+ All Categories
Home > Documents > The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1....

The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1....

Date post: 22-Sep-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 0 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
50
Videostyles of Televised Presidential Debates in the U.S. and Korea Hyoungkoo Khang, Ph. D Lecturer Hankuk University of Foreign Studies . INTRODUTION Chaffee (1975) conceived of the role of communication in the political process as political communication, which involves the exchange of symbols and messages among political actors and institutions, the general public, and the news media (McLeod, Kosicki, & McLeod, 2002). We can assume that political communication will be more accelerated and intensify during political campaigning because political campaigns are periods of time during which candidates for public office transmit information to potential voters via mass media in an attempt to create support for their candidacies and try to convince voters that they should vote in a particular way on election day. 1
Transcript
Page 1: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

Videostyles of Televised Presidential Debates in the U.S. and Korea

Hyoungkoo Khang, Ph. D

Lecturer

Hankuk University of Foreign Studies.

INTRODUTION

Chaffee (1975) conceived of the role of communication in the political process as

political communication, which involves the exchange of symbols and messages among

political actors and institutions, the general public, and the news media (McLeod,

Kosicki, & McLeod, 2002). We can assume that political communication will be more

accelerated and intensify during political campaigning because political campaigns are

periods of time during which candidates for public office transmit information to

potential voters via mass media in an attempt to create support for their candidacies and

try to convince voters that they should vote in a particular way on election day.

Echoing the criticism that television news’s “horse race” coverage of political

campaigns, in which they focus on who is winning rather than on the issues with

increasingly shorter sound bites, candidates have turned to another form of political

communication, candidate debates, to convey their messages and images to the voters

(Just, Crigler, & Wallach, 1990). Indeed, studies have shown that political debates are

more effective than network news in providing issue information to voters. Viewers also

can learn information about candidates (including their names, parties, images, and

positions on issues) from candidate debates.

After the historic first presidential debates between Kennedy and Nixon in 1960

and the 1976 debates between Ford and Carter, debates have become one of the most

1

Page 2: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

significant events in political campaigning (Chaffee, 1979). Debates can reach large

audiences (Pfau, 2003), and attract the greatest media coverage of any single campaign

event (Kaid, McKinney, & Tedesco, 2000). Debates also provide voters the most

convenient and direct access to the candidates, and offer a capsule summary of campaign

issues (Carlin, 1992). Presidential debates, thus, generate the greatest amount of public

interest and more citizen-to-citizen discussion than any other single campaign event

(Patterson, 2002).

While political candidate debates have played a dominant role in U.S. elections for

decades, “American-style” political debates have gained significance in the political

processes of other democracies (McKinney & Carlin, 2004). Several comparative studies

show that American campaign styles have been dominant in other countries, although

concurrently, different parameters of culture, political systems, and media systems can

promote or constrain symbols and messages exchanged between political candidates and

voters via political debates (Kaid & Holtz-Bacha, 1995).

Indeed, there have been few studies that compare and contrast political debates

across various cultural boundaries to unveil common patterns and highlight crucial

differences. For this study, the researcher utilized videostyles (Kaid & Johnston, 2001) to

explore similarities and differences in presidential debates between the two countries.

The purpose of this study is, therefore, to present an alternative way of looking at

communication by analyzing and categorizing cultural assumptions as they are found in

American culture in comparison and contrast with those in the Korean counterpart.

LITERATURE REVIEW

2

Page 3: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

Political Campaign Debates

Some scholars have suggested that presidential debates offer viewers the chance to

see the major candidates in a face-to-face confrontation while addressing the same topics

(Benoit & Wells, 1996; Hellweg et al., 1992). Jamieson (1987) explained that “as

messages running an hour or longer, debates offer a level of contact with candidates

clearly unmatched in spot ads and news segments” (p. 28). Kraus (2000) also argued that

presidential debates “serve the majority of the electorate better than any candidates’

personality and their positions on the issues” (p. 5).

Many studies have addressed the question of “Do debates matter?” since general

election televised presidential debates commenced in 1960. According to Bystrom,

Roper, Gobets, Massey, and Beall (1991), presidential debate research has focused on

three main areas: (1) the effects of decided versus undecided voters; (2) the effects of

viewer’s perceptions on candidates’ issues and images; (3) the effects of preexisting

candidate preferences and party affiliation, especially concerning “who won” the debate

in the war of persuasive attack and defense.

Pfau (2002) argued that presidential debates conducted during the general election

enhance viewer knowledge of the candidates and their issue positions. Holbrook (1996)

reported that presidential debates influence candidate evaluations and voting intentions.

His results demonstrated that receivers’ evaluations of candidate performance in TV

debates significantly influence post-debate vote intention. Some scholars argue that

exposure to debates may convey only minimal knowledge, but stimulates campaign

interest and discussion in formulating voting decision (McLeod, Bybee, & Durall, 1979).

Although there are mixed results about the effects of the televised presidential

debates, voters may obtain impressions about how candidates prepared for the debates.

3

Page 4: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

Voters may learn about issues and the personalities of the candidates, or about the

candidates’ political parties (Sears & Chaffee, 1979).

Using functional theory (Benoit, 1998), that political campaign messages have

three basic functions: acclaiming (self-praise), attacking (downgrade opponents), and

defending (response to attacks); Benoit and other scholars have examined the content of

debates (Benoit, 1999; Benoit, Blaney, & Pier, 2000, Benoit & Brazeal, 2002). In their

analysis of the 1988 Bush-Dukakis presidential debates, Benoit and Brazeal (2002) found

that acclaims were the most common function (59%), followed by attacks (33%), and

defense (8%). Overall, debates focus overwhelmingly on campaign issues, rather than

candidate character, and candidates in debates acclaim far more than attack their

opponents, with attacks outnumbering defenses (McKinney & Carlin, 2004).

Some scholars argue, however, that political debate research tends to

overemphasize the content of debates and ignore communication form (Hellweg et al.,

1992). Although contemporary television is a unique communication medium that

emphasizes visual component over verbal dimension (Graber, 1987; Jamieson, 1988), the

visual content of debates is certainly not reflected in the existing analysis of campaign

debates (McKinney & Carlin, 2004).

In his analysis of the visual component of television in the 1976 debates, Tiemens

(1978) found an imbalanced visual presentation of the two candidates (i.e., camera shot),

with Jimmy Carter favored over Ford. Davis (1978), however, found that Ford and Carter

had a similar frequency of eye contact with the camera throughout their three debate

series. McKinney and Carlin (2004) argued that visual presentation of debates may very

well affect how and what viewers learn from debate viewing. In a similar view, Lang

4

Page 5: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

(1987) noted, “what matters most in debates is not substance of what the candidates

say… but how well they say it and whether the candidate projects the image he strives to

project”(p. 211).

Presidential debates have also changed dramatically in format and production

techniques. The design and practice of presidential debates have evolved significantly

from the earlier press conference format to a single moderator questioning candidates, the

town hall debate featuring citizen questioners, and the more informal candidate round-

table conversation or “chat” debate utilized for the first time in 2000 (McKinney &

Carlin, 2004).

The visual presentation and nonverbal dynamics of a town hall debate differ greatly

from those of a traditional candidate-behind-podium debate. The town-hall debate is

viewed as better, addressing issues of greatest concern to voters (McKinney & Carlin,

2004). Issues raised by citizens in a town-hall debate generally resulted in much more

salience in the public mind than did questions asked by a journalist in the single

moderator debate (Kaid et al., 2000). In his analysis of debate formats during the 1984

Democratic primary, Pfau (1984) also found that different debate formats produced

differences in communication outcomes. Some studies (Carlin, Howard, Stanfield, &

Reynolds, 1991; Carlin et al., 2001) have demonstrated several relationships between

candidate clash and debate formats.

Videostyle. Kaid and Davidson (1986) originally conceived the videostyle

perspective as a way of understanding the three major components of a candidate's

presentation style used in political advertising: verbal, nonverbal and video production

component. While verbal content elements focus on the semantic characteristics of the

5

Page 6: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

candidate’s messages (i.e., issue vs. image, negative vs. positive, or explicit strategy),

nonverbal components of political advertising deal with the visual and audio elements

without specific semantic meaning (i.e., gestures, facial expressions, or sounds). As a

third component of videostyle, production techniques include camera angles and

movement, staging and setting, editing, and other techniques that can cause us to interpret

what we see in different ways.

Although the personal style of a candidate exists outside of mediated forms of

communication, a candidate might necessarily adapt to elements of television language to

reach voters through presidential debates or televised political advertising (Kaid &

Johnston, 2001). Therefore, patterns of techniques used and strategies employed should

be made evident by evaluating the candidate’s debates. Videostyle can be understood as a

framework that has been used to analyze televised political debates and to describe the

way candidates present themselves to voters through the television medium,

encompassing the strategies, narratives, and symbols that candidates decide to use in

political debates (Kaid & Johnston, 2001). The visual aspects of debates are thus a very

important part of a candidate’s videostyle.

Cultural Assumptions

Foschi and Hales (1979) point out that when culture is treated as a theoretical

variable “culture ‘x’ and culture ‘y’ serve to operationally define a characteristic ‘a’,

which two cultures exhibit to different degrees” (p. 246). There are dimensions on which

cultures can be different or similar that can be used to explain communication across

cultures. Communication is unique within each culture, and at the same time, there are

systematic similarities and differences across cultures.

6

Page 7: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

A number of scholars in various disciplines have developed schemas that have

shown to be relatively effectual in comparing culture (i.e., Hofstede’s, 1980, dimensions

of cultural variability; Kluckhohn & Strodtbeck’s, 1961, value orientations; Parsons &

Shils’s 1951, pattern variables). Gudykunst (1987) refers to these schemas as dimensions

of socio-cultural variability that affect the specific values and norms that predominate in

different cultures. In applying such schemas, it is important to remember that although

individual differences exist in every culture, the purpose of these dimensions is to explore

the dominant socio-cultural orientations for any given group. Although there are many

dimensions on which cultures differ, this research will focus on Hall’s notion of high-low

context culture, Hofstede’s dimensions of cultural variability, and nonverbal behaviors.

Hofstede (1983, 2001) empirically derived four dimensions of socio-cultural

variability: individualism-collectivism, uncertainty avoidance, power distance, and long-

term versus short-term orientation.

Individualism-collectivism is the major dimension of cultural variability used to

explain cross-cultural similarities and differences in behavior. Individualism-collectivism

exists at the cultural level (i.e., cultural norms/rules) and the individual level (i.e.,

individual values). For the objective of this study, the researcher intends to focus on the

cultural level individualism-collectivism.

Emphasis is placed on individual goals in individualistic cultures, while group

goals have precedence over individual goals in collectivistic cultures. The emphasis in

individualistic societies is on individual initiative and achievement, while emphasis is

placed on belonging to groups in collectivistic societies. This dimension is expected to

affect communication mainly through its influence on group identities and the

7

Page 8: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

differentiation between in-group and out-group communication (Gudykunst & Kim,

1992). In collectivistic culture, social control over individual behavior is stringently

reinforced through the maintenance of rigid hierarchical structure. This narrows the range

of acceptable individual variation in social behavior (Stewart, 1972). However,

Americans tend to believe that the individual has control of, and is responsible for, his or

her own life. In this cultural pattern, competition is encouraged and frontal attack is

considered as a matter of course (Stewart, 1972). Hofstede identified the U.S. as most

individualistic, while Korea as highly collectivistic. A recent study, however, suggests

that Koreans have as high a level of individualistic values as Americans, which implies

that traditional Korean values might have changed rather drastically since Hofstede’s

seminal study (Yoon, Kim, & Kim, 1998).

Another dimension that distinguishes Western from Eastern culture is uncertainty

avoidance. Uncertainty avoidance captures the cultural pattern of seeking stability,

predictability, and low stress rather than change and new experiences (Hofstede, 2001).

In other words, it involves a lack of tolerance in a socio-cultural system for ambiguity

and uncertainty, which expresses less tolerance for people or groups with deviant ideas or

behaviors and greater need for formal rules and absolute truth. People in high uncertainty

avoidance cultures try to avoid ambiguity. While Korean culture is placed high on the

uncertainty avoidance scale, American culture is low in uncertainty avoidance.

Power distance is the degree to which people tolerate inequality of power

distribution. In a high power distance society, hierarchy is strong and power is centralized

at the top. Individuals are very conscious of their rank, and superiors and subordinates

feel separate from one another. Korea is an example of a high power distance society. In

8

Page 9: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

a low power distance society, members of an organization feel relatively close to one

another and have a sense of equality as human beings.

In addition, Hofstede (1991) added time orientation to the former four dimensions.

This dimension focuses on the long-term versus short-term orientation that is related to

the choice of focus for people’s efforts: the future or the present. Generally, people from

East Asian countries such as China, Japan and Korea tend to have tradition based

orientation, while Latin Americans are more oriented to the present, and Western

countries such as Americans, Canadians and Europeans have more of a future orientation.

While individualism-collectivism defines broad differences between cultures,

Hall’s (1976) high-low context notion focuses on cultural differences on a basis of

communication processes. Although some scholars have argued that all the cultures Hall

labels as low-context are individualistic, and high-context are collectivistic in Hofstede’s

scheme (Gudykunst & Ting-Toomey, 1988), this notion has been one of the most well

known dimensions used to describe and account for cultural differences. Hall focuses on

communication patterns within cultures along the four dimensions of context, space,

time, and information flow. High context communication is characterized by a dominant

dependence on implicit, rather than explicit, message. Meaning in a high context culture

is derived primarily from the physical context or, is internalized within individuals in the

culture. In contrast, communication in low context cultures relies primarily on verbal

codes. Hall describes the United States and some Western countries as low context

cultures and Korea, Japan and Taiwan as high context culture (Gudykunst & Kim, 1992).

During everyday communication, nonverbal behaviors (i.e., dress, gestures, eye

contact, or intonation) are typically coordinated in ways that provide for their mutual

9

Page 10: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

performance. Historically, however, verbal and nonverbal messages have been studied

separately, as though they were independent rather than co-occurring and interrelated

phenomena (Jones & LeBaron, 2001). Research has strongly suggested that major

cultural differences are embedded in nonverbal behaviors, because they are basic core

values that are slow to change (Kim, 1992).

Everyday experience suggests that smiling is one of the most common nonverbal

signals used for communication among humans (Ekman & Friesen, 1971). While a smile

of happiness may be a universal expression, cultural, contextual, and personal influences

can affect its meaning and frequency, as well as degree of expansiveness (Kim, 1992).

Many Asian cultures suppress facial expression as much as possible. This has made them

seem less communicative and less expressive in both verbal and nonverbal behavior than

Americans (Yim, 1970). Adults, especially men, are not expected to smile frequently in

Korea because it seems to be a sign of shallowness and lack of respect.

Hand and arm gestures are another form of culture-specific expression that

indicates the intensity of an emotional state. Clearly, there are differences among cultural

groups as to what is considered an appropriate frequency and style of hand and arm

gestures (Eisenberg & Smith, 1971). In the Eastern view, adults who use many gestures

when they speak are thought to be childish, because repression of overt bodily expression

connotes self-control (Ramsey, 1984). On the other hand, Western culture is likely to use

strong expressive gestures to convey messages.

Cross-Cultural Perspectives of Political Debates

While political debates have been a dominant part of U.S. political campaigns, they

have not gained significance in the political culture of most other countries until recently.

This may be a result of differences in the institutional and political systems, cultural

10

Page 11: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

tradition, or history of democracy in the countries. As Kaid (2004) noted, most European

systems are characterized by multiparty systems, shorter campaign periods, party-

centered political culture, and public-controlled media. Compared to their European

counterparts, democracies in Asian and African countries have a relatively short history.

Seib (2000) noted that debates can symbolize the coming of political maturity, where

democracy is young and struggling. This assertion can be well illustrated by the 1994

debate in South Africa between Mandela and Klerk.

Although a number of countries have followed the U.S. in developing televised

debate traditions of their own, few studies of televised campaign debates occurring in the

democracies of the world (McKinney & Carlin, 2004): analysis of televised debates in

Canada (Blais & Boyer, 1996), in the former West Germany (Baker & Norpoth, 1981;

Schrott, 1990), in South Korea (Kang & Jaung, 1999), in Australia (Ward & Walsh,

1999), in Israel (Blum-Kulka & Liebes, 1999), and in New Zealand (Clark, 1999).

Furthermore , a majority of these studies failed to adopt diverse approaches across

national boundaries; except for Schrott’s (1984) comparative study of the 1980 U.S. and

West German debates and Matsaganis and Weingarten’s (2001) comparison of the 2000

U.S. presidential and Greek prime minister debates.

Matsagains and Weingarten explored the similarities and differences in format,

style, journalistic involvement, and issues covered in the first presidential debate of the

2000 U.S. election campaign and the Greek Prime Minister debate. Although there are

few similarities in terms of the candidates’ style, they found that political candidates used

knowledge of geography, culture, and history to formulate their campaigns and convey

their messages to the voters.

11

Page 12: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

Political Candidate Debates in Korea

Televised debates among presidential candidates began with the 1997 election

campaign since new election laws prohibited outdoor mass rallies during the campaign.

They instead encouraged candidates to rely more on a mass mediated campaign.

According to the 1997 Korean Election survey, more than 80 percent of the respondents

said that television had some, if not decisive, influences on their voting decision. This

implies that the first televised debates were highly effective and also influenced campaign

strategies of candidates. In the 2002 Korean presidential election, candidate debates were

held three times during the official campaign periods. Thus, the history of televised

political debates is quite short in Korea.

Scholars have relatively more interest in the effects of political debates on the

audience over the content of the televised debates. Using panel survey methods, Cheong

(2003) examined the influence of the presidential debates on voters’ cognition, and found

that televised debates activate remarks related to voters’ cognition of candidates. Lee

(2004) also conducted a survey to explore the effects of debates on voters’ change in

candidate image perception. The finding showed that the debates did affect the viewers’

change in the image of presidential candidates.

Hypotheses and Research Questions

The first group of hypotheses concerning verbal or message components of

videostyle is based on Hall’s (1981) differentiations of cultures on the basis of the

communication context continuum. In a low-context culture like the U.S., most

information is contained in the explicit verbal messages. Members of the low-context

culture tend to communicate in a direct manner and express conflict or dissatisfaction

openly using facts and data. On the other hand, in a high-context culture like Korea, most

12

Page 13: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

information is encoded in the physical context or internalized in the person, while little is

in the coded or explicit messages avoiding confrontation (De Mooij, 1997).

The cultural norms of individualism-collectivism and uncertainty avoidance can

also be utilized for the first group of hypotheses. Members of collectivistic cultures

suppress emotional displays that are contradictory to the mood of the group, because

maintaining group affect is a primary value, while members in individualistic cultures are

encouraged to express emotions because individual freedom tends to be considered as an

important value (Andersen, 1988). In a society of high uncertainty avoidance, members

in the society express less tolerance for people or groups with deviant behaviors and

greater need for formal rules (Hofstede, 1983). On the basis of these expectations, it is

hypothesized as follows:

H1-1: U.S. presidential candidates will emphasize issue more than image compared to Korean presidential candidates.

H1-2: U.S. presidential candidates will use more negative appeals than positive appeals compared to Korean presidential candidates.

H1-3: U.S. presidential candidates will rely more on logical appeal than Korean presidential candidates.

H1-4: U.S. presidential candidates will rely more on emotional appeal than Korean presidential candidates.

H1-5: U.S. presidential candidates will rely less on ethical appeal than Korean presidential candidates.

Along with the first set of hypotheses, the researcher is interested in the nature of

negative attacks, if candidates use negative appeals. In addition, it would be interesting to

examine if there are some differences in the purpose of the negative attacks. Thus, the

following research question was posed.

RQ1: What are significant similarities and/or differences in the uses and nature of negative attacks that candidates rely on, between the U.S. and Korea?

13

Page 14: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

The second aspect of videostyle relates to nonverbal content, and scholars have

long acknowledged the importance of nonverbal and visual cues. Many of these elements

are particularly important to the way candidates present themselves (Kaid & Davidson,

1986). People in Asian cultures are used to being reluctant in showing their emotions

(Andersen, Hecht, Hoobler, & Smallwood, 2003). In contrast, people in individualistic

cultures smile more than in normality oriented culture (Tompkins, 1984). This has made

Koreans seem less communicative and expressive in facial expressions and body

movements. In the U.S., eye contact indicates a degree of attention and interest, and

influences attitude change or persuasion, but Koreans tend to be uncomfortable with

direct and intensive eye contact until the relationship is firmly established (Andersen et

al., 2003). These cultural contexts lead to the second set of hypotheses as follows:

H2-1: U.S. presidential candidates will smile more than Korean presidential candidates.

H2-2: U.S. presidential candidates will use more body movements than Korean presidential candidates.

H2-3: U.S. presidential candidates will use more eye contact than Korean presidential candidates.

METHODOLOGY

A content analysis of the presidential debates was undertaken to analyze the verbal,

nonverbal components of the presidential campaigns in the U.S. and Korea.

Sample

The population of this study was defined as all televised presidential debates in

major network television during the presidential election campaigns in both countries.

The years used for comparison will be between 1992 and 2004 (1992, 1996, 2000, and

2004 for the U.S.; 1997 and 2002 for Korea). Because random sampling is almost

14

Page 15: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

impossible in cross-cultural research (Brislin & Baumgardner, 1971), the decision was

made to perform the analysis with as complete a set of all presidential debate statements

from each election year.

The recordings of U.S. presidential debates obtained from the C-SPAN archives.

The Korean presidential debates were obtained from the Korean Broadcasting System

(KBS) and Mun-Hwa Broadcasting Company (MBC).

The unit of analysis for the coding process was a candidate statement made in

response to a question or a response to a question answered by the other candidate in the

debates. This means that the statements are usually 60 seconds or longer, and each

statement was identifiable as coming from one candidate or the other.

Based on the defined unit of analysis, a total of 1225 samples were yielded. For this

study, the U.S. samples consisted of 671 debate statements, while Korean samples

consisted of 554 debate statements.

Coding Categories

The categories were adapted and modified from videostyle, which incorporates

verbal and nonverbal components. The categories include emphasis of debates (issue

debates emphasizing a policy issue or proposal, and image debates emphasizing the

candidate’s background, characteristics, or qualifications), focus of debates (positive

dimension focused on the candidate acclaiming him/herself or opponent, negative

dimension focused on criticisms of the opponent, including implied comparisons), facial

expressions (smiling, attentive/serious, and glaring/frowning), body movement (hand and

arm gestures; nonuse in which no hands and arms are used or hands are held downward,

mild use in which hands are raised below the shoulder line, and broad use in which hands

15

Page 16: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

are raised above the shoulder line or are very expressive), and eye contact (almost

always, sometimes, almost never).

For the nature of negative attacks in debates, categories include attack on (1)

personal characteristics of opponent, (2) issue stands/consistency of opponent, (3)

candidate's group affiliations/associations, (4) opponent’s background/qualifications and

(5) opponent’s performance in past offices/positions.

For the types of appeals used in debates, categories include logical appeals,

emotional appeals and ethical appeal. Logical appeals can be described as use of evidence

in debates, in which facts are presented in order to persuade viewers by use of statistics,

logical arguments, examples, etc. Emotional appeals in debates are designed to invoke

particular feelings or emotions in viewers. This appeal includes happiness, good will,

pride, patriotism, anger, etc. Ethical appeals emphasize a candidate’s qualifications as a

viable leader. These appeals can be made to enhance the credibility and trustworthiness

of a candidate by presenting all he/she has done, is capable of doing, and how reliable

he/she is.

Coding Procedure and Intercoder Reliability

A total of six coders (four for the U.S. and two for Korea) were hired and trained.

Before the actual coding and analysis of debates began, a reliability test was undertaken

to determine whether the category definitions were clear, objective, and mutually

exclusive, so that coders working independently, could achieve a high level of agreement.

A commonly used measure of intercoder reliability, Scott’s (1955) pi1, was employed in

1

16

Page 17: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

this study. The acceptable point for intercoder reliability estimate of each coding category

was higher than .80 (Hughes & Garrett, 1990).

The intercoder reliability was estimated for the U.S. and Korean coders,

respectively during the training session, and the index ranged from .82 to 1.0, which was

satisfactory. Scott’s pi was 1.0 for name of country, name of candidates, and candidate

status. The index was .90 for emphasis of issue vs. image, .88 for negative vs.

positive, .87 for nature of attack, .82 for type of appeals, 89 for issues, .90 for facial

expression, .91 for body movement, and .86 for eye contact.

Chi-squares for each category by country were run to test for similarities and/or

differences in observed frequencies.

RESULTS

Comparisons of Verbal Components

Hypothesis 1-1 predicted that U.S. presidential candidates would emphasize issue

more than image compared to Korean candidates. As shown in Table 1, there was no

statistically significant difference in emphasizing issues or images across the two

countries. Thus, hypothesis 1-1 was not supported.

Table 1. Emphasis and focus of debatesU.S. (%) Korea (%)

Emphasis Issue 555 (82.7) 439 (79.2)Image 116 (17.3) 115 (20.8)

2 = 2.389 (df=1), p= .066Focus Positive 452 (68.4) 366 (66.8)

Negative 209 (31.6) 182 (32.2) 2 = .347 (df=1), p= .278

Hypothesis 1-2 suggested that U.S. presidential candidates would use more

negative appeals than positive appeals in comparison with Korean candidates. The

17

Page 18: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

hypothesis was not supported. As revealed in Table 1, there is no statistically significant

difference between the two countries.

In Hypothesis 1-3, we predicted that U.S. candidates would rely more on logical

appeals than Korean presidential candidates. This hypothesis was not supported. The

results showed that there was no statistically significant difference in use of logical

appeals across the two countries.

Table 2. Types of appeals U.S. (%) Korea (%)

Logical Present 535 (79.7) 438 (79.1)Absent 136 (20.3) 116 (20.9)

2 = .083 (df=1), p= .387Emotional Present 176 (26.2) 102 (18.4)

Absent 495 (73.8) 452 (81.6) 2 = 10.572(df=1), p= .001Ethical Present 253 (37.7) 64 (11.6)

Absent 418 (62.3) 490 (88.4) 2 = 108.206 (df=1), p= .000

Hypothesis 1-4, maintaining that U.S. candidates would rely more on emotional

appeal than Korean candidates, was confirmed. Chi-square analysis shows that there is a

difference in the use of emotional appeals at a statistically significant level across the two

countries. The results show that American candidates rely more on emotional appeals

than Korean candidates. The finding indicates that American candidates use more

emotional appeals which are designed to invoke particular feelings or emotions, when

compared to Korean counterparts.

In Hypothesis 1-5, we predicted that U.S. candidates would use fewer ethical

appeals than their Korean counterparts. This expectation was not confirmed. The results

showed that U.S. candidates (38%) used more ethical appeals than their Korean

18

Page 19: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

counterparts (12%) at a statistically significant level. The result was significantly

different from our expectations.

Nature of negative appeals.

Table 3 shows that American candidates used significantly more personal

characteristic of opponents than their Korean counterparts. American candidates (13.4%)

used more personal characteristic attacks than Korean candidates (7.6%).

In examining difference in the use of issue stands of opponents, as shown in Table

3, the results show that American candidates used significantly more issue stands of

opponents to make attacks than their Korean counterparts. American candidates (41.3%)

also used more issue stands of opponents than Korean candidates (17.9%) to make an

attack.

Table 3. Nature of negative appeals U.S. (%) Korea (%)

Personal characteristics

Present 90 (13.4) 42 (7.6)Absent 581 (86.6) 512 (92.4)

2 = 10.734 (df=1), p= .001Issue stands Present 277 (41.3) 99 (17.9)

Absent 394 (58.7) 455 (82.1) 2 = 78.187 (df=1), p= .000Group affiliations

Present 56 (8.3) 153 (27.6)Absent 615 (91.7) 401 (72.4)

2 = 79.646 (df=1), p= .000Opponent’s backgrounds

Present 22 (3.3) 49 (8.8)Absent 649 (96.7) 505 (91.2)

2 = 17.219 (df=1), p= .000Opponent’s performance

Present 190 (28.3) 79 (14.3)Absent 481 (71.7) 475 (85.7)

2 = 34.985 (df=1), p= .000

Table 3 shows that Korean candidates in debates used significantly more group

affiliations of opponents than their American counterparts. A larger percentage of Korean

19

Page 20: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

candidates (27.6%) used group affiliations of opponents, while a smaller percentage of

American candidates (8.3%) relied on the negative appeal.

In use of opponent background, Table 3 shows that Korean candidates in debates

used opponent background significantly more than their American counterparts. For

debates, a larger percentage of Korean candidates (8.8%) used opponent background,

In another nature of negative appeals, Table 3 shows that American candidates used

opponent performance significantly more than their Korean counterparts, but only for

debates. For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance,

than their Korean counterparts (14.3%) in using negative appeals.

Regarding the nature of negative appeals, American candidates were likely to use

more personal characteristics, issue stands and opponent performance than their Korean

counterparts to make attacks. In contrast, Korean candidates relied more on group

affiliations and candidate background than the American candidates.

Comparisons of Nonverbal Components

Table 4. Nonverbal components by countryU.S. (%) Korea (%)

Facial expressions

Smiling 48 (7.2) 70 (12.6)Attentive/serious 602 (89.7) 482 (87.0)Frowning/glaring 21 (3.1) 2 (0.4)

2 = 22.108 (df=2), p= .000Body movement

Frequent 238 (35.5) 138 (24.9)Moderate 405 (60.4) 291 (52.5)Never 28 (4.2) 125 (22.6)

2 = 96.470 (df=2), p= .000Eye contact Almost always 495 (73.8) 455 (82.1)

Sometimes 117 (17.4) 19 (3.4)Almost never 59 (8.8) 80 (14.4)

2 = 64.892 (df=2), p= .000

Hypothesis 2-1 predicted that U.S. candidates were expected to smile more than

Korean candidates. As the findings in Table 4 show, the hypothesis was not supported.

20

Page 21: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

The results, instead, indicate that Korean candidates smiled more than American

candidates. The findings indicate that political debates seem to not reflect the expected

cultural orientation toward the use of smiles between two countries. The results showed

that Korean candidates (12.6%) smiled more than their American counterparts (7.2%).

Unlike previous studies that suggest that people in Oriental cultures do not readily show

emotions, and that Korean adults rarely smile, the findings indicate that Korean

candidates smiled more often than their American counterparts.

Hypothesis 2-2 predicted that U.S. candidates would use more body movements

than Korean candidates. The hypothesis was confirmed. Here, the results are in line with

the predicted cultural orientation toward the use of body movements between the two

countries. American candidates (35.5%) showed more body movements than their

Korean counterparts (24.9%).

In line with the last examination of nonverbal component of the videostyle,

Hypothesis 2-3 predicted that U.S. candidates would use more eye contact than Korean

candidates. The hypothesis was not supported. Korean candidates used almost always eye

contact more than their American counterparts at a statistically significant level, which

might contradict the cultural norms this study proposed. A slightly larger percentage of

Korean candidates (82.1%) used almost always eye contact, while a smaller percentage of

the American candidates (73.8%) maintained the eye contact.

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS

This study provided one opportunity to gain better understandings and insights into

how media phenomena are related to cultural orientation by comparing and contrasting

the videostyles of presidential debates across the United States and Korea.

21

Page 22: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

Examination of emphasized and focused verbal components of the debates reveals

that there were no significant differences between the two countries. In terms of verbal

components of the videostyles, these findings indicate that there seems to be little place

in debates for the exhibition of cultural norms. The nature of presidential debates might

overpower cultural norms, which are likely to be embedded in debates. In addition, the

results imply that American-style presidential debates have gained significance in the

political processes in Korea

In a deeper examination of the nature of negative attacks, the results provided

significant differences between the two countries. American candidates relied more on

opponents’ issue stands, past performance in political office, and personal characteristics,

while Korean candidates used significantly more opponents’ group affiliations and

backgrounds to make negative attacks. As Gudykunst and Kim observed (1992), the

emphasis in individualistic societies is on individual initiative and achievement, while

emphasis is placed on belonging to groups in collectivistic societies. In making attacks on

opponents, thus, American politicians tended to focus on an opponent’s issue stands,

personality and performance, believing that the individual has control of, and is

responsible for, his or her own life. In comparison, Korean candidates tend to rely more

on group identities of opponents by reflecting the highly collectivistic culture of Korean

society.

Turning to the question of nonverbal components of the videostyle, the findings

showed that American candidates used body movements more often than Korean

candidates. Korean candidates frequently used a limited gesturing of their hands, while

American candidates actively used both slight and broad body movements involving

22

Page 23: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

various gestures. Considering body movements as a mirror of the intensity of emotion,

the American candidates seemed to be more communicative and expressive than Korean

candidates. On the other hand, Koreans seem less communicative and less expressive in

body movements, because repression of overt bodily expressions connotes self-control in

Eastern cultures (Ramsey, 1984).

Compared to the previous research suggesting that people in Oriental cultures do

not readily use facial expressions, the findings of this study reveal that Korean candidates

smiled more than their American counterparts at large. As Porter and Samovar (1998)

argued, members of high power distance cultures tend to smile more in order to appear

polite or appease superiors. In addition to the cultural norm, Korean candidates’

increasing emphasis on candidate image and the television medium might be a good

rationale for the findings. Beginning with the presidential election held in 1987, Korean

politics started to apply more sophisticated campaign strategies based on marketing

techniques. With television predisposed to dramatic and visual imagery, Korean

candidates may have come to rely heavily on smiles to convey warmth and construct

favorable images. Although there has been evidence that nonverbal behaviors are

oriented through respective cultural actions, the results of the current study may imply

that presidential candidates in both countries perceived a smile as the social norm of a

universal expression of happiness that can accommodate positive images of presidential

candidates in political debates.

Throughout the content analysis of the presidential debates, to the extent that clear

differences exist between American and Korean cultural patterns, political debates, which

are a conspicuous indicator of cultural values, appear to manifest these differences at

23

Page 24: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

large. It should, however, be kept in mind that there might be other plausible causes for

these differences, such as the political system, the level of technology development, and

nature of communication forms.

The findings of this study also imply that “American-style” political debates have

gained significance in the political processes of Korean democracy, although different

parameters of culture, political systems, and media systems can promote or constrain

symbols and messages exchanged between political candidates and voters. Furthermore,

considering rapid development of new communication technologies, we can observe that

global diffusion of political communication tends to be manifest in Korea and many

democracies where varied aspects such as videostyle of debate have been adopted. The

fact also seems to be in line with findings of this study that Korean candidates showed

more smile and eye contact.

Although the findings of this study do provide new information and insights into

the nature of televised presidential debates in the United States and Korea, some

limitations should be noted when interpreting the results.

A primary limitation of this study is that it is not concerned with the effectiveness

of political debates. Future research needs to analyze the content and effectiveness of

political debates in order to more fully provide valuable information about the

relationship between the two countries and cultural orientations.

Given this limitation, one suggestion for future research is to examine the effects of

political debates on voters between the two countries. Studies employing a robust

experimental design will give further insight into the relationship between media

24

Page 25: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

representations of candidates and cultural values. In addition, we might determine the

degree of cognitive, affective and even behavioral effects on voters across culture.

Future research also should include more countries as representative of the Eastern

and Western cultures. Although they have different political systems, Taiwan and Japan

have similar cultural norms with many perspectives found in Korea. The U.K. would be a

good example of representative of the Western culture and also has similar cultural norms

found in the U.S. In addition, the U.K. has a similar political system to the Japanese

political system. This implementation can give solid generalizability of findings to this

study or yield more varied views.

REFERENCES

Andersen, P. A. (1988). Explaining intercultural differences in nonverbal communication. In L. A. Samovar & R. E. Porter (Eds.), Intercultural communication: A reader. Belmont, CA: Wadworth.

Andersen, P. A., Hecht, M. L., Hoobler, G. D., & Smallwood, M. (2003). Nonverbal communication across cultures. In W. B. Gudykunst (Ed.), Cross-cultural and intercultural communication (pp. 73-90). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Baker, K. L., & Norpoth, H. (1981). Candidates on television: The 1972 electoral debates in West Germany. Public Opinion Quarterly, 45, 329-345.

Benoit, W. L., Blaney, J. R., & Pier, P. M. (2000). Acclaiming, attacking, and defending: A functional analysis of nominating convention keynote speeches, 1960-1996. Political Communication, 17, 61-84.

Benoit, W. L., & Brazeal, L. (2003). A functional analysis of the 1988 Bush-Dukasis debates. Argumentations and Advocacy, 39, 219-233.

Benoit, W. L., & Wells, W. T. (1996). Candidates in conflict: Persuasive attack and defense in the 1992 presidential debates. Tuscaloosa, AL: University of Alabama Press.

Blais, A., & Boyer, M. M. (1996). Assessing the impact of televised debates: The case of the 1988 Canadian election. British Journal of Political Science, 26, 143-164.

Blum-Kulka, S., & Liebes, T. (1999). Peres versus Netanyahu: Television wins thedebate. In S. Coleman (Ed.), Televised election debates. London, UK: McMillan.

25

Page 26: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

Bystrom, D. G., Roper, C., Gobets, R., Massey, T., & Beall, C. (1991). The effects of a televised gubernatorial debate. Political Communication Review, 16, 57-80.

Carlin, D. B., Howard, C., Stanfield, S., & Reynolds, L. (1991). The effects of presidential debate formats on clash: A comparative analysis. Argumentation and Advocacy, 27, 126-136.

Carlin, D. B., Morris, E., & Smith, S. (2001). The influence of format and questions on candidates’ argument choices in the 2000 presidential debates. American Behavioral Scientist, 44, 2196-2218.

Carlin, D. P. (1992). Presidential debates as focal points for campaign arguments. Political Communication, 9, 251-265.

Chaffee, S. H. (1975). Political communication. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Chaffee, S. H. (1979). Approaches of U.S. scholars to the study of televised political debates. Political Communication Review, 5, 19-33.

Cheong, S. (2003). A study of the influence the 16th presidential TV debate on the voters’ cognition change: A panel study analysis. Korean Journal of Mass Communication, 47(6), 220-248.

Davis, L. K. (1978). Camera-eye contact by the candidates in the presidential debates of 1976. Journalism Quarterly, 55, 431-437, 455.

De Mooij, M. (1997). Global marketing and advertising: Understanding cultural paradox. Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage.

Eisenberg, A. M., & Smith, R., Jr. (1971). Nonverbal communication. New York, NY: Bobbs-Merrill.

Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17(2), 124-129.

Foschi, M., & Hales, W. (1979). The theoretical role of cross-cultural comparisons in experimental social psychology. In L. Eckensberger, W. Lonner, & Y. Poortinga (Eds.), Cross-cultural contributions to psychology (pp. 244-254). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.

Gudykunst, W. (1987). Cross-cultural comparisons. In C. Berger & S. Chaffee (Eds.), Handbook of communication science (pp. 847-889). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Gudykunst, W., & Kim, Y. Y. (1992). Communication with strangers: An approach to intercultural communication (2nd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Gudykunst, W., & Ting-Toomey, S. (1988). Culture and interpersonal communication. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

26

Page 27: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

Hall, E. T. (1976). Beyond culture. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.

Hellweg, S. A., Pfau, M., & Brydon, S. R. (1992). Televised presidential debates: Advocacy in contemporary America. Westport, CT: Greenwood.

Hofstede, G. (1983). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Hofstede, G., & Hofstede, G. J. (2005). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind. New York, NY: McGraw Hill.

Holbrook, T. M. (1996). Do campaigns matter? Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Holtz-Bacha, C., & Kaid, L. L. (1995). Television spots in German national elections: Content and effects. In L. L. Kaid & C. Holtz-Bach (Eds.), Political advertising in Western democracies (pp. 61-88). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Hughes, M. A., & Garrett, D. E. (1990). Intercoder reliability estimation approaches in marketing: A generalizability theory framework for quantitative data. Journal of Marketing Research, 27(May), 185-195.

Jamieson, K. H. (1988). Packaging the presidency: A history and criticism of presidential advertising. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Jones, S. E., & LeBaron, C. D. (2001). Research on the relationship between verbal and nonverbal communication: Emerging integrations. Journal of Communication, 51 (September), 499- 521.

Just, M., Crigler, A., & Wallach, L. (1990). Thirty seconds or thirty minutes: What viewers learn from spot advertisement and candidates debates. Journal of Communication, 40(3), 120-133.

Kaid, L. L. (2004). Political advertising. In L. L. Kaid (Ed.), Handbook of political communication research (pp. 479-503). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Kaid, L. L., & Davidson, D. K. (1986). Elements of videostyle: Candidate presentation through television advertising, In L. L. Kaid, D. Nimmo, & K. R. Sanders (Eds.), New perspectives on political advertising (pp. 184-209). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.

Kaid, L. L., & Holtz-Bacha, C. (1995). Political advertising in Western democracies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Kaid, L. L., & Johnston, A. (2001). Videostyles in Presidential campaigns: Style and content of televised political advertising. Westport, CT: Praeger.

27

Page 28: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

Kang, W. T., & Jaung, H. (1999). The 1997 presidential election in South Korea. Electoral Studies, 18(4), 599-608.

Kim, M. S. (1992). A comparative analysis of nonverbal expressions as portrayed by Korean and American print-media advertising. The Howard Journal of Communication, 3, 317-339.

Kluckhohn, F. R., & Strodtbeck, F. L. (1961). Variations in value orientations. Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.

Kraus, S. (2000). Televised presidential debates and public policy (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Lee, G. (2004). The effects of televised presidential debates on voters’ change in candidate image perception: The 2002 Korean presidential election. Korean Journal of Mass Communication, 48(2), 346-474.

Matsaganis, M., & Weingarten, C. (2001). The 2000 U.S. presidential debate versus the 2000 Greek prime minister debate. American Behavioral Scientist, 44, 2398-2409.

McKinney, M. S., & Carlin, D. B. (2004). Political campaign debates. In L. L. Kaid (Ed.). Handbook of political communication research (pp. 203-234). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

McLeod, D. M., Kosicki, G. M., & McLeod, J. M. (2002). Resurveying the boundaries of political communications effects. In Bryant, J. & Zillman, D. (Eds), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 215-267). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

McLeod, J. M., Bybee, C. R., & Durall, J. A. (1979). The 1976 presidential debates and equivalence of informed political participation. Communication Research, 6, 463-487.

Parsons, T., & Shils, E. (1951). Toward a general theory of action. Cambridge, MA: Havard University Press.

Patterson, T. E. (2002). The vanishing voter: Public involvement in an age of uncertainty. New York: Knopf.

Pfau, M. (2002). The subtle nature of presidential debate influence. Argument and Advocacy, 38(Spring): 251-161.

Pfau, M. (2003). The changing nature of presidential debate influence in the new age of mass media communication. Paper presented at the 9th Annual Conference on Presidential Rhetoric, Texas A&M University.

Plasser, F., & Plasser, G. (2002). Global political campaigning: A worldwide analysis of campaign professionals and their practices. Westport, CT: Praeger.

28

Page 29: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

Porter, R. E., & Samovar, L. A. (1998). Cultural differences in emotional expression: Implication for international communication. In P. A. Andersen & L. K. Guerrero (Eds.), Handbook of communication and emotion: Research, theory, applications, contexts (pp. 452-472). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Ramsey, S. (1984). Double vision: Nonverbal behavior East and West. In A. Wolfgang (Ed.), Nonverbal behavior: Perspectives, applications, intercultural insights (pp. 139-167). New York, NY: Hogrefe.

Schrott, P. R. (1984). The content and consequences of the 1980 televised debates in West Germany and the United States. Unpublished manuscript, State University of New York, Stony Brook.

Schrott, P. R. (1990). Electoral consequences of “winning” televised campaign debates. Public Opinion Quarterly, 54, 567-585.

Scott, W. A. (1955). Reliability of content analysis: The case of nominal scaling. Public Opinion Quarterly, 19, 321-325.

Sears, D. O., & Chaffee, S. H. (1979). Uses and effects of the 1976 debates: An overview of empirical studies. In S. Kraus (Ed.), The great debates: Carter vs. Ford, 1976 (pp. 223-261). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

Stewart, E. C. (1972). American cultural patterns: A cross-cultural perspectives. Chicago, IL: International Press.

Tak, J. (2000). A study of Americanization of Korean political advertising. Korean Journal of Advertising Research, 48, 27-44.

Tak, J., Kaid, L. L., & Lee, S. (1997). A cross-cultural study of political advertising in the United States and Korea. Communication Research, 24, 413-430.

Tiemens, R. K. (1978). Television’s portrayal of the 1976 presidential debates: An analysis of visual content. Communication Monographs, 45, 362-370.

Tompkins, S. S. (1984). Affect theory. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotions. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Ward, I., & Walsh, M. (1999). Leaders' debates and presidential politics in Australia. Televised Election Debates: International Perspectives, 43-65.

Yim, S. C. (1970). Customs and folklore. In S. S. Lee, et al. (Eds.), Korean studies today: Development and state of the field (pp. 199-220). Seoul, South Korea: Seoul National University.

Yim, T. S. (2003). Language and verbal communication across cultures. In W. B. Gudykunst (Ed.), Cross-cultural and intercultural communication (pp.53-72). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

29

Page 30: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

Yoon, K., Kim, C., & Kim, M. (1998). A cross-cultural comparison of the effects of source credibility on attitudes and behavioral intentions, Mass Communication & Society, 1(3/4), 153-173.

30

Page 31: The word “democracy” originates from the Greek words “demos” …  · Web view2008. 1. 22. · For debates, more American candidates (28.3%) used opponent performance, than

국문초록

본 연구는 정치커뮤니케이션의 대안적 형태인 대통령 후보자 토론에 반영된

문화적 가치를 연구하고자 했다. 한국과 미국의 대통령 후보자 토론의 비디오

스타일을 비교하고 대조해 봄으로써 정치토론이 어떤 방식으로 개별 문화를

반영하는 매개체로써 두드러진 역할을 하는지를 밝혀내 보고자 했다. 비디오

스타일의 두 가지 요소, 언어적 그리고 비언어적 요소를 내용분석 한 결과, 대통령

후보자 토론회는 각 국가의 문화적 가치, 예를 들자면 high-low context

communication, degree of uncertainty avoidance, and nonverbal expressions 의

관점에서 개별 국가의 특성을 대체로 반영하는 것으로 나타났다. 하지만, 문화적

맥락으로 대부분 설명할 수 있었던 기존 정치광고에 대한 문화비교 연구와는 달리,

텔레비전 정치 토론회는 토론의 본질적인 특성과 미국화가 비디오스타일에

반영되어 문화적 맥락을 바탕으로 한 가설 중 일부가 지지 받지 못하고 있는 것으로

나타났다.

31


Recommended