+ All Categories
Home > Documents > SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS LINKING ORGANIZATIONAL CRISES...

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS LINKING ORGANIZATIONAL CRISES...

Date post: 02-Sep-2019
Category:
Upload: others
View: 13 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
36
SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS LINKING ORGANIZATIONAL CRISES AND REACTIVE STRATEGIES VIA DIMENSIONS OF LEGITIMACY Heiko Breitsohl SDP 2009-005 ISSN 1867-5352 © by the author
Transcript

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS

LINKING ORGANIZATIONAL CRISES AND REACTIVE STRATEGIES VIA DIMENSIONS OF LEGITIMACY

Heiko Breitsohl

SDP 2009-005 ISSN 1867-5352

© by the author

LINKING ORGANIZATIONAL CRISES AND REACTIVE STRATEGIES VIA DIMENSIONS OF LEGITIMACY

HEIKO BREITSOHL

University of Wuppertal Schumpeter School of Business and Economics

Wuppertal, 42097, Germany Tel: +49-202-439-2906 Fax: +49-202-439-3516

e-mail: [email protected]

ABSTRACT

Research on organizational crises has typically focused on case studies or measures for crisis

prevention, while the social dimension of crises has remained largely unexplored. In some rare

exceptions, differences in the efficacy of reactive strategies depending on the type of crisis have

been reported. However, the mechanisms behind these differences remain unclear. This paper

aims to fill this gap by arguing that organizational crises and effective reactions are linked by

different dimensions of legitimacy perceived by stakeholders. To this end, after elaborating on

the role of legitimacy in organizational crises, typologies of crises as well as reactive strategies

are presented, accompanied by propositions about possible links.

1

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

INTRODUCTION

Studies of organizational crises have been a fruitful stream of research for several decades

(e.g. Hermann, 1963; Pearson & Clair, 1998; Shrivastava, Mitroff, Miller, & Miglani, 1988;

Turner, 1976; Yu, Sengul, & Lester, 2008), from which two main foci can be identified. First,

there is a rich body of literature presenting case studies of crises and analyzing with great

precision the development of catastrophic events. Examples are the Tenerife air disaster (Weick,

1990), the loss of the space shuttle Challenger (Vaughan, 1990), the industrial accident at Bhopal

(Shrivastava, 1992), or the Mann Gulch fire (Weick, 1993). These studies typically discuss

organizational sensemaking (Weick, 1988, 1993), intraorganizational regulatory relationships and

social control (Vaughan, 1990), and information processing (Rudolph and Repenning, 2002) as

factors contributing to crises. Second, the causes thus identified have prompted other authors to

provide prescriptions for organizational design in order to prevent crises or minimize their effects

(e.g., Hedberg, Nystrom, and Starbuck, 1976; Turner, 1976; Smart and Vertinsky, 1977).

Only more recently has research on organizational crises directed its attention to the role of

stakeholders, and the social environment within which crises unfold and upon which the

organization depends for survival (Marcus & Goodman, 1991; Pearson & Clair, 1998; Pfarrer,

Decelles, Smith, & Taylor, 2008; Yu et al., 2008). More specifically, the interaction between

organization and stakeholders as well as the organization’s influence on stakeholder perception

has been recognized as a field worthy of inquiry. In their study of organizational signalling

during crises, Marcus and Goodman (1991) drew upon the literature on organizational impression

management (e.g. Benoit, 1995; Elsbach, 2003; Schlenker, 1980; Tedeschi, 1981). They showed

that two types of signals – accommodative vs. defensive – sent in communicating with investors

differed in successfully evoking positive responses, depending on the kind of crisis. This implies

2

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

that organizations may actively influence stakeholder perception of reactions to crises, possible

easing recovery through improved stakeholder support. Similarly, Elsbach (2001) suggested that

organizations adjust their reactions depending on how foreseeable the threatening event was.

Although these valuable findings point to the importance of the topic and provide a starting

point, the social dimension of organizational crises remains understudied. For instance, theory on

impression management provides a suitable background for framing organizational reactions and

the study by Marcus and Goodman (1991) employs it effectively by pointing out differences in

efficacy between reaction types. However, it remains unclear why some reactions are useful in

overcoming a certain type of crisis while others are not. Filling this gap by identifying the

mechanism behind stakeholder perception of crises and organizational reactions may deepen our

understanding of the crisis phenomenon. It may also provide a basis for improved

recommendations for effective crisis management.

Therefore the aim of this paper is to present “missing links” between organizational crises and

corresponding effective reactions. To this end, I argue that the construct central to stakeholder

perceptions of crises is that of organizational legitimacy (e.g. Aldrich & Fiol, 1994; Dowling and

Pfeffer, 1975; Scott, 1995; Suchman, 1995), in that crises are characterized by a sudden loss of

legitimacy. More specifically, I suggest that different dimensions of legitimacy are affected by

different types of crises and by different organizational reactions. Crisis management strategies

are effective if the reaction on the part of the organization matches the dimension of legitimacy

affected by the crisis. In order to develop my argument, I proceed by explaining the suitability of

legitimacy as the central construct in organizational crises and by identifying four dimensions of

legitimacy from the literature. I then turn to developing a typology of organizational crises and

presenting a set of propositions linking these types to dimensions of legitimacy. In the following

3

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

section, I present a second set of propositions on links between legitimacy and reactions, based

on a typology of the latter. The paper closes with a brief discussion and conclusion.

ORGANIZATIONAL LEGITIMACY AS THE CENTRAL CONSTRUCT IN ORGANIZATIONAL CRISES

Like all change in and around organizations, the evolution of crises is embedded in a social

system constituted by various groups of stakeholders (e.g. Freeman, 1984). Such groups may

comprise employees, customers, suppliers, or stockholders. With regard to events such as

accidents and product safety issues, a new group of stakeholders – victims – may be created

(Marcus & Goodman, 1991; Shrivastava et al., 1988). Furthermore, stakeholders have

considerable influence on the cause of crises (Milburn, Schuler, & Watson, 1983), and their

spreading across organizations (Yu et al., 2008).

More generally, organizations depend on positive evaluation by stakeholders because such

evaluations have significant influence on decisions concerning membership, market transaction,

and financing (Carroll & Hannan, 2000). Crises threaten positive stakeholder evaluation in that

they cause a loss of shared values and commonly held beliefs (Pearson & Clair, 1998) and may

be regarded as a violation of uncertainty-reducing social codes (Carroll & Hannan, 2000; Milburn

et al., 1983). Stakeholders may punish such transgressions by terminating transactions,

boycotting sales activities, quitting jobs, or withdrawing funds (Yu et al., 2008).

These aspects are usefully framed through the construct of organizational legitimacy (e.g.

Ashforth & Gibbs, 1990; Dowling & Pfeffer, 1975; Meyer & Scott, 1983; Suchman, 1995).

According to Suchman (1995: 574), an organization is deemed legitimate if its actions are

“desirable, proper, or appropriate within some socially constructed system of norms, values,

beliefs, and definitions”. Because crises are perceived by stakeholders as a breach of social codes,

4

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

i.e. norms, values, beliefs, and definitions, they may be characterized as an unexpected loss of

legitimacy. In reaction to the problematic event, stakeholders challenge organizational

performance or values (Hirsch & Andrews, 1984). From this perspective, other issues, such as

financial hardship, are mostly consequences of reduced legitimacy, while the latter lies at the core

of organizational crises. Exceptions to this rule are loss of life or of assets occurring in events

triggering organizational crises, e.g. accidents, although the assessment of such losses may differ

in the perception of stakeholders (Billings, Milburn, & Schaalman, 1980).

Dimensions of Organizational Legitimacy

The literature on organizational legitimacy contains several classifications, differing in

breadth, depth, and delineations. Aldrich and Fiol (1994) distinguished between cognitive

legitimacy, which reflects the degree to which knowledge about an organization has spread, and

sociopolitical legitimacy, which focuses on the acceptance of the organization “as appropriate

and right, given existing norms and laws” (Aldrich & Fiol, 1994: 648). In its highest form,

cognitive legitimacy implies acceptance of an organization as part of the sociocultural landscape

to the point of being ‘taken for granted’. Aldrich and Ruef (2006) refined sociopolitical

legitimacy by subdividing it into two components. Moral acceptance results from conformance

with cultural values and norms, while regulatory acceptance requires conformance with

governmental rules and regulations.

A similar concept was proposed by Scott (1995) based on the seminal work by DiMaggio and

Powell (1983), comprising three types of legitimacy corresponding to three pillars of institutions:

regulative, normative, and cognitive. Regulative legitimacy is awarded based on accordance with

legal or quasi-legal requirements, where possible sanctions coerce organizations into adherence

to rules. By comparison, normative legitimacy tends to have a deeper, moral base, and thus

5

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

conformance to it is likely to stem from intrinsic as well as extrinsic motivation. It mirrors

perceived appropriateness in terms of values and norms, which govern what is important, and

how things should be done, respectively. Normative legitimacy is achieved through social

obligation. Attributions of cognitive legitimacy are governed by shared definitions and frames of

reference. They reflect the extent to which an organization and its activities are culturally

supported and conceptually correct, i.e. taken for granted. Elements of cognitive evaluation

specify the meaning of structural and procedural features of organizations – basic characteristics

of social systems (Ruef & Scott, 1998). The similarity of the typologies developed by Aldrich

and colleagues (Aldrich & Fiol, 1994; Aldrich & Ruef, 2006; Hunt & Aldrich, 1996), and Scott

(1995) have caused some researchers to equate their respective dimensions of regulative,

normative, and cognitive character (e.g. Zimmerman & Zeitz, 2002).

The most fine-grained typology has been provided by Suchman (1995), who distinguished

twelve types of organizational legitimacy by combining three dimensions (pragmatic, moral, and

cognitive) with two temporal textures (episodic vs. continual) and two foci (actions vs. essence).

Accordingly, pragmatic legitimacy comprises exchange, influence, and dispositional legitimacy.

It is based on stakeholders’ self-interest calculations with respect to expected value provided by

the organization, responsiveness and commitment to their larger interests, or – in an

anthropomorphic fashion – the interests and character of the organization. Moral legitimacy

comprises consequential, procedural, structural, and personal legitimacy. It reflects a normative

evaluation of what the organization accomplishes, which technologies and procedures it employs

in doing so, its structure and capacity for producing results, and the charisma of individual

organizational leaders. Cognitive legitimacy comprises comprehensibility and taken-for-

grantedness. It stems from the extent to which organizational activities are predictable and

plausible, and are perceived to be permanent and inevitable. In addition to the various flavors of

6

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

legitimacy outlined above, some authors have proposed legitimacy types related to their specific

research context, such as industry-derived legitimacy (Zimmerman & Zeitz, 2002), or corporate

environmental legitimacy (Bansal & Clelland, 2004).

The dimensions of these models of organizational legitimacy are broadly similar, yet they

exhibit some differences in their emphasis and delineation. Suchman’s (1995) pragmatic type of

legitimacy is treated by Aldrich and Ruef (2006) as an aspect of organizational learning and

therefore excluded from their typology. These authors, in turn, consider moral and regulatory

acceptance to be sub-dimensions of sociopolitical legitimacy, which, according to Scott (1995),

are two separate types. Thus, Scott (1995) places relatively strong emphasis on legal aspects of

legitimacy, while these are only implicitly incorporated in Suchman’s (1995) approach.

Moreover, Deephouse and Suchman (2008) noted that there is some confusion concerning the

meaning of the term normative legitimacy, since some authors tend to restrict normative aspects

to those associated with particular ethics of formal professions, as opposed to norms ubiquitous in

social systems. In consonance with the proposition put forward by Deephouse and Suchman

(2008), the term is used in the more general sociological sense within the context of this paper.

Although conceptually distinct, interrelations and (non-)correlations between dimensions of

legitimacy may exist. Suchman (1995) explained that both moral and cognitive legitimacy require

cultural frameworks, while pragmatic legitimacy rests on individual utility calculations.

Therefore, offering tangible rewards to stakeholders may be effective in gaining pragmatic

legitimacy, but may be regarded unacceptable in moral and cognitive terms. Similarly, the

discursive nature of both pragmatic and moral legitimacy stands in contrast to cognitive

legitimacy, whose implicit taken-for-granted base may be threatened by public discussion.

Furthermore, Scott (1995) pointed out that assessing the legitimacy of a certain organization on

different dimensions may lead to varying conclusions. For instance, normative standards may

7

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

require an organization to break regulative prescriptions. Also, the borderline between legitimate

and illegitimate is fuzzy in cases such as street gangs or organized crime, which are culturally

constituted yet illegal, or pornography, which is often legal but morally tainted (Solari, 2007).

Organizations suffering from ‘core-stigma’ due to their very nature fall into this category

(Hudson, 2008). However, the focus of this paper lies on organizations which are free from core-

stigma, but are exposed to ‘event-stigma’ resulting from a crisis.

It should be noted that there has been some debate as to how much influence an organization

has on its own legitimacy. While some authors propose that a single organization is relatively

weak and passive compared to the dynamics shaping its industry or economic sector (e.g. Dacin,

1997; DiMaggio & Powell, 1983; Meyer & Rowan, 1977; Oliver, 1991; Zucker, 1987), others

emphasize a wide variety of activities an organization can employ to actively influence its

environment and, consequently, the way it is perceived and attributed legitimacy (e.g. Aldrich &

Fiol, 1994; Ashforth & Gibbs, 1990; Dowling & Pfeffer, 1975; Suchman, 1995). Like Suchman

(1995), I take a middle road between those two approaches, conceptualizing the organization as

faced with external pressures and equipped with a repertory of strategies for influencing its

stakeholders. Institutional environments do shape the course of an organization’s evolution and

create boundaries of action for the organization, but not independently of it. Conversely,

stakeholders are susceptible to organizational legitimation activities to some extent, albeit a

limited one.

As described in the preceding portion of this paper, the respective literature suggests that

organizational crises are closely tied to a loss in reputation, built on shared values and beliefs

(Pearson & Clair, 1998), and a violation of social codes (Milburn et al., 1983; Yu et al., 2008).

Although the constructs of reputation and legitimacy may by considered distinct (Deephouse &

Carter, 2005), I follow Aldrich and Fiol (1994) in subsuming reputation under the antecedents of

8

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

organizational legitimacy, the definition of which comprises the aspects just noted. According to

various authors, organizational legitimacy consists of three dimensions (Scott, 1995; Suchman,

1995), although some scholars diverge slightly from that division (Aldrich & Ruef, 2006).

Treating moral (Suchman, 1995) and normative (Scott, 1995) legitimacy as equivalent, I employ

a concept of organizational legitimacy consisting of the four major dimensions identified in this

section, namely pragmatic, moral, cognitive, and regulative legitimacy. The remainder of this

paper is dedicated to providing a summary of the possible relationships between crisis types,

dimensions of legitimacy, and organizational reactions, and to creating a series of propositions on

those relationships.

CRISES TYPES AND DIMENSIONS OF LEGITIMACY

Types of Organizational Crises

Organizational crises are high-impact, low-probability events (Hermann, 1963). They prevent

the organization from reaching its goals and may threaten its very survival (Milburn et al., 1983;

Pearson & Clair, 1998). Thus, they are associated with the possibility of great loss (Billings et al.,

1980), while being ambiguous with respect to their causes, effects, and means of resolution

(Hermann, 1963; Pearson & Clair, 1998). Crises restrict organizational capabilities of

information processing and decision making by inducing individual stress, resulting in

misunderstandings and failures (Hermann, 1963; Smart & Vertinsky, 1984; Turner, 1976).

This rather broad characterization of crises allows the inclusion of a wide variety of events.

Consequently, the literature on organizational crises offers extensive lists of incidents related to

crises (e.g. Pauchant & Mitroff, 1992; Pearson & Clair, 1998), while few scholars have attempted

to develop consistent typologies. Exceptions include Marcus and Goodman (1991), Milburn et al.

9

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

(1983), Pauchant and Mitroff (1992), and Shrivastava et al. (1988). Interestingly, these typologies

are diverse in scope, dimensionality, delineation, and detail.

Milburn et al. (1983) presented a typology consisting of three dichotomous dimensions: the

degree of control the organization has over its external environment (high vs. low), the general

perceived character of the crisis (positive vs. negative), and the degree of organizational

susceptibility (high vs. low). This results in eight different types of crises, although the authors

did not specify these types or provide examples. Rather than a framework for describing or

analyzing past or future events, the model is intended as an instrument for deducing the

likelihood of an organization facing a severe crisis, given its characteristics as positioned in the

model space. It is also used to form hypotheses about antecedent aspects of organizations and

crises.

Pauchant and Mitroff (1992) took a very different approach by inductively developing a two-

dimensional concept of crises. Based on a survey of corporate managers, they identified six

clusters of crises, namely ‘external economic attacks’ (e.g. extortion), ‘external information

attacks’ (e.g. counterfeiting), ‘breaks’ (e.g. product defects), ‘occupational health diseases’,

‘psycho’ (e.g. copycats), and ‘megadamage’ (e.g. environmental accidents). These groups are

positioned on two dimensions denoted ‘technical/economic’ vs. ‘human/social’, and ‘normal’ vs.

‘severe’. The former specifies the underlying cause of a crisis, although the authors point out that

technical defects and social problems may be intertwined. The latter describes the extent to which

the “underlying cause can be explained more easily in terms of everyday or ‘normal’ human

behavior” (Pauchant & Mitroff, 1992: 29). Accordingly, analyzing crises types at the ‘severe’ end

of the spectrum, such as the ‘psycho’ cluster, requires “a great deal of psychological

sophistication” (Pauchant & Mitroff, 1992: 29).

10

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

Shrivastava et al. (1988) focused on events associated with industrial production. They

differentiated between production-related and consumption-related harm. Production-side harm

includes risks linked to the production system (personal or transportation accidents, system

failures), the production environment (e.g. occupational diseases), and post-production effects

(e.g. chronic pollution). Consumption-side harm includes risks linked to product use (misuse,

sabotage, defects), and side-effects of consumption (e.g. diseases such as smoking-related

cancer). These authors stressed that the events noted in their typology are merely the triggers of

the actual crises, which may consist of large-scale damage to human life and the environment as

well as large economic and social costs. Furthermore, they identified two sets of causes. First,

human, organizational, and technological factors, such as operator errors or sabotage, policy

failures, or faulty design, respectively, lead to the triggering event. Second, due to the interaction

of these causes with regulatory, infrastructural, and preparedness failures, triggering events

escalate into crises.

The classification presented by Marcus and Goodman (1991) adopts a shareholder perspective

by distinguishing between three crises types (accidents, product safety and health incidents, and

scandals) in terms of their deniability, and the ease of identification of victims. Thus, accidents

have an easily identifiable group of victims and are plausibly deniable, because they occur

unexpectedly. Conversely, scandals have a diffuse group of victims and are difficult to deny since

their causes are misdeeds within the organization. Product safety and health incidents lie between

the other two types on both dimensions.

In sum, the literature on organizational crises offers relatively few and very different attempts

at categorizing threatening events. While Milburn et al. (1983) deductively assess the general

exposure of an organization to crises – without regard to the character of the event – the other

typologies outlined above identify common traits according to which crises may be classified.

11

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

Pauchant and Mitroff (1992) cover a wide range of crises and group them into several clusters.

However, the consistence of this approach appears to be questionable. For instance, it remains

unclear why rumors are both highly technical and highly human in nature, and why they are

crises at all. Moreover, it appears implausible why events such as sabotage or tampering are

psychological in nature and particularly difficult to analyze, while boycotts are primarily

economic. By contrast, Shrivastava et al. (1988) offer a very stringently organized catalogue of

triggering events, in combination with suggestions on the causal structure of crises resulting from

such events. Unfortunately, their typology is restricted to industrial crises, ignoring problems

such as corruption and other illegal arrangements. Marcus and Goodman (1991) incorporate these

types, resulting in a comprehensive – albeit more coarse-grained – classification, which is able to

accommodate the types described by Shrivastava et al. (1988) in addition to covering non-

industrial crises. Their approach is also unique in that it takes a stakeholder perspective,

accounting for the social and psychological aspects of organizational crises.

Within the context of this paper, I employ a classification combining elements of those

summarized above. It is visualized in Figure 1. Like Marcus and Goodman (1991), I divide crises

into three broadly delineated types: scandals, product safety and health incidents, and accidents.

Adapting one criterion used by Milburn et al. (1983), I classify them in terms of the degree of

control the organization has over the triggering event, in principle, reflecting stakeholder

expectations of organizational control (Salancik & Meindl, 1984). Furthermore, I follow

Pauchant and Mitroff (1992) in distinguishing between two primary causes: human/social, and

technical.

12

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

FIGURE 1

Classification of crises

Degree of

control

high

low

Primary cause

human/social technical

Scandals

Product safety and health incidents

Accidents

Scandals comprise crises triggered by, e.g. bribery, insider trading, price-fixing, or sexual

harassment. They allow a relatively high degree of control since their roots often lie within the

organization, among its members. It is therefore generally feasible to identify the perpetrators and

possibly even prevent the crisis. Scandals have primarily human and social causes, since they are

based on member interaction and are influenced by social processes (e.g. Ashforth & Anand,

2003; Finney & Lesieur, 1982). Accidents comprise events categorized by Shrivastava et al.

(1988) as related to the production system, such as explosions, airplane crashes, or the sinking of

ships. By contrast, they are much more difficult to control, since they are often caused by the

complexity of tightly coupled systems (Perrow, 1984a). While failures of such systems are

relatively rare, even when compared with other crises, they do occur, are difficult to prevent, and

13

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

are often devastating in their effects. Due to these reasons, accidents are also primarily technical

in nature, although flawed human perception and decision making may contribute to their

development. Finally, product safety and health incidents comprise post-production effects and

consumption-side harms (Shrivastava et al., 1988), like sabotage, product defects and associated

recalls, and production-related pollution. Since these are not as rare and causally ambiguous as

accidents, but feature a combination of internal (production systems) and external (e.g.

consumers) influences, they are in the mid range in terms of controllability. The same is true for

the second criterion, due to their inherent mixture of technical and human causes.

Proposed Links

A few authors have pointed to a connection between crises and reputation, shared values and

beliefs, and social codes (Pearson & Clair, 1998; Milburn et al., 1983; Yu et al., 2008), which

may be subsumed under organizational legitimacy. Yet, in general, little is known about the exact

nature of this relationship. Hudson (2008), in contrasting persistent lack (core stigma) against

temporary loss of legitimacy (event stigma), proposed that core stigma is primarily related to

sociopolitical (Aldrich & Fiol, 1994) or moral, rather than pragmatic or cognitive (Suchman,

1995) legitimacy. Indeed, as Hudson (2008) elaborated, an organization may be pragmatically

and cognitively legitimate, yet core-stigmatized. While this description contributes to the

understanding of ‘spoiled image’ (Sutton & Callahan, 1987) of organizations, it remains unclear

how dimensions of legitimacy relate to event-stigma associated with crises. I therefore turn to

discussing possible links.

Scandals, such as those involving bribery, corruption or insider trading, are almost invariably

connected to ‘white-collar crime’ (e.g. Fisse & Braithwaite, 1983). The discovery of such deeds

is typically followed by criminal investigation, since they violate rules and regulation governing

14

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

bidding procedures or securities trading. Furthermore, scandals often evoke public outcry since

they point to greed among leading personnel in the ranks of the organization. They also put into

question the ability of the organization’s structures and processes to adequately police its

employees’ decisions (Marcus & Goodman, 1991). Since those responsible for the scandal often

draw significant financial benefit from their actions, they also appear as primarily focused on

their own well-being instead of their stakeholders’. Note that the problematic behaviors

associated with scandals are relatively unrelated to specific products, services, or industries.

Product safety and health incidents, such as product defects or sabotage, have a weaker legal

connotation than scandals, although they may violate standards of quality or consumer protection

laws. Moreover, they may create legal action from victims of faulty or tampered products.

However, they primarily call into question the organization’s status as an exchange partner, i.e.

provider of economic benefit. They also challenge previous perceptions of the organization’s

trustworthy, honest and reliable character. In a related vein, product safety and health incidents

cast doubt on organizational predictability and the capacity to deliver goods or services at a

constant level of quality.

Accidents, such as explosions or oil spills, are typically complete surprises to all parties

involved. They occur in tightly coupled systems and have their causes in the inherent complexity

of modern production or transportation technology. Therefore, accidents tend to be relatively free

from perceptions of moral injustice or deliberate wrongdoing, although there may be inquiries

into the efficacy of procedures for accident prevention. Neither do they cast serious doubt on the

organization’s disposition as a ‘good’ exchange partner. They do, however, evoke questions

concerning predictability and permanence, especially since accidents can be devastating enough

to jeopardize the organization’s existence (e.g. Sipika & Smith, 1993). Thus,

15

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

Proposition 1. Scandals affect regulative, normative (Scott, 1995) and

moral (Suchman, 1995) legitimacy more than pragmatic and cognitive

legitimacy.

Proposition 2. Product safety and health incidents affect pragmatic

legitimacy more than moral/normative, regulative, and cognitive

legitimacy.

Proposition 3. Accidents affect cognitive legitimacy more than

moral/normative, pragmatic, and regulative legitimacy.

CLASSES OF REACTIONS AND DIMENSIONS OF LEGITIMACY

A Typology of Reactions to Crises

Organizations react to crises in a variety of ways, which are all aimed at influencing

stakeholder perceptions of the organization’s role within the context of the crisis. One stream of

research which has contributed to the understanding of such tactics is impression management

theory (e.g. Elsbach, 2003; Schlenker, 1980; Tedeschi, 1981). Impression management can be

defined as “the conscious or unconscious attempt to control images that are projected in real or

imagined social interactions” (Schlenker, 1980: 6). Grounded in social psychology, the concept

originally focused on individual and intraorganizational behavior (e.g. Giacalone & Rosenfeld,

1989, 1991) and has since been applied to organizations in a variety of studies (e.g. Aldrich &

Fiol, 1994; Arndt & Bigelow, 2000; Bansal & Clelland, 2004; Elsbach, 2003; Elsbach & Sutton,

1992; Ginzel, Kramer, & Sutton, 1993; Lounsbury & Glynn, 2001).

The literature on impression management provides numerous typologies of activities directed

at influencing stakeholder perception (e.g. Arndt & Bigelow, 2000; Elsbach, 1994, 2003; Ginzel

et al., 1993; Schlenker, 1980; Suchman, 1995; Sutton & Callahan, 1987; Tedeschi & Melburg,

1984), varying in terms of their scale. For example, while Scott and Lyman (1968), and Tedeschi

(1981) distinguish between two strategies – excuses and justifications –, Schlenker (1980)

16

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

identifies no less than seven different activities. Moreover, the typologies differ in their scope, i.e.

the kinds of behavior they cover. Some authors focus on verbal accounts, such as denials or

excuses (e.g. Elsbach, 2003), whereas others include withdrawing from communication (Sutton

& Callahan, 1987) or restructuring (Pfeffer, 1981; Suchman, 1995). What’s more, there is no

consistent distinction between different impression management activities. Note how Suchman

(1995) named explanations as one kind of technique besides excuses and justifications.

According to Elsbach (2003), however, the latter two are subtypes of explanations. Similarly,

Elsbach (1994) described denials as accounts negating either the involvement in an event or the

occurrence of the event itself. However, theses two variants may alternatively be considered two

distinct tactics (Sutton & Callahan, 1987).

Several authors used a rather general classification by distinguishing between defensive and

accommodative behaviors, where defensive means avoiding association with a threatening event,

while accommodative means accepting responsibility for it (Elsbach, 1994, 2003; Marcus &

Goodman, 1991; see also Sutton & Callahan, 1987). Although this is a useful distinction, it does

not account for the various different types of reactions listed in the typologies noted above. More

specifically, there may be more than one step between an organization denying all possible

connections with a problematic incident, and the organization assuming full responsibility and

offering compensation for the damage inflicted on victims.

In order to obtain a more fine-grained overview of organizational reactions to crises, I

developed a new typology, drawing upon the measure used by Marcus and Goodman (1991) in

their study of the conflict between shareholders and victims during crises. These authors

operationalized accommodative tactics as “those in which managers accepted responsibility,

admitted to the existence of problems, and attempted to take actions to remedy a situation”

(Marcus & Goodman, 1991: 291). Based on this definition, I propose four classes of strategies,

17

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

defined by three dichotomous criteria. First, in its communication with stakeholders, the

organization may or may not agree that a problem, i.e. a crisis, exists to begin with. Second, if

there is a problem, the organization may or may not assume responsibility for the crisis. Finally,

if responsibility for the problem is accepted, the organization may or may not actively attempt to

solve it. The three criteria and the four resulting classes of reactions are shown in Table 1, along

with the individual reaction types within the classes, which are described below. Within each

class, individual strategies are ordered according to the same general logic, i.e. from resistant to

conforming (Oliver, 1991; Suchman, 1995).

Evasive strategies. The first class of reactions is employed if the organization seeks to avoid any

connection to the threatening event and the related stakeholder disapproval (Schlenker, 1980). It

may therefore attempt to retreat from the scene, conceal the problematic event, or simply deny

that it occurred. The most comprehensive form, retreat – or withdrawal (Sutton & Callahan,

1987) – entails not commenting on the unfolding crisis at all in order to “hide away until the

storm passes” (Schlenker, 1980: 134). This strategy offers the general advantage of providing the

organization with some time needed to gather information and resources, and to form a more

elaborate response. However, since stakeholders may be aware of the organization deliberately

refraining from communication, this may be effective only over short time spans. Extending

retreat over a longer period of time can be counterproductive, as will be discussed for several

reaction types later in this section of the paper.

Concealment is aimed at avoiding the necessity to cope with the confrontation following a

negative event. It may take passive forms aimed at leaving stakeholders in their ignorance, such

as avoiding situations in which the crisis must be revealed. Concealment may also be applied

more actively by providing deceptive information (Sutton & Callahan, 1987). For instance, the

vast majority of organizations studied by Arndt and Bigelow (2000) strived not to draw attention

18

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

TABLE 1

Reactions to organizational crises

Organization

acknowledges problem

accepts responsibility

actively seeks resolution

Strategies

Evasive no no no Retreat

Concealment

Denial

Defensive yes no no Disassociation

Excuses

Defining

Appreciative yes yes no Explanation

Justification

Apologies

Accommodative yes yes yes Restitution

Divorce

Restructuring

to the fact that they were restructuring, and, even when describing their new structure, kept quiet

about its novelty to the industry. If successful, concealing a crisis may allow the organization to

circumvent negative evaluation by stakeholders. Since this is rather unlikely, it may at least serve

as temporary measure, similarly to the tactic of retreat.

If stakeholders do suspect that a threatening event has occurred, the organization may choose

to deny the event (Suchman, 1995). The strategy of denial is a borderline case in that it may be

used in two different versions, asserting that the even did not occur, or that the organization had

nothing to do with (Schlenker, 1980; Elsbach, 1994, 2003).

19

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

Defensive strategies. The second class of reactions is employed if the organization does not

question whether a crisis is occurring, but refuses to accept responsibility for the event and its

consequences. To this end, the organization may disassociate itself from the crisis, make excuses

for its actions, or attempt to define the situation in more favorable terms. In the case of

disassociation, the goal is not to keep stakeholders from discovering the threatening event, but

from drawing a connection between the event and the organization (Tedeschi & Melburg, 1984;

see also Cialdini, 1989). This may be attempted by shifting the blame to other problematic

entities, such as business partners or an identifiable group of employees (e.g. Benoit & Brinson,

1994). Disassociation may therefore be considered one form of defense of noncausation

(Schlenker, 1980) or pleading lack of control over the situation (Benoit, 1995).

Excuses are aimed at downplaying the organization’s responsibility for the negative

consequences of an event (Scott & Lyman, 1968; Tedeschi & Melburg, 1984). Tedeschi and

Riess (1981) offered a typology of excuses. Accordingly, the actors may claim they did not

intend to cause the negative effects and were mistaken, uninformed, or deceived about the

potential results of their actions. They may also claim that the action causing the harm done was

beyond their control. Finally, excuses may take the form of claiming not to be the true

perpetrator, including the use of alibis and witnesses. For instance, the environment may be

blamed as a reaction to negative evaluation (Staw, McKechnie, & Puffer, 1983).

Alternatively, the organization may avoid the necessity to admit any wrongdoing through the

strategy of defining the situation as apparently discrediting, but really harmless (Sutton &

Callahan, 1987). This may be done by claiming that there is a general misunderstanding of the

crisis, i.e. that the situation is not as normatively objectionable as it seems to be. Bankruptcy, for

example, has been found to be defined by affected companies as an every-day phenomenon

20

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

independent of misconduct. Another type of defining involves the organization describing its

individual case as unique, implying that the violated norm does not apply.

Appreciative strategies. The third class of reactions requires that the organization accepts the

existence as well as its responsibility for a crisis, but does not offer any remedial action. Instead,

it reverts to explaining, justifying, or apologizing for its involvement. Explanations may be used

if responsibility for the event cannot be successfully avoided. A general fit with stakeholders’

norms and expectations may be preserved by constructing an account conveying that the

unfolding situation is at least understandable. For instance, Perrow (1984b) described how

accidents are labeled as ‘discrete’ in order to make them appear unfortunate but unavoidable in an

imperfect world, a tactic which provides the image of some conformance while avoiding the

necessity to redesign the system as a whole.

Justifications go one step further by providing “over-riding or compensating positive

consequences as outweighing the harm done” (Tedeschi & Melburg, 1984: 35). They signal

acceptance of the responsibility for an event – at least to some extent – but not for its implications

(Schlenker, 1980; Scott & Lyman, 1968). This is done by downplaying the negative connotations

of the event in an attempt to realign the organization’s image with stakeholder beliefs (Elsbach,

2003; Suchman, 1995). Justifications may take many forms, such as appeals to organizational

rules or missions, economic doctrines, or standards of equality (Tedeschi & Riess, 1981).

Apologies offer a confession of responsibility for the incident. Unlike justifications, they

include regret, the acceptance of punishable wrongdoing and possible restitution (Elsbach, 2003;

Tedeschi & Melburg, 1984). The purpose of apologies is to suggest that the organization is ill-

represented by the negativity of the crisis. They serve to redress the past, combined with a

21

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

promise of more appropriate behavior in the future (Schlenker, 1980). Due to their

comprehensive nature, apologies are the most conforming type of appreciative strategies.

Accommodative strategies. Finally, the fourth class of reactions involves the organization

taking full responsibility for the crisis and responding by offering restitution, divorcing from

problematic entities, or modifying its structure or processes. Restitution may be offered to

persons, groups, or organizations negatively affected by the crises as a form of prosocial behavior

(Tedeschi & Melburg, 1984). As a swift response to the perceived violation associated with

crises, this may provide some compensation, although this kind of relief may be mandated as a

result of legal action (James & Wooten, 2006; Marcus & Goodman, 1991; Shrivastava et al.,

1988). Charitable giving may also be considered one type of restitution after a threatening event

(Pfarrer et al., 2008).

While offering restitution to affected stakeholders may effectively provide temporary remedy

after a crisis, the strategy of divorce is more fundamental. The most common form of divorce is

executive replacement, which, in some cultures, has become institutionalized to the point of

taken-for-grantedness (Marcus & Goodman, 1991). Other subjects of divorce may be

organizational units or geographic locales, whereby the organization attempts to increase the

perceived – or actual – distance between itself and the problematic entity (Suchman, 1995).

Instead of actively trying to get rid of reminders of inappropriate situations of the past, the

threatened organization may also restructure by changing organizational processes or creating

monitors (Milburn et al., 1983; Suchman, 1995). These approaches signal some degree of

commitment to the future since they are aimed at preventing the reoccurrence of crises through

optimized structures and processes. Even in the event of another threatening situation, units such

22

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

as monitors or watchdogs suggest a more effective handling of the ensuing challenges (Pfarrer et

al., 2008; Pfeffer, 1981; Suchman, 1995).

Symbolism and associated risks in reactions to crises

An aspect related to the strategies outlined above which has received some attention is the

role of symbolism in and around organizations (e.g. Pfeffer, 1981; Pondy, Frost, Morgan, &

Dandridge, 1983; Russ, 1991). Organizational attempts to influence their evaluation by

stakeholders vary in their degree of symbolism, i.e. their degree of association with attitudes,

values, and perceptions vs. physical referents (Pfeffer, 1981). If stated as a dichotomy (Ashforth

& Gibbs, 1990), substantive management involves real change in goals, structures, and processes,

while symbolic management seeks to appear consistent with social values and expectations. Note

that most of the reaction types described above would be classified as symbolic activities, since

they focus primarily on stakeholder perception of the crisis and the organization’s involvement.

In fact, the accommodative class of reactions is the only one associated with actual change.

Moreover, symbolic and substantive actions may be combined in what is known as the practice of

decoupling (Meyer & Rowan, 1977; e.g. Fiss & Zajac, 2006; Westphal & Zajac, 1994, 1998). By

separating symbolic reactions from substantive ones, organizations can create a buffer against

external pressures on its structures and processes (Sutton & Galunic, 1996). Aside from the

greater effort required for substantive change, organizations often resort to symbolic action

because their actual influence is limited. Due to their dependence on external resource providers,

focusing on the perception of conformance is much more effective than attempting real

accommodation (Pfeffer & Salancik, 1978; Pfeffer, 1981).

Due to their partially symbolic nature, some of the possible reactions to threatening events

carry inherent risks which may severely curtail the intended effects of crisis management.

23

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

Specifically, these risks are associated with stakeholder trust, perceptions of control, and

conflicting demands and expectations. First, trust is essential to stakeholder relations and

organizational survival (e.g. Aldrich & Fiol, 1994). However, the evasive strategies outlined

above, namely retreat, denial, and concealment, are ‘double-edged’ in this respect (Ashforth &

Gibbs, 1990). Although they may serve the organization well in buying some time, stakeholder

demands have to be confronted eventually. A significant loss of trust – if not withdrawal from the

relationship – may ensue once stakeholders discover that the initial reaction was merely tactical

and misrepresentative in nature (Ginzel et al., 1993; Schlenker, 1980; Turner, 1976).

Second, organizations are expected to exert control over their environment. Some defensive

strategies, particularly disassociation and excuses, imply a lack of control on part of the

organization, although retaining an image of control, even in an effectively uncontrollable

environment, may buffer against negative evaluation by stakeholders (Salancik & Meindl, 1984;

Sutton & Callahan, 1987). Finally, organizations typically interact with diverse groups of

stakeholders, voicing different, sometimes conflicting demands and expectations. For instance,

some audiences may be generally more sympathetic, while others may be more antagonistic.

Depending on their respective power over and relationship with the organization, this may create

a dilemma in deciding whose interests should be addressed primarily (Ginzel et al., 1993; Marcus

& Goodman, 1991).

Proposed Links

Although a few scholars have investigated strategies of influence within the context of

problematic legitimacy (e.g. Ashforth & Gibbs, 1990; Elsbach, 1994, 2001; Elsbach & Sutton,

1992; Suchman, 1995), most of these works treat legitimacy as a one-dimensional construct. In a

rare exception, Suchman (1995) proposed that certain types of strategies for repairing legitimacy

24

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

may apply to specific dimensions of it. Accordingly, and in terms of my classification presented

above, denial and the creation of monitors address pragmatic legitimacy, excuses, justifications,

and restructuring can be employed to rebuild moral legitimacy, and explanation are useful in

defending cognitive legitimacy. Note that denial is an evasive reaction, whereas creating

monitors is rather accommodative. Similarly, excuses are defensive, justifications are

appreciative, and restructuring is accommodative. Thus, while there may be differences in the

compatibility of specific strategies with dimensions of legitimacy, there is no tendency as to what

class of behaviors may be suitable to restore a given aspect of legitimacy. However, Suchman

(1995) does not further explain the assignment of strategies to legitimacy dimensions as just

summarized, and his review does not comprise all legitimation strategies described in the

literature. Therefore, I turn to a discussion which covers all four dimensions of legitimacy as well

as the strategies classified above.

Evasive reactions to threatening events, such as retreat, concealment, and denial, are aimed at

avoiding confrontation with negative evaluation by stakeholders. This is attempted by refraining

from providing information, dissemination deceptive information, or repudiating any connection

to the event. In other words, while retreat may also be useful in buying time, evasive tactics are

employed in order to avoid any public discussion of the problematic incident. If such discussion

cannot be prevented, the goal is to restrict information, so as to keep stakeholder re-evaluation at

a shallow level, as opposed to in-depth investigation of the organization’s core values and raison-

d’être. Moreover, these reactions aid in upholding the impression that the organization is acting

in congruence with current rules (Scott, 1995). In consonance with Suchman (1995), I therefore

suggest that denial, as well as the two other evasive strategies – retreat and concealment – are

primarily linked to pragmatic, as well as regulative, legitimacy. It should be noted, however, that

25

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

evasive strategies carry the risk of backfiring if perceived as misrepresentations (Sutton &

Callahan, 1987; Ashforth & Gibbs, 1990).

Defensive reactions, such as disassociation, excuses, and defining, generally seek to distance

the organization from the threatening events, if the latter cannot be ignored or denied. In contrast

to evasive reactions, however, defensive strategies are more diverse in their specific goals.

Disassociation may be used to avoid stakeholder perceptions of there being anything problematic

about the organization – similar to denying connections to the crises. In addition, they are to

prevent ‘contagion’ of negativity from the event to the organization. Thus, disassociation may be

pragmatic, moral, and regulative in effect. Excuses are used to downplay this negativity by

appealing to a lack of intention or control. They are, as Suchman (1995) suggested, aimed at

moral legitimacy. The third defensive tactic, defining, is applied if the organization wants to

affect the connection between stakeholders’ understanding and evaluation of the situation,

making it appear more positive. It may therefore influence moral and cognitive legitimacy.

Appreciative reactions, such as explanations, justifications, and apologies, require the

acceptance of at least some responsibility of the crises. In this vein, explanations are an attempt to

make the organization’s conduct at least understandable, i.e. cognitively compatible (Suchman,

1995), by delivering a plausible account of what happened. Justifications have a more normative

flavor in that they stress positive connotations of the event, trying to outweigh any perceived

inappropriateness. Even more conformant, apologies signal full recognition of unacceptability

and regret of past actions, combined with an indication of possible remedial activities. Thus, they

are mostly aimed at reestablishing the organization’s moral integrity and rightness.

Accommodative reactions, such as restitution, divorce, and restructuring, go beyond the

acceptance of responsibility by comprising active responses to the crises, directing attention away

from the inglorious past to a more promising future. Restitution affects both moral and pragmatic

26

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

legitimacy, since it provides compensation for the damage done, while also conveying the image

of an organization striving to ‘make things right again’. Divorce, usually through the replacement

of personnel, signals the parting from old, ‘bad’, illegal influences, and reorientation towards

more morally appropriate, and regular, behavior (Suchman, 1995; Scott, 1995). Finally, changing

procedures and practices, and creating monitors, is aimed at demonstrating that the organization

posseses the capacity to perform its tasks in a morally acceptable and rule-conforming manner

and therefore should be considered a predictable exchange partner worthy of support. Thus,

Proposition 4. Pragmatic legitimacy is primarily influenced by evasive

and some accommodative reactions.

Evasive tactics keep stakeholder attention away from more fundamental reassessment and

thereby preserve taken-for-grantedness, while carrying the risk of undermining legitimacy.

Accommodative activities, particularly restitution and the creation of monitors, direct attention

back to a transactional level when general acceptability has been restored.

Proposition 5. Moral legitimacy is primarily influenced by the more

resistant defensive reactions, the more conformant appreciative reactions,

and by accommodative reactions.

Defensively, disassociation aims at reducing perceived negativity, while appreciatively,

justifications and apologies try to emphasize positive connotations of the event. Restitution,

divorce, and the reconfiguration of procedures suggest a favorable outlook.

Proposition 6. Cognitive legitimacy is primarily influenced by the least

resistant and the least conformant, i.e. the most neutral, reactions.

Defining and explanations seek to increase the understandability of the situation, preserving

compatibility to mental frames of reference and avoiding normative reevaluation. To some

extent, restructuring may additionally suggest increased future predictability and permanence.

27

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

Proposition 7. Regulative legitimacy is primarily influenced by evasive,

the most resistant defensive, and the most conformant reactions.

Retreat, concealment, denial, and disassociation maintain the perception that the

organizations’ actions adhere to established rules. Divorce and restructuring signal the departure

from illegal entities and reorientation towards conformance.

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION

The two preceding sections of this paper posit the existence of links between types of

organizational crises and dimensions of legitimacy on one hand, and links between dimensions of

legitimacy and reactions to crises on the other. From this, it follows that certain ways of reacting

to crises may be particularly effective for certain types of crises, connected by the respective

dimensions of legitimacy they affect. Although the literature on both organizational crises and

perception management is extensive, there are few sources combining the two subjects. What’s

more, none of them explicitly incorporate organizational legitimacy as a framework. Therefore, I

shall provide a brief discussion of how my argument matches extant findings for a link between

crisis types and classes of reactions, along with the combinations inferred from the above

explanations.

Scandals affect regulative legitimacy, since they are often a result of corporate crime. They

also influence normative legitimacy due to the morally reprehensible behaviors associated with

them. This suggests that scandals may be addressed by a wide variety of strategies: evasive and

accommodative reactions for regulative legitimation, and defensive, appreciative, and

accommodative reactions for moral legitimation. This is congruent with empirical evidence to

some extent. Marcus and Goodman (1991) found that “accommodative” (i.e. appreciative and

accommodative in my classification of reactions) signals evoked significantly more positive

28

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

responses by investors than “defensive” signals. Moreover, Elsbach (2001) proposed that

expressing consideration of stakeholders’ views may be more effective when facing relatively

foreseeable controversies. A possible implication is that evasive strategies should only be

employed if the violation of rules has not already become obvious.

Product safety and health incidents primarily affect pragmatic legitimacy in that they threaten

the organization’s image as a reliable exchange partner. Thus, they may be resolved by choosing

evasive or accommodative reactions. Similarly, Ashforth and Gibbs (1990) suggested that when

goal performance is challenged, the focal organization typically resorts to denial, concealment,

and coercive isomorphism (i.e. adopting established practices). This combination may constitute

decoupling (Meyer & Rowan, 1977), which may be difficult to uphold under increased scrutiny

by stakeholders. Marcus and Goodman (1991) reported that none of the two classes of signals

they studied (accommodative vs. defensive) was significantly more effective than the other.

Accidents primarily affect cognitive legitimacy since their surprising occurrence and causal

ambiguity challenge perceived comprehensibility and taken-for-grantedness. It follows that the

most neutral types of reaction – at the boundary between defensive and appreciative – may be the

optimal choice in countering them. There is limited congruence of this notion with the evidence

provided by Marcus and Goodman (1991), who found no evidence of efficacy for defensive

signals following accidents. Elsbach (2001) posited that unforeseeable controversies should best

be countered by communicating rationality. This seems to be broadly congruent with the

strategies of defining and explaining, although Elsbach described denial as most effective.

Finally, when abstracting away from individual crisis types, accommodative behaviors appear to

be the most promising because they address moral, regulative, and pragmatic legitimacy. This

does not come as a complete surprise, however, since it involves conforming unreservedly to

stakeholder expectations (e.g. Ashforth & Gibbs, 1990; Elsbach, 1994).

29

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

Organizational crises have only recently received attention by researchers from a social

interaction perspective. And while there have been successful attempts at incorporating image

restoration strategies in order to explain effective crisis management, the mechanisms behind the

social dimension of crises remained unclear. Therefore, in this paper, I introduce organizational

legitimacy as the missing construct linking crises types and organizational reactions. I provide

overviews of the literature on dimensions of legitimacy, types of crises, and types of reactions to

crises, respectively. For crisis types and organizational reactions, I also develop new typologies,

taking account of and refining extant approaches. Based thereon, I present two sets of

propositions. The first posits links between scandals and regulative and moral legitimacy, product

safety and health incidents and pragmatic legitimacy, and accidents and cognitive legitimacy. The

second suggests connections between these four dimensions of legitimacy and a variety of

reactive crisis management strategies. These proposed connections tie well into the extant

evidence and provide an explanation of why certain reactive strategies are more appropriate in

restoring normalcy after a threatening event.

While I argue that the ideas put forward in this paper refine and extend our knowledge about

organizational crises, some caution is in order. Aside from the caveats of attempting to defend

organizational legitimacy noted above, i.e. potential loss of trust and image of control, another

source of failure lies among the groups of stakeholders the organization has to face. Since these

groups may be highly diverse, so may be their demands and expectations. This may not only

considerably complicate information processing and decision making (Ginzel et al., 1993). In

some cases, the affected organization may have to choose which group of stakeholders to favor

over others (Marcus & Goodman, 1991). Moreover, the diversity of members of any one group

may also require employing a complex mixture of signals in order to regain legitimacy (Elsbach,

2003), rendering the already challenging task of crisis management even more daunting.

30

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

REFERENCES

Aldrich, H. E., & Fiol, C. M. 1994. Fools rush in? The institutional context of industry creation. Academy of Management Review, 19(4): 645-670.

Aldrich, H. E., & Ruef, M. 2006. Organizations evolving. London: Sage.

Arndt, M., & Bigelow, B. 2000. Presenting structural innovation in an institutional environment: Hospitals' use of impression management. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45(3): 494-522.

Ashforth, B. E., & Anand, V. 2003. The normalization of corruption in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 25: 1-52.

Ashforth, B. E., & Gibbs, B. W. 1990. The double-edge of organizational legitimation. Organization Science, 1(2): 177-194.

Bansal, P., & Clelland, I. 2004. Talking trash: Legitimacy, impression management, and unsystematic risk in the context of the natural environment. Academy of Management Journal, 47(1): 93-103.

Benoit, W. L. 1995. Accounts, excuses, and apologies: A theory of image restoration discourse. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

Benoit, W. L., & Brinson, S. L. 1994. AT&T: ‘Apologies are not enough’. Communication Quarterly, 42(1): 75-88.

Billings, R. S., Milburn, T. W., & Schaalman, M. L. 1980. A model of crisis perception: A theoretical and empirical analysis. Administrative Science Quarterly, 25(2): 300-316.

Carroll, G. R., & Hannan, M. T. 2000. The demography of corporations and industries. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Cialdini, R. B. 1989. Indirect tactics of image management: Beyond basking. In R. A. Giacalone & P. Rosenfeld (Eds.), Impression Management in the Organization: 45-56. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Dacin, M. T. 1997. Isomorphism in context: The power and prescription of institutional norms. Academy of Management Journal, 40(1): 46-81.

Deephouse, D. L., & Carter, S. M. 2005. An examination of differences between organizational legitimacy and organizational reputation. Journal of Management Studies, 42(2): 329-360.

Deephouse, D. L., & Suchman, M. C. 2008. Legitimacy in organizational institutionalism. In R. Greenwood, C. Oliver, R. Suddaby, & K. Sahlin-Andersson (Eds.), The Sage handbook of organizational institutionalism: 49-77. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

31

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

DiMaggio, P. J., & Powell, W. W. 1983. The iron cage revisited: Institutional isomorphism and collective rationality in organizational fields. American Sociological Review, 48(2): 147-160.

Dowling, J., & Pfeffer, J. 1975. Organizational legitimacy: Social values and organizational behavior. Pacific Sociological Review, 18(1): 122-136.

Elsbach, K. D. 1994. Managing organizational legitimacy in the California cattle industry: The construction and effectiveness of verbal accounts. Administrative Science Quarterly, 39(1): 57-88.

Elsbach, K. D. 2001. The architecture of legitimacy: Constructing accounts of organizational controversies. In J. T. Jost & B. Major (Eds.), The psychology of legitimacy: 391-415. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Elsbach, K. D. 2003 Organizational perception management. Research in Organizational Behavior, 25: 297-332.

Elsbach, K. D., & Sutton, R. I. 1992. Acquiring organizational legitimacy through illegitimate actions: A marriage of institutional and impression management theories. Academy of Management Journal, 35(4): 699-738.

Finney, H. C., & Lesieur, H. R. 1982. A contingency theory of organizational crime. Research in the Sociology of Organizations, 1: 255-299.

Fiss, P. C., & Zajac, E. J. 2006. The symbolic management of strategic change: Sensegiving via framing and decoupling. Academy of Management Journal, 49(6): 1173-1193.

Fisse, B., & Braithwaite, J. 1983. The impact of publicity on corporate offenders. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

Freeman, R. E. 1984. Strategic management: A stakeholder approach. Boston, MA: Pitman.

Giacalone, R. A., & Rosenfeld, P. 1989. Impression management in the organization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Giacalone, R. A., & Rosenfeld, P. 1991. Applied impression management. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Ginzel, L. E., Kramer, R. M., & Sutton, R. I. 1993. Organizational impression management as a reciprocal process: The neglected role of the organizational audience. Research in Organizational Behavior, 15: 227-266.

Hedberg, B. L. T., Nystrom, P. C., & Starbuck, W. H. 1976. Camping on seesaws: Prescriptions for a self-designing organization. Administrative Science Quarterly, 21(1): 41-65.

Hermann, C. F. 1963. Some consequences of crisis which limit the viability of organizations. Administrative Science Quarterly, 8(1): 61-82.

32

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

Hirsch, P., & Andrews, J. A. Y. (1984). ‘Administrators’ response to performance and value challenges: Stance, symbols, and behavior’. In T. J. Sergiovanni & J. E. Corbally (Eds), Leadership and organizational culture: 170-185. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

Hudson, B. A. 2008. Against all odds: A consideration of core-stigmatized organizations. Academy of Management Review, 33(1): 252-266.

Hunt, C. S., & Aldrich, H. E. 1996. Why even Rodney Dangerfield has a home page: Legitimizing the world wide web as a medium for commercial endeavors. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Academy of Management, Cincinnati, OH.

James, E. H., & Wooten, L. P. 2006. Diversity crises: How firms manage discrimination lawsuits. Academy of Management Journal, 49(6): 1103-1116.

Lounsbury, M., & Glynn, M. A. 2001. Cultural entrepreneurship: Stories, legitimacy, and the acquisition of resources. Strategic Management Journal, 22(6): 545–564.

Marcus, A. A., & Goodman, R. S. 1991. Victims and shareholders: The dilemmas of presenting corporate policy during a crisis. Academy of Management Journal, 34(2): 281-305.

Meyer, J. W., & Rowan, B. 1977. Institutionalized organizations: Formal structure as myth and ceremony. American Journal of Sociology, 83(2): 340-363.

Meyer, J. W., & Scott, W. R. 1983. Centralization and the legitimacy problems of local government. In J. W. Meyer & W. R. Scott (Eds.), Organizational environments: Ritual and rationality: 199-215. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Milburn, T. W., Schuler, R. S., & Watman, K. H. 1983. Organizational crisis. Part I: Definition and conceptualization. Human Relations, 36(12): 1141-1160.

Oliver, C. 1991. Strategic responses to institutional processes. Academy of Management Review, 16(1): 145-179.

Pauchant, T., & Mitroff, I. I. 1992. Transforming the crisis-prone organization. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Pearson, C. M., & Clair, J. A. 1998. Reframing crisis management. Academy of Management Review, 23(1): 59-76.

Perrow, C. 1984a. Normal accidents: Living with high-risk technologies. New York: Basic Books.

Perrow, C. 1984b. Normal accident at Three Mile Island. In F. Fischer & C. Sirianni (Eds.), Critical studies in organization and bureaucracy: 353-370. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.

Pfarrer, M. D., Decelles, K. A., Smith, K. G., & Taylor, M. S. 2008. After the fall: Reintegrating the corrupt organization. Academy of Management Review, 33(3): 730-749.

33

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

Pfeffer, J. 1981. Management as symbolic action: The creation and maintenance of organizational paradigms. Research in Organizational Behavior, 3: 1-52.

Pfeffer, J., & Salancik, G. R. 1978. The external control of organizations. New York: Harper & Row.

Pondy, L. R., Frost, P. J., Morgan, G., & Dandridge, T. C. (Eds.). 1983. Organizational symbolism. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Rudolph, J. W., & Repenning, N. P. 2002. Disaster dynamics: Understanding the role of quantity in organizational collapses. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47(1): 1-30.

Ruef M., & Scott, W. R. 1998. A multidimensional model of organizational legitimacy: Hospital survival in changing institutional environments. Administrative Science Quarterly, 43: 877-905.

Russ, G. S. 1991. Symbolic communication and image management in organizations. In R. A. Giacalone & P. Rosenfeld (Eds.), Applied impression management: 219-240. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Salancik, G. R., & Meindl, J. R. 1984. Corporate attributions as strategic illusions of management control. Administrative Science Quarterly, 29(2): 238-254.

Schlenker, B. R. 1980. Impression management. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Scott, W. R. 1995. Institutions and organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Scott, M. B., & Lyman, S. M. 1968. Accounts. American Sociological Review, 33: 46-62.

Shrivastava, P. 1992. Bhopal: Anatomy of a Crisis. London: Paul Chapman.

Shrivastava, P., Mitroff, I. I., Miller, D., & Miglani, A. 1988. Understanding industrial crises. Journal of Management Studies, 25(4): 285-303.

Sipika, C., & Smith, D. 1993. From disaster to crisis: The failed turnaround of Pan American Airlines. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management, 1(3): 138-151.

Smart, C., & Vertinsky, I. 1977. Designs for crisis decision units. Administrative Science Quarterly, 22(4): 640-657.

Smart, C., & Vertinsky, I. 1984. Strategy and the environment: A study of corporate responses to crises. Strategic Management Journal, 5(3): 199-213.

Solari, L. 2007. Entrepreneurship at the margins of society: Founding dynamics in gray (sex shops) and black markets (mafia). Research in the Sociology of Organizations, 25: 337-368.

Suchman, M. C. 1995. Managing legitimacy: Strategic and institutional approaches. Academy of Management Review, 20(3): 571-610.

34

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005

Staw, B. M., McKechnie, P. I., & Puffer, S. M. 1983. The justification of organizational performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 28: 582-600.

Sutton, R. I., & Callahan, A. L. 1987. The stigma of bankruptcy: Spoiled organizational image and its management. Academy of Management Journal, 30(3): 405-436.

Sutton, R. I., & Galunic, D. C. 1996. Consequences of public scrutiny for leaders and their organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 18: 201-250.

Tedeschi, J. T. (Ed.). 1981. Impression management theory and social psychological research. New York: Academic Press.

Tedeschi, J. T., & Melburg, V. 1984. Impression management and influence in the organization. Research in the Sociology of Organizations, 3: 31-58.

Tedeschi, J. T., & Riess, M. 1981. Identities, the phenomenal self, and laboratory research. In J. T. Tedeschi (Ed.), Impression management theory and social psychological research: 3-22. New York: Academic Press.

Turner, B. A. 1976. The organizational and interorganizational development of disasters. Administrative Science Quarterly, 21(3): 378-397.

Vaughan, D. 1990. Autonomy, interdependence, and social control: NASA and the space shuttle Challenger. Administrative Science Quarterly, 35(2): 225-257.

Weick, K. E. 1988. Enacted sensemaking in crisis situations. Journal of Management Studies, 25(4): 305-317.

Weick, K. E. 1990. The vulnerable system: An analysis of the Tenerife air disaster. Journal of Management, 16(3): 571-593.

Weick, K. E. 1993. The collapse of sensemaking in organizations: The Mann Gulch disaster. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38(4): 628-652.

Westphal, J. D., & Zajac, E. J. 1994. Substance and symbolism in CEOs’ long-term incentive plans. Administrative Science Quarterly, 39: 367-390.

Westphal, J. D., & Zajac, E. J. 1998. The symbolic management of stockholders: Corporate reforms and shareholder reactions. Administrative Science Quarterly, 43: 127-153.

Yu, T., Sengul, M., & Lester, R. H. 2008. Misery loves company: The spread of negative impacts resulting from an organizational crisis. Academy of Management Review, 33(2): 452-472.

Zimmerman, M. A., & Zeitz, G. J. 2002. Beyond survival: achieving new venture growth by building legitimacy. Academy of Management Review, 27(3): 414-431.

Zucker, L. G. 1987. Institutional theories of organization. Annual Review of Sociology, 13: 443-464.

35

SCHUMPETER DISCUSSION PAPERS 2009-005


Recommended