+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology,...

Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology,...

Date post: 28-Mar-2015
Category:
Upload: avery-preston
View: 213 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
Popular Tags:
23
Subitizing, pre- attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen
Transcript
Page 1: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise

Roy Allen & Peter McGeorgeSchool of Psychology, University of Aberdeen

Page 2: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Introduction 1

• Navigation through the environment

• Visual Indexing theory (e.g., Pylyshyn & Storm, 1988)

• Evidence from multiple-object tracking (MOT) task:

• Target Acquisition (TA) and Target Tracking (TT) phases

• TA – preattentive indexing (circa 4), then modality-specific attentional/strategic processes

• TT – iterative central executive processes (Allen et al., in press)

Page 3: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Introduction 2

• Also affected by the extent to which target elements can be grouped (Yantis, 1992):

• Yantis suggests tracking may involve a higher-order virtual object (virtual polygon) spontaneously formed from active indexes

• For example, novice participants given a grouping clue have an immediate advantage but this disappears with practice – suggesting an expertise effect, something reported by Allen et al., 2004 and associated with the TA phase’s modality-specific attentional/strategic (grouping?) processes.

Page 4: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Introduction 3

• Trick and Pylyshyn (1993,1994) suggest that subitizing – our apparent ability, with small numbers of elements, to instantly quantify them without the need to count them individually – is also underpinned by visual indexing

• i.e., the indexing of elements is preattentive and occurs in a parallel way that makes them all available simultaneously for subsequent attentional processing

Page 5: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Introduction 4

• Given that the MOT task and subitizing are thought to share a common, indexing process, we might expect performance on an enumeration task to benefit from stimuli that facilitate the formation of a virtual polygon and be hindered by those that resist it (i.e., stimuli composed of elements arranged linearly). Experiment 1 will investigate this

• Further, given Allen et al.’s (2004) findings, it might be expected that, in an enumeration task, experts will show better performance than novices when stimuli can be readily grouped, but equivalent levels of performance when grouping is effectively removed. Experiment 2 will investigate this

Page 6: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Experiment 1

• Participants: 27 psychology students (6 males) aged between 18 and 44 years (M = 25.3, SD = 7.93), all with normal or corrected-to-normal vision

• Materials: bitmaps of from 1 – 6 identical items (+’s), whose extents were always defined by a notional 50mm dia circle at the screen’s centre. Items were arranged in regular polygons (triangles, squares, pentagons, hexagons), straight lines and intermediate “collapsed” polygons – see over

• Stimuli were displayed using E-Prime running on a 350mhz Pentium II PC with 17-inch monitor set to a resolution of 800 x 600 (SVGA) at a viewing distance of approx 57cm.

Page 7: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

+

+

++

++

+++

+

+++

+

+

+

+

+

+

+

+

+

+ +

++

+

++

+

++

++

++

+

+ +

++

+ +++

+++

+++

++

+

Stimuli

Page 8: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Fixation – 500ms o

Blank – 500ms

++ +Stimulus presentation – 17/34/51ms

Response – unlimited time respond

Paradigm

Page 9: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

• Within-subjects

• Number of items (1 – 6) x arrangement (canonical, collapsed, linear) x presentation time (17, 34, 51ms – multiples of the refresh rate)

• Arrangements’ orientations were varied to reduce long-term memory pattern learning

• Presentation order completely randomised

• Total of 864 trials

• Duration circa 45 minutes

• Note: participants were told there were from 1 – 7 items in the stimuli to minimise any end effect (Simon, Peterson, Patel, & Sathian, 1998)

Design

Page 10: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Performance by number of items

60

70

80

90

100

1 2 3 4 5 6

Number of items

% a

ccu

racy

% correct

Data

Page 11: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

• Hierarchical regression analysis (% accuracy data) - significant quadratic component supportive of a two-process account of enumeration (i.e., subitizing/counting)

• Repeated-measures ANOVA (collapsed over arrangement): main effects of presentation time (longer durations lead to greater accuracy) and number of items (the greater the number of items the poorer the accuracy).

• Post-hoc t-tests for latter showed performance only varied significantly for trials containing 4 versus 5 and 5 versus 6 items (i.e., subitizing – 1-4 items; counting – 5-6 items)

• No significant interaction between presentation time and number of items

Results 1

Page 12: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

DataPerformance by Arrangement

40

60

80

100

1 2 3 4 5 6

Number of items

% a

ccu

racy ungrouped

canonical

collapsed

linear

Page 13: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

• Repeated-measures ANOVA, including arrangement but collapsed over presentation time, for trials containing 3 – 6 items:

• Main effects of number of items and arrangement, but these were moderated by a significant interaction between the two

• Post-hoc t-tests showed that arrangement only had a significant impact when there were more than 4 items per stimulus, such that accuracy for linear arrangements differed significantly from both canonical and collapsed arrangements

• Canonical and collapsed arrangements did not differ significantly from each other

Results 2

Page 14: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

• We have reaffirmed the fact that the number of items in the display has little influence on enumeration performance for stimuli containing up to four items

• Further, that once stimuli contain more than four items performance deteriorates with each additional item

• This reiterates the notion that two different processes seem to be involved, one dominating when the stimuli contain up to four elements (the subitizing range) and the other when the stimuli contain a greater number of elements (the counting range)

• Compared to the linear arrangements both the canonically-arranged stimuli and the collapsed versions of the canonical stimuli result in better performance

• However, grouping information only appears to benefit performance when the display contains more than four items. That is, the grouping advantage only occurs outside of the range in which subitizing is thought to occur

Conclusions:

Page 15: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Experiment 2• Participants: Eighteen students (8 females) at the University of Aberdeen, aged

between 18 and 27 (M = 20.50, SD = 2.77), and 18 civilian air traffic controllers (ATCs) (2 females) employed at Aberdeen airport, aged between 26 and 56 (M = 39, SD = 8.56), all with normal or corrected-to-normal vision

• The latter’s experience as ATCs ranged between 1 and 37 years (M = 14.83, SD = 11.11)

• ATCs were chosen because the nature of their work, in monitoring display screens for long periods of time, tracking multiple targets and responding appropriately, would qualify them as experts as regards an MOT task, something together with subitizing thought to be underpinned by visual indexing

• Materials: same as Experiment 1• Stimuli were displayed using Superlab Pro with 17-inch monitor at a viewing

distance of approx 57cm.• The design and procedure were the same as in Expt. 1 except that hardware

changes resulted in different stimulus presentation times (13 & 26ms – multiples of refresh rate) and “expertise” was an additional, between-subjects variable

Page 16: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Results 1• Hierarchical regression analysis (% accuracy data) – only

produced a significant quadratic component, supportive of a two-process account of enumeration (i.e., subitizing/counting), for the 26ms presentation times. 13ms performance appeared to show a linear decline without the subitizing “plateau”. All subsequent analyses were therefore only carried out using the 26ms data

• Repeated-measures ANOVA (collapsed over arrangement and with Expertise as the between-subjects variable): main effects of number of items and expertise moderated by a significant interaction between the two

• Post-hoc t-tests showed, for trials containing 1 – 4 items, novices performed as well as experts. For trials containing 5 and 6 items, experts were significantly more accurate than novices

Page 17: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Data

Performance by expertise

65

70

75

80

85

90

95

100

105

1 2 3 4 5 6

Number of items

% a

ccu

racy

experts

novices

Page 18: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Results 2• Repeated-measures ANOVA, including arrangement and

expertise, for trials containing 3 – 6 items:• Again, main effect of expertise (experts’ accuracy was

significantly greater than that of novices) • Again, main effects of number of items and arrangement, but

these were moderated by a significant interaction between the two (the effect of arrangement only manifested itself with greater than 4 items per stimulus)

• a marginal three-way interaction of number of items by arrangement by expert gave some support to the initial predictions, and this was confirmed by post-hoc t-tests that showed experts significantly out-performed novices on canonical and collapsed shapes, but not linear arrangements

Page 19: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Expertise by Arrangement

Expertise by Arrangement

60

70

80

90

100

canonical collapsed linear

type of arrangement

% a

ccu

racy

experts

novices

Page 20: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

Conclusions:

• Once again, the number of items in the display has little influence on performance for stimuli containing up to four items. once stimuli contain more than four items performance deteriorates with each additional item. Supports a two-process system

• Compared to linear arrangements both the canonically-arranged stimuli and the collapsed stimuli result in better performance. But, grouping information only appears to benefit performance significantly with displays of more than four items (i.e., during counting)

• Experts out-perform novices, but mainly this is with arrangements that are readily grouped (canonical, collapsed). When grouping is effectively removed performances do not differ significantly (linear)

Page 21: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

General Discussion 1

• These experiments have reinforced and added to our understanding about enumeration’s two processes

• To quantify from 1 to 4 items seems effortless and seldom inaccurate (subitizing)

• For greater than 4 items, quantification is more difficult, increasingly inaccurate and benefits from grouping information (counting, i.e., attentionally-based)

• Experts outperform novices overall, but significant differences in performance are evident for stimuli containing more than 4 items whose arrangement can be readily grouped

Page 22: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

General Discussion 2

• Previously, researchers have shown both a beneficial grouping effect (Yantis, 1992) and an expertise effect (Allen et al., 2004) in a dynamic multiple-object tracking (MOT) task

• This research has demonstrated similar grouping and expertise effects in a static enumeration task

• The enumeration task is very much like the target acquisition phase of the MOT task

• Therefore, whilst this research does not conclusively show that experts are better at the MOT task because of their superior grouping ability, it does provide strong evidence that, if this is the case, then experts already have such advantage before objects begin to move in the MOT task

Page 23: Subitizing, pre-attentive processes and expertise Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen.

ReferencesAllen, R., McGeorge, P., Pearson, D. & Milne, A.B. (2004). Attention and expertise in

multiple target tracking. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 337 - 347.Allen, R., McGeorge, P., Pearson, D. & Milne, A.B. (in press). Multiple-Target

Tracking: A role for Working Memory? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.

Pylyshyn, Z.W. & Storm, R.W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision, 3(3), 179-197.

Simon, T.J., Peterson, S., Patel, G., & Sathian, K. (1998). Do the magnocellular and parvocellular visual pathways contribute differentially to subitizing and counting? Perception and Psychophysics, 60, 451-464.

Trick, L.M. & Pylyshyn, Z.W. (1993). What enumeration studies can show us about spatial attention: Evidence for limited capacity preattentive processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(2), 331-351.

Trick, L.M. & Pylyshyn, Z.W. (1994). Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychological Review, 101(1), 1-23.

Yantis, S. (1992). Multi-element visual tracking: Attention and perceptual organization. Cognitive Psychology, 24(3), 295-340.


Recommended