+ All Categories
Home > Documents > The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

Date post: 28-Jan-2016
Category:
Upload: inara
View: 19 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
Description:
The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance. Peter Q. Pfordresher Department of Psychology Auditory Perception and Action Lab. Overview. Memory and planning Timing in performance Feedback in performance Musical deficits: The case of “bad” singing. 1. 2. 3. 4. Memory and Planning. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation
15
The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance Peter Q. Pfordresher Department of Psychology Auditory Perception and Action Lab
Transcript
Page 1: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

The Cognitive Bases of Music PerformancePeter Q. PfordresherDepartment of Psychology

Auditory Perception and Action Lab

Page 2: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

Overview

Memory and planning Timing in performance Feedback in performance Musical deficits: The case of “bad”

singing

Page 3: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

Memory and Planning

Errors and “what’s on your mind?”Freud’s best contribution!Lashley (1951): Errors suggest

hierarchical, not serial, organization

1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4

Page 4: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

Memory and planning

Serial ordering errorsTarget vs. intruder

Target/intruder relationshipsDistanceDirection

• Anticipation• Perseveration• Exchange

Examples

“But barkling water is bad for you” (intended: sparkling)Vousden et al., 2000

PerformedPerformed

IntendedIntended

Page 5: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

Memory and Planning

Errors constrained by structureRemain within a melodic line (Plamer & van

De Sande, 1993; Palmer, 1996)

Stay within a musical phrase (P&vDs, 1995)

Directional characteristics of planningAnticipations = thinking aheadMore anticipations = fewer errors (e.g.,

Drake & Palmer, 2000; Dell et al., 1997)

Faster tempo = fewer anticipations (Drake & Palmer, 2000; but not Palmer & Pfordresher, 2003)

Page 6: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

Memory and Planning Planning and distance

Greater distance for adultsGreater distance for slower tempi

The range model (Palmer & Pfordresher, 2003;

Pfordresher et al., 2006). Distance results fromSerial proximity Metrical similarity

(Serial proximity) (Metrical similarity)

Page 7: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

The range model

Even

t A

cti

vati

on

Level 2 X X X X X XLevel 1 X X X X X X X X X X X X

Current (planned) eventMetrical Grid

Serial = “tapering off” from currentMetrical = “up/down” pattern

Y = S M 1m m

m mx,i x x i

xta

,

i i x

i i x

Page 8: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

Timing Maintaining regularity: Two sources of

variability (Wing & Kristofferson, 1973):

Expressive timingPresent even in “deadpan” performances

(Palmer, 1989)Associated with structure (Todd, 1985)

Association with movement? (Sundberg & Verillo, 1999)

Relational invariance? (e.g., Repp, 1998) Problems:Ornaments (Desain & Honig, 1994)

“Swing ratios”

Page 9: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

Perceptual feedback Focus mostly on auditory

Altered auditory feedback What is necessary?

Presence of feedback?• Facilitates memory, but not necessary• Absence doesn’t disrupt piano (Repp, 1999)

• Though more important for singing

Timing of feedback? IMPORTANT• Disruption varies with delay amount• Probably function of rhythm

Page 10: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

Delayed auditory feedback

% IO

I diff

ere

nce

Absolute time:Gates et al. (1974)

Delay as % of IOI

-20

0

20

40

60

80

0% 50% 100% 150% 200%

IRI D

iffe

ren

ce +/- 1 SE

Relative time:Pfordresher & Benitez (2007)

Page 11: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

Perceptual feedback

Feedback contents? More complexRandom pitch sequences: no

disruption (Finney, 1997)

Serial shifts do disrupt (Pfordresher, 2005)

• Even when shift is a “variation” (Pfordresher, in press)

What is the role of feedback?NOT “feedback”!!!Rather, perception and action share a

common “plan” (Pfordresher, 2006)

Page 12: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

A framework for auditory feedback (Pfordresher, 2006)

Page 13: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

Musical deficits:“Bad” singing

Nature of the deficitMistuned notes

• May be influenced by vocal range

Compress pitch intervals NOT: contour errorsSing faster than they should (Dalla Bella et al., 2007)

Page 14: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

“Bad” singing

What causes bad singing? Still a question…Tone deafness (literally)?

• Congenital Amusia (Peretz et al., 2002)

• BUT: evidence that bad singers are good listeners (Bradshaw & McHenry, 200; Dalla Bella et al., 2007; Pfordresher & Brown, 2007)

Motor control? Not likely either… How prevalent is bad singing?

Probably ~10% of populationTwice as prevalent as true “tone deafness”

Page 15: The Cognitive Bases of Music Performance

Thank YouStudents who helped:

Ohio state Danielle Brink Grant Baldwin

UB Ece Yildirim Jennifer Walsh Jenny O’Sullivan

UTSA Brian Benitez (MS, 2005) Erik M. Gallemore (MS, 2005) Julliann Canady Augusto Petacchi Danielle Maddock Zachary Clay

Funding sources:

National Science Foundation BCS-0344892 BCS-0624592 BCS-0704516San Antonio Life Sciences Institute Grant #121075UTSA Faculty Research Award, 2003The Grammy Foundation

And to many, many participants…


Recommended