+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Psychopathic personality traits, empathy, and prosocial ...

Psychopathic personality traits, empathy, and prosocial ...

Date post: 22-Feb-2022
Category:
Upload: others
View: 10 times
Download: 1 times
Share this document with a friend
22
RUNNING HEAD: PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR Psychopathic personality traits, empathy, and prosocial behavior Esha Nagpal Department of Psychology, Georgetown University PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 1
Transcript

RUNNING HEAD: PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR

Psychopathic personality traits, empathy, and prosocial behavior

Esha Nagpal

Department of Psychology, Georgetown University

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 1

Abstract

Altruism is defined as voluntary behavior to assist others without the expectation of any

personal reward or gain. Some researchers have indicated a strong relationship between empathy

and subsequent altruistic or prosocial behavior. Studies have shown that fear, as an indicator of

helplessness, tends to elicit prosocial behavior in others. Its recognition is widespread

throughout the human population; however not all individuals respond in the same manner in

response to fear. Psychopathy, a disorder characterized by reduced empathy and remorse, has

been shown to effect individual’s responses to others’ emotions, specifically various studies have

shown that psychopaths have a deficit in recognizing, processing, and responding appropriately

to fear. Using a flavor-distribution paradigm featuring commercially sold “BeanBoozled Jelly

Beans,” the present research aims to examine the relationship between psychopathic personality

traits, fear verbal cues, and prosocial behavior. Twenty-eight adult participants who varied in

psychopathic personality traits, as measured by the PPI-R, participated in this study. Statements

expressing happiness, fear, anger, and disgust at the prospect of eating various jelly bean flavors

were paired with the various flavors. Participants were told they would be participating with a

partner and were told they would have to choose between sampling an appealing or unappealing

flavor and their partner, in reality a confederate, would be given the other flavor. No prior study

has examined actual behavior and how psychopathy relates to how people respond to these

emotional cues. Results showed that individuals with higher psychopathy scores were negatively

correlated with prosocial behavior in response to another individual’s fear verbal cues but not

with other emotional cues. These results support and extend past research indicating that

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 2

psychopathy is associated with impaired processing and responding to fear cues and suggest that

a greater understanding of emotional processing in psychopaths could lead to a deeper

understanding of the disorder and is a step towards improving the prognosis of the disorder.

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 3

Psychopathic personality traits, empathy, and prosocial behavior

Empathy is defined as an emotional reaction to another’s emotional state or condition

(Eisenberg et al., 1989). Research has shown that empathic concern promotes altruistic behavior

to alleviate another’s distress as signaled by distress cues, such as fear and sadness (Batson,

1987; Dovidio, Allen, and Schroeder, 1990). Fear is a type of distress cue that is commonly

accepted as signaling anxiety, apprehensive worry, or mental suffering. Signals that convey fear

are universally recognized throughout the human population and have been shown to elicit

prosocial behavior and inhibit aggression in healthy individuals (Blair, 2001; Batson, Fultz, and

Schoenrade, 1987; Batson, Duncan, Ackerman, Buckley, & Birch, 1981). However, not all

individuals are equally adept at recognizing, processing, and responding to the fear distress cue.

Psychopathy is a developmental disorder characterized by shallow affect and reduced empathy

and remorse (Hare, 1993). People with high psychopathic personality traits are specifically

impervious to fear cues as shown by various studies (Birbaumer et al., 2005; Blair, Jones, Clark,

& Smith, 1997). In a recent study using emotionally evocative statements, results showed that

psychopathic personality traits were correlated with deficiencies in recognition of fear-evoking

statements but not for other emotions (Marsh and Cardinale, 2012). The converging research has

demonstrated a relationship between the ability to process the fear distress cue and subsequent

prosocial behavior, specifically that psychopaths have a diminished ability to recognize fear and

thus fail to respond prosocially (Blair, 1999).

While research on psychopathy and prosocial behavior strongly supports the hypothesis

that the ability to recognize fear predicts individual differences in prosocial behavior, there is

limited research measuring actual behavior as a function of psychopathy. Aggression is a

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 4

difficult emotion to measure in an experimental setting; however, flavor distribution paradigms

are a well-validated means of assessing interpersonal behaviors related to empathy (Lieberman,

Solomon, Greenberg, & McGregor, 1999; McDermott et al., 2009). In these studies, participants

are asked to distribute some amount of hot sauce high in capsaicin to a study confederate. Prior

to the present research, no study has assessed the effects of expressing different kinds of

emotional feedback on a participant’s behavior or how psychopathic personality traits affects

one’s responses to another’s distress cues. The present research employs a flavor distribution

paradigm using commercially available candy called “Beanboozled” to approximate social

behavior in response to various emotional cues. The emotions conveyed at the prospect of

tasting various jelly bean flavors included happiness, fear, anger, and disgust. Using pairs of

appealing and unappealing jelly bean flavors, prosocial behavior was measured in terms of when

the participant was willing to sacrifice self-benefit by selecting the unappealing jelly bean for

himself and giving the other participant the appealing jelly bean. Our study will thereby assess

the relationship between participants’ emotion perception from verbal statements and their

empathically relevant social behaviors. Based on the well-established relationship between

psychopathy, deficits in fear recognition, and prosocial behavior, we hypothesized that we would

observe a negative correlation between scores on the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI-R)

and prosocial behavior in response to verbal fear cues but not in response to other emotional

cues. The benefits of this study include that it measures the extent to which people are willing to

sacrifice self-benefit for another person in relation to psychopathic personality traits. Because

empathy is a fundamental component of altruism, discovering correlates and predictors of

empathy is a logical path to study the individual differences in altruistic tendencies. Individuals

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 5

differ dramatically in their tendencies to help others at a personal cost, and by studying correlates

of empathy, research may help researchers determine the crucial factors that influence altruism.

Methods

Participants

Twenty-eight adult volunteers (21 females, 7 males, M age = 19.76 years, SD = 1.43

years, range = 18 - 22 years) were recruited from the Georgetown University undergraduate and

graduate community. The demographics of the sample included 20 (71%) who identified as

Caucasian, 1 (4%) as African American, 5 (18%) as Asian, and 2 (7%) as Latino. One participant

reported a psychiatric diagnosis of Major Depressive Disorder and Generalized Anxiety Disorder

and was receiving treatment for the conditions at the time of the study. The study was approved

by the Institutional Review Board at Georgetown University, and all participants provided

informed written consent.

Measurement of Psychopathy

Psychopathy was measured using the Psychopathic Personality Inventory - Revised (PPI-

R; Lilienfeld & Widows, 2005). The PPI-R is a 154 item self-report measure of both global

psychopathy and eight component traits of psychopathy. It measures the continuum of

psychopathic personality traits present in individuals using items supporting Cleckley’s

dimensional characterization of psychopathy (Lykken, 1995). The original and revised PPI have

been validated for use on both community and offender samples (Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996;

Berardino et al., 2005; Patrick et al., 2006).

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 6

. Participants answer items based on how accurately it applies to themselves using a 4-Point

Likert scale (1 = false, 2 = mostly false, 3 = mostly true, and 4 = true). The PPI-R strongly

correlates to the Psychopathic Checklist-Revised (PCL-R), which is an additional well-validated

measure of psychopathy (Hare, 2003).

Stimuli

The stimuli used in this study were images of various flavored jelly beans, presented in

pairs (Figure 1). Each condition contained one standard standard-flavored jelly bean (e. g.,

peach, coconut) and one unappealing flavor (e.g., barf, skunk spray). The pairs were determined

based on the appearance of the jelly beans, as the flavors in each pair looked identical (e.g. the

pear and the booger flavors were both green (Figure 1). In addition to the visual stimuli, sixty

emotional statements were generated to convey basic emotions - happiness, anger, disgust, or

fear - at the prospect of tasting the various jelly bean flavors. The order in which the jelly bean

pairs were presented was randomized for each participant as were the sixty statements conveying

the various emotions. Statements expressing happiness were always paired with an appealing

jelly bean; however, the statements expressing fear, anger, and disgust were randomized for the

unappealing flavors to ensure that there was not a confounding effect of specific flavors being

paired with specific statements. For instance, a specific statement expressing disgust such as

“Ewww” varied as to the unappealing flavor with which it was presented. In order to pilot the

stimuli, a list of the statements was given to participants, and they were told to circle the emotion

they thought each statement conveyed - happiness, fear, anger, or disgust. The statements were

analyzed using a binomial distribution with p=.5 to ensure participants would clearly identify

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 7

each one with the emotion it intended to convey. In order to obtain 30 happiness, 10 fear, 10

anger, and 10 disgust statements that were accurately identified, there were three different

handouts piloted with at least fifteen participants who responded to each statement. The

binomial distribution analysis took the number of participants who responded into account to

ensure each emotion conveyed was accurate.

Procedure

Participants entered the laboratory and were told they would be participating with another

student, who was already present but in reality, was a confederate. The confederates used were

always female but varied between four different research assistants. The experimenter gave the

participants instructions regarding the task, informing them that the study was examining how

control and choice affect sensory perception. The experimenter showed the participants a box of

the commercially available “Beanboozled Jelly Beans.” The participants were told that they

would be asked to type their impression at the prospect of tasting each flavor. The experimenter

then informed the participants that one of them would be in the “choice” condition and will have

the choice as to which flavor he wants to sample. The other participant would be in the “no

choice” condition and required to sample the other flavor. For instance, if the choices presented

are pear and vomit and the participant in the “choice” condition selects pear, the participant in

the “no choice condition will be given the vomit flavored jelly bean. Upon their arrival, the

experimenter told the participants that a coin flip would determine which condition each person

was in. While the experimenter did actually flip a coin in front of the participants to ensure the

believability of the deception, regardless of the results of the coin toss, the subject was always

told he would be in the “choice” condition and the confederate in the “no choice” condition. In

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 8

addition, participants were told that the jelly beans would be distributed based on the responses

at the end of the study.

Once the participants completed this computer task, they were given the PPI-R, a

demographic form, a psychological screening form, and a perception questionnaire to ensure the

deception used in the study was successful. Upon completion, the participants were debriefed

regarding the study’s intent and informed that the “other participant” was a confederate and no

one was required to eat any of the jelly beans. Participants received an unopened box of

“BeanBoozled” jelly beans in addition to their compensation for participating in the study.

Results

Our hypothesis was that the effect of fear on prosocial behavior would be negatively

correlated with scores on the Psychopathic Personality Inventory - Revised (PPI-R). In other

words, those with higher scores on the PPI-R would exhibit less prosocial behavior in response

to fear verbal cues. Responses from all 28 participants were included in the final analysis.

Removal of data from two subjects who expressed disbelief minimally affected data. PPI-R

scores ranged from 193 to 299 with a mean of 262.93 (SD=23.82). Linear correlations using

PPI-R scores and the frequency of prosocial behavior were computed for the three emotional

cues - fear, anger, and disgust. As shown in Figure 1, the correlations indicated that fear was the

only emotion that had a statistically significant impact on prosocial behavior in relation to PPI-R

scores, r(26)=-.36, p<.05. As shown in Figures 2 and 3, statistically significant correlations were

not found for anger r(26)=-.26, p>.05, and disgust r(26)=-.24, p>.05. The mean rates of giving

the unappealing jelly bean for each emotion were as follows: M(fear) = 3.21, M(anger) = 3.36,

and M(disgust) = 2.93. Overall, out of the 30 trials, the mean of participants choosing the

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 9

unappealing jelly bean was 9.5, which translates to 32% of the time. While the mean for

participants selecting the unappealing jelly bean for themselves was higher than that for fear, the

linear correlation in relation to PPI-R scores was not significant for anger or disgust but was for

fear.

When analyzed by gender for the seven male participants, statistically significant

correlations were not found for fear r(5) = -.63, p>.05, anger r(5) = -.59, p>.05, or disgust r(5) =

-.21, p>.05; however, the strongest correlation was with fear. The mean for males choosing the

unappealing jelly bean was 12.71 or 42% of the trials. For females, the correlations with PPI-R

scores were calculated for each emotion. The results were not significant for fear, r(19) = -.28,

p>.05, for anger, r(19) = -.19, p>.05, or for disgust, r(19) = -.34, p>.05. The mean for females

choosing the unappealing jelly bean was 8.43 or 28% of the trials. The gender differences in the

mean of selecting the unappealing jelly beans could be a result of the fact that females were used

as confederates in the study.

To ensure the deception was successful, participants were asked their impression of “the

other participant” after the study. Some examples of comments indicating the deception was

believed included, “She seemed pretty normal. Standard taste in jelly beans;” and “Her responses

were similar to mine though she tried to impose her perceptions on me to give her the better

tasting jelly beans.” Of the participants who completed the study, two reported some awareness

of deception when they filled out the perception questionnaire at the end of the study; however,

removal of these two subjects who indicated disbelief minimally affected the data. Therefore,

the results indicate that the hypothesis was correct. Psychopathy scores predicted prosocial

behavior in response to fear verbal cues.

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 10

Discussion

This study examined the relationship between psychopathic personality traits and

prosocial behavior in response to various emotions (happiness, fear, anger, and disgust). To our

knowledge, no study to date has assessed the impact of others’ fear on actual behavior as a

function of psychopathy. As hypothesized, results showed that psychopathic traits were

negatively correlated with prosocial behavior in response to another individual’s fear verbal cues

but not with other emotional cues. The hypothesis was tested using verbal stimuli that were

piloted to ensure the intended emotion was conveyed to participants. These results support and

extend previous findings that suggest that psychopathy is related to impairment in processing

social cues, specifically fear. This study provides more insight into the causes behind antisocial

behaviors that are characteristic of psychopaths.

There have been numerous converging studies, including neuroimaging studies

establishing a strong relationship between psychopathic personality traits and impairment in fear

recognition (Marsh, Finger, & Mitchell, 2008; Marsh & Blair, 2008; Jones et al., 2009).

Psychopaths are especially poor at recognizing, processing, and responding appropriately to fear,

where an appropriate response would be to assist the frightened person. One criminal who

scored very high on the Psychopathy Checklist said, “‘They are frightened, right? But, you see, I

don’t really understand it. I’ve been scared myself, and it wasn’t unpleasant’” (Hare, 1993, 44).

Studies have shown that the problem in fear recognition is isolated, as they do not have difficulty

recognizing happiness, disgust, or anger expressions. This deficiency in fear recognition is

associated with reduced amygdala function and suggests this may be the cause of the antisocial

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 11

behavioral tendencies exhibited by psychopaths - behavior that breaks social norms and violates

the rights of others.

A recent study conducted by Marsh and Ambady demonstrated that the ability to detect

fearful faces is correlated with prosocial behavior in an unrelated task, the present study

measured the effect of another’s fear on prosocial behavior towards that individual (Marsh &

Ambady, 2007). The results are consistent with the theory that the more adept one is at

identifying fear expressions, the greater the tendency for subsequent prosocial behaviors. This is

compatible with Cleckley’s construct of psychopathy that certain aspects of emotional response

are idiosyncratic (Cleckley, 1976). This research along with past studies indicates that the fear

expression plays a very important role in inhibiting antisocial behavior (Marsh, Ambady, &

Kleck, 2005). Impairment in recognition seems to interfere with the normal aversive response to

fear, which is a universal expression that serves as a distress cue.

The present study aimed to expand previous findings by directly measuring prosocial

behavior in response to verbal fear cues using a novel stimulus set of statements. The use of

verbal fear cues that are commonly used on a daily basis provides insight into behavioral

tendencies that those with psychopathic personality traits may exhibit in interpersonal

interactions. The results of this study indicate that individuals with more psychopathic traits do

not respond appropriately to the verbal fear cues conveyed to them by another individual.

Therefore, this study supports the theory that the deficits in processing fear may explain the lack

of empathic behaviors and instead the maladaptive behavioral tendencies of individuals with

callous and unemotional traits and psychopaths (Blair, 2006). The induction of empathy using

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 12

distress cues, particularly fear, is thought to increase the importance of the aversive stimulus

expressed by the victim, in this study represented by the confederate’s emotions.

There are some limitations of the study that must be taken into account. Since this study

was conducted using a healthy population, regarding the psychopathy scores, there was a limited

range and scores did not exceed 300 on the PPI-R, indicating there were not very many high-

psychopathy scores in the sample. There is an ongoing debate as to whether psychopathy is a

categorical or dimensional condition. That is to say, there are arguments for psychopathy as a

continuous disorder, in which there are different levels of psychopathic traits one possesses

(Skeem et al., 2011). However, there are also researchers who believe psychopaths are a discrete

group of individuals from the rest of humanity (Harris, Skilling, & Rice, 2001).

A limitation that can be easily eliminated was that the sample size was limited to the

Georgetown community. It would be beneficial to recruit additional participants from elsewhere

to allow the results to be extrapolated to a larger population. Another limitation was the absence

of a manipulation check regarding what emotion the participant was feeling each time he made a

decision between the two jelly bean flavors. A manipulation check would have ensured that the

participant actually factored in the various emotions expressed by the confederate when selecting

whether to give her the unappealing jelly bean.

In terms of future directions for this study, it is important to note that the present research

used a limited amount of deception to measure participants’ prosocial behavior towards others.

The actions measured were not actually performed while physically in the presence of the other

individual but instead were indicated through computer responses. While subjects believed their

responses would be converted into actions, this research could be extended by designing a study

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 13

in which participants see the consequences of their actions while performing the task. In

addition, an fMRI study that involves measuring prosocial behavior in response to others’ fear

would provide additional support for the concept that impairment in fear recognition results in

less prosocial behavior. Although these experiments would not be able to present opportunities

to simulate extreme psychopathic behaviors, they would still provide an accurate measure of

antisocial tendencies, such as bullying and violence.

Conclusion

As research regarding psychopathy, impairment in fear recognition, and its effect on

prosocial behavior is relatively new, this study employed a flavor-distribution paradigm to

measure actual behavior in response to others’ fear. The results of this study support past

research that indicates psychopathy is characterized by deficits in emotional processing,

especially fear, and empathic responses to fear cues. Using a novel measure of prosocial

behavior, the implications of this study are profound as they demonstrate a direct impact of

others’ fear on prosocial behavior associated with psychopathic personality traits. By

demonstrating a correlation between verbal fear cues and reduced prosocial behavior in

individuals with greater psychopathic personality traits, this research provides insight into social

interactions involving antisocial behavior and may explain why psychopaths do not hesitate to

perform these maladaptive behaviors. Future research should aim to expand upon these results

to further explore actual behavior in response to fear cues. This would increase the

understanding of the reduced empathic reaction in individuals with psychopathic tendencies and

hopefully lead to a means to improve the prognosis of the disorder.

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 14

References

Bar-Tal, D. (1986). Altruistic motivation to help: Definition, utility and operationalization.

Humboldt Journal of Social Relations, 13, 3-14.

Batson, C. D. (1987). Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic? Advances in

Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 65-122.

Batson, C. D., Duncan, B. D., Ackerman, P., Buckley, T., & Birch, K. (1981). Is empathic

emotion a source of altruistic motivation? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

40, 290-302.

Batson, C. D., Fultz, J., & Schoenrade, P. A., (1987). Adults’ emotional reactions to the distress

of others. Empathy and its Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Berardino, S. D., Meloy, J. R., Sherman, M., & Jacobs, D., (2005). Validation of the

psychopathic personality inventory on a female inmate sample. Behavioral Sciences and

the Law, 23, 819-835

Birbaumer, N., Veit, R., Lotze, M., Erb, M., Hermann, C., Grodd, W., & Flor, H., (2005).

Deficient fear conditioning in psychopathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging

study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 799–805.

Blair, R.J.R., (1999). Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic

tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 135-145.

Blair, R.J.R., (2001). Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders,

and psychopathy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 71, 727-731.

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 15

Blair, R. J. (2006). The emergence of psychopathy: Implications for the

neuropsychological approach to developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 414-442.

Blair, R. J. R., Jones, L., Clark, F., & Smith, M., (1997). The psychopathic individual: a lack of

responsiveness to distress cues? Psychophysiology, 34, 192–198.

Cleckley, H. (1976). The mask of sanity. St. Louis, MO: Mosby, Inc.

Corden, B., Critchley, H., Skuse, D., & Dolan, R. (2006). Fear recognition predicts differences in

social cognitive and neural functioning in men. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18,

889-897.

Dovidio, J. K., Allen, J. L., & Schroeder, D. A., (1990). The specificity of empathy-induced

helping: Evidence of altruistic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

59, 249-260.

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R.A., Miller, P.A., Fultz, J., Shell, R., Mathy, R.M., Reno, R. R., (1989).

Relation of sympathy and personal distress to prosocial behavior: A multimethod study?

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 55-66.

Hare, R.D. (1993). Without conscience: The disturbing world of psychopaths among us. New

York, NY: Guilford.

Hare, R.D. (2003). Manual for the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised, 2nd edn. Toronto:

Multi-Health Systems.

Harris, G.T., Skilling, T., & Rice, M.E. (2001). The construct of psychopathy. Crime and Justice,

28, 197-264.

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 16

Jones, A.P., Laurens, K.R., Herba, C.M., Barker, G.J., Viding, E. (2009). Amygdala hypoactivity

to fearful faces in boys with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. American

Journal of Psychiatry, 166(1), 95–102.

Lieberman, J. D., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & McGregor, H. A. (1999). A hot new way to

measure aggression: Hot sauce allocation. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 331–348.

Lilienfeld, S. O., & Andrews, B. P., (1996). Development and preliminary validation of a self-

report measure of psychopathic personality traits in noncriminal populations. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 66, 488-524.

Lilienfeld, S. O., & Widows, M. R. (2005). PPI-R: Psychopathic Personality Inventory - Revised

Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

Lykken, D. (1995). The Antisocial Personalities. Hillsdale, NJ. Erlbaum.

Marsh, A. & Blair, R. J. (2008). Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial

populations: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 454-465.

Marsh A.A. & Ambady, N. (2007). The influence of the fear facial expression on prosocial

responding. Journal of Cognitive Emotion, 21, 225-247.

Marsh, A. A., and Cardinale, E., (2012). Psychopathy, fear, and the amygdala: Psychopathy

corresponds to specific reductions in amygdala activation during fear-based moral

judgments. Under review.

Marsh A.A., Ambady, N., & Kleck, R.E. (2005). The effects of fear and anger facial expressions

on approach- and avoidance-related behaviors. Emotion, 5, 119––124.

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 17

Marsh, A.A., Finger, E.C., & Mitchell, D.G.V., et al. (2008). Reduced amygdala response to

fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and

disruptive behavior disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(6), 712–20.

Marcus, D. K., John, S. L., & Edens, J. F. (2004). A taxometric analysis of psychpathic

personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113(4), 626-635.

McDermott, R., Tingley, D., Cowden, J., Frazzetto, G., & Johnson, D. (2009). Monamine

oxidase a gene (MAOA) predicts behavioral aggression following provocation.

National Academy of Science, 106(7), 2118–23.

Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1993). Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 23–38.

Patrick, C. J., Edens, J. F., Poythress, N. G., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Benning, S. D., (2006).

Construct validity of the psychopathic personality inventory two-factor model with

offenders. Psychological Assessment, 18, 204-208.

Radke-Yarrow, M., Zahn-Waxler, C., Richardson, D. T., Susman, A., & Martinez, P. (1994).

Caring behavior in children of clinically depressed and well mothers. Child Development,

65(5), 1405–1414.

Skeem, J. L., Polaschek, D. L. L., Patrick, C. J., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). Psychopathic

personality: Bridging the gap between scientific evidence and public policy.

Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 12, 95-162.

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 18

Figure 1:Presentation of Flavor Options

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 19

Figure 2:Jelly Bean Flavor Pairings

Left Flavor Right FlavorMoldy Cheese CaramelPineapple SulfurCod TangerineAsphalt PlumButtered Popcorn Rotten EggTurnip RaspberryChocolate Pudding Canned Dog FoodVinegar PeppermintCoconut Baby WipesBarf PeachCotton Candy SalmonSand Toasted MarshmallowCopper Root BeerCappuccino DirtPear BoogerGreen Apple EarwaxWild Blackberry PetrolSkunk Spray LicoriceWildgrass WatermelonCentipede Strawberry JamBanana Pencil ShavingsSquid Mixed BerryFrench Vanilla ToenailsPrawns Bubble GumGrapefruit SardinesKiwi CaterpillarPaint Tutti FruttiPeanut Butter LiverAcid Cream SodaDr. Pepper Fried Beans

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 20

Fear  Verbal  Cues

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

150 200 250 300 350

PPI-­‐R  Scores

Prosocial  B

ehavior

Figure 3:Effect of Fear Verbal Cues on Prosocial Behavior

Anger  Verbal  Cues

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

120%

150 200 250 300 350

PPI-­‐R  Scores

Prosocial  B

ehavior

Figure 4:Effect of Anger Verbal Cues on Prosocial Behavior

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 21

Disgust  Verbal  Cues

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

150 200 250 300 350

PPI-­‐R  Scores

Prosocial  B

ehavior

Figure 4:Effect of Disgust Verbal Cues on Prosocial Behavior

PSYCHOPATHY, FEAR, AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 22


Recommended