+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Rosenblum Question Accusation

Rosenblum Question Accusation

Date post: 23-Feb-2018
Category:
Upload: idschun
View: 221 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 14

Transcript
  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    1/14

    hen

    is aquestion anaccusation

    K A R E N

    E R O S E N B L U M

    With in

    the stud y of n atura lly occurring talk, the sequencing of utterances,

    and

    in

    particular

    the

    structure

    of adjacency

    pairs',

    has

    drawn continuin g

    at tent ion (e.g., in Sacks 1974; Sacks etal. 1974; Schegloff 1968, 1979;

    Schegloff an d Sacks 1973; A tki n son an d D rew 1979). Ad jacency pairs are

    two-part sequenced utterances in the first part of wh ich a speaker specifies

    both

    the

    next speaker

    and the

    type

    of

    response appropriate,

    for

    example

    in summons/response, greeting/greeting, o r request/acceptance-denial ad-

    jacency pairs. W hile there m ay be options available in the construction of

    th e

    second part of an adjacency

    pair

    for ex ample, a request may be m et

    with an

    acceptance, denial,

    or a

    coun ter-request

    a

    failure

    to

    produce

    at

    least one of the expected second parts is noticeable and would merit

    explanation or

    action.

    For

    example,

    in a

    summons/response sequence

    a

    failure to answer a sum mon s might lead to a repetition of the summons,

    or a

    request

    for an

    explanation

    of the

    failure

    to

    answer (Schegloff 1968).

    Because of the

    constraints which surroun d

    the

    production

    of the

    second

    part of an adjacency

    pair,

    much attention has been directed toward its

    accomplishment.

    However, producing a response which

    falls

    within the

    range of

    expectations

    fo r

    that sequence

    is not

    actually

    the

    respondent s

    init ial problem. Rather,

    the first difficulty

    rests

    in

    recognizing some

    utteranceas the initiatin g firstpart of a sequence: an utterance must first

    be identified

    as a greeting, question, or sum m on s before one can begin the

    construction of a

    response. This

    is a

    problem

    of

    recognition

    faced by any

    respondent, and i t tu rns our a ttention to the first

    part

    of the adjacency

    pair.W hat are its markers? How is it recognized? Certainly each type of

    adjacency

    pair poses its own problems concerning

    the

    recognition of its

    first

    part.

    In the discussion wh ich follows, atten tion focuses on identifying

    th e

    markers

    of an

    accusation,

    the first part of the

    adjacency pair

    accusation/denial-acceptance.

    em iotic 65-1/2

    (1987),

    143-156. 0037-1998/87/0065-0143

    2.00

    MoutondeGruyter, Amsterdam

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bib

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    2/14

    144

    Karen

    E

    Rosenblum

    ckground

    Discussion about

    the

    worth

    of

    persons

    or

    actions

    is a

    commonplace

    of

    social

    life.

    Often

    such talk proceeds without

    any

    explicit identification

    of

    the norms

    o r

    values which

    are

    presumed

    to

    h ave been violated. Nonethe-

    less, some of these interactions a re recognizable as encom passing accusa-

    tions

    of serious wrongd oing. O n w hat basis do w e conclude that we face

    an

    accusation? When does an utterance become a charge?

    These are questions characteristic of discourse, or conversational,

    analysis, i .e., that analysis wh ich focuses on wh at speakers m ust kno w in

    order to identify such acts as, for example, declaratives, questions,

    requests, or suggestions (Gum perz 1982: 156). Tapp ing in to these

    accomplishments requires a search for the structure of discourse, a

    structure which

    is

    analogous

    to, but

    distinct from, that

    of

    phonology

    or

    grammar. In discourse analysis th e attempt is to uncover the culturally

    specific

    conversational structures which cut across settings and statuses,

    and to determine th e interactive functions which are accomplished by

    those structures (Sinclair and Coulthard 1975; Coulthard and Montgom-

    ery

    1981; Burton 1980). The goal is to produce ideotypical descriptions

    that can be dissected into

    significant

    components and used to produce

    typologies

    (Gumperz 1982: 157).

    Drawing on the assumptions and methods of discourse analysis, the

    discussion which follows explores the premise that accusations are

    recognizable by their structure as

    well

    as by their content, i.e., that

    accusations may be conveyed without an explicit articulation of norm

    violation. Indeed, in the utterances which

    will

    be examined here, there are

    only a few

    marginal instances

    of an

    actual naming

    of

    some norm

    violation.

    It

    seems reasonable that

    at

    least

    in the

    public domain people

    rarely level their criticism straightforwardly the possibility of m asking

    an

    accusation operating both as a resource and a trouble for interactants.

    Still,

    accusations

    are embedded and discovered within utterances. Thus,

    in

    the

    world, people routinely

    and more-or-less successfully

    make

    infer-

    ences abou t the presence of a charge o f wrongdoing. A re they relying only

    on their members know ledge of the culture s norm s and values, or do

    they

    supplement that with an appraisal albeit unselfconscious of the

    structure

    of an

    utterance?

    Because accusations are the first part of an adjacency pair, students of

    discourse have usually deduced their presence from

    the

    appearance

    of an

    appropriate second part. For example, speaker A uses a statement,

    question, o r command that is heard as requiring either an apology, or an

    excuse/explanation, or justification .... Wherever the responses to this

    type of act can be coded as an apology or excuse, I label the first-pair part

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bi

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    3/14

    W he n is a questionan accusation 145

    accuse

    (Burton 1981: 67). By contrast, this investigation focuses on the first

    part of the pair. Although responses m ay provide confirmation of the

    presence of an accusation, the aim here is to ascertain what any respondent

    must

    ascertain: whether

    one is the

    subject

    of

    strong criticism. Thus,

    the

    method

    here applies

    the

    ethnomethodological insight which infuses dis-

    course

    analysis,i.e.,

    that a researcher s goal is to m ake explicit the shared

    common-sense resources by which utterances are heard to h ave coherence

    in

    this case the resources by which utterances are heard to be accusations

    (Garfinkel

    1967, 1972;

    Mehan

    and

    Wood

    1975;

    Sacks

    1972;

    Turner

    1971,

    1974).The

    [researcher]...

    m ust pose as problematic how utterances come off

    as recog nizable un it activities. This requires

    [the

    researcher] to m ake explicit

    theresources [he/she] shares w ith participants in m aking sense of utterances

    in a stretch of

    talk

    (Turner

    1971:

    177). However, this is not to claim that

    utterances

    may be

    definitively

    classed as accusatory or

    non-accusatory.

    By its

    nature, talk is

    indexical (G arfinkel

    1967;

    Kjolseth 1972);

    i.e., an

    utterance

    m ay

    point to a v irtually infinite variety of meanings, an d thus understanding

    is

    always tied

    to (or

    delimited

    by) the

    context

    in

    which

    an

    utterance

    appears.

    Th us, we can never argue th at some utterance is an accusation, or that all

    accusations

    must take a particular form , but o nly that some types of talk are

    likely

    to be construed as accusations across a variety of settings.

    trategiesfor

    accusation

    in a

    public orum

    To examine the structure of an accusation, I have turned to Richard

    N ixo n s last three presidential press conferences. This is a setting

    likely

    to

    yield

    accusations not only because of its historical particularities, but

    because

    press conferences

    per se

    provide excellent opportunities

    fo r

    uncovering

    accusations. To press conferences, reporters bring their need

    for events on which to report. Such needs sometimes encourage them to

    challenge accounts or to pursue contradictions between and/or within

    accounts (Molotch

    an d

    Lester 1974). These impulses

    are

    bolstered

    by

    press

    conference

    norms which both ensure nominal responsiveness from

    th e

    convener of the

    press conference

    and

    prohibit

    an

    imm ediate punitive

    mobilization of

    social

    control.

    1

    Thus,

    in

    press conferences

    th e

    real

    disparities

    of power between interactants are temporarily circumscribed,

    and conditions more conducive to the leveling of accusations emerge.

    Finally, this is a setting amenable to analysis. Unlike other situations in

    which

    accusations m ay emerge, for this event there is a relatively full an d

    public record, a finite time-span established by the event itself, and a

    setting

    which

    greatly restricts the number and status of participants as

    well

    as the nature of their interaction.

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bi

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    4/14

    146

    aren E Rosenblum

    Still,

    th e

    sequencing

    of

    utterances

    in the

    press conference

    differs

    considerably

    from wh at would be found in na turally occurring conversa-

    tion.

    Reporters are, after all, expected to produce questions. Conversa-

    tional

    moves

    d o no t build on one another as they m ight in other settings;

    e.g., it is the respondent who designates the next speaker; reporters are

    limited to one turn per press conference; they are precluded from

    responding to an answer and may ask follow-up questions only at the

    discretion of the respondent; and reporters are expected to direct their

    remarks to the

    respondent

    and not

    other reporters,

    the

    audience,

    or the

    cameras. None of these norms are taken lightly by participants on either

    side of the

    pod ium .

    2

    While

    the no rm s which govern talk in this setting encourage caution in

    generalizing from press conferences to routine interaction, the norms

    themselves

    prim arily function

    to

    reduce reporters volu bility, prevent

    th e

    em ergence of d ialogues, and enfo rce the casting of utterances as questions.

    Only th e

    last

    of

    these

    is likely to

    seriously bear

    on the

    construction

    of

    accusations. Th at is, accusations are certainly m ore easily form ulated (and

    recognized)

    as

    declaratives tha n

    as

    interrogatives. H ow ever, there

    are few

    settings

    in

    which

    one may

    baldly charge misconduct; more

    often

    such

    sentiments m ust be em bedd ed within utterances w ith other ostensible aim s.

    Thus, press conferences offer only a slightly more formalized level of

    constraint than operates

    in

    most public interactions. That reporters

    sometimes use questions to level accusations is an achievement they share

    with

    a wide variety of others at a minimum, parents, teachers, police,

    and politicians. T hus, w hile press conferences are structured quite

    differ-

    ently

    from other interactions, they share with innumerable settings

    th e

    occasional production of an accusatory question.

    The

    ixon press

    conferences

    Perhaps the most noticeable feature of the press conference is its

    organization as a question-answer session. Questions and answers have

    held

    a special interest for students of discourse. They exemplify the

    interdependence

    of

    utterances (e.g.,

    see

    Schegloff

    and

    Sacks 1973; Sche-

    gloff

    1979; Ervin-Tripp 1970); they characterize the discourse in certain

    settings, e.g.,

    in

    court,

    th e

    classroom,

    or the

    press conference;

    and

    they

    display

    certain functions, e.g., to show relative status (e.g., Philips 1982),

    or to control th e topic of conversation (e.g., Heath 1978). Here questions

    were examined for the structure which would allow them to function as

    accusations.

    The data analyzed h ere are transcripts of presidential press conferences

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bi

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    5/14

    When is aquestionanaccusation? 147

    (Johnson

    1978) held October

    26,

    1973; February

    24,

    1974;

    and

    March

    6,

    1974.

    These transcripts were analyzed

    so as to

    include

    all

    those questions

    whichin any way

    addressed

    or

    alluded

    to law or

    norm violation

    by

    Nixon

    or his

    representatives.

    3

    The aim here was to determine which of those

    questionsgenerally addressed to law/norm violation were recognizable as

    accusations. Thisprocedureaimed

    to

    maximize

    the

    probability

    of

    unco-

    vering accusations, but rejected the assumption that questions which

    addressed

    law/norm violation were axiomatically accusatory.

    Reportersin these press conferences posed two broad types of ques-

    tions:

    those which asked

    for anaccountof

    events,

    and

    those which asked

    foran opinionor

    evaluation

    of

    events. Queries soliciting

    an

    account either

    asked

    about what might happen in hypothetical

    future

    events

    (41 ;

    N = 17),

    called here future-action questions,

    or

    they asked about

    a

    past

    event (17 ; N= 7), called here past-acts questions.

    4

    Solicitations of

    opinion

    (41 ;N =

    17), called opinion questions, were distinguished

    by

    their

    request for an appraisal as opposed to a detailing of events.

    Followingare examples of these three question types:

    after the

    tapes

    a re presented to Judge Sirica a nd

    they

    a re processed under the

    procedure outlined

    by the

    U.S.

    Court

    of

    Appeals,

    will

    you

    m ak e

    those tapes

    public?

    (Future

    action)

    who

    a uthorized

    M r. Rebozo to

    collect campaign

    contributions for your reelection

    or for the Republican Party? (Past acts)

    Now, M r.

    President,

    do you think you

    paid

    your

    fair share

    of taxes?

    (Opinion)

    Whilethere are various typologies which might have been imposed on

    these questions, this is the classification system which emerged most

    directly

    from

    the material. Recalling the setting of these questions, it is

    not

    so

    surprising that requests

    for

    accounts

    and

    opinions should emerge

    with such salience. Not only are questioners in press conferences moti-

    vated

    to gather reportable accounts, but the press conference itself is

    burea ucrat ica l ly

    structured' (Fishman 1980); i.e.,

    it

    both comes into

    being

    as a

    function

    of

    bureaucratic action

    and

    exists

    so as to

    attend

    to the

    activity

    of bureaucrats. Thus, the convener and his/her accounts and

    opinions

    is the central focusof attention.

    Future action

    questions When conferring poweraccomplishes

    an

    accusation

    Questions

    about

    another's

    future-action carrytheinteresting implication

    that

    the

    other

    is

    actually able

    to

    shape that future.'Will

    you

    give them

    the

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bi

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    6/14

    148 KarenE

    Rosenblum

    documents?

    implies that

    the

    documents

    are

    yours

    to

    give and/or that

    your

    compliance is essential to the transaction. Even in the presence of

    obvious duress

    Will you

    give

    me the

    money

    now

    that I ve

    got you

    cornered?

    a fu ture-action question indicates that the one who answers

    has some choice, i.e., power. Because the

    future

    is open and susceptible to

    manipulation,

    the one

    toward w hom

    a

    future -action question

    is

    addressed

    is imbued with some capability

    of

    shaping that

    future.

    Thus, future-ac tion questions a ttribute at least a minimal level of

    control or power to the one toward whom they are directed. However,

    because

    the

    attribution

    of

    power

    is

    inherent

    to the

    construction

    of the

    question rather than responsive to theactual statusof

    interactants

    their

    social consequence w ould

    appear

    to be

    limited.

    If

    future-ac tion questions

    indiscriminately attribute power, they are not especially

    socially

    meaning-

    u l However, almost half

    of the

    future-action questions

    in

    these press

    conferences

    were embellished by description, i.e., by the recounting or

    characterization

    of

    events, persons,

    or

    utterances.

    For

    example,

    in the

    following future-action questions notice both the recounting of Nixon s

    past statements and the characterization of the benefits which might be

    derived

    from a hypothetical course of action.

    You

    have

    referred to

    y ou r

    o wn

    personal

    desire to

    have com plete

    d isclosure and you

    have also

    men t ioned here

    this evening

    that

    anybody who heard the

    tape

    o f

    that

    March 21st m eet ing or i f f e r e n t people hearing that tape or reading the transcript

    might

    get i f f e r e n t impressions.Have you ever

    considered

    the

    option

    of making

    that

    tape

    and

    transcript

    public so that the Am erican peoplecan read i t and hear it

    and ma k e their own judgem ent on whathappened a t that m eet ing?

    You

    have spoken to night

    o f

    y ou r willingness

    to

    take quest ions under oa th

    in the

    White

    House f rom th e sen ior Dem ocra t ic and

    Republican

    m e m b e r s of the H ou s e

    Judiciary

    Commit tee .W ould

    you

    consider

    as an aid to

    rebuilding

    public

    confidence

    in y ou r leadership

    and in

    speeding

    up the

    procedure

    in

    (sic) taking

    questions in a

    public forum from

    the

    entire

    House

    Judiciary

    Committee?

    5

    The

    descriptive segments emphasized above

    are

    clearly gratuitous

    to

    the construction of the particular questions. Further, as these questions

    illustrate, a descriptive passage in a future-action question is likely to

    crystallize

    as a

    recommendation

    for a

    course

    of

    action.

    In

    these examples,

    the recommendation comes complete with the waving of unquestionable

    goals (e.g., healing the divisions in the country, rebuilding public confi-

    dence), and buttressed by the reiteration of Nixon s own past statement.

    Yet even without such explicit markers, the injection of a descriptive

    segment into a future-action question functions to at least tacitly endorse

    some line

    of action.

    Even

    the

    minimal descriptive segment

    as in the

    following questions has prescriptive implications:

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bi

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    7/14

    When is aquestionan accusation? 149

    to heal th e divisions in

    this

    country

    would

    you be

    willing

    to

    waive executive

    privilege

    to

    give

    th e

    Judiciary Committee what

    it

    says

    it

    needs

    to end any

    quest ions ofyour involvementinWatergate?

    some legal scholars including

    Senator Ervin

    have

    said

    that

    th e

    truth

    m l

    never

    be

    fully

    established

    unless all witnesses

    subject themselves

    or

    submit

    to

    cross-exam-

    ination. Are there circumstances under which you would submit to cross-

    examina t ion

    if it

    would serve

    to

    clear

    up

    this Watergate

    affair?

    Whet her or not the

    descriptive passage

    in

    these questions clearly emerges

    as a recommendation, it does set these questions off from unadorned

    future-action questions. In effect, the insertion of a descriptive passage

    creates a

    marked category

    of

    future-action questions.

    Descript ion,whethero fevents, persons, orutterances, is apurposeful,

    motivatedselection

    from a

    virtually

    infinite

    range

    of

    possibilities.

    A s

    such,

    it isunlikelyeither to beproduced orheard as merely descriptive Drew

    1978). Describing is not merely an appendage to other interactional

    w o r k ; rather it is often through constructing descriptions that certain

    interactional

    tasksmay beaccomplished AtkinsonandDrew 1979:107).

    The

    descriptive components of these future-action questions do not

    themselves

    charge wrongdoing. However,

    the

    addition

    of a

    descriptive

    componen t to afuture-action question does allowfor acomparison of 1)

    ability conveyedby theattribution ofpower inherentto thequestion), 2)

    desirability of

    some course

    of

    action

    as

    endorsed

    by

    description

    of

    actions/utterances),

    and 3) actuality i.e., the implied failure to have

    pursued thedesiredcourse).Through thecombination ofpower attribu-

    tion and the

    descriptionwhichaccomplishes

    a

    recommendation, wrong-

    doing

    m ay be

    implied without

    the

    question itself naming

    a

    specific norm

    violat ion.

    In

    such questions the accusation is precisely what McHugh 1970)

    characterized

    as the

    common-sense

    definition of

    deviance: having failed

    to actcorrectly whentheconditionswhichwould excuse such failurewere

    absent. The questions in these press conferences say in effect, here is

    something

    which you

    could

    do

    power attribution inherent

    to future-

    action

    question) which would

    fulfill

    your

    ow n

    stated goals and/or yield

    unquest ionable national benefits recommendation v ia description), yet

    you

    are not

    doing

    it. The

    descriptive component

    is

    vital

    to the

    accusation

    because

    itaccomplishes th erecommendation; th eattribution ofpower is

    similarly

    essential because it isthatwhich renders inexplicablethe

    failure

    to

    take

    the

    recommended action.

    W h a tso farmightb esaid about themarkersof anaccusatory question?

    At aminimum, impugning the merits of another s action requires both

    conferring

    power

    and proffering

    description

    of

    persons, actions,

    or

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bi

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    8/14

    150 Karen E. Rosenblum

    utterances. Q uestions which combine pow er attribution with description,

    i.e., future-action questions constructed

    with

    gratuitous description

    materializing as a

    recomm endation,

    are

    reasonably heard

    as

    accusations

    of

    failure

    or

    defect.

    Opinion and

    past acts

    questions The

    conferral

    and impugning of

    expertise

    Because a question is the outcome of an analysis about who can be

    expected to know what, it has the effect of conferring authority, i.e.,

    expertise,

    on the one

    toward whom

    it is

    directed. From

    a

    mundane

    request for the formulation of location or identification Where are

    you ?, Who are you ? to the questions teachers put to students and

    lawyers

    put to witnesses, questions are ostensibly addressed to those w ho

    may

    be expected to know. It is this property which makes a

    failure

    to

    know notable, occasionally even

    the

    source

    of

    trouble.

    In general, questions solicit the expertise which adheres to ascribed and

    achieved status. Opinion and

    past-act

    questions make the nature of this

    solicitation especially salient. When these opinion questions asked about

    Nixon s own thoughts or feelings, as in the following question, they

    addressed the ascribed domain on which he alone was an authoritative

    informant.

    I wond er if you could share

    with

    us you r thoughts, tell us what goes through your

    mind, when you hear people,

    people

    who love this country and

    people

    who believe

    in

    you, say reluctantly tha t perhaps you should resign or be impeached. (Opinion)

    When

    past-act

    questions asked

    for a

    definitive recounting

    of

    events,

    or

    when opinion questions asked

    him to

    apply

    his

    acquired expertise, they

    addressed an actual or putative achieved status.

    did

    you not consider the option of blowing the whistle, of turning that

    information

    over to the authorities immediately ...? (Past acts)

    Now, as we all know, you are an experienced student of the Constitution, and I

    think people w ould

    be

    interested

    to

    know what

    you

    consider

    to be an

    impeachable

    offense for a

    President. (Opinion)

    Do

    questions which attribute expertise also have

    the

    potential

    to

    make

    accusations? The presence of a descriptive element again contributes to

    this accomplishment. Ninety-four percent

    of the

    opinion

    and

    seventy-one

    percent of the past-act questions were prefaced by recountings and

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bi

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    9/14

    When is a questionan accusation 151

    characterizations of past events and utterances: 'You know in Congress

    there

    is a

    great deal

    of

    suspicion over

    any

    arrangement which will permit

    th e executive branch to investigate

    itself;

    'before you were elected, you

    wrote that

    to o

    many shocks

    can

    drain

    a

    nation

    of its energy';

    there

    have

    been reports that you felt that Mr. Cox was somehow out to get

    you'.

    However, recountings

    and

    characterizations whenadded

    to the

    conferral

    of

    expertise

    do not so

    inevitably yield

    the

    imputation

    of

    defect. Such

    formulations say

    only

    Here is a

    reconstruction

    of

    certain events

    or

    utterances, what is your own authoritative opinion on, or recounting of,

    them ?' Is the con ferral of expertise joined to recou ntings and characteriza-

    tions

    of

    action/utterances adequate

    to

    mark

    a

    question

    as an

    accusation?

    Theanswertothat appears todepend onwhetherthe recountings/char-

    acterizations function

    to

    void

    th e

    attribution

    of

    expertise. There

    are a

    variety

    of means by which that negation can be accomplished; one

    possibility

    is to

    highlight

    th e

    subjectivity

    of

    opinions, i.e.,

    to

    stress

    that

    opinions are

    tied

    to

    status.

    For

    example,

    in 1968, before you were elected, you wrote that to o many shocks can drain a

    nation of its energy and even cause a rebellion against creative change and

    progress.

    Do you

    think America

    is at

    that point now? (Opinion)

    Similarly,

    event reconstructions

    can be

    shown

    as

    variable,

    as in the

    following

    questions:

    M r.

    Haldeman, your former top aide in the White House, has been charged with

    perjury

    because

    he

    testified

    thatyou

    said

    it

    would

    be

    wrong

    to p ay

    hush money

    to

    silenceth e

    Watergate defendants,

    and

    last August

    you

    said that

    w as

    accurate.

    Can

    you,

    and

    will

    you, provideproofthat you die}indeed say itwould bewrong? (Past

    acts)

    M r.P resident, yo ur personal law yer,

    M r.

    H erb K almbach, entered

    a

    plea

    of

    guilty

    today to a

    crim inal charge

    of

    accepting 100,000

    in

    exchange fo r

    an

    ambassado-

    rial

    post, iny ou r capacity

    as

    President,

    you

    approve

    of

    am bassadors

    and

    send

    th e

    nominations

    to the

    Senate. Were

    y ou

    consulted

    in any

    manner

    on

    th is engagem ent

    and

    this

    contribution by M r. Kalmbach or anyone else in the White House .. .?

    (Past acts)

    Is it

    credible,

    can theA m erican people believe that yo ur closefriend, M r. Rebozo,

    for

    3

    years during which time

    you saw him

    w eekly sometimes, kept

    from

    you the

    fact tha the had 100,000incash

    from

    M r. Howard Hughes?Isthat credible?Is it

    credible that your personal attorney, Mr. Kalmbach, knew about this money for

    at least a yearand never told you about it?(Op inion question)

    O new ould thinkit adifferent matterto acknowledgeth esu bjectivityof

    opinions than

    to

    point

    out the

    disparity between reconstructions

    of

    past

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bibl

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    10/14

    152 Karen

    E

    Rosenblum

    events. Afte r all, opinions

    are

    only opinions;

    of

    course they

    are

    tied

    to

    social location. However, in these last three questions only the opinion

    question moves beyond a simple request for an account. In each of these

    three questions, background description tied Nixon to highly question-

    able activities.

    The first two

    past-act questions asked only

    for an

    event-

    recounting. In the opinion question, however, events as Nixon had

    depicted them were characterized as systematically illogical, i.e., incredi-

    ble . In the

    presence

    of

    such

    a

    reconstruction,

    the

    question itself

    is

    marked

    as

    a ploy.

    6

    Opinion questions

    may by

    their background recountings show events

    as variably reconstructed, then ask for an informed opinion about th e

    source or n atur e of such var iability. That is a qu alitatively

    different

    m atter than w hat is accomplished by past-act questions, w hich simply ask

    for

    an

    alternative recounting.

    In an

    opinion question,

    a

    foreground

    solicitation

    of an

    opinion

    m ay

    simply presume

    the

    legitimacy

    of a

    damning reconstruction offered by a background description. In the

    example

    above, the question was not whether Rebozo had told Nixon

    about the campaign contribution, but if such a reconstruction were

    credible .

    Thus,

    opinion questions display a potential for leveling charges

    which

    past-act

    questions do not. Both question-types m ay

    offer

    discredit-

    in g event-reconstructions, but past-act questions must ask whether the

    recon struction is accurate, w hile opinio n questions allow the description

    to stand w hile they ask for opinions about any number of other things.

    For

    example,

    in the

    following opinion questions

    the

    eminently noticeable

    background renders

    the

    foreground question merely ostensive.

    could

    you

    explain

    th e

    rationale

    of a law-and-order

    Administration covering

    up

    evidence, pr ima

    facia

    evidence, of high crimes an d misdemeanors?

    Apr i l

    21,

    1969,

    was a

    significant

    day for you in

    taxes

    and for the

    country, too.

    That

    is the notary date on the deed that allowed you to give your papers to the

    Government

    and pay

    just token taxes

    for two

    years.

    O n

    that same date,

    y ou had a

    tax

    reform message

    in which you

    said,

    and I

    quote: Special preferences

    in th e law

    permit

    far too

    many Amer icans

    to pay

    less than

    their fair

    share

    of taxes. Too

    many others bear too much

    of the tax

    burden. Now,

    M r.

    President,

    do you

    think

    you paid your

    fair

    share

    of

    taxes?

    The accusation in these opinion questions can be described as another

    com m on-sense

    charge: failing to stand as an expert by virtue of

    discrediting circumstances. In

    effect,

    this

    is an

    operational definition

    of

    untrustworthiness. y definition, questions w hich ask for

    one s

    opinion

    address

    an

    arena which ought

    to be a

    respondent s unchallengeable

    expertise more so even tha n questions about

    one s

    o wn past action. But

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bibl

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    11/14

    When

    is a

    question

    an

    accusation

    153

    with appropriate background recountings, these questions

    can

    discredit

    the same expertise they confirm, and thus accomplish an

    accusation.

    7

    ummary nd

    conclusions

    B yw h at markers does one determine th at a question lodges an accusation?

    Are there questions which stand as accusations even though they do not

    name

    a law or

    norm violation?

    Is the

    sense that

    one

    faces

    an

    accusation

    merely

    a matter of subjective appraisal? These are not questions of

    idiosyncratic

    interest. A ccusations

    are

    certainly

    as

    constitutive

    (Garfinkel

    1967;Cicourel 1973)

    as any

    other utterances

    they

    structure the

    perception

    ofevents and

    persons, they convey embedded instructions

    for

    understanding

    (Weider

    1973),

    and

    they

    create new

    practical circumstances (Mehan

    and

    Wood 1975: 140). While

    in an arena

    like

    the one

    examined here

    the

    consequences which follow from

    an

    accusation

    are

    dramatic

    and fairly

    accessible to observation, the consequentiality of accusatory questions in th e

    everyday

    world is no less substantive.

    Accusations cannot

    be

    reduced

    to any

    simple present/not present

    dichotomy even h urlin g an explicit charge of serious norm violation does

    not

    inevitably stand as an accusation. The premise here has been that

    accomplishing

    an utterance which serves as an accusation even without

    naming

    a supposed violation, requires an identifiable structuring of

    discourse. When are we likely to produce denials or acceptances, i.e., the

    second-part

    of the

    accusation adjacency pair? This investigation indicates

    that questions which

    ask

    about futures

    and

    make recommendations,

    and

    questions which simultaneously ask for opinions and discredit those

    opinions are reasonably construed as accusatory. Accusations may be

    accomplished

    by an

    attribution

    of

    pow er joined

    to a

    recommendation,

    or by

    an

    a t tr ibution

    of

    expertise coupled w i th description w hich specifically denies

    the possibility of expertise. This conclusion suggests that questions move

    toward accusation when they embody

    a

    self-cancelling

    opposition

    of

    elements the imputat ion and denial of expertise, the a t t r ibut ion of power

    and the imputat ion o f a failure to use it. It w ould also suggest th at accusatory

    questions

    are

    essentially rhetorical. Insofar

    as

    they debunk w hat they also

    confirm,

    accusatory questions are questions not meant to be answered.

    otes

    1. A

    fact

    highlighted by former Argentine President

    Leopoldo

    Galtieri in an interview w ith

    journalist Oriana Fallaci:

    4

    I accept your argument because

    you are a journalist,

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bibl

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    12/14

    154 KarenE Rosenblum

    Madame Journalist. If youwere not ajournalist, Ipromise youthat Iwouldn t have

    permitted you to say this' (Fallaci

    1982).

    2.

    Forexample, when Sarah McClendon, correspondent for the McClendon Wire Service,

    flatly and persistently contradicted Ronald Reagan in a 1982 presidential press

    conference, he r colleagues later responded

    with

    ridicule and a call fo r censure.

    3.

    Law/norm violation

    was

    conceived

    in the

    broadest possible terms. Questions were

    included if they pertained to the break-in at Democratic National Committee Head-

    quarters in the Watergate

    office

    building on June 17 , 1972; the alleged or confirmed

    activities to

    cover-up that break-in; allegations

    of

    criminal

    activity on the

    part

    of

    Nixon

    or his representatives; judicial proceedings or federal investigations regarding such

    activities; policy debates raised by such investigations; calls for resignation; or questions

    about Nixon s income

    tax

    returns

    and

    claimed deductions

    for the gift of his

    Vice

    Presidential papers.

    The

    transcripts

    as

    edited included

    33 of the

    original

    54

    question-

    answer

    sets. Eight

    of

    these

    33

    questions were double; i.e.,

    in one

    turn

    a

    speaker asked

    two

    discrete questions. Eachofthese double questionswascoded separately, increasing

    the numberto41.

    4. The

    classification

    'present

    action'

    was not

    applied

    to

    this material

    on the

    conviction that

    actionin theimmediate present isessentially inaccessible to analysis, i.e.,

    that

    in order

    to reflecton thepresentwemust renderitpast (Schutz 1967). Thusthequestion

    'Is

    Mr.

    Wilson, the attorney fo r Messrs. Haldeman and Ehrlichman, working with the White

    House

    or with you in

    concert

    in any way?

    while syntactically present tense

    has

    been

    classed

    as past

    action.

    5. All

    questions were classified

    in

    terms

    of

    their

    substance,

    rather than their syntactic form.

    Examplesof marginal classification are

    often

    helpful

    in illuminating coding procedures.

    For

    example,

    in

    'Have

    you

    ever considered

    the

    option

    of

    making

    that tape

    and

    transcript public?' or

    'Would

    yo u consider

    taking

    questions in a public forum? an

    argument might

    be

    made that these

    are

    solicitations

    of an

    opinion. However, reporters

    havenot actually asked for an opinion on releasing the tapesorpublic inquiries, but

    whether Nixon might actually do either of these at some future point. Thus, the

    questions are classed as

    future-action. When contrasted with,

    for

    example,

    'Is

    that

    correct, you are really not ruling the clemency out, and if so, why? a true

    solicitation

    of an

    opinion

    the

    distinction becomes clear.

    As a

    second instance

    of the

    problematics of coding:'Canyou and will yo u provide proof that you did indeed say it

    would be

    wrong?'

    is classed as past action because its central request is for an immediate

    recountingofpastaction,not apromise of a recountingin thefuture.Inall, however,

    there werefew cases in which consignment to a coding category was problematic.

    6. Note that the last question was so inflammatory that the reporter immediately

    proceeded with a'safer' past-act question:

    'And

    if this was a campaign contribution...

    who

    authorizedMr. Rebozo tocollect campaign contributions?

    7. A conclusion confirmed by Nixon s repeated denial of the need fo r expertise in response

    to opinion questions.

    eferen es

    Atkinson,

    J. Maxwell and

    Drew, Paul (1979). Order in

    Court:

    The

    organization

    o f verbal

    interaction in judicial settings London: MacMillan.

    Burton,

    Deirdre (1980).

    Dialogue and

    D iscourse London: Routledge

    and Kegan

    Paul.

    (1981).Analyzing spoken discourse.

    In

    Studies

    in

    DiscourseAnalysis Malcolm Coulthard

    and

    Mar t in

    Montgomery (eds.),61-81.London: RoutledgeandKegan Paul.

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bibl

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    13/14

    When

    is a

    question

    an

    accusation?

    155

    Cicourel,

    Aaron

    V. (1973). Cognitive Sociology.

    London: Penguin.

    Coul thard , Malcolm and Montgomery, Mart in (eds.) (1981). Studies in Discourse Analysis.

    London: Routledge and Kegan Paul .

    Drew, Paul (1978). Accusations:

    th e

    occasioned

    use of

    members knowledge

    of Religious

    geography in

    describing events. Sociology

    12,

    1-22.

    Ervin-Tripp, Susan (1970). Discourse agreement:

    H ow

    children answer quest ions.

    In

    Cognition and Language Learning.

    R .

    Hayes (ed.), 79-107.

    New

    York: Wiley.

    Fallaci, Oriana (1982). Th e A rgentine general w ho never fough t in a war: Interview w ith

    Argentine

    President Leopoldo Galtieri. Washington Post June 13.

    Fishman ,

    Mark (1980).

    Manufacturing the News.

    Aust in: Universi ty

    o f

    Texas

    Press.

    Garfinkel,

    Harold (1967).

    Studies

    in Ethnomethodology.

    Englewood

    Cliffs,

    N.J.: Prentice-

    Hall .

    (1972). Studies

    in the

    routine grounds

    of

    everyday activities.

    In Studies

    in

    Social

    Interaction. David Sudnow (ed.), 1-30.

    N ew

    York: Free Press.

    Gumperz, John (1982). Discourse Strategies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    H eat h, Shirley (1978).Teacher talk: Language in the classroom. In Lan guage in Education 9.

    Washington, D.C.: Center

    fo r

    Applied Linguistics.

    Joh nso n, George (ed.) (1978). The Nixon Presidential PressConferences. New York: Earl M .

    Coleman

    Enterprises.

    Kjolseth,

    Ro lf (1972). Mak ing sense: N atu ral language

    and

    shared knowledge.

    In Advances

    in

    th e Sociologyof Language

    II , Joshua Fishman (ed.), 50-76. T he

    Hague:

    Mouton.

    M c H ug h ,

    Peter (1970). A common-sense perception of deviance. In Recent Sociology 2,

    H an s

    P .

    Dreitzel (ed.), 152-181.

    New

    York: MacMillan.

    M eh a n , H u gh a nd

    Wood,

    Houston (1975). The Reality of Ethnomethodology. New York:

    Wiley.

    Molotch ,

    Harvey

    an d

    Lester, M arilyn (1974). New s

    a s

    purposive behavior:

    On the

    strategic

    use of rou tin e events, accidents, and scandals. American Sociological Review 39, 101-112.

    Philips, Susan

    U .

    (1982).

    T he

    social organization

    of

    quest ions

    and

    answers

    in

    cour t room

    discourse:

    A

    s tudy

    of

    changes

    of

    plea

    in an

    Arizona court.

    Paper

    delivered

    at the

    annual

    meeting of the American Sociological Association, San Francisco.

    Sacks, Harvey (1972).

    A n init ia l

    invest igat ion

    of the

    usability

    of

    conversat ional data

    fo r

    doing sociology.

    In Studies

    in

    Interaction

    David Sudnow (ed.), 31-74.

    New

    Y o r k : Free

    Press.

    (1974).

    On the analysabi l i ty o f stories by chi ldren. In Ethnomethodology. R oy

    Turner

    (ed.), 216-232. Middlesex: Penguin.

    Sacks, H arve y, Schegloff, Em anuel, a nd Jefferson, Gail (1974). A simplist systematics for the

    organization of

    t u r n - t a k i ng for conversat ion.

    Language 50,

    696-735.

    Schegloff,

    Em a nu e l (1968). Sequencing in conve rsat ional openings. American Anthropologist

    70, 1075-1095.

    (1979).

    No tes on a conversat ional practice: Fo rm ulat in g place. In

    Language

    an d

    Social

    Context.

    P. P.

    Giglioli (ed.),

    95-135.

    London: Penguin.

    Schegloff, Emanuel and Sacks, Harvey (1973). Opening up closings.

    Semiotica

    8 , 289-327.

    Schutz ,

    Alfred (1967). The Ph enomenologyof

    th e

    Social World.

    E va ns t on ,

    111 :

    Nor thwes tern

    Univers i ty

    Press.

    Sinclair,

    John

    an d

    Coulthard, Malcolm (1975).

    Toward

    an

    Analysis

    of

    Discourse:

    The

    English used by teachersandpupils.

    London: Oxford Universi ty Press.

    Turner ,R oy

    (1971).

    Words, ut terances, and act ivit ies. In Understanding Everyday

    Life.

    Jack

    Douglas (ed.),

    165-187.

    London: Rout ledge

    and

    Kegan Paul .

    (1974). I n t roduct ion . In Ethnomethodology. R oy Turner (ed.), 7-12. Middlesex: Pen guin.

    Weider ,

    D . Law rence (1973). Language and Social Re ality. T he Hague: Mouton.

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bibli

    Angemeldet | 193.196.8.103

    Heruntergeladen am | 02.04.13 15:54

  • 7/24/2019 Rosenblum Question Accusation

    14/14

    156

    aren E Rosenblum

    Karen E. Rose nblum is Associate Professor of Sociology and Anthr opo logy at George

    Mason Un iversity, Virginia.

    Her

    primary

    areasof

    research

    are sex and

    gender,

    the

    sociology

    of language, and

    deviance.

    Recent publications include The route to voluntary non-

    custody' (1985),

    'Care and

    autonomy:

    The

    conflicts

    in

    contemporary American gender

    (1986),

    and 'Revelatory or

    purposive? Making sense

    of a

    Female

    register'

    (1986).

    Bereitgestellt von | Institut fr Deutsche Sprache IDS) Bib


Recommended