+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Synergistic Effecn Macrophage Denved Cytokines: Highly ... · (0.1 p g or 0.8 pg), or Th7-a ( 1 pg,...

Synergistic Effecn Macrophage Denved Cytokines: Highly ... · (0.1 p g or 0.8 pg), or Th7-a ( 1 pg,...

Date post: 21-May-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 6 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
101
Synergistic Effecn of Macrophage Denved Cytokines: Consumption of a Highly Pdatable Substance and Plasma Corticosterone Alterations A thesis presented to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research. Carleton Cniversity. in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the degreec of Master of Science in Psycholopy Carleton Cniversi ty Ottawa, Ontario
Transcript

Synergistic Effecn of Macrophage Denved Cytokines: Consumption of a Highly Pdatable

Substance and Plasma Corticosterone Alterations

A thesis presented to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research. Carleton Cniversity. in

partial fulfillment of the requirements of the degreec of Master of Science in Psycholopy

Carleton Cniversi ty

Ottawa, Ontario

National Library I*I of Canada Bibliothèque nationale du Canada

Acquisitions and Acquisitions et Bibliographie Services services bibliographiques

395 Wellington Street 395. me Wellington Ottawa ON K f A O N 4 Ottawa ON K I A ON4 Canada Canada

The author has granted a non- exclusive licence allowing the National Library of Canada to reproduce, loan, distribute or sell copies of this thesis in microform, paper or electronic formats.

The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantial extracts fiom it may be printed or othewise reproduced without the author' s permission.

L'auteur a accordé une licence non exclusive permettant a la Bibliothèque nationale du Canada de reproduire, prêter, disûibuer ou vendre des copies de cette thèse sous la forme de microfiche/nlm, de reproduction sur papier ou sur format électronique.

L'auteur conserve la propriété du droit d'auteur qui protège cette thèse. Ni la thèse ni des extraits substantiels de celle-ci ne doivent être imprimés ou autrement reproduits sans son autorisation.

A bstract

It is becoming apparent that the proinflarnmatory cytokines. interleukin- 1 p (IL- 1 ).

interleukin-6 (IL-6) and tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TTL'F-a). mediate some of the

behavioral and hormonal responses to pathophysiological conditions. In addition to the

illness behavior associated with immune and cytokine challenge (e.g., reduced eating.

exploration, locomotion. the appearance of curled body posture. and soporific e ffects j.

such treamients rnay also provoke anhedonia. Moreover, there is reason to believe that

these treatments may act synergistically in promoting at least some of the actions associated

with endotoxin challenges. The present investigation assessed the potenrial synergistic

effects of macrophage-derive. cytokines on feeding and on plasma conicosterone

concena-arions. Male CD- I mice were rrained to drink chocolate milk. and dose-dependent

dtentions in consumption were measured following the wpmte administration of

inaapentoneal < i.p.) doses of lipopolys3ccharide t LPS). IL- 1 . IL-6 and TSF-a.

Corresponding altentions in plasma comcosterone lsvels were established in naive CD- 1

mice. It wris obsenled that LPS. IL- 1 and TKF-a each reduced chocolate milk

consumption and increrised corticosterone levsls. Based on preliminq findings. sub-

optimal doses of the cytokines were chosen in ordsr to asses' rhs synsrgistic actions of

combinations of IL-1 + IL-6, IL-6 + TSF-a. and IL4 + TXF-a on consurnpticrn and

homone alterations. Cytokine combinarions were adrninistercd in two successive i.p.

injections and consumption of chocolate milk was measured one hour later. Plasma

conicostsronc levels were again measured in a naive goup of mice. The combination of IL-

1 and TSF-cx synergistically reduced consumption of chocolritc milk and increassd

comcostcrone levels. In contrasr, neither the combinations of IL- i + L-6 nor IL-6 + T G -

a induced synergisric effects on behaviour or endocrine funciioning. The data m

discussed in terms of la) the anorexic vs the anhedonic actions of cytokine treatrnent. and

(b) the occurrence and function of synergistic effects af various cytokines.

Acknowledgements

1 would like to express my sincere thanks to Dr. Robert Zacharko and Dr. H p i e

Anisman, without whorn none of this would have k e n possible. I am also exmmely

gateful to Dr. Jerzey Kulczycki who spent so much tirnr analyzing my blood sarnples.

To everyone in the lab. I will always be grateful for your support. your good

humour and your neverending encouragement in rimes of Suess. In paflcular I am indsbted

to Yael Sela who was alwriys there to offer her opinion and her invaluable assistance when

i t was most needed.

To my famil y and friends. 1 appreciate your patience. your understanding and Jour

encouriigement (not to mention your cornputsr! i .

Thia rssearch m.35 supponed by a bIRC p n t .

Table of Contents

Title .................................... .,.. .................................................... L

Recommendation ............................................................................. ii

Abstract ......................................................................................... i i i

Acknowledgements ......................................................................... iv

Table of Contents ........................................................................... v

List of Figures ................................................................................ vi

1 . Introduction .......................... .... ..................................... 1

I I . hfultisystem Interactions .......................................................... 8

III . Cytokine Involvement in Endocnne/CSS Transmitter Arerations ............... 1 1

IV . Purpose ...................... .. ........................................................ Z I

.............................................................................. Experiments 1 - 1 21

Experiments 5-7 .............................................................................. 36

Gsneral Discussion .......................................................................... 45

Refersnces ............................. ,,., .................................................. 60

..................................................................................... Appendiu A 73

Appsndis B ......................... .. ....................................................... 80

..................................................................................... Appendis C X I

...................................................................................... Appendix D 8 2

..................................................................................... Appendix E 83

Appendix F .............................. .. ................................................. 81

Appendix G ..................................................................................... 85

Appendix H ..................................................................................... 86

..................................................................................... Appendis 1 87

List of Figures

Figure 1. Mean ( ~ S . E . M . ) consumption (mis) of a highly pslatable food arnong rnice

that received peripheral lipolysaccharide (LPS) or saline. Various doses of LPS (0.5 pg.

0.75 pg or 1 pg) were adrninistered intraperi toneally to male CD- 1 mice and consurnption

of chocolate milk was measured over a 1 hr. period commencing 90 minutes following

injection. Baseline values were calculated by averaging consumption over the three da?

period irnmediately pnor to testing. Recovery data were collected 24 hr. following testing.

Figure 2. Mean ( ~ s . E . M . ) consumption (mls) of a highly palatable food among mice

receiving peripheral interleukin-lp I L - ) . tumor necrosis factor-alphri (TSF-a) .

interlsukin-6 ( IL-6) or saline. Various doses of IL- 1 (0.025 pz. 0.05 pg. 0.1 pg. 0.2 pa.

or 0.8 ug). T S ï - a i 1 pz. 2 pg. or 4 pz) C or IL-6 (0.1 p g. 0.21 C g. 0.8 pg or 1.6 pg uere

administered inrrapsritonsally to male CD4 mice and cnnsuniption of chocolate milk was

mcasurrd ovcr (i 1 hr. period commencing cither 13 minutes (for IL-] and IL-6i or ?O

minutes (for TSF-a) following injection. Basdine values usrs clilculatrd bu averaging

consurnption o w ~ the threr day p e n d immediately prior to testing. Recovcv data w r e

cnllected 2 1 hr. follou.ing testing.

Figure 3 lrlsan t CS .E.M.) plasma conicostcronc concentrations i pgdl i among micc

that rcceived psripheral lipopolysaccharide (LPS) or sali ne. Various doses of LPS (0.5 pg

or 1 pg) were administered intraperitoneally to male CD-] mice and corticosrcrons

concentrations were measured 2 hr later.

Figure 1. Mem ( ~ s . E . M . ) plasma conicosterone concentr~tions (pg/dll in mice

receiving pcnpherril inisrleukin-l 1 1 ) tumor necrosis factor-alpha t TSF-u i .

interleukin-6 (IL-6). or saline. Various doses of IL- 1 (0.025 pg, 0.05 pg or O. 1 pg). IL-6

(0.1 p g or 0.8 pg), or Th7-a ( 1 pg, 2 pg or 4 pg)were adminisrered inmpentoneally to

male CD- 1 mice and plasma conicosterone concenaations were measured 1 hr later.

Figure 5. Effect of inmperitoneai adminisaation of 0.03 pg IL-1 + 0.1 pg IL-6

(upper panel), 0.1 mg IL-6 + 1 pg TNF-a (middle panel) or 0.025 pg IL- l + 1 pg TW-

a (lower panel) on mean (?s.E.M.) consumption (rnls) of a highly palatable food. Mais

CD-1 mice were injected with the cytokine combinations or 0.025 pg IL-l + Veh. 0- 1 pg

IL-6 + Veh. or 1 pg TSF-a + Veh and consumption of chocolarr milk was rneasured over

a 1 hr. pcriod cornrnencing 1 hr following injection. Baselinc values were calculated b',

avenging consumption over the three day p e n d immediatsly prior ro testing. Recoven-

data were cdlrcted 24 hr. following testing.

Figure 6. Effect of intraperitonenl administration of 0 . 0 3 pg IL- 1 + 0.1 pg IL-6. 0.1

pg IL-6 + 1 pg TXF-a or 0.025 pg IL-1 + 1 pg TSF-" on man ('S.E.M. 1 plasma

comcosieronc concentrations (pgdl). Male CD-I niics wcrc injcctcd wirh the c!.tohne

corntrinations or 0.025 pg IL-1 + Veh. 0.1 pg IL-h - k h . or 1 pg TSF-a înd plasma

corricostcrone levcls H erc measured 1 hr following injection.

Figure 7 . Mean (?s.E.>I.) consumption (in mlsi of (i highly plilarable food h>-

capsaicin- and vehicle-treated rnice following intrapsritoneal 3dministranon of

lipopolysaccharide ( LPS ) or saline. Seo-ncttai CD- I micc uwe uemd uit h subcutaneous

capsaicin or vshicle at Z days of agc. At 60 days of agc hoth groups of micc ucre injscrsd

with 0.5 pg LPS or saline. and consumption of chocol3te milk was rneasured over a I hr.

period cornmenci ng 90 minutes following injection. Baseline values were calculated hy

avenging consumption over the three day p e n d imrnediately pnor to testing. Recovery

data w r e collscted 24 hr. foIIowing testing.

Figure 8. Mean ( ~ s . E . M . ) plasma corcicosterone concenuations (pg/dl) in capsaicin-

or vehicle-aeated mice receiving in~aperitoneal lipopolysaccharide (LPS) or saline. Neo-

n a d male CD- I mice were treated with subcutaneous capsaicin or vehicle at 2 days of age.

At 60 days of age both groups of mice were injected with 0.5 pg LPS or saline and plasma

corticosterone concentrations were measured 2 hr later.

v iii

Introduction

It has become clear that the immune and cenaal nervous systems do not act

independently of one another. Cenaal nervous system (CNS) alterations come to affect

immune functioning, and conversely immune activation rnay promote increased endocrine

and tend neurotransmitter activity (Besedovsky, del Ray, Sorkin, Da Prada, Bum &

Honegger, 1983). Indeed, the view has been offered that the immune system stimulates

CXS acavity. and like environmental stimuli, may be associated with psychopathology. In

pmicular, it is thought diat immune activaaon acts like a saessor to provoke activation of

the hypothalmic-piniitq-adrenal (HPA) endocrine system, and may also elicit variations

of cenaal monoamines (Zalcman, Shanks & Anisman, 1993). As a result, behavioral

alterations may occur ahich are rerniniscent of those elicited by saessors (Dantzer &

Relley, 1989).

The mechanisms by which the immune system influences CNS processes remain to

be deduced. However, several candidates have k e n proposed in this respect. Whde these

systems are not mutually esclusive, it has been suggested that activation of the vatal

system. for example, may play a significant role in CNS-immune communication (Watkins

et al., 1991: Laye et al., 1993). In panicular, it is thought that immune activation by way of

vagal innervation may ulrimately come to excite hypothalamic factors (Gaykema, Dijkstra &

Tiiders, 1995: Fleshner, Goehler, Hermann, Relton, Maier 8: Watkins, 1995). Otha

investigators. however. have emphasized the role of cytokines released from activated

immune cells, panicularly macrophages (Besedovsky, del Rey, Sorhn & Dinarello, 1986).

In particular, antigen-stimulated macrophages release proinfiammatory cytokines

(interleukin- 1. interleukin-6 and nimor necrosis factor-a) which are instrumental in

signding other imrnunological cells, including T helper cells. In addirion, however. these

cytokines rnay gain access to the CNS, albeit in Iimiteù quantities, and among other things.

promote the release of corticotropin releasing hormone (CRH) from the paraventricular

nucleus (Pm-) of the hypothalamus, and either directly or indirectly elicit increased activity

of forebnin monoamines (Anisman, Zalcman & Zacharko, 1993; Lavicky & Dum. 1995).

There have been several reports consistent with the suggestion that cytokines play a

role in affecting CNS processes ( s e Schobitz, De Kloet & Holsboer, 1994; Watkins,

SIairr & Goehler, 1995a for review). Curiously, however, scant attention has k e n devoted

to the behavionl effects of cytokines, other than "sickness behavior" which often

accompanies irnrnunological challenge. In addition. although macrophage activation

promo tes concurrent release of interleukin- 1 (IL- 1 ), in terleukin-6 (IL-6) and tumor necrosis

factm-alpha (TNF-a), most studies that have considered the hormonal, neurotransrnitter

or behaviorai effects of these c ~ o k i n e s have assessed such effects individually. Yet, the

feu. studies thar evaluated the conjoint action of these cytokines have indicated that IL-1 and

IL-6 may act syergistically with respect to HPA functioning (Matta, Weatherbee & Sharp.

1997: Klir. Roth, Szelenyi, McClellan & Kluger, 1993; Perlstein. Whimdl, Abrams.

SIougey 8: Seta. 1993; Zhou. Shanks, Riechman, Liang, Kusnecov & Rabin, 1996). The

purpose of the present investigation is ro detail some of the synergistic effects of these

cytokines. i n particular, administntion of a bacterial endotoxin, such as lipopol ysaccharide

t LPS\. which promotes IL- 1 secretion (Rivier, Chizzonite, & Vale, 1989; Mefford.

.\lasrers. Heyes, & Eskay, 1991; Zanetti et al., 1992), has k e n s h o w to elicit sickness

bchwior. of which anorexia and anhedonia are pnmary symptoms (Hart, 1988: Danner g:

Kellcy. 1989: Plata-Salarnm 8r Borkoski, 1993: Exton, Bull & King, 1995; Yimiiya.

1996: Anisman. Kolckinaides & Merali, 1996). The present investigation xas conducted in

ordsr to determine whether IL-1, IL-6 and TNF-a influence anorexia/anhedonia. and

whether such behavioral effects are subject to the synergistic activity of these cytokines.

Additionally. a prelirninary study was conducted to ascenain whether destruction of vagal

affersnt fibers artmuates the illness behavior provoked by the bacterial endotoxin LPS.

kn0u.n tn mi\-are IL-1. IL-6 and TNF-a (Rivier et al., 1989; Zanetti et al., 1992).

Cvtokines and Vaoal Nerve Influence Immune-Bmin Communication

There is sufficient evidence to suggest that communication between the CNS,

pituitq-adrenal endocrine systern. and the immune systern is multidirectional. Activation

of immune ceiis resulting from pathophysiologicaI stimuli produces a CNS response

which. in tum, modulates the immune system. It is thought that this communication occurs

via alterations in neurotransmitter and hormonal activity (Blalock, 1981; Blalock & Smith,

1985; Berkenbosch, Wolven 8: Denjk, 1991; Dunn, 1990). The immune system,

recognizing that injury or infection has occurred, initiates rneraboiic processes aimed at

eliminating antigenic or pathogenic factors, while the neuroendocrine system ensures that

the menbolic demands of the host are met (Watkins et al., 1995; Blalock, 1991). Typically.

endogenous or exogenous threats to homeostatic mechanisms, which include injury,

bacterial infection or inflammation, give rise to a cascade of irnmunological alterations. In

this context. the host organism's pnmary attempt to restore homeostasis is charactenzed by

rnetsbolic and neuroendocrine changes collectively referred to as the acute phase response

(see Kent, Bluthe. Kelley b. Dantzer, 1992b for review). This response may be mediated

by, arnong orher things, activity of the HPA axis. In fact. it appears that IL-1 is exquisitely

effective in promoting secretion of CRH from the PVN, and is fundamental in the releâse

of pituitary adrenocorticotropin releasing hormone (ACTH) and adrenal corticosterone

(Sapolosby, Rivier, Yamamoto, Plotsky 8r Vale, 1987; Berkenbosch. Van Oers. Dsl Rey.

Tilders &: Besedovsky. 1987; Tumbull & Rivier, 1995). It was proposed, as well, that IL-

1 (and possibly other cytokines) may influence hypothalamic norepinephrine (NE), which

in rum may come to affect CRH release (Dunn. 1988; Dunn. 1990; Lavichy & Dunn.

1995). Additiondly, IL4 ma? also influence mesolimbic SE and dopamine (DA) and

might thus influence affect (Anisman et al., 1993).

In assessing the involvement of immune activation on endocrine and

neurotransmitter processes, numerous investigators have opted to evaluate the effects of the

endotoxin. LPS. which is a subcellular component derived from the outer membrane of

gram-negative bactena Endotoxins, normally present in a healthy host are typically not

found in circulation, but under pathological conditions rnay gain access to the bloodstrem.

As a result, LPS challenge may cause neuroendocrine and behavioral changes similar to

those seen during the acute phase response to infection (Dantzer & Kelley, 1989; Kent et

a1.,1992b).

It is generally believed that LPS does not cross the bld-brain banier, but rather

induces its effects through locaüy mediated signals which are relayed to neuronal targets

(Tilders, DeRij k, Van Dam, Vincent, Schotanus & Persoons. 1993). Peripheral

administration of LPS releases CRH from the hypothalamus, and hence, increases plasma

ACTH and conicosterone Ievels (Kakucska, Qi, Clark, & Lechan, 1993). Inasmuch as the

effects of LPS are attenuated or blocked by an IL- 1 receptor an tagonist (IL- Ira). it is Uely

thsr the effects of LPS are subserved by L-1 (Schotanus, Tilden & Berkenbosch, 1993:

Ebisui et al.. 1991). Moreover. there is reason to suspect that IL-1 may be acting conjointly

w i t h IL-6 and TSF-a to promote the endocrine effects engendered by LPS (Long.

Ortemess. Runkel, Vander & Kluger, 1990; Dunn. 19973; Zanem et al., 1997; Perlstein

er al.. 1993). Such an outcome is, of course, not surprising, given diat LPS fmt promotes

the appearance of TNF-a in the circulation, followed by IL4 and IL6 (Zanem et al..

1992). It remains unclear. however, whether CRH release is a d k c t result of the action of

the cytokines on the brain or is seconda* to endotoxin-induced cytokine-release in the

periphsry.

Recenrly. investigators have reponed that subdiaphngmatic vagotomy cm suppress

CRH and ACTH secrerion ordinarily resulting from LPS-induced HPA acavation

(Gaykema et al.. 1995). These data suggest a putative vagal conmbution to the immune

response follouing endoroxin administration (Laye et al., 1995). The sectioning of vagal

afferenis has also k e n shown to block IL4 induced ACTH secretion (but not

conicosterone secretion) (Kapcala, He, Gao, Peiper lk DeTolla, 1996). providing

additional suppon for the view that cytokines may be influencing the brain via pnmary

sensory afferents. Gaykema et al. (1995) studied c-fos expression in CRH neurons in the

PVN following endotoxin administration. Complete subdiaphragmatic vagotomy blocked

the ACTH response to low doses, and attenuated the response to high doses of LPS in the

rat, while leaving the corticosterone response intact. In addition, Fos expression in CRH

neurons in the PVN was suppressed following subdiaphragmatic vagal uansection. Taken

togeerheer, these studies indicate that immune-brain communication may be mediated by

vagai afferents, possibly through cytokine activation of peripheral sites.

nlness Behaviour: Si ~ n s and Svmptoms

Foilowing immune challenge, animals often exhibit non-specific symptoms of

illness, including fever, anorexia, decreasd water intake, hyperalgesia, increased

somnolence, piloerection, lethargy and reduced social exploration (see Hart, 1988 for

review). While such behaviors were once considered a secondary response to illness, they

are now regarded as an active pan of the host organism's anempt to restore homeostasis

(Dantzer 6r Kelley, 1989). One component of the febrile response includes increased body

remperature owing to s ndogenous pyrogen release. uhich stimulates the immune

response. Other sickness behavion that elevate body tempenture (eg. shivering and

piloerection among others), and reduce the incidence of metabolicdly expensive activities

(eg. somnolence, reduced libido. reduced social exploration), rnay well maximize the

organism's ability to contend with environmental challenges (Kent et al., 1992b). It has

k e n demonsmted rhat beha~iors associated with illness are not secondary to fever. as the

behavioral effects of inflarnrnatov stimuli can be dissociated from the pyrogenic effects of

the treatments. (O'Reilly, Vander & Kluger, 1988; Johnson, Propes & Shavit. 1996)

Evidence that pro-inflanmatory cytokines, such as IL-1, IL-6 and TSF-a, are

involved in the mediation of endotoxin-induced illness has k e n derived from investigations

which demonstrated that systemic administration of IL4 or TNT-a prior to LPS challenge

mitigates the p o p n i c effects of the endotoxin (Long, Monmoto. Nakamon Ji Murakami.

1992; Klir, McClellan, Kozak, Szelenyi, Wong & Kluger, 1995). In addition. the

peripheral administration of IL-1, IL-6 and TM-a results in behaviors reminiscent of

those seen in response to LPS challenge (Shirnomrnura et al., 1990; McLaughlin, Rogan,

Tou, Baile & Joy, 1992; Chance & Fischer, 1992; Goujon, Parnet, Aubert, G d a l i &

Dantzer, 1995). Predictabl y. cytokine receptor antagonists, as weil as antibodies directed

against the cytokines, prevent or limit the effects of LPS on these behaviors (Bluthe,

Danner & Keiley 1992; Plata-Salaman & Ffiench-Mullen, 1992). Taken together, these

studies support the notion that the behavioral components of the immune response to

endotoxin challenge may be mediated by some of the cytokines.

It would be premature to conclude that all sickness behaviors are mediated by an

identical mechanism, or conversely. that only one mechanism can mediate individual

behavinral components of the illness response. Langhans, Savoldelli and Weinganen

(1993). for exarnple, demonsuated that in the rat, peripheral administration of eirher IL-1

or LPS provoked anorexia. While the anorexia associated with LPS resulted from a

reduction in meai hequency, the IL-1 associated appetite suppression was expressed as a

reduction in both meal frequency and meal size. Paracetamol, which inhibits brain

cyclooxygenase, anenuated the IL- 1 -, but not the LPS-induced anorexia, suggesting rtiat

iL-1 affects fieding through a direct acnon on the brain (likely involving the pyrogenic

effects of IL-1). whereas other mechanisms may mediate the effects of LPS on feeding

behavior.

It should be emphasized that the pyrogenic and behavioral effects of the cytokines

do not appear to be mediated exclusively through tend mechanisms. DeRijk, Van

Rooijen, Tilders, Besedovsky, Del Rey and Berkenbosch (1991) reponed that in rats, the

elimination of penpheral macrophages (which secrete IL-1). attenuated the pyrogenic

response to LPS. Klir et al. (1995) found that systemic administration of TNF-a

attenuated LPS-induced fever in rats, whereas central administration had no such effect.

Bluthe et al.. (1992) demonsnated that, in rats, the decreased social exploration and

reduced body weight associated with peripheral LPS injection was attenuated by peripheral

IL- 1 ra, but not by intraventricular IL- 1 ra adminisuation. Taken together, these studies

indicate that peripheral cytokines are intimately involved in the sickness response associated

with immune acavation, but these data do not preclude the involvement of centxal

mechanisms in this respect.

There are several sources of evidence suggesting that ACTH and/or comlcosrerone

may be integrai ro some of the behaviorai/iiIness effects associated with immune activation

(Goujon et al., 1995; Johnson et aL.1996). For instance, while the febrile response

aswciated with endotoxin challenge is likely subserved, in part, by activation of

prostaglandin E (PGE), it appears that endogenous glucoconicoids may also play a role in

this respect (Morrow, McClellan, Conn & Kluger, 1993). It was reponed that pretreatment

of rats with dexamethasone abolished the fever produced by an acute systemic injection of

LPS. Conversel y. adrenalectornized rats showed an increased febrile-response to LPS

administration, which could be inhibited by acute or chronic treatment with dexamethasone

(Coelho. Souza 8: Pela, 1991). Funher, pretreatment with the glucocorticoid antagonist,

RU-38486 (following oral, or innacerebrokrenaicular administration), resulted in elevated

fevers relarive to those in evidence arnong vehicle-treated rats following acute LPS

administration (Morrow et al.. 1993: McClellan, Klir, Morrow 8: Kluger, 1991).

In addition to the potential effects on fever, there is evidence suggesting that

olucocomcoids may be involved in other symptoms of illness which are associated wirh C

LPS treament. Specifically, it was demonstrated that administration of LPS to

adrenalectornized rats provoked sickness behavior (eg. increased body temperature.

reduced social exploration and reduced body weighr) and that such an effect could be

an tagonized by comcosterone implants (Johnson et al., 1996). Moreover. Goujon et al.

(1995) found that in mice, subcutaneous IL- 1 or LPS administration reduced social

exploration. an effect which could be enhanced by adrenalectomy. The sarne effect was

evident among intact animals given subcutaneous injections of RU-38486, indicating that a

reduction in glucocomcoid concenmtion was likely related io the enhanced susceptibili~ to

the behavioral effects of LPS. Indeed, corticosterone implants had a protective effect on the

adrenaiectomized rats injected with IL-1, and partiaily protected mice injected with LPS. In

a similar fashion, Pezeshki, Pohl and Schobitz (1996) demonstrated that LPS

adminisrration in adrenalectomized rats, in doses which ordinarily induce fever and

anorexia, promoted a more profond reduction of locomotor activity relative to intact

endoxtoxin-treated animals. In effect, it seemed that the absence of corticosterone

peminzd greater development of illness behavior. As rnight be expected, the behavioral

effects (with the exception of the reduction in locomotor activity) could be attenuated by

prmeament with corticosterone prior to LPS administration.

An alternative accounting for some of the behavioral effects of immune activation,

including sickness behavior, is derived from the work of Blalock and colleagues (Smith.

Meyer & Blalock, 1982; Blalock, 1984; Blalock &: Smith, 1985). It was proposed that

acrivated lymphocytes secrete an ACT'I-I-like substance which may directly or indirectly

stimulate CNS processes. In fact, it was shown that following hypophysectomy, immune

challenge still increclsed plasma A C ï H levels, possibly reflecting the release of this

hormone h m activated Iymphocytes (Blalock, 1984). Subsequent studies conducted by

Dunn. Powell and Gnskin (1987) suggested that some of the observations derived by

Blalock moy have k e n due to incomplete pituitary ablation, which afforded an ACT'H

source distinct from 1 ymphocyte-ACïH secretion. While incomplete hypoph ysectomy ma'

have k e n a factor in the detection of ACTH release, it is likely that pimit. endocrines

released during immune activation promote CNS effects.

MCLTISYSTEM INTERACTIONS

Svmnnthenc Nsrvous System and the HPA Axis

As indicated earlier, the similarities between the effects of stressors and immune

challenge on endocrine and CNS processes gave nse to the proposition that immune

challenge may be interpreted by the brain as if it were a srressor (Dunn, 1990). C'pon

dctection of a stressor, the sympathetic nervous system is activated, causing rapid

alterations in catecholamine levels in the blood. It has been suggested that the initial

stressor-provoked penpheral NE release may be essential for activating immune processes

(Felten & Felten, 1991). Indeed, it appears that the sympathetic nervous system innervates

a wide variety of organs and tissues, including the primary and secondary lymphoid

organs, as weii as the spleen. Moreover, NE receptors are present on lymphocytes, and

these cells may be activated by factors that provoke sympathetic NE release (e.g, stressors)

(Madden & Livnat, 199 1). Like NE, it has k e n demonsmted that glucocorticoid receptors

are present in the spleen, thymus and lymph nodes, as weli as on the cell surface of

lymphocytes. Activation of these glucocomcoid receptors have the effect of reducing

immune activity (Keller, Schleifer & Deme~opou los , 1991). It has k e n sugpsted that

one of the functions of circulating conicosterone is that of Iimiring the extent to which the

immune system is activated by stressors, hence reducing the potenrial autoimmune effects

thar might otherwise occur (Munck. Guyre. & Holbrook, 1983). In effect. it seems that the

immune systern is inmnsically linked to borh HPA functioning and sympathetic nervous

system acti\+y.

Immune-CSS Communication

Consistent with the notion that immune activation has CNS effects. it has been

s h o w that antigenic challenge promotes catecholamine turnover in the brain and the

periphery. in particular, in rats and rnice, administration of sheep red blood celis (SRBC)

results in changes of NE tumover in the locus coeruleus and hypothalamus (Zalcrnan et al..

199 1). and DA tumover in mesocortical sites (Lacosta, Merali & Anisman. 1995). In

addition, S RBC adminisuarion alters serotonin (5HT) Ievels in the paravennicular and

supraoptic nuclei (Carlson, Felten, Livnat & Felten, 1987). These changes, dong with

increased electrophysiologic activiry in the PVN and in the locus coeruleus (Besedovsky,

Sorkin, Felix & Haas, 1977), corresponded with die immune response being mounted

(Carlson et al., 1987: Zalcman et al., 1991; Lacosta et al., 1995).

Like the effects of SRBC, it was recently demonsaated that neuroendocxine

activation rnay occur as a result of virai challenge or infectious disease. In panicular,

pathogens such as Newcastle disease vims N V ) and influenza vins are associated with

HPA activation (Dunn, Powell, M e i ~ & Srnall, 1989), and increased hypothalamic hiE

release (Dum & Vickers, 1994). Intraperitoneal injection of NDV also increased

hypothalamic concentraaons of tryptophan, the hZ carabolite 3-methoxy 4-

hydroxyphenylethylene_Elycol (MHPG), and the 5-hydrox~yptamine (5-HT) carabolite 5-

hydrox yindoieacetic acid (5-HIAA). In view of the observation that hypophysectomy

blocked elevarions in plasma comcosterone following hDV and LPS challenge, ir seems

that the corticosterone changes were probably not a result of viraI srimulation of the

adrenal. but rather involved HPA activation.

It will be recdled that there are sufficient data to suggest that IL-1 rnay funcnon as

an immunotransmirter to provoke endocrine secretion following immune challenge. Arnong

othcr things. IL-1 is a potent stimulator of CRH release, and hence A C T H and

conicosrerone secretion (Besedovsky et al.. 1986; Berkenbosch et al., 1987: Sapolsky et

al.. 1987: Wehrenberg. Saudan, Corder, Voltz, Giustina & Gaillard, 1994: Meiink

Parsadanirintz. Levin. Lenoir. Roberts & Kerdelhue, 1994). Moreover. central pretreament

with antngonists of IL-1 or CRH limited the A C T H and corticosterone variations induced

by eithsr IL- 1 or LPS trcatment (Dunn. 19922: Schotanus et al., 1993: Ebisui et al., 1994).

Tot unespectedly. the increased paraventricular CRH messenger RNA (mRKA) obsened

following intrapeitoned administration of LPS in rats. could also be abolished by

innûvenricular IL- h a pre-matment (Kakucska et al., 1993). In addition, dexamerhasone

was found to inhibit IL-ha mRNA levels in endotoxin-stimulateci human monocytes, and

inhibired the secretion of IL-lra. These effects could be reversed by the glucocomcoid

antctgonist RL' 38386 (Am et al., 1994).

In addition to these endocrine variations. there is reason to suppose that the effects

of inirnune challenge on exaahypothalarnic hT, DA. and 5-HT may involve cytokines. In

particular, it was dernonstrated that systernic adrninisnation of IL-I promotes severai

distinctive monoamine changes (eg. DA turnover in the prefrontal conex), which are

rerniniscent of the effects associated with suessors (Merali, Lacosta, & Anisrnan, 1997, in

press). Yet, IL-1 and SRBC have distinctly different effects with respect to some of the

centrai neurochemical changes obsenied. In par?icuIar. while SRBC provokes in vivo

accumbal DA release (Lacosta et al. 1995) IL- I does not produce such an outcorne, and IL-

6 reduces DA release (Merali et al., 1997 in press).

CYTOKINES INFLUENCE ENDOCRINE/CNS TRANSMITTERS

Immune activation involves the release of numerous cytokines, including IL- 1, IL-

3, IL-4, IL-5, IL-6, IL-10, IL-I), TNF-a, and interferon-y among othen. Most of these

cytokines have not k e n assessed extensively insofar as their endocrine and behaviorai

effects are concemed (see Berkenbosch, de Rijk, Del Rey 8. Besedovsky, 1990; Schobitz

et al., 1993; Turnbull & Rivier, 1995 for review). It is conceivable that in addition to the

pro-inflarnmatory cytokines, one or more of these other components may act to signal

endocnne/CKS processes. Nevenheless, the amilable data have suggested a prominenr role

for IL- 1. IL-6 and Th- in promoting an interface benveen endocrine and CSS activity.

Accordingly. the present review has k e n lirnited to these cytokines. In discerning potentiai

individual or synergistic effects of this subset of cytokines, one would be remiss if a more

detailed description of their actions were not described.

There are three forms of IL- 1, IL- 1 a, IL- 1P and IL-lra. Although IL- l(3 is the

major secreted fom, IL-la and IL-lp nevenheless appear to have similar effects on

numerous systems (Rothwell lk Luheshi, 1991). Studies in rats suggest, however, that IL-

1P has more prominent effects than IL- la with respect to HPA activation. For instance,

IL-1P is more effective than IL-la in promoting CRH release from the PVN and hence

ACTH release from the pituitary (Harbuz. Stephanou, Sarlis & Lighmian. 1992). In

addirion. the fever and therrnogenesis induced by inmcerebrovennicular (icv)

administration of IL- 1 P, but not IL- la, can be blocked with a centrai injection of a CRF

receptor antagonist (Long et al., 1990; Rothwell & Luheshi, 1994). Likewise, the receptor

antagonist, IL- Ira counteracts the HPA stimulating action of IL- 1 P (Dunn, 1995).

TNF exists in two foms, TNF-a md TNFP. which share approximately 50%

sequence homology. TNF-a (or c a c h e c ~ ) is secreted by a variety of immune cells,

whereas TM+ is exclusively secreted by activated lymphocytes. IL-6 is a B-cell growth

factor that (dong with IL-1) augments T-lymphocyte mitogenesis. Under homeostatic

conditions, the expression of IL-1, IL-6 and Tm-a is ordinarily low in most tissues,

peripheral and cenaal sites, while a marked increase in synthesis and secretion is detected

following inflammation or infection. Like IL-1, elevaaons of IL-6 and TNF-a not only

have paracrine and endocrine-like effects, but also autocrine effects which inhibit their oun

activity . hence lirni ting immune overshoots (Schobitz et al., 1993).

The mechanisrns by which the cytokines signal the brain following infection has yet

to be fully elucidated. Receptors for L- 1, L-6 and TNF-a have been localized in various

brain regions (including the hippocampus and hypothalamus), although it is unclear how

cytokines gain access to the brain. as they are large, lipophobic proteins thar are unlikely to

pass through the blood-brain barier (BBB). It appears. however, that the cytokines may

gain access to the brain via carrier-mediated transport. or possibly via regions lacking a

blood- brain b h e r (eg. the organum vasculosum of the lamina terminalis) (Watkins et al..

1995a). An alternative explmation for the influence of cytokines on behaviorai change is

derived largely fiom the observation rhat illness behviour c m be induced in the absence of

detectable levels of LPS or cytokines in the blood. In effect, cenaal activation may not be

dependent on blood-borne cytokines. Rather. it has k e n suggested that cytokines may

activate peripheral afferent nerves at or near the site of release. It will be recalled, for

exarnple that subdiaphragmatic vagotomy blocks many of the illness responses following

IL- 1, 'Ih'F-a and LPS, suggesting a system of immune-brain communication that is based

on humoral rather than blood-borne factors (Laye et al., 1995, Watkins et al., 1994; BIuthe

et al., 1991, Gaykema et al., 1995).

Alterations in IL-1 levels have been implicated in a number of physiological

changes resulting h m LPS administration. In order to assess the role of IL4 in LPS-

induced pyrogenesis,Denjk, Van Rooijen, Tilders, Besedovsky, Del Rey & Berkenbosch,

1991 and Marquette, Van Dam, Van Rooijen, Berkenbosch and Haour (1994) eliminared

peripheral macrophages through the administration of dichloromethylene diphosphonate

(CEMDP) liposomes. This treamient attenuated the nse in plasma IL-1 concenmtions in

response CO LPS and also abrogated the thexmogenic and fever response to LPS in rats.

These data suggest that peripherally produced IL-1 is involved in the pyrogenic effecrs of

LPS. However, penpheral injection of LPS has been shown to induce IL-l mRNA in rat

brain microglial cells (Buttini & Boddeke, 1995) and induce IL- I activation in ramified

microglia, as well as activated macrophages in the meninges and choroid plexus of the rat

brain (Van Dam. Brouns, Louisse & Berkenbosch. 1992). Thus, there appears to be a

central role of IL-1 in LPS-induced neural activation. Interestingly, depletion of

macrophages also prevented the plasma ACTH and corticosterone responses to su b-

pyrogenic doses of LPS. However, this effect on ACTH and corticosterone was not

evident in response to ppgen ic doses of LPS (Derijk et al., 1991). Evidently, the

pyrogenic acaons of IL-1 cm be disassociated from the ACTH variations following

endotortin treatment.

There are distinct variations in the kinetics of the various cytokines following LPS

administration (de Simoni, Dei Bo, De Luigi, Simard & Forloni. 1995). It has also bern

demonstrated that the pattern of rnRNA induction in rat brain following penpheral LPS

injection is different for each of the cytokines (Gam & Banfai. 1993). i n d i c a ~ g that they

may play temporally linked divergent roles following immune activation. For example,

nF-a Iwels peak approximately one hour following LPS administration. whereas. IL- 1

and IL-6 peak within 3-4 hours (Zanetti et al., 1992; Derijk et al., 1993: Schotanus et al..

1993; Fantuzzi, Di Santo, Sacco, Beniepi & Ghezzi,1995). In addition, de Simoni et al.

(1995) noted that the levels of TNF-a decline more rapidly than those of IL-1 and IL-6,

suggesting that TNF-a may initiate HPA activation following LPS administration, while

IL- 1 and IL-6 susrain the activation once it has k e n triggered

Evidence in favour of the conjoint action of IL- 1, IL-6 and TNF-a in mediating the

endocrine effects of LPS is derived from investigations evaluating the effect of cytokine

antibodies. For instance, the fact that sysvrnic administration of IL- 1 itself induces marked

HPA alterations suggests thar the role this cytokine is apparently more significant than

sustsining a previously excited HPA system. Of course, the possibility cannot be dismissed

that the relative conmbutions of the three pro-inflarnrnatory cytokines are dissimilar with

respect to inirïating vs. maintaining HPA activation. Moreover, recent investigations

involving the synergistic effects of IL-1, IL-6 and TNF-a revealed that these cytokines

mal- interact with respect to the activation of the HPA axis following LPS adminisuation

(Zhou et al.. 1996).

Pçrlstein et al. (1993) reponed that IL-6 antibody pre-treatment prevenred the LPS-

induced ACTH response ordinarily observed 7 and 1 hours after endotoxin adminisuarion.

Prsmatment with ei ther IL- 1 r antibody or TNTF-oL an tibody also blocked the 4 hour A m

response. but did not block the response observed 2 hours after endotoxin treatment. A

combinarion of IL-lr and TNF-a-antibody iirnited the 2 hour ACTH response. In

addition. it was obsen~ed that combined doses of Tb??-a and IL-6, as well as TSF-a and

IL-1. increased production of circulating ACTH to a greater extent than that produced by

these cytokines admnistered separately. Moreover, pretreament with an IL-6 antibody

complctely blocked the conjoint effecü of ThF-a and IL- 1 on the AC3T-I response. Since

TSF* and IL- 1 both stimulate IL-6 release, the data of this investigation suggest bat LPS

directly stimulates IL-6 release 2 hours afier adrninstration, but that TNF-a and IL-l

synergisricrtlly induce the variations of TL-6 observed at the 4 hour interval.

In conaast to the results reporteci by Perlstein et al. (1993), which emphasized the

contribution of IL-6 in mediatïng die endocrine variations associatecl with LPS aeannent,

Schotanus et al. (1993) suggested that this cytokine was not fundamental in the provocation

of the ACTH alterations. In particular, it was dernonstrated that peripheral IL-1

administration increased plasma ACTH, corticosterone and IL-6 concentrations. At low

doses, however, the ACTH and corcicosterone response to peripheral IL1 were not

accompanied by changes in plasma IL-6 concenmtions, indicating that the latter cytokine

was not involved in the ACTH response. In a subsequent snidy. dose-dependent elevations

of plasma ACTH, corticosterone and IL-6 concentrations were seen following peripheral

administration of LPS. The dose required to induce the median IL-6 response was 4 times

higher han the dose required to induce the A m and comcosterone elevations. These

results, dong with the demonstration that the ACTH. but not the IL-6, response to

endotoxin was blocked by peripherd IL-Ira, indicate that IL-6 did not play a prorninent

role in the initiation of the HPA response following endotoxin administration.

Additional evidence that IL-6 is not an essential mediator in the inflammatory

responsc to endotoxin is denved from investigations involving IL-6 deficient mice. Fattori

et al. (1991) found that these mice were able to generate an inflammatory response to LPS

administration. Corticosterone, as well as IL-1 levels in the IL-6 deficient mice were sirnilar

to the values detected in C O ~ D O ~ S following LPS administration. Similady, the reduction in

body weight and food intake following LPS adminstration was comparable in both groups

of animals. Unexpectedly, levels of TNF-a induced by LPS were three-fold higher in IL-6

deficient mice than in controls. which prompted the suggestion that the increased TNF-a

ma' compensate for the absence of IL-6. In effect, it appears that the corticosterone

response following LPS does not require IL-6, and IL-6 is not a necessary component of

the infiammatory response to endotoxin administration.

As indicated earlier, the mechanisms that mediate HPA activation following

pyrogenic doses of endotoxin may be distinct from those that mediate Iow doses of

endotoxin. Indeed, it has k e n demonstrated that LPS induces dose-dependent variations of

glucoconicoids, and that the relative conmibutions of various cytokines Vary as a function

of the LPS dose employed. Ebisui et al. (1994), for instance. found that when smaii

amounts of LPS (0.03 p a g ) were adrninistered intravenously to rats, the maximum

conicosterone response could be almost completely abolished by TNF*-antiserum, but

not by an an IL- 1 receptor antagonist. However. either TNF+-antisenim or IL-lra

anenuated the response when larger arnounts of LPS (10pg/kg) were administered. These

results suggest that, at low doses, ThF-a alone was responsible for mediahg the effects

of LPS on the HPA axis, while IL-I participated in stimulating ACTH secretion following

high doses. Moreover, following LPS administration, rmitment of IL-1 and TSF-a in

the provocation of ACTH release may occur at different times.

Anoirxia i s Infliienced hv Svner~st ic - Actions of C ~ ~ o h n e Treatment

Although it is clear that L I , IL-6 and TSF-a ail participate in the HPA response

to endotoxin. it is difficult to determine which cytokine initiates the HPA response. It will

be recalled that multiple behavioral and endocrine interactions exist between the cytokines.

Sot only do the cytokines act in synergistic ways, depending on the combination of

cytokines activated. but IL- 1, TL-6 and TW-a can al1 modulate the synthesis and secretion

of one anothsr. In view of the relative paucity of data conceming the interactive effects of

cytokines in modulating endocrine functioning. it is not surpnsing that still fewer studies

have foîussd on the behavioral synergisms that occur in response to cytokine challenge.

As indicatzd earlier, anorexia is a prominent behavioral response evident following

immune challenge. and has been assessed following adrninismtion of endotoxins. Dose-

dependent reductions in food intake and body weight have k e n demonstrated following

both peripheral (O'Reilly et al., 1987) and cenaal (hlcCanhy, Kluger, & Vander, 1986)

adminisûation of LPS. It has k e n suggested that the endotoxin-induced reduction in

feeding may be mediated. in pan, by the proinflammatory cytokines. Indeed, IL- 1. IL-6

and TSF-a. al1 of which are induced by LPS administration, suppress food intake (Plata-

Salaman, Oomura & Kai, 1988; Tracey et al., 1988; Socher, Freeman & Mariinez, 1988;

Michie, Sherman, Spriggs, Rounds, Christie & Wilmore, 1989; Fantino & Wieteska,

1993; Langhans et al., 1992; Fanon et al., 1993; Plata-Salaman, 1994). Inasmuch as the

anorexic effects can be induced by either peripheral or central administration of IL- 1 raises

the possibility that this effect is, at least in part, cenmiiy mediated (Shirnommura et al.,

1990, Chance & Fischer, 1991; McLaughlin et al., 1992). Cornmensurate with this view,

peripheral administration of IL- 1 antagonists blocked the anorexic effects of systernic K- 1

(McLaughlin et al., 1992). and centrai administration of IL- Ira attenuated the anorexia

induced by icv IL-1 (Plata Salamas & Ffrench-Mullen. 1992; Plata-Salaman & Borkoski.

1993). Kent. Bret-Dibat. Kelley & Danner (1996) demonstrated that central administration

of IL- 1 profoundly decreased operant responding for food reinforcment, and this effect

could be prevented by central IL- lra preueatment. Interestingl y, however, centrai IL- 1 ra

did not completely block the effect of peripheral IL-1. nor did peripheral IL-Ira block the

effects of centrai IL-1. These results suggest that IL-1 influences both peripheral and

cemal mechanisms to reduce food-motivated behavior.

TW-a and IL-6, it will be recalled, have also k e n s h o w to have a direct action

in die CNS, and it has been suggested diat TNF-a exens its anorecac effects through

centnll y mediated mechanisms. Rogers, Mctigue and Hermann (1 996) demonstrated that

the nauseus influence of gasaic relaxation, cnuld be evoked through unilateral TXF-a

injections into the dorsal vagal complex. In addition, Fantino & Wieteska (1991)

demonstrated that, in rats, central administration of TXF-or resulted in a decrease in food

and water intake, whereas peripheral administration had no effect. Moreover.

elec~ophoreticall y applied TNF-a suppressed the acovity of glucose-sensitive ne wons in

the lateral hypothalamus. (Plata-Slarnan et al,. 1988). Since IL-1 was found to produce

sirnilar effects, it was posited that some of the effects of T ' h W may have ken seconda7

to the induction of IL- 1 .

It has been suggested that IL-6 rnay play a role in the attenuation of anorexia

following ïL- 1 receptor blockade. Oldenburg et al. (1993) investigated the effects of IL-6

and IL-lr antibodies on anorexia in the mouse following sterile rurpentine abcess. The

reduction in food intake and resulting weight loss were attenuated by both L - l r and IL-6

antibody administration. The co-administration of IL-6 and IL-lr antibodies did not,

however, result in a sig~~ificantly greater reduction in the anorexic effects than those in

evidence following the administration of the each separate antibody. Based on the

additional observation that IL- l r blockade anenuated production of IL-6, the investigators

concluded that IL-6 may be the mare direct rnediator of anorexia and weight loss in this

model.

Although there is evidence that the anorexic effects of LPS and IL- 1 are similar to

one another, there are conflicting repons regarding the effects of endotoxin on meal

patterns. For example, O'Reilly et al. (1988) reponed that prolonged exposure to LPS did

not result in tolerance to the anorexic effects, whereas Langhans et al. (1993) demonsmted

that tolerance did deveiop following prolonged LPS exposure. Interestingly, Langhans et

al. also reponed that inaaperitoneal LPS reduced food intake by decreasing rneal

frequency. uirhout affecting meal size. In contrast to this finding, however. Plata-Salaman

(1991) found bat LPS signficantly reduced med size. It is important to note that while

Plata-Salaman (1991) administered LPS centrdly, Langhans et al. (1993) used peripheral

injections to induce anorexia. Thus, it is conceivable that different mechanisms subsenle

the anorexia, with only the central processes affecting meal size.

Although numerous effects of LPS, as already described. have been amibuted to

activation of IL-1, there is evidence that LPS and IL-1 induced anorexia are mediated

through different mechanisms. Mrhereas repeated injections of IL- 1 lead to sensitization of

the anorexic effects, prolonged exposure to LPS lead to tolerance, with no evidence of

cross-tolerance or cross-sensitization between the compounds (Langhans et al., 1993).

Moreover. the cyclooxygenase inhibiror, paracetamol, sipificantly artenuated the IL-1-.

but not the LPS-induced anorexia, suggesting a role for prostaglandins in the IL- 1 mediated

effects.

Vagotomy and Feeding

It will be recalled that several investigaton argued that communication between the

immune system and die CNS rnay occur through the vagal nerve. This proposition was

based on. arnong other things, the finding that subdiaphragmatic vagotomy attenuated the

hormone variations otherwise provoked by immune challenge or bacterial endotoxins

(Gay kema et al., 1995). Severing the vagus nerve eliminates sensory afferent fibres which

c q information conceming the gastrointes~d tract to the centrai nervous system. In

addition, efferent fibres that are involved in motor acrivities of the intestine and stomach are

also severed, which eliminates integrai sources of information conceming intestinal

content, as well as CNS control over the secretory and mocilic functions of the stomach

(Shapiro & Miselis, 1985). Given the important contribution of the vagus to intestinal

processes, it should not be surprising that subdiaphra,patic vagotomy, which involves

severing of the vagal-afferent and efferent innervation, influences feeding in humans and

in infrahuman subjects (Yang, Ratto, Gleason, Bellantone, Crucini 8: Meguid. 1993j.

S uch effects are not dietary dependent, and are readily apparent when dry food or more

palatable liquid diets are provided (Davis, Smith d Kung, 1994; Bernstein. 1995).

Davis et al. (1994) demonsmted that, in rats, the reduced consumption of a liquid

diet following vagotorny could be amibuted to a decline in the rate of licking during the

meal. which was not related to motor dysfunction. Analysis of the microstructure of the

licking response revealed that vagotomized rats had shoner Iicking bouts at the beginning

of the test, and a characteristically defined rapid decline in the licking rate during the test.

This behavioral profile was independent of the type of diet provided (rnilk, or one of 3

concenmtions of sucrose solution) suggesting that neither motivational factors, nor

nutritional content, were particularly relevant in inducing the pattern of behavioral change

in evidence. It was suggested that the decline in the rate of Lickng during the meai was

attributable to an accelerated clearance of fluid from the stomach, which enhanced negative

feedback s ipa l s from the gasmin te s~a l tract ordinarily conveyed to the CNS through

exaavagal pathways.

Recently, it has k e n suggested that the reduction in food uitake following

vagotomy c m be amibuted, at least in part, to peripheral endocrine factors (Rogers et al.,

1996). The dorsal vagal complex @VC) lacks a blood brain bamier, which rendes this site

susceptible to humoral signals such as cytokines and hormones. Since the DVC influences

parasympathetic activity associated with digestive processes, cytokines and ensuing

hormonal changes rnay exen effects on feeding through indirect signals acting on

parasympathetic functioning.

Anorexia and Anhedonia

Regardless of whether cytokines, vagal innervations, or a combination of the two

mediate the behaviod responses to endotoxin challenge, a note of caution must k

inte jested conceming the processes al iged with the anorexic symptoms. When describing

the "sickness behavior" aswiared with immune activation, most investigators have

concluded that compounds such as LPS induce a reduction in feeding and also decrease

rnorivation to consume food. On the one hand. the reduced motivation may reflect

diminished appe tire second. to illness, but on the other hand it may reflecr a diminution in

the rewarding attibutes ordinarily ascxibed to feeding.

It has recently been suggested that activation of the immune system results in

physiological and behaviord changes that resemble the characteristics of depression.

Indeed. i t has been demonstrated that in some cases, depression is associated with immune

activation, characrerized by an increase in plasma levels of cytokines such as IL- 1 and L-6.

and dysreplation of the HPA axis. The loss of appetire, reduced body weight, increased

somnolence and anorexia which follow cytokine or endotoxin adrninistranon in animals are

ail symptoms of depression. In addition, animal models of depression include the

attenuation of sucrose consumption and place-preference paradigms which are used to

study the rewarding properties of food and dnigs (Willner, Towell, Sampson.

Sophokleous & Muscat 1987; Papp, Wilner & Muscat, 1991; Suzuki et al., 1994). In

effect, the view can be taken that the reduced food consumption associated with endotoxin

administration may be a reflection of the potential anhedonic and depressionogenic effects

of this matment Indeed. it was demonsuated that penpheral pretreatrnent with LPS

abolished the reinforcing effects of cocaine in the place-preference paradigm (Suniki et al.,

1994).

Consistent with the view that LPS induces depressive-like effects. Yirmiya (1996)

demonsaated that a single dose of LPS suppressed sexuai behavior in male rats. and

reduced their preference for, and consumption of. a saccharin solution. Furthemore.

repeated treamen t wi t h clinicall y effective an tidepressan t agents (eg. irnipramine)

effectively elirninated the anorexic effects ordinarily induced by LPS treannent.

Purpose

In view of the observation diat the proinflarnrnatory cytokines may have synergistic

actions with respect to hormonal functioning. it was of interest to establish whether these

cytokines would also have such effects on behaviors associated with illness. In this

context. the present investigation was undenaken to determine the individual and conjoint

actions of IL- 1. IL-6 and TNF-a on the consumption of a highly palatable food. as u r l l as

the variations of plasma corticosterone levels. Inasmuch as few srudies have assessed

these cytokine actions, despite their recent chical usage (Meyen & Valentine, 1995). a

dose response curve was determined for LPS and for each of the cytokines, after which the

conjoint actions of suboptimal doses of the cytokines were evaluated.

Experiments 1-4

The initial Experirnents were conducted in order to evaiuate the anorexic effects, as

well as the comcosterone variations induced by LPS, IL-1, IL-6 and TNF-a. Dose-

response curves were established for these compounds, since sub-optirnal doses had to be

detemined to evduate potential synergistic actions of the components. As well, the initial

experiments sought to establish whether the behavioral effects of the treannents paraiieled

their poten tial hormonal actions.

hlaterials and Methods

S ut,-iects

Male CD-1 mice, 8-12 weeks of a g were obtained from Charles River Inc,

Laprairie Quebec. After arriva1 at the facility, mice were housed in groups of four and

acclimated to the laboratory for at lem 2 weeks. Room temperature was rnaintained at 21°

C. lighting uas maintained on a 12 hr Iighddark cycle (lights on at 0800). One week prior

to the beginning of the expenments. mice were separated into individual polypropylene

cages with wire mesh lids and maintained ad libitum on pellet mouse chow (5075 Rodent

aiet Autoclaved. Rdston Purina), and tap water.

Prior to LPS or cytokine administration, rnice were given free access to chocolate

milk (Sealtest. 1% ppartly skimmed) for one hour each da' during the light phase (bonles in

at 10:OO). Bottles were weighed at the beginning and end of the hour in order to detemine

consumprion < weig h ts were convend to volume). Follouling the establishment of a steady

rate of dnnking (3 consecutive days on which consumption varied by less than IO%), rnice

received an acute innaperitoneal (i.p.) injection of eidier vehicle or LPS, al one of several

doses. 90 min prior to receiving the chocolate milk. On the day following dnig

adminismtion. consumption was again measured in order to determine rate of recovery.

Likewise, in Expenments 2-4, following establishment of baseline consumption.

mice received an acute i.p. injection of either vehicie or IL-lP, IL-6 or TNF-a (at one of

several doses) and 45 min (in the case of IL-1 and IL-6) or 30 min (in the case of TM-a)

afterward, consump tion was measured.

D x s

LPS (frorn Escherichia coli serotype 026:B6 L-3755 Sigma) was dissolved in

sterile saline (9%) and administered i.p. 90 minutes pnor to receiving milk. Each mouse

received a single dose of 0.5. 0.75 or 1.0 pg (n=10 per group) AU h g s were

administered in a volume of 0.4 ml. Vehicle animals were injecte. i.p. with 0.1 ml of

sterile saline.

IL- 1 P was kindlp provided by Dr. Craig Reynolds (Biolngicai Response Modifiers

Program, National Cancer Institute, Fredenck, Md. U A . Produced by E.I. Duponr De

Nemours. specific activity 1.8 1 x 1 o4 units/pg). TSF-a ! specific activity=l. 1 x 10 5

units/m_o) and IL-6 (specific activity=7.0 x 103 unirs/pg) wsre obtainrd from R&D

Systems. AU drugs were suspended in srenle saline (9%. IL-1 and IL-6 u-sre

adrninistered i.p. -25 minutes prior to receiving milk. whereris TSF-a utas adrninistsred

i.p. 30 minutes prior to receiving milk. IL4 u a s administered in a single dose of 0.025.

0.05. 0.1. 0.1. or 0.8 pg in=lO per group) in (i volume of 0.1 ml. IL-6 ueas administered

i.p. in a single dose of 0.1. 0.2. 0.8 or 1.6 pg in=10 per group,. TSF-u was

administerrd i.p. in a single dose of 1 . 2 or 4 p g C < n = l O per group ).L'chiclr. animals in di

croups (n=10 psr group) were injected i.p. with 0.4 ml sterile saline. C

In paraIlel eipenments mice rcceived i-p. administrrition of sithsr the LPS'.

iL- 1. IL-6 TXF-a or their respective vehicles. and then either 120 minutes (in the case of

LPS) or 60 minutes afteward (in the case of the cytokinesi. mice uwe decapitarsd and

uunk blood was collectai in tubes containing 10 pl EDTA. The blood was centrifuged a

'Micc in the LPS study wcrc also uscd to cvaluatc the anxiogenic effecis O T endoroxin trcatmcnt. Thus. micc had reccivcd a 5 min cxposurc io a 1 i g h r - d a r k box at 90 min following LPS trcatmcnt. As thc cndotosin \vas withour cffcct O n light-dark pc r fo rmancc . i t was decmcd unnccessary io conduci an a d d i t ional cxpcrimcnt io cvriluatc thc alrcady wcll documcnicd hormonal actions o f LPS.

3500 rpm. for 15 minutes, and the plasma frozen and stored at -70° C. The plasma

corticosterone concenmtions were detemineci, in duplicate, by radioimmunoassay using

kits obtained From ICN Biomedicals 1nc2. These assays were found to yield less than

104Iinm- and interassay variability.

Results and Discussion

Variations of consumption of palatable diet

Figures 1 and 2 show the variations of chocolate milk consumption over a 1 hr

period as a function of the ûeatment rnice received.

LPS

On tesr day. an acute i-p. injecaon of LPS 90 min prior to tesring reduceù

consumption of chocolate milk (See Figure 1 ). A repeated-rneaures analysis of vanance

revealed a significant Treatrnent x Sampling pend intcncrion F(6.71)=6.839. pc.0 1 .

Newman-Keuls multiple cornparisons of the means comprising the simple effects of this

interaction ta = .05) indicated that among vehicle treated mice consumption did not ciiffer

over any of the three sampling periods. In conrnst. al1 doses of LPS provoked a significant

reduction in consumption compared to baseline levels. Consumption of mice retested 24 hr

after LPS treatment rstumed to baseline levels. Betwesn group cornparisons indicated that

on Test da!. consumption in LPS-treated animals al each dosage \seris significantly reduced

relative to vehiclrc treated mice.

Insen Figure i about hers

rL- 1 - IL-1 administration reduced consumption of chocolate milk (See Figure 7-Cpper

panel). and the analysis of variance indicated a significant Cytokine x Sarnpling period

'Owing to ihc cxpcnsc of thc assays. ihe cffccis of only somc of ihe dcses ubcrc dctcrmincd. The spccific doscs for each drug wcrc bascd on pilot studics.

Figure 1. Mean (?s.E.M.) consumption (ml) of a highly palatable food arnong rnice that

received periphenl lipopolysaccharide (LPS) or saline. Various doses of LPS (0.5 pg.

0.75 pg or 1 pg) were adrninistered inuaperitoneally to male CD-] mice and consumption

of chocolate mik was measured over a 1 hr. period cornmencing 90 minutes following

injection. Baseline values were calculated by averaging consumption over the three day

period irnmediately prior to testing. Recovery data were collected 24 hr. following tesring.

LPS dose ------- Saline

8

Test I

Recovery

interaction F( 10.108) = 5.435, pc.0 1. Multiple comparisons within groups revealed that

mice treated with 0.05, 0.1, 0.2 or 0.8 pg of IL-1 45 minutes pnor to testing reduced

consumption of chocolate mik relative to baseline levels. Consumption in al1 animals teste.

retumed to baseline levels 24 hr following treatment. Among vehicle ueared rnice or those

that received the lowest IL- 1 dose (0.025 pg) consumption did not Vary over the sampling

penods. Between group comparisons indicated that following cytokine neatment. mice in

the 0.05 - 0.8 pg groups also consumed significandy less chocolate milk than saline mated

animals.

----------------------------------*-

Insen Figure 2 about here

TSF-a

Analysis of varirtnce of chocohte milk consumption in the TSF-a ueatsd mice

yidded a significant C-iokine x Sampling p e n d interaction. F( 6. 72) = 3.978. p < - O 1 .

.MulnpIe cornpuisons of the simple effects comprising the interaction indicated that among

the vehicle rreatd rnice consumption did not diffsr over the 3 mnpling penods. In

connast. u i t h i n each of the three n F - a groups a signifierint dsclinc irom basclinc values

uas evidsnt on the da) of cytokine treritmcnt. followed by a significant increass 21 hr latcr.

Betwsen-group çoniparisons confirmai that the groups did not diffsr li t baselins or 24 hr

after cytokine treatmenr. The TNF-a was found tn rèduce consumption of the chxo1;ite

milk relative to saline-ûeated mice. but this diffrrence on1 y reachsd statistical significrtnce

among niice treated with the 1.0 pg dose (see Figure .-Middle plinel).

L-6 - In contras to the effects of IL-1 and TSF-a. systemic administration of IL-6 dia

n a significan tly affect consumption (Sce Figure 7-Lowsr panel J. Although ii small

decreass in chocolats milk intake was seen on the day of rreatrnent in mice receiving cither

0.1 or O.? pg of IL-6. this decrease did nor approrich statistical significiince. Indeed. at the

Figure t Mean (~s.E.M.) consumption (mi) of a highly palarable food among mice

receiving penpheral inrerleukin- 1 P (IL- 1 ), turnor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-a).

interleukin-6 (IL-6) or saline. Various doses of IL- 1 (0.025 pg, 0.05 pg, 0.1 pg. 0.2 pg.

or 0.8 pg--Upper panel). TNF-a (1 pg. 2 pg, or 4 pg--Middle panel). or IL-6 (0.11 g . 0 . 3 ~ g, 0.8 pg or 1.6 pg--Lower panel) were administered intrapentonedly ro male CD- 1

mice and consumption of chocolate mik was measured over a 1 hr. penod comrnencing

either 45 minutes (for IL-1 and ILK-6) or 30 minutes (for ThF-a ) following injection.

Baseline values were calculated by averaging consumption over the three da) period

immediatcly pnor to testing. Recoverp data were collected 21 hr. following rcsting.

doses Saline 0.025 ug 0.05 ug

.------.. a........ 0, 1 "g - m-- 0.2 ug - 0.8 ug

- 7 I

BaseIine Test Recovery

TNF doses

Baseline Test Recovery

IL-6 doses ---a--- Saline

0.1 ug . . . 0.2 ug - 0 8 u g - 7 6 u g

I

Baseline Test Recovery

higher doses tested (0.8 pg and 1.6 pg), a modest increase in consumption was detected

relative to baseline levels. Rates of consumption arnong vehicle treated mice did not differ

over the three sampling periods.

Surnrnarizing, the results of Experiments 1 -4 indicate that systernic administration

of IL-1, TNF-a, and LPS (which induces the synthesis and release of IL- 1, IL-6 and

RIT-a) dose dependently suppressed consumption of a palatable substance. In contrast,

IL-6 administration was wirhout effect. Although these findings are consistent with

previous repons of cytokine- and/or LPS-induced anorexia (Van der Meer. Sweep,

Pesman, Born & Hermus, 1995; Plata-Salaman, 1995; Sonti, ilyin B; Plata-Salaman.

1996), the possibility remains that the behavioral effects observed in the present

investigation may not simply reflect an anorexic response. Indeed, it has recendy been

argued that ihe administration of LPS (Yirmiya, 1996) and cytokines (Anisman et al.,

1996) results in behavioral alterations that may reflect anhedonia, and in the case of LPS

the behavionl changes were antagonized by chronic administration of an antidepressant.

Thus, it ought to be considered that the reduction in consumpaon of a palatable diet

following LPS or cytokine administration may k chancreristic of an anhedonic as well as

an anoresic response.

Hormonal Variations

Figures 3 and 4 show the variations of plasma comcosterone levels as a funcnon

of the treatment rnice received.

LPS - Consistent with earlier repons (Mefford et al., 199 1 ; Dunn, 1992a; Johnson et al.,

1992), treatment with LPS was found to dose-dependently increase plasma comcosterone

concentrations (See Figure 3). Analysis of variance revealed that corticosterone levels

varied as a function of the LPS Treatment, F(2, 23) = 47.763, p c.01, and testing.

Newman-Keuls multiple cornparisons indicated that at a i l doses of LPS tested. plasma

co~costerone levels were significan tly increased relative ro saline-treated animals (a =

-05).

Insen Figure 3 about here

------------------------------------

rL- 1 - When administered i.p. one hour prior to testing, the behaviorally active doses of

IL-1 increased plasma corticosterone levels in a dose-dependent manner (See Figure 4-

Upper panel). Analysis of variance revenled a signifimnt effect of Treatment F(3.36.~=10.3.

pc.01 and multiple cornparisons indicated that in mice treated with 0.05 and 0.1 pg of IL- 1

corticosterone levels were significantly increased relative ro saline-treaied animals.

Although nearment with 0.025 pg of IL-1 produced a modrst increase in plasma

conicosterone levels relative ro saline-treated mice. this increasc did nor approach statisticd

sipificsnce.

TXF-a

.As in the case of the IL-1 treatrnrnt. anlilysis of variance indicated thltt TSF-u

administered one hour prior ro tesring sign ificantly incrcaxd plaïniri comcosrerons Isvels

F(3.36)= 16.087. pc.0 1. Multiple compnisons revealed that plasma conicosterone Ievels in

mice treated with 1 pg of TNF-a were significantly incrsased whcn compÿred to mice

rreated with saline. 1. or 2 pg of TNF-a. The lower doses. however. yielded

comcosterone levels that did not differ From those of control miîe (Sre Figure l-AMiddls

panel).

Figure 3. Mean ( + S .E.M.) plasma corticosterone concentrations (pg/dl) among mice

that received peripheral iipopoIysaccharide (LPS) or saline. Various doses of LPS (0.5 pg

or 1 pg) were administered inaaperitoneally to male C D 1 rnice and comcosterone

concentrations were measured 2 hr later (* p 4 1 relative to conrrols 1.

Saline

Figure 4. Mean ( ~ s . E . M . ) plasma conicosterone concentrations (pg/dl) in mice

receiving peripheral interleukin- 1 P (IL- 1 ), tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-a).

interieukin-6 (IL-6) or saline. Various doses of IL- 1 (0.025 pg. 0.05 pg or O. 1 pg-Upper

panel), TNF-a (1 pg. 2 pg or 4 pg-Middle panel), or IL-6 ( 0 . 1 ~ g or 0.8 pg-Lower

panel) were administered intraperitoneall y to male CD- 1 mice and plasma comcosterone

concenuarions were measured 1 hr later (* pc.05 relative to controis 1.

Saline 0.025 ug 0.05 ug 0.1 ug

TNF

U A

In view of the observation that no significant behavioral effects of IL-6 were

detected, only two doses were chosen in order ro assess the effects of the cytokine on HPA

activation. Consistent with earlier reports (Dunn. 19%; Zalcman et al., 1994), the

analysis of variance confmed that there was no difference betwecn plasma comcosterone

levels in mice veated uith O. 1. or 0.8 pg of IL-6 and saline-ueatd mimals (See Figure 1-

Lower panel).

Taken together. it appean thût LPS, IL4 and TKF-a. each of which influenced

consumption. also had the effect of increasing plasma caticosterone concentration S . 1 n

concrast. matment with IL-6. which failed to affect food consumption. likrwise was

without effect on plasma corticosterone concentrations. I t is of considerabIe interest that in

each expsnment. behaviorally active doses of LPS and the cytokines wrre associated with

increases in HPA activity. Morsover. those doses of TSF-a and IL-6 which did not

significantly alter consumption uwe also uithout effsct on plasma cnnicosterone lrvcls. Ir

is conceivablç that rhe correspondence ktween the changes in consumption of a pdarribls

substance and the HPX sffects inducsd by LPS or cytokine adminisrarion rsflects a

similarit) in rhe mrchanism(s) that undsrlie both rhe anoresic and the hormonal responses

to periphsral cytokine cidrninistntion.

Esperiments 5-7

As indicated sarlier. thers is reason ro expsci that the pro-intlammaiory cytokines

may have jyergistic effects with respect to the behavioral and homonal vrinlitions.

Expcriments 3-7 assessed the syergistic actions of these cytok ines. S ub-opiimîl doses ( i n

tems of behavior and endocnne effects) of the cyrokines were chosen to assess the

synergistis effect of cwadminisuation of IL-1 and IL-6. IL-I and TSF-a. and IL-6 and

TKF-a on consumption of a palatable subsrance and plasma conicostzrone levels.

Table 1

Dnig Treaments Administered in Experiments 5-7

Ex~eriment 5 Ex~erirnent 6

Veh + Veh Veh + Veh

IL-1 + Veh L-6 + Veh

Veh + IL-6 Veh + TPLF-a

IL-1 + IL-6 IL-6 + TNF-a

E x m m e n t 7

Veh + Veh

TM-a + Veh

Veh + IL- 1

TNF-a + IL- I

Figure 5. Effect of inmperitoneal administration of 0.025 pg L I + O. 1 pg IL-6

(Upper panel), 0.1 mg IL-6 + 1 pg TNF-a (Middle panel) or 0.025 pg IL- 1 + 1 pg

TNF-a (Lower panel) on mean ( ~ s . E . M . ) consumption (mls) of ü highly palatable food.

Male CD- 1 mice were injected with the cytokine combinations or 0.075 pg IL- 1 + Veh. 0.1

pg IL-6 + Veh, 1 pg TM-a + Veh or Veh + Veh, and consumption of chocolate milk was

measured over a 1 hr. p e n d commencing 1 hr following injection. Baseline values were

calculated by averaging consurnption over the three day period irnmediately prior to resting.

Recovery data were collected 24 hr. following testing.

---a--- Veh + Veh * IL-1 + V e h ......... ~h..-.-. IL-6 + Veh

Baseline Test Recovery

---O--- Veh A Veh - IL-6 + Veh . ..........,... .... TNF + Veh

IL-6 - TNF

Baseline Test Recovery

---O--- Veh - IL-1 .........+........ .,. TNF - 11-1

Veh Veh Veh TNF

Baseline Test Recovery

vehicle mated mimals. In contrast to the IL-1 marnent, the andysis of variance revealed

that neither the main effect of IL-6, nor the interactions involving this variable approached

significance.

IL-6 +TNF- ci

The combination of O. 1 pg IL-6 + 1 pg TM-a did not act synergisrically to reduce

chocolate rnilk consumption (See Figure 5-Middle panel). A repeated masures ANOVA

revealed a significant TSF-a x Sarnpling p e n d interaction F(2,72) = 5.807, pc.01, and

multiple cornparisons indicated that on Test day injection of 1 pg TNF-a + Vehicle

significantly reducai the consumption of chocolate mi.& relative to baseline levels and

vehicle treated mice. Treatment with IL-6 did not influence consumption, and likewise the

interaction benveen T'\T-a and IL-6 was not significant.

IL4 + TYF-a

Figure 5 (Lower panel) shows the consumption of chocolate mik as a function of

the IL- 1 and TSF* treatments. Analaysis of variance of the consumption scores indicated

a significant interaction betwecn IL- 1. TNF-a and Sampling period, F C , Z ) = j . 175,

pc.0 1 . Mulnple cornparisons of the simple effects comprising this interaction confmrd

that arnong vehicle treated animals consumption was stable over sampling periods. As

expected. the low doses of IL-1 and TNF-a did not provoke changes of consumption.

However. the combination of these treatments markedly reduced consumption of chocolate

milk rdanve to baseline scores as well as in comparison with that of animals that received

only one of the ueaments. Regardless of the trament mice received. the consumption

exhibited 23 hr afteward was comparable to the baseline scores.

The results of the present investigation indicated that the co-administration of IL-6

with either IL4 or TNF-a was no more effective in suppressing the consumption of

chocolate rniik than the administration of IL4 or Th'F-a alone. Of course. the possibility

cannot be excluded hat a synergism would have appeared had other doses or àme frarnes

k e n assessed. It is panicularly interesting, however, that the combinaaon of sub-effective

doses of IL-1 and TNF-a markedly suppressed consumption in a synergistic fashion.

These results are consistent with recent repons indicating a synergistic effecr between IL- 1

and TBF-a on feeding in rats (Yang, Koseki, Meguid, Gleason & Debonis, 1991: Van

der Meer et al., 1995). The latter studies suggested that 1 - 1 and TNF-a act

synergistically to provoke anorexia, as reflected by the suppression of feeding in rats given

free access to lab chow.

Hormonal ~ a r i a t i o n s

Figure 6 shows the variations in plasma corticostcronr lewls as a function of the

re;itmttnt miie receivsd.

Insen Figure 6 about hue

------------------------------------

IL- 1 - IL-6

A n of variance of plasma sonicostcrone Icvels fcillowing the s e

~idministriiticin 01 0.025 p g of IL- 1 t O. 1 pg of IL-6 rsvcaled thrit the effects of IL-h did iior

intsrxt wir h thox of IL- 1. In contrasr. ueritmsnt w i t h IL- 1 significrintly incrcrissd

circulrtring mriicostsrone concentrations. Fi 1.36 = 79-82. p c . 0 1 . relarive to control

animlils. Tnrimim u i t h IL-6 was ineffective in affectin- the levsls of conicosterons (sec

Figure 6-Cpper p:ineI)

IL-6 + TSF-a

Trentnisnt wi th TSF-a increased plasma corticosterone levels. Fc 1. 36 > = 19.70. p

<.01. umhersris IL-6 was again found to be wirhout sffsct. As wsll. IL-6 uearment did not

augment the ricrions of TSF-a (See Figure 6-Middle panel). Thus. it sèems that the

Figure 6. Effect of intraperitoneal administration of 0.025 pg IL- 1 + 0.1 pg IL-6

(Upper panel). 0.1 pg IL-6 + 1 pg TNF-a (Middle panel) or 0.025 pg IL- I + 1 pg TNF-

a (Lower panel) on mean ('s.E.M.) plasma conicosterone concentrations (pg/dl). Male

CD4 mice were injected with the cytokine combinations or 0.025 pg IL- 1 + Veh. O. 1 pg

IL-6 + Veh, 1 pg TNF-a , or Veh + Veh, and plasma conicosteronr levels were

measured 1 hr following injection (*synergistic increase relative to conrrol animais pc.05).

Veh IL- t IL-6 IL- 1 + .. - -

Veh Veh Veh IL-6

i'eh IL-6 TNF iL-6 - * - L'en Ver -. -

Ver. l . 4 r

combination of these nvo cytokines was not more effective in provoking an increase in

corticosterone levels than the administration of TNF-a + Vehicle.

IL- 1 + TNF-a

As observed in the behavioral study, the co-administration of 0.025 pg IL- 1 and 1

pg T h l resulted in a synergistic increase in plasma conicosterone levels (See Figure 6-

h w e r panel). Analysis of variance reveded a significant IL-I x TNF-a interaction. Ft 1 .

36)= 4.01. pc.01. The separare adminisuanon of either IL-1 or TSF-a also significantly

increased levels of comcosterone (F(1,36)= 21.126, pc.01 and F( 1.36)=9.598. p<.0 1

repectively) relative to saline-treated animals. however. multiple cornparisons indicated that

when the cytokines were administrred jointly. the levels of conicostcrone exceeded those

seen in mice that received either treatment alone (a=.05).

It appsars that in rnice treated wi th low doses of IL4 and TSF-a. synergistic

effects may be induced wi th respect to consumption of a highl! palatrihlc substance and

circulatiq corricosterone levels. In conrrast. neithsr i h r combinarion of IL- 1 and IL-6. nor

that of IL-6 and T\T-a. was associared ufirh such synsrgisrns. 11 i3 imponani to note rhrit

the altsrations in consumption of a palatable food obsen~ed in the khavioral studirs appear

to correspond ro the hormonal variations. hevious investigators h w c reponed a synsrsism

between IL- 1 and TSF-a on HPA activity (Van der Meer sr al.. 19951. A s u s the case in

the present investigation. the reducrions in food intcike in the Van der .Meer study

corresponded to the increascs in corticosterone Ievels. Whils i t is premrirurs ro conclude

that the behaviorai and homonal variations observed are crtusally linked. it is interesting to

note thnt over the course of the three da' infusion p e n d in the Van der Meer study. borh

feeding and HPA activity retumed to badine levels at the snmè time.

General Discussion-

Sumerous mechanisms have k e n elucidated uhich anempt to describe

development of anoresia. including modifications of neurotrxmittsr levels. altsrations in

hormone and/or peptide levels. and modulation of hypnthalarnic or grisnointestinal

hormone andor peptide levels, and modulation of hypothalarnic or gasmintestinal

functioning. Feeding can be disrupted by satiety s ipals (e.g., leptin, CCK, bombesin)

(Gibbs, 1985; Smith, Jerome & Norgren, 1985; Edwards, Ladenheim & Ritter, 1986;

Corwin, Gibbs & Smith. 1991; Pekyrnounter et al., 1995; Campfield, Smith, Guisez,

Devos & Burn, 1995; Halaas et al., 1995; Sarraf et al., 1997; Plarnadon & Merali, 1997),

but it should not be surpnsing to find that this behavior is also infiuenced by factors

unrelated to satiety per se. For instance, it is well documented that feeding is reduced by

illness (Kent et al., 1996), avenive stimulation (Griffirhs, Shanks, & Anisman, 1992). and

psyhological factors such as srress and depression (Willner, 1985 ). At Ieast some of

these events rnay reflect biologically adaptive responses. For instance, the reduced feeding

assnciated with aversive events may be important so that organisms in stress situations

focus on eliminating the aversive stimuli, rather than becoming involved in food gathering.

Similarly. it has k e n suggested that the anorexia associated with illness is essential to

direct energ). resources to immune functioning, nther than to diven energy to

oasrrointesrinal processes (Han, 1988; Kent et al., 1992b). Thus, it was suggested that C

alterarions in cytokine levels promote various illness behaviors, which may include the

khaviorally adaptive response of diminished feeding (De Kloet. Oitzi 8: Schobitz, 19%:

Kent er al., 1996).

Esperimental parad ips involving both humans and infrahuman species have

demonsuated that acute or chronic administration of cytokines, such as IL-1, IL-6 and

TSF-a can result in anorexia (Plata-Saiaman, Oomura & Kai, 1987: Socher et al., 1988;

Chance & Fischer. 1991; Plata-Salaman & Borkoski, 1993: Myers et al., 1991).

Consistent with these observations. the results of Experiments 1-4 indicated that (a) in the

CD- 1 mouse peripheral administration of LPS, which induces IL- 1, IL-6 and TNF-a

release from macrophages, effectively provoked dose-dependent reductions in the

consumption of a palatable food, and (b) acute i.p. administration of IL-1B or TNF-a

provoked a dose-dependent reduction in the consumption of a palatable food, whereas IL-6

mament was without effect.

The finding that i.p. IL-6 did not suppress feeding is consistent with previous

reports that demon strated that perip heral administration of IL-6 is associated with modes t

reductions in consumption even with doses approaching phamiacological levels (Plata-

Salaman, 1988). Thus, although cenual IL6 matment is reliably associated with anorexia

(Plata-Salaman, 1988; Schobitz et al., 1995), the effect of peripheral administration is

much less pronounced even at high doses (Espat et al., 1996). The lack of effect in the

present investigation may therefore be amiburable to the relatively low doses used.

However, it is also significant that in the present investigation consumption of a highly

paiatable substance presented during the light cycle was assessed, whereas in the Plata-

Salaman study, fiee feeding volume was detemined noctumally. Thus, while Plata-

Salarnan might have been assessing anorexia, consumption in the present investigation mai

have k e n an index of anhedonia. rather rhan anorexia per se. Of course, the procedures

used in the present investigation do not permit dissocianon of the anorexic and the

anhedonic effects of the cytokine treatments and such a conciusion is highly provisional.

It will be recalled that LPS induces the release of severai cytokines, which have the

potential to operate synergistically in determining hormonal and behavioral changes.

Indeed. it was demonsuated that I L 1 and IL-6 had interactive effects in the provocation of

corticosterone (Zhou et al., 1996) and ACTH secretion (Perlstein, Mougey, Jackson Br

Neta. 199 1; Matra et al., 1992). while IL-1 and TNF-a synergistically decreased b l d

glucose levels (Vogel, Henricson 8: Neta, 199 1). and induced anorexia (Yang et al .,

1991). In the present investigation L I and IL-6 were not found to have synergistic actions

with respect to chocolate milk consumption, nor did the combination of TNP-a and IL-6

have effects beond those elicited by TNF-a alone. However, the reduction of

consumption in mice that received the combination of low doses of IL-1 and TNF-a was

appreciabiy greater than the additive effects of the two treatments. In effect, it seemed that

these cytokines acted synergistically in suppressing palarable food consumption. It is of

considerable interest that previous investigations (Yang et al., 1994. Van der Meer et al..

1995) reporthg the synergistic effects of IL-I and TNF-a on food consumption found

that the most significant decline in food intake (Purina rat chow) occmed during

nocturnally associated ingestion intervals. The reduction in food intake diurnaily, although

significant, was less conspicuous. In the present investigation, the palatable diet was

provided during the light-phase, since it was of interest to establish the effects of the

cytokines on consumption of a highly palatable food in sated animals. Under such

conditions IL-I and TNF-a stdl interacted synergistically to produce a profound

suppression of consumption, a finding consistent with the proposition that the matment

lcad to an anorexia independent of effects on satiety per se.

It must be underscored bat in the present investigation a limited number of test

doses were used. and consumption of chocolate rnilk was assessed fiom 1-2 hr following

adminisrration of the cytokines, and plasma comcosterone was assessed 1 hr after

administrarion. It is certainly conceivable that synergistic actions of other cytokine

combinations would have k e n detected at other doses or at other post-adminisuao'on

in te~als . In fact. Zhou et al. (1996) found that a 10 ng inmvenous (iv) dose of bodi IL-1

and IL-6 s ~ c r ~ s t i c a l l y increased ACTH secretion in rats 30 minutes following

administration. followed by a return to baseline levels by 60 minutes. Ceadminismtion of

100 ng of IL- 1 and IL-6 also produced maximal effects 30 minutes after administration.

although at 60 ACTH levels were sri11 significantly elevated. Matra et al. (1992)

have like~eise reponsd that when injected iv, 100 ng of L I and IL-6 produced a maximal

effect o n ACTH levels which occurred soon after administration (20 min). Further to this

point. Pcrlstein et al. (1991) found that when adrninistered i.p., 10 ng of IL-la combined

with 1 p g of IL-6 produced a synergistic increase in the eariy ACTH ïesponse (30-60

minutes later). while at 1-3 hr pst-injection the ACTH response was similar to that elicited

by IL- 1 alone. In effect. it seems that the synergistic actions of the two treatments may not

plasma ACTH level occumd 120 minutes following the coadrninismtion (i.p.) of 1 pg

ThF-a and 10 ng IL- la, whereas the combination of 1 pg TNF-a with 1.35 k g of IL-6

produced a peak synergistic effect on ACTH levels from 30-60 minutes foilowing

administration. In light of these time-dependent synergis tic effects of the cytokines on

ACTH variations. it is premature at this juncture to conclude that cytokine combinations

other han IL-1 and TNF-a do not engender synerpstic actions with respect to either

palatable food intake or hormonal variations.

Hormonal changes associated with the administration of LPS and die cytokines

indicate that. in almost every expenment, elrvations of conicostsrone levels wcurred

contemporaneously with the anorexic and/or anhedonic effects. Indeed. in Expenments 1 -

3, the doses of TSF-a. IL4 and LPS which prrduced significan! reductions in

consumption of a palatable food. were also associatcd u i t h significrinr increase, of

circulxing corticosterone levels. Moreover, those doses of TSF-a anci IL-h which did not

producc sign i ficmt reductions in chcrolate milk consurnption u-cre also wi t hout effecr on

conicosterons l s \ds . Ir might k noted ar this juncture thsr d r h o u g h wiritioni; of HP.-\

activir>, have typically bsen associatsd with a\wsi\.s sL.snr5. ~ippetitive stimuli ma!

influence CRH. ACTH and corticosterone IsveIs N c Intosh. Kent. k: Merali. 1996 t . and

cnnvsrsely alterations of HPA homonai activir) mriy impact on fesding (Srrack.

Sebasrian. Schwanz 6: Dallmcin. 1995). Thus. hm i z reasoii ro aupposs rhat an

association esists, a lki t not necessarily a causal one. k turen the etTscts of the cytokines

on anorexiri rand/or rinhedonia) and on HPA functioning.

Ir ma? be cidded parenthrtically at this juncturc that Gclin. Irloldauer. Iresjo and

Lundholm ( 1993) reponed that in mice uith intact adrenal glands. TSF-a and IL- 1 4 5 0

and 10 ng/d~day. respectively) did not influence food consumption. Howevcr. in

adrenalectoniizrd animals, these rreatrnents reduced consumption significantl y. and a

funher dccline was provoked by the combination of the trerttments. In effect.

adrenslcctom) potentiated the anoresic respon w to the cytokines. Interestingly .

funher decline was provoked by the combination of the treatments. in effect,

adrenalectomy potentiated the anorexic response to the cytokines. Interesùngly,

pretreament with pharmacologicai doses of hydrocortisone (2.5 mg) abolished the anorexic

response to the individual or cornbined doses of IL-I and TNF-a. Taken together, these

data suggest that the anorexic effect of TNF-a and IL- 1 (or their combination) is probably

not a direct result of the corticosterone changes induced by the treatments. Clearly, if

hormonal factors were solely responsible for the anorexia, then adrenalectomy should have

antagonized the effecrs of the cytokines.

As in the behavioral smdy, there was no evidence of an increase in plasma

corticosterone levels following peripheral IL-6 adminisnation. Although previous studies

reponed increases in ACTH levels following central IL-6 marnent (e.g., Matta et al..

1 W6), it appears that peripheral IL-6 aeatment, at least at the doses selected in the present

investigation. does not have a comparable influence on corcicosterone concentranons. It

should be underscored that in rodents, IL-6 does not appear to stimulate HPA activity to the

sarne degree as IL- I or TNF-a (Matra et al., 1992: Dunn, 1992a; Perlstein et al., 1993:

Van Haasteren et al., 1991). Indeed, only moderate increases in ACT'H or comcosterone

levels are seen after administration of large (i.e., 100 ng icv) doses of IL-6. The results of

the present investigation are thus consistent with studies showing that low/moderate doses

of IL-6 are insufficient ?O produce significant changes in levels of corticosterone.

While it does appear that IL-6 cm directly activate the HPA axis, there is some

evidence that it may be more important for sustaining, rather than initiating, KPA activation

as a result of inflammation (Zhou et al., 1996). Moreover, Fatton et al. (1 994) suggested

activation of the KPA axis c m and does occur even in the absence of IL-6. The!;

demonsaated that IL-6 deficient rnice are capable of mounting a normal inflarnmatory

respnnse to systemic tissue danage generated by the i.p. administration of LPS. Both wild-

type and IL-6-deficient mice showed similar induction of comcosterone and rnild anorexia

following LPS, suggesting that the physiological contribution of IL-6 to the KPA response

in the case of systemic inflammation is negligible.

Commensurate witb such findings. it was demonstrated in the present investigation

that the increases of comcosterone following the c~admuiistration of either IL4 and IL-6

or TNF-a and IL-6, were not appreciably greater than those evident following

administration of either IL-1 or T N F u It is possible that a synergistic effect would have

k e n observed following the administration of higher doses of the cytokines, or if

endocrine measures had been taken at a different time following marnent. Nevenheless,

results such as these suggest thst peripheral mamient with a low dose of IL-6 did not

activate unique corticosterone secretagogues in addition to those influenced by the separate

adminisnation of IL- 1 andor TW-a (Matta et al., I992).

The CO-administration of IL-1 and TNF-a, as indicated earlier, produced a

synergistic increase in plasma corticosterone levels measured one hour following treament.

These results were not unexpected, given the dramatic synergisric effects of the two

cytokines on the behavior measured in this, and previous investigations (Long et al., 1992:

Sonti et al., 1996; Bluthe et al., 1993). It wiil be recalld that Van der Meer et al (1995)

reponed that the co-adminismtion of peripheral L-1 and TNF-a acted synergistically in

inducing anorexia, and increasing plasma ACTH and corcicosterone levels in rats. Thus,

the results of the present investigation support the suggestion that low doses of peripheral

cytokines can act synergistically to influence HPA activity and feeding in the mouse. Given

the overlapping biological functions of IL- 1 and TNF-a it is not surprising that these two

cytokines can act in a synergistic fashion, although the reason for the existence of two such

similar, and apparendy redundant, mechanisms is not clear. Perhaps, as suggested by Van

der Meer et al. (19951, the two systems CO-exist in order to increase the host's sensitivity to

subtle inflanmatory stimuli and to maxirnize the ability to respond to such threats in an

appropriate rnanner.

Although the CO-administration of IL4 and TNF-a has been shown to have

synergistic effects on a number of behavioral (e.g., reduction in food intake and mia l

exploration) and metabolic measures (e.g., increased plamsa corticosterone levels,

depression of blood glucose levels). it must be emphasized at this point, that the synergistic

actions of cytokine combinations do not necessarily extend to a l l behaviors or hormonal

actions (Vop l et ai., 1991: Perlstein et ai., 1993; Bluthe et al., 1994; Van der Meer et al.,

1995). It would be premature to conclude. for instance. b a t the lack of a synergistic effect

of the combination of IL- 1 and IL-6 on food consumption or plasma comcosterone levels

would preclude a synergistic effect on social exploration or b l d glucose levels.

Moreover, as indicated earlier, it would be essentiai to evaluate the hormonal and

behavioral actions of the cj~okine combinations across a wide range of pst-administration

in tervals.

The view was offered that cytokine-induced anorexia was a cennally mediated

effect. since icv administration of low doses of cytokines (in the nano,gmn range)

effectively reduced feeding (Plata-Salaman et al., 1987; Chance & Fischer, 1991; Myers et

al.. 1991: Plata-Salaman, Sonti, Borkoski, Wilson & Ffrench-Mullen, 1996). Reductions

in food intake were not seen following periphçral administration of cytokines, unless

rehnvely high doses (in the microgram range) were administered (McLaughlin et al.,

1992,). Funhcr, the ami-pyretic, indomethacin, blocks both the anorexia and fever

provoked by cytokine or LPS neatment, a finding consistent with the position that such

effects are mediated by a common, centrai mechanism (Langhans, Hariacher & Schmer

1989: Crestani, Seguy 8: Dantzer, 1991). Moreover, the anorexic effects of cenûally

administered cytokines could be blocked by pre-treatmenr with the appropnate receptor

antagonists appfied icv, supporting the contenrion that the suppression of feeding was

mediated by direct action in the CNS (Plata-Salaman & Ffrench-Mullen, 1992).

It was recently shown. however, that peripherai mechanisms may also contribute to

cytokine-induced anorexia. In particular, Kent et al. (1996), demonstrated that food-

motivated behavior in rats was reduced to a comparable extent following either i.p. or icv

IL- 1 beamen t Peri p herall y admùiistered IL- 1 required a much larger dose to d u c e food-

motivated beh avior, but decreased the latency to reduced responding relative to that seen

following icv administration. 'Ihus these data provisiond y suggest that peripheral

mechanisms may also be involved. Consistent with this conclusion, it was demonstrated

that the anorexic effects of i.p. L - 1 were blocked by pretreatment with an i.p. (but not an

icv) injection of IL-lra. Kent et al. (1992a) showed that cytokine-induced anorexia could

be dissociated from the febnle response. by using central IL- lm to block the pyrogenic. but

not the anorexic effects of icv IL-1 treament. Moreover, Bret-Dibat, Kent, Couraud,

Creminon & Dantzer (1994) reported that in addition to reducing food-motivated behavior,

peripheral LPS rreatment increased levels of substance P, neurokinin A and calcitonin gene-

relatai product in spinal cord afferents. It would thus appear that both penpheral and

central mechanisms subsente the anorexia induced by cytokines.

It has k e n suggested that peripheral cytokine signalhg to the cenaal nervous

system is mediated by the vagal afferent fibres (Bluthe et al., 1991; Laye et al.. 1995:

Gaykema et al.. 1995; Kent et al., 1995). In particular, subdiaphragmatic vagotomy has

been shown to attenuate or block the behavioral effects of i.p. administration of either LPS

or IL- 1 (Bluthe et al., 1994: Bluthe, Michaud, Keiley Lk Dantzer 1996a: 1996b). Moreover,

such a marnent attenuated the increased plasma ACTH and corticosterone secretion

ordinarily observed following i.p. administration of either IL- 1 or LPS (Gaykema et al..

1995; Fleshner et al., 1995: Kapcala et al., 1996). The alterations in ACTH secretion

following subdiaphragmatic vagotomy appear to be specific to immune-related stressors, as

hypoglycemic stimulation of ACTH secretion was unaffected by sectioning of the vagus

(Kapcala et al., 1996). It is important to note as well, that the effects of vagotomy on

ACTH and comcosrerone levels do not appear to reflect a generalized reducnon in HPA

responsivi ty , but instead are likel y relared specifically to immune activation. In particular,

vagotomy does not affect increases in plasma corticosterone levels produced by elecaic

shock (see discussion in Watkins, Maier & Goehler, 199%). Moreover, while

subdiaphragrnatic vagotomy completely blocks PVN c-fos induction foilowing cenaal or

peripheral LPS administration, it potentiates PVN c-fos induced by footshock in rats (Wan,

Wetmore, Sorensen. Greenberg & Nance, 1994).

The findings of the aforementioned investigations have led to the suggestion that the

influence of subdiaphragmatic vagotomy is Limited to those functions which are mediate.

by the transmission of messages from peripheral cyokines to the brain (Watkins, Maier &

Goehler, 1995b). Indeed, Bluthe et al. (1996a; 1996b) demonsaated that vagotomy

anenuates the behavioral effects of peripheral, but not subcubcutaneous, innavenous, or

central administration of IL- 1. Presumably, sectioning of the vagus prevents the activation

of vagal afferents ordinarily stimulated by peripheral cytokines. Thus. such a matment

would be espec td to preclude the essential factor rhat would othenvise lead to increased

brain cytokine levels that ultimately mediate ilIness responses, including fever and

anorexia. It should be emphasized that while the vapus may be an imponant route for

cytokine-brain communication, such innervation does not preclude the existence of alternate

communication pathways for immune signalling of the CKS.

In addition to a potentid role for immune-brain communication, vagal afferent

fibres have also been implicated in the mediation of feeding. For example. it was reponed

that selscrive sectioning of va@ afferents. but not efferents. biocked the satiating effects of

peripherdly administered CCK (Smith. Jerome, Cushin, Etemo & Simansky, 198 1 ; Smith

et a.. 1985). South and Rirter (19881, and Bluthe et al. (1996a; 1996b) subsequently

demonstrated that direct application of capsaicin to subdiaphrapatic vagal afferents also

attenuated CCK inducrd suppression of food intake. Likewise, capsaicin influenced

variations of feeding ordinarily provoked by bombesin neatment (Kent & Merali. 1996:

SlcIntosh & Merali; 1996). It was funher demonstrated that, in rnice, vagotomy attenuated

the decrease in responding for food reward following peripheral administration of IL-1 or

LPS (Bret-Dibat et al.. 1993; Kent et al., 1996). Such findings are not unexpected in view

of the observation that vagal afEerent fibres project to the paravenaicular nucleus of the

hypothalamus, which appears to be an integml area in the mediation of feeding. It should

also be considered that the activity of glucose sensitive neurons in the lateral hypothalamus

(LH) is suppressed following iontophoretic application of IL-1 and TNF-a, whereas

glucose-sensitive neurons in the venuornedial hypothalamus are excited following

the application of IL-1 (Plata-Salaman et al., 1988; Kuriyama, Hori, Mori & Nakashima,

1990). Alterations in the activity of both the LH and VMH have k e n associated with

changes in the rnicrosmicnire of feeding behavior, including decreases in meal fRquency

and meal size. as well as increases in post-prandial intermeal intervals (Plata-Salaman,

1997). Similar microsmictural alterations in feeding patterns are evident following

subdiphragmatic vagotomy (Davis et al., 199-2; 1995).

In view of the proposed relationship between immune activation of vagal afferents

and feeding, it was hypothesized that destruction of these afferent fibres would attenuate

the reduced consumption of a palatable f ' induced by i.p. LPS marnent. As outlined in

Appendix A, capsaicin administered to neonatal mice su bcutaneously selectively destroy s

approximately 9 5 5 of C fiber afferents (Holzer, 199 1). Accordingly, it was expected that

neonatal capsaicin administration would antagonize the anorexia otheruise induced by LPS

(See Appendix A). The results of this supplemeniary expriment failed, however, to

support this hypothesis. The consumption of chocolate mik by rnice ueated wth capsaicin

did not differ significantiy from that of vehicle treated animals following an i.p. injection of

LPS. In addition, there were no significant differences in corticosterone levels between

either g o u p of mice.

Several possibilities, in addition to non-involvement of vagal afferents in post-LPS

feeding, exist to account for the observed outcorne. The most parsimonious explanation for

the lack of behavioral differentaàon among capsaicin and vehicle treated mice is indequate

vagal intemption. In the present investigation, capsaicin neated neonatal mice exhibited

mild respiratory distress and cutaneous vasodilation for a bnef p e n d immediately

following h g administration. In conrrast, here was no evidence of eidier physical or

respiratory discomfon among vehicle treated mice. Nonetheless, the results of the eye-wipe

test conducted at 30 days of age (data not shown). which is used as an index of afferent

fibre destruction, failed to reveal any ciifferences in the response of either the capsaicin or

vehicle treated anirnals. It is therefore conceivable that the failure to provoke effects on

LPS-induced feeding and corticosterone levels rnay be amibutable to incomplete destruction

of the afferent fibres following capsaicin administration.

An altemarive explanation for the apparent lack of effect among capsaicin mated

rnice is that the reduction in consumption of chocolate mik induced by LPS was rdated to

activanon of central mechanisms, which precluded the appearance of putative peripheral

effects of capsaicin. This hypothesis is not a likely one since the low dose of LPS

employed (0.5 pg) would have primarily influenced peripheral mechanisms. Indeed,

preliminary studies revealed that this dose of LPS elicited only a moderate decline in the

consurnption of chocolate rnilk in othensise unueated mice, accompanied by a modest

increase of corticosterone levels. Funhermore, Dunn (1992b; Dum & Chuluyan, 1994:

Lavicky % Dunn, 1995) demonsrrated rhar when LPS was administered i-p., the minimal

effective dose which increased cerebral catecholarnine metabolisrn was 1 pg. with the peak

effect occumng between 2-8 hours following dmg administration. Thus, it is likely that a

0.5 pg dose of LPS would not be sufficient to mgger cenaally mediated changes in HPA

activity.

The possibility that capsaicin did not effectively disnipt vagal afferents cano t be

readil y dismissed. As indicated earlier, injection of capsaicin resulted in imrnediate

behavioral changes consistent with vagotomy. Nevenheless, it should be ernphasized that.

as adults, these mice did not show a disturbed corneal sensitivity as assessed by the

eyewipe tesr. Interestingly, a very ment study conducted by Bret-Dibat et al. (1997) also

reveded that. in contrast to surgical vagotomy. capsaicin did n a block the decline in food-

morivated behavior ordinarily produced by LPS or IL- 1 treatment. In this study a senes of

7 i.p. capsaicin injections (75 rng/kg), administered on successive days, was used to

destmy C-fiber afferents in adult mice, and the effectiveness of the capsaicin mamient was

verified by alterations in comeal and pain sensitivity, and by attenuation of CCK-induced

satiety. It has likewise been reporred (Tilders et al., 1994) that capsaicin treatment in rats

did not attenuate the ACTH response to a 2.5 mgkg i-p. dose of LPS. Clearly, the findings

of the present investigation concerning the lack of an effect of capsaicin on chocolate rnilk

consumption and plasma corticosterone levels are consistent with both of these reports. It is

possible, as suggested by Bret-Dibat et al. (1997), that the signaihg pathway ùiat

ûansmits information regarding penpheral cytokines to the brain is not exclusively

comprised of capsaicin- sensitive C-afferen ts.

The question remains as to whether the reduction in consumption of a palatable

food, such as the chocolate milk, represents an anorexic effect, or would be more

appropriately considered a reflecrion of anhedonia. It will be recailed that several

differences can be identified between the feeding behavior assessed in this investigation and

feeding measured in other laboratones ( e g , Yang et al., 1994; Van Der Meer et al., 1995:

Plata-Salaman et al., 1996; Sonti et al., 1996; Bret-Dibar et al., 1997). In the present

investigation, mice were given ad libitum access to lab chow. The chocolate milk was

only presented dunng the light-cycle, shortly after the lights were tumed on. Diumal

associated feeding would be expected to be minimal following the relative hyperphagia

which occurs nocturnally (Plata Salaman et al. 1996; Sonti et al., 1996; Yang et al., 1996).

Indeed, mice would not be expected to be 'hungry'. Despite these factors, sated mice

consumed a substanrial amount of chocolate milk during a one hour pend , implying a

strong motivation to consume a palatable substance. Indeed, it has been argued that the

consurnption of palatable diets under some circumstances (e.g., following stressor

exposure) may represent an ided method of assessing anhedonic effects associated with

such treannents (Willner et al., 1987; Papp et al., 1991). While not dismissing the

potenaal effects of cytokines on anorexia, it is possible that the reduction in consumption of

chocolate milk observed in the present investigation following the administration of LPS,

Th- and IL- 1 may be a reflection of a decrease in sensitivity to rewards, similar to that

observed following stressors.

A very recent snidy conducted by S wiergiel, Srnagin & Dunn (1 997), assessed the

effect of infection with influenza virus, LPS and IL- 1 on the ingestive behavior of rnice. In

this study, both intake of food pellets and the consumption of sweetened condensed milk

were measured following acute i.p. injections of LPS or IL-1, or intranasal innoculation

with influenza virus. It was observed that LPS, IL-1 and infection with a lethal dose of the

v i n s were associated uith a reduction in both food pellet intake and consumption of milk.

In contrast, exposure to sublethal doses of influenza virus caused a lesser reduction in food

pellet intake. but was without effect on milk consumption. Thus it would zppear that mice

infested with sublethal doses of influenza virus were more motivated to drink the highly

palamble rnilk than to eat food pellets. B a s d on these results. it might be argued that the

reduction in food pellet intake reflects anorexia, while the reduction in the consumption of

condensed milk reflrcts anhedonia. Indeed. in the same study, pretreatment with

indomerhacin significcintly attenuated the rduction in both food pellet and milk intake

caused by IL- 1 and LPS. Iii conmst, in mice infected with a lethal dose of influenza virus.

chronic indomethacin attenusted the reduction in food pellet intake. but was without effect

on the rsduction of rnilk intake.

Xnhedonic-like behaviors have previously k e n demonsmted following the

adniinistrrttion of cytokines or LPS. Peripheral LPS administration markedly reduced

hedonic behsvior in male rats. as reflected by a decrease in consumption of saccharin

snlurion. and a suppression of sexual behavior (Yirmiya, 1996). Anisman et al. (1996),

showed that, in rats, peripheral IL-2 t r e a m n t dismpted responding for rewarding brain

stimulrition. accompanied by decreased DA release from the nucleus accurnbens. Dopamine

nctiviry in this ai-ea has k e n implicated in motivationai disturbances associated with goal-

dirscted responding to positively reinforcing stimuli. In connast to the effects of IL-2.

however, rats treated with IL-1 showed proficient responding for rewarding brain

s tirnulahon, and normal accumbal DA efflux, although there w as evidence of increased

accurnbd 5-FIT utilization in these mimals. Interestingly, peripherat administration of IL-6

(which had no influence on feeding in the present investigation), also dismpted accurnbal

DA efflux, although it did not affect responding for rewarding brain stimulation (Anisman,

Kokkinidis, Borowski, & Merali, 1997). Together, these data suggest that the effects of

IL-1 are distinct from those of IL-2 and IL-6. Furthemore, the behavioral and

neurochernical profiles associated with IL4 administration are not consistent with

treatments beiieved to have anhedonic action (e-g., stressors, neuroleptics). In effect, when

coupled with the data of the present investigation, it would appear iikely that the actions of

IL-1 would be more comfortably interpreted in terms of the potential anorexic effecrs of

such a treamient. Of course. this does not imply that the effects of TNF-a are independent

of anhedonic effects. Indeed, it is conceivable that creatments such as LPS, which promote

release of IL-1 and TKF-a, suppress feeding owing to both anorexic and anhedonic

effects. It remains to be determined whether sy stemic administration of ThTF-a influences

responding for rewarding stimuli that do not have an appetitive component (e-g., rewarding

brain stimulrtrion).

Finally. it ought to be underscored that the potential hormonal and behavioral

consequences of cytokine administration is only a newly evolving research area, and

understandably . the available data are limited. Comple te dose response Cumes, and time-

response curves, have not been reponed with respect to the behavioral actions of the

cytokines. As a result, data such as those of Anisman et al. (1997) must be considereù as

provisional, at best, pending more detailed elabration of cyokine actions. Inasmuch as the

conclusion of the present investigation is denved, in part, from the Iirnited literarure

available, the notion that the cytokine-induced feeding effects involve anorexic rather than

anhedonic effects, must likewise be consmed as tentative.

References

Anismn, H., Kokkinidis, L, Borowski, T. & Merali, Z. (1997). Diffixential effects of interleukin (IL)- lP, IL-2 and IL6 on responding for rewarding lataal hypothalamic

stimulation. Brain Research, in press.

Anisman, H., Kokkinidis, L. & Merali, 2. (1996). Interleukin-2 decreases accumbal

dopamine efflux and responding for rewarding l a d hypothalamicstimulation. Brain

Research, 731, 1-1 1.

Anisman, H., Zalcman, S . & Zacharko, R.M. (1993). The impact of stressors on immune

and cenual neuromsminer activity : Bidirec tional communication. Reviews in

Keuroscience, 4, 147- 1 80.

Arzt, E., Sauer, J., Pollmacher, T., Labeur, M., Holsboer, F., Reul, J.M.H.M. & Stalla.

G .K. ( 1993). Glucoconicoids suppress interledcin- 1 receptor antagonist synthesis

following induction by endotoxin. EndocrinoIom. 1 33. 672-677.

Berkenbosch. F., de Rijk. R., Del Rey, A. & Besedovsky, H. (1990).

Seuroendocnnology of interleukin- 1. In J.C. Porter & D. Jezova (Eds.), Circulatinq

Reoiilaton* Factors and Neuroendocrine Function. (pp. 303-3 11). New York:

Plenum.

Berkenbosch, F.. Van Oers, J., Del Rey, A., Tilders. F. & Besedovsky, H. (1987).

Corticouopin-releasing-factor-producing neurons in the rat activated by interleukin-1.

Science. 238, 523-526.

Berkenbosch. F., Wolvers, D.A.W. & Derijk, R. (1991). Neurwndocrine and

immunological mechanisms in suess-induced immunomodulation. Journal ofSteroid

Biochemistn. and Molecular Biolo~v. 40, 639-637.

Bernstein, I.L. (1995). Neural mediation of food aversions and anorexia induced by tumor

necrosis factor and tumors. Xeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. SQ, 177- 18 1.

Besedovsky, H., del Rey, A. Sorkin, E., Da Prada, M., Burri, R. & Honnegger, C. (1983). The immune response evokes changes in noradrenergic neurons. Science,

22 1, 564-566. -

Besedovsky, H., del Rey, A., Sorkin, E. & Dinareiio, C.A. (1986). Immunoregulatory

feedback between interleukin- l and glucocorticoid hormones. Science. 233, 652-654.

Besedovsky, H.O., Sorkin, E.. Felix, D. & Haas, H. (1977). Hypothalarnic changes

during the immune response. Europan Journal of Irnmunolow. 7, 323-325.

Blalock, J.E. (1 994). The syntax of immune-endocrine communication. Immunolo~

Todav. 15, 504534.

Blalock, J.E. (1984). The immune system as a sensory organ. Journal of I m m u n o l o ~

132, 1067- 1070.

Blalock, J.E. & Smith, E.M. (1985). A complete regulatory loop between the immune and

neuroendocrine systems. Federation Roceedings FASEB. 44, 108- 1 1 1.

Bluthe, R.M., Dantzer, R., dr Kelley, K.W. (1 992). Effects of interleukin-1 receptor

antagonist on the behavioral effects of lipopolysaccharide in rat. Brain Research. 575,

3 18-320.

Bluthe, R.M., Michaud, B., Kelley, K., & Dantzer, R. (1996a). Vagotomy anenuates

behavioral effects of interleukin-1 injected peripherally but not cenmily. KeuroReoon,

7, 1485-1488. -

Bluthe, R.M., Michaud, B., Kelley, K., & Dantzer, R. (1996b). Vagotomy blocks behavioral effects of interleukin-l injected via the intrapentoneal route but not via other

sysremic routes. NeuroRepon. 7, 2823-2827.

Bluthe, R.M., Pawlowski, M., Suarez, S., Parnet, P., Pittman, Q., Kelley, K.W. & Dantzer. R. (1 994). Synergy between tumor necrosis factor a and interleukin-1 in the

induction of sickness behavior in mice. Psvchoneuroendocrinokyv. 19, 197-207.

Bluthe, R.M., Walter, V., Parnet, P., Laye, S., Lestage, J., Verrier, D., Poole, S.,

S tenning, B .E., Kelley, K. W. & Dantzer, R. (1 994). Lipopolysaccharide induces

sickness behavior in rats by a vagai mediated mechanism. Life Sciences. 317, 499-

503.

Bret-Dibat, J.L., Creminon, C., Couraud, J.Y ., Kelley, K.W., Dantzer, R., & Kent, S.

(1997). Systemic capsaicin premünent fails to block the d m in food-motiated behavior induced by lipopol ysaccharide and hterleukin- 1 p Brain Research Bulletin,

47 443-449. -9

Bret-Dibat, J.L., Kent, S., Couraud, J.Y., Crerninon, C., & Dantzer, R. (1994). A

behaviorall y active dose of lipopoly saccharide increases sensory neuropeptides levels

in mouse spinal cord. Neuroscience Letters. 173, 205-209.

Buttini, M. Br Boddeke, K. (1995). Penpheral Lipopolysaccharide stimulation induces interleukin- l p mesenger RNA in rat brain microglial cells. Neuroscience. 65, 513-

530.

Campfield, L.A., Smith, F.J.. Guisez, Y.. Devos, R. & Bum, P. (1995). Recombinanr

mouse OB protein: Evidence for a penpherd signal linking adiposity and cenaal neural

networks. Science. 269, 536-539.

Carlson, S.L., Felten, D.L., Livnat, S. Br Felten, S.Y. (1987). Alterations of monomines

in specific central autonomie nuclei follouing immunization in mice. Brain Behavior

Immunolocrv, 1, 52-63.

Chance, W.T. Bc Fischer, J.E. (199 1). Aphagic and adipsic effects of interleukin- 1. Brain

Research. 568, 36 1-264.

Coelho, M.M., Souza, G.E.P. & Pela, I.R. (1992). Endotoxin-induced fever is modulated

by endogenous glucoconicoids in rats. American Journal of Phvsiologv. 263, R423-

R337.

Convin. R.L.. Gibbs, J. & Smith, G.P. (1991). Increased food intake after type A but not

type B cholecystokinin receptor blockade. Phvsiology and Behavior. 50, 255-258.

Crestani, F., Seguy, F. & Dantzer R. ( 199 1 ). Behavioural effects of peripherally injected

interleukin- 1 : roie of prostaglandins. Brain Research. 542, 330-335.

Dantzer, R. & Kelley, K.W. (1989). Suess and immunity: An integrated view of

relationships between the brain and the immune system. Life Sciences. 44. 1995-

2008.

Davis, J.D., Smith, G.P. & Kung, T M . (1994). Abdominal vagotomy alters the structure

of the ingestive behavior of rats ingesting liquid diets. Behavioral Neuroscience. 108.

767-779.

Davis. J.D.. Smith. G.P. & Kung. T.M. (1995). Atdominril vqotorny arrenuates rhs

inhibiting effect of mannitol on the ingesrive behavior of rats. Behavior~l

Neuroscience. 109, 16 1 - 1 67.

De Kloet. E.R.. Oitzl, M.S . . &: Schobitz. B. t 1993). Cytokines and the brain

conicostcroid receptor balance: Relevancc to pathoph ysiolng), of neurcxndosrins-

immune communication. P s ~ c h o n e u r ~ e n d o c n n 0 1 0 ~ ~ ~ . 19. 12 1 - 1 31.

DsrijL. R.. Van Rooijen. S.. Tilders. F.J.H.. Besedovsk). H.O.. Del Re'. .A. 6

Berkenbosch. F. i 1991). Sslective depietion of rnxrophrigs~ prsvsnts piluitan.-

adrcnal activation in response to subpyrogenic. but not to pyrogenic. dose, of büctsrial

endotosin in rm. Endocrinolooy. 128. 330-38.

De Simoni. M.G.. Del Bo.. R.. De Luigi. A.. Sirnard. S. 22 Forloni. G. i 1995). Cenrrcil

endotosin induces diffcrent patterns of interleukin t IL i- 1 P and 11-6 messsnger

rihonuclcic acid espressinn and IL-6 secretion in the brain and p s r i p h q .

Dunn. A.J. ( 1988). Systemic intcrleukin- l adniinismtion stimulirrrs hypothalaniic

norepinephnnt mewbolism parallelling the inçrerised plasma corricosterorte.

Sciences. 43, 429-335.

Dunn. A.J. ( I W O ) . Interleukin- 1 as a stimulator of horn~ons seçrerion. P r o ~ r r s s in

?le~irolorv. Endocnnolciov and Immiinolocv. 7. 26-74.

Dunn, A. (1992a). The role of interIeukin-1 and tumor necrosis factor a in the

neurochemicai and neuroendocrine responses to endotoxin. Brain Research Bulletin ,

29, 807-812. -

Dunn. -4. ( 1992 b). Endotoxin-induced activation of cerebral catecholarnine and serotonin

mstabolism: Cornparison with interleukin-1. The Journal of Pharmacologv and

Experimental Theramutics. 26 1, 964-969.

Dunn, A.J. i 1995). Interactions between nervous system and immune systern: Implications

of psychopharmacology. In F.E. Bloom and D.J. Kupfer (Eds., .

Ps\~chopharmacolo,mr: A 4th Generation of Prozress. New York: Raven Press. pp.

7 19-732.

Dunn. A . J . & Chuluyan. H.E. (1994). Endotoxin elicits normal uyptophan and indoiamine

rssponsss but impaired catecholarnine and pituitanl-adrenril rssponsss in endorosin-

resimnt mics. Life Sciences, 53, 847-853.

Dunn. A.J.. Poweil. 41.L.. 8: Gaskin. J.M. ( 1987 i . Virus-induccd increrisez in plasma

corticostsrone: A technical comment on Smith. Meyer and Biaiock. Science. 2-33.

1323- 1421.

Dunn. A . J . . Powell, ML. . Meitin. C. & Small, P.A. ( 1989). Vinis infection as a stressor:

Intlursnza vinis elzvares plasma conicosterone. and brriin rrytnphan concentrations of

VHPG md tryprophan. Phvsioloev B: Behavior. 15. 59 1-594.

Dunn. A . J . d;r L'ickers. S .L. ( 1994). Keurochemiclil and nrurosnd~nne responsss to

Ssu~sastls dissase virus adminisnation in rnice. Brain Rescirch. 643. 103- 1 12.

Ehisui. O.. Fukrita. J.. Murakami, S., Kobayashi. H.. S s g w a . H.. Muro. S.. Hanaokii.

1.. Saito. Y., Masui, Y., Ohmoto, Y., Imura, H. & Sakao. K. (1994. Effect of 11-1

receptor iiotctgonist and antiserum to TNF-a on LPS-induced plasma ACTH and

corticosterone rise in rats. American Journal of Phvsioloq. 266. EW6-E992.

Edv rtrdb. G.. Lridenheim. E.E. & Ritter. R . ( 1986 1. Dorsornediri1 hindbrain pmicipation

i n ~holsc!~stokinin-induced satiety. American Joiirnnl of Physiolocy. 25 1. R97 1-977.

Espat, J.J., Auffenberg, T.. Rosenberg, J.J.. Rogy. M.. Martin. D.. Fang, C H . .

Hasselgren. P.O.. Copeland. E.M., & Moldawer. L.L. (1996). Ciliaq neurotrophic

factor is catabolic and shares with IL-6 the capaciry to induce an acute phase response.

American Journal of Phvsiolo~y27 1, R 185-R 190.

Exton. M.S.. Bull. D.F. & King. M., G. (1995). Behavioral conditioning of

lipopoly saccharide-induced anorexia. Phvsiolow and Behavior. 57, 10 1 -405.

Fantino, M. 6; Wieteska, L. ( 1 992). Evidence for a direct central anorectic effwt of turnor- necrosis-factor-alpha in the rat. Phvsioloev &Behavior, 53. 477-483.

Fantuzzi. G.. Di Santo. E.. Sacco. S.. Benigni. F. 8: Ghrzzi. P. ( 19951. Rok of the

hypothalamus-pituitq-adrenal axis in the regulation of TSF production in rnics.

Effect of stress and inhibition of endogenous giucocorticoids. The Journal of

ImmunoIocv. 155, 3552-3555.

Fattori. E.. Cappellctti. M.. Costa. P.. Sellitro. C.. Cantoni. L.. Carclli. M.. Fagpioni. R . .

Fantuzzi. G.. Ghczzi. P. & Poli. V. (199-11. Defective intlanirnritory responsc in

inrerlsukin 6-deficisnt mice. Journal of Experimennl Medicine. 180. 1213- 130.

Felten. S.Y. & Felten. D.L. ( 199 1). Innenrarion of Iymphoid tissue. In R . Ader. D.L.

Felten and S. Cohen c Eds. i. Ps~chcineuroimmunoloo~. San Diego: Aç~dsmic Press.

pp. 27-72.

Fleshner. M.. Gocshkr. L.E.. Hermann. J.. Relton. J.K.. Muer. S.F. 8: W'arkins. L.R. t 1995). Interlsukin-lp induced conicostsrone elevation and hypthriiamic XE

dcpletion is vnpally mcdiared. Brain Rcsearch Bulletin. 37. 605-6 1 0 .

Gatti. S.. & Bartfai. T. (1993). Induction of tumor necrosis f;tctor-u mR3.4 in the brain

aftsr peripherd sndotoxin ueament: Cornparison with inisrleukin-l faniily and

interleukin-6. Brain Research. 624, 29 1-293.

Gaykemn. R.P ..A.. Dijksm. 1. & Tildsrs. F.J.H. ( 1995). S~bdiaphra~matic va=otorny

suppresscs rndotoxin-induced activation of hyxxhalamiç conicouopin-rslzasins

hormone neurons and .ACTH secretion. Endocrinolooy. 1 36. 17 l7--XO.

Gibbs, J. (1985). Effect of bombesin on feeding behavior. Life Sciences. 77, 147- 153.

Goujon, E., Parnet. P., Aubert, A., Goodall, G . & Dantzer, R. (1995). Corticosterone regulates behaviorai effects of lipopolysaccharide and interleukin-1 p in mice.

Arnerican Journal of Phvsioloov. 269, R154-R 159.

Gnffiths, J., Shanks, N., & Anisman, H. (1992). Sm-specific alterations in

consumption of a pdatable diet following repeated stressor exposure. P h m a c o l o ~

Biochemisw and Behavior. 42, 219-227.

Hailas, J.L., Gajiaala, K.S., Maffei, M., Cohen, S.L., Chait, B.T., Rabinowitz, D.,

Lallone, R.L., Burley, S .K. & Friedman, J.M. (1 995). Weight-reducing effects of

the plasma protein encoded by the obese gene. Science. 269, 543-546.

Hart, B. (1988). Biological basis of the behavior of sick anirnals. Neuroscience and

Biohehwioral Reviews. 12, 133- 137.

Harbuz. 51.S.. Stephanou, A., Sarlis, N. & Lighrman, S.L. (1992). The effects of recombinant human interleukin (IL-)- 1 a, IL-P. or IL-6 on hypothalamo-piniitary-

adrsnai axis activation. Journal of E n d d n o l c r ~ v . 133, 349-355.

Holzer, P. (199 1). Capsaicin: Cellular targets. mechanisms of action. and selectivity for

thin sensory neurons. Pharmacolozv Review, 43, 133-201.

Johnson. R.W., Propes, M.J. & S havit, Y. (1996).Conicosterone modulates behaviorai

and mctabolic effects of lipopolysaccharide. Amencan Journal of Phvsiologv. 270.

R192-Rl98.

Kakucskri, I., Qi, Y., CIark, B.D. & Lechan, R.M. (1993). Endotoxin-induced

comcotropin-releasing hormone gene expression in the hypothalarnic paravenuicular

nucleus is mediated cenwlly by interleukin-1. Endocnnologv. 133, 8 15-87 1.

Kapcala. L.P., He, J.R., Gao, Y., Pieper, J.O. & DeTolla, L.J. (1996). S~bdiaphra~gnatic vagotomy inhibits inm-abdominal interleukin- 1 p stimulation of

adrenoconicotropin secretion. Brain Research. 728, 217-254.

Keller, S ., Schleifer,

immune function

D.L. Felten and

S .I. & Demetrikopoulos M.K. ( 199 1 ). Stress-induced changes in

in animais: Hypothalmo-Pituitary-Adrenal influences. In R. Ader.

N.Cohen (Eds.). 1. San Diego: Academic Press. pp. 77 1-789.

Kenr, P. & Merali, Z. (1996). Does bombesin (BN) mediate stress responses through CRF

release? Effects of bombesin on brain CRF, plasma ACTH, conicosrerone and

catecholamines. Summer Neuropeptide Conference, Edganown. Mass.

Kent, S., Bluthe, R.M., Danuer, R., Hardwick, A.J., Kelley, K., Rothwell. S. &

Vannice. J.L. ( l992a). Different receptor mechanisms mediate the pyrogenic and

khavior;il effects of interleukin- 1. Proceedin r s of the Sational Academv of Science,

B. 91 17-9120.

Kent. S.. Bluths. R.M., Kelley, K.W. & Dantzer. R. ( 1992bi. Sickness behavior a'; a

neup target for drug development. Trends in Ph,umxolocicril Scisnce.1. 21-28.

Kent. S.. Bret-Dibar. J.L.. Kslley. K.\V. & Dantzsr. R . (19961. \Iechanisms of sickness

inducsd dscreases in food-motivatsd behwior. Ssurol;cisncs and Biokhaviorril

Rsviews. 20. I7 1 - 175.

Klir. J.J.. McClellrin. J.L.. Kozak. IV.. Szrlen>i. Z.. Wong. G.H.W. & Kluger. M. (1995 i. Systemic but not central administration of tuninr necrosis factor-a attcnulitcs

LPS-induced fsvsr in rats. Americnn Journ;il of Phvsiolorr\., 268. RIXO- R G 6 .

Klir, J.J.. Roth. J.. Szelenyi. S.. klcClsllrin. J.L. 6r Klusrr. M. t 1993 1 . Rolr of

h>~pothrilamic interleukin-6 and tumor necrnsis factor-a in LPS fevsr in rat. Amsrican

Journal of Phvsiologv. 265. RZ 12-RS 17.

Kuriyrima. K.. Hori. T.. Mon. T., & Nakashima. T. ( 1990). Actions of Interferon-a and

inrerleukin- 1 on the glucose-sensitive neurons in the vsnrromsdial hyothalanius.

Brai n Resexch Bulletin. 21. 803-8 10.

Lacosra. S.. e l . 2.. Zalcman. S. & Anisman. H. t 1995 1. Tirnedependent in vivo

mcsolimhic dopamine vxiations following antigsnic challensr.. Bnin Rssecirch. 664.

225-230.

Langhans, W., Hariacher. R. & Scharrer, E. ( 1989). Veraparnil and indomerhacin

attenuate endotoxin-induced anorexia. Phvsioloc.,~ and Behavior. 46, 535 -539.

Langhans. W.. Savoldelli. D & Weinganen, S. (1993). Cornparison of the feeding responses to bacrerial lipopolysaccharide and interleukin- l p. Phvsiolo~v and

Behavior, $3, 633-649.

Lavicky, J. Br Dunn, A.J. (1995). Endotoxin administration srimdates cerebrai

catecholamine release in fieeiy moving rats as assessed by microdiai ysis. Journal of Xeuroscience Research. 40, 407-4 1 3.

Laye. S.. Bluthe. R N . Kent. S., Combe. C.. Medina, C.. Parnet. P.. Kelley. K. &

Dantzer. R. ( 1995). Subdiaphragmatic vagotomy blocks induction of IL- 1 P mRSA in

mice tirain in response to peripheral LPS. ..lmerican Journal of Physioloo\.. 268.

Rl327-Rl-331.

Long. S C . 54onmoto. .4.. Nakamori. T. 8: I\.lurdkami. S. ( lC)!Z). Systsmic injection of TSF-a attenuates fever due ro IL-IP and LPS in rats. Amencan Journal of

Physiolorzv. 263. R9X7-R99 1.

Long. S.C.. Ottsrness. 1.. Kunkel. S.L.. Vander. A . J . tk KIuger. b1.J. ( 1990 ). Roles of interleukin 1 p and rumor necrosis factor in lipopolysacchlirid fever in rata. Americm

Journal of P h y s m v . 259. R72-I-R718.

Madden. K.S. 8r Livnat. S. ( 199 1 ). Carecholamine action and immunologie reactivity. In

R. Ader. D.L. Feltsn and K. Cohen (Eds. i. Psvchons~iroirnrnunoIo~t~. San Diego:

Academic Press. pp.283-3 10.

hlarquette, C.. Van Dam. A.M.. Van Rooijen. S.. Berkenbosch. F. 8: Haour. F. < 19941.

Penpheral macrophage depletion prevents down regularion of cenrd interleukin- l

receptors in mice after endotoxin challenge. P s ~ ~ c h o n e ~ i r ~ n d o c n n o l o ~ ~ . 19. 1 89- 196.

Marta. S.G.. Weatherbee. J. 8: Sharp, B.M. (1991). A cenûal niechanism is involved in

the secretion of .ACTH in response to IL-6 in rats: Cornplirison to and interaction uith IL- I P. Seiirosndocnnoloev. 56. 5 1 6-525.

McCanhy, D.O., Kluger, M.J. & Vander, A. (1986). Effect of centraily administered

interleukin- 1 and endotoxin on food intake of fasted rats. Phvsiolo-v 8; Behavior. 36, 745-749.

McClellan, J.L., Klir, J.J., Morrow. L.E. & Kluger, M.I. (19%). Central effects of

~lucoconicoid receptor antagonist RU- 38486 on lipopol ysaccharide and Suess- C

induced fever. Amencan Journal of Phvsioloev. 267 . R7705-R7 1 1.

McLaughlin. C.L., Rogan, G.J., Tou. J., Baile. C.A. & Joy. W.D. ( 1992). Food intake and body temperature responses of rats to recombinant human interleukin- l g and a

mpeptide interleukin- 1P antagonist. Phvsiology and Behavior. 52. 1 135- 1 160.

McIntosh. J.. Kent. P. &: Merali. 2. (1996). Microdialysis reveüls thrtt both food ingestion

and stress enhance CRF release at the centnl arnygdaloid nucleu,. Socictv for

Seuroscisnce Abstracts. 22, 1408

XlcIntosh. J . & Merali, 2. ( 1996). 1mrnobiliz;ition stress is associated with increaszd

reienss of CRF and bomksin-iike peptides at the cenrnl nucleus of the amygdrila.

Summer Seu ro~pr ide Con ference. Edganown. Jlass.

Mefford. 1.5.. Masters. C.F., Heyes. M.P.. & Eskriy. R.L. t 199 1 1. Cytokine-inducsd

activation of the neurwndocrine stress asis psrsists in sndoto\in-rolcrant mice. Brriin

Research, 347. 337-330.

,Melk Parsadmianrz, S.M.. Levin. K.. Lenoir. V.. Roberts. J.L. & Kerdelhue. B. ( i991r.

Human interleuh 1P: Conicotropin releasing factor and .ACTH relrasr and gsne

expression in the mals rat: In vivo and in vitro studics. Journal of Scurosciencc

Resexch, 77. 675-682.

Merdi. 2.. Lacosta. S. & Anisman, H. (1997). Effects of IL-iP and rnild stress on

ai teration of norepinephrine. dopamine and serotonin neurotran srnision: .4 resjond

microdirilysis study. Bnin Research. in press.

Michie. H.R.. Sherman. M.L., Spnggs, D.R., Rounds. J.. Christie, M. & Wilmore, D.

( 1989). Chronic TXF infusion causes anorexia but not accelerateù ni trogen loss.

Annals of Surggrv. - - 209, 19-24.

Morrow, L.E.. McClellan. I.J.. Conn, C.A. & Kluger, M.J. (1993). Gl~cocorticoids alter

fever and IL-6 responses to psychological stress and ro lipopolysaccharide. Amencan

Journal of Phvsiology. 264 . R1010-R1016. Munck. A.. Guyre. P.M. & Holbrook. N.J. (1984). Physiological functions of

glucoconicoids in stress and their relation to pharmacological actions. Endoc.ne

Reviews. 5. 25-31.

Mycrs. R.D.. Lopez-Valpuesta, F.J.. Minano. F.J.. Wooten. M.H.. Barwick. V.S. Br

Wolpe. S.D. f 1991). Fever and feeding ir: the rx Actions of inrrahypothdamic

intsrlsukin-6 compared to macrophage inflamniritory protein- 1 P ( MIP- 1 b, Journal of

Neuroscisnce Research.3, 3 1-37.

S1yxs. C.A. % \.'dentins. A.D. ( 1995). Seurologicid and psychi:irric. ad\ m e sffsct:. of

imn:uiiolo~ic:il therapy. C M Drim. 3. 56-68.

S . b - J.I.. hcrssn. L.L.. Gosdsn. 41.. Chrtpnian. D. br Huni. S. P. i 1983 i. Dohs-

dspsndcnr cffecrs of capsaicin on pr imq. sensory ncuro;;z in nsonntril rat. Jo~irnaI of

Ssiirciïçiencs.3. 799-406.

Oldsnhurg. H.S.X.. Rogy. M A . . Lazarus. D.D.. Van Zee. K.J. . Kscler. B .P..

Chizzonite. R A . Lowry. S.F. b: lrloldawr. L.L. ( 9 W 1 . Cachexia and the acutc-

pha5s protein responss in inflammation are rrgu1ate.d b' intsrlsubin-6. European

Joiirn;il of Immunoloov. 23. 1889- 1893.

O'Reilly. B.. \'ander. A . J . & Kluger. M.J. (1938). Effscrh of chronic infusion of

lipopol~srtccharide on food intake and body temperariirc of the rat. Physiolo,o\ and

Bshnvior. 42. 287-29 1.

Papp. 51.. Willncr. P. & Muscat. R. (199 1). An animal modd of cinhedonia: iitrenuxinn of

sucrose consumption and place prefsrence condi tioning by chrnn i i unprcdicrahlc niild

mess. P ~ ~ ~ i h o p h ; i ~ i ; i c 0 1 0 ~ \ ~ , 103. 255-259.

Pelleyrnounter. M.A., Cullen, M.J., Baker, M.B.. Hecht, R., Winters, D.. Boone. T. &

Collins. F. (1995). Effects of the obese gene product on body weight regulation in

ob/ob mice. Science. 269, 540-543.

Perlstein, R.S., Moughey, E.H., Jackson, W.E. 8( Neta, R. (1991). Interleukin-I and

interleukin-6 act synergistically to stimulate the release of adrenoconicoaopic hormone

in vivo. Lvm~hokine and Cvtokine Research. 10, 14 1 - 136.

Perlstein, R.S.. Whitnall, M.H., Abrams, J.S., Mougey, E.H. & Neta. R. (1993).

Synergistic roles of interleukin-6, interleukin- 1, and turnor necrosis factor in the

adrenoconicouopin response to bacterial lipopolysaccahande in vivo. Endocrinoloov,

132, 946-952. -

Pezrshki. G.. Pohl. T. &: Schobitz. B. ( 1996). Corticosterone conuols interlcukin- 1 P and

sicknesi; behavior in the nt. Journal of Seurwndocrinoloc~. 8 . 129 - 175.

Planiondon. H. 8: Slerdi. 2. (1997). Spontaneous mcal is associatsd with disrinct changes

in the endopenous levels of bombesin-li ke peptides and conicotropin releasing factor

i n spccific brain nuclei. Amencm Joiimnl of Phvsiolos\*. 271. R268-R271.

Plata-Salaman. C.R. ( 1988). Food intaks suppression h! immunornodulators.

Se~irosciencs Research Comm~inic;trions. 3. 159- 165.

Plata-Salaman. C.R. ( 199-1). Md patterns in response to th.: inuacerebrovcnaicular

adminicacltion of interlcukin- 1 P in rats. Phys ic i lo~- and Bshwior. 33. 727-733.

Plata-Salaman. C.R. (1995). Cytokines and feeding suppression: An integrarive vieu from

neurologie to molccular levels. Nutrition. 1 1 iSupp). 671-677.

Plata-Salaman. C R . (1996). Cytokine action in the nsn-ous sysrem at pathophysioiogicd

versus phmiacological concentrations. In Friedman iEd. ,. ND$. Druns of Abuse,

and rhe Sruroimmune .4xis. N.Y: Plenum pp. 19 1- 197.

Plan-Salaman. C.R. (1997). Anoresiri dunng acuts and chronic dissase: Relcvancs of

neurotrrinsniirrer-peptide-cytokine interactions. Su t r i tion. 1 3. 1 59 - 1 60.

Plata-Salaman. C.R. & Borkoski, J.P. (1993). Cenually administered bacterial

lipopolysaccharide depresses feeding in rats. Phamacolov Biochemisw and

Behavior. 46,787-79 1.

Plata-Salaman. C.R. & Ffrench-MuIlen, J.M.H. ( 1992). InuacerebroventricuIar

administration of a specific IL- 1 receptor antagonist blocks food and water intake suppression induced by interleukin- 1 P. Phvsioloov and Behavior. 5 1, i 277- 1179.

Plata-Salaman. C R . , Oomura. Y. & Kai, Y. (1988). Tumor necrosis factor and interleukin-lp: suppression of food intake by direct acrion in the cenual nervous

systsm. Brnin Research, 418. 106- 1 11.

Plata-Srilaman. C.R.. Sonti. G.. Borkoski. J.P.. Wilson. C.D.. 8: Ffiench-'tlullen.

J.M.H. ( 1996). Anorexia induced by chronic central 3dministration of cytokines 3

cstimnted pparhophysiologicaI concenrntions. Phvsiolorv and Behwior. 60. 867-875.

Rivier. C.. Chizzonite. R . & Vale. W. (1989,). In the mouse. the activation of the

h>.potha!;iniic-pit~1it;~'-adrenal axis by a lipopol~~sücc haridc c endotoxin 1 is medilitcd

through intcrlsukin- 1. Endocrinolow. 135. IR(W2405.

Rogers. R.C.. SlcTigue. D M . &: Hermann. G.E. t 19%). \ 'asil + control of digestion:

Slodu1:irion bu central neunl and penphsrdl endocrine factors. Seumscisncc cind

Bicibeh:i\.ior:il Review. 20. 57-66.

Rorhu-el!. S. 1.. 8: Luhcshi, G. ( 1991). Phmacolog'; of intsrleukin- 1 actions in the

brai n. A d w m s i n Pharmacoloov, 25. 1-20.

Srtpolsky. R.. Rivier. C.. Ymamoto, G.. Plotsky. P. 8: Valc. W. i 1987 i. Interlsuhn- 1

stimulate\ the sccretion of hypothalamic conicotropin-releasing fsctor. Science. 2-38.

Sarraf. P.. Frsderich. R.C., Turner, E M . , Ma, G.. Jaskowiak. S.T.. River D.J.. Flier.

J.S.. Loasll. B.B.. Fraker. D.L Br Alexander. H.R. ( 1997 ). Multiple cytokines and

acure inilmmlition raise mouse leptin levles: PotentiaI rolr in inflarnmatoq anoresia.

The Journal of Es~erimental Medicine. 185. 17 1 - 175.

Schobitz, B., De Kloet, E.R. & Holsboer, F. (1994). Gene expression and function of

interleukin 1, interleukin 6, and nimor necrosis factor in the brain. Pro-ss in

Neurobiology. 44, 397-432.

Schobitz, B.. Pezeshki, G., Pohl, T., Hernrnann, U., H e i ~ c h , P.C., Holsboer, F., &

Ruel, J.M.H.M. (1995). Soluble interleukin-6 (IL-6) receptor augments centrai effects

of IL-6 in vivo. FASEB Journal. 9, 659-664.

Schotanus, K., Tilders. F.J.H. & Berkenbosch, F. (1993). Hurnan recombinant

interleuh- 1 receptor antagonist prevents adrenocomcoaopin, but not interIeukin-6

responses to bacterial endotoxin in rats. Endocrinolog. 1 13, 246 1-2468.

Shapiro, R.E. Miselis, R.R. (1985). The central organization of the vagus nerve

innervating the stomach of the rat. The Journal of Cornnarative Ne'eurolom 238, 473-

488.

Shimornmura, Y.. Shimizu, H., Takahashi, M., Uehara, Y.. Negishi. M., Sato, Y..

Inukai. T.. Kobayashi. 1. & Kobayashi. S. (1990). Effects of peripheral

administration of recombinant interleukin-l on feeding behavior of the rat. Life

Sciences. 47, 21 85-2192.

Smith, E.M.. Meyer, W.J. & Blalock. J.E. (1982). Virus-induced comcosterooe in

hypoph ysecromized mice: A possible lymp hoid adrend axis. Science. 2 1 8, 1 3 1 1 -

Smith, G.P., Jerome. C.. Cushin, B.J., Eterno. R. & Simansky, K.J. (1981). Abdominai

vagotomy blocks the satiety effect of cholecy stokinin in the rat. Science. 2 1 3, 1036-

1037.

Smith, G.P., Jerome, C. B; Norgren, R. (1985). Afferent axons in abdominal vagus

mediate sanety effect of cholecystokinin in rats. American Journal of Phvsiolo-v. 219,

R638-631

Socher, S.H., Friedman, A. & Maninez, D. (1988). Recombinant hurnan nuiior necrosis

factor induces acute reductions in food intake and body weight in mice. Journal of

Exmrimental Medicine. 167, 1957- 1962.

Sonti, G., Iiyin, S.E., & Plata-Salaman, C. (1996). Anorexia induced by cytokine

interactions at pathophysiologicai concenuations. American Journal of P h v s i d o ~

m, R1394-1402.

South, E.H., & Ritter, R.C. (1988). Capsaicin application to cenaal or penpheral vagal

fibres attenuates CCK satiety. Peptides. 9, 60 1-6 12.

Suack, A.M., Sebastian, R.J., Schwartz, M.W. & Daliman, M.F. (1995). Glucoconicoids and insulin: Reciprocal signals for energy balance. Amencan Journal

gf Phvsioloav. 268, R142-RI49

Suzuki, T., Funada, M., Sugano, Y., Misawa, M., Okutomi, T., Soma, G. & Mizuno. D.

(1994). Effects of lipopolysaccharide fiom pantoea agglomerans on die cocaine-

induced place preference. Life Sciences. 51, 75-80.

Swiergiel, A.H., Smagin. G. K. dk Dunn, A.J. (1997). Muenza virus infection of mice

induces anorexia: Cornparison with endotoxin and interleukin-1 and the effects of

indomethacin. Pharmacoloov. Biochemisw and Behavior. 57, 389-396.

Tilders, J.J.H., DeRijk, R.H., Van Dam, AM., Vincent, V.A.M., Schotanus, K. &

Persoons, J.H.A. (1993). Activation of the h>pothalmus-piniitary-adrenal axis by

bacterial endotoxins: Routes and intermediate signals. Psvchoneuroendocrinolorn.. 19.

209-232.

Tracey, KJ., Wei, H., hlanogue, K R . , Fong, Y., Hesse, D.G., Nguyen, H.T., Kuo.

G.C., Beutler, B., Cotran, R.S., Cerami, A. 8; Lowy, S.F. (1988). Cachectin/tumor

necrosis factor induces cachexia, anemia, and inflammation. Journal of Ex~enrnental

Medicine. 167, 12 1 1 - 122%

Turnbull, A.V. & Rivier, C. (1995). Replation of the HPA axis by cytokines. Brain, Behavior and Immuniry. 9, 253-775.

Van

Van

Van

Dam, A.M., Brouns, M., Louisse, S. & Berkenbosch. F. (1992). Appearance of

inleukin-1 in macrophages and in ramified microglia in the brain of endotoxin-ueated

rats: a pathway for the induction of non-specific symptoms of sickness? Brain

Research. 588, 29 1-296.

der Meer. M.J.M., Sweep, C.G.J.F.. Pesman, G.J.. Bonn. G.F.. & Herrnus. R.M.M. (1995). Synergism between IL-1P and TNF-a on the activity of the

pituitary-adrenal axis and on food intake of rats. Amencan Journal of Physiologu,

Haasteren, G.A.C., Van der Meer, M.J.M., Hennus. A.R.M.M.. Linkels. E.. Klootwijk. W.. Kaptein. E., Van Toor. H.. Swsep. C.G.J.. Visser. T.J. &: Ik Greef. W .J. ( 1994). Different effects of continuous infusion of intsrlsukin- 1 and

interleukin-6 on the hypothalamic-hypophysial-thymid axis. Endocrinolos-. 135.

1336- 1745.

Vo@. S.S.. Henricson. B.E. 8: Seta. R. (1991 ). Roles of interleukin-1 and tumor

necrosis factor in lipopol ysaccharide-induced hypo$yccmiri. Brain. Behwior and

Tmmunitv. 59, 2494-2398.

Wan. W.. Wetmore. L.. Sorensen. C.52.. Grcsnhcrg. A H . & h n c r . D.41. ( 199-1,.

Seural and biochsmical mediators of endotoxin and srrrsii-induced d o s expression in

the rar bnin. B n i n Research BuIlsrin. 34. 7- 14.

Watkins. L.R.. Goehler. L.E.. Relton. J.K.. Tartaglia. S.. Silherr. L.. Manin . D. 6.

h i e r . S. F. ( 1994). Blockade of inrerlsukin- 1 inducd h>perthennia hy

subdiaphrqmatic vagotorny: evidence for vap l niedirition of immunebr in

communication. Yeuroscience Letrers, 1 83. 27-3 1 .

Watkins. L.R.. Maier. S.F. & Goehler. L.E. i 19952 i. Cyrokine-to-hin communication:

A rrvicu 8: analysis of alternative rnechmisms. Life Sciences. 57. 1 0 1 1 - 1026.

Watkins. L.R.. Maier. S.F. & Goehler, L.E. (1995b). Immune activation: the rolr of pro-

inflnmrnrito- cytokines in inflammation, illness rrsponses and pothological pain

States. Pain. 67. 289-303.

Wehrenberg. W.B.. Saudan, P.. Corder. R., Voltz, D.M., Glustina, -4. & Gaillard. R.C. (1991). Acute and chronic effects of systemic adrninisnatîon of interleukin-1 p and

endotoxin on the hypothalamic-pituitary-&na1 axis in the rat. Endocrine Journal. 2.

273-277.

Willner, P. ( 1985 ). k ~ r e s s i o n : A ~svchobiolo~ical svnthesig S.Y .: Wiley

Willner, P.. Towell, A.. Sarnpson, D., Sophokleous, S. & Muscat. R. (1987). Reduction

of sucrose preference by chronic unpredictable rnild stress. and its restoration by a

nicyclic antidepressant. Psvcho~harmacolo~v. s, 358 -361.

Yang. 2.. Koseki, M.. Meguid. M. M.. Gleason. J.R.. & &bonis D. (l9Wi. Synergistiç effsct of rhTSF-a and rhlL- la in inducing anorexia in rats. .-\mericrin Journal of

Phvsiolorv. - - 267, R 1056-R 1064.

Yang. Z.. Ratro. C.. Gleason, J.R., Bellatone. R.. Crucitti. F. 8: hleguid. 5.1.M < 1992 >.

Influence of anterior subdiaphragmsric vagotom). and TPS on cri feeding behlivior.

Ph~ciolocv and Behxior. 5 1 . 9 19-926.

Yirniiya. R . i 1990). Endoiosin produces a depressive-iike cpisodc i n rm. Brain Rescrirch,

711. 167- 174.

Zal~man. S.. Green-Johnson. J.M.. .12urray. L.. Sance. D.M.. Dyck. D.. .Anisman. H. 6: Grssnkrg . .A. t 1 994 1. Cy tokine-specific central monoaniine alterririons induccd by

inrsrleukin i ILI- 1 , IL-? and IL-6. Bmin Reseiirch. 63.3. 40-Jc).

Zrtlcnun. S.. Shanks, X 8: Anisman, H. (1991 j .

norepinephrine and dopamine following antigen

69-76.

Zanetri. G.. Hsusm;inn, D.. Gerain, i., Kohlsr, f .. Abbet. P.. Barras. C.. Lucri>. R .

Glriuser. 51 % Baumganner. J. (1992). Cytokine production dter intriivenous or

pcritnnccil gram negative bacterial challenge in micc. The Journal of Immunoloev. 6 .

1890- 1 S W .

Zhou, D., Shanks, N., Riechman, S.E., Liang, R., Kusnecov, A.W. & Rabin, B.S . ( 1996). lnterleukin 6 modulates interleukin- 1 and stress-induced activation of the hypodialamic-pimituy-adrend axis in male rats. Neuroendocrinolo~. 63, 227-236.

Appendix A

Experiment 8

In order to further investigate the involvement of vagal afferent fibres in the

mediaaon of the behaviord and endocrine effects following LPS-indu& immune

activation, capsaicin was used to eliminate afferent fibres in neonatal CD4 rnice. As

indicated earlier, subdiaphragmatic vagotomy severs both afferent and efferent vagal fibers.

Thus, the effects of this neamnt on LPS-induced immune or endocrine variations may

refelect the actions of the endotoxin on messages to the CNS or, altematively, the effects of

negative fedback mechanisms on endocrine or behwioral processes (Tiiders et al.. 1993).

L.nlike vagotomy, neonatd treatment with capsaicin has k e n s h o w to selectively eliminate

afferent vagal fibers, while sparing efferent fibers (Holzer. 1991). Thus. effects of this

matment on LPS-induced immune, endocrine or behaviod effects cm be ascribec? more

direclly to the interference of messages from the penpheral to central nervous system.

Experimenr 8 was conductrd to assess the effects of capsaicin-elicited vagal disturbances

on the behavioral and hormonal changes exened by LPS ûeatment.

Materials and hiethods

Suhiects

Teonatal male CD-I rnice fiom an in-house breeding colony were used. Mice were

injectrd subcutaneously with capsaicin at 2 days of age and then retumed to their mother.

Follouing weaning (at 21 days) mice were separated into groups of 4 and housed under the

conditions previousl y described. At 60 days of age mice were individually housed for one

week prior to tesring.

Dni ps

Capsaicin was suspended in 108 Tween 80, 90% distilled water and 10% ethanol

i9YZ). Mice were administered an acute 50 mgkg subcutaneous injection of capsaicin or

vehicle (Tween 80, distilled water. 95% ethanol). This procedure, which has been used by

Dr. 2. Merali, has k e n shown to eliminate up to 95% of vagal afferent fibres (Nagy.

Iversen, Goeden, Chapman, & Hunt, 1983).

Method

At 60 days of age mice in the capsaicin and vehicle groups (n=12 per group) were

separately housed for one week pnor to k i n g trained in the chocolate miUr paradigm, and

as in Experiment 1, tested follouing mtment with LPS or saline. On the day of the test.

mice were aven a single intraperitoneai injection of 0.5 pg LPS or saline 90 minutes pnor

to receiving rnilk, and consumption was measured and compared to baseline levels as

previously described. Consumption was measured again 21 hours following the test period

in order to determine rate of recovery. In a parallel study the effects of capsaicin on the

plasma corticosierone changes elicited by the endotoxin were assessed. The dose of LPS,

based on Experiment 1, was one that produced submâuirnal effects on behavior and

hormone factors. thus precluding the possibilit y of a lack of effect owing to a dose that was

too high.

Results and Discussion

Yariations in consumption of a palatable food

Analysis of variance of chocolate milk consumption indicated that rhere was a

significant LPS x Sampling period interaction F(2,88)=11.082, pc.01, as well as a

significant main effect of LPS marnent F(1,41)= 10.9 16, pc.0 1. In contrast, neirher the

main effect nor any of the interactions involving capsaicin treatment reached statistical

significance. As seen in Figure 7, in both vehicle- and capsaicin-ûeated p u p s there was

a significant decline in consumption of chocolate milk, relative to control animals,

following a single i.p. injection of 0.5 pg of LPS 90 minutes pnor to testing. In both

capsaicin- and vehicle-mated groups, the rate of consumption of control mice was similar

across sampling periods. Post-hoc multiple comparisons, in fact, confmed that there was

no difference in the reduccion of rnilk consumption by capsaicin-treated mice that received

Figure 7. Mean (?s.E.M.) consumption (in mls) of a highly palatable food by

capsaicin- and vehicle-neated mice following inwperitoneal administration of

lipopolysaccharide (LPS) or saline. Neenatal CD-1 mice were rreated with subcutaneous

capsaicin or vehicle at 2 days of age. At 60 days of age both groups of mice were injected

with 0.5 pg LPS or saline, and consumption of chocolate milk was measured over a 1 hr.

pend comrnencing 90 minutes following injection. Baseline values were calculated by

averaging consumption over the three day period immediarely prior to testing. Recovery

datawere collected 24 hr. following testing.

I

Baseline 6

Test Recovery

Figure 8. Mean ( ? s.E.M.) plasma romcosterone concentrations (pg/dl) in capsaicin- or

vehicle-treated mice receiving inwperitoneal lipopolysaccharide (LPS) or saline. Seo-natal

male CD-1 mice were ueated with subcutaneous capsaicin or vehicle at 7 days of age. At 60

days of age b t h groups of rnice were injected with 0.5 pg LPS or saline and plasma

cortjcostemne concennations were measured 2 hr later.

Appendix B

ANOVA S ummary Tables for Experimen t 1

Experirnent 1 : Lipopolysaccharide (LPS) Dose Response Curve (chocolate milk consump tion)

Two-Factor repeated Measures AVOVA

Source df Sum of Squares hlean Square F-test pvalue

Dose (A) 3 12.726 4.242 1.37 -2674 Subj. W. Groups 36 1 1 1.446 3.096 Days (B) 2 18.649 9.324 27.155 .O00 1 AB 6 14.07 2,354 6.829 .O001 Enor 72 24.723 -343

LPS Dose Response Curve (plasma conicosrerone concenaanons)

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test p-value

Dose (A) 2 3 154.662 1577.33 1 47.763 .O00 1 Error 21 693.501 3 3 .O24 To ta1 23 3848.162

Appendix C

ANOVA Surnmary Tables for Experiment 2

Experiment 2: Interleukin- 1 (IL-1) Dose Response Curve (chocolate milk consumption)

Two-factor Repeated Measures ANOVA

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test

Dose (A) 5 18.73 1 3.746 1.169 Subj. W. Groups 51 173.005 3.204 Days (BI - 7 27.321 13.661 38.495 AB 10 19.286 1.929 5.435 Error 108 38.326 ,355

p-value

IL- 1 Dose Response Curve (plasma corticosterone concentrations)

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test p-value

Dose (A) 3 1439.669 379.89 10.3 .O00 1 Error 36 1677.333 46.593 Total 39 31 17.002

Appendix D

ANOVA Summary Tables for Experiment 3

Experiment 3: Tumor necrosis factor-alpha (ThTF-a) Dose Response Curve (chocolate mi& consumption)

Two-factor Repeated Measures ANOVA

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test p-value

Dose (A) 3 3.299 1.1 .42 -74 Subj. W. Groups 36 94.341 2.62 1 Days (B) - 3 1 1.222 5.61 1 19.082 . O00 1 AB 6 10.547 1.758 5.978 .O00 1 Error 72 21.171 2 9 4

TSFa Dose Response Curve (plasma corticosterone concentrations)

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test

Dose (A) 3 1030.2 15 343 -405 16.087 Error 36 768.48 2 1.237 To ta1 39 1798.695

p-value

. O00 1

Appendix E

ANOVA Summary Tables for Experiment 4

Experiment 1: Interleukin-6 (IL-6) Dose Response Curve (chocolate milk consumprion)

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test p-value

Dose (A) 4 18.60 1 4.65 1.678 -1717 Subj. W. Groups 45 124.7 1 3 2.77 1 Days (B j 2 2.103 1.05 1 4.098 ,0198 AB 8 2.321 2 9 1.131 .3506 Error 90 23.088 -257

IL-6 Dose Rssponse C w e (plasma conicosterone concenaations)

One- f x tor ASOVA

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test

Dose i.4) - 3 43.022 23.0 1 1 3 3 5 Error 2 1 864.34 41.164 Total 23 908.367

p-value

S936

ANOVA S ummary Tables for Experiment 5

Experiment 5: Interleukin- 1 (IL- 1) + Interleukin-6 (IL-6) Ceadminismtion (chocolate rnilk consumption)

Three-factor Repeated Measures ANOVA

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test

S N L - 1 (A) 1 Sd/IL-6 (B) 1 AEl 1 Subj. W. Groups 36 Samplingperiod(C) 2 AC 2 BC 2 mc - 7 C x subj W. Groups 73

p-vaiue

IL- 1 + IL-6 Co-administration (plasma corticosterone concentrations)

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test p-vaiue

IL-1 (A) IL-6 (B) AB Error

Appendix G

ANOVA Surnrnary Tables for Experirnent 6

Experirnent 6: Interleukin-6 (IL-6) + Tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-a) C e adminismtion (c hocolate milk consumpaon)

Three-factor Repeated Measures ANOVA

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test

SaML-6 (A) 1 SaVTSF- a (B) 1 AB 1 Subj. W. Groups 36 Sampling period (C) 2 AC - 3

BC - 7 ABC 2 C x suhj W. Groups 72

IL-6 i TSF- a Co-administration (plasma corticosterone concenûations)

T w - factor .ASOVA

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test

IL-6 (.A i I 604.506 604.506 19.795 TSF- a ( B j 1 15.055 15.055 -493 AB 1 61.951 61.95 1 2.029 Error 36 1099.359 30.538

pvalue

p-value

Appendix H

ANOVA Sumrnary Tables for Experiment 7

Experiment 7: Interleukin- 1 (IL-1) + Tumor necrosis factor-alpha (R'JF- a) Co- adminis~ation (chocolate ndk consumption)

Three- factor Repeated Measures ANOV A

Source df Sum of Squares

Sal/ TNF- a (B) 1 3.37 AB 1 .O3 8 Subj. W. Groups 36 74.231 SampIing penod (C) 2 4.455 AC - 7 6.236 BC 2 12.638 ABC - 7 3.601 C x subj W. Groups 72 25.016

Mean Square F-test

IL- 1 + D7- a Co-administration (plasma corticosterone concenûations)

Two-factor ASOVA

Source d f Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test

IL-1 (A) 1 969.23 969.24 21.126 TW- a (B) 1 330.365 440.365 9.958 AB 1 183.783 183.784 4.006 Error 36 165 1.63 45.879

p-value

p-value

Appendix I

ANOVA Summary Tables for Experiment 8

Experiment 8: Effect of nw-natal capsaicin treaanent on response to Lipopolysaccharide (LPS) administration (chocolate rnilk consumption)

Three-factor Repeated Measures ANOVA

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test p-value

VeNCap (A) SU LPS (B) AB Subj. W. Groups ~ampling period (C) AC BC ABC C x subj W. Groups

Effect of neo-natal capsaicin treatrnent on response to LPS administration (plasma comcosterone concenuations)

Two- factor AYOVA

Source df Sum of Squares Mean Square F-test p-value

VeNCap (A) 1 17,541 17.541 -4 13 S296 SaVLPS (B) 1 3432.968 3432.986 80.802 .O00 1 AB 1 .O03 .O03 .O0006 .9938 Error 16 679.78 42.486

IMAGE EVALUATION TEST TARGET (QA-3)

APPLlE O i IMAGE. lnc - = 1653 East Main Street - -. - Rochester, NY 14609 USA -- --- Phone: 716/482.0300 -- -- - - Fw: 71 6/288-5989

O 1993. Appiied Image. Inc.. All Rights Resewed


Recommended