+ All Categories
Home > Documents > The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

Date post: 02-Jun-2018
Category:
Upload: eka-pratiwi-lusman
View: 215 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 23

Transcript
  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    1/23

    Academy

    of

    Management

    Journal

    2007, Vol. 50, No. 5, 9871008.

    THEVERYSEPARATEWORLDSOFACADEMICANDPRACTITIONERPERIODICALSINHUMAN

    RESOURCEMANAGEMENT:IMPLICATIONSFOREVIDENCE-BASEDMANAGEMENT

    SARA

    L.

    RYNES

    TAMARAL.GILUKKENNETHG.BROWNUniversityofIowa

    Itishardlynewsthatmanyorganizationsdonot

    implementpracticesthatresearchhasshowntobe

    positively associated with employee productivityand firm financial performance (e.g., Hambrick,1994;Johns,1993;Pfeffer&Sutton,2000).Indeed,the failure to implement research-supported prac-

    tices

    has

    been

    observed

    in

    nearly

    every

    field

    wherethere is a separation between those who conduct

    researchandthosewhoareinapositiontoimple-

    mentresearchfindings(Lewis,2003;Rogers,1995;Straus,Richardson,Glasziou,&Haynes,2005).Thegapbetweenscienceandpracticeissoper-

    sistent and pervasive that some have despaired ofitseverbeingnarrowed.Nevertheless,overthepastdecade or so, attempts to deal with the problemhave evolved in the form of movements toward

    evidence-based practice in such fields as medi-cine,education,marketing,rehabilitation,andpsy-chology (APA Task Force, 2006; Ford, 2005; Law,2002;

    Southworth

    &

    Conner,

    1999;

    Straus

    et

    al.,

    2005).In the field of management, the nascent move-

    ment toward evidence-based practice is known asevidence-basedmanagement,orEBM.Accordingto Rousseau, Evidence-basedmanagementmeanstranslating principles based on best evidence into

    organizational practices. Through evidence-basedmanagement,practicingmanagersdevelopintoex-pertswhomakeorganizationaldecisionsinformedbysocialscienceandorganizationalresearchpartofthezeitgeistmovingprofessionaldecisionsaway

    from

    personal

    preference

    and

    unsystematic

    experi-

    ence toward thosebasedon the best available sci-

    entificevidence(2006:256).For evidence-based management (EBM) to take

    root, it is necessarythough far from sufficientthat managers be exposed to, and embrace, scien-

    TheauthorswouldliketothankBennettPostlethwaiteforinsightfulcommentsonearlierversionsofthisarticleandToddDarnoldandJaclynThollfortheirassistanceincodingthedata.

    tificevidence.Althoughthispointmayseemobvi-

    ous, it is hardly trivial. For example, unlike

    medicine, education, or law, management is not

    trulyaprofession(Leicht&Fennell,2001;Trank&

    Rynes,2003).Assuch,thereisnorequirementthat

    managersbeexposedtoscientificknowledgeabout

    management,

    that

    they

    pass

    examinations

    in

    ordertobecomelicensedtopractice,orthattheypursue

    continuing education in order to be allowed to

    maintaintheirpractice.Furthermore,sincethefirst

    choiceofmostmanagersseekinginformationisto

    consult other managers (e.g., Brown & Duguid,

    2002; Wenger, McDermott, & Snyder, 2002) and

    sinceextremelyfewmanagersreadacademicpub-

    lications(Rynes,Colbert,&Brown,2002),theques-

    tion of how to inform managers about scientific

    evidenceisanythingbuttrivial.

    One way in which aspiring managers can learn

    about

    management-related

    evidence

    is

    through

    for-

    maleducation.However,eventheacquisitionofa

    formalmastersorbachelorsdegreeinbusinessisno guarantee that a student has learned evidence-

    based principles. This is because many textbooks

    donotcoverresearchfindings,andmanyindivid-

    uals teaching in business schools do not have

    Ph.D.s and are unlikely to know about scientificevidence in their field of instruction (Trank &

    Rynes, 2003). Furthermore, there are millions of

    managerswhodonotholdformaldegreesinman-

    agement.Howmightthesemanagersreceiveinfor-mation

    that

    is

    consistent

    with

    the

    best

    available

    scientific

    evidence

    about

    how

    various

    management

    practicesinfluencebusinessoutcomes?Onepossiblewayisthroughperiodicalsaimedat

    practitioners,

    either

    in

    specialty

    areas

    or

    in

    general

    management. For example, in the area of humanresource (HR) management, Rynes, Colbert, andBrown (2002) found that by far the most widelyread periodical is HR Magazine, which is pub-lished by HRsmajor professional association, theSocietyforHumanResourceManagement(SHRM)and has a circulation of more than 200,000. An-

    987

    Copyrightof theAcademyofManagement, all rights reserved.Contentsmaynotbe copied, emailed,posted toa listserv, orotherwise transmittedwithout the copyrightholders express

    written permission. Users may print, download or email articles for individual use only.

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    2/23

    988

    Academy

    of

    Management

    Journal October

    otherHR periodical that is relativelywidely read,

    and that aims specifically to create a bridge be-tweenscientistsandpractitionersofHR,isHumanResourceManagement.Alternatively, inthecaseofgeneralmanagement,themosthighlyregardedpe-riodicalistheHarvardBusinessReview,whichhasa circulation of 240,000 and is published in 12

    languages.

    HBR

    is

    another

    publication

    that

    at-

    temptstobridgetheworldsofscienceandpractice

    and that has at least some readership among HRmanagers,directors, and vice presidents (Rynes etal.,2002).Inthiseditorial,weexaminetheextenttowhich

    three important HR-related research findings arebeing translated and transferred to practitio-ners via these three widely read periodicals. We

    thendiscusssomeimplicationsofourfindingsfortheprospectsofEBMinHRMandinvitecommen-taries from other individuals who are in a goodposition to reflecton our findings. First, however,we

    explain

    how

    we

    chose

    our

    topics

    of

    study.

    WHATSCIENTIFICEVIDENCESHOULD

    WESTUDY?

    Because the task of moving toward EBM is so

    daunting,prioritiesmustbesetastowhatspecifictypes of scientific evidence are most important totranslateandtransfer.Attheriskofmakingasome-whatobviouspoint,RousseauandMcCarthy(2007)

    arguedthatscholarsshouldbeginEBMbyfocusing

    on issues about which there is a clear scientific

    consensus

    on

    findings.

    In

    addition,

    an

    issue

    shouldbe important rather than trivial (Priem & Rosen-

    stein,2000).Touseamedicalanalogy,weshouldfocusonnumberonekillerissuesbeforemovingon to less consequential concerns. Third, weshould focus most of our attention on topics forwhich the scientific findings are not obvious to

    practitionersthat is, on problems that managers,left to their own devices, will likely solve bydoing something other than what sound researchevidence would support (Gordon, Kleiman, &Hanie,1978;Priem&Rosenstein,2000).

    StudyingPractitioners Views

    In the HR area, previous research has already

    identifiedanumberofclearscientificfindingsthatarenotobvioustopractitioners.Specifically,Rynesand her colleagues (Rynes et al., 2002) surveyednearly 1,000 HR vice presidents, directors, andmanagerstoidentifywhichof35well-documentedresearch findings HR practitioners widely disbe-lieve. Their results showed widespread disagree-mentorlackofknowledge(i.e.,morethan50per-

    cent of practitioners actively disagreeing with or

    not knowing about) the following research

    findings:1

    Intelligence predictsjobperformancebetterthan

    conscientiousness(Schmidt&Hunter,1998).

    Screening for intelligence results in higherjobperformancethanscreeningforvaluesorvalues

    fit

    (Meglino

    &

    Ravlin,

    1998;

    Schmidt

    &

    Hunter,

    1998).

    Being very intelligent is not a disadvantage for

    performingwellon a low-skilledjob (Hunter,

    1986;Schmidt&Hunter,1998).

    Personality inventories vary considerably interms of how well they predict applicantsjob

    performance(Barrick&Mount,1991;Gardner&

    Martinko,1996).

    Integrity tests successfully predict whether

    someonewillsteal,beabsent,orotherwisetake

    advantage of employers, even though individu-

    als

    can

    fake

    good

    on

    them

    (Ones,

    Viswesvaran,& Schmidt, 1993; Ones, Viswesvaran, & Reiss,

    1996).

    Integrity tests do not have adverse impact on

    racialminorities(Ones&Viswesvaran,1998).

    Goalsettingismoreeffectiveforimprovingper-

    formance than is employee participation in de-cisionmaking (Locke, Feren,McCaleb, Shaw,&

    Denny, 1980; Locke & Latham, 1990; Wagner,

    1994).

    Thetendencytomakeerrorsinperformanceap-

    praisal is very difficult to eradicate through

    training(London,Mone,&Scott,2004).

    Peoples

    actual

    behavior

    suggests

    that

    pay

    is

    muchmoreimportanttothemthantheyimplyinsurveys (Rynes, Gerhart, & Parks, 2005; Rynes,

    Schwab,&Heneman,1983).

    As these findings show, the two largest areas in

    which a gap looms between research results and

    practitionerknowledgeorbeliefsare(1)theimpor-

    tanceofintelligenceinpredictingjobperformance

    and (2) the usefulness of personality and integrity

    tests for predictingjob performance and counter-

    productive work behaviors. However, Rynes and

    colleagues

    (2002)

    did

    not

    determine

    the

    extent

    to

    which the HR research community regarded each

    oftheir35itemsasimportant.

    Web

    Survey

    of

    HR

    Researchers

    Therefore, to provide this third necessary piece

    ofinformationforprioritizing researchfindingsfor

    1SeeRynesetal.(2002)foradditionaldocumentation

    regardingtheseresearchfindings.

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    3/23

    2007

    Rynes,

    Giluk,

    and

    Brown 989

    EBM, we conducted a Web-based survey of HR

    researchexperts.Specifically,wesurveyedtheed-itorial board members of fourjournals: PersonnelPsychology(PP),theJournalofAppliedPsychology

    TABLE1

    EditorialBoardMembersAssessmentsoftheMostFundamentalFindingsfrom

    Human

    Resources

    Researcha

    (JAP),theAcademyofManagementJournal(AMJ),and Human Resource Management (HRM).2 Eachboard member was asked the following question,

    which

    was

    answered

    in

    an

    open-ended

    format:

    In

    your opinion,what are the fivemost fundamental

    findings from human resources research that allpracticing managers should know? Your answerneednotcitespecificstudiesweareinterestedinfundamental,generalizableprinciples.Inall,208board members were contacted. Usable responseswere received from 85 board members, for a 41percentresponserate.Ofthese,174servedonone

    board,30ontwoboards,andfouronthreeboards.Toanalyzetheresults,thefirstauthorevaluated

    Finding

    Generalmentalabilityisthestrongest,oroneof

    the

    strongest,

    predictors

    of

    performance

    Setting

    goals

    and

    providing

    feedback

    is

    a

    highly

    effectivemotivationalpractice

    HRpracticesareimportanttoorganizational

    outcomes

    Structuredinterviewsaremorevalidthan

    unstructured

    ones

    Valid

    selection

    practices

    are

    very

    important

    to

    Number

    of

    Responses

    22

    22

    21

    16

    15

    theitems,sortedthemintotheme-basedcategories,and attached tentative names to the categories.3The

    second

    author

    was

    then

    provided

    with

    the

    category names and asked to independently sortthe items. The two raters agreed on 71 percent of

    thecategorychoices,andthethirdauthorresolved

    thedifferences.

    Table 1 presents our results for the six topicsreceiving at least ten mentions. This table showsthat seven of the nine items identified by Rynes,Colbert,andBrown(2002)asexhibitinglargegapsbetweenscientificfindingsandpractitionerbeliefs

    are also regarded as very important findings by

    researchers: the three items pertaining to intelli-

    gence

    (also

    known

    as

    general

    mental

    ability,

    orGMA), the three items relating to personality,

    and the item concerning the effectiveness of goalsettingforimprovingperformance.4

    performanceoutcomes

    Personality

    is

    related

    to

    performance

    11

    aFindingswithtenormoreresponsesonaWeb-basedsurvey

    ofboardmembersfromasetofacademicjournals.

    CombiningthefindingsfromRynesetal.(2002)

    and the board member survey reveals that three

    content

    areas

    stand

    out

    as

    both

    containing

    clear

    and

    important research findings and suffering a gapbetween HR researchers and HR practitionersevaluations of these findings: the importance of

    intelligence or GMA for performance; the impor-

    tance

    of

    goal

    setting

    and

    feedback

    for

    performance,and the validity ofpersonality (of which integrity

    tests are one representation) for predicting perfor-

    mance.Thus,thesebecamethethreecontentareasexamined for coverage in practitioner and bridgejournalsduringthemainphaseofourresearch.

    2In the case ofAMJ, we surveyed only those board

    membersforwhomHRwasaprimaryresearcharea.ForHRM,wesurveyedonlythoseboardmemberswhowereacademics.

    3Itemscouldalsobesortedintomorethanonecate-

    gory, if appropriate. For example, the item, Cognitiveabilityandpersonalitytestsarevalidpredictorsofper-formance,

    was

    sorted

    into

    both

    the

    general

    mental

    abil-

    ityandpersonalitycategories.4Illustrative responses for the GMA category in-

    cluded, Cognitive ability is the single most importantpredictor of human performance, Ability tests havehigh validity, and General mental ability is a validpredictorofalljobperformance.Responsesforthegoalsettingcategoryincluded,Goalsreallymatter,Settingspecific, difficult attainable goals increases perfor-mance, Specific, difficult goals with feedback arehighly effective motivators, and The power of goal-settingandsimilarmotivational techniques.Responses

    ResearchQuestions

    Threemajorquestionsgovernedourexamination

    ofcoverageofthesethreetopicsintelligence, per-sonality, and goal settingin practitioner andbridgeperiodicals:

    1. Howmuchcoveragedideachofthesethreetop-

    ics receive inmajor practitioner and bridge pe-riodicalsbetween2000and2005?

    2. To what extent is the content of coverage in

    practitioner and bridge journals consistent or

    for personality included, Conscientiousness predicts

    performanceinmostjobs,[Weshould]hirepeopleonthebasisofabilityandpersonality,andEffectofper-sonalityonperformance.

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    4/23

    990

    Academy

    of

    Management

    Journal October

    inconsistent with peer-reviewed researchfindings?

    3. Whatsourcesofevidencearepresentedineachperiodical?

    METHODS

    Sample

    To investigate the extent towhich the three im-

    portantHRresearchfindingshadreceivedcoveragein practitioner and bridgejournals since Rynes et

    al. (2002) collected their data (in 1999), we con-

    structed a database of articles from HR Magazine,HumanResourceManagement(HRM),andtheHar-vardBusinessReview(HBR)forthesix-yearperiod200005. Each of these periodicals represents asomewhat different slice of the practitioner do-main.HRMagazineisaspecialistperiodical,focus-

    ing on HR managers. Patterns of both readership

    and

    authorship

    suggest

    that

    HR

    Magazine

    has

    avery strong practitioner focus. For example, 84.2

    percentofthepractitionerssurveyedbyRynesandhercoauthors(Rynesetal.,2002)usuallyoral-waysreadHRMagazine,andveryfewofitsarti-cleshave academic authors or coauthors (only 6.6percent between 2000 and 2005). HRM is also aspecialist periodical, read by fewer practitioners

    than HR Magazine, but more likely to be read bythose with higher education and position levels

    (Rynes et al., 2002). Between 2000 and 2005, aca-demics authored the majority of HRM articles(64%); practitioners authored 20 percent, andmixes

    of

    academics

    and

    practitioners

    wrote

    16

    per-

    cent.Finally,HBRisthemostwidelyreadandmost

    highly respected generalmanagement bridgejour-

    nalformanagers.Weregarditasmoreofabridg-ing than practitionerjournal because it is readby both academics and managers, and because itsarticles are almost evenly authored by academicsand practitioners (from 2000 through 2005, aca-demics authored 41 percent;practitioners, 45per-cent;andcombinations,14percent).5The intent of the content analysis was to seek

    5We realize there are other practitioner and bridge

    periodicals that contain HR-related content. However,webelieve that the three selected periodicals representthe clearest exemplars of the three genres (specialistpractitioner, specialistbridge, andgeneralist bridge), atleastinNorthAmerica.Forexample,DeadrickandGib-son (2007) also chose HR Magazine and HRM as theirprofessional-orientedcomparisonpointstotwoHRac-ademicjournals,theJournalofAppliedPsychologyandPersonnel

    Psychology, in their analysis of the HR re-

    search-practicegap.

    coverage of GMA, personality, and goal setting in

    thethreeperiodicals, witharticlesbeingtheunitof

    analysis. However, we excluded all articles that

    werenotatleastafullpagelong,asarticlesoflessthanapagearequitecommoninHRMagazineandgenerallydonotcontainresearch-relevant informa-tion(e.g.,bookandproductreviews,currentevent

    updates,

    awards,

    or

    profiles

    of

    companies

    or

    HR

    practitioners). In addition, we eliminated the

    Forethought sections of HBR (because thesepiecesare,again,lessthanapagelong),aswellas

    HBRs fictional case studies. Application of these

    criteriaresultedinthecodingof1,490articles:785forHRMagazine,168forHRM,and537forHBR.

    Database

    Informationabouteachofthe1,490articleswas

    gathered from Business Source Premier and en-

    tered into a spreadsheet. For each article, we re-corded

    the

    abstract

    and

    the

    first

    five

    keywords

    listedbyBusinessSourcePremier,aswellasbasicbibliographic information (e.g., authors, journal,

    volume,andpagenumbers).Inaddition,wecoded

    whethertheauthorswereallacademics,allpracti-

    tioners,oramixofacademicsandpractitioners.

    Tofacilitatearticlesearchesonparticulartopics,

    webeganbycreatingamasterlistofthekeywords

    thatoccurredinHRMagazine,HRM,andtwotop-tier academicjournals specializing in HR content

    (theJournal ofApplied Psychology and Personnel

    Psychology).6 This process resulted in 289 key-

    words.

    To

    reduce

    this

    large

    number

    of

    keywords,allthreeauthorsjointlyusedthecardsortmethod

    to create a smaller set of broader categories. For

    example,

    the

    general

    categoryselectionincluded

    thefollowingkeywords:abilitytesting,appli-cations for positions, assessment centers, cog-nitive abilities test, employee screening, em-

    ployee selection, employment interviewing,

    employment tests, examinations, interview-

    ing, interviews, and personality tests. We

    placed all keywords that were difficult to classify

    inamiscellaneous category.Thesestepsresulted

    in

    57

    initial

    categories.

    An

    advanced

    graduate

    stu-

    dentinhumanresourcesthenperformedthesame

    6We used two academicjournals, a bridgejournal,

    andapractitionerjournalinHRingeneratingkeywordsinordertomakesurethatbothpractitionerandacademic

    concepts of the field of HR management were incor-porated. We did not incorporate HBR at the keywordgeneration stage because it is a general managementjournal with many keywords being clearly outside therange of HR management (e.g., marketing, operationsmanagement).

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    5/23

    2007

    Rynes,

    Giluk,

    and

    Brown 991

    card sort using the author-generated categories,

    placing 76 percent of the items in the author-gen-eratedcategories.Discussionbetweenthethirdau-

    TABLE2

    PercentCoverageofGeneralMentalAbility,

    Personality,andGoalSetting,byPeriodicala

    thor and the graduate student was used to createconsensusontheremainingkeywords.Tosearchforarticlesrelatedtotheuseofeither

    Topic

    HR

    Magazine

    HumanResource HarvardBusiness

    Management Review

    intelligence/GMA or personality in selection, we

    initially

    conducted

    a

    broad

    search

    by

    focusing

    on

    articles including any of the keywords that re-

    Ability 0.0% 1.2% 0.4%

    Personality

    0.4

    1.2

    0.6

    Goalsetting

    0.6

    0.6

    0.6

    flectedeitherselection orrecruiting7(sincethetwo functions often occur simultaneously and aredifficulttoseparateinpractice),oranymentionof

    Total

    number

    ofarticles

    785 168 537

    intelligence,oranypersonalitytrait.Thissearchyielded 98 articles from HR Magazine, 21 fromHRM, and 23 from HBR. Interrater reliability wasnot an issue, since the keywords were taken di-

    rectly from the spreadsheet. However, becausesomeofthekeywordswereverybroad(e.g.,psy-chologicaltests,collegestudents),notallofthekeyword-identifiedarticlestrulyfocusedonselec-tion.

    To

    deal

    with

    this

    reality,

    the

    first

    and

    second

    authors independently reviewed all 142 articlesand highlighted those they thought were inappro-

    priatelycategorized.Thefewcasesofdisagreement

    (lessthan10percentineachofthethreecategories)

    wereresolvedviajointdiscussion.Theomissionofnon-selection-relatedarticlesresultedinasubsetof116 articles: 91 from HR Magazine, 20 from HRM,and5fromHBR.Similarstepswerefollowedforgoalsettingthat

    is,initialkeywordsearches(keywordswerefeed-back,goals,andgoalsettinginpersonnelman-

    agement),

    followed

    by

    examination

    of

    abstractsand article content to eliminate irrelevant articles

    (e.g.,oneson360-degreefeedbackthatdidnotcon-tainanydiscussionofgoals).Thesestepsproduced12goalsettingarticles:5fromHRMagazine,1fromHRM,and6fromHBR.

    RESULTS

    ResearchQuestion1:ExtentofCoverage

    Ourfirstsearchwasforarticlesrelatedtotherole

    of GMA injob performance. Despite the high de-

    gree

    of

    importance

    placed

    by

    research

    academics

    on the findings related to the intelligence-perfor-mancelink,oursearchrevealedalmostnocoverageof this topic in the three practitioner and bridgeperiodicals. Specifically, HR Magazine had no ar-ticles (of 785 total, 0%) regarding GMA over that

    timeperiod(seeTable2).HRMhadtwoarticles(of

    7Keywords for recruiting were college stu-

    dents,

    employeesrecruiting of, and help-wantedadvertising.

    aFigures

    represent

    the

    percentages

    of

    all

    full-length

    articles

    appearingineachperiodicalbetween2000and2005generated

    by

    both

    keyword

    and

    manual

    searches

    of

    article

    content

    by

    two

    ofthethreeauthors(seethetextfordetails).Tobeincludedin

    thetable,articlesonpersonalityhadtodiscusspersonalityin

    the

    context

    of

    selection

    (as

    opposed

    to

    postselection

    manage-

    mentofdifferentpersonalitytypes).

    168, 1.2%) that discussed the ability-performance

    link,

    and

    HBR

    also

    had

    two

    (of

    537,

    0.4%).8

    Results were not much different for personality

    orgoalsetting.Theroleofpersonalityinselectionwasthetopicofthreearticles(0.4%)inHRMaga-zine,twoinHRM(1.2%),andthree(0.6%)inHBR.Similarly, there were five articles (0.6%) on goal

    setting in HR Magazine, one (0.6%) in HRM, and

    threeinHBR(0.6%).

    Thus,thereisacleargapintheextentofcoverage

    of GMA, personality, and goal setting between ac-

    ademicjournalsontheonehand9andpractitionerand bridge periodicals on the other. The nearly

    nonexistent

    coverage

    of

    intelligence,

    personality,

    andgoalsettingbypractitionerandbridgejournals

    isconsistentwith(andmaybelinkedto)Ryneset

    al.s (2002) finding that the largest gaps between

    research findings and practitioner beliefs occur in

    theseareas.

    ResearchQuestion2:ResearchConsistency

    ofCoverage

    Beyondthisdifferenceinquantityofcoverage,it

    is also interesting to examine the extent to which

    8Ingeneral,wewouldnotexpectHBRtoprovideas

    much relative coverage of HR issues as the other twoperiodicals,giventhatitisageneralmanagement(ratherthananHR-focused)periodical.

    9Forexample,analogousfiguresinJournalofApplied

    Psychology,atop-tieracademicjournalinthisarea,were3.2percentforGMA,5.9percentforpersonality,and2.5percentforgoalsetting.Figuresforanothertop-tieraca-demicjournal, Personnel Psychology, were 6.9 percentforGMA,6.3percentforpersonality,and4.2percentforgoalsetting.

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    6/23

    992

    Academy

    of

    Management

    Journal October

    coverageofthesetopicsinpractitionerandbridge

    journals is consistent with research evidence. Wediscusseachofthethreetopicsinturn.

    Intelligence/GMA. NoarticlesonGMAappearedin HR Magazine over the relevant time period.However, two articles in HRM did deal with theroleofGMAinselection.Inthefirst,OLeary,Lind-

    holm,

    Whitford,

    and

    Freeman

    (2002)

    explained

    the

    recruitmentandselectionpracticesoftheU.S.fed-

    eralgovernment.Thesepracticesincludetheuseofa variety of cognitive and noncognitive tests de-signedtomatchindividualsabilities,personality,andsocialskillswiththerequirements offourdif-ferent occupational groups (administrative sup-port, professional, managerial, and trades/labor).The authors cited a considerable amount of aca-

    demic research on both the validity and utility ofalternativeselectiondevicesandprovideddescrip-tions of how research findings guide OPMs inter-nalselectionandplacementresearch.Inshort,thearticle

    frequently

    references

    the

    academic

    literature

    onGMAandishighlyconsistentwithit.The other relevant HRM article was part of a

    special issue (Burke, Drasgow, & Edwards, 2004)

    designed to illustrate how psychology-based re-

    searchcanbeusefullyappliedinHRmanagement.Articlesforthisissuewereauthoredbyacademic-practitioner teams in nine areas of HR practice,includingrecruitmentandselection.Becauseofthespecialissuesoverridingfocusontheapplicability

    of psychological research, the article on recruit-

    ment and selection (Ryan& Tippins, 2004) is also

    highly

    consistent

    with

    research

    evidence.

    For

    ex-ample,RyanandTippins(2004)drewonprevious

    researchtocomparevariousselectiontools(includ-ingGMAtests,integritytests,andmeasuresofcon-scientiousness) on validity, costs, and sizes of av-eragegroupdifferencesinscores(e.g.,maleversusfemale, and white versus black, Hispanic, and

    Asian). In addition, they discussed various selec-

    tiontoolsandstrategiesintermsofboththeiruse-

    fulnessforreducingadverseimpactandtheirlikelyimpact on applicants perceptions (an importantaspect in recruitment). Thus, the HRM articles on

    GMA,

    though

    not

    numerous,

    are

    highly

    consistent

    withresearchfindings.

    HBR also published two articles on intelligenceduringthisperiod.InHiringforSmarts,Menkespresented a largely research-consistent argumentforassessingintelligencewhenhiringmanagers:

    Somuchhasbeenwrittenaboutleadershipperson-

    alityandstylethathiringmanagersareindangerofneglectingthemostcriticalfactorinexecutivessuc-cess:intelligence.... Historically, theonlyreliablemeasureofsuchbrainpowerhasbeenthestandardIQ test which, for good reasons, is rarely used in

    business settings. But in rejecting IQ testing alto-gether,hiringmanagershaveturnedtheirbacksonthe single most effective assessment of cognitiveabilities, simply because there isnt a version thatappliestothecorporateworld.Theyhavedismissedtheonemethodthatcouldhelpthemidentifybusi-nessstars.(2005:100)

    He

    recommended,

    as

    a

    remedy,

    situational

    inter-views that focus on cognitive subjects associated

    withexecutivework:accomplishingtasks,working

    withandthroughothers,andjudgingoneself.The

    questions shouldnt require specific industry ex-pertiseorexperience.Anyknowledgetheycallformust be rudimentary and common to all execu-tives (Menkes, 2005: 102). This recommendationisconsistentwithaconsiderableamountofempir-

    icalevidence(e.g.,Latham&Saari,1984;Schmidt

    &Hunter,1998,2000)andprovidesacounterpoint

    to the commonly held (but incorrect) assumption

    that

    intelligence

    can

    only

    be

    assessed

    with

    intel-

    ligencetests.

    In the second article related to intelligence,DeepSmarts,LeonardandSwapwrote:

    When a person sizes up a complex situation and

    comestoarapiddecisionthatprovestobenotjustgood but brilliant, you think, That was smart.Afteryouvewatchedhimdothisafewtimes,yourealizeyoureinthepresenceofsomethingspecial.Itsnotrawbrainpower,thoughthathelps.Itsnotemotional intelligence, either, though that, too, isofteninvolved.Itsdeepsmarts,thestuffthatpro-

    duces that mysterious quality, good judgment.

    (2004:

    88)

    This articlemaps lesswellontopeer-reviewed re-

    search

    findings

    than

    does

    the

    Menkes

    (2005)

    arti-

    cle. For example, what Leonard and Swap calldeep smarts is what academic researchers callexpertjudgmentaprocesswherebyindividuals

    subconsciously match complex environmental

    stimuliwithsomedeeplyheldcategory,pattern,or

    feature acquired over many years of experience

    (Dane & Pratt, 2007; Simon, 1996). As such, the

    termdeepsmartscontributes towhatresearchers

    call

    construct

    proliferation,

    or

    creating

    new

    la-

    bels for phenomena that have already been well-

    researchedunderanothername.

    Inaddition,byintroducingthewordsmartsto

    indicate a combination of intelligence and experi-

    enceinaparticulartypeofjoboractivity,allthree

    constructs (smarts, intelligence, and experience)

    become muddied. And although it is true that in-

    telligence (what the authors call raw smarts) isinsufficientforproducingdeepexpertknowledge,it will still be the best predictor of it at any givenlevel of experience (i.e., holding experience con-

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    7/23

    2007

    Rynes,

    Giluk,

    and

    Brown 993

    stant). This is because of what intelligence is and

    thewayitworks:

    Intelligence is the ability to grasp and reason cor-

    rectlywithabstractions(concepts)andsolveprob-lems.However,perhapsamoreusefuldefinitionisthatintelligenceistheabilitytolearn.Higherintel-ligenceleadstomorerapidlearning,andthemore

    complex

    the

    material

    to

    be

    learned,

    the

    more

    this

    istrue.... Why doesGMA predictjob performance?

    The primary reason is that people who are moreintelligent learn morejob knowledge and learn itfaster.... Evenwhenworkershaveequaljobknowl-edge the more intelligent workers have higherjobperformance. This is because there are problemsthat come up on thejob that are not covered bypreviousjobknowledge,andGMAisuseddirectlyon thejob to solve these problems. (Schmidt &Hunter,2000:35)

    Moregenerally,keywordsearchesinHBRturned

    up a number of additional articles that furthermuddy

    the

    construct

    waters

    with

    respect

    to

    in-

    telligence and its relationship tojob performance.For example, in the period 200005, HBR con-tainedmorearticlesthatcoveredemotionalintel-ligence and social intelligence (e.g., Coutu,2004;Goffee& Jones, 2005;Goleman, 2000, 2004)thanarticlesthatcoveredintelligenceorcogni-tiveability,despitethefactthatemotionalintelli-gence and social intelligence have far weaker re-search bases in top-tier peer-reviewed psychology

    journals and that some definitions of emotionalintelligence are so broad as to include nearly all

    important

    human

    traits,

    including

    a

    hefty

    chunk

    ofGMA(Murphy,2006).

    Insum,ofthetwoperiodicalsthataddressedtheusefulness of intelligence in selection, only HRMprovided research-consistent information. How-ever,HBRprovidedmixedcoverage,withthearti-clebyMenkes(2005)providingresearch-consistent

    information, but articles by Coutu (2004), Goffee

    and Jones (2005), Goleman (2000, 2004) and Leo-

    nard and Swap (2004) providing either research-inconsistent or, at best, only partially research-consistentinformation.

    Personality.

    The

    two

    HRM

    articles

    that

    covered

    GMA in a research-consistent fashion (OLeary et

    al.,2002;Ryan&Tippins,2004)alsoreviewedthe

    researchevidenceonthevalidityofvariousaspects

    of personality in selection. As such, although onewouldnotdescribeHRMscoverageoftheseissuesasextensive,itisconsistentwiththebestavail-able scientific evidence on personality, as wasHRMscoverageofGMA.AlthoughHRMagazinedidnotcoverGMAatall

    intherelevantperiod,itdidpublishthreearticlesonpersonalityassessmentasapredictorofvarious

    behaviors:Andrews(2005),Bates(2002),andKrell

    (2005).Andrews(2005)beganwithadiscussionof

    personal and business ethics and then asked

    whether personality tests can help detect thoselikely to engage in unethical or other counterpro-ductive behaviors. For themost part, she took theresearch-consistentpositionthattheycan,citinga

    variety

    of

    research

    psychiatrists

    and

    psychologists

    tosupportthecase.

    On the other hand, some claims made in the

    article go far beyond scientifically substantiated

    evidence.Forexample,atonepoint,Andrewscited

    a senior vice president of HR as saying, You canpickupamultitudeofcluesaboutapersonschar-acterby simply having a restaurantmeal together.Youllseehowtheyinteractwiththewaiterorthe

    people sitting at adjacent tables. I sometimes say,

    Gee, how much of a tip do you think we shouldleave? Then, based on whatever percentage theysuggest, I ask why. I want to see how they makethose

    decisions.

    A

    lot

    of

    it

    bears

    on

    how

    they

    view

    theworldinamoregeneralsense(2005:56).Thistype of screeningbehavior is not supported by re-

    search findings. Rather, it is an example of using

    non-job-related criteria that are likely to reflect a

    hiringmanagerspersonalpredilectionsmorethanacandidatesabilitytodoajob.Assuch,thisquoterepresentsaselectiontacticthatislowinvalidity

    and utility but high in exposure to potential legalliability.

    AsecondHRMagazinearticlebyBates(2002)is

    alsoamixofresearch-consistentandquestionable

    claims.

    For

    example,

    in

    keeping

    with

    research

    evi-dence,hewrotethatconsensusisbuildinginthe

    research community that five factors shape our

    overallpersonality(Bates,2002:30).However,the

    fivetraitshecited(needforstability,whetherweare solitary or social, whether we strive more forinnovation or efficiency, the degree to which we

    stick to our positions or accept others ideas, and

    whether we are more linear or flexible in our ap-

    proachtogoals[Bates,2002:30])arenotentirely

    consistent with the Big Five that have generally

    been used in selection research: emotional stabil-

    ity,

    extraversion,

    openness

    to

    experience,

    agree-

    ableness,andconscientiousness(Barrick&Mount,

    1991; Digman, 1986; McCrae & Costa, 1987). This

    confusion was exacerbated later in the article,

    wherethefivedimensionsofaproprietaryperson-

    ality inventory offered by a consulting firm were

    listedasneedforstability,extraversion,original-ity, accommodation, and consolidation (Bates,2002:31).In addition, Bates stated that there are no

    wrong answers to personality testsonly resultsthat suggest an individual is better-suited to one

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    8/23

    994

    Academy

    of

    Management

    Journal October

    typeofworkthananother(2002:30).Althoughitistruethatcertainpersonalitytraits(suchasextra-version) are more predictive of performance insomejobs than others, one of the Big Five traits(conscientiousness)hasbeenfoundtobeapositivepredictorofperformanceinalljobtypes(Barrick&Mount, 1991). In addition, scores on three of the

    Big

    Five

    factors

    (conscientiousness,

    emotional

    sta-

    bility, and agreeablenessthe factors that domi-nate most personality-based integrity tests) havebeenfoundtobegoodpredictorsofcounterproduc-tivebehaviorssuchasfighting,stealing,andabsen-teeism over alljob categories (Ones et al., 1993).Moreover, when employers use personality-basedintegrity tests, they are certainly counting someanswers as better than others, regardless of thejobinquestion.

    Thefinalpersonality-relatedarticleinHRMaga-zine (Krell, 2005) describes how personality testsarebeingusedforavarietyofpurposesotherthanexternal

    hiring.

    These

    include

    individual

    develop-

    ment, team communications, conflict resolution,coaching, and placement.Overall, the article con-

    tains a mixture of research-consistent and -incon-

    sistentstatements,alongwithsomeclaimsthatare

    difficulttolinktoanyclearresearchliterature.Ontheresearch-consistentside,Krell(2005),like

    Bates(2002),correctlyindicatedthatmostexpertsbelieve there are between four and six basic per-sonalitydimensionsandthatacceptanceofperson-

    alityassessmentisgrowing.Healsodescribedone

    companys use of concurrent empirical validation

    of

    personality

    measures

    (a

    desirable

    practice

    if

    suf-ficient sample sizes are available) and referred

    readerstoavarietyofonlinesupporttoolsforusingpersonality assessments (including SHRM whitepapersontestvalidationandusingpersonalityas-sessmentsinselection,andalegalreportabouttheuseofintegritytesting).

    On the other hand, the article also discussed a

    numberofpracticesthatdonothaveclearresearch

    foundationsandmaybeproblematic.Forexample,one quoted executive enthused: The science be-hind cultural fit is extremely important and goes

    right

    to

    the

    bottom

    line

    (Krell,

    2005:

    51).

    In

    fact,

    the scienceof cultural fit suggests that although

    there are clear relationships between cultural fit

    and employee satisfaction and retention, results

    with respect tojob or unit performance are muchmoreopentoquestion(e.g.,Janis,1983;Meglino&Ravlin,1998).In another place,Krell quoted a consultantwho

    argued:UsingpersonalityassessmentstoconfirmHR professionals instincts is a benefit of thesetools.... You know you like them.... Now youcan determine exactly why that is and use that

    criteria[sic]forselection,development,andreten-

    tion(2005:4950).Fromascientificperspective,

    this suggested use of personality assessments

    amountstocapturingthecurrentdecisionmodelof a decision maker. Unfortunately, however, itdoesnotdemonstratethatanapplicantsoassessedcandothejoborthatthedecisionmakerscurrent

    model

    is

    a

    valid

    one.

    In

    fact,

    related

    research

    (on

    employment interviews) suggests that interviews

    are considerably more valid if managers are not

    allowed to develop preconceptions (e.g., byviewing resumes) prior to conducting interviews

    (e.g., McDaniel, Whetzel, Schmidt, & Maurer,1994). As such, this suggested use of personalityassessments is more likely to entrench idiosyn-craticjudgments of hiringmanagers, providing an

    auraofscientificrespectabilitytowhataremerely

    individual assumptions, predilections, or biases

    thatselectionresearchershavebeenarguingagainst

    foryears.Overall,

    then,

    the

    few

    articles

    appearing

    in

    HR

    Magazine on the topic of personality assessmentrepresent a mixture of research-consistent and re-

    search-inconsistentinformation.Althoughtheyac-

    curatelyportraytheascendancyoffive(giveortake

    one) dimensions of personality in the research

    realmandthefactthatsomeofthedimensionsare

    differentially associated with performance on dif-

    ferenttypesofjobs,theydonotconveythefactthatconscientiousnessisapredictorofperformancein

    alljobs,orthatacombinationofconscientiousness,

    emotional stability, and agreeableness is a good

    general

    predictor

    of

    counterproductive

    behaviorsacrossoccupations.Inaddition,theymakeanum-

    ber of claims that are inconsistent with existing

    research

    findings.

    The

    general

    sense

    that

    we

    were

    leftwithafterreadingthesearticleswasanimpres-sionthattheyoverpromisedastowhatpersonalityassessments can do, underexplained the differ-

    ences between types of personality assessments,

    and overreached in terms of their legitimate

    applications.

    Turning to HBR, we found 12 articles that con-

    tain personality-related keywords and 4 that con-

    tain

    selection

    keywords.

    However,

    upon

    exam-

    ining the articles, we found that very few of

    them make any direct references to the use of

    personality variables in selection. Rather, most

    focusonthemanagementofindividualswithpar-

    ticular (usually problematic) personality char-acteristics (Waldroop & Butler, 2000), or discuss

    how dysfunctional personality characteristics of

    CEOscanbebettermanagedorself-managed(e.g.,Goffee & Jones, 2000; Khurana, 2002; Maccoby,2000;Tedlow,2001).Anothersetofarticlesfocusesonhowleadersrelationshipswithcloseconfidants

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    9/23

    2007

    Rynes,

    Giluk,

    and

    Brown 995

    (Sulkowicz, 2004), coaches (Berglas, 2002), or fol-

    lowers(Offermann,2004)candegenerateintopsy-chologically destructive patterns that compromisealeaderseffectiveness.Additionally,thefourarti-cles about selection in general (Bennis& OToole,2000; Butler & Waldroop, 2004; Sorcher & Brant,2002;Wetlaufer,2000)focusmostlyonhiringpro-

    cedures

    (e.g.,

    agreeing

    on

    the

    job

    description,

    cre-

    ating interview questions, resolving political con-

    flicts)andcandidateskillsorbehaviorsratherthanpersonalitytraits.However,twoofthesearticlesdealatleastpartly

    with the evaluation of personality traits in a CEOselectioncontext.InDontHiretheWrongCEO,Bennis and OToole (2000: 174175) warnedagainstcandidateswhoactlikeCEOs.... Boardsoften are seduced by articulate, glamorousdarewe say itcharismatic dreamerswho sendmulti-ple frissons down their collective spines.... Infact,(however),manyofthegreatestcorporatelead-ers

    come

    up

    short

    on

    the

    charisma

    scale,

    because

    charismatypicallygoeshand-in-handwithinflatedego. Similarly, in The Curse of the SuperstarCEO, Khurana also warned about the dangers ofcharismatic leaders: When companies look fornew leaders, the one quality they seek above allothersischarisma.Theresult,moreoftenthannot,isdisappointmentorevendisaster(2002:60).Inother words, the two HBR articles that deal withpersonalityinleaderselectionareessentiallywarn-

    ingsagainstcharismaticleaders.

    How do these warnings square with academic

    research

    on

    charismatic

    leadership?

    In

    one

    sense,

    itisdifficulttomakecomparisons,becausetheterm

    charismaticleaderseemstobeuseddifferentlyinthe academic and practitioner literatures.10 In thepractitionerliterature, charismaisasynonymforcharm or mysticism. Indeed, Khurana traced thewordcharismatothevariouscharisms,orgifts

    of the Holy Spirit, that Christians may possess(2002:60).Thisuseofthewordsuggeststhatcha-

    rismaisstyleratherthansubstance.Relatedly,HowellandShamirwrote,Theoriesofcharismaticleadership have been accused of promoting a he-

    roic

    leadership

    stereotype

    (Beyer,

    1999;

    Yukl,

    1998),whichdepictsleadersasheroicfiguresthat

    aresingle-handedlycapableofdeterminingthefate

    and fortunes of groups and organizations. In this

    heroic conception, the leader is omnipotent, andfollowers are submissive to the leaders will anddemands(2005:96).In contrast, in the academic literature, charis-

    10WethankAmyColbertforhelpininterpretingthe

    academicandpractitionerliteraturesonleadership.

    matic leadership tends to be defined much more

    broadlyandisoftenequatedwithtransformational

    leadership, especially transformational leader-

    ships visioning and role modeling dimensions. Itincludes not only having a dynamic, charismaticstyle,butalsocommunicatingacompellingvisionand serving as a role model of the values of an

    organization.

    In the academic literature, personalized and so-

    cialized charismatic leadership are often distin-

    guished. Personalized charismatic leaders tend tobe described as self-centered and sometimes even

    manipulative.Theyareinterestedinpursuingtheirowngoals,ratherthanthegoalsofacollective:

    Inthepersonalizedrelationship,followersarecon-

    fused and disoriented beforejoining the relation-ship, and the relationship provides them with aclearersenseofselfandgreaterselfconfidence.Thistype of relationship is based mainly on followerspersonalidentification withtheleader,ratherthanon their identification with or acceptance of the

    leadersmessage.(Howell&Shamir,2005:100)

    Incontrast,socializedcharismaticleadersworkfor

    thegoodofthecollective:

    Inthesocializedrelationship,followershaveaclear

    senseofselfandaclearsetofvalues,andthechar-ismaticrelationshipprovidesthemwithameansforexpressingtheirimportantvalueswithintheframe-workofacollectiveaction.Followersinthistypeofrelationshipderivetheirsenseofdirectionandself-expressionnotfrompersonalidentification withthe

    leader

    but

    from

    the

    leaders

    message.

    In

    this

    rela-tionshipfollowersplaceconstraintsontheleaders

    influence, play an active role in determining thevaluesexpressedbytheleader,arelessdependentontheleader,andarelessopentomanipulationbytheleader.(Howell&Shamir,2005:100)

    Thisdistinctionhelpstoexplainwhytheauthors

    inHBRseecharismaticleadershipasgenerallyneg-

    ative,butacademicresearchersseeitasambiguous,

    though generally more positive. Overall, there is

    accumulatingevidencethatdemonstratesboththepositive and negative outcomes of charismatic

    leadership

    (Howell

    &

    Shamir,

    2005:

    97).

    However,

    moreoftheacademicevidencefallsonthepositive

    side(Judge&Piccolo,2004)aresultthatisprob-

    ablydueinparttothefactthatacademicstendto

    measure charismatic leadership in a way that is

    consistent with socialized charismatic leadership.

    In contrast, Khurana (2002) and Bennis and

    OToole(2000)seemtobedescribingpersonalizedcharismaticleadership,orthedarksideofchar-ismaticleadership.Viewed from the vantage point of EBM, it is

    significant that the HBR articles on personality

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    10/23

    996

    Academy

    of

    Management

    Journal October

    including those that deal with management and

    self-management, as well as selectionmake nomentionwhatsoever of the huge scientific discov-

    eryoftherobustBigFivepersonalityfactors.This

    omissionisparticularlystrikinginthatthediscov-ery of the Big Five goes back more than 20 years(e.g., Digman, 1986; McCrae & Costa, 1987). As

    such,

    none

    of

    the

    excitement

    that

    academics

    havefelt about being able to consolidate research evi-

    dence on these meta factors of personality (asopposed to having to deal with dozens or even

    hundreds of narrower personality traits) has

    reached HBRs audience. Similarly, the academicdistinction between personal and socialized

    charismaticleadershipseemstohavepassedHBRsnotice,despitethefactthatitmighthelptoclearup

    someoftheconflictingviewsaboutthemeritsand

    shortfallsofcharismaticleaders.

    Overall, in all threejournals, the amount of at-

    tention

    paid

    to

    personality

    is

    not

    very

    great,

    and

    certainly smaller than the amount of attention de-

    votedtoitinacademicjournals.However,itscov-

    erageinHRMtendstoberesearch-consistent,while

    coverage in HR Magazine represents a mixture ofresearch-consistent (e.g., the Big Five and the po-

    tential for using personality in selection is clearly

    there) and non-research-consistent reporting (e.g.,

    occasional recommending of nonbehavioral inter-

    viewquestionsorquestionsthathavenothingtodowith the predictive dimensions of the Big Five).Finally, treatment of personality in HBR seems to

    be

    completely

    divorced

    from

    academic

    research

    on

    personality, withnomentionoftheBigFive,con-tinueddiscussionofnarrowratherthanbroadper-sonality traits, and no research-based summary of

    generalizablepersonality-performance relationships.

    Goal setting. Turning next to goal setting, wefoundthatlessthan1percentofthearticlesinHR

    Magazinefocusontheusefulnessofgoalsettingforimproving performance.Of the five articles in HRMagazinethatmentionsgoalsetting,threeofthemmentions it rather incidentally (i.e., as part of a

    variable pay system in Frase-Blunt [2001] and

    Garvey

    [2000],

    or

    as

    an

    available

    feature

    in

    an

    on-

    line performance management system in Robb

    [2004]).

    However, in The Under-management Epi-

    demic, Tulgan (2004) hit the basic findings from

    goalsettingresearchrightonthehead.Specifically,hediscussedthevalueofspecific,challenging,and

    meaningful goals; accurate monitoring and docu-

    mentation of progress toward goals, and specific

    feedback on performance with guidance for im-provement(Latham,2006).Tulganwentontosay:

    Inanefforttobehands-offandnotbecomeamuch-

    maligned micro-manager, supervisors have gone

    to the opposite extreme and completely abdicated

    theirprimaryroleasmanagers.... Under-manage-mentistheoverwhelmingcommondenominatorin

    mostcasesofsuboptimalworkplaceperformanceat

    alllevels.Theunder-managedworkerstrugglesbe-

    cause his supervisor is not sufficiently engaged to

    provide

    the

    direction

    and

    support

    he

    needs

    and,

    therefore,isunabletohelpwithresourcesandprob-

    lem-solving.Themanagercannotjudgewhatexpec-

    tationsarereasonable,andhecannotsetgoalsand

    deadlines that are ambitious but still meaningful.

    (2004:119)

    Inshort,Tulganprovidedagoodexplanationofthestrongresearchfindingthatgoalsettingwithfeed-back is a far more effective motivator of perfor-mance,onaverage,thanisempowerment(Latham,2006;Lockeetal.,1980;Rynesetal.,2002).

    In

    the

    final

    article

    in

    HR

    Magazine

    that

    we

    exam-

    ined, Carrison (2003) focuses on a particular form

    ofgoalsetting:settingdeadlines.Hedescribescom-

    monalities in management practices over three

    largeconstruction projectsthatallmanagedtomeet

    ambitious scheduling goals. These commonalities

    includedgivingthegoalsagreatdealofpublicity,

    stressing the schedule at allpoints in the process,

    holding emergency meetings at the first signs of

    slippage,holdingallmanagersaccountabletoeach

    other,gettingmanagersinputonandcommitment

    totheschedule,andcelebratingon-timemilestones

    along the way. All these principles are consistentwith

    the

    results

    of

    goal

    setting

    research

    (Latham,

    2006). In short, when HR Magazine did report ongoal

    setting

    as

    the

    central

    topic

    of

    interest,

    it

    tended

    todosoinaresearch-consistentfashion.

    HBR published six articles that deal at leastpartlywithgoalsorgoalsetting.Onceagain,how-

    ever,someofthearticlesaretangentialtotheissues

    covered by thewell-documented body of goal set-

    ting research. For example, one article deals with

    assessmentsofindividualmotivationsandcompet-

    ing commitments (Kegan & Lahey, 2001), and an-

    other discusses ways to reframe goals to tap into

    individual

    differences

    in

    motivation

    (Nicholson,

    2003).

    However, three articles discuss principles of

    goal setting that map onto academic research.These articles focus mostly on the principles offrequent feedback with respect to progress towardgoals,aswellastheimportanceofgoalacceptance.For example, in Management by Whose Objec-tives?,Levinsonarguedthatoneofthereasonsforthe failure of Management by Objectives is thatunitmanagersareforcedtocommittogoalsthey

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    11/23

    2007

    Rynes,

    Giluk,

    and

    Brown 997

    dontbelievearerealistic(2003:107).11Relatedly,Parcells(2000)focusedontheimportanceofsettinggoalsthatpermitsmallwins,ratherthananul-timate goal that seems unattainable. In the thirdarticle, Turning Great Strategy into Great Perfor-mance, Mankins and Steele (2005) emphasizedtheimportanceofcommunicatingstrategicgoalsin

    simple,

    concrete

    language

    and

    of

    clearly

    identify-

    ingpriorities.Alltheserecommendationsarecon-

    sistentwithgoal setting research, althoughno ref-erence is made to this research, and pieces of therelevant goal setting findings are not emphasized(particularly, theimportanceofsettingdifficultbutattainablegoals).Finally,HRMpublishedonehighlyresearch-con-

    sistentarticleongoalsetting(Londonetal.,2004).

    This article was part of the same special issue onapplicationsofpsychologicalresearchtoHRman-agementthatwasmentionedinprevioussections.In summary, coverage of goal setting in the se-

    lected

    practitioner

    and

    bridge

    publications

    was

    quitescarce,particularlywhenthelargeeffectsizesfound in goal setting research are taken into ac-

    count.Moreover,approximatelyhalfofthearticles

    thatdidmentiongoalsettingdidsoonlyperipher-

    ally. Of the very small number of all articles thatdealt more than incidentally with goal setting,however,thecoveragewaslargelyresearch-consis-tent(particularlywithrespecttotheimportanceofgoalacceptance).

    Summary.Our analysis of ResearchQuestion 2

    suggeststhatwithrespecttotheimportanceofin-

    telligence

    or

    GMA

    to

    job

    performance,

    there

    hasbeen only sporadic (but accurate) transfer of re-

    search findings to HRM, limited but mostly re-search-inconsistenttransfertoHBR,andnotransferto HR Magazine. With respect topersonality, theresultsforHRMmirrorthosewithrespecttointel-ligenceverylimited,butresearch-consistent,cov-

    erage.InthecaseofHRMagazine,coverageisalsoat a very low level (

    1%), and claims are a mix

    of research-consistent and research-inconsistent.However, on the positive side, HR Magazine is atleast transmitting information about there being

    five

    (or

    so)

    basic

    personality

    characteristics,

    which

    cannot be said of HBR. In fact, HBR mentioned

    neither the discovery of the Big Five personality

    traits,northeacademicliteratureoncharismaticor

    transformational leadership. With respect togoalsetting,wefoundonerelevantandresearch-consis-tentarticleinHRM;fivearticlesinHRMagazine,ofwhichthreeprovidedonlyperipheralcoverageandtwoprovidedresearch-consistentinformation;and

    11

    HBRoriginallypublishedthisarticlein1970.

    six in HBR: three tangential and three research-

    consistent,althoughtheircoveragewaspartial.

    ResearchQuestion3:SourcesofEvidence

    The preceding analyses suggest little correspon-

    dence between what is being published in aca-

    demic

    versus

    practitioner

    and

    bridge

    journals

    with

    respecttothethreemostimportantfindingsofHR

    research (as perceived by researchers). Areas con-

    sideredtobeveryimportantbyresearchersreceive

    little coverage in practitioner and bridgejournals

    and,whentheydoreceivecoverage,itisaslikelytoberesearch-inconsistentasresearch-consistent, ex-ceptinHRM.This situation makes the question of who, or

    what,iscitedasevidenceinpractitionerandbridge

    journalsaninterestingone.Thus,weexaminedall

    152 articles that dealt with selection/recruitment

    (n 141)orgoalsetting(n 11)toexaminewhatsources

    of

    evidence

    each

    periodical

    used.

    These

    analyses provide some indication as to whatsources of information are viewed as most legiti-

    mateorcredibleateachperiodical.

    HRM. We tallied the evidentiary bases of the

    threejournals in different ways, because the con-

    tent and format of each periodical differ. Of the

    three, HRM most closely resembles top-tier aca-demicHRjournals such as theJournal ofAppliedPsychology and Personnel Psychology. For exam-

    ple,likearticlesinacademicjournals,HRMarticles

    tendtociteafairnumberofpeer-reviewedresearch

    articles

    as

    sources

    of

    evidence

    (36.7

    citations

    onaverage,with a standard deviation of 20.2). In ad-

    dition, journals receiving the most citations in

    HRM are research- rather than practice-oriented.Specifically, the top fivejournals cited in HRMover this period were all peer-reviewed ones: theJournal ofApplied Psychology (9.8% of all cita-

    tions),PersonnelPsychology(6.4%),theAcademy

    of ManagementJournal (5.6%), HRM (4.4%), and

    theAcademy of Management Review (2.5%). In

    contrast,HBRandHRMagazine(neitherofwhich

    ispeer-reviewed) eachaccountedforonly1.1per-

    cent

    of

    HRMs

    total

    citations.

    Another similarity to top-tier journals is that

    most HRM articles are either original research or

    literature reviews. For example, of the 21 recruit-

    ment,selection,andgoalsettingarticlesfoundbe-

    tween 2000 and 2006, 7 reported the results of

    surveyresearch(eitherquestionnaire- orinterview-

    based),6werebasedoneithersingle-ormultiple-

    organizationcasestudies,5presentedliteraturere-views,2reportedtheresultsofexperiments,and3presented typologies or best practices based oneithercasesorqualitativeanalyses.

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    12/23

    998

    Academy

    of

    Management

    Journal October

    HRMagazine.Thisperiodicalwasanalyzedina

    rather different way in order to reflect the typicalcontentofitsarticles.MostarticlesinHRMagazineare produced by staffwriters, consultants, or free-

    TABLE3

    TypesofEvidenceCitedinRecruitment,

    Selection,andGoalSetting

    Articles

    in

    HR

    Magazinea

    lancejournalistswhopresentmultipleviewpointsonsomecurrenttopicofinterest.Intheprocessofreporting, HR Magazine authors generally inter-

    view

    a

    variety

    of

    people

    about

    the

    selected

    topic.

    Type

    of

    Evidence

    Percentage

    of

    Total

    Evidence

    Cited

    Percentage

    within

    CategoryFollowing a quick review of the 96 articles on re-

    cruitment, selection, or goal setting, we devised acoding scheme to capture most of the evidentiarysourcesusedinHRMagazine.At the broadest levelof analysis, two coders in-

    dependentlyrecordedthenumberoftimesanarti-clecitedevidencefromeachofthefollowingfourcategories: people, quantitative data or surveys,

    laws or regulations, and case law or legal settle-ments. At the next-lower level of analysis, typesof people were further subclassified as (1) profes-sionals,managers,or employeesof companies, (2)consultants

    or

    vendors,

    (3)

    attorneys,

    (4)

    authors,

    (5) academics, (6) professional or trade associa-tionrepresentatives,(7)applicants,or(8)psychol-

    ogists and psychiatrists. In addition, quantitative

    data or surveys were characterized by the follow-

    ing sources: (1) individual companies, (2) profes-sional or trade associations, (3) government, (4)consultingfirms,and(5)academia.Detailedcodinginstructions for each category were given to bothcoders and are available upon request from the

    authors.

    We initially assessed reliability by counting the

    percentage

    of

    times

    that

    the

    two

    coders

    (the

    secondauthor and an undergraduate student) reached ex-

    actly the same tally for a source of evidence. Thisinitialanalysisshowedagreementof79percentorbetter on all but three categories: professionals,managers, or company employees; consultants orvendors; and company data and surveys. The first

    authorthenindependentlycodedspecificcasesof

    disagreement(n 116,or8.4percentoftotalcases)inthesethreecategories;sheagreedwithoneofthefirst two coders in 82 of the cases. For these 82cases,themajoritycodingwasused;fortheremain-

    ing

    33

    items

    (2.4

    percent

    of

    all

    coded

    categories),

    themedianfigurewasused.

    Table3revealsthatonly15percentofthearticles

    in HR Magazine on the investigated topics citedquantitativedataasaformofevidence.Instead,HRMagazine offers evidence primarily in the form ofquotes from various categories of people (78 per-cent of cited evidence), with 39 percent of suchquotescomingfrompracticingprofessionals,man-agers,andemployees,36percentcomingfromcon-sultants or vendors, and 4 percent coming fromacademics. Thus, overall, there are few links to

    People 78

    Professionals/managers/employees

    39

    Consultants/vendors 36

    Academics 4

    Attorneys 4

    Authors/publishers 4

    Professional/tradeassociation 4

    representatives

    Applicants 3

    Psychologists/psychiatrists 1

    Other 5

    Quantitativedata/surveys 15

    Consulting

    firms

    26Individualcompanies

    24

    Professional/trade

    associations

    24

    Government 17

    Academia 3

    Other 6

    Laws/legal

    regulations 4

    Caselaw/legalsettlements 4

    a

    Analysis

    is

    based

    onHRMagazinearticles(200005)con-

    tainingatleastoneselectionorrecruitmentkeyword(n 98)

    and

    surviving

    a

    subsequent

    check

    for

    relevant

    content

    by

    the

    firsttwoauthors(n 91).

    either academics or quantitative data (from any

    source)inHRMagazine.HBR.

    Because

    of

    the

    diversity

    in

    the

    types

    of

    articlesitpublishes,HBRrequiredyetanothertypeof coding scheme.Generally, our 30-article subsetcontained three types of HBR articles. In the first

    (n 14;47%),individuals(usuallyacademics)pre-

    sented their own research findings. In the second

    typeofarticle(n

    13;43%),individuals(usually

    consultants) offered advice or assessment on a

    topicbasedontheirown(ortheirintervieweesor

    clients)

    expertise.

    In

    the

    third

    and

    smallest

    (n

    3)

    set, HBR staffers presented articles on rather dis-

    paratetopics(e.g.,managingmillionaires, thequal-

    ity of resilience, and working as a room service

    waiterattheRitzCarlton).

    In these three types of articles, HBR, like HR

    Magazine,didnotincludemuchquantitativedataasevidence.Specifically,only8(27%)ofthearti-

    cles cited any quantitative data, with more thantwo-thirds of these cases coming from academic-authoredarticles.Similarly, most HBR articles (24; 80%) did not

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    13/23

    2007

    Rynes,

    Giluk,

    and

    Brown 999

    include

    formal

    research

    documentation,

    either

    withinorattheendofanarticle.Ofthesixthatdid,threewereauthoredbyacademics,twobyconsult-ants,andonebyanHBRstaffer.Acrossarticles,20percent of the citations made were to other HBRarticles; 20 percentwere to academic articles, andthe rest were to other types of sources (mainly

    books).

    However,

    the

    main

    source

    of

    evidence

    in

    HBR is the anecdote. One hundred percent of theHBRarticlesinoursampleusedanecdotesorsto-riestomaketheircases.

    Summary.Overall,HRMpresentsevidenceina

    way that is quite similar to that of academicjour-nals: academic works are cited and referenced,methods are described (although not in as muchdetail), and either original data or a literature re-

    viewfiguresinmostarticles.Incontrast,HRMag-azine gets the vast majority of its information orevidencefrominterviewswithpracticingmanagersor consultants. Neither academics nor peer-reviewed

    research

    play

    an

    important

    role

    in

    the

    typical HR Magazine article. Finally, HBR fallssomewhatinbetween,presentinglittlequantitative

    dataandfewresearchcitations.However,approx-

    imately 50 percent of HBRs articles are academi-callyauthoredorcoauthored,suggestingthatrele-vant research data and quantitative evidence mayplay some behind-the-scenes role in the construc-tionofthosearticles.

    DISCUSSION

    Our

    most

    striking

    finding

    is

    that

    bridge

    and

    prac-titionerjournalshavebarelycoveredtopicsthatHR

    researchersbelieve tobe among theirmost impor-tantfindings.Inotherwords,ourresultssuggestavery significant failure of academic research totransfer to important practitioner sources of infor-mation. The fact that practitioner-oriented period-

    icalsprovidesolittlecoverageofthesetopicsand

    that,when they do, theirmessages are often quite

    different from the ones transmitted by academicjournals,maybemorethancoincidentallylinkedtoRynesetal.s(2002)findingthatinallthreeareas,

    practitioner

    beliefs

    diverge

    considerably

    from

    researchfindings.

    In presenting this evidence, we are well aware

    thatourstudyisnotwithoutlimitations.Forexam-

    ple,wehavenotreviewedseveraltypesofalterna-tivemedia that practitionersmay use to get infor-mation about HR practices, such as informationalWeb sites provided by professional associations(e.g., SHRM or the Human Resource Planning So-ciety), business books (Furnham, 2000), and busi-nessdailies(e.g.,WallSt.Journal,FinancialTimes)and weeklies (e.g., BusinessWeek, Fortune). Nor

    haveweexaminedthemostfrequentlyusedsource

    of information for HR practitionersother HR

    practitioners. However, earlier evidence suggests

    thatonaverage,informationprovidedbyHRpeersabout intelligence, personality, and goal setting ismorelikelytobeinaccurate thanaccurate(Rynesetal.,2002).

    Another

    limitation

    is

    that,

    of

    the

    many

    HR-re-

    latedperiodicals, weonlyexaminedonepractition-

    er and two bridging periodicals. Thus,we left out

    otherpractitioner outletswithsmallercirculations

    than HR Magazine (e.g., HR Executive), some HRbridgingjournalswithnarrowercontentthanHRM(e.g.,HRPlanningJournal),andsomegeneralman-agementperiodicalswithsmallercirculations thanHBR(e.g.,theMITSloanManagementReviewand

    California Management Review). However,we be-

    lieve we selected the most appropriate exemplars

    ineachofthesethreeimportantclassesofperiod-

    icals; note that Deadrick and Gibson (2007) alsopicked

    HR

    Magazine

    and

    HRM

    as

    counterpoints

    to

    the HR academicjournals they examined (specifi-cally, theJournal ofApplied Psychology and Per-

    sonnelPsychology).

    Despitetheselimitations, webelieveourresults

    raiseseriousquestionsforbothresearchandprac-

    tice in HR. For the remainder of this article, we

    speculate about the potential implications of our

    findings, first forHR researchers, and then forHRperiodicals and the future of HR as a profession.

    Finally,weplaceourresultsinthebroadercontext

    of science-practice gaps over a whole range of

    disciplines.

    QuestionsandChallengesforResearchers

    For

    some

    time

    now,

    academic

    management

    re-

    searchers have been losing ground to consultants(and more recently, journalists [e.g., Friedman,

    2006]) as sources of ideas and advice for practi-

    tioners and policy makers (Abrahamson & Eisen-

    man, 2001; Bartlett, 2007; Rigby, 2001). Unfortu-

    nately, this decline is occurring at the same time

    that academics dependence on practitioners for

    resources

    is

    increasing

    (Trank

    &

    Rynes,

    2003)

    and

    global competition and growth are increasing the

    needforbothmoreeffectiveandmoresustainable

    organizations (Abrahamson & Eisenman, 2001;

    Bansal&Gao,2006).Somebelievethatourfailure

    tomattermore(Hambrick,1994)isapproachingacrisisstage(e.g.,Bartlett,2007).

    The decline of academic influence in theworld

    ofpolicyandpracticeraisesanumberofquestions.Perhaps the first one is,Are our major researchfindingstrulyunimportanttopolicyandpractice?Certainly,meta-analyticfindingsofeffectsizesfor

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    14/23

    1000

    Academy

    of

    Management

    Journal October

    therelationshipsbetweenintelligence,personality,

    andgoalsettingontheonehandandindividualorunit performance on the other, suggest otherwise(Latham, 2006; Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). If thesefindings arenot in factunimportant, thenwhydotheyreceivesolittlepositivecoverageamongprac-titionerandbridgepublications?Inthecaseofin-

    telligence,

    it

    seems

    that

    the

    reasons

    reflect

    some

    combination of socio-political, legal, and ego-pro-

    tectivefactors(Boehm,1982;Gould,1996;Pinker,2002). However, the explanations for goal settingandpersonalityaremoremysterious.With respect togoal setting,one possible expla-

    nationisthatitspositiveeffectshavebeenknownfor so long that they are no longer news. How-ever, our review of Personnel Psychology and the

    Journal ofApplied Psychology over 200005 sug-gests that goal setting is still a vibrant area of re-search.Inaddition,thefactthatthousandsofnewindividualsentertheHRprofessioneveryyearsug-gests

    that

    information

    about

    the

    sizable

    effects

    of

    goal settingmightwellbe news to a lotofprac-titioners.Furthermore,for-profitorganizationsnearly

    alllivebyfinancialgoals,whichreceiveintensescru-

    tinyandemphasis(somewouldargueoveremphasis)

    on at least a quarterly basis. And finally, executivepay is increasingly a function of meeting explicitgoals. Given all this, why is goal setting researchnearlyabsentfromthepractitionerliterature?With respect to personality, restricted coverage

    of the Big Five dimensions is also a bit puzzling.

    Clearly, personality is an area of great interest

    among

    the

    general

    public,

    and

    managers

    commonlystresstheimportanceofpersonality-joborperson-

    ality-organizationfitinhiring(Bretz,Rynes,&Ger-hart, 1993; Kristof-Brown, 2000). Moreover, theMyers-Briggs Personality Type Indicator is stillwidelyusedinbusiness,withmanagersofteniden-tifyingthemselvesasanESFPoranINTJ(two

    Myers-Briggstypes)asawayofexplainingtheirbehavior.AndourexaminationofHBRrevealedatleastsomedegreeoffascinationwiththepersonal-ity flaws of employees, particularlymanagers. So,we are puzzled: Why is there so little coverage of

    the

    really

    big

    scientific

    discovery

    of

    the

    Big

    Five?

    Let us for a moment make the reasonable as-

    sumption (see Latham, 2006; Schmidt & Hunter,

    1998,2000)thattheresearchfindingsinthesethree

    areas are truly important to managers.12 Further

    inpractitioner and bridgeperiodicals a result of

    academics reluctance topublish in such outlets?

    Many researchers believe that this is at least par-

    tially the case, arguing that there are insufficientincentivesforacademicstopublishinpractitioneroutlets (e.g., Shapiro, Kirkman, & Courtney, 2007;Vermeulen,2007).However,academicsatthemost

    prestigious

    schools

    (e.g.,

    Harvard;

    Stanford;

    MIT;

    UC, Berkeley; the University of Pennsylvania) do

    notseemreluctanttopublishinHBRorotheruni-

    versity-sponsoredpublications thatarespecifically

    designed to bridge the gaps between research and

    practice (e.g., the California Management Reviewand MIT Sloan Management Review). Similarly,academics are strongly represented in the bridgepublication HRM. Finally, the fact that they are

    poorlyrepresentedinHRMagazineseemstobeas

    mucharesultofeditorialpolicyasacademicslackofinterestinpublishingthere.13

    Still,itseemsclearthattheunderlyingincentivestructures

    in

    business

    schools,

    along

    with

    many

    academics limited interest in producing articlesfor practitioner or bridge publications, are a sub-

    stantialpartoftheproblem.Inaddition,however,

    some who have studied the problem believe that

    some academics are not so much uninterested as

    fearfulofattemptingtomovefrompurelyacademicpublishingdomainstothosethataremoreoriented

    toward practitioners. For example, Vermeulen(2007)notedthatformanyseasonedacademics,it

    is actually much harder to write bridge or practi-

    tioner articles than academic ones. Although this

    may

    not

    be

    true

    for

    everyone,

    there

    is

    little

    doubtthatdifferentskillsareinvolvedandthattheytake

    timeandefforttodevelop(e.g.,Staw,1995).Carry-

    ing

    the

    argument

    somewhat

    further,

    Markides

    wor-

    riedthatbyattemptingtodevelopincentives andsystems that encourage both academic and mana-gerially relevant research, we may get ourselves

    stuckinthemiddle(2007:763)forexample,by

    presenting an inconsistent image to others or by

    failingtomasterthetrade-offsinherentinnotonly

    different, but in some ways incompatible, activi-

    ties.Totheextentthatfearfulnessisafactortobe

    reckoned

    with,

    academics

    might

    benefit

    from

    more

    explicitactivitiesdesignedtodevelopthecraftof

    being simultaneously rigorous and relevant (e.g.,

    Shapiro et al., 2007; Tushman & OReilly, 2007;

    Vermeulen,2007).

    questionsthusarise,suchas:Isthelackofcoverage

    trait or construct in all of psychology, and the most12

    Forexample,withrespecttointelligence,SchmidtandHuntersay:Noothertraitnotevenconscientious-nesspredicts so many important real-world outcomessowell.Inthissense,intelligenceisthemostimportant

    successful traitinappliedpsychology(2000:4).13

    Anelectronicmailcommunication fromtheeditor

    ofHRMagazineonDecember13,2000,tothefirstauthorsuggeststhispolicy.

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    15/23

    2007

    Rynes,

    Giluk,

    and

    Brown 1001

    Anotherimportantquestiontoaskisthefollow-

    ing: Even if we assume that the research results

    examined in thispaper are important forpracti-

    tioners,areHRacademicsproducingworkwithan

    optimal mix of research topics? Research on theutilityofusingvalidselectionsystemsleaveslittle

    doubt that getting the right people into the right

    organizations

    and

    the

    right

    jobs

    can

    make

    a

    big

    difference (Schmidt & Hunter, 1981; Tenopyr,

    1981). Popular business publications have deliv-

    eredasimilarmessageregardingselectioninthe

    bestseller Good to Great, for example, Collins(2001) wrote about the importance of getting theright people on the busyet most practitioners

    still arent aware of some of the most important

    findings from selection research. Given this, is it

    fruitful for HR researchers to continue to pursueever-more

    specialized

    knowledge

    on

    selection

    techniques?

    We

    believe

    it

    is

    time

    for

    a

    serious

    discussion

    aboutwhethertheacademicmarketplaceforideasis producing an optimal solution with respect to

    academic HR research. First, a case can be made

    that scholars have already gathered most of the

    low-hanging fruit that is likely to make a bigdifferencetoselectionoutcomes(e.g.,generalizable

    validity of intelligence, conscientiousness, and

    structured interviews). As such, additional effort

    might be more profitably expended on better dis-seminating this research, or on studying some of

    theareasthatDeadrickandGibson(2007)foundto

    be

    of

    far

    greater

    interest

    to

    practitioners

    (e.g.,

    com-

    pensationandrewards).Second,thereisatleastsomeevidencethatboth

    the academic and practitioner markets are calling

    forarebalancingofHRresearch.Forexample,one

    consultancysurveypresentedatthe2006AcademyofManagementmeetingssuggestedthatthetopfive

    research needs of HR vice presidents were execu-

    tive compensation, compensation and benefits,

    special skills development, leadership develop-

    ment, and outsourcing (Fay, 2007). Similarly, inourownsurveyoftheJournalofAppliedPsychol-ogy,

    Personnel

    Psychology,

    HRM,

    and

    AMJ

    editorial

    boards, we asked, What are five fundamentalquestionsthatHRresearchershaveyettoanswer?A first-pass sort of the responses into categories

    yieldedthefollowingfivemajorthemes:

    How can/should HR systems be aligned with

    strategy and how can they be made internallyconsistent?

    How do HR practices affect firm performance(e.g.,processesandcausaldirections)?

    What are the most important contingencies orcontextual moderators of HR practice-perfor-

    mancerelationships?

    What are the trade-offs involved in various HR

    policy decisions (e.g., validity versus diversity,fitversusflexibility,personalizedtreatmentver-

    susfairness)?

    Why

    does

    HR

    have

    such

    low

    status

    in

    organiza-

    tions,andwhatcanbedoneaboutit?

    A few thoughts come to mind in looking at thislist.First,theseseemtobeverybigpictureques-tions.(Insayingthis,though,weshouldpointoutthat the individual items comprising some of thecategories were often much more micro; for in-stance,individualswonderedaboutspecificpolicy

    trade-offsorparticularcontingencies.)Second,the

    questions seem to be framed mostly at the organi-

    zational, rather than individual, level of analysis,

    linking to the types of strategic HR issues dis-cussed

    by

    Becker

    and

    Gerhart

    (1996)

    and

    Becker

    andHuselid (2006). Third, they are questions thatwould seem to be of great interest to practitioners

    aswell as academics. Indeed, Becker and Huselid

    (2006) argued that the growth of strategic HR re-

    search has increased managerial interest in HRsacademic findings. Moreover, they suggested that

    strategicHRwasofbroadinterestinsidetheacad-

    emy as well, pointing to the fact that among allarticles published inAMJ since 1990, three of the

    ten most highly cited articles were in the area of

    strategicHR:BeckerandGerhart(1996),Deleryand

    Doty

    (1996),

    and

    Huselid

    (1995).However,thenumberofunresolvedissuesthat

    are related to contingencies, nonlinearities, trade-

    offs

    and

    so

    on

    also

    seems

    to

    raise

    some

    steep

    chal-

    lenges for evidence-based management. Althoughmeta-analysis holds the promise of identifyingmainoraverageeffectsthatcanprovideabasis

    for managerial action (Rousseau, 2006), concepts

    suchascontingencies,configurations,complexity,

    equifinality, and trade-offs all raise questions

    abouttheextenttowhichaveragefindingscanbeusefully generalized (e.g., Benbya & McKelvey,

    2007;

    Cappelli

    &

    Sherer,

    1991;

    Doty,

    Glick,

    &

    Hu-

    ber,1993).Thus,anotherchallengeforHRacadem-

    icsisthis:Istheexistenceofconfigural,contingent,

    orpath-dependent effects sufficiently extensive as

    tomakeevidence-basedmanagement(ormanage-

    mentasscience)impractical?

    Finally, a somewhat related question is this:

    Given that scientific findings changeand some-timesevenreversethemselvesovertime,howdowe(oreven,shouldwe)persuademanagerstouseourbestavailablescientificevidence?Thisissueis certainly not unique to HR management, as a

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    16/23

    1002

    Academy

    of

    Management

    Journal October

    glance at a recent news story (Study Now SaysEstrogenIsSafe)shows:

    Nearly five years after government scientists told

    women that taking estrogen replacement therapyincreased their risk of heart attacks and strokes,researchershaveconcludedthatthedrugsareben-eficialformanyafterall.Continuinganalysisofthe

    original

    data

    indicates

    that

    the

    researchers

    raised

    afalse alarm for most women and that, if women

    begintakingthehormonesshortlyaftermenopause,thedrugsdonotraisetheriskofheartdiseaseand,infact,mightlowerit.Thelatestpieceofevidence,intodaysNewEnglandJournalofMedicine,showsthat taking estrogen for seven years or more aftermenopausereducescalcificationofthearteriesoneofthekeyindicatorsofatherosclerosisbyasmuchas60%.(WashingtonPost/LATimes,2007:1A)

    Similar examples of flip-flopping are readilyidentifiable in HR. For example, during the late

    1970s

    and

    early

    1980s,

    HR

    underwent

    a

    dramaticshiftfromthereigningdogmaofsituational spec-

    ificitytooneofbroadgeneralizabilityformanyphenomena,butmostnotably,fortheroleofintel-ligence in job performance (Schmidt & Hunter,1981). Similarly, the oldnotion thatpersonalitywas a poor predictor of performance has been re-

    placed with the new notion that conscientious-ness is a generalizable predictor of performance

    and that other Big Five traits also predict perfor-manceincertainbroadclassesofjobs(e.g.,Barrick&Mount,1991).Similarly,theoldnotionthatsat-isfaction and performance are virtually unrelatedhas

    been

    replaced

    with

    the

    new

    finding

    that

    the

    truemeancorrelationbetweensatisfactionandper-

    formanceis.30(Judge,Thoresen,Bono,&Patten,2001).Finally,eventhemeta-analyticfindingthat

    structuredinterviewsaremorevalidthanunstruc-turedones(e.g.,McDanieletal.,1994)hasrecentlybeen challenged (Oh, Postlethwaite, Schmidt, &McDaniel,2007).GiventheapparentinstabilityofsomeofthemajorscientificHRfindings,whataretheimplicationsforourroleaspotentialadvisorstopractice?

    QuestionsandChallengesforHRPeriodicals

    Althoughthecurrentfindingsraiseseriousques-

    tions for HR academics, they also provide chal-lengesforHRperiodicals.Oneofthefirstquestionsthat comes to mind is this:Arepractitioner andbridgejournalsdoingenoughtoeducatetheirread-ers about how to evaluate the strength of variousclaims? One of the patterns we observed in bothHBRandHRMagazinewastheoverwhelmingten-dency to focus on claims and testimonials from

    individuals that were unsupported by any refer-

    encestoempiricalevidence.Intheabsenceofsuch

    evidence, readers are left completely to their own

    devicesinchoosinghowtodecideamongcompet-ing claims. Evidence suggests that under such cir-cumstances,peoplearelikelytochoosetheclaimsthat most closely conform with their prior beliefs

    (e.g.,

    Tetlock,

    2000).

    As

    a

    result,

    the

    odds

    that

    any-

    one will actually learn something new or change

    his or her behavior as a result of reading such

    periodicalswouldseemtobequitesmall.14

    Onceagain,thisissueishardlyrestrictedtocov-

    erage of HR research. Indeed, issues of the rela-tivecredibilityofcompetingscientificclaimshavebecome part of the national political conversa-tion (e.g., Begley, Conant, Stein, Clift, & Philips,

    2007; Gore, 2007; Mooney, 2006; Sarnoff, 2001).

    Assciencebecomesmoreandmoresubjecttoma-

    nipulation by commercial and political interests,

    peoplebecomeincreasingly accustomedtotheideathat

    you

    can

    find

    an

    expert

    who

    will

    argue

    anything

    and that no ones point of view is more valuablethananyoneelses(Gutek,1997).However,asCarl

    Sagan once said, If all ideas have equal validity

    thenyouarelost,becausethen,itseemstome,no

    ideashaveanyvalidityatall(quotedinShermer,2002:vi).

    Thus, it seems to us that any periodical that

    aspirestobeeducationalhasasocialobligationtofindwaysofdifferentiating amongthestrengthsof

    alternative claims.Moreover,wethinkthisobliga-

    tion is particularly important for periodicals that

    are

    associated

    with

    educational

    institutions

    or

    pro-fessional associations, since both types of institu-

    tions are important in privileging certain lines of

    thought

    and

    delegitimizing

    others

    (e.g.,

    Green-

    wood,Suddaby,&Hinings,2002).Intheabsenceofempirical evidence, the only clues of credibilityoffered to readers are the presumed status of the

    speakers,asindicatedbytitlesand/orshortbiogra-

    phies. Thus, like Abrahamson and Eisenman

    (2001), we believe that it is necessary to educate

    practitioners about how to better evaluate claims,

    evidence,andresearchfindings.This,ofcourse,is

    not

    just

    a

    challenge

    for

    periodicals

    alone,

    but

    also

    for researchers themselves, to use as many means

    as possible (teaching, consulting, and transla-

    tionsforpractitionerandbridgejournals).

    Inaddition,wewonderwhetherpractitionerand

    bridgejournals might do more toprofessionalize

    practitioners through increased coverage of ab-

    14Of course, the likelihood of behavioral change is

    oftenquitesmallevenwhenstrongevidenceisprovided(e.g.,Sherman,Nelson,&Steele,2000).

  • 8/10/2019 The very separate worlds of academyc and practitioners[1].docx

    17/23

    2007

    Rynes,

    Giluk,

    and

    Brown 1003

    str


Recommended