+ All Categories
Home > Documents > “STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION”. Discuss.

“STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION”. Discuss.

Date post: 03-Jun-2018
Category:
Upload: sneha-rawlani
View: 221 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 21

Transcript
  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    1/21

    Person perception is fundamentally important to everyday social life (Smith & Collins, 2009). It

    encompasses evaluations, judgments and impressions of target others (Macrae & Bodenhausen,

    2000) and inferences about their mental states, goals and traits (Quinn & Rosenthal, 2012). Thinking

    categorically about others shapes person perception (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000) and

    stereotypes modulate the formation of person percepts (Quadflieg, 2013). Simply put, stereotypes

    are schemas (i.e., mental representations) about groups of people (Moskowitz, 2005). They help

    perceivers make sense of their social worlds by aiding explanation and serve as energy-saving mental

    devices (McGarty, Yzerbyt & Spears, 2002; Macrae, Milne & Bodenhausen, 1994). Categorical

    thinking provides the flexibility that the person perception process demands (Macrae &

    Bodenhausen, 2000) and stereotypes can be adjusted to the perceivers needs (Sherman, 2004).

    Therefore, stereotypes are thought of as handy cognitive tools (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Macrae et

    al., 1994; McGarty et al., 2002; Sherman, 2004).

    In contrast, Gordon Allport (1954), who was highly influential, believed that stereotypes arefaulty exaggerations, they lead to error and bias and are permeated by prejudice. Much

    research since then has followed the same tack (Jussim, 2012), especially in the case of

    stereotypes related to ethnicity, gender and race (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). Given this

    controversy, are stereotypes truly tools that aid perception or do they simply create

    misperception?

    Thereafter, this essay aims to explore the assertion that Stereotypes are cognitive tools

    designed to facilitate the process of person perception. Arguments for the functional

    purposes of stereotypes in the person perception process will be put forth. Some

    contradictory views of stereotyping being unconstructive and harmful will be addressed.

    Resource Conservation

    Some psychologists believe perceivers to be cognitive misers who rely on stereotypes as

    shortcuts instead of thinking more deeply (Sherman, Bessenoff & Frost, 1998). But others

    argue for the functionality of stereotypes, believing that people are not simply lazy; they

    employ stereotypes because they are simplifying mental structures. Macrae et al.s (1994;

    Study 2 & 3) work was seminal to demonstrating the same. Participants concurrently

    performed an impression formation and a prose-monitoring task. They were primed with astereotype label, and so expected to assign more stereotypic traits to a given target. It was

    predicted that they would perform better on the other task under this condition, as

    activating the stereotype through the label would make impression formation easier thus

    allocating more resources to the prose task. This is indeed what they found, indicating that

    forming impressions through stereotypes is the perceivers go-to option, and better

    performance on the concurrent task reveals that stereotypes do indeed simplify the

    process. Despite providing this insight, a pitfall emerges in that the procedure is too

    transparent about the impression formation task, potentially evoking demand

    characteristics. Additionally, the judgment task involved assigning trait descriptions to

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    2/21

    targets from a predetermined set, which could have perpetuated stereotypical thinking that

    might not have otherwise occurred.

    Macrae, Hewstone and Griffiths (1993) similarly establish the heuristic usefulness of stereotyping

    under processing load. They watched a video of a woman providing information about herself, half

    of which was stereotype-consistent and the other half was inconsistent. Prior to the video,

    participants were primed with an occupational label. Load was manipulated by having participants

    rehearse an 8-digit number prior to the video that they were told would be later tested for recall.

    Creating load on attentional resources was theorized to reduce allocation of resources to the

    subsequent video-watching and so, those under load would subsequently recall more stereotype-

    consistent information. And this did happen.

    In more recent research, Allen, Sherman, Corney, Stroessner (2009; Study 1) also looked at

    processing capacity, except their findings revealed a different pattern. Under high cognitive load,

    having strong stereotypes resulted in attending toward stereotype-inconsistent information. Load

    was similarly manipulated through rehearsal of an 8-digit number. Impressions of a Black male

    target were to be formed through provided behavioural descriptions. These appeared on a

    computer screen in pairs and on some trials had to be responded to by pressing keys. If a descriptive

    item was attended to more than its counterpart, key-press response to it would be quicker - this

    was their prediction. Contrary to previous research, they found that under load, responses were

    actually faster and more accurate for stereotype inconsistent information. They replicated their

    findings in a second study, with the difference being a White female target instead. The stereotype

    of women is positively valenced whereas that of African Americans is negatively valenced, and so

    they wanted to test both in their paradigm to demonstrate the generality of their findings,

    demonstrating methodological foresight. They successfully obtained the same results, achievingrobustness. Another strength, as the researchers point out, is their direct measurement of attention

    as opposed to reliance on measures of memory that only suggest attentional use.

    Encoding Flexibility

    The pattern Allen et al. (2009) found fits in with Sherman et al.s (1998) encoding-flexibility model

    (EFM). According to EFM, stereotype-consistent information, being easy to comprehend and

    confirmatory of prior knowledge, receives little attention and finer details are not thoroughly

    encoded but the basic gist is extracted in case of possible use. Attention is instead redirected toward

    inconsistent information because it is harder to make sense of and receives more perceptual andcontextual encoding (encoding of sensory details and specifics of contexts), whereas consistent

    information receives only the cursory glance that is conceptual attention. Therefore, stereotypes

    enable this dual process form of encoding that is flexible and sophisticated and particularly

    important when resources are low and the need for efficiency becomes acute. Efficiency is defined

    as the ratio of social information gained to effort expended. Sherman, Conrey and Groom (2004)

    provide empirical support for the model. Their experiment comprised the same impression

    formation task as that of Allen et al. (2009), except with different stereotypes. The load

    manipulation was also the samerehearsal of the 8-digit number. Their data showed faster

    reaction times to consistent information under high and low load conditions, whichindicates confirmatory bias toward stereotype-relevant information. However, reaction

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    3/21

    times to inconsistent items were faster under by high load. The researchers explicate that

    existence of the EFM can be inferred not from preference toward inconsistent information

    but from the shiftof attention toward inconsistent information (and away from consistent

    information), and its greater perceptual encodingwhich is exactly what their results

    reflected. Perceivers can only be sensitized to unexpected information if they possess priorexpectancies. So categorical thinking provides the flexibility the person perception process

    demands - by delivering either stability or plasticity as required (Macrae & Bodenhausen,

    2000).

    McKimmie, Masters, Masser, Schuller and Terry (2013; Study 3) provide further evidence for

    EFM, investigated in a juror decision-making scenario, a context having more powerful and

    weighty consequences than everyday social life. Participants were given a transcript of a

    criminal case and had to determine the defendants guilt, measured through a

    questionnaire. Defendant stereotypicality was manipulated by varying gender (females areviewed as counterstereotypical). Strength of the defendants case was also manipulated,

    measured through multiple-choice questionnaire. They hypothesised that case strength

    would be given more attention in the case of stereotypical defendants (males), than that of

    counterstereotypical defendants (females). Being incongruent with gender-expectations, a

    female defendant requires greater perceptual encoding, reducing the resources available

    for analysing the evidence. Their hypothesis was confirmed and they concluded that jurors

    are cognitive optimizers (p. 344) rather than cognitive misers, as they enlist different

    depths of information processing depending on the defendant. To elaborate, a

    counterstereotypical defendant benefits from more thorough impression formation, and astereotypical defendant receives fuller evaluation of evidence, and these two modes of

    operation are thought to reflect cognitive versatility. This study provides sound theoretical

    insight and highlights practical implications of stereotype use. However, it is limited in that

    the participants were students acting as jurors. In reality, jurors are presumably better

    trained at making judgments and might think in different ways, also given the high pressure

    of an actual courtroom scenario.

    Perceptual Efficiency

    While the studies outlined so far relay the cognitive economy of stereotypes, recent

    evidence moreover suggests that perceptual efficiency is another function of stereotyping

    (Cloutier & Macrae, 2007). In fact, perceptual operations guide categorical thinking even

    before cognitive effects can surface (Cloutier & Macrae, 2007). For example, Cloutier,

    Macrae and Mason (2005) aimed to investigate the ease with which social information can

    be gleaned from faces. Participants were shown faces that had to be recognized in terms of

    either gender (categorization process) or familiarity - some were faces of celebrities

    (identification process). They manipulated complexity of processing by inverting faces in one

    study, degrading faces in a second study and presenting faces rapidly in a third, to cover arange of difficult viewing conditions. As predicted, categorizing on gender basis was easier

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    4/21

    than identification, as indicated by faster reaction times and greater accurate responses.

    Cloutier and Macrae (2007) made compatible discoveries using the same paradigm. Using

    facial inversion, in one study (Study 2) they manipulated presence of hairstyles and found

    that, while gender categorization was easier than identification, it was easier still when

    hairstyle was depicted than when it was not. In another study (Study 3) they investigated

    race as category-specifying cue, whilst maintaining the facial inversion manipulation. Faces

    had to be categorized as Caucasian or African American and on some trials, faces were

    presented in a green hue so that skin tone was indiscernible. Once again, categorization was

    much harder in the absence of skin tone information. Thus, some featural details serve as

    critical category-specifying cues. Furthermore, the researchers state that perhaps it is not

    incidental that the dominant categories in routine perceptions (race, age, sex) are ones that

    are cued by easily discernible features. All in all, the present work along with Cloutier et al.s

    (2005) work indicate that categorical construal is prompted by basic featural cues and isresistant to disruptions (distortion of viewing conditions, as in the experiments) and these

    are functional properties that imply stereotyping as a process that streamlines social

    processing (Macrae et al., 2005).

    Further backing comes from neuroscientific evidence provided by Quadflieg et al.s (2011)

    study confirming the modulation of person perception through stereotyping at the basic

    level of cortical functioning. In a social judgment task, participants viewed photographs of

    people in either gender-stereotypical or counterstereotypical occupational roles and had to

    determine gender of the person depicted. While doing so, fMRI measured activity in brainregions thought to comprise the neural system of person perception (Gobbini & Haxby,

    2007, as cited in Quadflieg et al., 2011). Results did reveal increased activity in said regions.

    Moreover, when there was no element of social judgment, only low-level visual areas were

    activated, indicating the treatment of targets as simple perceptual entities rather than

    socially significant (Quadflieg et al., 2011). This brings to focus the role of processing goals,

    elucidating that stereotyping should not be taken for granted as, not only does it contribute

    to perceiving others but it does so in meaningful, goal-oriented ways. Another point to

    note, this piece of work along with Cloutier et al.s (2005) and Cloutier and Macraes (2007)

    research use pictorial representations to elicit stereotypes rather than verbal stimuli (as isdone in many a studies), which is a methodological strength as it better clarifies the role

    played by perceptual operations (Macrae et al., 2005).

    Enhancing Comprehension

    Stereotype use can be examined in terms of easing the burden of perception but also, from

    the very aim of better understanding and making meaning of the new individuals we

    encounter. They ease perception when information processing is constrained (as per

    evidence provided hitherto), but also they are used differentially depending on the

    situational variables to best aid comprehension of novel others (McGarty et al., 2002). For

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    5/21

    example, Hoshino-Browne and Kunda (2000) found that when a topic sensitive to a racially

    stereotyped-group arises during interaction with a group member, the relevant stereotype

    is used to predict that persons reactions and avoid conflict andinsensitivity. Interestingly, in

    the same situation, some participants actually stereotyped less with the motivation to

    appear unprejudiced.

    Apart from social stereotypes, person inferences are also formed using trait-categories that

    provide more individuating information (Quinn & Rosenthal, 2012). Some have found

    stereotypes to be associatively richer and more distinctive and therefore more informative

    (Andersen & Klatzky, 1987). They are also accessed more quickly and better encoded in

    memory than traits (Andersen & Klatzky, 1990). Yet others have found individuating

    information to be more predictive of character (Kunda & Spencer, 2003). However,

    stereotypes might actually assist trait-based inferences as when a targets behaviour is

    stereotype-consistent, trait inferences are more likely to be made. Additionally, when targetbehaviour is stereotype-inconsistent, attention shifts to form situational inferences to

    explain the discrepancy (Ramos, Garcia-Marques, Hamilton, Ferreira & Acker, 2012).

    Therefore, stereotypes can also be advantageous by prompting alternate encoding of

    behaviour (Ramos et al., 2012). The adaptability of stereotypes emerges again in that, they

    can either act as gate-keepers, extracting expected information, or redistribute efforts

    toward novel information by undermining themselves should the need arise (Allen et al.,

    2009). The EFM previously discussed operates on the same principle.

    Another way stereotyping fulfills comprehension goals is by aiding everyday mind readingor empathic accuracy - the ability to infer other peoples thoughts, as discovered by Lewis,

    Hodges, Laurent, Srivastava, Biancarosa (2012, p. 1040). Their experiment used the

    stereotype of new mothers. Participants inferred multiple thoughts of multiple new

    mothers, following viewing a video of the targets naturalistic behaviour. Accuracy was

    measured by comparing participants inferences to the targets actual reported thoughts,

    carried out by 6 coders (achieving inter-coder reliability). The more stereotypic knowledge

    applied, the more accurate the overall empathic inferences were. Albeit, the researchers

    point out that this finding may not generalize to other stereotypes that are not as

    accurate/factual, which reduces theoretical generalizability. A plus point on the other hand,is the element of ecological validity. Participants made inferences based on viewing

    naturalistic behaviour of real-life persons, which presumably better captures the dynamics

    of perceptions in reality. This is very pertinent because in laboratory settings, perception

    operates in more passive ways than it would in reality. To elaborate, real world perceivers

    choose the kind and quantity of information obtained whereas laboratory perceivers are

    limited to pre-selected stimuli and behaviour represented only by verbal or pictorial forms

    (Smith & Collins, 2009). The present study corrects for this problem at least partially.

    Perception Or Misperception?

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    6/21

    Now that the purposive nature of stereotypes across a range of processing contexts and

    objectives has been demonstrated, a few challenging perspectives should be considered.

    For starters, stereotypes are conventionally thought to be inaccurate overgeneralizations,

    caricatures of social reality (Park & Judd, 1993, p. 110). However, actually determining

    accuracy is very hard to do empirically as there exist different criterions for accuracy andmore obviously, statements cannot be made about the accuracy of stereotypes in general

    because there exist countless stereotypes of all kinds, some socially shared and some

    individually contrived (Park & Judd, 1993). More fundamentally, if stereotypes are so

    inaccurate and erroneous why are they so routinely used, how are they able to aid

    explanation and how can a process so rudimentary and basic be so deficient (McGarty et al.,

    2002; Macrae et al., 1994)? A potential solution to the puzzle: stereotyping provides viable

    although potentially erroneous judgments at little cognitive cost and these characterizations

    are adequate enough for the interactions we routinely face and should the need arise, they

    can be adjusted and improved upon with a little extra effort (Macrae et al., 1994; Gilbert &Hixon, 1991). The value of such an inferential system, after all lies in its rapid, automatic and

    effortless provision of inferential knowledge (Gilbert, 1989, as cited in Macrae et al., 1994).

    Another pressing issue is that many believe prejudice is intrinsically linked to stereotyping

    and is practically inevitable (e.g., Allport, 1954). Firstly, it is important to clarify that in terms

    of perceiving groups, stereotyping has a mainly cognitive position, whereas prejudice is

    related to affect (Schneider, 2004). Even so, it has been shown that both high-prejudice and

    low-prejudice individuals activate stereotype, even against conscious will or awareness, but

    only the latter inhibit stereotypes with the motivation to prevent prejudice (Devine, 1989).Granted there is a link between the two concepts as high-prejudiced individuals had to

    activate and apply relevant stereotypes to then enact prejudice but the separation of

    stereotyping from prejudice is still evident as stereotyping does not necessitate prejudice.

    Moreover, the motivation to avoid prejudice can inhibit activation of stereotypes or allow

    activation but inhibit application, such as when egalitarian goals or salient egalitarian norms

    dominate, further demonstrating that prejudice is a step further demonstrating that

    stereotyping and prejudice are not interchangeable processes (Kunda & Spencer, 2003).

    Lastly, literature in this area tends to focus on race, gender and age stereotypes and thesefunction uniquely (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). Pitfalls such as stereotype threat (being

    pressured by expectations of a stereotype that one belongs to) and self-fulfilling prophecies

    (fulfilling expectations simply because they exist) occur mostly in relation to said categories

    (Jussim, 2012). Some researchers contend that such consequences should not be considered

    in the analysis of stereotype use for they do not really provide insight about accuracy or the

    effectiveness of the perceptions formed (Jussim, 2012). Be that as it may, Macrae and

    Bodenhausen (2000) propose that these social categories actually represent the

    fundamental divides in society and so too are important to perceivers social

    representations. They also point out that not all categories are alike in their functioning,their activation, application and validity, and so they should not always be treated equally.

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    7/21

    Therefore, the issues that surface in regard to some sensitive and complex categories need

    not necessarily apply to stereotyping as a process in general.

    In summation, stereotypes exist because of their functional value and so whether they are

    good or bad, or true or false is irrelevant to understanding the process and only when the

    moral and attitudinal issues surrounding stereotypes are put aside will we be able to do so

    (Tyler & Aboud, 1973). Therefore, stereotypes are a fundamentally valuable part of the

    person perception system as expressed by Gilbert and Hixon (1991), the ability to

    understand new and unique individuals in terms of old and general beliefs is certainly

    among the handiest tools in the social perceivers kit. To borrow Lewis et al.s (2012)

    comparison, when reading a story, just as the reader looks beyond the words on the page

    and turns to prior knowledge and expectations to fill in gaps in understanding, so too the

    social perceiver reaches beyond directly observable stimuli and unto stereotypes to fill in

    the perception of a novel person.

    Taylor, D. M., & Aboud, F. E. (1973). Ethnic stereotypes: Is the concept necessary?. Canadian

    Psychologist/Psychologie Canadienne, 14(4), 330.

    Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: their automatic and controlled components. Journal

    of personality and social psychology, 56(1), 5.

    Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1993). Definition and assessment of accuracy in social stereotypes.

    Psychological review, 100(1), 109.

    STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    8/21

    PERCEPTION.

    Student number: 610051145

    Module code: PSY 3251

    Word count: 2996

    Person perception is fundamentally important to everyday social life (Smith & Collins, 2009).

    It encompasses evaluations, judgments and impressions of target others (Macrae &

    Bodenhausen, 2000) and inferences about their mental states, goals and traits (Quinn &

    Rosenthal, 2012). Thinking categorically about others shapes person perception (Macrae &

    Bodenhausen, 2000) and stereotypes modulate the formation of person percepts (Quadflieg,

    2013). Simply put, stereotypes are schemas (i.e., mental representations) about groups of

    people (Moskowitz, 2005). They help perceivers make sense of their social worlds by aiding

    explanation and serve as energy-saving mental devices (McGarty, Yzerbyt & Spears, 2002;

    Macrae, Milne & Bodenhausen, 1994). Categorical thinking provides the flexibility that the

    person perception process demands (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000) and stereotypes can be

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    9/21

    adjusted to the perceivers needs (Sherman, 2004). Therefore, stereotypes are thought of as

    handy cognitive tools (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Macrae et al., 1994; McGarty et al., 2002;

    Sherman, 2004).

    In contrast, Gordon Allport (1954), who was highly influential, believed that stereotypes are

    faulty exaggerations, they lead to error and bias and are permeated by prejudice. Much

    research since then has followed the same tack (Jussim, 2012), especially in the case of

    stereotypes related to ethnicity, gender and race (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). Given this

    controversy, are stereotypes truly tools that aid perception or do they create misperception?

    Thereafter, this essay aims to explore the assertion that Stereotypes are cognitive tools

    designed to facilitate the process of person perception. Evidence for the functional purposes

    of stereotypes in the person perception process will be put forth. Some contradictory views of

    stereotyping being unconstructive and harmful will be addressed.

    Resource Conservation

    Some psychologists believe perceivers to be cognitive misers who rely on stereotypes as

    shortcuts instead of thinking more deeply (Sherman, Bessenoff & Frost, 1998). But others

    argue for the functionality of stereotypes, believing that people are not simply lazy; they

    employ stereotypes because they are simplifying mental structures. Macrae et al.s (1994;

    Study 2 & 3) work was seminal to demonstrating the same. Participants concurrently

    performed an impression formation and a prose-monitoring task. They were primed with a

    stereotype label, and so expected to assign more stereotypic traits to a given target. Also

    expected was better performance on the other task under this condition, as activating the

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    10/21

    stereotype through the label would make impression formation easier thus allocating more

    resources to the prose task. This is indeed what they found, indicating that forming

    impressions through stereotypes is the perceivers go-to option, and better performance on

    the concurrent task reveals that stereotypes do indeed simplify the process. Despite providing

    this insight, a pitfall emerges in that the procedure is too transparent about the impression

    formation task, potentially evoking demand characteristics. Additionally, the judgment task

    involved assigning trait descriptions to targets from a predetermined set, which could have

    perpetuated stereotypical thinking that might not have otherwise occurred.

    Macrae, Hewstone and Griffiths (1993) similarly establish the heuristic usefulness of

    stereotyping under processing load. They watched a video of a woman providing information

    (both stereotype-consistent and inconsistent) about herself. Prior to the video, participants

    were primed with an occupational label. Load was manipulated by having participants

    rehearse an 8-digit number prior to the video that they were told would be later tested for

    recall. Creating load on attentional resources was theorized to reduce allocation of resources

    to the subsequent video-watching and so, those under load would subsequently recall more

    stereotype-consistent information. And this did happen.

    In more recent research, Allen, Sherman, Corney, Stroessner (2009; Study 1) also looked at

    processing capacity, except their findings revealed a different pattern. Under high cognitive

    load, having strong stereotypes resulted in attending toward stereotype-inconsistent

    information. Load was similarly manipulated through rehearsal of an 8-digit number.

    Impressions of a Black male target were to be formed through provided behavioural

    descriptions. These appeared on a computer screen in pairs and on some trials had to be

    responded to by pressing keys. If a descriptive item was attended to more than its counterpart,

    key-press response to it would be quicker - this was their prediction. Contrary to previous

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    11/21

    research, they found that under load, responses were actually faster and more accurate for

    stereotype inconsistent information. They replicated their findings in a second study, with the

    difference being a White female target instead. The stereotype of women is positively

    valenced whereas that of African Americans is negatively valenced, and so they wanted to

    test both in their paradigm to demonstrate the generality of their findings, demonstrating

    methodological foresight. They successfully obtained the same results, achieving robustness.

    Another strength, as the researchers point out, is their direct measurement of attention instead

    of relying on measures of memory that only suggest attentional use.

    Encoding Flexibility

    The pattern Allen et al. (2009) found fits in with Sherman et al.s (1998) encoding-flexibility

    model (EFM). According to EFM, stereotype-consistent information, being easy to

    comprehend and confirmatory of prior knowledge, receives little attention and finer details

    are not thoroughly encoded but the basic gist is extracted in case of possible use. Attention is

    instead redirected toward inconsistent information because it is harder to make sense of and

    receives more perceptual and contextual encoding (encoding of sensory details and specifics

    of contexts), whereas consistent information receives only the cursory glance that is

    conceptual attention. Therefore, stereotypes enable this dual process form of encoding that is

    flexible and sophisticated and particularly important when resources are low and the need for

    efficiency becomes acute. Efficiency is defined as the ratio of social information gained to

    effort expended. Sherman, Conrey and Groom (2004) provide empirical support for the

    model. Their experiment comprised the same impression formation task as that of Allen et al.

    (2009), except with different stereotypes. The load manipulation was also the same

    rehearsal of the 8-digit number. Their data showed faster reaction times to consistent

    information under high and low load conditions, which indicates confirmatory bias toward

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    12/21

    stereotype-relevant information. However, reaction times to inconsistent items were faster

    under by high load. The researchers explicate that existence of the EFM can be inferred not

    from preference toward inconsistent information but from the shiftof attention toward

    inconsistent information (and away from consistent information), and its greater perceptual

    encodingwhich is exactly what their results reflected. Perceivers can only be sensitized to

    unexpected information if they possess prior expectancies. So categorical thinking provides

    the flexibility the person perception process demands - by delivering either stability or

    plasticity as required (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000).

    McKimmie, Masters, Masser, Schuller and Terry (2013; Study 3) provide further evidence

    for EFM, investigated in a juror decision-making scenario, a context having more powerful

    and weighty consequences than everyday social life. Participants were given a transcript of a

    criminal case and had to determine the defendants guilt, measured through a questionnaire.

    Defendant stereotypicality was manipulated by varying gender (females are viewed as

    counterstereotypical). Strength of the defendants case was also manipulated, measured

    through multiple-choice questionnaire. They hypothesised that case strength would be given

    more attention in the case of stereotypical defendants (males), than that of

    counterstereotypical defendants (females). Being incongruent with gender-expectations, a

    female defendant requires greater perceptual encoding, reducing the resources available for

    analysing the evidence. Their hypothesis was confirmed and they concluded that jurors are

    cognitive optimizers (p. 344) rather than cognitive misers, as they enlistdifferent depths of

    information processing depending on the defendant. To elaborate, a counterstereotypical

    defendant benefits from more thorough impression formation, and a stereotypical defendant

    receives fuller evaluation of evidence, and these two modes of operation are thought to reflect

    cognitive versatility. This study provides sound theoretical insight and highlights practical

    implications of stereotype use. However, it is limited in that the participants were students

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    13/21

    acting as jurors. In reality, jurors are presumably better trained at making judgments and

    might think in different ways, also given the high pressure of an actual courtroom scenario.

    Perceptual Efficiency

    While the studies outlined so far relay the cognitive economy of stereotypes, recent evidence

    moreover suggests that perceptual efficiency is another function of stereotyping (Cloutier &

    Macrae, 2007). In fact, perceptual operations guide categorical thinking even before cognitive

    effects can surface (Cloutier & Macrae, 2007). For example, Cloutier, Macrae and Mason

    (2005) aimed to investigate the ease with which social information can be gleaned from

    faces. Participants were shown faces that had to be recognized in terms of either gender

    (categorization process) or familiarity - some were faces of celebrities (identification

    process). They manipulated complexity of processing by inverting faces in one study,

    degrading faces in a second study and presenting faces rapidly in a third, to cover a range of

    difficult viewing conditions. As predicted, categorizing on gender basis was easier than

    identification, as indicated by faster reaction times and greater accurate responses.

    Cloutier and Macrae (2007) made compatible discoveries using the same paradigm. Using

    facial inversion, in one study (Study 2) they manipulated presence of hairstyles and found

    that, while gender categorization was easier than identification, it was easier still when

    hairstyle was depicted than when it was not. In another study (Study 3) they investigated race

    as category-specifying cue, whilst maintaining the facial inversion manipulation. Faces had to

    be categorized as Caucasian or African American and on some trials, faces were presented in

    a green hue so that skin tone was indiscernible. Once again, categorization was much harder

    in the absence of skin tone information. Thus, some featural details serve as critical category-

    specifying cues. Furthermore, the researchers state that perhaps it is not incidental that the

    dominant categories in routine perceptions (race, age, sex) are ones that are cued by easily

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    14/21

    discernible features. All in all, the present work along with Cloutier et al.s (2005) work

    indicate that categorical construal is prompted by basic featural cues and is resistant to

    disruptions (distortion of viewing conditions, as in the experiments) and these are functional

    properties that imply stereotyping as a process that streamlines social processing (Macrae et

    al., 2005).

    Further backing comes from neuroscientific evidence provided by Quadflieg et al.s(2011)

    study confirming the modulation of person perception through stereotyping at the basic level

    of cortical functioning. In a social judgment task, participants viewed photographs of people

    in either gender-stereotypical or counterstereotypical occupational roles and had to determine

    gender of the person depicted. While doing so, fMRI measured activity in brain regions

    thought to comprise the neural system of person perception (Gobbini & Haxby, 2007, as cited

    in Quadflieg et al., 2011). Results did reveal increased activity in said regions. Moreover,

    when there was no element of social judgment, only low-level visual areas were activated,

    indicating the treatment of targets as simple perceptual entities rather than socially significant

    (Quadflieg et al., 2011). This brings to focus the role of processing goals, elucidating that

    stereotyping should not be taken for granted as, not only does it contribute to perceiving

    others but it does so in meaningful, goal-oriented ways. Another point to note, this piece of

    work along with Cloutier et al.s (2005) and Cloutier and Macraes (2007) research use

    pictorial representations to elicit stereotypes rather than verbal stimuli (as is done in many a

    studies), which is a methodological strength as it better clarifies the role played by perceptual

    operations (Macrae et al., 2005).

    Enhancing Comprehension

    Stereotype use can be examined in terms of easing the burden of perception but also, from the

    very aim of better understanding and making meaning of the new individuals we encounter.

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    15/21

    They ease perception when information processing is constrained (as per evidence provided

    hitherto), but also they are used differentially depending on the situational variables to best

    aid comprehension of novel others (McGarty et al., 2002). For example, Hoshino-Browne

    and Kunda (2000) found that when a topic sensitive to a racially stereotyped-group arises

    during interaction with a group member, the relevant stereotype is used to predict that

    persons reactions and avoid conflict and insensitivity. Interestingly, in the same situation,

    some participants actually stereotyped less with the motivation to appear unprejudiced. The

    adaptability of stereotypes to perceivers needs is thus once again evident.

    Apart from social stereotypes, person inferences are also formed using trait-categories that

    provide more individuating information (Quinn & Rosenthal, 2012). Some have found

    stereotypes to be associatively richer and more distinctive and therefore more informative

    (Andersen & Klatzky, 1987). They are also accessed more quickly and better encoded in

    memory than traits (Andersen & Klatzky, 1990). Yet others have found individuating

    information to be more predictive of character (Kunda & Spencer, 2003). However,

    stereotypes might actually assist trait-based inferences as when a targets behaviour is

    stereotype-consistent, trait inferences are more likely to be made. Additionally, when target

    behaviour is stereotype-inconsistent, attention shifts to form situational inferences to explain

    the discrepancy (Ramos, Garcia-Marques, Hamilton, Ferreira & Acker, 2012). Therefore,

    stereotypes can also be advantageous by prompting alternate encoding of behaviour (Ramos

    et al., 2012). The adaptability of stereotypes emerges again in that, they can either act as gate-

    keepers, extracting expected information, or redistribute efforts toward novel information by

    undermining themselves should the need arise (Allen et al., 2009). The EFM previously

    discussed operates on the same principle.

    Another way stereotyping fulfils comprehension goals is by aiding everyday mind reading

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    16/21

    or empathic accuracy - the ability to infer other peoples thoughts, as discovered by Lewis,

    Hodges, Laurent, Srivastava, Biancarosa (2012, p. 1040). Their experiment used the

    stereotype of new mothers. Participants inferred multiple thoughts of multiple new mothers,

    following viewing a video of the targets naturalistic behaviour. Accuracy was measured by

    comparing participants inferences to the targets actual reported thoughts, carried outby 6

    coders (achieving inter-coder reliability). The more stereotypic knowledge applied, the more

    accurate the overall empathic inferences were. Albeit, the researchers point out that this

    finding may not generalize to other stereotypes that are not as accurate/factual, which reduces

    theoretical generalizability. A plus point on the other hand, is the element of ecological

    validity. Participants made inferences based on viewing naturalistic behaviour of real-life

    persons, which presumably better captures the dynamics of perceptions in reality. This is very

    pertinent because in laboratory settings, perception operates in more passive ways than it

    would in reality. To elaborate, real world perceivers choose the kind and quantity of

    information obtained whereas laboratory perceivers are limited to pre-selected stimuli and

    behaviour represented only by verbal or pictorial forms (Smith & Collins, 2009). The present

    study corrects for this problem at least partially.

    Perception Or Misperception?

    Now that the purposive nature of stereotypes across a range of processing contexts and

    objectives has been demonstrated, a few challenging perspectives should be considered. For

    starters, stereotypes are conventionally thought to be inaccurate overgeneralizations,

    caricatures of social reality (Park & Judd, 1993, p. 110). However, actually determining

    accuracy is very hard to do empirically as there exist different criterions for accuracy and

    more obviously, statements cannot be made about the accuracy of stereotypes in general

    because there exist countless stereotypes of all kinds, some socially shared and some

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    17/21

    individually contrived (Park & Judd, 1993). More fundamentally, if stereotypes are so

    inaccurate and erroneous why are they so routinely used, how are they able to aid explanation

    and how can a process so rudimentary and basic be so deficient (McGarty et al., 2002;

    Macrae et al., 1994)? A potential solution to the puzzle: stereotyping provides viable

    although potentially erroneous judgments at little cognitive cost and these characterizations

    are adequate enough for the interactions we routinely face and should the need arise, they can

    be adjusted and improved upon with a little extra effort (Macrae et al., 1994; Gilbert &

    Hixon, 1991). The value of such an inferential system, after all lies in its rapid, automatic and

    effortless provision of inferential knowledge (Gilbert, 1989, as cited in Macrae et al., 1994).

    Another pressing issue is that many believe prejudice is intrinsically linked to stereotyping

    and is practically inevitable (e.g., Allport, 1954). Firstly, it is important to clarify that in

    terms of perceiving groups, stereotyping has a mainly cognitive position, whereas prejudice is

    related to affect (Schneider, 2004). Even so, it has been shown that both high-prejudice and

    low-prejudice individuals activate stereotype, even against conscious will or awareness, but

    only the latter inhibit stereotypes with the motivation to prevent prejudice (Devine, 1989).

    Granted there is a link between the two concepts as high-prejudiced individuals had to

    activate and apply relevant stereotypes to then enact prejudice but the separation of

    stereotyping from prejudice is still evident as stereotyping does not necessitate prejudice.

    Moreover, the motivation to avoid prejudice can inhibit activation of stereotypes or allow

    activation but inhibit application, such as when egalitarian goals or salient egalitarian norms

    dominate, further demonstrating that prejudice is a step further demonstrating that

    stereotyping and prejudice are not interchangeable processes (Kunda & Spencer, 2003).

    Lastly, literature in this area tends to focus on race, gender and age stereotypes and these

    function uniquely (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). Pitfalls such as stereotype threat (being

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    18/21

    pressured by expectations of a stereotype that one belongs to) and self-fulfilling prophecies

    (fulfilling expectations simply because they exist) occur mostly in relation to said categories

    (Jussim, 2012). Some researchers contend that such consequences should not be considered

    in the analysis of stereotype use for they do not really provide insight about accuracy or the

    effectiveness of the perceptions formed (Jussim, 2012). Be that as it may, Macrae and

    Bodenhausen (2000) propose that these social categories actually represent the fundamental

    divides in society and so too are important to perceivers social representations. They also

    point out that not all categories are alike in their functioning, their activation, application and

    validity, and so they should not always be treated equally. Therefore, the issues that surface

    in regard to some sensitive and complex categories need not necessarily apply to stereotyping

    as a process in general.

    In summation, stereotypes exist because of their functional value and so whether they are

    good or bad, or true or false is irrelevant to understanding the process and only when the

    moral and attitudinal issues surrounding stereotypes are put aside will we be able to do so

    (Tyler & Aboud, 1973). Therefore, stereotypes are a fundamentally valuable part of the

    person perception system as expressed by Gilbert and Hixon (1991), the ability to

    understand new and unique individuals in terms of old and general beliefs is certainly among

    the handiest tools in the social perceivers kit. And to borrow Lewis et al.s (2012)

    comparison, when reading a story, just as the reader looks beyond the words on the page and

    turns to prior knowledge and expectations to fill in gaps in understanding, so too the social

    perceiver reaches beyond directly observable stimuli and unto stereotypes to fill in the

    perception of a novel person.

    References

    Allen, T. J., Sherman, J. W., Conrey, F. R., & Stroessner, S. J. (2009). Stereotype strength and

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    19/21

    attentional bias: Preference for confirming versus disconfirming information depends on

    processing capacity.Journal of experimental social psychology, 45(5), 1081-1087.

    Andersen, S. M., Klatzky, R. L. (1987). Traits and social stereotypes: Levels of categorization

    in person perception.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(2), 235-246.

    Andersen, S. M., Klatzky, R. L., & Murray, J. (1990). Traits and social stereotypes: Efficiency

    differences in social information processing.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

    59(2), 192-201.

    Cloutier, J., Mason, M. F., & Macrae, C. N. (2005). The perceptual determinants of person

    construal: Reopening the social-cognitive toolbox.Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 88(6), 885-894.

    Cloutier, J., Macrae, C. N. (2007). Who or what are you?: Facial orientation and person

    construal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37(6), 1298-1309.

    Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled

    components.Journal

    of Personality and Social Psychology,

    56(1), 5-18.

    Dijksterhuis, A., van Knippenberg, A. (1996). The knife that cuts both ways: Facilitated and

    inhibited access to traits as a result of stereotype activation.Journal of Experimental Social

    Psychology, 32(3), 271-288.

    Gilbert, D. T., Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation and application of

    stereotypic beliefs.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(4), 509-517.

    Judd, C. M., Park, B. (1993). Definition and assessment of accuracy in social stereotypes.

    Psychological

    Review, 100(1),

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    20/21

    109-128.

    Jussim, L. (2012). Social perception and social reality: Why accuracy dominates bias and self-

    fulfilling prophecy. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Kunda, Z., Spencer, S. J. (2003). When do stereotypes come to mind and when do they color

    judgment? A goal-based theoretical framework for stereotype activation and application.

    Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 522-544.

    Lewis, K. L., Hodges, S. D., Laurent, S. M., Srivastava, S., & Biancarosa, G. (2012). Reading

    between the minds: The use of stereotypes in empathic accuracy. Psychological Science,

    23(9), 1040-1046.

    Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). Social cognition: Thinking categorically about

    others.Annual Review of Psychology, 51(1), 93-120.

    Macrae, C. N., Hewstone, M., & Griffiths, R. J. (1993). Processing load and memory for

    stereotype based information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23(1), 77-87.

    Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (1994). Stereotypes as energy-saving

    devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

    66(1), 37-47.

    McGarty, C., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Spears, R. (Eds.). (2002). Stereotypes as explanations: The

    formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups. Cambridge: Cambridge University

    Press.

    McKimmie, B. M., Masters, J. M., Masser, B. M., Schuller, R. A., & Terry, D. J. (2012).

    Stereotypical and counterstereotypical defendants: Who is he and what was the case

    against her?. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 19(3), 343-354.

    Moskowitz, G. B. (2005). Social cognition: Understanding self and others. New York, NY:

    Guilford Press.

  • 8/12/2019 STEREOTYPES ARE DESIGNED TO FACILITATE THE PROCESS OF PERSON PERCEPTION. Discuss.

    21/21

    Ramos, T., Garcia-Marques, L., Hamilton, D. L., Ferreira, M., & Van Acker, K. (2012). What I

    infer depends on who you are: The influence of stereotypes on trait and situational

    spontaneous inferences.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6), 1247-1256.

    Schneider, D. J. (2005). The psychology of stereotyping. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Sherman, J. W., Conrey, F. R., & Groom, C. J. (2004). Encoding flexibility revisited: Evidence

    for enhanced encoding of stereotype-inconsistent information under cognitive load. Social

    Cognition, 22(2), 214-232.

    Sherman, J. W., Lee, A. Y., Bessenoff, G. R., & Frost, L. A. (1998). Stereotype efficiency

    reconsidered: Encoding flexibility under cognitive load.Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 75(3), 589-606.

    Smith, E. R., Collins, E. C. (2009). Contextualizing person perception: Distributed social

    cognition. Psychological Review, 116(2), 343-364.

    Taylor, D. M., Aboud, F. E. (1973). Ethnic stereotypes: Is the concept necessary?. The

    Canadian

    Psychologist, 14(4), 330-338.

    Quadflieg, S., Flannigan, N., Waiter, G. D., Rossion, B., Wig, G. S., Turk, D. J., & Macrae, C. N.

    (2011). Stereotype-based modulation of person perception. NeuroImage, 57(2), 549-557.

    Quinn, K. A., Rosenthal, H. E. (2012). Categorizing others and the self: How social memory

    structures guide social perception and behavior. Learning and Motivation, 43, 247-258.


Recommended