+ All Categories
Home > Documents > The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

Date post: 10-Apr-2018
Category:
Upload: hamletquiron
View: 224 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 14

Transcript
  • 8/8/2019 The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

    1/14

    The Social-Emotional Processing Stream: Five CoreConstructs and Their Translational Potential forSchizophrenia and Beyond

    Kevin N. OchsnerBackground: Cognitive neuroscience approaches to translational research have made great strides toward understanding basic mecha-

    nisms of dysfunction and their relation to cognitive deficits, such as thought disorder in schizophrenia. The recent emergence of Social

    Cognitiveand AffectiveNeuroscience haspaved theway forsimilar progressto be made in explaining themechanisms underlying thesocial

    and emotional dysfunctions (i.e., negative symptoms) of schizophrenia and that characterize virtually all DSM Axis I and II disorders more

    broadly.

    Methods: This article aims to provide a roadmap for this work by distilling from the emerging literature on the neural bases of social and

    emotional abilities a set of key constructs that can be used to generate questions about the mechanisms of clinical dysfunction in general

    and schizophrenia in particular.

    Results: To achieve these aims, the first part of this article sketches a framework of five constructs that comprise a social-emotional

    processing stream. The secondpart considershow futurebasic research might flesh out this framework andtranslational work might relate

    it to schizophrenia and other clinical populations.

    Conclusions: Although the review suggests there is more basic research needed for each construct, two in particularone involving the

    bottom-uprecognitionof socialand emotionalcues,the secondinvolving theuse of top-down processesto draw mentalstateinferences

    are most ready for translational work.

    Key Words: Amygdala, cingulate cortex, cognitive neuroscience,

    emotion, prefrontal cortex, schizophrenia, social cognition, transla-

    tional research

    From time to time, we all fail to respond adaptively to lifeschallenges. For individuals with clinical disorders, however,these failures might be chronic and pervasive. An essential

    goal of behavioral and neuroscience research is to understand how

    and why this happens. One influential approach has been to usebasic cognitive neuroscience models to describe how and whenclinical symptoms arise from dysfunction in core mechanisms ofattention, memory, and other higher cognitive processes. Thistranslational approach has taken basic cognitive neurosciencemodels of prefrontal function and applied them to the study ofpositive symptoms in schizophrenia. This work has shown, forexample, that individuals with schizophrenia show disorder-specific behavioral deficits in maintaining task contexts that bothpredict thought disorder symptoms and resolve with treatment(14). This work has been less successful, however, in explain-ing the social and emotional dysfunctions that characterizenegative symptoms in schizophrenia and many DSM Axis I and IIdisorders more broadly (2).

    The rapid development of social cognitive and affectiveneuroscience (SCAN) as distinct disciplines (57) offers oppor-tunities for these kinds of translational bridges to be built. Theproliferation of new SCAN findings is both a blessing and cursefor basic and clinical neuroscientists, however. On one hand,new findings can provide material for building new kinds ofbridges (8,9). On the other hand, with the multiple approaches

    and methods this new work employs, it can be difficult to figureout how diverse pieces of data fit together into core neurofunc-tional constructs. Identifying these constructs is essential, be-cause our theoretical models of them determine what scientificquestions we ask about their basic nature and translationalpotential. Given that performance on behavioral measures ofsocial cognition and emotion might predict functional outcomesin schizophrenia (1015), the time is ripe for neuroscienceresearch to examine the brain systems underlying these abilitiesin schizophrenia and beyond.

    The overarching goals of this article are to distill a set of keyconstructs from the growing data on the neural bases of socialand emotional abilities that can be used to generate questionsabout the mechanisms underlying negative symptoms in schizo-phrenia and, by extension, clinical disorders of emotion moregenerally. Toward these ends this article has two parts. The firstbriefly sketches a framework in which five constructs comprise asocial-emotional processing stream. The second considers howfuture basic research might flesh out this framework and trans-lational work might relate it to the study of negative symptoms inschizophrenia and other clinical disorders. In this regard thearticle was motivated by the need to provide a framework for theCNTRICS (Cognitive Neuroscience for Treatment Research toImprove Cognition in Schizophrenia) initiative (2), which isconcerned with adapting measures from cognitive, social, andaffective neuroscience for use in clinical trials in schizophrenia.

    The Social-Emotional Processing Stream

    The basic premise of this framework is that, in many if not allcases, human social and emotional behaviors are highly inter-twined. Consider, for example, how a social cognitive or anaffective neuroscientist might study different aspects of a socialinteraction. The social cognitive neuroscientist might focus onhow each person draws inferences about the momentarythoughts and feelings of their interaction partner as well the

    From Columbia University, New York, New York.

    Address reprint requests to Kevin Ochsner, Ph.D., Department of Psychol-

    ogy, Columbia University, 369 Schermerhorn Hall, 1190 Amsterdam Av-

    enue, New York, NY 10027; E-mail:[email protected].

    Received January 29,2008;revised March 21,2008;acceptedApril21, 2008.

    BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:48 610006-3223/08/$34.00doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.024 2008 Society of Biological Psychiatry

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]
  • 8/8/2019 The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

    2/14

    partners enduring traits and tendencies. The affective neurosci-entist might focus on each persons emotional response, howeach person regulates it, and how each partner identifies eachothers emotional expressions. Although these social and emo-tional questions have historically been the province of differentdisciplines, are the phenomena of interest completely distinct?This review argues that they are not: how you assess theintentions (e.g., aggressive) and dispositions (not usually that

    way) of another person is part of the appraisal process thatassesses what emotion that person is expressing and determines

    your emotional response to the person (e.g. fear) as well as howyou might regulate that response (e.g., judging the aggression tobe circumstantial) (for discussion see 16).

    The common intertwining of social cognitive and affectivephenomena makes sense, given that many researchers believeemotions arise from appraisals of the goal relevance of a stimulusand that people are typically the most goal-relevant stimuli in ourdaily lives. This is not to say that we cannot experience emotionsin non-social contexts (e.g., disgust at trash) but rather that it isdifficult to have social interactions without emotion. This mightexplain why the paradigms used in social cognitive and affectiveneuroscience research are strikingly similar: ostensibly socialcognitive tasks often involve affective processes (including atti-tudes), and ostensibly affective tasks often use social stimuli (likefaces or social images). It might also explain why functionalimaging and lesions studies of social cognitive and affectivephenomena consistently implicate a common set of brain sys-tems (16).

    With this in mind, this article uses the term social-emotionalprocessing stream to refer to the set of psychological and neuralprocesses that encode socially and emotionally relevant inputs,represent their meaning, and guide responses to them. Thesections that follow sketch five core constructs that are the keyconstituents of this stream. Selection of these constructs wasguided by two factors. First, human and animal data had tosuggest that there are reliable neural correlates of the ability/construct in question. Second, theoretical models of socialcognition and emotion (1621) were used to guide grouping ofbehavioral phenomena under each construct.

    The end product is the heuristic model illustrated in Figure 1,

    where the term construct refers to categories of social cognitiveand affective abilities that are valid and distinct in so far as theyhave been tied to distinct but related sets of neural systems.These constructs lie along a rough hierarchy of processesengaged when we initially learn the value of a stimulus (Con-struct 1); subsequently re-encounter it and recognize its value(Construct 2); understand the beliefs and feelings of a personstimulusthat could be oneselfin a bottom-up, experiential

    (Construct 3) or top-down, attributional manner (Construct 4); ortry to regulate responses to a stimulus in a context appropriatemanner (Construct 5). Here, value refers to whether a givenstimulus is good or bad or should be approached or avoided,

    whereas response refers to the behaviors we measure asevidence that this value has been computed. Because currentdata do not allow us to clearly disentangle the neural correlatesof the valuation and response stages, these two terms are oftenused in combination here.

    Construct 1: Acquisition of Social-Affective Valuesand Responses

    The first construct concerns the universal need to learn whichstimuli and actionswhether social or non-sociallead to aver-

    sive as opposed to appetitive outcomes. For decades, acquisitionof social-affective values and responses has been studied insimple animal models of conditioning and reward learning thatonly recently have been extended to humans with functionalimaging and patient studies. Together, these data provide per-haps the strongest evidence for any of the proposed constructs.

    The two neural systems most strongly implicated in affectivelearningthe amygdala and striatumare evolutionarily oldsubcortical structures that receive multi-modal perceptual inputs

    Figure 2. Regions implicated primarily in ability/Construct 2 as well asConstructs 1 and 35. (A) Transparent view of right hemisphere showingsubcortical regions (amygdala and ventral striatum) involved in condition-ing and reward learning.Nearby structures(hippocampus and caudate) arealso shown to provide anatomical reference points. Dorsal and lateral re-gions implicated in the regulation of affective responses (Construct 4) alsoare shown here. (B) Medial view of left hemisphere showing cingulate andfrontal cortical regions described in the text. Mid (m) cingulate has beenimplicated in pain andpain empathy (Construct 3).Ventral (v)regions havebeenimplicatedin thecontextual aspectsof affectivelearning (Construct 2).Dorsal (d)/rostral (r) regions have been implicated in higher-level mentalstate inference and regulation (Constructs 4 and 5). (C) Transparent lateraland axial cut-out views of the insula, which is involved in representingsomatovisceral information involved in multiple constructs (25). See textfor details. PFC, prefrontal cortex.

    1. Acquisition of1. Acquisition of

    social-affectivesocial-affectivevalues andvalues andresponsesresponses

    2. Recognizing2. Recognizing

    and responding toand responding tosocial-affectivesocial-affective

    stimulistimuli

    3.3. EmbodiedEmbodied simulationsimulationor low-levelor low-level

    mental state inferencemental state inference

    4. High-level mental4. High-level mentalstate/trait inferencestate/trait inference

    5. Context-sensitive5. Context-sensitiveregulationregulation

    Mirror systemAction observation

    Pain empathy

    Conditioning

    Reward learning

    Nonverbal cuesBiological Motion

    PreferencesAttitudes

    Impression formationAttribute mental states/intentions

    Theory of mind

    ExtinctionReversal learning

    ChoiceReappraisal

    Figure 1. Diagrammatic illustration of the relationships between five pro-posed core abilities/constructs for social and emotional behavior. Exem-plars of each are listed underneath the box naming each ability/construct.For illustrative purposes a linear flow of information is shown between thesystems underlying each proposed ability/construct, although in principlethe systems underlying each construct might operate independently or invarious combinations. See text for details.

    K.N. Ochsner BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:48 61 49

    www.sobp.org/journal

  • 8/8/2019 The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

    3/14

    and are interconnected with autonomic control centers andneuromodulatory systems (2224) (Figure 2 A). Classically lim-bic regions such as the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) andinsula (see next section) also play key roles in affective learning

    via interconnection with the amygdala and striatum (Figures 2Band 2C).

    The amygdalas role in affective learning has been elaboratedprimarily with classical (aka Pavlovian) fear conditioning para-

    digms in which an initially neutral conditioned stimulus (CS;e.g., a tone) is repeatedly paired with an intrinsically aversiveunconditioned stimulus (UCS; e.g., a shock). Over time, the CScomes to elicit behavioral conditioned responses (CRs; e.g.,freezing) that might be similar to those initially elicited by theUCS. Elegant animal experiments have shown that the CS-UCSassociation involves interconnections between the basal andlateral amygdala nuclei and that the behavioral components ofthe CR depend on brainstem centers that receive projectionsfrom the basolateral complex via the central nucleus (25).Human imaging and lesion studies have confirmed the role of theamygdala in classical fear conditioning (e.g., 26,27) and haveextended animal work by showing that the amygdala is criticalfor acquiring conditioned fear responses to social stimuli that

    might act as CSs, such as faces or facial expressions of anger(28,29).

    The ventral portions of the striatum (VS) are critical forlearning which stimuli or behavioral responses predict rewardingor reinforcing outcomes (24). For example, with simple stimulus-reward association paradigms, single unit recording studies innonhuman primates have shown that the function of the VS is

    well-described by a simple learning model in which dopaminerelease enables VS neurons to encode the timing of an expectedreward, with release adjusted either upward or downward as afunction of whether that expectation is met (24). Human imagingstudies have corroborated this model by showing that VS activityincreases when a participant anticipates or receives an unex-pected monetary reward (30) and that it varies as a function of

    whether theyve been led to expect a reward that occurs at anunexpected time (31,32).

    Although the amygdala and VS play critical roles in learningwhich stimuli predict aversive and rewarding outcomes, respec-tively, these are not their only roles in acquiring affectiveresponses. For example, the amygdalas central nucleus mightplay a more general role in orienting attention to and encodinginto memory affectively salient stimuli, which might relate to itsrole in signaling when the reward-related value of a stimuluschanges (33) and consolidation of memories for affectivelyarousing experiences (34). Furthermore, interactions betweenthe amygdala and VS might be critical for learning more complexaffective associations (22,35,36).

    Information the amygdala and VS send to the medial portionsof the orbitofrontal and ventral mPFC (vmPFC) is important forrepresenting the affective valence of stimuli as it is updatedacross contexts (36,37). In animal studies, orbitofrontal cortex(OFC) neurons fire in response to various kinds of motivationallyrelevant stimuli and update this firing more rapidly than the VS oramygdala as stimulus-reward associations change (37). In likefashion, human functional imaging studies have shown that

    vmPFC and OFC might respond to both rewarding and aversiveoutcomes and are sensitive to changing reward values (3842).

    Taken together, extant evidence suggests that the amygdala, VS, and vmPFC/OFC form a circuit essential for encoding theaffective value of stimuli. One caveat to these data, however, isthat a few studies have shown directly that these structures are

    important for acquiring the affective value of social stimuli, perse. Perhaps the most salient example is a recent study showingthat the amygdala is essential for conditioned fear responsesacquired by observing others undergo the conditioning proce-dure (43). Given these data, the connectivity of these systems,and data described in the next section that these structuresrespond to nonverbal social cues (such as faces) whose affectivesignificance presumably has already been learned, it is safe to

    assume that simple affective learning systems are involved insocial learning. As learning becomes more social, howeverforexample involving drawing inferences about mental statesadditional structures such as dorsal regions of mPFC (see Con-struct 4) might also become important (43).

    Construct 2: Recognition of and Response toSocial-Affective Stimuli

    Once the social-affective value of a stimulus has been learned,it is important that an organism can quickly identify it in thefuture and respond appropriately. The systems important foraffective learning described in the preceding text and posteriorcortical regions involved in representing nonverbal cues areimportant for this ability.

    Perhaps the best-known finding in this domain is that theamygdala is critical for the recognition of stimuli that directly orindirectly signal the presence of a potential threat, such as thefaces of seemingly untrustworthy individuals (4446) and fearfulfacial expressions and the widened eyes and enlarged eye whitesthat uniquely characterize them (47,48). Topics of debate includethe extent to which task factors, levels of anxiety and depression,and genetic factors determine the magnitude and attentionalindependence of the amygdalas response to these fear cues(4956). Given that the amygdala also responds to novel andpositive stimuli and that it is sensitive to the configural meaningof specific eye gaze/facial expression combinations (57), somehave offered a broader conceptualization of the amygdala as asurveillance system that continuously monitors the environ-ment for affectively relevant stimuli and modulates activity inperceptual and memory systems to detect and encode them(58,59). On this view, ambiguous and novel stimuli are (poten-tially) relevant until an organism learns otherwise (59).

    The striatal and medial prefrontal systems described earlieralso have been implicated in recognizing stimuli whose valuethey encode. Thus, imaging studies have shown that the VS and

    ventromedial PFC respond to the faces of attractive people(60,61) or consumer goods that one would like to purchase (62),presumably because of their learned (or perhaps innate) reward

    value. Ventromedial PFC and nearby regions of the anteriorcingulate cortex also respond during like/dislike or preferencejudgments for various kinds of stimuli (6365), presumablybecause indicating with a key press that one likes a stimulus iseither an instance of expressing a preference one has alreadyacquired or is an instance of learning that one has this prefer-ence.

    Patterns of connectivity are also useful for understanding thefunctional roles in social-affective learning and recognition foreach of these regions. For example, the fact that the amygdalareceives multimodal perceptual inputs, including some that (atleast in rodents) might provide quick inputs that bypass thecortex, suggests that this structure might be well suited for a roleas a surveillance system. Another region whose pattern ofconnectivity might relate to its function is the insula, the corticalregion connecting the temporal and frontal lobes that liesbeneath the Sylvian Fissure (Figure 2C). The insula has been

    50 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:4861 K.N. Ochsner

    www.sobp.org/journal

  • 8/8/2019 The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

    4/14

    described as viscerotropic mapwith its posterior regions re-ceiving ascending somatosensory information, including painthat projects forward to anterior regions that are interconnected

    with frontal regions implicated in attention, control, and speecharticulation (6668). This mapping might explain why bothregions might be activated by pain but only the anterior has beenassociated with the experience and recognition of facial expres-sions of disgust (69,70), an emotion that involves the oral

    expulsion of potential contaminants. Although some have ar-gued that the anterior insula is critical for disgust (69), this hasbeen questioned, because it also responds to other aversive facialexpressions, memories, and images (71,72); might be activeduring classical conditioning (26); and is active (on the right)

    when one interoceptively detects ones own heartbeat (73).These data motivate the view that the anterior insula plays ageneral role in negative affective experience (72,74).

    Cortical regions around the superior temporal sulcus (STS)also play a role in the recognition of social/affective values. This

    was first identified in single unit recording studies in nonhumanprimates and has since been extended in numerous functionalimaging studies in humans. In human imaging studies, the STSresponds to a variety of nonverbal cues that might include

    images of moving eyes, lips, mouths, grasping movements, andabstract stimuli that depict biologically plausible motion (7577).The latter type of stimulus includes well-known point-light

    videos showing individuals walking, dancing, or engaging inother social or motivationally relevant actions (78). These regionslie just anterior to the temporal parietal junction (TPJ), which hasbeen implicated in controlling the focus of attention (79) as wellas in the representation of beliefs (80) (see Construct 4). Theclose proximity these regions might make sense, given that theperception of nonverbal cues, such as the direction of anotherpersons eye gaze, might automatically orient our attention in thedirection that person is looking (81). Exactly how these regionscommunicate with one another, however, and the extent to

    which they represent perceptual, attentional, or higher-level

    semantic information is currently a topic of debate (82).In sum, extant evidence strongly suggests that regions in-

    volved in learning the affective value of a stimulus also supportrecognition of it later on and that superior temporal regions areimportant for recognizing nonverbal social cues. Important ques-tions remain, however, about how best to characterize thefunction of these regions. For example, some characterize theaffective learning regions as having the specific function ofrecognizing a particular kind of stimulus (e.g., a fearful or disgustfacial expression) (69), whereas others characterize them interms of processes that are not domain-specific (e.g., 20,72).

    Construct 3: Embodied Simulation or Low-Level MentalState Inference

    There is more to understanding the meaning of a social-affective stimulus than simply being able to place it in theappropriate category as a fearful expression, a preferred product,or an attractive face. Indeed, theory and research suggests that,beyond such simple recognition judgments, the meaning or

    value of stimulus is embodied in our experience of it. In somesense, all of our experiences are embodiedwe inhabit physicalbodies that feel pain and whose palms sweat and musclescontract in the readiness for action. For present purposes, the keynotion is that these responses are important components not justof our own direct first-person experience but might be used asembodied simulations that help us vicariously understand theexperience of others as well (8385).

    Neuroscience data supporting this claim come from studiesasking whether the neural systems involved in the execution ofa motor act, the experience of pain or an emotion, also are active

    when a participant observes another person engaging in thatsame act or having the same kind of experience. As illustrated inFigure 3A, to the extent that common systems are involved, it hasbeen argued that the perception of others is supported by orshares the same representations that support first-person expe-rience (86,87).

    The first data of this sort came from single unit recordingstudies in nonhuman primates showing that approximately 25%of neurons in the ventral premotor and inferior parietal cortex(Figure 3B) would fire when the animal performed an action as

    well as when it observed the experimenter or another animalperforming an action with the same goal if not the identicalmeans of execution (e.g., a different means of grasping a cup)(88). These mirror neurons were interesting because theyseemed to encode the intention behind an action regardless of

    who performed it, and it was hypothesized that their activationcould provide the basis for understanding the intentions behindthe actions of another person. Human imaging research using theshared representation logic (Figure 3A) later provided for con-

    verging evidence for the existence of a similar human mirrorsystem (89,90), although individual mirror neurons have yet tobe observed directly.

    Subsequent studies extended this logic to other domainswhere the activation of shared representations has been hypoth-esized to provide a basis for empathy. For example, numerousstudies of pain empathy have shown the activation of tworegions that receive ascending nociceptive inputsthe mid-

    Regions involved inRegions involved in

    experiencing:experiencing:

    ActionAction

    PainPain

    EmotionEmotion

    Regions involvedRegions involved

    in observing:in observing:

    ActionAction

    PainPain

    EmotionEmotion

    SharedShared

    representationsrepresentations

    RostralInferiorParietal Ventral

    Premotor

    (BA 44)

    Mid

    Cingulate

    Insula

    C.C.

    D.D.

    B.B.A.A.

    Trans

    parent

    Trans

    parent

    Late

    ralV

    iew

    Late

    ralV

    iew

    MedialV

    iew

    MedialV

    iew

    Late

    ralV

    iew

    Late

    ralV

    iew

    Figure 3. The logic behind and regions implicated in studies of sharedrepresentations (see Construct 3). (A) Venn diagram showing the sharedrepresentation logic. The idea is that regions commonly activated duringthe first person experience of an action, pain, or emotion (left circle) mightalso be activatedwhen observingothers experiencing action, pain, or emo-

    tion (right circle). To the extent that these regions overlap, the informationrepresented there might play a dual or shared role in supporting the directexperiential understandingof onesown and others actions. (B) Semi-trans-parent lateral view of the right hemisphere showing inferior parietal andventral premotor regions implicated in the putative mirror system forprogramming and recognizing intentional actions. (C) Medial view of theleft hemisphere showing a mid cingulate region implicated in pain experi-ence and pain empathy. (D) Transparent lateral and axial pop-out views ofthe right hemisphere showing the insular cortex. Highlighted are anteriorregions of the insula implicated in pain experience, pain empathy, disgustexperience and disgust empathy, and negative affective experience moregenerally. See text for details. BA, Brodmann area.

    K.N. Ochsner BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:48 61 51

    www.sobp.org/journal

  • 8/8/2019 The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

    5/14

    region of the anterior cingulate cortex (Figure 3B) and theanterior insula (Figure 3D)when individuals directly experi-ence and when they observe others experiencing physical pain(9195). Similar findings were obtained in a study of disgust,

    which found that sniffing disgusting odors and watching othersdoing the same activated overlapping portions of the anteriorinsula (96) (Figures 2C and 3D).

    The assumption in all of these studies is that common activity

    in the parietal, premotor, cingulate, and/or insular corticesprovides the basis for the vicarious empathic experience andtherefore understanding of anothers actions, pain, or disgust.This is consistent with recent findings that individuals withautismwho exhibit gross impairments of social behaviormight show reduced activation in the prefrontal portions of theso-called mirror system (97). There are a couple of problems

    with this assumption, however. First, the regions in question havebeen activated by a variety of motor actions and/or affectiveexperiences, and at present it is not possible to determine whethercommon activity in the insula, for example, reflects the experienceof disgust, pain, or some other kind of negative affective experi-ence. Thus, it is possible that when I experience pain I am afraid,but when I see you experience pain I am disgusted. Second, to

    date, no studies have provided behavioral measures that could verify that direct and vicarious experience is similar or thatactivity in shared representation regions supports accuratejudgments about and understanding of another persons ex-perience. For example, it would be desirable to show thatactivity in ventral premotor cortex or the mid-cingulate cortexmight predict an individuals ability to accurately judge theintentions behind an action or the nature of their painfulexperience. Although analytic and behavioral methods havebeen and are being developed that can address these issues, theyhave not yet been applied to imaging studies of shared repre-sentations. In future work, measures of behavioral mimicry orcorrelations between ones self-reported experience and theexperience that others judge you are having could address this

    issue (95,98100).Finally, it is important to note that the shared representation

    logic might be used to study not just how we understand othersbut how we recognize the meaning of their actions and respondto them as well. This has been shown in studies of the neuralresponse to social rejection, which have shown that mid-cingu-late and insula regions implicated in pain also are active whenone experiences rejection (101). These data suggest that weunderstand what it means to be socially isolated in part byexperiencing what it would be like to be physically hurt (102).

    In sum, although extant data are consistent with the notionthat the activation of shared representations enable us to simulatethe experience of others, it is not yet clear when and how thesesimulations truly match the experience of others and enable us to

    accurately understand them. Another way of stating this is thatthe bottom-up, stimulus-driven activation of shared representa-tions might support the vicarious understanding of another per-sons experience, but the nature of that understanding remains to bedetermined (16).We do know that this understanding is low-level inthe sense that the supporting systems represent the experientialproperties of a stimulus rather than higher-level symbolic inter-pretations of it, which is considered in the next section.

    Construct 4: High-Level Mental State/Trait InferenceOne problem in interpreting the meaning of social stimuli is

    that they often are ambiguous. Take, for example, the image ofa smiling face commonly used in many studies of facial expres-

    sion recognition. The assumption is that the smile unambigu-ously communicates happiness. Anyone who has played cards orbought a used car knows that this is not the case, however, andthat the meaning of a smile is determined by the context in whichit is displayed. Importantly, it seems that in many cases therecognition and low-level motor and affective simulation pro-cesses described under Constructs 2 and 3 are insufficient forrepresenting these complex types of intentional mental states

    (103,104). To understand them, we must use higher-level (pos-sibly symbolic) representations of mental states to take intoaccount situational/contextual information that constrains themeaning of a social action.

    Perhaps the most well-studied example comes from studies oftheory of mind (TOM) that employ variants of the false belieftask. In this task participants read vignettes describing the actionsof a character who possesses a false belief about the state of the

    world (105,106). The participants task is to correctly assess thatbelief. Because this judgment cannot be made on the basis ofperceptual information, general knowledge about the physical

    world, or information that the participant herself knows to be thecurrent true state of affairs, it is often considered to be the besttest of individuals ability to represent the mental states of other

    people (107). This task was originally developed to assess thedeveloping childs capacity to understand and explain the be-havior of others in terms of internal mental states, such as theirbeliefs, desires, feelings, and goals. As the first task adapted tostudying mental state inference in human functional imagingresearch, numerous studies have since employed vignette oreven cartoon variants of it (105). In general, they have shownactivation of a network of regions including dorsal and rostralmPFC and adjacent paracingulate cortex, the posterior cingulate/precuneus, temporal-parietal junction, STS, and the temporalpole. Sometimes referred to as the mentalizing network (108)(Figure 4), portions of this networkbut most commonly themPFCalso have been activated during other tasks that presum-ably rely upon the ability to infer mental states. These include

    playing strategic games against a human opponent (109111),watching video clips of abstract shapes whose movements seemintentional (104,112,113), and forming or retrieving an impres-sion of a person from a photograph of their face (114,115). ThemPFC is the primary focus here because it is the most reliablyactivated across studies and the bulk of attention has beenfocused on unpacking its functional organization.

    Intriguingly, some of the same mPFC regions implicated inmental state inference have also been implicated in accessingand making judgments about ones own mental states andenduring traits. For example, judging your emotional response to

    Figure 4. Regions implicated in high-level mental state inference (seeConstruct 4). The shading of each oval indicates the degree to which ithasbeen commonly activatedacross multiple kinds of tasks that dependupon the ability to understand others behavior in terms of internalbeliefs, feelings, goals, and intentions. See text for details. MPFC, medialprefrontal cortex.

    52 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:4861 K.N. Ochsner

    www.sobp.org/journal

  • 8/8/2019 The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

    6/14

    a photograph activates regions of the mPFC also activated whenjudging the emotion of the people in that photo (116,117).Similarly, judging whether trait words describe you or close othermight also activate common and mPFC regions (118). These data,like the data on embodied simulation and low-level mental stateinference, suggest that some of the same processes used to makejudgments about the self are used to make judgments aboutothers. In this case, the processes are higher-level and involve

    the representation of belief states, which makes sense given thatthe mPFC is an integrative region that receives inputs from dorsallateral and parietal regions implicated in working memory andspatial attention, as well as orbitofrontal regions that representthe motivational value of a stimulus (119). Intriguingly, mPFCsends projections to autonomic and endocrine centers that mightenable current beliefs to influence visceromotor response chan-nels, such as heart rate and galvanic skin response (120,121).

    Self and other judgments might not depend upon entirelyoverlapping regions of the mPFC, however. Some experimentshave shown, for example, that there might be distinct mPFCsubregions associated with accessing information about self orother (114,117,118,122,123). Exactly how mPFC is organized

    with respect to making attributions about self or other is a current

    topic of debate. Some data suggest that ventral/perigenual asopposed to dorsal and rostral regions are more strongly associ-ated with judgments about self and others, respectively(114,122). Other data suggest, however, that there might be otherdimensions of organization related to self and other judgmentthat might explain this apparent difference (118,124). Consider,for example, that retrieving exemplars of affective categories(e.g., generating machete from the cue weapons) activatesdorsal and rostral and mPFC (125,126), whereas expressing apreference for a stimulus or receiving a reward or punishmenttends to activate more ventral regions of the mPFC (117). Thesedata suggest that dorsal/rostral mPFC might be important for theexplicit categorization of mental states (whether they are yourown or someone elses), whereas ventral mPFC provides a

    coarser representation of the motivational value of a stimulus thatcan guide action in the absence of explicit mental state attribu-tions (16,117).

    In sum, the mPFC clearly plays a key role in mental stateinference, although the specific contributions of individual sub-regions to this ability remains to be clarified. That being said, themPFC is by no means the only important component of aputative mentalizing network. Indeed, much recent attentionhas been focused on the roles of superior temporal regions inrepresenting nonverbal visual cues that might provide clues tothe intentions of others (see Construct 2) (75), of the TPJ inrepresenting beliefs (106), of the precuneus in self awareness(127), and the temporal pole in representing emotion knowledge(128). Unpacking the individual contributions to mental stateinference of each of these regionsand whether and how similarsystems are used for understanding ones own mental states asopposed to the mental states of otherswill be an importantfocus for future basic research (16,82).

    Finally, it is worth noting that research on Constructs 3 and 4are in many ways interrelated. Basic research on both constructshas been concerned with how we understand our own actionsand experiences as well as those of others and the nature of therelationship between them. They differ, however, in the kinds ofrepresentations under investigation. Work on low-level mentalstate inference focuses on perceptual, motor, visceral, andaffective representations that might support direct experientialunderstanding, whereas work on high-level mental state infer-

    ence focuses on more abstract, semantic, and categorical repre-sentations that might support a symbolic or descriptive under-standing of experience and action.

    Construct 5: Context-Sensitive RegulationThe final construct concerns the ability to regulate ones

    judgments about and behavior toward others in a contextappropriate manner. As illustrated in Figure 5, this regulatoryability manifests itself in at least three ways, with each form ofregulation differing in complexity and depending upon relatedbut distinct sets of underlying neural systems (21).

    The first can be termed description-based regulation be-cause it involves the use of mental state inference, language,memory, and selective attention to reinterpret or reappraise themeaning of a social-affective stimulus (129,130). For example,one might explicitly reappraise an initially insulting remark if onecould determine that it was in fact unintentionally hurtful. Here,one might use working memory to hold in mind a linguisticnarrative about the other persons mental stateswhile at thesame time directing attention to their facial expressions and bodymovements to verify that the remark was meant to be playful and

    withholding the pre-potent tendencies to interpret their action asaggressive and respond in kind. Reappraisal has been studied byasking participants to reinterpret the meaning of affectivelyarousing photographs or anxiety-provoking situations in waysthat either diminish or enhance their affective response (131). Byand large, this work has shown that reappraisal depends uponactivity in dorsal and lateral prefrontal regions implicated inlanguage, attention, memory, and response selection (oftencollectively referred to as cognitive control) as well as in mPFCregions implicated in mental state inference (for reviews see21,132). Activity in these control systems modulates activity inregions implicated in emotional responding, such as the amyg-dala or insula. Within these general constraints, the specificfrontal regions activated across studies have varied considerably,however, which might have to do with the variability in thespecific reappraisal strategies employed in each experiment(21,132). In addition, studies to date have focused primarily onnegative affect, and less attention has been paid to the questionof whether these same neural systems are used for regulatingpositive emotions or any single specific emotion.

    Figure 5. Diagrammatic illustration of three types of context-appropriateregulation (see Construct 5). Boxes list brief descriptions of each form ofregulation; right panels list neural systems upon which they depend. Therelative position of the boxes representing each type of regulation illus-trates relationshipsamong them. See textfor details. mPFC,medial prefron-tal cortex.

    K.N. Ochsner BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:48 61 53

    www.sobp.org/journal

  • 8/8/2019 The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

    7/14

    A second regulatory ability might be termed outcome-basedregulation because it depends on the re-mapping or re-learningof contingencies between stimuli or actions and affective out-comes. In contrast to description-based regulation, which de-pends upon high-level mental descriptions of the affective valueof a stimulus, this form of regulation depends upon updating the

    value of the stimulus via direct experience with the affectiveoutcomes associated with it. Perhaps the most well-studied

    example of this form of learning is extinction of the conditionedfear response. As described earlier, fear conditioning involveslearning that an initially neutral stimulus (the CS) predicts theoccurrence of an intrinsically unpleasant outcome (the UCS). Dur-ing extinction the CS is repeatedly presented without the UCS.Over time, conditioned responses to the CS diminish as theorganism learns that one no longer needs to fear that theunpleasant UCS will soon follow. Recording and lesion studies inanimals as well as functional imaging studies in humans haveimplicated a region of the ventromedial/medial orbital frontalcortex in this ability (133,134).

    On the basis of these results, some have characterized thismPFC region as having an inhibitory function. In the context ofthe work reviewed in the preceding text, however, it can be seen

    that this region is similar to those implicated in studies of rewardlearning, preference judgments, and certain kinds of social orself-reflective inference. Seen in this light, extinction learning canbe seen as a form of updating or recontextualizing the affective

    value of the stimulus. This interpretation is consistent with other work implicating orbital regions in another variant of outcome-based regulation known as stimulus-reward reversal learning. Inreversal learning experiments an individual is led to expect arewarding outcome whenever one of two stimuli (e.g., A but notB) is selected. After this association is learned to criterion, thestimulus-reward association is reversed and stimulus B is nowassociated with the reward, whereas stimulus A is not. In animalsand humans, lesions of ventromedial/medial OFC impair thisability (135137), and these regions are active during imaging

    studies of reversal learning (138). In social contexts OFC lesionsmight manifest this deficit in properly evaluating the contextual

    value of a stimulus in interesting ways. Problems might includecomments and actions that are inappropriately intimate or sex-ual, failing to appreciate social faux pas, and affect that is greateror lesser than the expected norm for a situation, especially whenself-conscious emotions (such as embarrassment) would inhibitinappropriate behavior (139143). In the past, these deficits

    were grouped broadly under the descriptive label disinhibition.The third regulatory ability, termed choice-based regulation,

    involves weighing the relative values of choice options tobalance short-term versus long-term gains. In this form ofregulation, the act of making a choice to favor one type of gainor the other has a de-facto regulatory effect upon behavior. Theclassic example comes from seminal studies of the developingchilds ability to delay gratification (144). The earliest of theseexperiments were conducted in the late 1960s with child partic-ipants ranging in age from 4 to 6 years (and in other laterexperiments with kids all the way up through early adolescence).During the task, the child sits across the table from an experi-menter who places a bowl of marshmallows, cookies, or someother tempting treat on the table between them. The child is toldthat the experimenter must leave the room for a few minutes. Ifthe child can wait until the experimenter returns, she can havetwo treats, but if she can not wait, then she is allowed to have justone and must ring a bell (also located upon the table) to let theexperimenter (who was in another room) know that this hap-

    pened. The child is thus faced with a self-regulatory dilemma: tohave one delectable treat now or to withhold desire for it in favorof having two treats later on. The idea here is that this choicebetween short- and long-term gains models for the developingchild the kind of dilemmas adults face in everyday life, includingchoices like eating fattening foods and smoking cigarettes,

    whose immediate pleasures come at the price of poorer long-term health and longevity. In longitudinal studies, Mischel et al.

    found that the amount of time a child could wait to consume thetreat predicts a number of important adult outcomes, includingscores on standardized aptitude tests, income and educationlevels, and tendencies to have positive social relationships andnot engage in substance abuse (145).

    Recently, functional imaging studies have begun to examinethe neural bases of this ability with a paradigm borrowed frombehavioral economics known as temporal discounting. In thetemporal discounting paradigm individuals are given a choicebetween receiving a smaller amount of money (or similarly

    valued consumer good) immediately as opposed to a larger sumof money (or more highly valued consumer good) at some timedown the road (146). Individuals vary in the extent to whichtheyre willing to trade-off short-term cash-in-hand for a larger

    longer-term payoff, with some discounting the higher value ofthe longer-term gain to a greater extent than others. Imagingresults (147) have shown that when individuals choose theimmediate gain, activity is observed in regions associated withexpressing preferences, affective learning, and reward (e.g.,mPFC and ventral striatum). Strikingly, the loci of mPFC activa-tion include ventral and perigenual regions similar to thoseimplicated in outcome-based regulation. By contrast, whenindividuals choose the long-term gain, they show greater activityin dorsal and ventral lateral PFC as well as lateral OFC. Strikingly,these regions have all been implicated in description-basedregulation as well as response selection and inhibition moregenerally. Thus (as outlined in Figure 5), regulating behaviorthrough choice might involve a functional trade-off between

    systems involved in outcome-driven learning as opposed toguiding behavior on the basis of high-level mental representa-tions of stimulus meaning (cf. 148). This might be becauseindividuals might solve the delay dilemma in a variety of ways,including relying on their assessments of the current motivational

    value of a stimulus, which is updated as they pick the immediategain, as opposed to using reappraisal, which might allow them tofocus on the more abstract long-term goal (149,150).

    In sum, important strides have been taken toward elucidatingthe neural bases of three ways of regulating behavior in acontextually appropriate manner. Nevertheless, a number ofimportant questions remain. Perhaps foremost among them is thequestion of what specific computational processes are imple-mented in any putative control region and how that computation

    is recruited similarly or differently for each means of regulation.Another important question is how each type of regulation mightdiffer as a function of the emotion or response one is attemptingto control. The study of this topic is still quite new, and it remainsto be seen why related but perhaps different regions are acti-

    vated across studies of ostensibly similar forms of regulation.

    Realizing the Translational Potential of the Framework

    The goal of this review is to sketch a simple framework fororganizing both basic and translational research on the neuralbases of human social cognitive and emotional behavior. Thejuxtaposition of basic and translational approaches is important,

    54 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:4861 K.N. Ochsner

    www.sobp.org/journal

  • 8/8/2019 The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

    8/14

    because translational research is always a two-step process. Inthe first step, basic research provides models for understandingnormative behavior in healthy individuals. In the second step,translational research takes these models and applies them toclinical populations to help elucidate potential mechanisms ofdysfunction. This two-step progression has been the model forprior cognitive neuroscience-inspired research on the neuralbases of attentional and high-level cognitive deficits in schizo-phrenia (151153). This article provides a blueprint for similarprogress in the domain of social and emotional functioning.

    The previous section of this article described the first step inthis progression by providing a brief synthesis and synopsis ofextant data for the neural bases of five core abilities/constructsthat might underlie social cognition and emotion. The goal of thissection is to describe how the next step might be taken byproviding a few examples of the way in which this frameworkcan be used to generate questions about the way in which eachconstruct might be influenced by clinical disorders in general andschizophrenia in particular.

    Toward this end, Figure 6 presents a new version of Figure 1that again lists each of the putative ability/constructs but this timeprovides sample questions that might be addressed in futurebasic and translational research. The basic questions are onesthat were raised in the discussion of each construct in thepreceding section and are mentioned again in the following textas they are relevant to translational issues. It is important to notethat both basic and translational questions are listed here,because the ability to take the second (i.e., translational) step isalways predicated on how big a first (i.e., basic) step one hastaken already. Indeed, translational research is only as goodas the basic science models that motivate it. With that in mind,lets consider one or two translational examples of the way in

    which each construct might illuminate understanding of negativesymptoms in schizophrenia. As described in detail elsewhere(154158), these symptoms include a pervasive lack of emo-

    tional expressivity, abnormal emotional experience, lack ofmotivation, and asociality. Although behavioral work is describ-ing these symptoms with increasing specificity, as of yet littleneuroscience work has investigated the neural mechanisms from

    which the symptoms presumably arise.

    Construct 1: Translational Research on Aquisition of Social-Affective Values and Responses

    Basic research has yet to fully investigate how the neuralsystems for affective learning might be involved in the acquisi-tion of information about social as compared with non-socialcues. As a consequence, translational research on that topic willhave to wait, at least for the moment. Translational researchcould progress immediately, however, by building on one of thestrongest foundations of research in all of social cognitive andaffective neuroscience. Consider that behavioral research hasbegun to suggest that individuals with schizophrenia might havea normal internal experience of emotion in the moment but thatthey fail to anticipate or expect that future events will elicit theseemotions (11). As mentioned in the preceding section, thisdistinction between the anticipation/expectation and immediateexperience (or consummation) of a stimulus has been related to

    the function of the central striatum, mPFC, and amygdala (159). With that work as a foundation, imaging studies could usewell-studied reward learning paradigms to determine whetherindividuals with schizophrenia fail to recruit the ventral striatumduring the anticipation of a rewarding stimulus and mPFC whenit is experienced. In like fashion, fear conditioning paradigmscould be used to determine whether individuals with schizophre-nia show the normal acquisition of conditioned responses(which are essentially expectations of an aversive stimulus)mediated by the amygdala. Once basic research clarifies the wayin which social cues (such as facial expression) might dependupon these circuits, additional studies could help clarify whenand how individuals with schizophrenia effectively recruit theneural systems for learning the affective significance of social as

    compared to non-social stimuli. This would allow determinationof whether deficits are stimulus-general or specific to socialstimuli per se.

    Some work has already borne out some of these predictionsin animal and human behavioral research. For example, animalmodels have suggested that affective learning deficits might befound in schizophrenia (160,161), and human behavioral studieshave shown deficits in some forms of affective and non-affectiveconditioning (162164) and reward-related decision-making(165). Human imaging studies have just begun to examineappetitive forms of learning in schizophrenia. The results of ahandful of initial studies are converging to suggest that individ-uals with schizophrenia might fail to recruit reward relatedregions, such as the ventral striatum (166169). This work has

    shown that striatal activation during anticipation predicts variousnegative symptoms, such as anhedonia, which augers well forfuture work seeking to relate neural markers of affective learningto negative symptoms and functional outcomes in schizophrenia.

    Construct 2: Translational Research on Recognizing andResponding to Social-Affective Stimuli

    Basic research has begun to unravel the question of whetherbrain systems support the recognition of social-affective stimuliby implementing expression-specific or process-specific compu-tations. As this work continues to unfold, translational researchcan begin investigating the way in which judgments about thesestimuli vary as a function of ones clinical status. The idea here

    1. Acquisition of1. Acquisition of

    social-affectivesocial-affective

    values andvalues andresponsesresponses

    2. Recognizing2. Recognizing

    and responding toand responding tosocial-affectivesocial-affectivestimulistimuli

    4. High-level mental4. High-level mentalstate/trait inferencestate/trait inference

    5. Context-sensitive5. Context-sensitiveregulationregulation

    Basic: Link to accuracy/behavior?Translational: Deficit in activationof shared representations?

    Basic: Learning for social cues?Translational: Deficits inlearning or anticipation?

    Know moreKnow more Know lessKnow less

    Basic: Expression orprocess specificity?Translational: Alterationof evaluative judgments?

    Basic: Functional organization of

    MPFC subregions?

    Translational:Deficits inintentional or emotional inference?

    Basic: Regulatory functions of

    specific PFC subregions?Translational:Deficits inregulatory subprocesses?

    3.3. EmbodiedEmbodied simulationsimulationor low-levelor low-level

    mental state inferencemental state inference

    Figure 6. Diagrammatic illustration modified from Figure 1 to show howeach proposed ability/construct can be used to generate questions forfuture research. Basic questions include issues of current controversy anddebate aboutthe fundamental neural bases at eachability/construct. Trans-lational questionsapply insightsgained from basic research to understand-

    ing dysfunction in schizophrenia or other clinical disorders more generally.The gradient at the bottom of the Figure roughly represents our currentstate of basic knowledge about each ability/construct. Thus, the greatestamount of research has been devoted to understanding the first ability/construct and the least has been devoted to understanding ability/Con-structs 4 and 5.

    K.N. Ochsner BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:48 61 55

    www.sobp.org/journal

  • 8/8/2019 The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

    9/14

    would be to use knowledge of the specific functional rolesplayed by specific brain systems to test hypotheses aboutsocial-emotional recognition deficits for a given population. Inthe case of schizophrenia one could ask whether an individualstendency to fear or avoid others is related to dysfunction inneural systems supporting the recognition of different types ofsocial stimuli. For example, it is possible that individuals withschizophrenia would show abnormal activation of the amygdala

    during the perception of faces that are considered to be untrust-worthy (4446).Research could also move beyond this distinction to ask

    questions about systems involved in conscious as compared withnon-conscious stimulus perception. In healthy individuals, theamygdala responds to untrustworthy faces even when they arenot attended and responds to fear expressions even when theyare presented subliminally (45,46,170). This has been taken asevidence for the relatively automatic encoding of such threat-related stimuli by the amygdala, and one could ask whether theautomatic processing of these stimuli is disrupted in schizophre-nia. If the automatic recognition of social-emotional cues isintact, then one could ask whether the conscious expression ofevaluative preferences for stimuli, which has been shown to

    depend upon the mPFC, might be abnormal.Alternatively, subtle nonverbal social cues might be especially

    problematic for individuals with schizophrenia, especially cuesthat ambiguously convey the intentions of another person. Thusone might expect heightened amygdala activation to neutralfaces or to patterns of eye gaze that normally are not consideredthreatening in healthy individuals. Finally, paradigms used toinvestigate the role of the STS in recognizing nonverbal cues(e.g., biological motion) or the role of mPFC in expressingevaluative preferences might also be used to test hypothesesabout the kinds of social cues that are problematic for individuals

    with schizophrenia.In the past decade a great deal of work has been devoted to

    making progress on these issues by evaluating some kinds of

    social and emotional recognition in individuals with schizophre-nia. In general, the results of this work support the idea that themechanisms underlying Construct 2 might be dysfunctional inschizophrenia, with the majority of work focusing on theirimpairments in recognizing facial expressions of emotion (171)that might be persistent and predict functional outcomes(172,173), and that might be related to a broader deficit inrecognizing faces in general, regardless of their expression(174,175). Functional magnetic resonance imaging and electro-physiological studies have begun to suggest that this deficitmight involve reduced activity in the amygdala, insula, andrelated structures (176179) as well impaired structural encodingof faces (180,181).

    Construct 3: Translational Research on Embodied Simulationor Low-Level Mental State Inference

    Perhaps the most important question facing basic research onembodied simulation/low-level mental state inference is whetherand how activity in putative shared representation systems isrelated to actual behavior. Importantly, this includes behavioralmeasures of the ability to accurately identify or mimic the actions,thoughts, and emotions of others. As mentioned earlier, insightsinto these issues are just now appearing on the research horizon(100). As they come closer, translational imaging studies couldturn to investigating three kinds of questions about the functionof shared representations in individuals with schizophrenia. First,they could ask whether systems related to action programming,

    pain, or emotion are activated normally when they are experi-enced in the first person. Second, they could ask whether thesesystems are activated normally during the third-person observa-tion of another person having these experiences. And third, theycould ask whether the systems activated for first- and third-person action/experience overlap in individuals with schizo-phrenia in the same way as and to the same extent that they doin healthy individuals, thereby providing evidence for the status

    of shared representations. The answers to these questions couldhave important implications for understanding social behavior inindividuals with schizophrenia. For example, if patients fail tonormally activate shared representations when observing theactions and experiences of others, they might lack some of theelements essential for building a direct experiential understand-ing of the internal states of othersan understanding thatmotivates prosocial behavior, helping, and the formation ofsocial bonds (16,87).

    Because research on this construct has thus far depended onthe use of measures of overlapping brain activity, little behavioral

    work has explored potential deficits in schizophrenia. Onenotable exception is an electromyographic study showing nor-mal emotion-related facial expressivity as well as expected facial

    mimicry responses to pictures of facial expressions (182). Thismight suggest that shared representations are intact in schizo-phrenia, at least to some degree. The task for future work will beto supplement behavioral studies of this construct, which them-selves are relatively new, with imaging work examining theseissues in individuals with schizophrenia. Other clinical popula-tions (such as individuals with autism) suffering from impair-ments in social and emotional abilities have shown abnormalactivity in shared representation systems (183), however, whichsuggests that imaging methods might be able to detect potentialdeficits in individuals with schizophrenia as well.

    Construct 4: Translational Research on High-Level MentalState/Trait Inference

    Unlike most of the work on low-level mental state inference,work on high-level inference has tended to employ paradigmsthat provide behavioral measures of performance so that activityin neural systems can be related to the ability to accurately inferor make judgments about the mental states of others. It is not yetclear, however, whether and how regions supporting high-levelinferencesuch as mPFCmight fractionate into subregionssupporting distinct but related processes (117).

    That being said, given the consistency with which manyelements of the putative mentalizing network have beenactivated across tasks, there is a good basis for translational workto begin asking questions about the integrity of these systems inindividuals with schizophrenia. Here the logic is much like thatdescribed for Construct 3. Functional imaging studies first coulddetermine whether individuals with schizophrenia show normalactivation of mPFC and related regions while making judgmentsabout their own mental states or dispositional traits. Next, studiescould determine whether normal activation is shown when theymake similar judgments about other people. And finally, it couldbe determined whether self or other-related activity depends tothe same extent and in the same way upon overlapping neuralsystems. In this way research could attempt to parse the neuralbases of dysfunctional mental state inference in schizophrenia todetermine what kind of neural systemsand by extension, whatkind of psychological processesfunction abnormally. If indi-

    viduals with schizophrenia show abnormal activity in the tem-poral pole, for example, but not in the STS or mPFC, then one

    56 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:4861 K.N. Ochsner

    www.sobp.org/journal

  • 8/8/2019 The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

    10/14

    might infer that the semantic but not perceptual or inferentialcomponents of mental state inference have been impacted.

    To date, behavioral and neuroscience research has madesignificant progress toward documenting deficits in the ability ofindividuals with schizophrenia to make mental state attributions.Notably, however, this work has not proceeded in the ordersuggested in the preceding text. For the most part, it hasparalleled work on Construct 2, which concerns the bottom-up

    recognition of social-emotional cues in others, by examining theuse of higher-level processes to make mental state attributionsabout those cues. This means that work has not yet carefullyexamined the extent to which the neural systems for attributionsabout ones self and others are or are not common or distinct andare or are not impaired, in patients as compared with controls.Instead, as shown in recent reviews and meta-analyses, behav-ioral studies have focused on documenting consistent deficits ina variety of tasks requiring mentalizing about others as well asshowing that these deficits might relate to actual social behaviorand remain significant even after controlling for generalizedcognitive deficits (184186). In like fashion, imaging studieshave begun investigating the neural correlates of performingthese tasks and have found both functional deficits in activation

    of mPFC, amygdala, STS, and other components of the mental-izing network (9,187). To the extent that self-attributions aboutmental states have been examined, it has been with tasksexamining perceptions of agentic control over action rather thanattributions about emotions or traits (9). Future work might serveto bridge the gap between these literatures, perhaps in the wayssuggested earlier.

    Construct 5: Translational Research on Context-SensitiveRegulation

    Although the neural systems involved in context-sensitiveregulation of behavior have (to date) received the least attention(at least in human research) of any of the constructs, there issufficient coherence in the extant data to provide the basis fortranslational endeavors (21). The primary emphasis of currentbasic science work has been on identifying the neural systemssupporting the regulation of negative affective responses withreappraisal or extinction, and these methods could be extendedto study the ability of individuals with schizophrenia to success-fully recruit prefrontal control regions on the one hand andmodulate systems involved in generating affective responses onthe other. To the extent that individuals with schizophrenia havegenerally heightened tendencies to perceive threats and/or main-tain top-down goals, regulation might prove difficult. This diffi-culty might manifest itself as heightened activity in the amygdala(or related structures) or diminished activity in medial or lateralPFC or both.

    Few basic science studies have investigated the systemsimportant for choice-based regulation, but the results do suggestcompeting hypotheses for schizophrenia. On one hand, patientsmight favor short-term gains to the extent that they are unable torecruit lateral prefrontal regions to maintain cognitive represen-tations of long-term goals that can be used to inhibit affectiveresponses to immediately available stimuli. On the other hand,patients would favor long-term gains to the extent that currentlyavailable stimuli generate no expectation for pleasure that com-petes (see Construct 2). Critical here will be the extent to whichprogress is made on the basic science front to determine whetherdifferent regulatory dynamics are involved for positive as com-pared with negative emotions and for different types of regula-tory strategies (21). As these issues become clear, it might be

    possible to determine whether individuals with schizophreniahave problems not just with one of the three broad types ofregulation described here but rather with specific ways ofimplementing reappraisal, with particular kinds of choices (e.g.,between relatively rewarding as compared with relatively aver-sive options), or with extinction for particular kinds of affectiveresponses.

    Because basic science research on the neural bases of con-

    text-sensitive regulation have only begun to be established in thepast few years, it is not surprising that translational work on thisability has only barely begun in individuals with schizophreniaand has moved ahead only a bit more in other populations, suchas depressive subjects. Although the bulk of imaging work todate has focused on cognitive forms of regulation, the one studyto directly study emotion regulation in individuals with schizo-phrenia examined the ability to behaviorally regulate emotionexpression, which has been related to prefrontal activity in onlyone imaging study thus far (129). This behavioral study reportedthat individuals with schizophrenia might be impaired in theability to upregulate but not downregulate the behavioralexpression of positive emotion (188), an ability that mightpredict long-term mental health outcomes (189191). Other

    clinical groups, such as depressive subjects, have shownapparent dysfunction in the prefrontal-amygdala dynamicsunderlying the successful use of cognitive strategies (such asreappraisal) to regulate emotion (192,193). It remains forfuture work to determine whether and how the neural bases ofthese and other forms of regulation are intact in schizophreniaor other disorders.

    Conclusions

    It is said that the purpose of science is to carve nature at itsjoints. For this article, the hope is that the current frameworkcarves the biggest joints appropriately, even if it gets some of thesmaller ones wrong. To a certain extent this is to be expected,given that social cognitive and affective neuroscience are disci-plines that have come into their own only in the past 510 years.Indeed, it takes time for a field to mature and for core findings tobecome solidified. This consideration motivated the gradientshown at the bottom of Figure 6, which is meant to convey that(to date) basic research has provided the greatest breadth anddepth of core findings for the constructs described at the leftside of the figure, with Construct 2which concerns thebottom-up recognition of social and emotional cuesbeingthe most ready for immediate translational work. The excep-tion to this rule is Construct 4, which concerns the use oftop-down processes to draw inferences about mental statesand traits. As noted earlier, although there is more basic workto be done, paradigms for tapping the core systems underlyingthis construct are sufficiently developed to provide reliable

    vehicles for translational research.In large part, the need for more basic work in humans can be

    traced to the strengths and limitations of prior work that has beenbased primarily on animal models. Consider that the basicmechanisms underlying Constructs 1 and 2 (subsuming simpleforms of affective learning and recognition) are conserved acrossspecies. For decades, this has meant that one could study them ina rodent or nonhuman primate model without the need of atechnique like functional imaging to study them in humans. Aproblem arises, however, when one wants to move beyondthese simple forms of learning to those subsumed underConstructs 35: the first two ability/constructs cannot account

    K.N. Ochsner BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:48 61 57

    www.sobp.org/journal

  • 8/8/2019 The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

    11/14

    for social-affective abilities that depend upon higher-levelprocesses present only in humans. Before the advent offunctional imaging, it was difficult if not impossible to studythe neural bases of abilities like mental state inference andcertain forms of context-sensitive regulation (like reappraisal).In certain ways, this makes the rapid progress of basicresearch in the past decade all the more impressive and theprospects for translational research and the success of the

    CNTRICS initiative all the more exciting.

    Completion of this article was supported by National Institutesof Health Grant MH076137 and National Institute on DrugAbuse Grant DA022541.

    The author reports no biomedical financial interests or po-tential conflicts of interest.

    I would like to thank Tor Wager for assistance in preparingFigure 2.

    1. Barch DM, Carter CS, MacDonald AW 3rd, Braver TS, Cohen JD (2003):Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: Diagnostic specificity,4-week course, and relationships to clinical symptoms. J Abnorm Psy-chol112:132143.

    2. Carter CS,Barch DM (2007):Cognitive neuroscience-basedapproachesto measuringand improving treatment effects on cognition in schizo-phrenia: The CNTRICS initiative. Schizophr Bull33:11311137.

    3. HolmesAJ, MacDonald A 3rd, Carter CS,Barch DM,Andrew Stenger V,Cohen JD (2005): Prefrontal functioning during context processing inschizophrenia and major depression: An event-related fMRI study.Schizophr Res 76:199206.

    4. MacDonald AW 3rd, Carter CS, Kerns JG, Ursu S, Barch DM, Holmes AJ,et al. (2005): Specificity of prefrontaldysfunction and context process-ing deficits to schizophrenia in never-medicated patients with first-episode psychosis.Am J Psychiatry162:475484.

    5. LiebermanMD (2007): Social cognitiveneuroscience: A reviewof coreprocesses.Annu Rev Psychol58:259289.

    6. Ochsner KN,Lieberman MD (2001): Theemergence of socialcognitiveneuroscience.Am Psychol56:717734.

    7. Ochsner K (2007): Social cognitive neuroscience: Historical develop-

    ment, core principles,and future promise. In:Kruglanksi A, Higgins ET,editors. Social Psychology: A Handbook of Basic Principles, 2nd ed. NewYork: Guilford Press, 3966.

    8. Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, Lane R (2003): Neurobiology ofemotion perception II:Implicationsfor majorpsychiatricdisorders.BiolPsychiatry54:515528.

    9. Brunet-Gouet E, Decety J (2006): Social brain dysfunctions in schizo-phrenia: A review of neuroimaging studies. Psychiatry Res 148:7592.

    10. Earnst KS, Kring AM (1997): Construct validity of negative symptoms:An empirical and conceptual review. Clin Psychol Rev17:167189.

    11. Gard DE, Kring AM, Gard MG, Horan WP, Green MF (2007): Anhedoniain schizophrenia: Distinctions between anticipatory and consumma-tory pleasure. Schizophr Res 93:253260.

    12. Kee KS, Green MF, Mintz J, Brekke JS (2003): Is emotion processing apredictor of functional outcome in schizophrenia? Schizophr Bull29:487497.

    13. Kee KS, Kern RS, Green MF (1998): Perception of emotion and neuro-cognitive functioning in schizophrenia: Whats the link? Psychiatry Res81:5765.

    14. Kring AM, Alpert M, Neale JM, Harvey PD (1994): A multimethod, mul-tichannelassessment of affective flattening in schizophrenia. Psychia-try Res 54:211222.

    15. MueserKT, DoonanB, Penn DL,Blanchard JJ,BellackAS, Nishith P, etal.(1996): Emotion recognition and social competence in chronic schizo-phrenia.J Abnorm Psychol105:271275.

    16. Olsson A, Ochsner KN (2008): The role of social cognition in emotion.Trends Cogn Sci12:6571.

    17. Beer JS, Ochsner KN (2006): Social cognition: A multi level analysis.Brain Res 1079:98105.

    18. Gilbert DT (1998): Ordinary personology. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, edi-tors. The Handbook of Social Psychology, vol. 2 (4th ed). New York:McGraw-Hill, 89150.

    19. Lieberman MD (2003): Reflective and reflexive judgment processes: Asocial cognitive neuroscience approach. In: Forgas JP, WilliamsKR, vonHippel W, editors. Social Judgments: Explicit and ImplicitProcesses.NewYork: Cambridge University Press, 44 67.

    20. Ochsner KN,Barrett LF (2001): A multiprocessperspectiveon theneu-roscience of emotion. In: Mayne TJ, Bonanno GA, editors. Emotions:Currrent Issues and Future Directions. New York: The Guilford Press,3881.

    21. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ (2005): The cognitive control of emotion. Trends

    Cogn Sci9:242249.22. Holland PC, Gallagher M (1999): Amygdala circuitry in attentional andrepresentational processes. Trends Cogn Sci 3:6573 [Record as sup-plied by publisher].

    23. Pitkanen A, Kelly JL, Amaral DG (2002): Projections from the lateral,basal, and accessory basal nuclei of the amygdala to the entorhinalcortex in the macaque monkey. Hippocampus 12:186205.

    24. Schultz W (2004): Neuralcoding of basic rewardtermsof animallearn-ing theory, game theory, microeconomics and behavioural ecology.Curr Opin Neurobiol14:139147.

    25. LeDoux JE (2000): Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci23:155184.

    26. Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Dolan RJ (2002): Fear conditioning in hu-mans: The influence of awareness and autonomic arousal on func-tional neuroanatomy. Neuron 33:653663.

    27. LaBar KS, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA (1998): Human

    amygdala activation during conditioned fear acquisition and extinc-tion: A mixed-trial fMRI study. Neuron 20:937945.

    28. Morris JS, Ohman A, DolanRJ (1998): Consciousand unconscious emo-tional learning in the human amygdala. Nature 393:467470.

    29. Morris JS, Dolan RJ (2004): Dissociable amygdala and orbitofrontalresponses during reversal fear conditioning. NeuroImage 22:372380.

    30. Knutson B, Adams CM, Fong GW, Hommer D (2001): Anticipation ofincreasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens.

    J Neurosci21:RC159.31. McClure SM, Ericson KM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD (2007):

    Time discounting for primary rewards. J Neurosci27:57965804.32. Seymour B, ODohertyJP, Dayan P, KoltzenburgM, Jones AK,Dolan RJ,

    et al. (2004): Temporal difference models describe higher-order learn-ing in humans. Nature 429:664667.

    33. Holland PC, Gallagher M (2004): Amygdala-frontal interactions andreward expectancy. Curr Opin Neurobiol14:148155.

    34. Cahill L, Haier RJ, Fallon J, Alkire MT, Tang C, Keator D, et al. (1996):Amygdalaactivityat encodingcorrelated withlong-term, free recall ofemotional information. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93:80168021.

    35. Everitt BJ, Parkinson JA, Olmstead MC, Arroyo M, Robledo P, RobbinsTW (1999): Associative processes in addiction and reward. The role ofamygdala-ventral striatal subsystems.Ann N Y Acad Sci877:412438.

    36. MurrayEA (2007): Theamygdala, rewardand emotion.Trends CognSci11:489497.

    37. Rolls ET (2000): The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cereb Cortex10:284294.

    38. Elliott R, Dolan RJ, Frith CD (2000): Dissociable functions in the medialand lateral orbitofrontal cortex: Evidence from human neuroimagingstudies. Cereb Cortex10:308317.

    39. Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET (2004): The functional neuroanatomy of thehuman orbitofrontal cortex: Evidence from neuroimaging and neuro-psychology. Prog Neurobiol72:341372.

    40. ODoherty J, Critchley H, Deichmann R, Dolan RJ (2003): Dissociatingvalence of outcome from behavioral control in human orbital andventral prefrontal cortices. J Neurosci23:79317939.

    41. Knutson B, Fong GW, Bennett SM, Adams CM, Hommer D (2003): Aregion of mesial prefrontal cortex tracks monetarily rewarding out-comes: Characterization with rapid event-related fMRI. NeuroImage18:263272.

    42. ODoherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C (2001):Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human or-bitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci4:95102.

    43. Olsson A, Nearing KI, Phelps EA (2007): Learning fears by observingothers: The neural systems of social fear transmission. Social Cognitiveand Affective Neuroscience 2:311.

    44. Adolphs R, Tranel D, DamasioAR (1998):The humanamygdala in socialjudgment. Nature 393:470474.

    58 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:4861 K.N. Ochsner

    www.sobp.org/journal

  • 8/8/2019 The Social Emotional Procesing Stream Five Core Construct and Their Translational Potencial for SCH

    12/14

    45. Engell AD, Haxby JV, Todorov A (2007): Implicit trustworthiness deci-sions: Automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala. JCogn Neurosci19:15081519.

    46. Winston JS, Strange BA, ODoherty J, Dolan RJ (2002): Automatic andintentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness offaces. Nat Neurosci5:277283.

    47. Adolphs R, Gosselin F, Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Schyns P, Damasio AR(2005): A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdaladamage. Nature 433:6872.

    48. Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ (2003): Distinct spatialfrequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expres-sions. Nat Neurosci6:624631.

    49. AndersonAK, ChristoffK, PanitzD, DeRosaE, GabrieliJD (2003): Neuralcorrelates of the automatic processing of threat facial signals.J Neuro-sci23:56275633.

    50. Bishop SJ (2007): Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: An integra-tive account. Trends Cogn Sci11:307316.

    51. Hariri AR, Holmes A (2006): Genetics of emotional regulation: The roleof the serotonin transporter in neural function. Trends Cogn Sci 10:182191.

    52. Pessoa L, McKenna M, Gutierrez E, Ungerleider LG (2002): Neural pro-cessing of emotional faces requires attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A99:1145811463.

    53. Pessoa L, Padmala S (2005): Quantitative prediction of perceptual de-cisions during near-threshold fear detection. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A

    102:56125617.54. PessoaL, Padmala S,MorlandT (2005): Fate ofunattendedfearful faces

    in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources andcognitive modulation. NeuroImage 28:249255.

    55. Vuilleumier P (2005): Howbrains beware: Neuralmechanisms of emo-tional attention. Trends Cogn Sci9:585594.

    56. Whalen PJ, Kagan J, Cook RG, Davis FC, Kim H, Polis S, et al. (2004):Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites. Science306:2061.

    57. Adams RB Jr., GordonHL, Baird AA, AmbadyN, Kleck RE (2003): Effectsof gaze on amygdala sensitivity to anger and fear faces. Science 300:1536.

    58. Phelps EA (2006): Emotion and cognition: Insights from studies of thehuman amygdala.Annu Rev Psychol57:2753.

    59. Whalen PJ (1998): Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: Initial neuroimagingstudies of the human amygdala. Current Directions in Psychological

    Science 7:177188.60. Kampe KK, Frith CD, Dolan RJ, Frith U (2001): Reward value of attrac-

    tiveness and gaze. Nature 413:589.61. ODohertyJ, Winston J,Critchley H,Perrett D,BurtDM, DolanRJ (2003):

    Beauty in a smile: The role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facialattractiveness. Neuropsychologia 41:147155.

    62. KnutsonB, Rick S, Wimmer GE, Prelec D, Loewenstein G (2007): Neuralpredictors of purchases. Neuron 53:147156.

    63. Cunningham WA, Johnson MK,Gatenby JC,Gore JC,Banaji MR (2003):Neural componentsof social evaluation.JPersSocPsychol85:639649.

    64. ZyssetS, Huber O, FerstlE, vonCramon DY (2002): Theanteriorfronto-median cortex and evaluative judgment: An fMRI study. NeuroImage15:983991.

    65. Zysset S, Huber O, Samson A, Ferstl EC, von Cramon DY (2003): Func-tional specialization within the anterior medial prefrontal cortex: Afunctional magnetic resonance imaging study with human subjects.

    Neurosci Lett335:183186.66. Craig AD (2002): How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the

    physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci3:655666.67. Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ (1982): Insula of the old world monkey. III:

    Efferent cortical output and comments on function. J Comp Neurol212:3852.

    68. Mufson EJ, Mesulam MM (1982): Insula of the old world monkey. II:Afferent cortical input and commentson the claustrum.J Comp Neurol212:2337.

    69. Calder AJ, Lawrence AD, Young AW (2001): Neuropsychology of fearand loathing. Nat Rev Neurosci2:352363.

    70. Wager TD,Feldman Barrett L (2004): From affectto control: Functionalspecialization of the insula in motivation and regulation. PsycExtra.

    71. Phan KL, Wager TD, Taylor SF, Liberzon I (2004): Functional neuroim-aging studies of human emotions. CNS Spectr9:258266.

    72. Wager TD,BarrettLF,Bliss-Moreau E,Lindquist K,Duncan S,KoberH, etal. (2008): The neuroimaging of emotion. In: Lewis M, Haviland-JonesJM, Barrett LF, editors. The Handbook of Emotion, 3rd ed. New York:Guilford, 249271.

    73. Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ (2004): Neuralsystems supportinginteroceptiveawareness. NatNeurosci7:189195.

    74. Craig AD (2004): Human feelings: Why are some more aware thanothers? Trends Cogn Sci8:239241.

    75. Allison T, Puce A, McCarthy G (2000): Social perception from visualcues: Role of the STSregion. Trends Cogn Sci4:267278.

    76. Pelphrey KA, Morris JP, Michelich CR, Allison T, McCarthy G (2005):Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posteriortem-poral cortex: An FMRIstudyof eye,mouth andhand movements. CerebCortex15:18661876.

    77. Pelphrey KA,Singerman JD, Allison T, McCarthy G (2003): Brain activa-tion evoked by perception of gaze shifts: The influence of context.Neuropsychologia 41:156170.

    78. Puce A, Perrett D (2003): Electrophysiology and brain imaging of bio-logical motion. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci358:435445.

    79. Astafiev SV, Shulman GL, Corbetta M (2006): Visuospatial reorientingsignals in the human temporo-parietal junction are independent ofresponse selection. Eur J Neurosci23:591596.

    80. Saxe R, Powell LJ (2006): Its the thought that counts: Specific brainregions forone componentof theory of mind. Psychol Sci17:692699.

    81. Pourtois G, Grandjean D, Sander D, Vuilleumier P (2004): Electrophysi-ological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces.Cereb Cortex14:619633.

    82. Mitchell JP (2008): Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is notselective for theory-of-mind. Cereb Cortex18:262271.

    83. BarsalouLW, Niedenthal PM,BarbeyA, RuppertJ (2003):Socialembod-iment. In:Ross B, editor. ThePsychology of Learningand Motivation.SanDiego: Academic Press, 4392.

    84. Barsalou LW (2003): Situated simulation in the human conceptualsystem. Language& Cognitive ProcessesSpecial Issue: Conceptual repre-sentation 18:513562.

    85. Niedenthal PM, Barsalou LW, Winkielman P, Krauth-Gruber S, Ric F(2005): Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. PersSoc Psychol Rev9:184211.

    86. Decety J, Grezes J (2006): The power of simulation: Imagining onesown and others behavior. Brain Res 1079:414.

    87. Sommerville JA, Decety J (2006): Weaving the fabric of social interac-tion: Articulating developmental psychology and cognitive neuro-science in the domain of motor cognition. Psychon Bull Rev 13:179200.

    88. Gallese V, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G (2004): A unifying view of the basis ofsocial cognition. Trends Cogn Sci8:396403.

    89. Iacoboni M, Molnar-Szakacs I, Gallese V, Buccino G, Mazziotta JC, Riz-zolatti G (2005): Grasping the intentions of others with ones ownmirror neuron system. PLoS Biol3:e79.

    90. Uddin LQ, Iacoboni M, Lange C, Keenan JP (2007): The self and socialcognition: The role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons.Trends Cogn Sci11:153157.

    91. Jackson PL,Brunet E, Meltzoff AN,Decety J (2006): Empathy examinedthrough the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel ver-sus how you feel pain. Neuro


Recommended