+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

Date post: 26-Dec-2015
Category:
Upload: mrmare
View: 10 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
Description:
reading subskills
Popular Tags:
26
[ s R L CL E T I N IDENTIFICATION OF SUB-SKILLS OF READING COMPREHENSION BY MAXIMUM LIKELIHOOD FACTOR ANALYSIS Donald Spearritt RB-72-4 This Bulletin is a draft for interoffice circulation. Corrections and suggestions for revision are solicited. The Bulletin should not be cited as a reference without the specific permission of the author. It is automati- cally superseded upon formal publication of the material. Educational Testing Service Princeton, New Jersey February 1972
Transcript
Page 1: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

~[s~8R LC L~ E

TIN

IDENTIFICATION OF SUB-SKILLS OF READING COMPREHENSION

BY MAXIMUM LIKELIHOOD FACTOR ANALYSIS

Donald Spearritt

RB-72-4

This Bulletin is a draft for interoffice circulation.

Corrections and suggestions for revision are solicited.

The Bulletin should not be cited as a reference without

the specific permission of the author. It is automati­

cally superseded upon formal publication of the material.

Educational Testing Service

Princeton, New Jersey

February 1972

Page 2: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

IDENTIFICATION OF SUB-SKILLS OF READING CO~ITREHENSION BY MP~IMUM

LIKELIHOOD FACTOR ANALYSIS

Donald Spearritt

University of Sydney

Abstract

Differing interpretations have been made of the results obtained by

Davis in a large-scale study of the mental skills involved in reading

comprehension among twelfth grade students. On the basis of a uniqueness

analysis and a varimax-rotated principal components analysis, Davis maintained

that five of his eight postulated skills were experimentall~ distinguishable.

A refactorization of Davis's data by R. L. Thorndike suggested that except

for word knowledge, the reading skills were not separately distinguishable.

The present study consists of a further refactorization of Davis's data}

taking more advantage of their factor analytic design possibilities} and

employing the more comprehens ive procedures now available for maximum

likelihood factor analysis. Word knowledge and three other skills were

shown to be separately distinguishable, but the latter three skills were

very highly correlated and thus could be predominantly measuring a single

bas ic ability.

Page 3: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

IDENTIFICATION OF SUB-SKILLS OF READING CQMPREHENSIONBY ~~IMUM

LIKELIHOOD FACTOR ANALYSIS1

Donald Spearriti

University of Sydney

1. Introduction

The nature of the mental skills involved in understanding what

one reads was considered by E. L. Thorndike in 1917, and continues to be a

subject of speculation, investigation, and disagreement. Thorndike (1917)

concluded that reading comprehension was basically a process of reasoning.

Educators and. reading sp~cialists, however, have continued to draw up

lists and. taxonomies of the skills involved in reading comprehension,

and the early efforts of measurement specialists assumed that there

were separate skills which were worth measuring. Gates (1927), for

ir~tance, included in his battery measures of reading to appreciate general

significance, reading to understand precise directions, reading to note

details, and reading to predict the outcome of given events. From research

associated with the development of the Cooperative Reading Comprehension

Tests, Davis (1944) concluded that there were nine distinguishable skills

in reading. His :findings were challenged by Thurstone (1946), whose

reanalysis of Davis's correlations, employing communalities in the

diagonal cells, indicated that the correlations could be adequately

explained in terms of one common factor, presumably representing dif:ferences

in a general reading ability.

Later experimental evidence relating to the differentiability

of the skills involved in reading comprehension has been obtained by

Derrick (1953) and by Vernon (1962), among others. Derrick was unable to

Page 4: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-2-

differentiate reliably among the tasks involved in answering factual

questions, inferential questions, and questions requiring critical

judgments. Vernon obtained a clear differentiation between vocabulary

and reading comprehension tests, and at least an indication that ractual

and inferential questions may be measuring dirrerent skills.

2. Recent Analyses of Experimental

Data on Reading Comprehension Sub-Skills

The nature of the mental skills involved in reading comprehension

has been investigated again recently by Davis (1968), in a comprehensive

and well-designed study. Following a review of earlier experimental

studies of reading comprehension, he selected eight skills and constructed

items to measure each of these skills, each item being based on a different

passage for comprehension in order to avoid the problem of experimental

dependence among responses to items. At'ter appropriate item analyses,

two sets of twelve items were selected to represent each of the rollowing

skills:

"recalling word meanings,"

"drawing inferences about the meaning or a word from context,"

"finding answers to questions answered explicitly or in paraphrase,"

"weaving together ideas in the content,"

"drawing inferences from the content,"

"recognizing a writer's purpose, attitUde, tone, and mood,1l

"identifying a writer's techniques,"

"following the structure of a passage."

One set of twelve items in each of these categories formed Form C of the

experimental test, which was administered without time limit to twelfth­

grade pupils in academic high schools. The other eight subsets of

Page 5: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-3-

twelve items were incorporated into an equivalent form of the test, Form

D, which was administered to the same pupils after an interval of one or

two days. Analyses of the data were based on the scores of 988 pupils,

designated by Davis as Sample 3, who had taken both forms of the test.

For cross-validation purposes, the group '..ras subdivided into Sample 1

(494 pupils) and Sample 2 (494 pupils) by using alphabetical lists of

names within schools to assign one pupil to Sample 1, the next to Sample 2,

and so on.

Davis employed a uniqueness-analysis technique to analyze his data,

as he claimed that I1highly refined statistical techniques must be employed

if tiny amounts of variance unique to each skill in a set are to be

detected" (Davis, 1968, p. 509). The technique consists, in effect, of

determining the proportion of variance of a particular skill which can

be accounted for by the best-weighted combination of the other seven

skills,the weights being determined by multiple-regression procedures

in a cross-validating sample eitner across samples (Sample 1 vs, Sample 2),

across days (Form C vs. Form D), or across various combinations of both

days and samples, correcting this proportion for attenuation to take

account of unreliability in both the test itself and the weighted composite

score based on the other skills, and subtracting the adjusted proportion

f'rom unity. The percentage of unique variance in the nonerror variance

of the eight reading skills for Davis's Sample 3 is shown in the first

two data colu~ns of Table 1; the values in the first column were arrived at

Insert Table 1 about here

after the appropriate cross-validating procedures were applied with

correlation matrices obtained within single testing sessions, while

those in the second column involve correlations across days, i.e., correlations

Page 6: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-4-

between Form C and Form D tests, and vice versa. From these resu.1Lts, Davis

concluded that reading comprehension among mature readers is not a unitary

trait, and that substantial parts of the mental abilities used in the

eight skills are independent of one another. He claims in effect that

Skills 1, 3, 5, 6, and 8 are experimentally distinguishable.

In a subsequent research report, Davis (1971) presents the results

of a principal components analysis of the same data, after rotation to the

normalized varimax criterion. '11his type of analysis was applied independently

to two correlation matrices. '1be first matrix was formed from the inter­

correlations of sub-test Cl with sub-tests D2 through DB, sub-test C2

with sub-tests D3 through DB, and so on, while the second consisted of

the intercorrelations of sub-test Dl with sub-tests C2 through C8, and

so on. Davis identified the following factors in each of the two matrices:

knowledge of word meanings (Skill 1), recognition of writer's purpose,

attitude, tone and mood (Skill 6), ability to get literal sense meaning of

details (Skill 3) and a combination of weaving together ideas in the content,

and drawi!~ inferences from the content (Skills 4 and 5), apparently a

reasoning ability underlying both deductive and inductive processes.

The skills identifying distinguishable factors in these analyses are

indicated in Columns 3 and 4 of Table 1.

In an address on "Reading as Reasoning," given to Division 15 of

the Ameri.can Psychological Association in September,. 1971, Robert L.

Thorndike (1971) presented the results of his refactorization of Davis's

data using reliability coefficients in the diagonal cells, and showed

that the non-chance variance in the sets of eight tests could be completely

accounted for by three factors. He pointed out that Davis, in his principal

components analysis, had analyzed error variance as well as reliable

variance. 'I'horndike I s first factor accounted for more than 9310 of the

Page 7: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-5-

non-chance variance. His rotated factor 10adingSI which are presented in

Table 1 1 showed that the word knowledge test (Skill 1) could be distinguished

from the other reading tests in terms of its factor pattern, but none of

the other eight skills was separately distinguishable. His paper emphasizes

the importance of the first factor in tests of reading comprehension,

which he believes to be largely a measure of reasoning.

The same data, then, appear to lead to very different results and

conclusions depending on the technique employed in their analysis. This

state of affairs must be disconcerting to uniqueness analysts and factor

analysts alike, quite apart from the uncertainty it creates about the

differentiability of reading skills.

3. A Further Refactorization of Davis's 1968 Data

The aim of the present paper is to examine further this question of

differentiability by exploiting the factor analytic approach more fully

than either Davis or Thorndike has done in analyzing Davis's data. Both

Davis and Thorndike based their factor analyses on the correlation matrices

between eight variables for the full sample (N = 988), tHe two matrices

being formed from the upper and lower triangular matrix respectively of

the Form C vs. Form D correlations. The employment of this procedure

makes it necessary to identify a factor representing a particular skill

on the basis of one high test loading only, or on the basis of the absence

of a significant loading for one test only. Davis's data, however, are

admirably suited to the application of standard factor analytic procedures,

in which a factor is defined by at least two and preferably three or more

measures of a hypothesized skill. For each of his eight hypothesized

skills, Davis prepared "an essentially equivalent pair" (DaVis, 1968,

p. 51'!) of twelve-item tests. Thus, skills which are experimentally

Page 8: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-6-

dist:i.nguishable should appear as factors on '~lhich an equivalent pair

of tests have the highest loadings, or in a simple structure solution,

the only significant loadings. For factor analysis purposes, then, the

basic correlation matrix becomes a 16 variable matrix of the form!

ClC2

c8DlD2

D8

Cl C2 ••••.•••••. c8 Dl D2 •••••••••••• DB

j.nstead of an eight variable matrix. Each pupil has a score on each of

16 tests. The fact that all of the tests are not taken on the same day

is not regarded as a limiting feature in factor analytic studies, ',{hich are

qui. te often based on measurements gathered over a number of days. 'dhile

a 16 variable correlation matrix based on 988 cases could have been factored,

there 'vrere cross-vallclati.on advantages in analyzing the two 16 variable

con elation matrices representing Sample 1 and Sample 2 respectively,

each based on 4911. cases.

'rhe design of Davis I s study lent itself pa.rticularly 'dell to the

use of the maxiJl11.lln likelihood factor analytic procedures developed by

J~reskog and others (1968, 1969, 1970, 1971) since it provided a clear

basis for specifying a theoretical factor model, and since the maximwa

likelihood procedures allO\ol a statistical test of the goodness of fit of

the obtained data to the model. 'rhe application of these proceduTes in the

form proposed puts the unique variance of the skills i.nto the common

Page 9: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-7-

factor space, and allows such variance to emerge as a factor if the factorial

solution is acceptable at the required level of statistical significance.

The test data were originally collected to test the hypothesis that

the eight reading skills '..rere in fact all differentiable, even thou[.,h they

might share a considerable amount of' common variance. Thin tlypott.es is can

be represented in oblique simple structure form, as follows:

HyPothesized Structure 1

FactorsTest I II III ..................... 'lIn

Cl (Recalling word meanings) x 0 0 0Dl ( 11 It It ) X 0 0 0

C2 0 x 0 0D2 0 x 0 0

C3 0 0 x 0D3 0 0 x 0

c8 (Following structure)D8 (" ,,)

oo

oo

oo

xx

The structure was hypothesized to be oblique as the tests all involved reading

comprehension and positive correlations among factors might therefore be expected.

In orthogonal and near-orthogonal solutions, skills may be differentiable in

terms of their specific pattern of loadings on the various factors, but if they

measure some unique aspect of reading comprehension, it should be possible to

isolate them by allowing the factors to become more highly correlated.

A restricted maximum likelihood factor analysis was made of the 16 variable

correlation matrix for Sample 1, using ACOVSF, a general computer program for

analysis of covariance structures (J8reskog, Gruvaeus, & van Thillo, 1970).

The following constraints were imposed:

L Each factor was required to have equal loadings on the two designated

tests (as they were equivalent by definition) and zero loadings on all other

tests.

Page 10: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-8-

2. Diagonal cells in the correlation matrix among factors \-rere

set at unity.

3. Unique variances '...rere required to be identical for each test

within an equivalent pair.

Except for these restrictions, the matrices of factor loadings, factor

correlations, and uniquenesses '...rere free to take any value required by the

minimization procedures.

The program produced maximum likelihood estimates of factor loadings

in the spec ified form, but given that the hypothesized structure '...rere true,

the fit for the obtained data was very poor indeed (X2 =158.48, df = 92,

£ ~ .001). It was similarly poor for Sample 2 (x2 = 157.92, df = 92,

£ <, .001). 1,lhen the program was reapplied to both Sample 1 and Sample 2

correlation matrices without placing equality constraints on paired factor

loadings and paired unique variances, a slightly improved fit was obtained

for Sample 1 (-1 = 99.43, df = 76, 12. = .037) and for Sample 2 (-1 = 104.38,

df = 76, £ = .017), but £ values were still below .05, the predesignated level

for an acceptable degree of fit. Hypothesized structure 1 \.;as accordingly

rejected, indicating that the eight hypothesized skills could not be

experimentally identified as separate skills.

With this hypot.~esis rejected, there appeared to be no clear a priori

or theoretical basis for postulating how many separate skills were involved in

the sixteen tests, or which tests would be associated with which skills.

An unrestricted maximwn likelihood procedure developed for exploratory

factor analysis (JBreskog & van Thillo, 1971) was therefore applied to

the data to determine the minimum number of factors needed to account

for the observed correlations. This program sho-wed that at least three

factors were needed to account for the correlations in Sample 1 (X2 = 9l.41,

df = 75, £ = .096), and at least four factors in Sample 2 (X2 = 64.43,

df = 62, £ = .392). Varimax-rotated factor loadings for the three-factor ana

Page 11: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-9-

four-factor solution for Samples 1 and 2 are ~resented in Table 2.

Insert Table 2 about here

Inspection of the loadings in Table 2 suggests that Factor I in all

four solutions might be tentatively labelled as reasonir.g in readi.ng on

account of the high loadings of Skills 5, 1+, and 3 and the subs tant ial

loadings of all other skills except vocabulary (recalling word meanings).

Factor II in all solutions is clearly a vocabulary factor. Except for the

three-factor solution for Sample 2, Factor III is largely a composite

of Skills 6, and 7, recognizing a writer's purpose, attitude, tone, mood,

and techniques. Factor IV is probably best interpreted as the ability

to follow the structure of a passage, a description which also applies

to Factor III in the three-factor solution for Sample 2.

In the strict application of the maximum likelihood procedures, this explor-

atory factor analysis would be undertaken with one sample only, and the results

used to generate an hypothesized structure matrix to be tested by the restricted

maximum likelihood procedures on the other sample. As the object of the present

study was to identity as many reading comprehension skills as possible, the

four-factor solution as '",ell as the three-factor solution in Smnple 1 was used

to assist in the generation of an hypothesized structure matrix. This decision

was supported by the results obtained when the unrestricted maximum likelihood

procedure was applied to the data for the combined samples, i.e., Sample 3,

N = 988. In this sample, three factors were insufficient to account for the

observed correlations (X 2 = 119.87, df = 75, p = .001), whereas four factors- -were adequate (x2

= 74.09, df = 62, £ = .140).

The exploratory factor analyses indicated that only the skills of reasoning

in reading, vocabulary knowledge, recognizing a writer's purpose, attitude,

mood, and technique, and following the structure of a passage, were likely to

be differentiable. On this basis, hypothesized structure matrix 2 was developed:

Page 12: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-10-

Hypothesized Structure 2 Factors

Test I II III IV

Cl 0 x 0 0Dl 0 x 0 0

C2 x 0 0 0D2 x 0 0 0C3 x 0 0 0D3 x 0 0 0C4 x 0 0 0D4 x 0 0 0C5 x 0 0 0D5 x 0 0 0

c6 0 0 x 0D6 0 0 x 0C7 0 0 x 0D7 0 0 x 0

c8 0 0 0 xD8 0 0 0 x

The hypothesized matrix was again set up as an oblique simple structure

matrix, on the grounds that any overlap in the skills measured by the different

tests could be represented by the correlations among factors.

Thi.s structure was tested by applying the ACOVSF restricted maximum likelihood

program to the ::.6 variable correlation matrix. I-lhen the paired test

loadings within each factor were required to be of equal value, and a similar

restriction was applied to the unique variances of each test within a pair,

the hypothesized structure was rejected for Sample 1 (X2= 208.53, df = 114,

:£ < .001) and also for Sample 2 (i i:::: 212.75, df = 114, :£ < .001). When no

equal value constraints were imposed, the hypothesized structure matrix was

againrejected both for Samplel(Jf = 153.35, df = 98, :£ < .001) and for

Sample 2 (X2 = 155.02, df = 98, :£ < .001). Thus, even when correlations

between factors were virtually unity in some instances, it was not possible to

achieve an acceptable degree of fit to these hypothesized structure matrices.

Page 13: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-11-

It was then argued that if the four skills identified in the exploratory

factor analys is i,rere S epm.'atel;)' dis tingui.shable, they should appear as

separate factors in an analysis based on the relevant tests only. For this

analysis, the Skill 5 tests ·vrere tal<::en to be the best measures of reasoning

in read.ing, and Skill 6 tests as the best measures of recognizing a ''''riter's

purpose, attitude, tone, and mood, as they generally had the highest

loadings on the relevant factors in the exploratory factor solutions. 'TIms,

hJ~othesized structure 3 took tbis form, again conceived as an oblique

simple structure:

Hypothesized Structure 3

Test

Cl (Recalling i·rord meanings)Dl ( " II " )

Ix;.c

Factorsn IIIo 0o 0

IVoo

C5 (Dray,{ing inferences from content)D5 (" II I! ")

c6 (Recognizing "rriter I s purpose,attitude, tone, and mood)

D6 ( II II " )

c8 (Following structure of passage)DB (" " II I! )

oo

oo

oo

xx

oo

oo

oo

xx

oo

oo

oo

xx

This st;i:'ucture w'as tested by applying the ACOVSF restricted. maximum

likelihood program to the appropriate eigl:Jt variable correlation matrix.

'Vlhen the restriction of equality ims imposed on the paired test loadings

",ithin each :factor and the unique variances of equivalent tes ts, the

;>hJ~othesized struct1n'e was rejected for Sample 2 (X- = 44.34, df = 22,

12. = .003): the structure 'was first tested in Sample 2, as the exploratory

factor analysis indicated that 13.t least four factors ',{ere required to account

for the correl? tiol'l's in this sample. 11hen no equal val.ue constraints '..rere

imposed, the l~~othesized structure was accepted for Sample 2 (X2 = 22.93,

Page 14: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-J 2-

Qf = 14, E = .061) and also for Sample 1 (X2

= 18.35, £f = 14, E = .191).

The factor loadings and factor correlation matrices for these two solutions

are presented in Table 3.

Insert Table 3 about here

It is clear from 'l'able 3 that the four skills of recalling word

meanir~s, draWing inferences from the content, recognizing a writer's

purpose, attitude, tone, and mood, and following the structure of a passage

are differentiable as separate skills. Except for Skill 3, finding answers

to questions answered explicitly or in paraphrase, this result is in general

agreement with Davis's findings for his uniqueness analysis. As a matter

of interest, the ACOVSF restricted maximum likelihood program was applied

to a 10 variable correlation matrix to test an extended version of hypothesized

structure 3, in which two measures of Skill 3 defined a fifth factor. If

five factors had been needed to account for the correlations, this extended

2structure would have been accepted for Sample 1 (x = 33.83, df = 25, E = .111),

but rejected for Sample 2 (X2= 39.00, df = 25, £ = .037). There was

thus insui'ficient evidence to support the claim that Skill 3 '"ras a separately

dist inguishable skill.

Tucker and Lewis (1970) have raised a number of questions about the

application of the likelihood ratio or significance test associated with

maximum likelihood factor analys is. They point out that in contrast with the

usual situation, rejection of the statistical hypothesis leads to rejection

rather than acceptance of the scientific hypothesis, in this case, the

specified factor model. They also assert that the statistical hypothesis

"lill almost certainly be rejected for very large samples at any usual level

of significance. In addition, they present empirical evidence to show that

the likelihood ratio procedure sometimes yields more factors than can be

Page 15: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-13-

meaningfully interpreted.. They propose a form of reliability coefficient to

indicate how' well a specified factor model represents the relationships

among the attributes being investigated, and suggest that if this coefficient

has a high value, the meaningfulness of the factor results might be used as

a guide in determining the number of factors to be accepted.

There was little doubt from tbe present study that four factors could

be readily interpreted. To allo\ol greater account to be tal,en of the criterion

of meaningfulness, hO'flever, and to checJ, further the possibility that Skill 3

might be identifiable as a separatel;}' distinguishable Skill, direct oblimin

rotational procedt~es were applied to the matrices of urrrotated factor

loadings produced by the unrestricted maximU>ll 1ikelihoocl analyses for four-

factor and five-factor solutions for ScU"nples 1, 2, and 3. For each of these

solutions, Tucker's reliability coefficient was .995 or above. As a reasonably

higb. level o.f correlations could be expected among factors, the oblimin

solution ,,,as specified to yield fairly oblique factors. 3 Factor patterns

and factor correlations for each of these solutions are given in Tables

4, 5, and 6.

I!lliert Tables 4, 5, and 6 about here

In general, the four-factor solutio!lli in Tables 4 to 6 identify the

same factors as were distinguished by the varimax-rotated solutions in

Table 2, namely, reasonin.g in reading (Skills 2, 3, 4, and 5), vocabulary

knowledge (Skill 1), recognizing a "ll.'iter's purpose, attitUde, mood, and

technique (Skills 6 anci 7) and follo'wing the structure of a passage

(SkUl 8) 0 Except for Sample 3, 'where variance from Factors III and IV

tends to be absorbed into Factor I, correla.tions &'llong i'actors are high.

Page 16: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-14-

In the five-factor rotated solutions, factors of reasoning in reading (Skills

2, 3, 4, and 5), vocabulary knowledge (Skill 1), and following the structure

of a passage (Skill 8) are identifiable in all three samples. The factor

identified as recognizing a writer's purpose, attitude, mood, and technique

(Skills 6 and 7) in the four-factor solution of Sample 1 has split into two

factors in the five-factor solution, cutting across these skills rather than

consolidating them as separate skills. This factor is discernible in

Samples 2 and 3, but is not clearly defined. Some weak support for the

separate identifiability of Skill 2 and Skill 3 can be discerned in the solutions

for Sample 2 and Sample 3, respectively, but it is not consistent across

samples and is not sufficient to establish their separate identifiability.

All in all, the five-factor solutions fail to provide a more meaningful

interpretation than that available from four factors, and do not support

the proposition that Skill 3 is a separately distinguishable skill.

4. Discussion

The maximum likelihood factor analyses support the hypothesis that the

four skills of recalling word meanings, drawing inferences from the content,

recognizing a writer's purpose, attitude, tone, and mOOd, and following the

structure of a passage are identifiable as separate skills, but do not support

the hypothesis that Skill 3, finding ar~wers to questions answered explicitly

or in paraphrase, is an additional separately distinguishable skill. 'I.'he

direct oblimin solutions, though less precise, give much the same results.

Davis's conclusions are therefore confirmed, except with respect to Skill 3.

Although these four skills have been shown to be experimental~distin­

guishable, attention must be dra'wn to the fact that Skill 5, drawing inferences

from the content, was selected as the best of four measures of "reasoning in

reading, " and Skill 6, recognizing a writer's purpose, attitude, tone, and

mOOd, was selected as the best measure of a factor on which Skill 7 was also

Page 17: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-15-

represented. Thus, it is conceivable that hypothesized structure 3 might be

accepted as providir~ a satisfactory fit to the appropriate correlation matrix

for Skills 1, 3, 7, and 8 as well as Skills 1, 5, 6, and 8. If this were

so, however, Skill 3 would have to be regarded as a measure of tlreasoning

in reading," and Skill 7 as a measure involving the recognition of a writer's

purpose, attitude, tone, mood and, perhaps, technique rather than as measures

of the skills they were purportedly developed to measure.

It must be further stressed that although the four skills are differentiable,

some of them are only just so. Vocabulary is the best differentiated, as

in both the Davis and Thorndike analyses. Maximum-likelihood second order

factor analyses of the factor correlations in Table 3 showed that it could

not in fact be subsumed under one general factor with the other three skills.

~~ile the logic of factor analysis made it unfeasible to test whether these

three skills could themselves be subsumed under one general factor, it is

clear that when the correlations between vocabulary and other factors in the

lower section of Table 3 are excluded from consideration, the remaining

correlations are extraordinarily high. Thus, although certain comprehension

skills can be differentiated, present types of reading comprehension tests,

as distinct from word knowledge tests, largely measure one basic ability,

which may well correspond to the label of tlreasoning in reading. I' Wnether

they should do so, and whether comprehension skills can be separated from

inference skills by more careful test construction, as Carroll (1969, 1971)

suggests, is a matter which needs to be further investigated.

Page 18: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-1..0-

References

Carroll, J. B. From comprehension to inference. In M. P. Douglass (Ed.),

Thirty-Third Yearbook, Claremont Reading Conference. Claremont, Calif.:

Claremont Graduate School, 1969. Pp. 39-44.

Carroll, J. B. Defining language comprehension: Some speculations. Research

Memorandum 7l-9. Princeton, N.J.: Educational Testing Service, 1971.

Davis, F. B. Fundamental factors of comprehens ion in reading. Psychometrika.

1944, 2., 185-197.

Davis, F. B. Research in comprehension in reading. Reading Research Quarterly,

1968, i, 499-545.

Davis, F. B. Psychometric research on comprehension in reading. Graduate

School of Education, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, N.J., 1971.

Derrick, C. Three aspects of reading comprehension as measured by tests of

different lengths. Research Bulletin 53-8. Princeton, N.J.: Educational

Testing Service, 1953.

Gates, A. 1. Gates silent reading tests. Ne'(T York: Bureau of Publications,

Teachers College, Columbia University, 1927.

Harman, H. H. Modern factor analysis. (2nd ed.) Chicago: University

of Chicago Press, 1967.

JClreskog, K. G. A general approach to confirmatory maximum likelihood factor

analys is. Psychometrika, 1969, .li, 183-202.

JtSreskog, K. G., Gruvaeus, G. T., & van Thillo, M. ACOVS: A general

computer program for analysis of covariance structures. Research Bulletin

70-15. Princeton, N.J.: Educational Testing Service, 1970.

JtSreskog, K. G., & Lawley, D. N. New methods in maximum likelihood factor

analysis. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology,

1968, 2l, 85-96.

Page 19: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-17-

JBreskog, K. G., & van 1hillo, M. New rapid algorithms for factor analysis

by unweighted least squares, generalized least squares and maximum

likelihood. Research Memorandum 71-5. Princeton, N.J.: Educational

Testing Service, 1971.

Thorndike, E. L. Reading as reasoning: A study of mistakes in paragraph

reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1917, §., 323-332.

Thorndike, R. L. Reading as reasoning. Address delivered to Division 15,

American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C., September, 1971.

Thurstone, L. L. Note on a reanalysis of Davis's reading tests. Psychometrika,

1946, 11, 185-188.

Tucker, L. R, & Lewis, C. A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood

factor analysis. In L. R Tucker, W. D. Love, & C. Lewis,

Topics in Factor Analysis II. ONR Technical Report, Contract U. S. Navy/

OO014-67-A-0305-0003. Champaign, Ill.: University of Illinois at

Urbana, 1970. Pp. 1-18.

Vernon, P. E. The determinants of reading comprehension. Educational and

Psychological Measurement, 1962, 22, 269-286.

Page 20: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-18-

Footr~tes

1This work was supported by the National Institute of Child Health

and Human Development, under Research Grant l-POI-HD01762 to Educational

Testing Service.

~his study was undertaken when the author was working at Educational

Testing Service during a period of leave from the University of Sydney,

where he is Professor of Education. The author wishes to thank John B.

Carroll and Ledyard R Tucker for helpful comments and Marielle van Thillo

for advice on the computer programs employed in the study.

3Delta = -.1 (Harman, 1967, p. 336).

Page 21: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

Tab

le1

Sum

mar

yo

fD

avis

and

Tho

rndi

keA~alyses

of

Dav

is's

1968

Rea

ding

Dat

a

Dav

is-U

niqu

enes

sA

nal

ysi

sD

avis

-Pri

nci

pal

Com

pone

nts

Th

orn

dik

e-F

acto

rA

nal

ysi

sA

nal

ysi

s-

Per

cen

tag

eo

fu

niq

ue

var-

Sk

ills

iden

tify

ing

dis

tin

-R

ota

ted

Fac

tor

Loa

ding

a*ia

nce

inn

on

-err

or

var

ian

ceg

uis

hab

lefa

cto

rsin

Dav

is's

CR.

L.

Tho

rndi

ke)

Tes

tco

mpo

nent

an

aly

sis

Cro

ss-v

ali

-C

ross

-vali

-d

atio

nby

dat

ion

by

Tes

tFo

rms

Tes

tF

onus

Fac

tor

IF

acto

rII

Fac

tor

III

and

Day

s(W

ith

ind

ays)

(acr

oss

day

s)M

atri

x1

Mat

rix

2M

1M

2V.

ilM

2M

lM

2

1.

Rec

alli

ng

wor

dm

eani

ngs

3529

XX

7674

---

---

2.D

raW

ing

infe

ren

ces

abo

ut

I74

7222

2821

the

mea

ning

of

aw

ord

-18

-X

-fr

omco

nte

xt

II

3.F

ind

ing

answ

ers

to0 I

qu

esti

on

san

swer

ed13

7X

X70

6021

5124

--ex

pli

cit

lyo

rin

para

-p

hra

se

4.W

eavi

ngto

get

her

-id

eas

inj

(X

:\49

1

7963

22--

I5

5-

the

con

ten

t(

5.

Dra

win

gin

fere

nce

sfr

omI

2318

(X

6548

-54

4425

the

con

ten

tI I

6.R

ecog

nizi

nga

wri

ter'

sI

8X

7562

2139

35p

urp

ose

,att

itu

de,

14X

-I

ton

ean

dm

ood

7.Id

en

tify

ing

aw

rite

r's

83

IXJ-

71'70

3844

---

tech

niq

ues

8.F

bllo

win

gth

est

ructu

reo

f15

12X

)--

6664

4655

----

ap

assa

ge

~

....

--~

-S

kil

lsco

nsi

der

edto

be

Nos

.1

,3,5

,6,8

.N

os.

1,3

,(4

,5),

6.

No.

1ex

per

imen

tall

yd

isti

ng

uis

hab

le*D

ecim

alp

oin

tso

mit

ted

.

Page 22: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-20-

Table 2

Varimax-Rotated Factor Solutions Based on Three-Factor and Four-Factor

Maximum Likelihood Solutions o~ Davis's 1968 Reading Data

.'.

Sample 1 (N = 494) Sample 2 (N = 494)'Ihree-~actor Four-~actor 'Ihree-~actor Four-~actor

Test solution solution solution solutionTest Form 2 2 2 2p(x ) = .C96 p(X ) = .680 p(X ) = .032 p(X ) = .392I II III I II III rr I II III I II III IV

1. Recalling word meanings Cl .28 .78 .25 .26 .76 .25 .16 .29 .72 .24 .26 .78 .21 .21Dl .35 .52 .30 .25 .52 .28 .28 .24 .63 .18 .23 .56 .26 .17

2. Drawing inferences about C2 .55 .40 .38 .48 .39 .38 .28 .47 .55 .33 .43 .46 .38 .29the meaning o~ a wordD2 .57 .37 .36 .54 .35 .37 .21 .59 .48 .28 .53 .40 .39 .24

~rom context

3. Finding answers toquestions answered C3 .60 .26 .36 .52 .25 .37 .31 .64 .37 .23 .56 .30 .40 .18explicitly or in para- D3 .65 .29 .38 .53 .28 .38 .40 .70 .29 .33 .64 .24 .36 .28phrase

4. Weaving together ideas in c4 .57 .35 .41 .l~7 .34 .41 .33 .57 .47 .21 .47 .37 .47 .15the content D4 .68 .36 .31 ·57 .35 .32 .38 .61 .49 .33 .59 .45 .32 .28

5. Drawing inferences ~rom C5 .66 .24 .20 .64 .23 .20 .25 .59 .21 .30 .64 .19 .15 .25the content D5 .66 .30 .36 .68 .28 .36 .21 .59 .39 .26 5<:: .35 .32 .22. '"

6. Recognizing a writer's c6 .49 .34 .51 .43 .33 .51 .24 .45 .49 .34 .32 .36 .52 .31purpose, attitude, D6 .42 .35 .67 .35 .34 .66 .26 .50 .48 .35 .40 .38 .47 .31tone and mood

7. Identi~ying a writer's C7 .48 .42 .60 .38 .41 .60 .31 .54 .49 .40 .41 ·37 .53 .36techniques D7 .62 .31 .41 .46 .30 .40 .45 .45 .47 .42 .39 .40 .39 .39

8. Following the structure c8 .67 .35 .35 .46 .34 .30 .. 60 .54 .37 .46 .53 .32 .29 .42o~ a pa'ssage D8 .65 .26 .37 .49 .24 .36 .45 .43 .35 .68 .43 .31 .29 .63

Page 23: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-21-

Table 3

Maximum Likelihood Factor Solutions

Under Hypothesized Structure 3

Sample 1 Sample :2

Test Factors FactorsI II III IV I II III IV

C1. Recalling word meanings .77 0 0 0 .84 0 0 0D1. II II II II II II .75 0 0 0 .70 0 0 0

C5. Drawing inferences from 0 .71+ 0 0 0 .69 0 0the contentD5. II II II II II II 0 .86 0 0 0 .78 0 0

c6. Recognizing a writer'spurpose, attitude, 0 0 .80 0 0 0 .76 0tone and mood

r.6. II tI II II II II 0 0 .83 0 0 0 .79 0

c8. Following the structure 0 0 0 .85 0 0 0 .83of' a passageD8. tI tI II II II " 0 0 0 .80 0 0 0 .82

Factor Factor Correlations Factor CorrelationsI II III IV I II III IV

I. \{ord knowledge 1.00 .75 .85 .81 1.00 . '75 .84 .78

II. Reasoning in reading .75 1.00 .88 .90 .75 1.00 .88 .93

III. Recognition of writer'spurpose, attitude, .85 .88 1.00 ·90 .84 .88 1.00 .93mood

IV. Followir~ structure .81 .90 .90 1.00 .78 .93 .93 1.00

Page 24: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-22-

Table 4

Direct Oblimin Solutions of Unrestricted Maximum Likelihood

Factor Matrices for Sample 1 (N = 494)

Test Test Four-Factor Solution Five-Factor SolutionFormI II III IV I II III IV V

1. Recalling word meanings Cl .04 .87 -.01 -.05 .04 .86 .00 -.04 .01Dl -. ()l.~ .50 .11 .19 -.03 .48 .09 .19 .06

2. Drawing inferences aboutC2 .29 .22 .24 .13 .27 .21 .15 .12 .15the meaning of a wordD2 .45 .18 .23 .00 .44 .17 .20 .00 .07from context

3. Finding answers toquestions answered C3 .37 .02 .23 .19 .41 .02 .28 .15 -.02explicitly or in Para- D3 .30 .05 .21 .34 .30 .04 .22 .31 .05phrase

4. Weaving together ideas in c4 .24 .13 .29 .21 .25 .13 .25 .19 .09the content D4 .40 .17 .07 .29 .38 .15 .02 .27 .13

5. Drawing inferences from C5 .68 .05 -.08 .12 .66 .04 -.05 .10 .04-the content D5 .68 .04 .18 -.01 .63 .05 .11 -.01 .13

6. Recognizing a writertsc6 .20 .09 .51 .05 .00 .00 .01 -.01 1.00purpose, attitude, D6 .01 .06 .78 .05 .04 .08 .64 .04 .14tone and mood

7. Identifying a writerfs C7 .03 .16 .63 .12 .04 .16 .58 .10 .10techniques D7 .15 .07 .25 .42 .13 .06 .16 .41 .17

8. Following the structure c8 .07 .15 .03 .70 .06 .13 .03 .68 .07of a passage D8 .21 .00 .20 .45 .21 .00 .17 .41 .10

Factor Factor Correlations Factor Correlations

I 1.00 .63 .75 .78 1.00 .60 .70 .77 .63

II .63 1.00 .72 .63 .60 1. 00 .68 .60 .60

III .75 .72 1.00 .74 .70 .68 1.00 .70 .68

Dr .78 .63 .74 1.00 .77 .60 .70 1.00 ·59V .63 .60 .68 .59 1.00

Page 25: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-23-

Table 5

Direct Oblimin Solutions of Unrestricted Maximum Likelihood

Factor Matrices for Sample 2 (N = 494)

Test ,Test Four-Factor Solution Five-Factor SolutionFormI II III IV I II III IV V

1. Recalling word meaningsC1 .00 .89 -.05 .02 .00 1.03 -.06 .00 -.01Dl .04 .57 .07 .05 .05 .3r( .12 .07 .16

2. Drawing inferences about C2 .27 .30 .15 .20 .00 .01 -.03 .01 .99the meaning of a wordD2 .49 .19 .15 .08 .44 .11 .14 .08 .18from context

3. Finding answers toquestions answered C3 .61 .06 .19 -.01 .58 .07 .18 .00 '.06explicitly or in para- D3 .69 -.05 .10 .13 .67 .01 .11 .14 -.03phrase

4. Weaving together ideas in c4 .46 .15 .29 -.01 .42 .10 .26 -.02 .18the content D4 .54 .26 .03 .10 .52 .20 .08 .14 .05

5. Drawing inferences from C5 .76 -.03 -.13 .07 .70 -.02 -.17 .07 .08the content D5 .56 .16 .08 .04 .53 .15 .08 .05 .06

6. Recognizing a writer's c6 .08 .13 .36 .34 .06 .08 .32 .31 .18purpose, attitude, I6 .21 .15 .27 .28 .20 .12 .25 .27 .12tone and mood

7. Identifying a writer's C7 .18 .10 .33 .39 .16 .08 .31 ·37 .11techniques D7 .14 .20 .15 .40 .1L" .16 .16 .40 .08

8. Following the structure c8 .38 .09 .00 .40 .35 .06 .00 .40 .08of a passage DB .06 .05 -.03 .81 .05 .06 -.04 .76 .08

Factor Factor Correlations Factor Correlations

I 1.00 .68 .56 .82 1.00 .58 .48 .79 .68

II .68 1.00 .59 .67 .58 LOO .47 .59 .61

III .56 .59 1.00 .50 .48 .47 1.00 .45 .43

rJ .82 .67 .50 1.00 .79 .59 .45 1.00 .64V .68 .61 .43 .64 1.00

Page 26: Spearrit - Identification of Sub-skills of Reading

-24-

Table 6

Direct Oblimin Solutions of Unrestricted Maximum Likelihood

Factor Matrices for Sample 3 (N = 988)

Test Test Four-Factor Solution Five-Factor SolutionFormI II III IV I II III IV V

Recalling word meanings Cl .07 .78 -.01 -.02 .07 .75 -.02 -.05 .10l. Dl .ll .60 .01 .07 -.05 .70 .05 .08 -.062. Drawing inferences about C2 .59 .23 .02 .01 .22 .27 .04 .14 .27~he meaning of a word D2 .71 .12 -.01 -.06 ·33 .19 .11 .06 .25from context

3. Finding answers toquestions answered C3 .82 -.04 -.03 -.06 ·32 .10 .43 .04 -.01explicitly or in para- D3 .82 -.06 -.04 .07 .25 .04 ·32 .32 .00phrase

4. Weaving together ideas in c4 .73 .07 .05 -.04 .21 .17 .28 .09 .16the content D4 .68 .17 -.11 .03 .34 .24 .10 .22 .09

5. Drawing inferences from C5 .72 -.04 -.25 .00 ·52 .02 .04 .22 .00the content D5 .82 .01 -.08 -.09 .45 .08 .16 .06 .20

6. Recognizing a writer's c6 .68 .07 .19 .03 .04 .09 .13 .19 .46purpose, attitude, D6 .75 .04 .23 .01 -.05 .15 .46 .13 .24tone and mood

7. Identifying a writer's C7 .76 .06 .21 .05 -.01 .16 .34 .22 .27techniques D'7 .63 .10 .04 .15 .09 .12 .()( .42 .23

8. Following the structure c8 .59 .12 -.10 .28 .12 .14 .04 .62 .00of' a passage DB .65 .02 -.01 .27 .06 .02 .08 .62 .11

Factor Factor Correlations Factor Correlations

I 1.00 .76 -.01 .41 1.00 .56 .64 .72 .50II .76 1.00 .16 .28 .56 1.00 .66 .70 .66

III -.01 .16 1.00 .03 .64 .66 1.00 .74 .63IV .41 .28 .03 1.00 ·72 .70 .74 1.00 .63if .50 .66 .63 .63 1.00


Recommended