in life, diminishing the allelic

frequency differences between the groups. Alternatively Super-Seniors are self-reported

and may not have meet study criteria at time of enrolment. Other classical means of

misclassification are through laboratory errors such as mislabelled samples and arranging

sample plate with all controls and all cases. These were controlled for with strict laboratory

procedures which included, double checking, two person plate preparations, and including

cases and controls on each plate prepared.

4.2.5. Sample Size

Sample size calculations were based on an OR value of 1.5, as this is a magnitude

of association that is plausible. Calhoun et al. noted initial associations reported are

inflated and in estimating power, a lower value should be used ([476]. The current design

has a power of 0.8 for an effect size of 1.5; however, if the effect size is reduced to 1.25

the study’s power drops to 0.3 [453]. As aging is a complex trait with potentially many

genes contributing small effects, it is plausible that effect sizes could be much lower than

1.5 and would require a much larger sample size to see an association.

4.2.6. Lack of Personality Testing

There was no formal personality testing done with the Super-Seniors or the control

group in the parent study. In a sub-study conducted by a directed studies student in our

laboratory, a group of 69 Super-Seniors were interviewed four to seven years after

recruitment. They displayed the typical pattern of high extraversion, conscientious,

Page 94: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

82

openness, agreeableness and low neuroticism found in the literature, when compared to

a younger and diseased burdened population (manuscript in preparation). Personality

testing was not part of the information captured at study enrollment, and testing requires

time consuming work through subject contact, coordinator time, and trained employees

for personality assessment. Four to seven years after recruitment, only 69 European-

ancestry Super-Seniors were available for the personality sub-study.

Based on previous research, in our lab and in the literature, it is inferred that the

Super-Seniors and controls have different personality profiles and therefore different allelic

frequencies in the selected candidate genes. Volunteer bias, however, could render that

untrue.

Personality is known to be a factor in volunteer bias, where volunteers who consent

to studies are typically more open and agreeable than non-volunteers [488,491], and those

than agree to long term follow up are more extraverted [488,491]. Subjects who

volunteered for the Super-Senior Study may be particularly high in extraversion, openness

and agreeableness, giving them similar personality profiles.

All study designs come with limitations. Candidate gene designs have better power

to detect smaller associations [477]; which serves the unique Super-Senior population

well, as recruitments of sufficient numbers for designs such as GWAS would be difficult.

The current design is able to detect moderate effects but still suffers from design

limitations, including phenotype definition, marker choices, misclassification of exposure,

sample size and undefined differences between cases and controls.

4.3. Recommendations

4.3.1. Candidate Gene Based Design

Longevity is a complex trait with many factors impacting the phenotype,

complicated further by the pleiotropic natures of longevity genes [484]. The candidate

gene method is a good design due to low costs, quick outcomes and directed design [492],

but there are some improvements that can be made to the current design. These include

Page 95: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

83

refined hypotheses for candidate gene selection, the use of modeling for analysis, and

added quality control SNPs.

Directed hypotheses for biological functions may also help illuminate true

associations in the Super-Seniors. Given the established personality profile [79,80,82,86]

and association of personality related genes to longevity (DRD4 [493], SLC6A4 [398], and

TH [494]), a hypothesis could be made as to which personality related biological systems

could be involved in longevity. Dopamine and serotonin are good candidate systems due

to their relationship to learning [111,113,114] and stress [111,149], respectively. Focusing

on one of these neurotransmitter systems could help identify true associations and

interrelated markers. Using the networking program GeneMANIA [495], related genes can

be used to from a candidate list to test for association. For example the original candidate

gene list was input into GeneMANIA (figure 4.1), showing a relatively sparse network with

few relationships between genes. When networks are mapped for serotonin receptors

(figure 4.2), however, the network becomes much more inter-related, making associations

potentially easier to identify by revealing other candidate genes to investigate, or building

models to explain phenotypes. The networks in figure 4.1 and 4.2 reveal new genes for

investigation that did not surface in the literature search.

Page 96: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

84

Figure 4.1. Candidate gene network

Black nodes represent input genes. Grey nodes are related genes, scored and ranked in decreasing size for how informative the gene is in the network of queried genes [495]. GeneMANIA uses large public datasets, such as e!Ensembl and NCBI, to find related genes [495].

Page 97: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

85

Figure 4.2. Serotonin receptors and associated BDNF, TPH1 and TPH2 network Black nodes represent input genes. TH is present as gene of interest in this network with other genes such as PAH, S1PR1 and GPR26 that did not show up in the literature search but may be of functional interest in the regulation of serotonin.

Derringer et al. used a candidate systems approach to identify aggregated SNPs

in the dopamine system. Associated SNPs were used in a regression model to show SNPs

across different genes accounted for variance in sensation-seeking behaviours [496].

Derringer et al. went further to develop a genetic risk score for sensation-seeking

behaviours [496], although there have been no replication studies to date. The serotonin

system could be used to look at stress scores or neuroticism scores in the Super-Seniors,

giving the design more focus and direction towards looking for genetic contributions to

aging phenomena such as inflammaging.

Page 98: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

86

Candidate systems could help with the discovery of related genes to study but this

may also increase the number of SNPs to test. Bayesian statistical approaches could help

mitigate the loss of power from multiple testing but require a prior assumption regarding

the effect size of each SNP being tested [497]. These effects can be difficult to predict,

especially when exploring new phenotype and polymorphism relationships. The use of

Bayesian methods can further be impeded by the lack of user friendly software, requiring

coding knowledge to execute. Programs such as SNPTEST, however, are becoming more

readily available and easier to use, making the methods more accessible to the genetic

community. Bayesian methods are likely to be more influential in large designs, such as

GWAS, where multiple corrections for hundreds of thousands of tests can mask

associations.

Alternatively, after initial association testing, model building could be conducted

with associated markers. Using logistical regression models could help control for known

environmental confounders [498], and build genetic scores for certain phenotypes, such

as stress response.

With a better system for selecting candidate genes, improvements should be made

on the markers selected. SNP selection can be done through a tagSNP approach, but

built in quality control checks should be selected as well. Adding extra SNPs that are

known to be in linkage disequilibrium with each other could serve as a quality control check

within the control group [477], but would also raise the cost of genotyping. By testing these

known SNPs for their level of linkage disequilibrium, the study would have greater control

for variation in linkage disequilibrium between SNPs. VNTRs should always be included if

available in candidate genes as they may represent missing heritability between what is

predicted in heredity studies and what is seen the GWAS data [459]. Before inclusion,

VNTRs should be assessed for the functional properties they could contribute, which

includes tissue specificity [459].

Changes to the candidate design should focus on investigating neurological

systems such as serotonin for gene selection, use a modeling approach and include SNPs

to confirm linkage disequilibrium. While these changes can be implemented in future

Page 99: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

87

candidate designs there are other recommendations for the Super-Senior Study that are

not as easily accomplished.

4.3.2. Super-Senior Study Design

Issues to improve upon in the parent study design include sample size and

phenotype testing in both populations.

An increased sample size would alleviate challenges with power, especially for

subgroup analysis. Recruitment has re-opened for the Super-Senior Study and collection

of additional participants is underway. With more participants enrolled, even larger

selections of candidate genes could take place. If the sample size doubled, there would

be sufficient power to detect an effect size of 1.35 [453]. Even if the sample size were

doubled, the design is limiting. As the effects of associations are predicted to be weak, it

is plausible that thousands of cases and controls would be needed for detection [484].

Sample pooling with other aging groups could increase the sample size enough to detect

these effects, and will be a consideration for our lab in the future.

It would be ideal to have the results of personality testing for all Super-Seniors and

controls, when looking for genes that could affect personality related physiological

functions. Such data would allow for a more conclusive analysis through logistical

regression with personality traits and markers. The genetics of personality is still a

developing field and suffers from technological constraints in linking genotype to

expression to physiological response and to phenotype. These processes happen in an

organ that is not easily sampled and produces a large volume of electrical data, making

visualization difficult. While there are promising candidates, there are no confirmed genes

that contribute to personality. Therefore, we cannot claim that differences in proposed

personality genes do indeed cause differences in personality phenotypes, which would be

expected to impact behavioural choices influencing longevity.

The recommend design would still maintain the current control group, increase the

sample size, and conduct either personality or stress testing in the study sample. Linking

longevity to neurological processes that govern personality is complex and many more

studies will be needed to understand how these two phenotypes could be connected. The

Page 100: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

88

personality and aging fields are both making headway to discover the complex and

nuanced ways that our genetics can affect our health, behaviours and perspectives.

Future expansion and culmination of smaller association studies could lead to

advancements of biological knowledge and lead to better heath care for an aging

population.

4.4. Future Developments

Association studies are important stepping stones to develop our understanding of

biological phenomena such as aging or discovering the biological mechanisms that dictate

our behavioural, emotional and thought processes. Association studies build upon one

another and future developments with better designs could help solve questions about

missing heritability in genetics and change our approaches to healthcare.

4.4.1. Towards Integrative Approaches

Missing heritability is the unaccountable difference between the heritability found

in twin studies and the phenotypic variation that can be explained through sequence

differences [460]. Personality is a moderately heritable complex trait, with twin studies

estimating it to be approximately 40% [40,65]; however, there are currently only

hypotheses regarding the genes involved in personality and the small effect sizes of

associated polymorphisms would not explain the observed 40% heritability. Extreme

neurological phenotypes, such as schizophrenia and bipolar disorder both which are about

80% heritable [460], have not fared much better in the search for associations, with only

a fraction of risk being attributed to rare mutations and common genetic factors [460].

Closing the gap on this missing heritability may not come from more association

studies of single SNPs, but from focusing on interactions between SNPs, other genes,

and environmental effects. Logistical regression will be an important tool to help

researchers accomplish this. Using candidate systems to model SNPs and develop risk

scores could be used to look for interactions across different genes and show how they

contribute to complex phenotypes. This kind of modeling can capture epistasis effects of

other genes, which may better help explain the heritability of traits.

Page 101: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

89

Environmental factors can confound genetics studies, particularly those of

heritability, as characteristics such as lifestyle habits, wealth, education and social status

can be inherited from family [464]. Further, environmental factors can induce epigenetic

regulation that can become heritable [460]. These effects may be causing a ‘phantom’

heritability [460], where the reported heritability is inflated from these interactions.

Logistical regression can also help with environmental confounders as known

environmental characteristics can be added as a covariate to models. Models like these

could be used to explain heritability and also to develop mortality risk scores including

personality related genes.

4.4.2. Personality and Personalized Medicine Interventions for Longevity

Personality has been shown to be an excellent predictor of health status [50,499]

and healthy behaviours [60,95]. Personality is already comparable to current standard

metrics such as SES [92] and out performs SES in predicting quality of life in old age [500].

Longevity itself is not a good predictor of healthy aging as it measures factors that

influence the length of life, not factors behind long term health [32].

Conscientiousness and neuroticism are the best predictors of mortality risk [93,94],

and would be good candidates for developing genetic scores of mortality risk with

candidate genes. Neuroticism is an exciting trait to study due to its association with the

stress response [501]. Risk scores could eventually be developed from genes controlling

neuroticism in the serotonin system and other stress related pathways that could predict

risk for inflammaging. This method of profiling could help medical professionals make

more informed decisions about treatment options in patients and could help modify

unhealthy behaviours.

Personalized medicine is a movement to make clinical decisions based on the

unique health profile of patients, which includes considering their genes, proteins and

environment when making a diagnosis and treatment plan [502]. Much of the focus of

personalized medicine has been on tailoring individual cancer treatments or using genetic

information as a clinical guide for drug dosage, optimizing efficacy and safety of treatments

Page 102: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

90

[502]. Yet, there are a growing number of genetic scores being developed that can be

used as behavioural predictors.

A recently developed ‘smoking quit success genotype score’ has been developed

by Uhl et al. [503]. The score was shown to not only be predictive of smoking cessation

success in adults, but was used in a longitudinal study with Baltimore adolescents to show

it could be used to predict the rate and escalation of addictive substances use [503]. This

score could be used to predict smoking and addictive substance behaviours, allowing

healthcare providers an opportunity to administer preventative measures in at-risk youth.

Personalize medicine could be used to implement preventative measures in identified at-

risk groups and genetic personality scores could help identify individuals who need or do

not need extra resources.

By understanding and profiling the genetics of personality, treatment and therapy

plans could be built around a specific patient needs. Friedman et al. found that different

personality types used certain health services with varying frequencies [504]. The

scaffolding of treatments and therapies could be created based on genetic scores, and

fine tuned with patients to produce tailored plans; such as incorporating additional

assistance with coping strategies for individuals profiled to be high in neuroticism or

offering more social support to introverted patients.

Ultimately the goal of personalized medicine is best summarized by Benjamin

Chapman as a means to “identify those at risk for health problems before these problems

develop, so that preventive efforts can be successfully implemented” [39]. Personalized

medicine could help deliver more effective treatments and preventions for better patient

outcomes. With better outcomes and prevention of chronic disease there is an opportunity

for more individuals to reach Super-Senior status and to help current Super-Seniors

maintain their health.

Page 103: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

91

References

1. Statistics Canada. Canada Year Book 2012. Ottawa, Ontario: Minister of Industry; 2012.

2. Department of Economics and Social Affairs Population Division. World Population Ageing 2013. New York; 2013.

3. Alkema L, Raftery AE, Gerland P, Clark SJ, Pelletier F, Buettner T, et al. Probabilistic projections of the total fertility rate for all countries. Demography. 2011 Aug;48(3):815–39.

4. Raftery AE, Lewis SM, Aghajanian A. Demand or ideation? Evidence from the Iranian marital fertility decline. Demography. 1995 May;32(2):159–82.

5. Depp CA, Jeste D V. Definitions and predictors of successful aging: a comprehensive review of larger quantitative studies. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry; 2006;14(1):6–20.

6. Glatt SJ, Chayavichitsilp P, Depp C, Schork NJ, Jeste D V. Successful Aging: From Phenotype to Genotype. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:282–93.

7. Marengoni A, Angleman S, Melis R, Mangialasche F, Karp A, Garmen A, et al. Aging with multimorbidity: a systematic review of the literature. Ageing Res Rev. .; 2011 Sep;10(4):430–9.

8. McDaid O, Hanly MJ, Richardson K, Kee F, Kenny RA, Savva GM. The effect of

Page 104: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

92

multiple chronic conditions on self-rated health, disability and quality of life among the older populations of Northern Ireland and the Republic of Ireland: a comparison of two nationally representative cross-sectional surveys. BMJ Open. 2013 Jan;3(6):1–5.

9. Campolina AG, Adami F, Santos JLF, Lebrao ML. Effect of eliminating chronic diseases among elderly individuals. Rev Saude Publica. 2013 Jun;47(3):514–22.

10. Butler-Jones D. Report on the State of Canada Public Health in Canada 2010, Growing older-Adding life to years. 2010.

11. Canadian Institute for Health Information. Seniors and the Health Care System : What Is the Impact of Multiple Chronic Conditions ? 2011.

12. Franse L V., Di Bari M, Shorr RI, Resnick HE, Van Eijk JTM, Bauer DC, et al. Type 2 Diabetes in Older Well- Functioning People : Who Is Undiagnosed? Diabetes Care. 2001;24(12):2065–70.

13. Leong A, Dasgupta K, Chiasson J-L, Rahme E. Estimating the population prevalence of diagnosed and undiagnosed diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2013 Oct;36(10):3002–8.

14. McBrien KA, Manns BJ, Chui B, Klarenbach SW, Rabi D, Ravani P, et al. Health care costs in people with diabetes and their association with glycemic control and kidney function. Diabetes Care. 2013 May;36(5):1172–80.

15. Alzheimer Society of Canada. Rising Tide : The Impact of Dementia on Canadian Society. 2010.

Page 105: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

93

16. Herrmann N, Derrick TY, Balshaw R, Sambrook R, Lesnikova N, Lanctot KL. The Relation Between Disease Severity and Cost of Caring for Patients With Alzheimer Disease in C ... Can J Psychiatry. 2010;55(12):768–75.

17. World Health Organization. World Health Statistics. Geneva, Switzerland; 2014.

18. Gershon AS, Warner L, Cascagnette P, Victor JC, To T. Lifetime risk of developing chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: a longitudinal population study. Lancet. 2011 Sep 10;378(9795):991–6.

19. World Health Organization. World Health Statistics 2008. Geneva, Switzerland; 2008.

20. Maleki-Yazdi RM, Kelly SM, Lam SS, Marin M, Barbeau M, Walker V. The burden of illness in patients with moderate to severe chronic obstructive pulmonary disease in Canada. Can Respir J. 2012;19(5):319–24.

21. Canadian Cancer Society’s Advisory Committee on Cancer Statistics. Canadian Cancer Statistics 2015. Toronto; 2015.

22. Public Health Agency of Canada. Economic burden of illness in Canada 2005-2008. Government of Canada, Ottawa, Canada. Ottawa, Ontario; 2014.

23. Statistics Canada. The 10 leading causes of death, 2011 [Internet]. 2014 [cited 2015 Jun 10]. Available from: http://www.statcan.gc.ca/pub/82-625-x/2014001/article/11896-eng.htm

24. Public Health Agency of Canada. Tracking Heart Disease and Stroke in Canada.

Page 106: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

94

Ottawa, Ontario; 2009.

25. Certified General Accountants Association of Canada. Shifting the Burden of Health Care Costs - Where to? Certified General Accountants Association of Canada; 2010.

26. Knickman JR, Snell EK. The 2030 problem: Caring for aging Baby Boomers. Health Serv Res. 2002 Aug;37(4):849–84.

27. Kurpas D, Mroczek B, Bielska D. The correlation between quality of life, acceptance of illness and health behaviors of advanced age patients. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2013;56(3):448–56.

28. Peel NM, McClure RJ, Bartlett HP. Behavioral determinants of healthy aging. Am J Prev Med. 2005;28(3):298–304.

29. Prus SG. Comparing social determinants of self-rated health across the United States and Canada. Soc Sci Med. 2011;73(1):50–9.

30. Denton M, Walters V. Gender differences in structural and behavioral determinants of health: an analysis of the social production of health. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48:1221–35.

31. Hodge AM, English DR, Giles GG, Flicker L. Social connectedness and predictors of successful ageing. Maturitas. 2013;75(4):361–6.

32. McKee KJ, Schüz B. Psychosocial factors in healthy ageing. Psychol Health. 2015;30(June 2015):607–26.

Page 107: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

95

33. Yates L, Djousse L, Kurth T. Exceptional Longevity in Men. Arch Intern Med. 2013;168(3):284–90.

34. Courneya KS, Hellsten L-AM. Personality correlates of exercise behavior, motives, barriers and preferences: An application of the five-factor model. Pers Individ Dif. 1998 May;24(5):625–33.

35. Vollrath M, Torgersen S. Who takes health risks? A probe into eight personality types. Pers Individ Dif. 2002 May;32(7):1185–97.

36. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Diagnostic Manual. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

37. McCrae RR, Costa PT. Personality trait structure as a human universal. Am Psychol. 1997;52(5):509–16.

38. Goldberg LR. The structure of phenotypic personality traits. Am Psychol. American Psychological Association; 1993;48(1):26–34.

39. Chapman BP, Hampson S, Clarkin J. Personality-informed interventions for healthy aging: conclusions from a National Institute on Aging work group. Dev Psychol. 2014;50(5):1426–41.

40. Bouchard TJ, Loehlin JC. Genes, evolution, and personality. Behav Genet. 2001 May;31(3):243–73.

Page 108: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

96

41. Balestri M, Calati R, Serretti A, De Ronchi D. Genetic modulation of personality traits: a systematic review of the literature. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2014 Jan;29(1):1–15.

42. Briley DA, Tucker-Drob EM. Genetic and Environmental Continuity in Personality Development : A Meta-Analysis. Psychol Bull. 2014;140(5):1303–31.

43. Barelds DPH, Luteijn F. Measuring personality: A comparison of three personality questionnaires in the Netherlands. Pers Individ Dif. 2002;33:499–510.

44. Aluja A, García Ó, García LF. Replicability of the three, four and five Zuckerman’s personality super-factors: Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis of the EPQ-RS, ZKPQ and NEO-PI-R. Pers Individ Dif. 2004;36:1093–108.

45. Saggino A. The Big Three or the Big Five? A replication study. Pers Individ Dif. 2000;28:879–86.

46. Zuckerman M, Kuhlman DM, Joireman J, Teta P, Et Al. A comparison of three structural models for personality: The Big Three, the Big Five, and the Alternative Five. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;65(4):757–68.

47. Chapman BP, Roberts B, Duberstein P. Personality and longevity: knowns, unknowns, and implications for public health and personalized medicine. J Aging Res. 2011;759170.

48. Laverdière O, Gamache D, Diguer L, Hébert E, Larochelle S, Descôteaux J. Personality organization, five-factor model, and mental health. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2007;195(10):819–29.

Page 109: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

97

49. Johnston RG, Brown AE. Maternal trait personality and childbirth: The role of extraversion and neuroticism. Midwifery. 2013;29(11):1244–50.

50. Löckenhoff CE, Sutin AR, Ferrucci L, Costa PT. Personality traits and subjective health in the later years: The association between NEO-PI-R and SF-36 in advanced age is influenced by health status. J Res Pers. 2008;42:1334–46.

51. White JD. Personality, temperament and ADHD: A review of the literature. Pers Individ Dif. 1999;27:589–98.

52. Bagby RM, Bindseil KD, Schuller DR, Rector NA, Young LT, Cooke RG, et al. Relationship between the five-factor model of personality and unipolar, bipolar and schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry Res. 1997;70:83–94.

53. Jerram KL, Coleman PG. The big five personality traits and reporting of health problems and health behaviour in old age. Br J Health Psychol. 1999;4:181–92.

54. Sutin AR, Terracciano A, Deiana B, Naitza S, Ferrucci L, Uda M, et al. High neuroticism and low conscientiousness are associated with interleukin-6. Psychol Med. 2010;40:1485–93.

55. Phillips AC, Carroll D, Burns VE, Drayson M. Neuroticism, cortisol reactivity, and antibody response to vaccination. Psychophysiology. 2005;42:232–8.

56. Bibbey A, Carroll D, Roseboom TJ, Phillips AC, de Rooij SR. Personality and physiological reactions to acute psychological stress. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;90(1):28–36.

Page 110: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

98

57. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D. Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2010;136(5):768–821.

58. Duberstein PR, Chapman BP, Tindle HA, Sink KM, Bamonti P, Robbins J, et al. Personality and risk for Alzheimer’s disease in adults 72 years of age and older: a 6-year follow-up. Psychol Aging. 2011;26(2):351–62.

59. Parker JDA, Majeski SA, Collin VT. ADHD symptoms and personality: Relationships with the five-factor model. Pers Individ Dif. 2004;36:977–87.

60. Bogg T, Roberts BW. Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: A meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychol Bull. 2004 Nov;130(6):887–919.

61. Chapman BP, van Wijngaarden E, Seplaki CL, Talbot N, Duberstein P, Moynihan J. Openness and conscientiousness predict 34-week patterns of Interleukin-6 in older persons. Brain Behav Immun. 2011;25(4):667–73.

62. Sutin AR, Terracciano A, Deiana B, Uda M, Schlessinger D, Lakatta EG, et al. Cholesterol, triglycerides, and the Five-Factor Model of personality. Biol Psychol. 2010;84(2):186–91.

63. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Four ways five factors are basic. Pers Individ Dif. 1992;13(6):653–65.

64. Aldwin CM, Spiro A, Levenson MR, Cupertino AP. Longitudinal findings from the normative aging study: III. Personality, individual health trajectories, and mortality. Psychol Aging. 2001 Sep;16(3):450–65.

Page 111: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

99

65. Bae HT, Sebastiani P, Sun JX, Andersen SL, Daw EW, Terracciano A, et al. Genome-wide association study of personality traits in the long life family study. Front Genet. 2013 Jan;4(May):65.

66. Pilia G, Chen W-M, Scuteri A, Orru M, Albai G, Dei M, et al. Heritability of Cardiovascular and Personality Traits in 6,148 Sardinians. PLOS Genet. 2006;2(8):1207–23.

67. Givens JL, Frederick M, Silverman L, Anderson S, Senville J, Silver M, et al. Personality traits of centenarians’ offspring. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57:683–5.

68. Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(1):1–25.

69. McCrae RR, Costa PT, Pedroso de Lima M, Simões a, Ostendorf F, Angleitner a, et al. Age differences in personality across the adult life span: parallels in five cultures. Dev Psychol. 1999;35(2):466–77.

70. Roberts BW, Del Vecchio WF. The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2000;126(1):3–25.

71. McCrae RR, Costa PT, Ostendorf F, Angleitner a, Hrebícková M, Avia MD, et al. Nature over nurture: temperament, personality, and life span development. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78(1):173–86.

72. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Age changes in personality and their origins: comment on Roberts, Walton, and Viechtbauer (2006). Psychol Bull. 2006;132(1):26–8.

Page 112: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

100

73. Roberts BW, Jackson JJ. Sociogenomic Personality Psychology. J Pers. 2008;76(6):1523–44.

74. Deaux K. Sex and Gender. Annu Rev Psychol. 1985;36:49–81.

75. Chapman BP, Duberstein PR, Sörensen S, Lyness JM. Gender differences in Five Factor Model personality traits in an elderly cohort. Pers Individ Dif. 2007;43:1594–603.

76. Goodwin RD, Gotlib IH. Gender differences in depression: The role of personality factors. Psychiatry Res. 2004;126:135–42.

77. Feingold A. Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 1994;116(3):429–56.

78. Costa PT, Terracciano a, McCrae RR. Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: robust and surprising findings. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;81(2):322–31.

79. Terracciano A, Löckenhoff CE, Zonderman AB, Ferrucci L, Costa PT. Personality Predictors of Longevity: Activity, Emotional Stability, and Conscientiousness. Psychosom Med. 2008;70(6):621–7.

80. Iwasa H, Masui Y, Gondo Y, Inagaki H, Kawaai C, Suzuki T. Personality and all-cause mortality among older adults dwelling in a Japanese community: a five-year population-based prospective cohort study. Am J Geriatr psychiatry. 2008 May;16(5):399–405.

Page 113: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

101

81. Fry PS, Debats DL. Perfectionism and the Five-factor Personality Traits as Predictors of Mortality in Older Adults. J Health Psychol. 2009 May;14(4):513–24.

82. Wilson RS, Mendes de Leon CF, Bienias JL, Evans D a, Bennett D a. Personality and mortality in old age. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2004;59(3):P110–6.

83. Friedman HS, Tucker JS, Schwartz JE, Martin LR, Tomlinson-Keasey C, Wingard DL, et al. Childhood conscientiousness and longevity: health behaviors and cause of death. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;68(4):696–703.

84. Andersen SL, Sun JX, Sebastiani P, Huntly J, Gass JD, Feldman L, et al. Personality Factors in the Long Life Family Study. Journals Gerontol Ser B-Psychological Sci Soc Sci. 2013;68(5):739–49.

85. Shipley BA, Weiss A, Der G, Taylor MD, Deary IJ. Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Mortality in the UK Health and Lifestyle Survey: A 21-Year Prospective Cohort Study. Psychosom Med. 2007;69(9):923–31.

86. Duberstein PR, Sörensen S, Lyness JM, King DA, Conwell Y, Seidlitz L, et al. Personality is associated with perceived health and functional status in older primary care patients. Psychol Aging. 2003;18(1):25–37.

87. Ebstein RP. The molecular genetic architecture of human personality: beyond self-report questionnaires. Mol Psychiatry. 2006;11:427–45.

88. Ashton MC, Lee K. Honesty-Humility, the Big Five, and the Five-Factor Model. J Pers. 2005;73(October 2005):1321–54.

Page 114: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

102

89. Lee K, Ashton MC, Wiltshire J, Bourdage JS, Visser BA, Gallucci A. Sex, Power, and Money: Prediction from the Dark Triad and Honesty-Humility. Eur J Pers. 2013;27:169–84.

90. Wasylkiw L, Fekken GC. Personality and self-reported health: Matching predictors and criteria. Pers Individ Dif. 2002;33:607–20.

91. Kinnunen ML, Metsäpelto RL, Feldt T, Kokko K, Tolvanen A, Kinnunen U, et al. Personality profiles and health: Longitudinal evidence among Finnish adults. Scand J Psychol. 2012;53:512–22.

92. Roberts BW, Kuncel NR, Shiner R, Caspi A, Goldberg LR. the Power of Personality: A comparative analysis of the predictive validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2007;2(4):313–45.

93. Weiss A, Costa PT. Domain and facet personality predictors of all-cause mortality among Medicare patients aged 65 to 100. Psychosom Med. 2005;67(14):724–33.

94. Flynn KE, Smith MA. Personality and health care decision-making style. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2007;62(5):P261–7.

95. Booth-Kewley S, Vickers RR. Associations between major domains of personality and health behavior. J Pers. 1994;62(September):281–98.

96. Solberg Nes L, Segerstrom SC. Dispositional Optimism and Coping : A Meta-Analytic Review. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2006;10(3):235–51.

Page 115: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

103

97. Kaasinen V, Maguire RP, Kurki T, Brück a., Rinne JO. Mapping brain structure and personality in late adulthood. Neuroimage. 2005;24:315–22.

98. Lemaître H, Crivello F, Grassiot B, Alpérovitch A, Tzourio C, Mazoyer B. Age- and sex-related effects on the neuroanatomy of healthy elderly. Neuroimage. 2005;26:900–11.

99. Munro C a, Winicki JM, Schretlen DJ, Gower EW, Turano K a, Muñoz B, et al. Sex differences in cognition in healthy elderly individuals. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2012;19(June 2015):759–68.

100. Kapogiannis D, Sutin A, Davatzikos C, Costa P, Resnick S. The five factors of personality and regional cortical variability in the baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34(March 2012):2829–40.

101. DeYoung CG, Hirsh JB, Shane MS, Papademetris X, Rajeevan N, Gray JR. Testing Predictions From Personality Neuroscience: Brain Structure and the Big Five. Psychol Sci. 2010 Jun;21(6):820–8.

102. Deckersbach T, Miller K, Klibanski A, Fischman A, Dougherty DD, Blais MA, et al. Regional cerebral brain metabolism correlates of neuroticism and extraversion. Depress Anxiety. 2006;23:133–8.

103. Kim SH, Hwang JH, Park HS, Kim SE. Resting brain metabolic correlates of neuroticism and extraversion in young men. Neuroreport. 2008;19(8):883–6.

104. Mars RB, Grol MJ. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, working memory, and prospective coding for action. J Neurosci. 2007;27(8):1801–2.

Page 116: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

104

105. Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET. The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: Evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Prog Neurobiol. 2004;72:341–72.

106. Seger CA. The basal ganglia in human learning. Neurosci. 2006;12(4):285–90.

107. Vartanian O, Goel V, Lam E, Fisher M, Granic J. Middle temporal gyrus encodes individual differences in perceived facial attractiveness. Psychol Aesthetics, Creat Arts. 2013;7(1):38–47.

108. Bjørnebekk A, Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Grydeland H, Torgersen S, Westlye LT. Neuronal correlates of the five factor model (FFM) of human personality: Multimodal imaging in a large healthy sample. Neuroimage. 2013;65:194–208.

109. Blankstein U, Chen JYW, Mincic AM, McGrath PA, Davis KD. The complex minds of teenagers: Neuroanatomy of personality differs between sexes. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:599–603.

110. Haas BW, Omura K, Constable RT, Canli T. Emotional conflict and neuroticism: personality-dependent activation in the amygdala and subgenual anterior cingulate. Behav Neurosci. 2007;121(2):249–56.

111. Allman JM, Hakeem a, Erwin JM, Nimchinsky E, Hof P. The anterior cingulate cortex. The evolution of an interface between emotion and cognition. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2001;935:107–17.

112. Cohen MX, Jan-Christoph, Schoene-Bake, Elger CE, Weber B. Connectivity-based segregation of the human striatum predicts personality characteristics. Neuroforum. 2009;15(1):29–30.

Page 117: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

105

113. Ridderinkhof KR, Van Den Wildenberg WPM, Segalowitz SJ, Carter CS. Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain Cogn. 2004;56:129–40.

114. Vink M, Kleerekooper I, van den Wildenberg WPM, Kahn RS. Impact of aging on frontostriatal reward processing. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:2305–17.

115. Spanagel R, Weiss F. The dopamine hypothesis of reward: Past and current status. Trends Neurosci. 1999;22:521–7.

116. Williams LM, Gordon E. Dynamic Organization of the Emotional Brain: Responsivity, Stability, and Instability. Neurosci. 2007;13(4):349–70.

117. Knutson B, Fong GW, Adams CM, Varner JL, Hommer D. Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI. Neuroreport. 2001;12(17):3683–7.

118. Hooker CI, Verosky SC, Miyakawa A, Knight RT, D’Esposito M. The influence of personality on neural mechanisms of observational fear and reward learning. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:2709–24.

119. Wacker J, Chavanon M-L, Stemmler G. Investigating the dopaminergic basis of extraversion in humans: A multilevel approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006;91(1):171–87.

120. Jackson J, Balota DA, Head D. Exploring the relationship between personality and regional brain volume in healthy aging. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32(12):2162–71.

Page 118: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

106

121. Schilling C, Kühn S, Romanowski A, Schubert F, Kathmann N, Gallinat J. Cortical thickness correlates with impulsiveness in healthy adults. Neuroimage. 2012;59(1):824–30.

122. Wright CI, Feczko E, Dickerson B, Williams D. Neuroanatomical correlates of personality in the elderly. Neuroimage. 2007;35(1):263–72.

123. Knutson B, Momenan R, Rawlings RR, Fong GW, Hommer D. Negative association of neuroticism with brain volume ratio in healthy humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:685–90.

124. Xu J, Potenza MN. White matter integrity and five-factor personality measures in healthy adults. Neuroimage. 2012;59(1):800–7.

125. Ormel J, Bastiaansen A, Riese H, Bos EH, Servaas M, Ellenbogen M, et al. The biological and psychological basis of neuroticism: Current status and future directions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(1):59–72.

126. Harenski CL, Kim SH, Hamann S. Neuroticism and psychopathy predict brain activation during moral and nonmoral emotion regulation. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2009;9(1):1–15.

127. Feinstein JS, Stein MB, Paulus MP. Anterior insula reactivity during certain decisions is associated with neuroticism. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2006;1:136–42.

128. Brown SM, Hariri AR. Neuroimaging studies of serotonin gene polymorphisms: exploring the interplay of genes, brain, and behavior. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2006;6:44–52.

Page 119: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

107

129. Koechlin E, Hyafil A. Anterior prefrontal function and the limits of human decision-making. Science (80- ). 2007;318:594–8.

130. Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Fox PT, Bzdok D, Hensel L. Functional Segregation of the Human Dorsomedial Prefrontal Cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2014;1–18.

131. Chouinard PA, Paus T. The primary motor and premotor areas of the human cerebral cortex. Neurosci. 2006;12(2):143–52.

132. Kotyuk E, Duchek J, Head D, Szekely A, Goate AM, Balota D a. A genetic variant (COMT) coding dopaminergic activity predicts personality traits in healthy elderly. Pers Individ Dif. 2015 Aug 1;82:61–6.

133. Petrides M. Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2005;360(April):781–95.

134. Bigler ED, Mortensen S, Neeley ES, Ozonoff S, Krasny L, Johnson M, et al. Superior temporal gyrus, language function, and autism. Dev Neuropsychol. 2007;31(September 2015):217–38.

135. Silani G, Lamm C, Ruff CC, Singer T. Right Supramarginal Gyrus Is Crucial to Overcome Emotional Egocentricity Bias in Social Judgments. J Neurosci. 2013;33(39):15466–76.

136. Rankin KP, Rosen HJ, Kramer JH, Schauer GF, Weiner MW, Schuff N, et al. Right and Left Medial Orbitofrontal Volumes Show an Opposite Relationship to Agreeableness in FTD. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2004;17(4):328–32.

Page 120: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

108

137. Habas C, Guillevin R, Abanou A. Functional connectivity of the superior human temporal sulcus in the brain resting state at 3T. Neuroradiology. 2011;53:129–40.

138. DeYoung CG, Peterson JB, Higgins DM. Sources of Openness/Intellect: Cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. J Pers. 2005;73(August 2005):825–58.

139. De Souza LC, Volle E, Bertoux M, Czernecki V, Funkiewiez A, Allali G, et al. Poor creativity in frontotemporal dementia: A window into the neural bases of the creative mind. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(13):3733–42.

140. Davidson PSR, Anaki D, Ciaramelli E, Cohn M, Kim a. SN, Murphy KJ, et al. Does lateral parietal cortex support episodic memory? Evidence from focal lesion patients. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(7):1743–55.

141. Schildkraut JJ. The Catecholamine Hypothesis of Affective-Disorders - a Review of Supporting Evidence. Am J Psychiatry. 1965;122(5):509–22.

142. Coppen A. The Biochemistry of Affective Disorders. Br J Psychiatry. 1967;113(504):1237–64.

143. Panksepp J. The Neurochemistry of Behavior. Annu Rev Psychol. Annual Reviews; 1986;37(1):77–107.

144. Podell JE, Sambataro F, Murty VP, Emery MR, Tong Y, Das S, et al. Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in working memory updating. Neuroimage. 2012;62(3):2151–60.

Page 121: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

109

145. Crossman AR, Neary D. Neuroanatomy: An Illustrated Colour Text. In: Neuroanatomy: An Illustrated Colour Text. 4th ed. Elsevier Limited; 2010.

146. Pełka-Wysiecka J, Ziętek J, Grzywacz A, Kucharska-Mazur J, Bienkowski P, Samochowiec J. Association of genetic polymorphisms with personality profile in individuals without psychiatric disorders. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry. 2012 Jan;39(1):40–6.

147. Arnsten AF, Li B-M. Neurobiology of executive functions: Catecholamine influences on prefrontal cortical functions. Biol Psychiatry. 2005 Nov;57(11):1377–84.

148. Jacobs BL, Azmitia EC. Structure and function of the brain serotonin system. Physiol Rev. 1992 Jan;72(1):165–229.

149. Carver CS, Miller CJ. Relations of serotonin function to personality: current views and a key methodological issue. Psychiatry Res. 2006 Sep;144(1):1–15.

150. Peters R. Ageing and the brain. Postgrad Med J. 2006 Feb;82(964):84–8.

151. National Human Genome Research Institute. The Human Genome Project Completion: Frequently Asked Questions [Internet]. 2010 [cited 2015 Sep 21]. Available from: https://www.genome.gov/11006943

152. Nussbaum RL, McInnes RR, Willard HF. Thompson & Thompson Genetics in Medicine. 6th ed. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company; 2001.

153. Little PFR. Structure and function of the human genome. Genome Res. 2005;15:1759–66.

Page 122: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

110

154. Tautz D, Renz M. Simple sequences are ubiquitous repetitive components of eukaryotic genomes. Nucleic Acids Res. 1984;12(10):4127–38.

155. Li YC, Korol AB, Fahima T, Nevo E. Microsatellites within genes: Structure, function, and evolution. Mol Biol Evol. 2004;21(6):991–1007.

156. Fondon JW, Hammock EAD, Hannan AJ, King DG. Simple sequence repeats: genetic modulators of brain function and behavior. Trends Neurosci. 2008 Jul;31(7):328–34.

157. Kashi Y, King DG. Simple sequence repeats as advantageous mutators in evolution. Trends Genet. 2006;22(5):253–9.

158. King DG, Soller M, Kashi Y. Evolutionary tuning knobs. Endeavour. 1997;21(l):36–40.

159. Wren JD, Forgacs E, Fondon JW, Pertsemlidis a, Cheng SY, Gallardo T, et al. Repeat polymorphisms within gene regions: phenotypic and evolutionary implications. Am J Hum Genet. 2000 Aug;67(2):345–56.

160. Klenova E, Scott AC, Roberts J, Shamsuddin S, Lovejoy E a, Bergmann S, et al. YB-1 and CTCF differentially regulate the 5-HTT polymorphic intron 2 enhancer which predisposes to a variety of neurological disorders. J Neurosci. 2004;24(26):5966–73.

161. MacDonald GH, Itoh-Lindstrom Y, Ting JPY. The transcriptional regulatory protein, YB-1, promotes single-stranded regions in the DRA promoter. J Biol Chem. 1995;270(8):3527–33.

Page 123: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

111

162. Sirand-Pugnet P, Durosay P, Brody E, Marie J. An intronic (A/U)GGG repeat enhances the splicing of an alternative intron of the chicken beta-tropomyosin pre-mRNA. Nucleic Acids Res. 1995;23(17):3501–7.

163. Zhang M, Ou H, Shen YH, Wang J, Wang J, Coselli J, et al. Regulation of endothelial nitric oxide synthase by small RNA. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102(47):16967–72.

164. Babushkina NP, Kucher AN. Functional role of VNTR polymorphism of human genes. Russ J Genet. 2011;47(6):637–45.

165. McKusick VA, Kniffin CL. Cathechol-O-Methyltransferase; COMT [Internet]. Online Mendelian Inheritance in Man. 2014. Available from: http://www.omim.org/entry/116790

166. Tenhunen J, Salminen M, Lundstrom K, Savolainen R, Ulmanen I. Genomic organization of the human catechol O-methyltransferase gene and its expression from two distinct promoters. Eur J Biochem. 1994;223:1049–59.

167. Axelrod J, Tomchick R. Enzymatic O-Methylation of Epinephrine and Other Catechols. J Biol Chem. 1958;233(3):702–5.

168. Jeffery DR, Roth JA. Characterization of membrane-bound and soluble catechol-O-methyltransferase from human frontal cortex. J Neurochem. 1984;42:826–32.

169. Lotta T, Vidgren J, Tilgmann C, Ulmanen I, Melen K, Julkunen I, et al. Kinetics of human soluble and membrane-bound catechol O- methyltransferase: A revised mechanism and description of the thermolabile variant of the enzyme. Biochemistry. 1995;34(13):4202–10.

Page 124: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

112

170. Matsumoto M, Weickert CS, Beltaifa S, Kolachana B, Chen J, Hyde TM, et al. Catechol O-methyltransferase (COMT) mRNA expression in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of patients with schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28(8):1521–30.

171. Rivett JA, Eddy BJ, Roth JA. Contribution of sulfate conjugation, deamination, and O-methylation to metabolism of dopamine and norepinephrine in human brain. J Neurochem. 1982;39:1009–16.

172. Matsumoto M, Shannon Weickert C, Akil M, Lipska BK, Hyde TM, Herman MM, et al. Catechol O-methyltransferase mRNA expression in human and rat brain: evidence for a role in cortical neuronal function. Neuroscience. 2003;116:127–37.

173. Chen J, Lipska BK, Halim N, Ma QD, Matsumoto M, Melhem S, et al. Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am J Hum Genet. 2004;75(5):807–21.

174. Malhotra AK, Kestler LJ, Mazzanti C, Bates J a., Goldberg T, Goldman D. A functional polymorphism in the COMT gene and performance on a test of prefrontal cognition. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(4):652–4.

175. Rosa A, Peralta V, Cuesta MJ, Zarzuela A, Serrano F, Martínez-Larrea A, et al. New evidence of association between COMT gene and prefrontal neurocognitive function in healthy individuals from sibling pairs discordant for psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(June):1110–2.

176. Gelernter J, Kennedy JL, Van Tol HHM, Civelli O, Kidd KK. The D4 dopamine receptor (DRD4) maps to distal 11p close to HRAS. Genomics. 1992;13(1):208–10.

Page 125: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

113

177. Van Tol HH, Bunzow JR, Guan HC, Sunahara RK, Seeman P, Niznik HB, et al. Cloning of the gene for a human dopamine D4 receptor with high affinity for the antipsychotic clozapine. Nature. 1991;350(6319):610–4.

178. Asghari V, Sanyal S, Buchwaldt S, Paterson a, Jovanovic V, Van Tol HH. Modulation of intracellular cyclic AMP levels by different human dopamine D4 receptor variants. J Neurochem. 1995;65:1157–65.

179. Simpson J, Vetuz G, Wilson M, Brookes KJ, Kent L. The DRD4 Receptor Exon 3 VNTR and 5 ’ SNP Variants and mRNA Expression in Human Post-Mortem Brain Tissue. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2010;153B(6):1228–33.

180. Van Tol HH, Wu CM, Guan HC, Ohara K, Bunzow JR, Civelli O, et al. Multiple dopamine D4 receptor variants in the human population. Nature. 1992;358:149–52.

181. Chang F-M, Kidd JR, Livak KJ, Pakstis AJ, Kidd KK. The world-wide distribution of allele frequencies at the human dopamine D4 receptor locus. Hum Genet. 1996;98:91–101.

182. Ding Y-C, Chi H-C, Grady DL, Morishima A, Kidd JR, Kidd KK, et al. Evidence of positive selection acting at the human dopamine receptor D4 gene locus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99(May):309–14.

183. Schoots O, Van Tol HHM. The human dopamine D4 receptor repeat sequences modulate expression. Pharmacogenomics J. 2003;3(6):343–8.

184. Van Craenenbroeck K, Clark SD, Cox MJ, Oak JN, Liu F, Van Tol HHM. Folding efficiency is rate-limiting in dopamine D4 receptor biogenesis. J Biol Chem. 2005;280(19):19350–7.

Page 126: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

114

185. Grimsby J, Chen K, Wang LJ, Lan NC, Shih JC. Human monoamine oxidase A and B genes exhibit identical exon-intron organization. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1991;88(May):3637–41.

186. Shih JC, Grimsby J, Chen K, Zhu QS. Structure and promoter organization of the human monoamine oxidase A and B genes. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 1993;18(1):25–32.

187. Ferreira AR, Machado GM, Diesel TO, Carvalho JO, Rumpf R, Melo EO, et al. Allele-specific expression of the MAOA gene and X chromosome inactivation in in vitro produced bovine embryos. Mol Reprod Dev. 2010;77:615–21.

188. Stabellini R, Moreira De Mello JC, Hernandes LM, Pereira L V. MAOA and GYG2 are submitted to X chromosome inactivation in human fibroblasts. Epigenetics. 2009;4(July 2015):388–93.

189. Benjamin D, Van Bakel I, Craig IW. A novel expression based approach for assessing the inactivation status of human X-linked genes. Eur J Hum Genet. 2000;8:103–8.

190. Shih JC, Chen K, Ridd MJ. Monoamine oxidase: from genes to behavior. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1999;22:197–217.

191. Sabol SZ, Hu S, Hamer D. A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter. Hum Genet. Springer-Verlag; 1998 Jan;103(3):273–9.

192. Buckholtz JW, Meyer-Lindenberg A. MAOA and the neurogenetic architecture of human aggression. Trends Neurosci. 2008 Mar;31(3):120–9.

Page 127: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

115

193. Denney RM, Koch H, Craig IW. Association between monoamine oxidase A activity in human male skin fibroblasts and genotype of the MAOA promoter-associated variable number tandem repeat. Hum Genet. 1999;105:542–51.

194. Wu Y-H, Fischer DF, Swaab DF. A promoter polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene is associated with the pineal MAOA activity in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Brain Res. 2007;1167:13–9.

195. Nishioka SA, Perin EA, Sampaio AS, Cordeiro Q, Cappi C, Mastrorosa RS, et al. The role of the VNTR functional polymorphism of the promoter region of the MAOA gene on psychiatric disorders. Rev Psiquiatr Clin. 2011;38:34–42.

196. Meyer J, Ginovart N, Boovariwala A, Sagrati S, Hussey D, Garcia A, et al. Elevated monoamine oxidase A levels in the brain. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:1209–16.

197. Dannlowski U, Ohrmann P, Konrad C, Domschke K, Bauer J, Kugel H, et al. Reduced amygdala-prefrontal coupling in major depression: association with MAOA genotype and illness severity. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2009;12:11–22.

198. Huang Y-Y, Cate SP, Battistuzzi C, Oquendo M a, Brent D, Mann JJ. An association between a functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase a gene promoter, impulsive traits and early abuse experiences. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1498–505.

199. Brunner HG, Nelen M, Breakefield XO, Ropers HH, van Oost B a. Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science. 1993;262(5133):578–80.

Page 128: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

116

200. Lesch KP, Balling U, Gross J, Strauss K, Wolozin BL, Murphy DL, et al. Organization of the human serotonin transporter gene. J Neural Transm Gen Sect. 1994;95:157–62.

201. Ramamoorthy S, Bauman AL, Moore KR, Han H, Yang-Feng T, Chang AS, et al. Antidepressant- and cocaine-sensitive human serotonin transporter: molecular cloning, expression, and chromosomal localization. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1993;90:2542–6.

202. Jonassen R, Landrø NI. Serotonin transporter polymorphisms (5-HTTLPR) in emotion processing: implications from current neurobiology. Prog Neurobiol. 2014 Jun;117:41–53.

203. Pauwels PJ. 5-HT Receptors and Their Ligands. Tocris Reviews. Bristol, UK; 2003.

204. Lesch KP, Gutknecht L. Pharmacogenetics of the serotonin transporter. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry. 2005 Jul;29(6):1062–73.

205. McEwen BS. Invited review: Estrogens effects on the brain: multiple sites and molecular mechanisms. J Appl Physiol. 2001;91(9):2785–801.

206. Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils a, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, et al. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274(5292):1527–31.

207. Paaver M, Kurrikoff T, Nordquist N, Oreland L, Harro J. The effect of 5-HTT gene promoter polymorphism on impulsivity depends on family relations in girls. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2008 Jul;32(5):1263–8.

Page 129: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

117

208. Bradley SL, Dodelzon K, Sandhu HK, Philibert R a. Relationship of serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms and haplotypes to mRNA transcription. Am J Med Genet - Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;136 B:58–61.

209. Heinz A, Braus DF, Smolka MN, Wrase J, Puls I, Hermann D, et al. Amygdala-prefrontal coupling depends on a genetic variation of the serotonin transporter. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(1):20–1.

210. Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Drabant EM, Verchinski B a, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, et al. 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(6):828–34.

211. Du L, Bakish D, Hrdina PD. Gender differences in association between serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and personality traits.Pdf. Psychiatr Genet. 2000;10:159–64.

212. Battersby S, Ogilvie AD, Smith CAD, Blackwood DH, Muir WJ, Quinn JP, et al. Structure of a variable number tandem repeat of the serotonin transporter gene and association with affective disorder. Psychiatr Genet. 1996;6:177–81.

213. Avula R, Rand a, Black JL, O’Kane DJ. Simultaneous genotyping of multiple polymorphisms in human serotonin transporter gene and detection of novel allelic variants. Transl Psychiatry. 2011;1(July):e32.

214. Eliseeva I a, Kim ER, Guryanov SG, Ovchinnikov LP, Lyabin DN. Y-Box-Binding Protein 1 and Its Functions. Biochemistry. 2011;76(13):1402–33.

215. Lovejoy E a., Scott a. C, Fiskerstrand CE, Bubb VJ, Quinn JP. The serotonin transporter intronic VNTR enhancer correlated with a predisposition to affective disorders has distinct regulatory elements within the domain based on the primary

Page 130: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

118

DNA sequence of the repeat unit. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17(August 2002):417–20.

216. Hranilovic D, Stefulj J, Schwab S, Borrmann-Hassenbach M, Albus M, Jernej B, et al. Serotonin transporter promoter and intron 2 polymorphisms: Relationship between allelic variants and gene expression. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:1090–4.

217. Ogilvie AD, Battersby S, Bubb VJ, Fink G, Harmar AJ, Goodwin GM, et al. Polymorphism in serotonin transporter gene associated with susceptibility to major depression. Lancet. 1996;347:731–3.

218. Fan JB, Sklar P. Meta-analysis reveals association between serotonin transporter gene STin2 VNTR polymorphism and schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:928–38, 891.

219. Kazantseva A V, Gaysina D a, Faskhutdinova GG, Noskova T, Malykh SB, Khusnutdinova EK. Polymorphisms of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR, A/G SNP in 5-HTTLPR, and STin2 VNTR) and their relation to personality traits in healthy individuals from Russia. Psychiatr Genet. 2008;18:167–76.

220. Ali FR, Vasiliou S a., Haddley K, Paredes UM, Roberts JC, Miyajima F, et al. Combinatorial interaction between two human serotonin transporter gene variable number tandem repeats and their regulation by CTCF. J Neurochem. 2010;112:296–306.

221. O’Malley KL, Rotwein P. Human tyrosine hydroxylase and insuline genes are contiguous on chromosome 11. Nucleic Acids Res. 1988;16(10):4437–46.

222. Kobayashi K, Kaneda N, Ichinose H, Kishi F, Nakazawa a, Kurosawa Y, et al. Structure of the human tyrosine hydroxylase gene: alternative splicing from a single gene accounts for generation of four mRNA types. J Biochem. 1988;103(6):907–12.

Page 131: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

119

223. Nagatsu T, Levitt M, Udenfriend S. Tyrosine Hydroxylase: The initial step in norepinephrine biosynthesis. J Biol Chem. 1964;239:2910–7.

224. Polymeropoulos MH, Xiao H, Rath DS, Merril CR. Tetranucleotide Repeat Polymorphism at the Human Tyrosine-Hydroxylase Gene (Th). Nucleic Acids Res. 1991;19(13):3753.

225. Puers C, Hammond HA, Jin L, Caskey CT, Schumm JW. Identification of repeat sequence heterogeneity at the polymorphic short tandem repeat locus HUMTH01[AATG]n and reassignment of alleles in population analysis by using a locus-specific allelic ladder. Am J Hum Genet. 1993;53:953–8.

226. Meloni R, Albanèse V, Ravassard P, Treilhou F, Mallet J. A tetranucleotide polymorphic microsatellite, located in the first intron of the tyrosine hydroxylase gene, acts as a transcription regulatory element in vitro. Hum Mol Genet. 1998;7(3):423–8.

227. Meloni R, Biguet NF, Mallet J. Post-Genomic Era and Gene Discovery for Psychiatric Diseases : There Is a New Art of the Trade ? Mol Neurobiol. 2002;26:389–403.

228. Zhang L, Rao F, Wessel J, Kennedy BP, Rana BK, Taupenot L, et al. Functional allelic heterogeneity and pleiotropy of a repeat polymorphism in tyrosine hydroxylase: prediction of catecholamines and response to stress in twins. Physiol Genomics. 2004;19(September 2004):277–91.

229. Albanèse V, Biguet NF, Kiefer H, Bayard E, Mallet J, Meloni R. Quantitative effects on gene silencing by allelic variation at a tetranucleotide microsatellite. Hum Mol Genet. 2001;10(17):1785–92.

Page 132: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

120

230. Wei J, Ramchand CN, Hemmings GP. Possible association of catecholamine turnover with the polymorphic (TCAT)n repeat in the first intron of the human tyrosine hydroxylase gene. Life Sci. 1997;61(14):1341–7.

231. Wei J, Ramchand CN, Hemmings GP. Association of polymorphic VNTR region in the first intron of the human TH gene with disturbances of the catecholamine pathway in schizophrenia.pdf. Psychiatr Genet. 1995;5:83–8.

232. Persson ML, Wasserman D, Geijer T, Jönsson EG, Terenius L. Tyrosine hydroxylase allelic distribution in suicide attempters. Psychiatry Res. 1997;72:73–80.

233. Anney RJ, Olsson C a, Lotfi-Miri M, Patton GC, Williamson R. Nicotine dependence in a prospective population-based study of adolescents: the protective role of a functional tyrosine hydroxylase polymorphism. Pharmacogenetics. 2004;14:73–81.

234. De Benedictis G, Carotenuto L, Carrieri G, De Luca M, Falcone E, Rose G, et al. Gene/longevity association studies at four autosomal loci (REN, THO, PARP, SOD2). Eur J Hum Genet. 1998;6:534–41.

235. Persson M-L, Wasserman D, Jonsson EG, Bergman H, Terenius L, Gyllander A, et al. Search for the influence of the tyrosine hydroxylase (TCAT)(n) repeat polymorphism on personality traits. Psychiatry Res. 2000 Jul;95(1):1–8.

236. Etter PD, Bassham S, Hohenlohe P a, Johnson E a, Cresko W a. Molecular Methods for Evolutionary Genetics. Mol Methods Evol Genet Methods Mol Biol vol 772. 2011;772(3):157–78.

237. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate : a Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B. 1995;57(1):289–300.

Page 133: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

121

238. Williams LM, Gatt JM, Kuan SA, Dobson-Stone C, Palmer DM, Paul RH, et al. A Polymorphism of the MAOA Gene is Associated with Emotional Brain Markers and Personality Traits on an Antisocial Index. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34(7):1797–809.

239. Beaver KM, Wright JP, DeLisi M, Vaughn MG. Dopaminergic Polymorphisms and Educational Achievement: Results From a Longitudinal Sample of Americans. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(4):932–8.

240. Murdoch JD, Speed WC, Pakstis AJ, Heffelfinger CE, Kidd KK. Worldwide Population Variation and Haplotype Analysis at the Serotonin Transporter Gene SLC6A4 and Implications for Association Studies. Biol Psychiatry. 2013 Dec;74(12):879–89.

241. Yang M, Kavi V, Wang W, Wu Z, Hao W. The association of 5-HTR2A-1438A/G, COMTVal158Met, MAOA-LPR, DATVNTR and 5-HTTVNTR gene polymorphisms and antisocial personality disorder in male heroin-dependent Chinese subjects. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry. 2012;36(2):282–9.

242. Lauretto MS, Nakano F, Faria SR, Pereira C a B, Stern JM. A straightforward multiallelic significance test for the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium law. Genet Mol Biol. 2009;32(3):619–25.

243. El Galta R, Hsu L, Houwing-Duistermaat JJ. Methods to test for association between a disease and a multi-allelic marker applied to a candidate region. BMC Genet. 2005;6 Suppl 1:S101.

244. Taylor MD, Whiteman MC, Fowkes GR, Lee AJ, Allerhand M, Deary IJ. Five Factor Model Personality Traits and All-Cause Mortality in the Edinburgh Artery Study Cohort. Psychosom Med. 2009;71(6):631–41.

Page 134: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

122

245. Wilson RS, Krueger KR, Gu LP, Bienias JL, De Leon CFM, Evans DA. Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in a defined population of older persons. Psychosom Med. 2005;67(6):841–5.

246. Martin P, da Rosa G, Siegler IC. Personality and longevity: findings from the Georgia Centenarian Study. Age (Omaha). 2006 Dec;28(4):343–52.

247. Masui Y, Gondo Y, Inagaki H, Hirose N. Do personality characteristics predict longevity? Findings from the Tokyo Centenarian Study. Age (Omaha). 2006;28(4):353–61.

248. Friedman HS, Kern ML, Reynolds CA. Personality and Health, Subjective Well-Being, and Longevity. J Pers. 2010 Feb;78(1):179–215.

249. Ni X, Bismil R, Chan K, Sicard T, Bulgin N, McMaiti S, et al. Serotonin 2A receptor gene is associated with personality traits, but not to disorder, in patients with borderline personality disorder. Neurosci Lett. 2006 Nov;408(3):214–9.

250. Schosser A, Fuchs K, Scharl T, Schloegelhofer M, Kindler J, Mossaheb N, et al. Interaction between serotonin 5-HT2A receptor gene and dopamine transporter (DAT1) gene polymorphisms influences personality trait of persistence in Austrian Caucasians. World J Biol Psychiatry. Informa Scandinavian; 2010;11(2):417–24.

251. Jokela M, Keltikangas-Järvinen L, Kivimäki M, Puttonen S, Elovainio M, Rontu R, et al. Serotonin receptor 2A gene and the influence of childhood maternal nurturance on adulthood depressive symptoms. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:356–60.

252. Stein MB, Chartier MJ, Kozak M V, King N, Kennedy JL. Genetic linkage to the

Page 135: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

123

serotonin transporter protein and 5HT(2A) receptor genes excluded in generalized social phobia. Psychiatry Res. 1998 Dec;81(3):283–91.

253. Nomura M, Kusumi I, Kaneko M, Masui T, Daiguji M, Ueno T, et al. Involvement of a polymorphism in the 5-HT2A receptor gene in impulsive behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2006;187(1):30–5.

254. Nishiguchi N, Matsushita S, Suzuki K, Murayama M, Shirakawa O, Higuchi S. Association between 5HT2A receptor gene promoter region polymorphism and eating disorders in Japanese patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50(2):123–8.

255. Bobb AJ, Addington AM, Sidransky E, Gornick MC, Lerch JP, Greenstein DK, et al. Support for association between ADHD and two candidate genes: NET1 and DRD1. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2005 Apr;134B(1):67–72.

256. Park L, Nigg JT, Waldman ID, Nummy KA, Huang-Pollock C, Rappley M, et al. Association and linkage of alpha-2A adrenergic receptor gene polymorphisms with childhood ADHD. Mol Psychiatry. 2005 Jun;10(6):572–80.

257. Fulcutake M, Hishimoto A, Nishiguchi N, Nushida H, Ueno Y, Shirakawa O, et al. Association of alpha(2A)-adrenergic receptor gene polymorphism with susceptibility to suicide in Japanese females. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2008 Aug;32(6):1428–33.

258. Comings DE, Gonzalez NS, Li C, MacMurray J. A “line item” approach to the identification of genes involved in polygenic behavioral disorders: The adrenergic alpha(2A) (ADRA2A) gene. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2003 Apr;118B(1):110–4.

259. Silva de Cerqueira CC, Polina ER, Contini V, Coelho Marques FZ, Grevet EH, Iglesias Salgado CA, et al. ADRA2A polymorphisms and ADHD in adults: Possible

Page 136: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

124

mediating effect of personality. Psychiatry Res. 2011 Apr;186(2-3):345–50.

260. Guan L, Wang B, Chen Y, Yang L, Li J, Qian Q, et al. A high-density single-nucleotide polymorphism screen of 23 candidate genes in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: suggesting multiple susceptibility genes among Chinese Han population. Mol Psychiatry. Nature Publishing Group; 2009 May 8;14(5):546–54.

261. Clarke T-K, Dempster E, Docherty SJ, Desrivieres S, Lourdsamy A, Wodarz N, et al. Multiple polymorphisms in genes of the adrenergic stress system confer vulnerability to alcohol abuse. Addict Biol. 2012;17(1):202–8.

262. Ma X, Sun J, Yao J, Wang Q, Hu X, Deng W, et al. A quantitative association study between schizotypal traits and COMT, PRODH and BDNF genes in a healthy Chinese population. Psychiatry Res. 2007;153(1):7–15.

263. Tsai SJ, Liao DL, Yu YWY, Chen TJ, Wu HC, Lin CH, et al. A study of the association of (Val66Met) polymorphism in the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene with alcohol dependence an extreme violence in Chinese males. Neurosci Lett. 2005;381(3):340–3.

264. Chang Y-H, Lee S-Y, Chen S-L, Tzeng N-S, Wang T-Y, Lee IH, et al. Genetic variants of the BDNF and DRD3 genes in bipolar disorder comorbid with anxiety disorder. J Affect Disord. 2013 Dec;151(3):967–72.

265. Ozan E, Okur H, Eker C, Eker OD, Gonul AS, Akarsu N. The effect of depression, BDNF gene val66met polymorphism and gender on serum BDNF levels. Brain Res Bull. 2010 Jan;81(1):61–5.

266. Frustaci A, Pozzi G, Gianfagna F, Manzoli L, Boccia S. Meta-analysis of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene (BDNF) Val66Met polymorphism in anxiety disorders and anxiety-related personality traits. Neuropsychobiology. 2008;58(3-

Page 137: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

125

4):163–70.

267. Arias B, Aguilera M, Moya J, Saiz PA, Villa H, Ibanez MI, et al. The role of genetic variability in the SLC6A4, BDNF and GABRA6 genes in anxiety-related traits. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2012 Mar;125(3):194–202.

268. Terracciano A, Tanaka T, Sutin AR, Deiana B, Balaci L, Sanna S, et al. BDNF Val66Met is Associated with Introversion and Interacts with 5-HTTLPR to Influence Neuroticism. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010 Apr;35(5):1083–9.

269. Hiio K, Merenaekk L, Nordquist N, Parik J, Oreland L, Veidebaum T, et al. Effects of serotonin transporter promoter and BDNF Val66Met genotype on personality traits in a population representative sample of adolescents. Psychiatr Genet. 2011 Oct;21(5):261–4.

270. Hünnerkopf R, Strobel A, Gutknecht L, Brocke B, Lesch KP. Interaction between BDNF Val66Met and dopamine transporter gene variation influences anxiety-related traits. Neuropsychopharmacology. Nature Publishing Group; 2007 Dec 28;32(12):2552–60.

271. Lee H-J, Kang R-H, Lim S-W, Paik J-W, Choi M-J, Lee M-S. No association between the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene Val66Met polymorphism and post-traumatic stress disorder. Stress Heal. 2006 Apr;22(2):115–9.

272. Terracciano A, Piras MG, Lobina M, Mulas A, Meirelles O, Sutin AR, et al. Genetics of serum BDNF: Meta-analysis of the Val66Met and genome-wide association study. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2013 Dec;14(8):583–9.

273. Shih P, Er T, Chang J. An association study between genetic variants at mu-opioid receptor , dopamine transporter , catechol- O-methyltransferase , and dopamine genes and risk of Parkinson ’ s disease. 2013;18(2):279–87.

Page 138: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

126

274. Retz W, Roesler M, Kissling C, Wiemann S, Huennerkopf R, Coogan A, et al. Norepinephrine transporter and catecholamine-O-methyltransferase gene variants and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in adults. J Neural Transm. 2008 Feb;115(2):323–9.

275. Bosker FJ, Hartman C a, Nolte IM, Prins BP, Terpstra P, Posthuma D, et al. Poor replication of candidate genes for major depressive disorder using genome-wide association data. Mol Psychiatry. 2011 May;16(5):516–32.

276. Klengel T, Heck a., Pfister H, Brückl T, Hennings JM, Menke a., et al. Somatization in major depression - clinical features and genetic associations. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2011;124(1):317–28.

277. Proitsi P, Lupton MK, Reeves SJ, Hamilton G, Archer N, Martin BM, et al. Association of serotonin and dopamine gene pathways with behavioral subphenotypes in dementia. Neurobiol Aging. 2012 Apr;33(4):791–803.

278. Marco-Pallarés J, Cucurell D, Cunillera T, Krämer UM, Càmara E, Nager W, et al. Genetic Variability in the Dopamine System (Dopamine Receptor D4, Catechol-O-Methyltransferase) Modulates Neurophysiological Responses to Gains and Losses. Biol Psychiatry. 2009 Jul;66(2):154–61.

279. Krämer UM, Cunillera T, Càmara E, Marco-Pallarés J, Cucurell D, Nager W, et al. The Impact of Catechol-O-Methyltransferase and Dopamine D4 Receptor Genotypes on Neurophysiological Markers of Performance Monitoring. J Neurosci. 2007;27(51):14190–8.

280. Jagannathan K, Calhoun VD, Gelernter J, Stevens MC, Liu J, Bolognani F, et al. Genetic Associations of Brain Structural Networks in Schizophrenia: A Preliminary Study. Biol Psychiatry. 2010 Oct;68(7):657–66.

Page 139: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

127

281. Ettinger U, Joober R, De Guzman R, O’Driscoll GA. Schizotypy, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and dopamine genes. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006 Dec;60(6):764–7.

282. Tadic A, Victor A, Baskaya Ö, von Cube R, Hoch J, Kouti I, et al. Interaction Between Gene Variants of the Serotonin Transporter Promoter Region (5-HTTLPR) and Catechol O-Methyltransferase (COMT) in Borderline Personality Disorder. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2009 Jun;150B(4):487–95.

283. Calati R, Porcelli S, Giegling I, Hartmann AM, Möller H-J, De Ronchi D, et al. Catechol-o-methyltransferase gene modulation on suicidal behavior and personality traits: review, meta-analysis and association study. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45(3):309–21.

284. Hirata Y, Zai CC, Nowrouzi B, Beitchman JH, Kennedy JL. Study of the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) Gene with High Aggression in Children. Aggress Behav. 2013;39(July 2012):45–51.

285. Hallikainen T, Lachman H, Saito T, Volavka J, Kauhanen J, Salonen JT, et al. Lack of association between the functional variant of the catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) gene and early-onset alcoholism associated with severe antisocial behavior. Am J Med Genet. 2000 Jun;96(3):348–52.

286. Sengupta SM, Grizenko N, Schmitz, Norbert Schwartz G, Ben Amor L, Bellingham J, de Guzman, Rosherrie Polotskaia A, et al. COMT Val(108/118)Met gene variant, birth weight, and conduct disorder in children with ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006 Nov;45(11):1363–9.

287. Jugurnauth SK, Chen C-K, Barnes MR, Li T, Lin S-K, Liu H-C, et al. A COMT gene haplotype associated with methamphetamine abuse. Pharmacogenet Genomics. 2011;21(11):731–40.

Page 140: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

128

288. Montag C, Jurkiewicz M, Reuter M. The Role of the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) Gene in Personality and Related Psychopathological Disorders. Cns Neurol Disord Targets. 2012;11(3):236–50.

289. Samochowiec J, Hajduk A, Samochowiec A, Horodnicki J, Stepien G, Grzywacz A, et al. Association studies of MAO-A, COMT, and 5-HTT genes polymorphisms in patients with anxiety disorders of the phobic spectrum. Psychiatry Res. 2004 Aug;128(1):21–6.

290. Enoch M-A, Xu K, Ferro E, Harris CR, Goldman D. Genetic origins of anxiety in women: a role for a functional catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism. Psychiatr Genet. 2003;13:33–41.

291. Lo Bianco L, Blasi G, Taurisano P, Di Giorgio A, Ferrante F, Ursini G, et al. Interaction between catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val(158)Met genotype and genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia during explicit processing of aversive facial stimuli. Psychol Med. 2013 Feb;43(2):279–92.

292. Henderson AS, Korten AE, Jorm AF, Jacomb PA, Christensen H, Rodgers B, et al. COMT and DRD3 polymorphisms, environmental exposures, and personality traits related to common mental disorders. Am J Med Genet. 2000 Feb 7;96(1):102–7.

293. Mier D, Kirsch P, Meyer-Lindenberg A. Neural substrates of pleiotropic action of genetic variation in COMT: a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry. Nature Publishing Group; 2010 Sep;15(9):918–27.

294. Smyrnis N, Avramopoulos D, Evdokimidis I, Stefanis CN, Tsekou H, Stefanis NC. Effect of Schizotypy on Cognitive Performance and Its Tuning by COMT val158 Met Genotype Variations in a Large Population of Young Men. Biol Psychiatry. 2007 Jan;61(7):845–53.

Page 141: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

129

295. Silberschmidt AL, Sponheim SR. Personality in relation to genetic liability for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: Differential associations with the COMT Val(108/158) Met polymorphism. Schizophr Res. 2008 Mar;100(1-3):316–24.

296. Matthews N, Vance A, Cummins TDR, Wagner J, Connolly A, Yamada J, et al. The COMT Val158 allele is associated with impaired delayed-match-to- sample performance in ADHD. Behav Brain Funct. 2012 May;8:25.

297. Pazvantoğlu O, Güneş S, Karabekiroğlu K, Yeğin Z, Erenkuş Z, Akbaş S, et al. The relationship between the presence of ADHD and certain candidate gene polymorphisms in a Turkish sample. Gene. 2013 Oct;528:320–7.

298. Li T, Du J, Yu S, Jiang H, Fu Y, Wang D, et al. Pathways to Age of Onset of Heroin Use: A Structural Model Approach Exploring the Relationship of the COMT Gene, Impulsivity and Childhood Trauma. PLoS One. 2012;7(11):e48735–e48735.

299. Lin WY, Wu BT, Lee CC, Sheu JJ, Liu SH, Wang WF, et al. Association Analysis of Dopaminergic Gene Variants (Comt, Drd4 and Dat1) with Alzheimer’s Disease. J Biol Regul Homeost Agents. 2012;26(3):401–10.

300. Papenberg G, Bäckman L, Nagel IE, Nietfeld W, Schröder J, Bertram L, et al. Dopaminergic Gene Polymorphisms Affect Long-term Forgetting in Old Age: Further Support for the Magnification Hypothesis. MIT Press; 2013;25(4):579.

301. Konrad K, Dempfle A, Friedel S, Heiser P, Holtkamp K, Walitza S, et al. Familiality and Molecular Genetics of Attention Networks in ADHD. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2010;153B(1):148–58.

302. Mill J, Xu X, Ronald A, Curran S, Price T, Knight J, et al. Quantitative trait locus

Page 142: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

130

analysis of candidate gene alleles associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in five genes:DRD4, DAT1, DRD5, SNAP-25, and5HT1B. Am J Med Genet Part B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;133B:68–73.

303. Joyce PR, McHugh PC, Light KJ, Rowe S, Miller AL, Kennedy MA. Relationships Between Angry-Impulsive Personality Traits and Genetic Polymorphisms of the Dopamine Transporter. Interplay Neural Circuitry Reward Impuls Addict. 2009;66(8):717–21.

304. Reese J, Kraschewski A, Anghelescu I, Winterer G, Schmidt LG, Gallinat J, et al. Haplotypes of dopamine and serotonin transporter genes are associated with antisocial personality disorder in alcoholics. Psychiatr Genet. 2010;20(4):140–52.

305. Kazantseva A, Gaysina D, Malykh S, Khusnutdinova E. The role of dopamine transporter (SLC6A3) and dopamine D2 receptor/ankyrin repeat and kinase domain containing 1 (DRD2/ANKK1) gene polymorphisms in personality traits. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011 Jun;35(4):1033–40.

306. Anghelescu I, Klawe C, Singer P, Fehr C, Hiemke C, Quante A, et al. Low novelty seeking and high self directedness scores in alcohol-dependent patients without comorbid psychiatric disorders homozygous for the A10 allele of the dopamine transporter gene. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2010 Mar;11(2):382–9.

307. Gerra G, Garofano L, Pellegrini C, Bosari S, Zaimovic A, Moi G, et al. Allelic association of a dopamine transporter gene polymorphism with antisocial behaviour in heroin-dependent patients. Addict Biol. 2005;10(3):275–81.

308. Hoenicka J, Ponce G, JiméNez-Arriero M, Ampuero I, RodríGuez-Jiménez R, Rubio G, et al. Association in alcoholic patients between Psychopathic Traits and the additive effect of allelic forms of theCNR1 andFAAH endocannabinoid genes, and the 3â€2 Region of theDRD2 Gene. Neurotox Res. Springer-Verlag; 2007 Jan;11(1):51–9.

Page 143: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

131

309. Gorwood P, Limosin F, Batel P, Hamon M, Ades J, Boni C. The a9 allele of the dopamine transporter gene is associated with delirium tremens and alcohol-withdrawal seizure. Biol Psychiatry. 2003 Jan 1;53(1):85–92.

310. Huang C-C, Lu R-B, Shih M-C, Yen C-H, Huang S-Y. Association study of the dopamine transporter gene with personality traits and major depressive disorder in the Han Chinese population. Pharmacogenet Genomics. 2011;21(2):94–7.

311. Lasky-Su J, Lange C, Biederman J, Tsuang M, Doyle AE, Smoller JW, et al. Family-based association analysis of a statistically derived quantitative traits for ADHD reveal an association inDRD4 with inattentive symptoms in ADHD individuals. Am J Med Genet Part B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B(April 2007):100–6.

312. Savitz J, Hodgkinson CA, Martin-Soelch C, Shen P-H, Szczepanik J, Nugent AC, et al. DRD2/ANKK1 Taq1A polymorphism (rs1800497) has opposing effects on D2/3 receptor binding in healthy controls and patients with major depressive disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. Cambridge Journals Online; 2013;16(09):2095–101.

313. Wang T-Y, Lee S-Y, Chen S-L, Huang S-Y, Chang Y-H, Tzeng N-S, et al. Association between DRD2, 5-HTTLPR, and ALDH2 genes and specific personality traits in alcohol- and opiate-dependent patients. Behav Brain Res. 2013 Jan;250(0):285–92.

314. He M, Yan H, Duan Z-X, Qu W, Gong H-Y, Fan Z-L, et al. Genetic distribution and association analysis of DRD2 gene polymorphisms with major depressive disorder in the Chinese Han population. Int J Clin Exp Pathol. 2013;6(6):1142–9.

315. Wang F, Simen A, Arias A, Lu Q-W, Zhang H. A large-scale meta-analysis of the association between the ANKK1/DRD2 Taq1A polymorphism and alcohol dependence. Hum Genet. 2013;132:347–58.

Page 144: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

132

316. Gong P, Zhang H, Chi W, Ge W, Zhang K, Zheng A, et al. An Association Study on the Polymorphisms of Dopaminergic Genes with Working Memory in a Healthy Chinese Han Population. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2012;32(6):1011–9.

317. Lu R-B, Lee J-F, Huang S-Y, Lee S-Y, Chang Y-H, Kuo P-H, et al. Interaction between ALDH2*1*1 and DRD2/ANKK1 TaqI A1A1 genes may be associated with antisocial personality disorder not co-morbid with alcoholism. Addict Biol. 2012;17(5):865–74.

318. Hu M-C, Lee S-Y, Wang T-Y, Chen S-L, Chang Y-H, Chen S-H, et al. Association study of DRD2 and MAOA genes with subtyped alcoholism comorbid with bipolar disorder in Han Chinese. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry. 2013 Oct;40(0):144–8.

319. COMINGS DE, COMINGS BG, MUHLEMAN D, DIETZ G, SHAHBAHRAMI B, TAST D, et al. The Dopamine-D2 Receptor Locus as a Modifying Gene in Neuropsychiatric Disorders. Jama-Journal Am Med Assoc. 1991 Oct;266(13):1793–800.

320. Kraschewski A, Reese J, Anghelescu I, Winterer G, Schmidt LG, Gallinat J, et al. Association of the dopamine D2 receptor gene with alcohol dependence: haplotypes and subgroups of alcoholics as key factors for understanding receptor function. Pharmacogenet Genomics. 2009;19(7):513–27.

321. Wu C-Y, Wu Y-S, Lee J-F, Huang S-Y, Yu L, Ko H-C, et al. The association between DRD2/ANKK1, 5-HTTLPR gene, and specific personality trait on antisocial alcoholism among Han Chinese in Taiwan. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2008 Jun;147B(4):447–53.

322. Ponce G, Jimenez-Arriero MA, Rubio G, Hoenicka J, Ampuero I, Ramos JA, et al. The A1 allele of the DRD2 gene (TaqI A polymorphisms) is associated with

Page 145: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

133

antisocial personality in a sample of alcohol-dependent patients. Eur Psychiatry. 2003 Nov;18(7):356–60.

323. Huang S-Y, Lin W-W, Wan F-J, Chang A-J, Ko H-C, Wang T-J, et al. Monoamine oxidase-A polymorphisms might modify the association between the dopamine D-2 receptor gene and alcohol dependence. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2007;32(3):185–92.

324. Dick DM, Wang JC, Plunkett J, Aliev F, Hinrichs A, Bertelsen S, et al. Family-based association analyses of alcohol dependence phenotypes across DRD2 and neighboring gene ANKK1. Alcohol Exp Res. 2007;31(10):1645–53.

325. Lin S-C, Wu P-L, Ko H-C, Wu JY-W, Huang S-Y, Lin W-W, et al. Specific personality traits and dopamine, serotonin genes in anxiety–depressive alcoholism among Han Chinese in Taiwan. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry. 2007 Jan;31(7):1526–34.

326. Bau CHD, Almeida S, Hutz MH. The TaqI A1 allele of the dopamine D2 receptor gene and alcoholism in Brazil: Association and interaction with stress and harm avoidance on severity prediction. Am J Med Genet. 2000 Jun;96(3):302–6.

327. Limosin F, Loze JY, Dubertret C, Gouya L, Ades J, Rouillon F, et al. Impulsiveness as the intermediate link between the dopamine receptor D2 gene and alcohol dependence. Psychiatr Genet. 2003;13(2):127–9.

328. Nyman ES, Loukola A, Varilo T, Ekelund J, Veijola J, Joukamaa M, et al. Impact of the Dopamine Receptor Gene Family on Temperament Traits in a Population-Based Birth Cohort. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2009;150B(6):854–65.

329. Whitmer AJ, Gotlib IH. Depressive rumination and the C957T polymorphism of the

Page 146: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

134

DRD2 gene. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2012;12(4):741–7.

330. Lee SH, Lee B-H, Lee J-S, Chai YG, Choi MR, Han DMR, et al. The Association of DRD2 -141C and ANKK1 TaqIA Polymorphisms with Alcohol Dependence in Korean Population Classified by the Lesch Typology. Alcohol Alcohol. 2013;48(4):426–32.

331. Tsuchimine S, Yasui-Furukori N, Sasaki K, Kaneda A, Sugawara N, Yoshida S, et al. Association between the dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) polymorphism and the personality traits of healthy Japanese participants. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2012;38(2):190–3.

332. Szczepankiewicz A, Dmitrzak-Weglarz M, Skibinska M, Slopien A, Leszczynska-Rodziewicz A, Czerski P, et al. Study of Dopamine receptors genes polymorphisms in bipolar patients with comorbid alcohol abuse. Alcohol Alcohol. 2007;42(2):70–4.

333. Blum K, Braverman ER, Wu S, Cull JG, Chen TJ, Gill J, et al. Association of polymorphisms of dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2), and dopamine transporter (DAT1) genes with schizoid/avoidant behaviors (SAB). Mol Psychiatry. 1997;2(3):239–46.

334. Vereczkei A, Demetrovics Z, Szekely A, Sarkozy P, Antal P, Szilagyi A, et al. Multivariate Analysis of Dopaminergic Gene Variants as Risk Factors of Heroin Dependence. PLoS One. 2013 Jun;8(6):e66592–e66592.

335. Blasi G, Lo Bianco L, Taurisano P, Gelao B, Romano R, Fazio L, et al. Functional Variation of the Dopamine D-2 Receptor Gene Is Associated with Emotional Control as well as Brain Activity and Connectivity during Emotion Processing in Humans. J Neurosci. 2009;29(47):14812–9.

336. Nakajima M, Hattori E, Yamada K, Iwayama Y, Toyota T. Association and

Page 147: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

135

synergistic interaction between promoter variants of the DRD4 gene in Japanese schizophrenics. Japan Society of Human Genetics; 2007;52(- 1):- 86.

337. Golimbet VE, Gritsenko IK, Alfimova M V, Lezheiko T V, Abramova LI, Barkhatova AN, et al. Dopamine receptor DRD4 gene polymorphism and its association with schizophrenia spectrum disorders and personality traits of patients. Zhurnal Nevrol i Psikhiatrii Im S S Korsakova. 2005;105(9):42–7.

338. Kereszturi E, Kiraly O, Csapo Z, Tarnok Z, Gadoros J, Sasvari-Szekely M, et al. Association between the 120-bp duplication of the dopamine D4 receptor gene and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Genetic and molecular analyses. Am J Med Genet Part B Neuropsychiatr Genet. Wiley Subscription Services, Inc., A Wiley Company; 2007;144B(2):231–6.

339. Lai JH, Zhu YS, Huo ZH, Sun RF, Yu B, Wang YP, et al. Association study of polymorphisms in the promoter region of DRD4 with schizophrenia, depression, and heroin addiction. Brain Res. 2010;1359:227–32.

340. Yang J-W, Jang W-S, Hong SD, Ji YI, Kim DH, Park J, et al. A case-control association study of the polymorphism at the promoter region of the DRD4 gene in Korean boys with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: Evidence of association with the − 521 C/T SNP. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry. 2008 Jan;32(1):243–8.

341. Lee HJ, Lee HS, Kim YK, Kim SH, Kim L, Lee MS, et al. Allelic variants interaction of dopamine receptor D4 polymorphism correlate with personality traits in young Korean female population. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2003 Apr;118B(1):76–80.

342. Lakatos K, Nemoda Z, Toth I, Ronai Z, Ney K, Sasvari-Szekely M, et al. Further evidence for the role of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene in attachment disorganization: interaction of the exon III 48-bp repeat and the-521 C/T promoter polymorphisms. Mol Psychiatry. 2002;7(1):27–31.

Page 148: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

136

343. Bookman EB, Taylor RE, Adams-Campbell L, Kittles RA. DRD4 promoter SNPs and gender effects on Extraversion in African Americans. Mol Psychiatry. 2002;7(7):786–9.

344. Ronai Z, Szekely A, Nemoda Z, Lakatos K, Gervai J, Staub M, et al. Association between Novelty Seeking and the-521 C/T polymorphism in the promoter region of the DRD4 gene. Mol Psychiatry. 2001;6(1):35–8.

345. Bellgrove MA, Hawi Z, Lowe N, Kirley A, Robertson IH, Gill M. DRD4 gene variants and sustained attention in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Effects of associated alleles at the VNTR and-521 SNP. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2005 Jul;136B(1):81–6.

346. Bau CHD, Roman T, Almeida S, Hutz MH. Dopamine D4 receptor gene and personality dimensions in Brazilian male alcoholics. Psychiatr Genet. 1999 Sep;9(3):139–43.

347. Li T, Xu K, Deng H, Cai G, Liu J, Liu X, et al. Association analysis of the dopamine D4 gene exon III VNTR and heroin abuse in Chinese subjects. Mol Psychiatry. 1997;2(5):413–6.

348. Garpenstrand H, Ekblom J, Hallman J, Oreland L. Platelet monoamine oxidase activity in relation to alleles of dopamine D-4 receptor and tyrosine hydroxylase genes. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1997;96(4):295–300.

349. Mill J, Curran S, Kent L, Richards S, Gould A, Virdee V, et al. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and the dopamine D4 receptor gene: evidence of association but no linkage in a UK sample. Mol Psychiatry. 2001 Jul;6(4):440–4.

Page 149: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

137

350. Hawi Z, McCarron M, Kirley A, Daly G, Fitzgerald M, Gill M. No association of the dopamine DRD4 receptor (DRD4) gene polymorphism with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in the Irish population. Am J Med Genet. 2000 Jun 12;96(3):268–72.

351. Lasky-Su J, Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, Franke B, Brookes K, Sonuga-Barke E, et al. Partial Replication of a DRD4 Association in ADHD Individuals Using a Statistically Derived Quantitative Trait for ADHD in a Family-Based Association Test. Autism Atten Deficit Hyperact Disord. 2007 Jan;62(9):985–90.

352. Lee KY, Joo E-J, Ji YI, Kim D-H, Park J, Chung I-W, et al. Associations between DRD s and schizophrenia in a Korean population: multi-stage association analyses. Exp Mol Med. 2011;43(1):44.

353. Rosenberg S, Templeton AR, Feigin PD, Lancet D, Beckmann JS, Selig S, et al. The association of DNA sequence variation at the MAOA genetic locus with quantitative behavioural traits in normal males. Hum Genet. 2006;120(4):447–59.

354. Li D, Lin H. Meta-study on association between the monoamine oxidase A gene (MAOA) and schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2008;147B(2):174.

355. Ni X, Sicard T, Bulgin N, Bismil R, Chan K, McMain S, et al. Monoamine oxidase A gene is associated with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatr Genet. 2007;17(3):153–7.

356. Lee S-Y, Hahn C-Y, Lee J-F, Huang S-Y, Chen S-L, Kuo P-H, et al. MAOA Interacts With the ALDH2 Gene in Anxiety-Depression Alcohol Dependence. Alcohol Exp Res. 2010 Jul;34(7):1212–8.

357. Philibert RA, Wernett P, Plume J, Packer H, Brody GH, Beach SRH. Gene

Page 150: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

138

environment interactions with a novel variable Monoamine Oxidase A transcriptional enhancer are associated with antisocial personality disorder. Biol Psychol. 2011;87(3):366–71.

358. Lee S-Y, Hahn C-Y, Lee J-F, Chen S-L, Chen S-H, Yeh TL, et al. MAOA-uVNTR Polymorphism May Modify the Protective Effect of ALDH2 Gene Against Alcohol Dependence in Antisocial Personality Disorder. Alcohol Exp Res. 2009 Jun;33(6):985–90.

359. Samochowiec J, Leach KP, Rottmann M, Smolka M, Syagailo Y V, Okladnova O, et al. Association of a regulatory polymorphism in the promoter region of the monoamine oxidase A gene with antisocial alcoholism. Psychiatry Res. 1999 Apr;86(1):67–72.

360. Nilsson KW, Wargelius H-L, Sjoberg RL, Leppert J, Oreland L. The MAO-A gene, platelet MAO-B activity and psychosocial environment in adolescent female alcohol-related problem behaviour. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008 Jan;93(1-2):51–62.

361. Passamonti L, Fera F, Magariello A, Cerasa A, Gioia MC, Muglia M, et al. Monoamine Oxidase-A Genetic Variations Influence Brain Activity Associated with Inhibitory Control: New Insight into the Neural Correlates of Impulsivity. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59(4):334–40.

362. Jönsson EG, Norton N, Gustavsson JP, Oreland L, Owen MJ, Sedvall GC. A promoter polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene and its relationships to monoamine metabolite concentrations in CSF of healthy volunteers. J Psychiatr Res. 2000 Jan;34(3):239–44.

363. Deckert J, Catalano M, Syagailo Y V, Bosi M, Okladnova O, Bella D Di, et al. Excess of high activity monoamine oxidase A gene promoter alleles in female patients with panic disorder. Hum Mol Genet. 1999;8(4):621–4.

Page 151: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

139

364. Norton N, Kirov G, Zammit S, Jones G, Jones S, Owen R, et al. Schizophrenia and functional polymorphisms in the MAOA and COMT genes: No evidence for association or epistasis. Am J Med Genet. 2002;114(March):491–6.

365. Jansson M, McCarthy S, Sullivan PF, Dickman P, Andersson B, Oreland L, et al. MAOA haplotypes associated with thrombocyte-MAO activity. BMC Genet. 2005 Jan;6(1):46.

366. Tadic A, Rujescu D, Szegedi A, Giegling I, Singer P, Möller H-J, et al. Association of a MAOA gene variant with generalized anxiety disorder, but not with panic disorder or major depression. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2003 Feb;117B(1):1–6.

367. Wang K-S, Liu X, Aragam N, Jian X, Mullersman JE, Liu Y, et al. Family-based association analysis of alcohol dependence in the COGA sample and replication in the Australian twin-family study. J Neural Transm. 2011 Sep;118(9):1293–9.

368. Sengupta SM, Grizenko N, Thakur GA, Bellingham J, DeGuzman R, Robinson S, et al. Differential association between the norepinephrine transporter gene and ADHD: role of sex and subtype. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2012;37(2):129–37.

369. Xu XH, Knight J, Brookes K, Mill J, Sham P, Craig I, et al. DNA pooling analysis of 21 norepinephrine transporter gene SNPs with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: No evidence for association. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2005 Apr;134B(1):115–8.

370. Forero DA, Arboleda GH, Vasquez R, Arboleda H. Candidate genes involved in neural plasticity and the risk for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of 8 common variants. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2009;34(5):361–6.

371. Buttenschon HN, Jacobsen IS, Grynderup MB, Hansen AM, Kolstad HA, Kaerlev

Page 152: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

140

L, et al. An association study between the norepinephrine transporter gene and depression. Psychiatr Genet. 2013;23(5):217–21.

372. Xu X, Hawi Z, Brookes KJ, Anney R, Bellgrove M, Franke B, et al. Replication of a Rare Protective Allele in the Noradrenaline Transporter Gene and ADHD. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2008 Dec;147B(8):1564–7.

373. Haenisch B, Linsel K, Bruess M, Gilsbach R, Propping P, Noethen MM, et al. Association of Major Depression With Rare Functional Variants in Norepinephrine Transporter and Serotonin(1A) Receptor Genes. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2009 Oct;150B(7):1013–6.

374. Baune BT, Hohoff C, Mortensen LS, Deckert J, Arolt V, Domschke K. Serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) association with melancholic depression: a female specific effect? Depress Anxiety. 2008 Jan;25(11):920–5.

375. Hu M, Retz W, Baader M, Pesold B, Adler G, Henn FA, et al. Promoter polymorphism of the 5-HT transporter and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett. 2000 Oct;294(1):63–5.

376. Nakamura M, Ueno S, Sano A, Tanabe H. The human serotonin transporter gene linked polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) shows ten novel allelic variants. Mol Psychiatry. 2000 Jan;5(1):32–8.

377. Paaver M, Nordquist N, Parik J, Harro M, Oreland L, Harro J. Platelet MAO activity and the 5-HTT gene promoter polymorphism are associated with impulsivity and cognitive style in visual information processing. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2007;194(4):545–54.

378. Assal F, Alarcon M, Solomon EC, Masterman D, Geschwind DH, Cummings JL. Association of the serotonin transporter and receptor gene polymorphisms in

Page 153: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

141

neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2004;61(8):1249–53.

379. Heils A, Teufel A, Petri S, Stober G, Riederer P, Bengel D, et al. Allelic variation of human serotonin transporter gene expression. J Neurochem. 1996 Jun;66(6):2621–4.

380. Cao J, Hudziak JJ, Li D. Multi-Cultural Association of the Serotonin Transporter Gene (SLC6A4) with Substance Use Disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38(9):1737–47.

381. Calati R, Gressier F, Balestri M, Serretti A. Genetic modulation of borderline personality disorder: Systematic review and meta-analysis. J Psychiatr Res. 2013 Oct;47(10):1275–87.

382. Lei X, Chen C, He Q, Chen C, Moyzis RK, Xue G, et al. Sex determines which section of the SLC6A4 gene is linked to obsessive-compulsive symptoms in normal Chinese college students. J Psychiatr Res. 2012 Sep;46(9):1153–60.

383. Wang T-Y, Lee S-Y, Chen S-L, Chang Y-H, Chen S-H, Chu C-H, et al. Interaction between Serotonin Transporter and Serotonin Receptor 1 B genes polymorphisms may be associated with antisocial alcoholism. Behav Brain Funct. 2012;8:18.

384. Landaas ET, Johansson S, Halmoy A, Oedegaard KJ, Fasmer OB, Haavik J. No association between the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism 5-HTTLPR and cyclothymic temperament as measured by TEMPS-A. J Affect Disord. 2011 Mar;129(1-3):308–12.

385. Klauke B, Deckert J, Reif A, Pauli P, Zwanzger P, Baumann C, et al. Serotonin transporter gene and childhood trauma - A G*E effect on anxiety sensitivity. Depress Anxiety. 2011;28(12):1048–57.

Page 154: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

142

386. Marini S, Bagnoli S, Bessi V, Tedde A, Bracco L, Sorbi S, et al. Implication of serotonin-transporter (5-HTT) gene polymorphism in subjective memory complaints and mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2011;52(2):E71–4.

387. Tadic A, Elsaesser A, Storm N, Baade U, Wagner S, Baskaya O, et al. Association analysis between gene variants of the tyrosine hydroxylase and the serotonin transporter in borderline personality disorder. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2010 Feb;11(1):45–58.

388. Sadeh N, Javdani S, Jackson JJ, Reynolds EK, Potenza MN, Gelernter J, et al. Serotonin Transporter Gene Associations With Psychopathic Traits in Youth Vary as a Function of Socioeconomic Resources. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010 Aug;119(3):604–9.

389. Garcia LF, Aluja A, Fibla J, Cuevas L, García O. Incremental effect for antisocial personality disorder genetic risk combining 5-HTTLPR and 5-HTTVNTR polymorphisms. Psychiatry Res. 2010;177(1–2):161–6.

390. Maurex L, Zaboli G, Öhman A, Åsberg M, Leopardi R. The serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and affective symptoms among women diagnosed with borderline personality disorder. Eur Psychiatry. 2010;25(1):19–25.

391. Wray NR, James MR, Gordon SD, Dumenil T, Ryan L, Coventry WL, et al. Accurate, Large-Scale Genotyping of 5HTTLPR and Flanking Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms in an Association Study of Depression, Anxiety, and Personality Measures. Biol Psychiatry. 2009 Sep;66(5):468–76.

392. Akkermann K, Paaver M, Nordquist N, Oreland L, Harro J. Association of 5-HTT gene polymorphism, platelet MAO activity, and drive for thinness in a population-based sample of adolescent girls. Int J Eat Disord. 2008;41(5):399–404.

Page 155: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

143

393. Gallinat J, Mueller DJ, Bierbrauer J, Rommelspacher H, Juckell G, Wernicke C. Functional cortical effects of novel allelic variants of the serotonin transporter gene-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) in humans. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2007 Sep;40(5):191–5.

394. Pascual JC, Soler J, Cortes A, Menoyo A, Barrachina J, Ropero M, et al. Association between the serotonin transporter gene and personality traits in bordeline personality disorder patients evaluated with Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ). Actas Esp Psiquiatr. 2007;35(6):382–6.

395. Heiser P, Dempfle A, Friedel S, Konrad K, Hinney A, Kiefl H, et al. Family-based association study of serotonergic candidate genes and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a German sample. J Neural Transm. 2007;114(4):513–21.

396. Perez M, Brown JS, Vrshek-Schallhorn S, Johnson F, Joiner Jr. TE. Differentiation of obsessive-compulsive-, panic-, obsessive-compulsive personality-, and non-disordered individuals by variation in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene. J Anxiety Disord. 2006;20(6):794–806.

397. Ikeda M, Iwata N, Suzuki T, Kitajima T, Yamanouchi Y, Kinoshita Y, et al. No association of serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in Japanese patients: association analysis based on linkage disequilibrium. J Neural Transm. 2006 Jul;113(7):899–905.

398. Gondo Y, Hirose N, Arai Y, Yamamura K, Shimizu K, Takayama M, et al. Contribution of an affect-associated gene to human longevity: Prevalence of the long-allele genotype of the serotonin transporter-linked gene in Japanese centenarians. Mech Ageing Dev. 2005 Nov;126(11):1178–84.

399. Golimbet VE, Alfimova M V, Shcherbatikh T, Kaleda VG, Abramova LI, Rogaev EI.

Page 156: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

144

Serotonin Transporter Gene Polymorphism and Schizoid Personality Traits in Patients with Psychosis and Psychiatrically Well Subjects. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2003;4(1):25–9.

400. Golimbet VE, Alfimova M V, Sherbatikh T V, Rogaev EI. Serotonin Transporter Gene Polymorphism and Personality Traits Measured by MMPI. Russ J Genet. 2003 Jan;39(4):435–9.

401. Neumeister A, Konstantinidis A, Stastny J, Schwarz MJ, Vitouch O, Willeit M, et al. Association between serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism (5HTTLPR) and behavioral responses to tryptophan depletion in healthy women with and without family history of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59(7):613–20.

402. Matsushita S, Yoshino A, Murayama M, Kimura M, Muramatsu T, Higuchi S. Association study of serotonin transporter gene regulatory region polymorphism and alcoholism. Am J Med Genet. 2001 Jul;105(5):446–50.

403. Preuss UW, Soyka M, Bahlmann M, Wenzel K, Behrens S, de Jonge S, et al. Serotonin transporter gene regulatory region polymorphism (5-HTTLPR), [H-3]paroxetine binding in healthy control subjects and alcohol-dependent patients and their relationships to impulsivity. Psychiatry Res. 2000 Sep;96(1):51–61.

404. Kotler M, Cohen H, Kremer I, Mel H, Horowitz R, Ohel N, et al. No association between the serotonin transporter promoter region (5-HTTLPR) and the dopamine D3 receptor (BalI D3DR) polymorphisms and heroin addiction. Mol Psychiatry. 1999 Jul;4(4):313–4.

405. Ishiguro H, Saito T, Akazawa S, Mitushio H, Tada K, Enomoto M, et al. Association between drinking-related antisocial behavior and a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene in a Japanese population. Alcohol Exp Res. 1999 Jul;23(7):1281–4.

Page 157: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

145

406. Sander T, Harms H, Dufeu P, Kuhn S, Hoehe M, Lesch KP, et al. Serotonin transporter gene variants in alcohol-dependent subjects with dissocial personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1998 Jun 15;43(12):908–12.

407. Ribasés M, Fernández-Aranda F, Gratacòs M, Mercader JM, Casasnovas C, Núñez A, et al. Contribution of the serotoninergic system to anxious and depressive traits that may be partially responsible for the phenotypical variability of bulimia nervosa. J Psychiatr Res. 2008;42(1):50–7.

408. Glenn AL. The other allele: Exploring the long allele of the serotonin transporter gene as a potential risk factor for psychopathy: A review of the parallels in findings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):612–20.

409. Li W, Yang Y, Lin J, Wang S, Zhao J, Yang G, et al. Association of serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) polymorphisms with schizophrenia susceptibility and symptoms in a Chinese-Han population. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry. 2013;44(0):290–5.

410. Carlstrom EL, Saetre P, Rosengren A, Thygesen JH, Djurovic S, Melle I, et al. Association between a genetic variant in the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) and suicidal behavior in patients with schizophrenia. Behav Brain Funct. 2012;8:24.

411. Windemuth A, Calhoun VD, Pearlson GD, Kocherla M, Jagannathan K, Ruano G, et al. Physiogenomic analysis of localized fMRI brain activity in schizophrenia. Ann Biomed Eng. 2008 Jun;36(6):877–88.

412. Geijer T, Jonsson E, Neiman J, Persson ML, Brene S, Gyllander A, et al. Tyrosine hydroxylase and dopamine D4 receptor allelic distribution in Scandinavian chronic alcoholics. Alcohol Exp Res. 1997 Feb;21(1):35–9.

Page 158: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

146

413. Tochigi M, Otowa T, Hibino H, Kato C, Otani T, Umekage T, et al. Combined analysis of association between personality traits and three functional polymorphisms in the tyrosine hydroxylase,) monoamine oxidase A, and catechol-O-methyltransferase genes. Neurosci Res. 2006 Mar;54(3):180–5.

414. Tsuchimine S, Yasui-Furukori N, Kaneda A, Saito M, Nakagami T, Sugawara N, et al. No association between polymorphism in tyrosine hydroxylase and personality traits in healthy Japanese subjects. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2010;64(2):196–8.

415. Hu J, Chan LF, Souza RP, Tampakeras M, Kennedy JL, Zai C, et al. The role of tyrosine hydroxylase gene variants in suicide attempt in schizophrenia. Neurosci Lett. 2014 Jan;559:39–43.

416. Celorrio D, Bujanda L, Caso C, Landabaso M, Carlos Oria J, Ogando J, et al. A comparison of Val81Met and other polymorphisms of alcohol metabolising genes in patients and controls in Northern Spain. Alcohol. 2012 Aug;46(5):427–31.

417. Giegling I, Moreno-De-Luca D, Calati R, Hartmann AM, Moeller H-J, De Ronchi D, et al. Tyrosine Hydroxylase and DOPA Decarboxylase Gene Variants in Personality Traits. Neuropsychobiology. 2009;59(1):23–7.

418. Giegling I, Moreno-De-Luca D, Rujescu D, Schneider B, Hartmann AM, Schnabel A, et al. Dopa decarboxylase and tyrosine hydroxylase gene variants in suicidal behavior. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2008 Apr;147B(3):308–15.

419. Dahmen N, Volp M, Singer P, Hiemke C, Szegedi A. Tyrosine hydroxylase Val-81-Met polymorphism associated with early-onset alcoholism. Psychiatr Genet. 2005 Mar;15(1):13–6.

420. Sadahiro R, Suzuki A, Shibuya N, Kamata M, Matsumoto Y, Goto K, et al.

Page 159: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

147

Association study between a functional polymorphism of tyrosine hydroxylase gene promoter and personality traits in healthy subjects. Behav Brain Res. 2010 Mar;208(1):209–12.

421. Zetzsche T, Preuss UW, Bondy B, Frodl T, Zill P, Schmitt G, et al. 5-HT1A receptor gene C-1019 G polymorphism and amygdala volume in borderline personality disorder. Genes Brain Behav. 2008 Apr;7(3):306–13.

422. Ates O, Karakus N, Sezer S, Bozkurt N. Genetic association of 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B gene polymorphisms with migraine in a Turkish population. J Neurol Sci. 2013 Mar 15;326(1-2):64–7.

423. Kranzler HR, Hernandez-Avila C a, Gelernter J. Polymorphism of the 5-HT1B receptor gene (HTR1B): Strong within-locus linkage disequilibrium without association to antisocial substance dependence. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002 Jan;26(1):115–22.

424. Lappalainen J, Long JC, Eggert M, Ozaki N, Robin RW, Brown GL, et al. Linkage of antisocial alcoholism to the serotonin 5-HT1B receptor gene in 2 populations. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(11):989–94.

425. Soyka M, Preuss UW, Koller G, Zill P, Bondy B. Association of 5-HT1B receptor gene and antisocial behavior in alcoholism. J Neural Transm. 2004;111(1):101–9.

426. Ni X, Chan D, Chan K, McMain S, Kennedy JL. Serotonin genes and gene–gene interactions in borderline personality disorder in a matched case-control study. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry. 2009 Jan;33(1):128–33.

427. Lee S-Y, Wang T-Y, Chen S-L, Huang S-Y, Tzeng N-S, Chang Y-H, et al. Interaction Between Novelty Seeking and the Aldehyde Dehydrogenase 2 Gene in Heroin-Dependent Patients. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2013 Jun;33(3):386–90.

Page 160: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

148

428. Roussos P, Giakoumaki SG, Georgakopoulos A, Robakis NK, Bitsios P. The CACNA1C and ANK3 risk alleles impact on affective personality traits and startle reactivity but not on cognition or gating in healthy males. Bipolar Disord. 2011;13(3):250.

429. Hsu DT, Mickey BJ, Langenecker SA, Heitzeg MM, Love TM, Wang H, et al. Variation in the Corticotropin-Releasing Hormone Receptor 1 (CRHR1) Gene Influences fMRI Signal Responses during Emotional Stimulus Processing. J Neurosci. 2012;32(9):3253–60.

430. Reuter M, Weber B, Fiebach CJ, Elger C, Montag C. The biological basis of anger: Associations with the gene coding for DARPP-32 (PPP1R1B) and with amygdala volume. Behav Brain Res. 2009 Sep;202(2):179–83.

431. Limosin F, Romo L, Batel P, Adès J, Boni C, Gorwood P. Association between dopamine receptor D3 gene BalI polymorphism and cognitive impulsiveness in alcohol-dependent men. Eur Psychiatry. 2005;20(3):304–6.

432. Staner L, Hilger C, Hentges F, Monreal J, Hoffmann A, Couturier M, et al. Association between novelty-seeking and the dopamine D3 receptor gene in bipolar patients: A preliminary report. Am J Med Genet. 1998;81(2):192–4.

433. Thome J, Weijers HG, Wiesbeck GA, Sian J, Nara K, Boning J, et al. Dopamine D3 receptor gene polymorphism and alcohol dependence: relation to personality rating. Psychiatr Genet. 1999 Mar;9(1):17–21.

434. Duaux E, Gorwood P, Griffon N, Bourdel MC, Sautel F, Sokoloff P, et al. Homozygosity at the dopamine D-3 receptor gene is associated with opiate dependence. Mol Psychiatry. 1998 Jul;3(4):333–6.

Page 161: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

149

435. Light KJ, Joyce PR, Luty SE, Mulder RT, Frampton CMA, Joyce LRM, et al. Preliminary evidence for an association between a dopamine D3 receptor gene variant and obsessive-compulsive personality disorder in patients with major depression. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2006 Jun;141B(4):409–13.

436. Dick DM, Aliev F, Latendresse S, Porjesz B, Schuckit M, Rangaswamy M, et al. How Phenotype and Developmental Stage Affect the Genes We Find: GABRA2 and Impulsivity. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2013 Jun;16(3):661–9.

437. Kurrikoff T, Lesch K-P, Kiive E, Konstabel K, Herterich S, Veidebaum T, et al. Association of a functional variant of the nitric oxide synthase 1 gene with personality, anxiety, and depressiveness. Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24(4):1225–35.

438. Luciano M, Huffman JE, Arias-Vasquez A, Vinkhuyzen AAE, Middeldorp CM, Giegling I, et al. Genome-Wide Association Uncovers Shared Genetic Effects Among Personality Traits and Mood States. Am J Med Genet Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2012;159B(6):684–95.

439. Hong LE, Wonodi I, Stine OC, Mitchell BD, Thaker GK. Evidence of Missense Mutations on the Neuregulin 1 Gene Affecting Function of Prepulse Inhibition. Schizophr From Genet to Treat. 2008 Jan;63(1):17–23.

440. Xu K, Anderson TR, Neyer KM, Lamparella N, Jenkins G, Zhou Z, et al. Nucleotide sequence variation within the human tyrosine kinase B neurotrophin receptor gene: association with antisocial alcohol dependence. Pharmacogenomics J. 2007;7(May 2006):368–79.

441. Gerra G, Leonardi C, Cortese E, D’Amore A, Lucchini A, Strepparola G, et al. Human Kappa opioid receptor gene (OPRK1) polymorphism is associated with opiate addiction. Am J Med Genet Part B Neuropsychiatr Genet. Wiley Subscription Services, Inc., A Wiley Company; 2007;144B(6):771–5.

Page 162: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

150

442. Hernandez-Avila C a., Covault J, Gelernter J, Kranzler HR. Association study of personality factors and the Asn40Asp polymorphism at the mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1). Psychiatr Genet. 2004 Jun;14(2):89–92.

443. Ohi K, Hashimoto R, Nakazawa T, Okada T, Yasuda Y, Yamamori H, et al. The p250GAP Gene Is Associated with Risk for Schizophrenia and Schizotypal Personality Traits. PLoS One. 2012 Apr;7(4):e35696–e35696.

444. Basoglu C, Oner O, Ates A, Algul A, Bez Y, Cetin M, et al. Synaptosomal-Associated Protein 25 Gene Polymorphisms and Antisocial Personality Disorder: Association With Temperament and Psychopathy. Can J Psychiatry-Revue Can Psychiatr. 2011 Jun;56(6):341–7.

445. Kim Y-R, Woo J-M, Heo SY, Kim JH, Lim S-J, Yu B-H. An Association Study of the A218C Polymorphism of the Tryptophan Hydroxylase 1 Gene with Eating Disorders in a Korean Population: A Pilot Study. Psychiatry Investig. 2009 Mar;6(1):44–9.

446. Gutknecht L, Jacob C, Strobel A, Kriegebaum C, Müller J, Zeng Y, et al. Tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene variation influences personality traits and disorders related to emotional dysregulation. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. Cambridge Journals Online; 2007;10(03):309–20.

447. Yasuda Y, Hashimoto R, Ohi K, Fukumoto M, Umeda-Yano S, Yamamori H, et al. Impact on schizotypal personality trait of a genome-wide supported psychosis variant of the ZNF804A gene. Neurosci Lett. 2011 May;495(3):216–20.

448. Stefanis NC, Hatzimanolis A, Avramopoulos D, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Stefanis CN, et al. Variation in Psychosis Gene ZNF804A Is Associated With a Refined Schizotypy Phenotype but Not Neurocognitive Performance in a Large Young Male Population. Schizophr Bull. 2013 Nov;39(6):1252–60.

Page 163: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

151

449. Li T, Yu S, Du J, Chen H, Jiang H, Xu K, et al. Role of Novelty Seeking Personality Traits as Mediator of the Association between COMT and Onset Age of Drug Use in Chinese Heroin Dependent Patients. Janda KD, editor. PLoS One. 2011 Aug 17;6(8):e22923.

450. Pap D, Gonda X, Molnar E, Lazary J, Benko A, Downey D, et al. Genetic variants in the catechol-o-methyltransferase gene are associated with impulsivity and executive function: Relevance for major depression. Am J Med Genet Part B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2012;159 B(8):928–40.

451. Hasler R, Salzmann A, Bolzan T, Zimmermann J, Baud P, Giannakopoulos P, et al. DAT1 and DRD4 genes involved in key dimensions of adult ADHD. Neurol Sci. Springer Milan; 2015;36:861–9.

452. Bhowmik A Das, Chaudhury S, Dutta S, Shaw J, Chatterjee A, Choudhury A, et al. Role of functional dopaminergic gene polymorphisms in the etiology of idiopathic intellectual disability. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35(7):1714–22.

453. Skol AD, Scott LJ, Abecasis GR, Boehnke M. Joint analysis is more efficient than replication-based analysis for two-stage genome-wide association studies. Nat Genet. 2006;38(2):209–13.

454. de Moor MHM, Costa PT, Terracciano A, Krueger RF, de Geus EJC, Toshiko T, et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for personality. Mol Psychiatry. 2012 Mar;17(3):337–49.

455. Terracciano A, Esko T, Sutin AR, de Moor MHM, Meirelles O, Zhu G, et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies identifies common variants in CTNNA2 associated with excitement-seeking. Transl Psychiatry. 2011 Oct;1:e49–e49.

Page 164: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

152

456. Vandenbergh DJ, Zonderman a B, Wang J, Uhl GR, Costa PT. No association between novelty seeking and dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) exon III seven repeat alleles in Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging participants. Mol Psychiatry. 1997;2(5):417–9.

457. Terracciano A, Balaci L, Thayer J, Scally M, Kokinos S, Ferrucci L, et al. Variants of the serotonin transporter gene and NEO-PI-R Neuroticism: No association in the BLSA and SardiNIA samples. Am J Med Genet Part B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B(8):1070–7.

458. Brummett BH, Siegler IC, McQuoid DR, Svenson IK, Marchuk D a, Steffens DC. Associations among the NEO Personality Inventory, Revised and the serotonin transporter gene-linked polymorphic region in elders: effects of depression and gender. Psychiatr Genet. 2003;13(1):13–8.

459. Brookes KJ. The VNTR in complex disorders: the forgotten polymorphisms? A functional way forward? Genomics. 2013 May;101(5):273–81.

460. Labrie V, Pai S, Petronis A. Epigenetics of major psychosis: progress, problems and perspectives. Trends Genet. 2012 Sep;28(9):427–35.

461. Hu X-Z, Lipsky RH, Zhu G, Akhtar L a, Taubman J, Greenberg BD, et al. Serotonin transporter promoter gain-of-function genotypes are linked to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;78(May):815–26.

462. Wells TT, Beevers CG, McGeary JE. Serotonin transporter and BDNF genetic variants interact to predict cognitive reactivity in healthy adults. J Affect Disord. 2010;126(1-2):223–9.

Page 165: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

153

463. Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Goldman AL, Verchinski BA, Chen G, Kolachana BS, et al. Evidence of biologic epistasis between BDNF and SLC6A4 and implications for depression. Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13(7):709–16.

464. Cournil A, Kirkwood T. If you would live long, choose your parents well. Trends Genet. 2001;17(5):233–5.

465. Cedar H, Bergman Y. Linking DNA methylation and histone modification: patterns and paradigms. Nat Rev Genet. 2009;10(5):295–304.

466. Feng J, Zhou Y, Campbell SL, Le T, Li E, Sweatt JD, et al. Dnmt1 and Dnmt3a maintain DNA methylation and regulate synaptic function in adult forebrain neurons. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(4):423–30.

467. Miller CA, Gavin CF, White JA, Parrish RR, Honasoge A, Yancey CR, et al. Cortical DNA methylation maintains remote memory. Nat Neurosci. Nature Publishing Group; 2010;13(6):664–6.

468. Mill J, Dempster E, Caspi A, Williams B, Moffitt T, Craig I. Evidence for monozygotic twin (MZ) discordance in methylation level at two CpG sites in the promoter region of the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene. Am J Med Genet Part B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2006;141(4):421–5.

469. Duman EA, Canli T. Influence of life stress, 5-HTTLPR genotype, and SLC6A4 methylation on gene expression and stress response in healthy Caucasian males. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2015;5(1):2.

470. van der Knaap LJ, Riese H, Hudziak JJ, Verbiest MMPJ, Verhulst FC, Oldehinkel AJ, et al. Adverse Life Events and Allele-Specific Methylation of the Serotonin Transporter Gene (SLC6A4) in Adolescents: The TRAILS Study. Psychosom Med. 2015;77(3):246–55.

Page 166: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

154

471. Yang JW, Choi EY, Park MJ, Lee MA. Expression of tyrosine hydroxylase is epigenetically regulated in neural stem cells. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2011;414(4):712–8.

472. Dammann G, Teschler S, Haag T, Altmüller F, Tuczek F, Dammann RH. Increased DNA methylation of neuropsychiatric genes occurs in borderline personality disorder. Epigenetics. Taylor & Francis; 2011 Oct 27;6(12):1454–62.

473. Brown SE, Weaver ICG, Meaney MJ, Szyf M. Regional-specific global cytosine methylation and DNA methyltransferase expression in the adult rat hippocampus. Neurosci Lett. 2008;440(1):49–53.

474. Ladd-Acosta C, Pevsner J, Sabunciyan S, Yolken RH, Webster MJ, Dinkins T, et al. DNA methylation signatures within the human brain. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81(6):1304–15.

475. Gibbs JR, van der Brug MP, Hernandez DG, Traynor BJ, Nalls MA, Lai S-L, et al. Abundant Quantitative Trait Loci Exist for DNA Methylation and Gene Expression in Human Brain. PLoS Genet. 2010;6(5):e1000952.

476. Colhoun HM, McKeigue PM, Smith GD. Problems of reporting genetic associations with complex outcomes. Lancet. 2003 Mar;361(9360):865–72.

477. Tabor HK, Risch NJ, Myers RM. Candidate-gene approaches for studying complex genetic traits: practical considerations. Nat Rev Genet. 2002 May 1;3(May):391–7.

478. Sher L. Current methodological Issues in Candidate Gene Association Studies in Psychiatric Disorders. Jefferson Journal of Psychiatry. 2012.

Page 167: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

155

479. Zondervan KT, Cardon LR. The complex interplay among factors that influence allelic association. Nat Rev Genet. 2004 Feb;5(2):89–100.

480. Lins TC, Abreu BS, Pereira RW. TagSNP transferability and relative loss of variability prediction from HapMap to an admixed population. J Biomed Sci. BioMed Central Ltd; 2009 Jan 14;16(1):73.

481. Smith EM, Wang X, Littrell J, Eckert J, Cole R, Kissebah AH, et al. Comparison of linkage disequilibrium patterns between the HapMap CEPH samples and a family-based cohort of Northern European descent. Genomics. 2006 Oct;88(4):407–14.

482. Lundmark PE, Liljedahl U, Boomsma DI, Mannila H, Martin NG, Palotie A, et al. Evaluation of HapMap data in six populations of European descent. Eur J Hum Genet. 2008;16(9):1142–50.

483. Savitz JB, Ramesar RS. Genetic variants implicated in personality: A review of the more promising candidates. Am J Med Genet - Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004;131 B(September 2003):20–32.

484. Christensen K, Johnson TE, Vaupel JW. The quest for genetic determinants of human longevity: challenges and insights. Nat Rev Genet. 2006;7(6):436–48.

485. Lewis SJ, Brunner EJ. Methodological problems in genetic association studies of longevity--the apolipoprotein E gene as an example. Int J Epidemiol. 2004;33(5):962–70.

486. Rosenthal R. The Volunteer Subject. Hum Relations. 1965;18(4):389–406.

Page 168: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

156

487. Piliavin JA, Siegl E. Health Benefits of Volunteering in the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study. J Health Soc Behav. SAGE Publications; 2007 Dec 1;48(4):450–64.

488. Dollinger SJ, Leong FTL. Volunteer Bias and the Five-Factor Model. J Psychol. 1992;127(1):29–36.

489. Lönnqvist J-E, Paunonen S, Verkasalo M, Leikas S, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Lönnqvist J. Personality characteristics of research volunteers. Eur J Pers. 2007 Dec;21(8):1017–30.

490. Okun MA, Yeung EW, Brown S. Volunteering by older adults and risk of mortality: a meta-analysis. Psychol Aging. 2013 Jun;28(2):564–77.

491. Marcus B, Schütz A. Who Are the People Reluctant to Participate in Research? Personality Correlates of Four Different Types of Nonresponse as Inferred from Self‐and Observer Ratings. J Pers. 2005;73(4):959–84.

492. Tan Q, Kruse T a., Christensen K. Design and analysis in genetic studies of human ageing and longevity. Ageing Res Rev. 2006;5(4):371–87.

493. Grady DL, Thanos PK, Corrada MM, Barnett JC, Ciobanu V, Shustarovich D, et al. DRD4 Genotype Predicts Longevity in Mouse and Human. J Neurosci. 2013;33(1):286–91.

494. Tan Q, Bellizzi D, Rose G, Garasto S, Franceschi C, Kruse T, et al. The influences on human longevity by HUMTHO1.STR polymorphism (Tyrosine Hydroxylase gene). Mech Ageing Dev. 2002 Jul;123(10):1403–10.

Page 169: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

157

495. Warde-Farley D, Donaldson S, Comes O, Zuberi K, Badrawi R, Chao P, et al. The GeneMANIA prediction server: biological network integration for gene prioritization and predicting gene function. Nucleic Acids Res. 2010;38:214–20.

496. Derringer J, Krueger RF, Dick DM, Saccone S, Grucza R a., Agrawal a., et al. Predicting Sensation Seeking From Dopamine Genes: A Candidate-System Approach. Psychol Sci. 2010;21:1282–90.

497. Stephens M, Balding DJ. Bayesian statistical methods for genetic association studies. Nat Rev Genet. Nature Publishing Group; 2009;10(10):681–90.

498. Hutchison KE, Stallings M, McGeary J, Bryan A. Population Stratification in the Candidate Gene Study: Fatal Threat or Red Herring? Psychol Bull. 2004;130(1):66–79.

499. Turiano N a, Pitzer L, Armour C, Karlamangla A, Ryff CD, Mroczek DK. Personality Trait Level and Change as Predictors of Health Outcomes : Findings From a National Study of Americans (MIDUS). J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2012;67(1):4–12.

500. Weber K, Canuto A, Giannakopoulos P, Mouchian A, Meiler-Mititelu C, Meiler A, et al. Personality, psychosocial and health-related predictors of quality of life in old age. Aging Ment Health. 2015;19(2):151–85.

501. Gunthert KC, Cohen LH, Armeli S. The role of neuroticism in daily stress and coping. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1999;77(5):1087–100.

502. Tremblay J, Hamet P. Role of genomics on the path to personalized medicine. Metabolism. 2013;62:S2–5.

Page 170: Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniorssummit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/15917/etd9366_JNelson.pdf · Personality Genetics and Health in Super-Seniors by Jessica Marit

158

503. Uhl GR, Walther D, Musci R, Fisher C, Anthony JC, Storr CL, et al. Smoking quit success genotype score predicts quit success and distinct patterns of developmental involvement with common addictive substances. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19(1):50–4.

504. Friedman B, Veazie PJ, Chapman BP, Manning WG, Duberstein PR. Is personality associated with health care use by older adults? Milbank Q. 2013;91(3):491–527.


Recommended