+ All Categories
Home > Documents > THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR...

THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR...

Date post: 23-Jul-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 2 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
15
The authors wish to express their gratitude to the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation for a Transcoop 1 Grant in support of this research and to an anonymous reviewer for many helpful suggestions. A portion of the research has been presented as The trouble with turns in talking at the International Pragmatics Association conference in Mexico City (July 1996). Pragmatics 7:3.309-323 International Pragmatics Association THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In the course of her BBC interview, Princess Diana stated regarding herself: "She won't go quietly". And yet she did- her interview is preeminently quiet, and at the same time extraordinarily eloquent: "I've got tremendous knowledge about people and about how to communicate". Ever since hundreds of millions of people (Times, November 21, 1995; Kurzon 1996: 217) viewed this telecast, attitudes toward Princess Diana seem to have become even more polarized than before. But we have become convinced that the attention given to her - and to this historic interview - has not been simply idle curiosity about the rich and famous of this world. Matters important to the human condition in our modern culture were illuminated in a genuinely dramatic fashion. And whether one considers the decision of the participants to telecast the interview or the reactions recorded in the international press, the interview must be considered a resounding success. In Germany, for example, the weekly newsmagazine Stern (December 28, 1995) made Princess Diana "the woman of the year" (26) and referred to "the interview of the year" (28), and the Berlin daily newspaper Tagesspiegel commented on her "sensational BBC interview" (December 22, 1995: 4) and included her interview along with Winston Churchill's WW II radio broadcasts as "high points" (August 23, 1996: 23) in BBC history. There are a number of reasons why this interview is both unusual and important for research on language use. Lasting 52 min, 15 sec and involving 301 occasions in which utterances are initiated by interviewer or interviewee, it provides a wealth of analytic material in both the audio and the video modalities for the study of media dialogue. Although Princess Diana is not strictly speaking a politician, her role in the royal family gives her extraordinary political importance and high profile. The topics considered in the interview are intimately personal and sensitive, such as are not ordinarily discussed in public at all. They concern primarily weaknesses rather than strengths of the interviewee who reveals them. By comparison with political interviews that we have studied, this one had a much larger audience, and international press commentaries have indicated that it was well received by a worldwide public. Martin Bashir showed himself to be an exceptionally DOI: 10.1075/prag.7.3.02kow
Transcript
Page 1: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

The authors wish to express their gratitude to the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation for a Transcoop1

Grant in support of this research and to an anonymous reviewer for many helpful suggestions. A portion of

the research has been presented as The trouble with turns in talking at the International Pragmatics

Association conference in Mexico City (July 1996).

Pragmatics 7:3.309-323

International Pragmatics Association

THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION:PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR

IN THE BBC INTERVIEW1

Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell

1. Introduction

In the course of her BBC interview, Princess Diana stated regarding herself: "She won't goquietly". And yet she did- her interview is preeminently quiet, and at the same timeextraordinarily eloquent: "I've got tremendous knowledge about people and about how tocommunicate". Ever since hundreds of millions of people (Times, November 21, 1995;Kurzon 1996: 217) viewed this telecast, attitudes toward Princess Diana seem to havebecome even more polarized than before. But we have become convinced that the attentiongiven to her - and to this historic interview - has not been simply idle curiosity about therich and famous of this world. Matters important to the human condition in our modernculture were illuminated in a genuinely dramatic fashion. And whether one considers thedecision of the participants to telecast the interview or the reactions recorded in theinternational press, the interview must be considered a resounding success. In Germany,for example, the weekly newsmagazine Stern (December 28, 1995) made Princess Diana"the woman of the year" (26) and referred to "the interview of the year" (28), and the Berlindaily newspaper Tagesspiegel commented on her "sensational BBC interview" (December22, 1995: 4) and included her interview along with Winston Churchill's WW II radiobroadcasts as "high points" (August 23, 1996: 23) in BBC history. There are a number of reasons why this interview is both unusual and important forresearch on language use. Lasting 52 min, 15 sec and involving 301 occasions in whichutterances are initiated by interviewer or interviewee, it provides a wealth of analyticmaterial in both the audio and the video modalities for the study of media dialogue.Although Princess Diana is not strictly speaking a politician, her role in the royal familygives her extraordinary political importance and high profile. The topics considered in theinterview are intimately personal and sensitive, such as are not ordinarily discussed inpublic at all. They concern primarily weaknesses rather than strengths of the intervieweewho reveals them. By comparison with political interviews that we have studied, this onehad a much larger audience, and international press commentaries have indicated that it waswell received by a worldwide public. Martin Bashir showed himself to be an exceptionally

DOI: 10.1075/prag.7.3.02kow

Page 2: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

310 Sabine Kowal & Daniel C.O ‘Connell

good interviewer who, while remaining low profile himself, succeeded in getting at centralissues with very succinct questions. Finally, the interview is one in which a woman isinterviewed by a man - an unusual opportunity for gender comparisons.

2. Three models of idealized communication

Assuming then that the interview was clearly an instance of successful communication, wewish to use it as a data base to test three models of idealized communication. These include(1) the ideal delivery (Clark & Clark 1977; Clark 1996), (2) the simplest systematics for theorganization of turn-taking in conversation (Sacks, Schegloff, & Jefferson 1974), and (3)the maxim of Quantity (Grice 1975), as recently applied by Kurzon (1996) to PrincessDiana's "violation of the maxim of Quantity in terms of hyponomy" (217).

(1) The ideal delivery was first presented by Clark and Clark (1977) in apsycholinguistic context:

For there to be a speech "error" there must be a "correct" way of executing a sentence, and this will

be called the ideal delivery. When people know what they want to say and say it fluently, they are

giving an ideal delivery. Actors saying their lines, except when making deliberate errors, come close

to the ideal delivery, and so do practiced readers and orators. For theories of speech production the

ideal delivery is of central importance. They all assume that people strive for the ideal delivery, and

every deviation points to something that has gone wrong in planning or execution. (261)

More recently, Clark (1996) has restated the principle:

Every use of a word, phrase, or sentence has an ideal delivery -- a flawless presentation in the given

situation . . . . It is flawless in that it is fluent, and the pronunciation, intonation, speed, and volume

are appropriate to the circumstances. It is the delivery speakers would make if they had formulated

what they were going to say before speaking and could follow through on that plan. (254)

In an ideal delivery, therefore, Princess Diana and Martin Bashir would be expected tospeak with a preformulated fluency: flawlessly, without hesitations, and without longpauses. This pattern should be even more predictable because the two parties had carefullygone over the material and questioning beforehand (Kurzon 1996: 217).

(2) The simplest systematics of turn-taking was originally presented in anethnomethodological context by Sacks, Schegloff, and Jefferson (1974) and includes anideal of temporal organization in turn-taking for conversation: Time is essential to itseconomy. This would logically exclude inordinately long pauses or gaps, and the timeintended is measurable real time. Recently, Clark's (1996) presentation of turn-takingprinciples has reiterated this ideal: "If speakers project turn completions, speaker switchesshould often be accomplished with little or no gap at all, and they are" (322).

The application of the turn-taking model to interview data is clearly controversialbecause the model has been explicitly restricted to conversation and many conversationanalysts have in fact limited the model to this speech genre. Nonetheless, there has beenprecedent for an application of the model to interviews (e.g., Greatbatch 1988) and, moregenerally, to "institutional contexts" (Pomerantz & Fehr 1997: 64). One could argue froman economy of media time that the same tight pattern of turn-taking would be expected in

Page 3: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

Theoretical ideals and their violation 311

an interview as prototypically in conversations. Long between-speaker pauses should notoccur frequently, either before Princess Diana speaks or before Martin Bashir speaks.

(3) Another idealization is basic to Kurzon's (1996) approach to the interview. Itis derived from Grice's (1975) prescriptive maxim of Quantity:

1. Make your contribution as informative as is required (for the current purposes ofthe exchange).2. Do not make your contribution more informative than is required. (45)

According to Kurzon (1996), Princess Diana's replies manifest a "violation of the [first partof the] maxim of Quantity in terms of hyponomy" (217) with respect to her references tothe royal family and to Prince Charles. By this, Kurzon means that she avoided "referringexplicitly to a particular person or group of people" (217), that she was "withholdinginformation" (217), and that she "managed to generalize various references" (218). He didnot analyze Martin Bashir's references.

A number of other methodological questions arose in the course of our analyses, allof which are closely related to one or another of the three idealizations discussed above.

Our own transcribing of the interview revealed that Princess Diana's contributionswere much more difficult to transcribe than were Bashir's. Our tentative explanation forthis we have formulated as a hypothesis that her articulation rate (based on our ownsubjective impression) was much faster than his. Such a rapid articulation would clearlybe less readily perceptible to the TV audience, i.e., it would not be quite "appropriate to thecircumstances" (Clark 1996: 254), and therefore violate an ideal delivery. Implied by the simplest systematics is an agonistic or power relationship of takingor holding the floor. Kurzon's (1996) formulations seem to imply such an agonistic settingfor this interview: "Bashir attempts to make the Princess equate explicitly. . ." (219);"Bashir once again tries to get an explicit answer. . ." (220); "Bashir asks a very directquestion. . ." (222); "Bashir doesn't give up. . ." (224); and "the interviewer does not let heroff the hook" (225). Recent studies also characterize the interviewer's role as the dominantrole (Greatbatch 1988: 401), as "power semantics" (O'Donnell 1990: 211), and as "powerrelationships" (Penz 1996: 76). Given that the interview involved a woman questioned bya man, agonism would be expected to be maximized in terms of interruptions on the partof the male interviewer. Our own impression, however, is that agonism was by and largeabsent and that Bashir was instead both compassionate and understanding in his interviewstyle. Empiri-cal criteria must be found in the data of the interview to decide this issue.

3. Data base and analyses

A transcript of the entire interview was prepared from an audio recording by both authorstogether; all doubtful instances were reviewed until there was agreement. There were atotal of 11,701 syllables in the entire interview, 8,806 in Princess Diana's contributions and2,895 in Martin Bashir's. In addition, an almost complete transcript (11,565 syllables of theinterview, or 98.8%) was prepared independently of the authors by a student (a nativespeaker of English). A third partial transcript consisted of the eight passages published inKurzon (1996). The latter two transcripts were compared with the authors' transcript.Changes therein from the authors' transcript were analyzed in terms of four categories usedby O'Connell and Kowal (1994: 126 f.; see also Lindsay & O'Connell 1995): additions,

Page 4: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

312 Sabine Kowal & Daniel C.O ‘Connell

deletions, substitutions, and relocations. Ten of the longest uninterrupted contributions of both interviewer (range: 47-80

syllables) and interviewee (range: 137-214 syllables) were subjected to temporal analysisby means of a Siemens Oscillomink L and a Fundamental Frequency Meter FFM 6502 (F-JElectronics). Pauses were measured in milliseconds (msec) with a minimum cut-off pointof 120 msec. Speech rate was defined as syllables per second (syl/sec) of total time,articulation rate as syl/sec of ontime, pause duration as sec/pause, phrase length assyl/pause, and pause percentage as pause time/total time. The eight passages used in Kurzon(1996) were similarly analyzed.

The following categories of speech initiations on the part of Princess Diana andMartin Bashir were analyzed: (1) Smooth initiations with a pause; (2) smooth initiationswithout a pause; (3) overlaps (in which both speakers finish, but the second speaker startsto speak before the first speaker has concluded); (4) interruptions (in which the secondspeaker prevents the first speaker from finishing by beginning to speak); and (5) minimalresponses, including one and two syllable items such as MM or MM-HM. In the case ofinterruptions, incompleteness was assessed by syntactic and prosodic criteria.

From the authors' transcript, two lists of both nominal and pronominal referencesto members of the royal family and to Prince Charles on the part of both Princess Diana andMartin Bashir were prepared.

4. Results

Temporal Measures. Means and standard deviations of temporal measures for the 10contributions of Princess Diana and Martin Bashir are presented in Table 1. None of themeans were significantly different from one another. Specifically, the hypothesis that thearticulation rate of Princess Diana would be significantly faster than that of Martin Bashirwas not confirmed. The identical within-speaker pause durations for Princess Diana andMartin Bashir are of considerable interest in light of the very different durations of thebetween-speaker pauses which will be considered below.

A total of 500 pauses were analyzed in this study. They consisted of the 236 pausesoccurring within the 10 contributions of Princess Diana's and Martin Bashir's turns and alsoall of the 264 between-turn pauses of the interview. Frequencies of occurrence for all thesepauses are presented in Table 2 according to their duration intervals and according to theirlocation within or between speakers. A chi square statistic comparing the duration intervalsof pauses within speakers (M = 0.60 sec, SD = 0.34) and between speakers (M = 1.39 sec,SD = 0.97) was highly significant: P (5) = 199.9, p < .001. The between-speaker pauses2

immediately preceding utterances of Princess Diana (M = 0.94 sec, SD = 0.87) and thoseimmediately preceding utterances of Martin Bashir (M = 1.79 sec, SD = 0.88) were alsosignificantly different from one another: P (5) = 80.0, p < .001. 2

According to Clark's (1996) ideal delivery, there is a "one-second limit" whichreflects "how little silence is tolerated" (268). Hence, percentages of within-speaker pauseslonger in duration than 800 milliseconds and the ratio of syllables per vocal hesitation(including filled pauses, repeats, and false starts) are presented in Table 3 for PrincessDiana and Martin Bashir. For purposes of comparison, results of preliminary analyses fromtwo TV interviews of Hillary Clinton (with Kati Couric) and of Bill Clinton (with Wolf

Page 5: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

Theoretical ideals and their violation 313

Blitzer) are included.

Table 1

Means (M) and Standard Deviations (SD) for Temporal Data of 10 Excerpts of Princess

Diana and 10 Excerpts of Martin Bashir: Speech Rate, Articulation Rate, Pause Duration,

Phrase Length, and Percentage of Pause Time/Total Time

Measure Statistics Princess Diana Martin Bashir

Speech Rate M 4.20 4.34 SD 0.25 0.47Articulation Rate M 5.86 5.68 SD 0.30 0.42Pause Duration M 0.61 0.61 SD 0.10 0.14Phrase Length M 9.3 11.4 SD 1.4 4.0Percentage of Pause Time/To- tal Time M 27.8 24.5 SD 3.4 6.4

Table 2

Distribution of Frequency Intervals (in Sec) of Various Durations for Pauses Within-

Speakers (Princess Diana, PD, and Martin Ba-shir, MB) and Between-Speakers (before PD

or MB)

Duration Within-Speakers Total Between-Speakers TotalIntervals PD MB PD MB

0.12 - 1.00 159 45 204 91 24 1151.01 - 2.00 21 10 31 22 66 882.01 - 3.00 1 0 1 9 34 433.01 - 4.00 0 0 0 3 10 134.01 - 5.00 0 0 0 2 2 45.01 - 6.00 0 0 0 0 1 1 Total 181 55 236 127 137 264

Page 6: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

314 Sabine Kowal & Daniel C.O ‘Connell

Table 3

Percentage of Pauses Longer than 800 Milliseconds Over All Pauses (%) and Syllables Per

in the Present Study; Comparisons of the Same Measures in Interviews by Hillary Clinton

and Bill Clinton

Speaker % Syl/VH

Princess Diana 21.5 57Martin Bashir 27.3 223Hillary Clinton 5.6 84Bill Clinton 31.5 33

In all cases, pauses longer than 800 milliseconds occurred. Vocal hesitationsoccurred in all the interview settings. The interview data with the most intense antecedentpreparation are those of the interviewer Martin Bashir. And this is reflected in the fact thathe had only 13 vocal hesitations altogether. By the same comparison, Princess Dianawould seem to be comparable to both of the Clintons.

Table 4 presents the frequencies of occurrence for the various categories of speechinitiation by each of the two speakers. Of all the utterances initiated by Princess Diana andMartin Bashir, respectively, 80.4% and 95.8% were smooth transitions involving ameasurable pause. These pauses were also extraordinarily long, in contrast to the hypothesisderived from the simplest systematics for idealized turn-taking in conversation. PrincessDiana responded to questions more quickly than Bashir resumed his questioning: Not onlywere her pauses significantly shorter than his (0.94 < 1.79 sec) as indicated above, but also16 of her smooth transitions were without a pause, whereas only one transition by Bashirwas without a pause.

Table 4

Frequencies of Various Categories of Speech Initiation in the TV Interview of Princess

Diana by Martin Bashir

Category Princess Diana Martin Bashir

Smooth with Pause 127 137Smooth without Pause 16 1Overlap 4 2Interruption 0 3Minimal Responses 11 0Total 158 143

Page 7: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

Theoretical ideals and their violation 315

As Table 4 indicates, there are only three interruptions in the entire interview, lessthan 1% of all speech initiations. This finding is an empirical confirmation of ourantecedent intuition that the interview was minimally agonistic. At a glance, the fact thatall three were Bashir's interruptions of Princess Diana seems in accord with the genderhypothesis that men interrupt women more often than women interrupt men (e.g., James& Clarke 1993: 231). However, the research of Kowal, Barth, Egemann, Galusic, Kögel,Lippold, Pfeil and O'Connell (in press) on political interviews indicates that in this speechgenre not gender but interviewer role led to significantly more interruptions. But in viewof Bashir's long transitional pauses, even these few interruptions are paradoxical. As furtheranalyses showed, all three interruptions seem instead quite appropriate or even helpful inthe situation. In one case for example, Princess Diana was clearly stuck, but had notfinished; her voice hovered with a so. Bashir waited a full 2 sec before he resumed hisquestioning. The ideal of not interrupting becomes quite irrelevant in such a setting.Finally, Princess Diana was the only one who used minimal responses quite in accord withthe rule that interviewers refrain from spontaneous back-channel signals (Holly 1992: 35).

Table 5

Number of Syllables (Syl), Changes from Authors' Transcript, and Syl/Change in Student's

Transcript and Kurzon's (1996) Excerpts

Transcript Measure Diana Bashir Total

Student's Number of Syl 8709 2856 11565 Number of Changes 234 23 257 Additions 22 0 22 Deletions 102 12 114 Substitutions 109 11 120 Relocations 1 0 1 Syl/Change 37 124 45 Syl/Additions 396 -- 526 Syl/Deletions 85 238 101 Syl/Substitutions 80 260 96 Syl/Relocations 8709 -- 1156Kurzon's Number of Syl 708 229 937 Number of Changes 27 3 30 Additions 4 1 5 Deletions 3 1 4 Substitutions 20 1 21 Relocations 0 0 0 Syl/Change 26 76 31 Syl/Additions 177 229 187 Syl/Deletions 236 229 234 Syl/Substitutions 35 229 45 Syl/Relocations -- -- --

Page 8: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

316 Sabine Kowal & Daniel C.O ‘Connell

Transcripts. Because of the authors' experience that Princess Diana was moredifficult to transcribe than Martin Bashir, changes of both the student's transcript andKurzon's (1996) passages from the authors' transcript were analyzed. In Table 5, thesechanges are presented along with the syllables per change, separately for Princess Diana'sand Martin Bashir's parts of the transcript. More changes were to be found in the formerthan in the latter in both the student's transcript (234 > 23) and in Kurzon's passages (27 >3). In both transcripts, the relative number of syllables in Diana's text is three times thatof Bashir's text (8709/2856 syl and 708/229 syl). The changes involved in both transcriptsare, however, disproportionately greater than would be expected from the differences insyllable number (10 and 9 times, respectively, instead of 3 times), and thisdisproportionality is summarily reflected in the smaller syl/change ratio for Princess Dianain both transcripts (37 < 124 syl/change in the student's transcript; 26 < 76 syl/change inKurzon's passages). In other words, our own subjective impression that Diana's speech wasmore difficult to transcribe accurately is confirmed by this finding. The reasons for thisdifficulty seem to reside in the soft-spoken quality of Princess Diana's voice and the longduration of her utterances rather than in her articulation rate.

Changes were for the most part one syllable in length. The types of changes are alsoinformative because of their differential frequency. Relocations were negligible (0.3%;e.g., it's now in the authors' transcript became now it's) and additions very few in number(9.4%; e.g., there did not occur in the authors' transcript). Most common were deletions(41.1%; e.g., all in the authors' transcript did not occur) and substitutions (49.1%; e.g.,thought occurred instead of felt in the authors' transcript), and both were accounted forsystematically by subcategories of changes similar to those found by O'Connell and Kowal(1994) for German corpora and by Lindsay and O'Connell (1995) for an Americantelevision interview.

The combined 118 deletions from the student's transcript and Kurzon's passageswere accounted for mainly by function words (54.2%), interjections of the type MM, MM-HM, and UH (22.9%), and word fragments (21.2%). In other words, 98.3% of the deletionswere not lexically pertinent to the content. The longest deletion was from the student'stranscript, five syllables of Bashir's text: with Mr. Gilby. The likely reason for this deletionis the fact that both interviewer and interviewee were speaking simultaneously.

Substitutions were also systematic. Of the combined 141 in the student's transcriptand Kurzon's passages, 52.5% were changes in morphology, 34.8% were changes to otherwords, and 6.4% changes from or to an interjection. Accordingly, 93.7% of the sub-stitutions were thus accounted for.

References to the royal family and Prince Charles. Since Kurzon (1996) claimedthat Princess Diana violated "the maxim of Quantity in terms of hyponomy" (217), Tables6 and 7 summarize all nominal and pronominal references to the royal family and toPrince Charles respectively. The references are listed separately according to theiroccurrence in Princess Diana's and Martin Bashir's contributions. However, the latter havebeen analyzed only by the authors and were disregarded in Kurzon (1996). The sequenceof references in both tables is: nominal references used by both Princess Diana and MartinBashir; pronominals used by both; nominals used by one or the other; pronominals used byone or the other; and finally, others as a residual category used only by Kurzon. Phrasescontaining both a nominal and a pronominal reference are listed under the nominalcategory. Non-occurrence is designated as a zero, whereas non-entry, i.e., unavailability

Page 9: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

Theoretical ideals and their violation 317

of data, is marked by a dash.

Table 6

Number of References to the Royal Family by Princess Diana and Martin Bashir in the

Authors' and Kurzon's (1996: 226) Analyses

Reference Princess Diana Martin Bashir Authors Kurzon Authors Kurzon

people 22 13 2 --(royal) family 3 3 7 --the monarchy 6 6 4 --children 12 -- 4 --(your son) (Prince) 10 -- 3 -- Williamboy(s) 6 -- 1 --child 2 -- 1 --the enemy 2 -- 1 --they/them (family) 12 -- 5 --they/them (sons) 14 -- 1 --he (William) 2 -- 1 --anybody/anyone/ 9 -- 1 -- everybody/nobody/ no one/someHarry 4 -- 0 --(friends on) my hus- 2 -- 0 -- band's side Her Majesty 1 1 0 --the system 1 1 0 --the net 1 -- 0 --my husband's depart- 1 -- 0 -- ment the establishment 1 -- 0 -- that I married into that (the) environment 1 3 0 --the palace 0 -- 1 --the royal household 0 -- 2 --the Queen 0 -- 2 --you (William) 6 -- 0 --others -- 20 0 -- Totals 118 47 36 --

Page 10: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

318 Sabine Kowal & Daniel C.O ‘Connell

Table 7

Number of References to Prince Charles by Princess Diana and Martin Bashir in the

Authors' and Kurzon's (1996: 226) Analyses

Reference Princess Diana Martin Bashir Authors Kurzon Authors Kurzon

husband 16 17 9 --Prince of Wales 4 2 6 --man 1 -- 1 --(royal) couple 3 -- 1 --husband and I/you 3 -- 1 --he/him 13 -- 7 --team 4 -- 0 --papa 1 -- 0 --Charles 1 2 0 --you/yourself and the 0 -- 2 -- Prince of WalesCharles and I/me 2 -- 0 --we/us/ourselves/ 63 -- 0 -- neither of usboth 7 -- 0 --you/the two of you 0 -- 4 --others -- 7 -- --Totals 118 28 31 --

What is immediately evident from Tables 6 and 7 is that the counts of the presentauthors are not in agreement with Kurzon's. Even apart from the fact that Kurzondisregarded Princess Diana's use of pronominal forms, the numbers for nominal referencesare different. We have included all of Bashir's references on the ground that PrincessDiana's references cannot be properly analyzed and understood except as consequences ofBashir's references in his questioning.

The first eight entries in Table 6 account for all the nominal references to the royalfamily on the part of both interviewer and interviewee. It is clear from the transcript thatthe first three listed there (people, the [royal] family, and the monarchy) were introducedby Bashir before they were used by Princess Diana. In terms of total contributions insyllables, the syl/reference ratios of Princess Diana are almost the same as Bashir's formembers of the royal family (76 < 80) and lower than Bashir's for Prince Charles (76 < 93).In other words, she spoke about the other members of the royal family just as much as shespoke about her husband. And 69% (82/118) of her references to her husband were toherself and him together. Bashir, on the other hand, inquired somewhat more about theroyal family than about Prince Charles.

Page 11: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

Theoretical ideals and their violation 319

5. Discussion

We introduced Princess Diana's interview with a presentation of the concept of the idealdelivery. What exactly could we expect of Princess Diana, were she presenting an idealdelivery? Two rather contradictory requisites emerge from the recent description given byClark (1996). On the one hand, "pure hesitations are by far the most common disfluency"(269). In other words, "speakers stop until they have formulated the next element and thencontinue" (269). But on the other hand, in terms of the ideal speaker, "speakers could avoidrepairs if they took enough time before speaking" (271). If, then, what is described as themost common disfluency is the only means of avoiding repairs, one would have to makeuse of disfluency precisely in order to present an ideal delivery. However, both PrincessDiana and Martin Bashir used pauses circa and greater than "the one-second limit" (Clark1996: 268), though certainly not excessively (21.5% and 27.3%, respectively). Both usehesitations, Princess Diana far more frequently despite her long pauses.

The fact of the matter is that the abstract ideal delivery is not only unrealistic, it hasbeen watered down situationally and circumstantially by Clark (1996: 254) to the point ofunidentifiability. Even "performance errors" are now considered by him both "systematicand essential to the successful use of language" (388). The ideal delivery is an entityinvented out of the implicit conviction that language must be entirely formulated inadvance, i.e., before onset of an utterance. It isn't, it shouldn't be, and, in fact, it cannot be,because in an interview the interviewee has to react to the interviewer's questions.Obviously, Princess Diana did not primarily "strive for the ideal delivery" (Clark & Clark1977: 261) but for efficient communication. She took the risk of being hesitant in order tomake herself understood. Hence, the motivation for longer pauses is not so much cognitiveas social.

The fact that Princess Diana didn't entirely prepackage her language is a majorfactor in making her contributions marvelously eloquent. The relevant principle is thatpeople are thinking speakers; they formulate not only as they think and before they speakbut also as they speak. Psychologically, formulation not only precedes but also co-occurswith articulation, and people strive not to be correct, but to communicate to their interlocu-tor(s) what they wish to share with her or him or them. It is precisely in this sense thatlanguage is more a tool than a rule, as Bühler (1934) and others (e.g., Hörmann 1976) havelong ago claimed. The ideal delivery can ultimately cope with pauses only as down-time,not as genuine silence throughout which "an intersubjectively established, temporarilyshared social world" (Rommetveit 1974: 29) remains actively intact.

The second idealization of speaking encountered with regard to Princess Diana'sinterview is the simplest systematics of Sacks, Schegloff, and Jefferson (1974). If indeed"transitions (from one turn to the next) with no gap and no overlap are common" (p. 700f.) in idealized conversation, then the 80% of Diana's and 96% of Bashir's turns whichbegin with pauses cannot be predicted by the simplest systematics - at least in their highfrequency of occurrence. They introduce a new economy of time, not unlike Clark'sspeakers who fall into "the most common disfluency" by not speaking. And if pauses asshort as 0.2 sec are really to be considered "significant" (Levinson 1983: 328), then Diana's(M = 0.94 sec) and even more notably Bashir's (M = 1.79 sec) between-speaker pausesmust be considered most unusual. The long silence indicates not just, or not even primarily,

Page 12: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

320 Sabine Kowal & Daniel C.O ‘Connell

that the speaker is having difficulty packaging the message, but that she or he is using thesilence socially: to prepare and/or impress the listener.

But we - along with the interviewer, the interviewee, and about 200 million others -have declared the interview a resounding success. What can be said in defense of theseextraordinarily long between-speaker pauses? We wish to contend that the silence itselfis functional, indeed necessary for the proper negotiation of such a delicate interview. Thesilent time is shared time. On the part of Bashir, there is a deferential stance toward aperson who is both royalty and is very vulnerable. Hence, he waits unusually long on theaverage before asking another question. His is an extraordinary ability to defer in time.And on the part of Princess Diana, her long silences before responding mirror her reflective,serious approach to this interview. Both interlocutors are being actively silent in oneanother's presence. For the millions of viewers around the world, this would hardly appearto be down-time, much less a breakdown or disturbance. Quite the contrary, the silencesweeps the audience into the mood of the interview in a compelling way.

The third idealization is Kurzon's (1996) argument that Princess Diana violated "themaxim of Quantity in terms of hyponomy" (217) with respect to members of the royalfamily, including her husband, the Prince of Wales. From an empirical standpoint, we havequestioned whether Grice's explanatory framework is appropriately applied by Kurzon towhat was really happening in the interview. Our reasons are as follows:

(1) Princess Diana actually adopted both a level of generality and a mode ofreference which closely corresponded to those established by Bashir's questioning. It wasBashir whose very first question was about the royal family. He was also the first tomention both people and the monarchy and the only one to use the terms the palace, theroyal household, and the queen. These modes of reference were established at the verybeginning of the interview and were maintained by both interviewer and intervieweethroughout.

(2) Martin Bashir asked no questions about specific members of the royal familyother than the Prince of Wales, Prince William, and Prince Harry. What is perhaps mostsurprising of all and is not even noted by Kurzon: No questions whatsoever were askedabout the queen herself. Bashir referred to the queen twice, but both times only to introducea question about another topic. Obviously, Martin Bashir is the one who was avoidingquestions about the queen. His questions about the Prince of Wales were all answered quiteappropriately and identifiably: about her husband (16 times), about herself and her husbandtogether (e.g., team, couple, us, both; 82 times), by name (Charles; 3 times [Kurzon's Table1 has only 2]), with a rather endearing term (papa; once), about him alone (e.g., him; 13times), and about him as the Prince of Wales (4 times). The interviewer, however, did notspecifically mention the name Charles even once. Princess Diana mentioned PrinceWilliam by name ten times, Prince Harry three times; Bashir mentioned Prince Williamthree times and Prince Harry not at all. In other words, Kurzon to the contrary notwith-standing, Bashir did have "qualms about naming members of the royal family" (221);Princess Diana did not.

(3) Princess Diana followed closely the requirements of both diplomacy andtelevision interviews. She is a seasoned diplomat, quite well aware that anything radicalor dramatic said by her would immediately become a worldwide sound byte. She seemedin this regard to have been far more prudent than Kurzon thought she should have been inaccord with Grice's maxim of Quantity.

Page 13: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

Theoretical ideals and their violation 321

(4) In fact, all of Princess Diana's replies were clearly in accord with Grice's (1975)advice regarding "the current purposes of the exchange" (45). A test for this assertion isto inquire of each of Kurzon's passages given in demonstration of his case, whether therewas an alternative reply available to Princess Diana which would have remained bothprudent and truthful.

For example, Kurzon has claimed that Princess Diana "tries to avoid statingexplicitly that she suffered from bulimia herself" (218). At that point in the interview, shehad just finished a long, detailed acknowledgement of her bulimia. To have repeated thisadmission again really would have violated the second maxim of Quantity, "Do not givemore information than is required" (Grice 1975: 52) by its sheer redundancy. In the samecontext, Kurzon has insisted that Princess Diana's use of "you don't discuss it with people""can only be interpreted as 'members of the royal family'" (218). This is simply not the case.Here she used the term people far more inclusively: You don't discuss bulimia withanybody.

According to Kurzon, "Bashir attempts to make the Princess equate explicitly'people' with 'the royal family' or even 'the royal household'" (219). But in fact he didneither. Bashir's inquiry actually followed immediately after a very clear "and" on PrincessDiana's part:

Excerpt 1

Bashir: who was asking those questionsDiana: people around me people

in this environment andBashir: the royal householdDiana: people

in my environment yes yeap

It is quite clear from the excerpt that Bashir simply made her and his own. The obviousimport of his question is whether the royal household was also "asking these questions"(220). And Princess Diana replied appropriately.

In general, then, Princess Diana said exactly what she wanted to communicate - ap-propriately for the circumstances. That is precisely why she allowed the interview to betelecast.

Clark (1996: 143) has formulated three assumptions which underly Grice's maxims:

Assumption 1 What is said is logically prior to what is implicated.Assumption 2 The way listeners determine what is said is different in

principle from the way they "work out" what isimplicated.

Assumption 3 What is said is well defined for every type of utterance.

But Clark considered all three of these principles problematic (143-146) and so declined

Page 14: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

322 Sabine Kowal & Daniel C.O ‘Connell

to accept them as anything more than "rules of thumb" (146). We too consider them to beso circumstantially hedged in terms of the limitation to "the current purposes of theexchange" (Grice 1975: 45) as to serve no predictive purpose.

Finally, the issue of agonism requires some comment. Bashir's long between-speaker pauses, his several cooperative interruptions, and his deferential, composed mannerall belie an agonistic relationship between interviewer and interviewee. Any interviewsetting is obviously asymmetrical, but asymmetry does not necessitate a relationship ofpower or dominance. And even though the interviewer was a man and the interviewee awoman, there is no evidence that the interviewer's role need be considered the dominantrole (Greatbatch 1988: 401) anymore than dominance and agonism need be consideredessential to ordinary conversation.

With regard to all three idealizations considered here - ideal delivery, simplestsystematics, and the maxim of Quantity - the evidence from this interview necessitates amore subtle formulation. These models prove to be modest descriptions of some dialogicalsettings, but not testable theories of performance quality:

The ultimate criterion for the success of a conversation is not "the smooth interchange of speaking

turns" (Cutler & Pearson 1986: 139) or any other prescriptive ideal, but the fulfillment of the

purposes entertained by two or more interlocutors. (O'Connell, Kowal, & Kaltenbacher 1990: 345)

References

Bühler, K. (1934) Sprachtheorie: Die Darstellungsfunktion der Sprache. Stuttgart: Gustav Fischer.

Clark, H.H. (1996) Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Clark, H.H., & E.V. Clark (1977) Psychology and language: An introduction to psycholinguistics. New York:

Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Cutler, A., & M. Pearson (1986) On the analysis of prosodic turn-taking cues. In C. Johns-Lewis (Ed.),

Intonation in discourse. London: Croom Helm, pp. 139-155.

.

Greatbatch, D. (1988) A turn-taking system for British news interviews. Language in Society 17: 401-430.

Grice, H.P. (1975) Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J.L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics, Vol. 3:

Speech acts. New York: Seminar Press, pp. 41-58.

Hörmann, H. (1976) Meinen und Verstehen: Grundzüge einer psychologischen Semantik. Frankfurt am Main:

Suhrkamp Verlag.

Holly, W. (1992) Was kann Kohl, was Krenz nicht konnte? Deutsch-deutsche Unterschiede politischer

Dialogrhetorik in zwei Fernsehinterviews. In J. Dyck, W. Jens, & G. Ueding (Eds.), Rhetorik 11: 33-50.

James, D. & S. Clarke (1993) Women, men, and interruptions: A critical review. In D. Tannen (Ed.), Gender

and conversational interaction. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 231-280.

Kowal, S., H.-C. Barth, H. Egemann, G. Galusic, C. Kögel, N. Lippold, A. Pfeil & D.C. O'Connell (in press)

Unterbrechungen in Medieninterviews: Geschlechtstypisches Gesprächsverhalten? Germanistische Linguistik.

Page 15: THEORETICAL IDEALS AND THEIR VIOLATION: PRINCESS DIANA …€¦ · PRINCESS DIANA AND MARTIN BASHIR IN THE BBC INTERVIEW 1 Sabine Kowal and Daniel C. O’Connell 1. Introduction In

Theoretical ideals and their violation 323

Kurzon, D. (1996) The maxim of Quantity, hyponymy and Princess Diana. Pragmatics 6: 217-227.

Levinson, S.C. (1983) Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Lindsay, J.S., & D.C. O'Connell (1995) How do transcribers deal with audio recordings of spoken discourse?

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 24: 101-115.

O'Connell, D. C., & S. Kowal (1994) The transcriber as language user. In G. Bartelt (Ed.), The dynamics of

language processes: Essays in honor of Hans W. Dechert. Tübingen: Gunter Narr, pp. 119-142.

O'Connell, D. C., S. Kowal, & E. Kaltenbacher (1990). Turn-taking: A critical analysis of the research

tradition. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 19: 345-373.

O'Donnell, K. (1990) Difference and dominance: How labor and management talk conflict. In A. Grimshaw

(Ed.), Conflict talk. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 210-240.

Penz, H. (1996) Language and control in American TV talk shows: An analysis of linguistic strategies.

Tübingen: Gunter Narr.

Pomerantz, A., & B.J. Fehr (1997) Conversation analysis: An approach to the study of social action as sense

making practices. In T.A. van Dijk (Ed.), Discourse as social interaction. Discourse studies: A

multidisciplinary introduction, Vol. 2. London: Sage, pp. 64-91.

Rommetveit, R. (1974) On message structure: A framework for the study of language and communication.

London: Wiley.

Sacks, H., E.A. Schegloff., & G. Jefferson (1974) A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking

in conversation. Language 50: 696-735.


Recommended