+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Underestimating Youth’s Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

Underestimating Youth’s Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

Date post: 14-Apr-2018
Category:
Upload: yugesh-d-panday
View: 215 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 40

Transcript
  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    1/40

    Social Psychology of Education 3: 139, 1999.

    1999 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands.1

    Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schoolsand Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    THOMAS L. GOOD, SHARON L. NICHOLS and DARRELL L. SABERSUniversity of Arizona

    Abstract. Many media reports contend that American youth are dangerous, self-possessed, and care

    too little about their personal education or the broader society. Not only are youth criticized, but their

    teachers and schools are subjected to exaggerated criticism. We argue that the average, undifferen-

    tiated view of youth as presented by the media is inaccurate and pejorative. Researchers have added to

    the chorus of unfounded negative assertions about youth and their schools. As an example, LaurenceSteinberg, in Beyond the Classroom, argued that American students levels of academic achievement

    were woefully inadequate. He argued that students did not do enough homework, that they cheated,

    and that pervasive, negative peer pressure undermined academic achievement. The purpose of our

    study was to examine students attitudes and perceptions in three major areas cheating, homework,

    and peer pressure. Using data from over 700 students drawn from one public high school and one

    junior high school, our results indicated that students beliefs and norms concerning cheating, home-

    work, and peer pressure varied widely in terms of gender, GPA, and school context. Our data failed

    to replicate the findings from Beyond in that (a) our average findings were more positive than those

    reported in that study, and (b) we showed that the reporting of average was highly misleading. Our

    findings provided evidence that one cannot describe American students in a general, sweeping way.

    Average statements about youth, especially adolescent youth, are apt to be very misleading.

    Introduction

    Recently, the media has been ravaged by claims of manufactured stories and

    sloppy work in checking the accuracy of reported data. For example, reporters

    writing for the Boston Globe and the New Republic have been fired for fabricating

    stories, people, and quotations (McFeatters, 1998). In this paper, we deal with

    one target of inaccurate media reporting youth and their education. According

    to much of the media, American youth are dangerous, self-possessed, and care

    too little about their personal education or the broader society. Descriptions of

    youth are primarily negative and often present scathing critiques of their behavior

    and attitudes (e.g., drug-use, physical violence). Contemporary media (televisionaccounts, films, newspaper and magazine articles, cartoons, etc.) often present

    youth as lacking in morality and civic responsibility (Giroux, 1996; Youniss &

    Yates, 1997). Youth are criticized about many aspects of their lives from poor

    school achievement to low personal morality. Not only are youth criticized, but

    their teachers and schools are subjected to exaggerated criticism. Although the

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    2/40

    2 THOMAS L. GOOD ET AL.

    medias excessive condemnation of teachers and schools constitutes an important

    part of the contemporary social context, the major focus of this paper will be on

    the representation of adolescents, especially adolescents in school.

    In this paper, we first argue that an average, undifferentiated view of youth as

    presented in the media is both inaccurate and pejorative. We then argue that these

    undifferentiated views of youth and schools (largely negative in tone and presented

    void of any context) are not only misleading characterizations but also lead to

    inappropriate strategies for treating youth and school improvement. The systemic

    communication of low expectations about the ability and commitment of youth,

    teachers, and schools has unfortunate consequences for morale, performance, and

    funding. Further, we examine illustrative examples to show how the media and

    popular press sometimes present misleading depictions of youth that fly in the

    face of research findings and illustrate how research reports may contribute to the

    problem. As an example, we cite a book by Steinberg et al. (1996), Beyond the

    Classroom, which illustrates problems of undifferentiated reporting. Finally, we

    present findings from two studies we conducted that explore students perceptionsof and behavioral orientations toward schooling which challenge claims made in

    Beyond the Classroom and illustrate how educational researchers and social scient-

    ists can contribute to poor communication by relying too heavily on the concept of

    average.

    Average and Negative Depictions

    YOUTH AND SOCIAL ISSUES

    One columnist recently contended that television news programming has been in-

    strumental in creating an inaccurate image making America appear to be under

    siege by armies of teenagers (Jackson, 1997). The medias bias against youthwas illustrated in a recent report from the Berkeley Media Studies Group which

    analyzed roughly 8,000 television newscasts from 26 different stations in Cali-

    fornia during the Fall of 1993 (Dorfman, Woodruff, Chavez, & Wallack, 1997).

    This group found that about 68% of the stories that focused on violence concerned

    youth violence. Further, 84% of those studies focused on only the salient event

    (the blood and gore) and provided no contextual information about the crime or

    any societal issues possibly related to youth crime (e.g., the easy availability of

    guns and alcohol; the absence of after-school programs).

    Researchers have contended that the impact of media coverage of violence de-

    pends on how stories are depicted. For example, Iyengar (1991) identified news

    stories as either episodic (showing events without contextual information) or them-atic (placing incidents in context). Dorfman et al. (1997) described episodic stories

    as containing mere facts, details, and narrow descriptions of the crime; whereas,

    in contrast, thematic stories tend to include not only detailed information about

    the crime but also information about the circumstances surrounding the incident

    (e.g., was alcohol involved?) as well as information about public prevention and

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    3/40

    UNDERESTIMATING YOUTH 3

    community responses to future similar crimes. Clearly, how a story is reported

    will differentially affect audience opinion about the crime and crime participants.

    Indeed, Dorfman et al. (1997) stated that when reporting about youth and youth

    violence, not only were there a plethora of negative, violent stories about youth,

    but the episodic framing of the story lead audience members to blame youth.

    The vilification of youth in the public media is mirrored by many citizens

    pejorative views of youth. In a recent survey conducted by Public Agenda, Farkas

    and Johnson (1997) reported that adult Americans primarily describe teenagers and

    children negatively using the adjectives rude, irresponsible, and wild. Although

    earlier generations have also been critical of adolescent youth, todays critiques not

    only appear harsher but also have extended downward to include even children.

    Although the media and citizen opinions might focus attention on youth poverty

    and abuse and the minimal, unequal, educational resources that many youth are

    afforded (Biddle, 1997; Good, 1996; Natriello, 1996; U.S. General Accounting

    Office, 1995a, 1995b), the clear tendency is to blame youth not to understand

    or to help. The Public Agenda survey makes it clear that many citizens do notworry about the limited resources and low investment in attention and opportunity

    that youth receive; rather, many Americans worry most about youths character

    deficits.

    If such conclusions were rare isolated events advocacy for youth would

    be unnecessary. However, many documents confirm that American citizens are

    actively concerned about the moral values of youth. As a case in point, a 24-

    member, non-partisan panel of leading citizens authored a 1998 report entitled, A

    call to civil society: Why democracy needs moral truths (Associated Press, 1998).

    Although this report decried a supposed general decline in the social morality of

    Americans, part of its focus targeted a supposed lack of morality among American

    youth. And to combat this, the report called for supporting charter schools, charac-ter education programs, and parental school choice. In contrast, the report did not

    call for more direct investments in youth (e.g., expanded afternoon programs). As

    suggested above, such depictions of youth are episodic in nature. A more thematic

    approach might consider a range of social and contextual issues related to youth

    and their supposed lack of morality.

    Indeed, the recent saliency of youth crime as reported in the media from an

    episodic perspective (e.g., the Jonesboro and Springfield murders of teens by teens)

    suggests that teens are solely to blame for these heinous acts, thus youths morality

    is rapidly declining. Although teens should be held accountable for their actions,

    we also suspect that thematic issues lie behind such incidents (e.g., easy access to

    guns, lack of parental supervision, and need for more school counselors). Thus,

    attention is focused on youth as scapegoat targets rather than on issues one mightdo something about such as violence prevention, reducing gun accessibility, and

    improving community resources.

    Critics of youth have expressed concerns about youths many putative failings.

    In particular, youths self-centeredness and irresponsibility have received notable

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    4/40

    4 THOMAS L. GOOD ET AL.

    comment. However, we cannot find any data to support the claim that todays

    youth, as a group, are more self-centered and irresponsible than youth of previous

    eras. Indeed, the data, if anything, point to a different conclusion. Research evid-

    ence, summarized by Youniss and Yates (1997), suggests that todays youth, when

    given the chance, are actively involved in community service. These researchers

    examined both early and contemporary studies concerned with community service

    and the development of social responsibility in youth. They concluded that youth

    are diverse and highly differentiatedin the ways they participate in their community

    (i.e., some are altruistic and prosocial; others are not). Such a conclusion, based

    on extant research, is both more differentiated and more positive than citizens

    perceptions about youth as indicated by Public Agenda poll data we cited earlier.

    Additionally, results from a New York Times/CBS News Poll (1998) reveal a

    general commitment by youth to prosocial behavior. For example, when asked

    how often they lie to their parents, 6% said never, 55% of the sample said hardly

    ever, 33% said some, and only 4% said a lot. In terms of alcohol and drug use,

    86% of the sample reported that they hardly ever or never drink alcohol, wine, orbeer. Similarly, 94% of the sample reported that they hardly ever or never smoke

    marijuana or pot. This suggests that many youth do nothave a character deficit.

    Needless to say, these results received scant attention in the national media.

    Other data provide additional evidence that youth should not be described in

    monolithic, negative terms. Other 1998 results from the joint New York Times/CBS

    poll illustrate that students vary in their perceptions of various academic and social

    attitudes by gender and age. For example, when students were asked whether they

    thought growing up is harder, easier, or about the same for them than for their

    parents, 55% of girls (in contrast to 43% of boys) reported that they thought it

    was harder. When students were asked how positive or negative they felt about

    themselves, 39% of girls (in contrast to 54% of boys) felt very positive while, atthe other extreme, 10% of girls (in contrast to only 2% of boys) reported feeling

    very negative about themselves. When asked about cheating on an exam, 58% of

    the total sample reported yes. However, this time students differed significantly,

    depending on their age or gender. For example, 53% of girls versus 62% of boys

    reported they had cheated on a test or an exam, and 52% of 1315 year olds

    compared with 67% of 1617 year olds reported they had cheated on a test or

    an exam.

    ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE

    Indeed, the media have painted an unfair, negative, and monolithic picture of youthmorality. Another media attack on youth has centered on the supposed lack of

    achievement motivation and school performance among young persons. At least

    since the publication ofA Nation at Riskin 1983 (National Commission for Excel-

    lence in Education, 1983), it has been popular to claim that students inadequate

    performance in public schools constitutes a threat to national security. Sweeping

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    5/40

    UNDERESTIMATING YOUTH 5

    criticism of schooling was manifested in the National Education Summit of 1996

    where policy makers (including President Clinton) and business leaders met to

    discuss possible solutions to a supposed national decline in student achievement.

    One general solution proposed by Summiteers was a call for higher standards

    for all students (National Education Summit, 1996). However, as suggested by

    Sabers and Sabers (1996), a call for higher standards for all students is misdir-

    ected, and, as Biddle (1997) argued, the call for vague, higher standards fails

    to address real, underlying problems for many students, such as unequal public

    school funding. Media generalizations about students supposedly low academic

    performance (which lead to sweeping and unsuccessful solutions) fail to consider

    complex mediating variables of student achievement (such as SES and educational

    resources) as well as wide variation among students. This general (monolithic)

    and negative reporting on students achievement presents an inadequate picture of

    students highly differentiated performance in American schools.

    TEACHERS AND SCHOOLS

    Youth do not stand alone as victims of a largely negative and often pejorative

    press. Public schools and teachers are also characterized as inadequate by the

    media. Various policy documents have characterized teachers and schools in terms

    of average, and these undifferentiated portrayals are labeled and presented by

    the media as inadequate characterizations embraced, indeed elaborated on, by

    those media sources. This is unfair and inaccurate. Despite the popular press and

    some researchers willingness to emphasize average findings, research evidence

    repeatedly finds that the performance of American students, teachers, and schools

    is widely varied (Brookover, Beady, Flood, Schweitzer, & Wisenbaker, 1979; Good

    & Brophy, 1986; Rutter, 1983). To know one school or a few schools is not thesame as knowing all schools in America for contextual problems, student attend-

    ance rates, and achievement vary notably from school to school, even in schools

    that serve similar student populations. Some schools create high levels of student

    achievement; some schools generate strongly positive attitudes among students;

    some schools do both; some do neither (Goodlad, 1983). America has both horrible

    schools (Kozol, 1991) and successful schools (Rose, 1995).

    And if this were not a sufficient problem, the impact of individual teachers

    on students achievement also shows wide variation (see, for example, Good &

    Weinstein, 1986). But this is hardly surprising since teachers vary in the resources

    and support they receive. Some teachers teach huge classes in unsafe, unattractive

    buildings with inadequate supplies. Other teachers teach small classes in attract-ive settings with ample libraries, modern laboratories, and adequate technological

    support. In some schools (both rich and poor) the efforts of individual teachers are

    aided by a school climate of support and mutual concern for student learning, but

    in other schools the collegial climate fosters isolation and negativism (Rosenholtz,

    1989).

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    6/40

    6 THOMAS L. GOOD ET AL.

    Although considerable evidence has appeared suggesting that the media have

    greatly overstated the general failure of American students in comparison to those

    in other countries (Berliner & Biddle, 1995; Bracey, 1991; Sandia National Labor-

    atories, 1993), today the media continue to focus on student problems while ignor-

    ing student success. Indeed, as Berliner and Biddle (1998) argue, the media has

    played an overarching role in societys negative and narrow perceptions of youth.

    They outline specific instances wherein the media is guilty of episodic reporting,

    not only by failing to comment on contextual issues specific to the story, but also by

    failing to consider broader cultural and societal issues that have relevance to their

    reports. Others, too, have commented upon the fact that the media, including even

    government reports, focus on problems to such an extent that a fair appraisal of

    American students and their schools is not presented to the public (Gough, 1994).

    Indeed, an entire book has been published that bears tribute to the irresponsibility

    of the press when it comes to education (Maeroff, 1998).

    Although some writers have attempted to bring some of the successes of Amer-

    ican education to public attention, the tendency to believe and to report theworst about American youth and their teachers continues. Indeed, some have ar-

    gued that because of their unrelenting tendency to report only the negative qualities

    of youth, schools, and teachers, the media have become allies of critics who

    desire to tear down our public schools (Berliner & Biddle, 1998). Independent of

    whether a conspiracy or an alliance exists, there are ample data to show that the

    press is more likely to identify with negative rather than positive aspects of schools

    and student performance (Rose, Gallup, & Elam, 1997). Further, some claim that

    the medias distortion of the public school has a negative effect upon citizens

    beliefs about schools in general but not the perception of schools in their immediate

    area. Indeed, the authors of a recent Phi Delta Kappa/Gallup Poll conclude that,

    the low grades given the nations public schools are primarily media-induced.Whereas people learn first hand about their childrens schools, they learn about the

    nations schools primarily from the media (Rose, Gallup, & Elam, 1997, p. 47).

    Recently, the College Board released data which showed that students perform-

    ance in math was strong and improving while their performance in reading was

    essentially stable. However, the primary focus of the medias reporting of these

    recent results has been primarily negative claiming that student performance,

    although improving in mathematics, is not good enough for the modern world

    and that grade inflation in American schools is rampant. Ironically, news stories

    about grade inflation typically fail to report evidence that many youth appear highly

    committed to academic achievement. To illustrate, the most recent results from the

    College Board exams show that the number of youth taking and successfully

    passing advanced placement exams is at an all-time high. And, in a recent newsarticle it was pointed out that since 1987 the proportion of students with A averages

    had increased from 28% to 37% (Jameson, 1997). Later, in the same article it was

    noted that 32,000 students were able to enter college as sophomores or juniors

    because of their advanced high school work, and that a record 400,000 students

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    7/40

    UNDERESTIMATING YOUTH 7

    were entering college with advanced placement credits. Ironically, in this article,

    no possible connection was made between students receiving As in high school

    and their advanced placement progress. Is it possible that, in some cases, students

    actually earned their higher grades? In fact, it seems probable that both things may

    be true (i.e., there is grade inflation, and many students today are exceeding the

    performance of yesterdays students on advanced placement tests). Yet, the media

    appear unwilling to deal with complexity, and when they simplify matters, they

    tend to favor reporting negative slants on youths achievement.

    As another example of questionable reporting, we cite a headline that appeared

    on September 1, 1997 in the Arizona Daily Star that preceded a story by Bill

    Schackner on NAEP test scores which was reprinted from the Pittsburgh Post-

    Gazette Although SAT scores fell, number of As increased (Schackner, 1997).

    This article exhibited at least three problems. Despite its flagrantly negative head-

    line, the article revealed that: (a) math SAT scores increased, and (b) reading SAT

    scores stayed about the same. In addition, although the article discussed issues

    of grade inflation extensively, it (c) failed to report that students made notableprogress on advanced placement tests. If we wanted to quibble further, we might

    also note that high school students scores went up in their math assessment scores

    (a subject that is taught in high school) and stayed the same in reading (a subject

    not taught in most high schools). Nor was this the only negatively found media

    report on the topic. Although some writers were more balanced in their approach

    (Applebome, 1997), most news stories were decidedly critical in reporting these

    NAEP results.

    Or, to cite another example, the recent extraordinary good news that one-fifth

    of American students entering college were eligible for credit through advanced

    placement was so underreported in the press that we were prompted to obtain the

    College Board press release on the topic to see if part of the medias interpret-ation/reporting was due to this primary document (College Board Press Release,

    1996). In contrast to newspaper reports, the press release was generally positive in

    tone. Indeed part of the press release title suggested that the numbers of students

    obtaining advanced placement credit will increase by 50% by the year 2000. Ad-

    ditionally, the press release included sections on College standards and no grade

    inflation, Participation growing for women and minorities, and Positive role

    models for teens. Information on these topics was strikingly absent from media

    reports on students achievement. This suggests that in reporting on grade inflation

    and student achievement, some media writers appear to work hard to recast the

    news release from a positive and affirming story to one portraying a more negative

    perspective of youth.

    It is beyond the scope of this paper to explore in detail the motivation of apublic press that exhibits a selective bias for more negative reporting on youth, their

    teachers, and their schools. This is a complex topic that merits extended treatment

    elsewhere. Here, we examine only one possible factor that seems to be involved

    the complexity of educational research and the way educators report their findings,

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    8/40

    8 THOMAS L. GOOD ET AL.

    especially when they write for popular audiences. It would appear that the public,

    press, and social scientists alike need to become more sensitive to diversity to

    variations among students, teachers, and schools. Given these variations, when it

    comes to education, the concept of average is often virtually meaningless.

    To sum up, we have argued for two major conclusions regarding the media.

    First, current media depictions of youth and education tend to be highly negative

    in tone. Second, the media tend to describe youth and education in monolithic

    terms; both are largely described as undifferentiated averages. In many contexts,

    such depictions are highly misleading. When variability is strikingly large (i.e.,

    when youth behavior is highly variable or there are many good schools and poor

    schools as well as average schools) portrayals that report only average findings are

    highly misleading.

    Problems in Research Reporting: A Case Example

    Unfortunately, it is not only policy makers and the media who make sweeping

    and unfounded policy assertions about youth and their schools. Some researchers

    also allow themselves to join this chorus of negativism. For example, Laurence

    Steinberg, the major author of Beyond the Classroom (Steinberg et al., 1996)

    henceforth referred to as Beyond argued that American students level of aca-

    demic achievement is woefully inadequate (especially when compared with stu-

    dents in other countries), and this is caused by parent apathy and a peer culture

    that discourages achievement. Such a conclusion makes questionable assumptions,

    is negative in tone, and ignores the possibility that youth, their parents, and their

    peers might differ widely in such matters.

    Indeed, the reporting in Beyond exemplifies the argument that we made earlier

    about the inadequacies of episodic reporting. Steinberg inadequately describedcontextual variation within or among schools and students, and this led him to

    argue that the policy recommendations he proposed should apply to all students and

    schools. Thus, like the popular press, this publication glamorizes and reifies

    the concept of average. For example, Steinberg wrote:

    More than one-third of the students we surveyed showed signs of being emo-

    tionally disengaged from school, as indexed by measures of mind-wandering,

    lack of interest, or inattentiveness. Half of the students we surveyed say their

    classes are boring. A third say they have lost interest in school, that they are

    not learning very much, and that they get through the school day by fooling

    around with their classmates. And remember, ours was a sample of aver-

    age students in average American schools not a sample of high-riskyoungsters in high-risk school settings. (p. 71).

    Although we found it difficult to believe that any sample based on only nine high

    schools could represent average students in average American schools, we were

    motivated to explore the technical details of the research sample, findings, and

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    9/40

    UNDERESTIMATING YOUTH 9

    methodology on which Beyond was supposedly based. Accordingly, we explored

    all the cited articles found in an Appendix in Beyond, which, according to the

    author, These articles contain technical details about the research design and data

    analysis (p. 199). In our examination of all listed publications, we could not find

    adequate descriptions of the sample or data analysis. In particular, we found no

    detailed information about within- and between-school variations on the dependent

    measures nor variation across types of students in different settings.

    Although Beyond offered some general insights, we judge that its conclusions

    and policy recommendations were strikingly unjustified and overgeneralized. How-

    ever, despite our conclusion that the policy statements were too general, often

    unsupported by data, and in some cases flatly wrong, the book quickly proved

    to be attractive both to the media and educational policy makers (see, for example,

    Leslie, 1996; Shanker, 1996a, 1996b). Indeed, we wonder if the averages reported

    in the book describe any individual school in his sample adequately.

    Unfortunately, claims made in Beyond often paraphrased negative and mono-

    lithic judgments about youth that appear frequently in the media today. Whendescribing their supposed results about students cheating, for example, Steinberg

    wrote:

    Two-thirds of the students in our sample say they cheated on a school test

    during the past school year. [And] nearly nine out ten students in our sample

    say they copied someone elses homework some time during the past year.

    (p. 18)

    Such reporting is inherently misleading because there is likely to be variation

    in both how students define concepts like cheating (e.g., letting someone copy

    my test vs. copying someone elses test) and the degree to which they engage

    in different types of cheating. In addition, they gloss over important definitional

    issues. Do students define cheating the same way as adults? And does the typeof cheating engaged in differ markedly among students, as it does among adults

    (who might cheat by underreporting earned income, not paying a parking ticket,

    exceeding the speed limit, or engaging in seriously illegal or immoral acts). Thus,

    to make sweeping claims about students cheating without investigating such vari-

    ations seems irresponsible. Further, it seems likely that the degree of student cheat-

    ing however defined will vary sharply from student to student, as well as from

    school to school.

    Cheating is but one example of the many factors that Steinberg used in exploring

    youths attitude toward school (other of the many issues raised included students

    excessively cutting school, parents failure to press students for good grades, low

    rates of student academic engagement, too little homework, etc.). Another conclu-sion reached by Steinberg was that the peer culture (as studied in nine American

    high schools) did not support student achievement, and there was little that parents

    or schools could do about the situation. Steinberg wrote, The adolescent peer

    culture in contemporary America demeans academic success and scorns students

    who try to do well in school. Schools are fighting a losing battle against a peer

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    10/40

    10 THOMAS L. GOOD ET AL.

    culture that disparages academic success (p. 19). As part of the data provided to

    defend this argument, Steinberg argued:

    Fewer than one in five students say their friends think it is important to get

    good grades in school. Less than one-fourth of all students regularly discusstheir school work with their friends ... nearly 20% of all students say they do

    not try as hard as they can in school because they are worried about what their

    friends might think. (p. 19)

    To conclude that peer groups exert an almost intractable and pernicious effect on

    students achievement seemed to us both premature and likely wrong. Studies of

    schools as learning communities have consistently shown wide variation in how

    students perceive and react to school environments (Battistich, Solomon, Watson,

    & Schaps, 1997; Shouse, 1996; Solomon, Battistich, Dong-Il, & Watson, 1997).

    Serious scholarship on peer groups and peer cultures has provided clear evidence

    that peer cultures have important influences on student attitudes toward school

    and academic work. But, peer attitudes toward school work do not constitute a

    monolithic and unalterable set of forces. Bank (1997), in a comprehensive review

    of youth in school settings, argued that peer cultures take different forms and that

    youth are also influenced by parents and by schools.

    On the basis ofBeyond, many media sources endorsed policy recommendations

    for schooling and adolescents we thought to be misguided (Leslie, 1996; Shanker,

    1996a, 1996b). Thus, it seemed important (a) to examine possible variability in

    adolescents attitudes toward school more closely, and (b) to assess whether Stein-

    bergs pejorative descriptions of youths attitudes and academic behavioral patterns

    could be supported.

    Purpose of This Study

    The purpose of our study was to examine students attitudes and perceptions in

    three major areas of focus including cheating, homework, and peer pressure. In

    Beyond, Steinberg claimed that students do not do enough homework, they cheat,

    and that pervasive inappropriate peer pressure undermines high student achieve-

    ment claims that mirror negative judgments about youth frequently made in

    the media. These claims seemed to us to be overly negative and to ignore vari-

    ability associated with such forms of youth behavior. Nor were these claims the

    only examples of negatively toned, overly simple generalizations to be found in

    Beyond. Indeed, Appendix A provides other examples from Steinberg illustrating

    these types of claims. We could not take up all of these issues, however, but we

    decided to assess Steinbergs claims about youth cheating, homework completion,and susceptibility to peer influence with a new sample of students and high schools.

    In our study, we addressed two research questions. First, to what extent do the

    negative conclusions reported by Steinberg about homework, cheating, and peer

    pressure apply in the new schools we studied. Second, would we find significant

    and substantial variations for these issues among students who differed by schools

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    11/40

    UNDERESTIMATING YOUTH 11

    they attended as well as by common demographic features frequently found in

    research on youth.

    Our choice of independent variables for our study was straightforward. As noted

    earlier, considerable evidence has appeared suggesting that youth conduct differs

    among schools, so we chose two, different schools for our study. In addition, we de-

    cided to focus on two demographic variables gender and level of prior academic

    achievement. Reasons for these latter decisions should be set forth.

    GENDER RELATED DIFFERENCES

    For some time it has been well known that students attitudes toward school differ

    as a function of gender. Tenenbaum (1940) collected essays on the topic, Do you

    like school? from 639 sixth- and seventh-grade students. The findings indicated

    that, in general, students liked school; but girls reported more positive affect to-

    wards school than boys. Roughly two-thirds of girls had distinctly positive attitudes

    towards school as compared to slightly less than one-half of the boys. Similar

    gender findings have continued to be reported over time (e.g., Jackson, 1968).

    Further, related findings have suggested that female students find schools fairer

    than do males (e.g., Nichols & Good, 1998). In marked contrast to female students

    more favorable perceptions of school, a substantial literature has suggested that

    female students, as a group, receive less academic support from teachers than do

    male students. Given that researchers continue to find different patterns of teacher

    feedback to students and different forms of assigned academic tasks on the basis

    of student gender, studies that wish to explore students perceptions of schooling

    should certainly examine gender as a mediating factor (see Jones & Gerig, 1994;

    Jones & Wheatley, 1990).

    PRIOR ACHIEVEMENT

    Oddly, despite the obvious fact that some students are consistently more likely

    to experience academic success, strong relationships between student satisfaction

    with school and academic achievement have been difficult to establish. In part,

    this may reflect the fact that academic success is but one source of satisfaction

    in schools. Students are more than academic learners, they are also social beings

    (McCaslin & Good, 1996). Indeed, in some cases, a students academic success

    may come at the expense of social relationships (e.g., complying with the teachers

    request to correct a friends wrong answer may irritate the friend). Similarly, some

    high-success students may become bored in school, etc.Jackson (1968) tried to explain the absence of a relationship between school

    satisfaction and achievement this way: Perhaps were attitudes to interact with

    achievement, they may have to be extreme, and extreme attitudes, either positive

    or negative, may be much rarer than is commonly thought" (p. 81). However,

    as Jackson also noted, the lack of a documented relationship between academic

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    12/40

    12 THOMAS L. GOOD ET AL.

    success and school satisfaction merits our continued attention because it appears

    to contradict our common sense. And, although the relation between student sat-

    isfaction and student achievement may be complex, a substantial field of research

    has appeared exploring the effects of student aptitude and achievement on numer-

    ous school-related dispositions (see, for example, Gustafsson & Undheim, 1996;

    Snow, Corno, & Jackson, 1996). This suggested that differences in prior achieve-

    ment among students will mediate students beliefs and behavior if one asks about

    specific aspects of school rather than general, normative statements about school

    and schooling. When students school experiences are placed in a specific context,

    students behaviors and perceptions (e.g., peer support for academic activity) may

    be sharply mediated by prior achievement status.

    To summarize then, in the research we report, we examined how students in a

    new sample of schools responded to questions about homework, cheating, and peer

    pressure and how those responses varied depending on the school they attended,

    their gender, and the level of their prior achievement.

    Method

    PROCEDURES

    Data for the study were collected in two public schools in a Southwestern city

    one a junior high, the other a senior high school and similar techniques were used

    for gathering data in the two venues.

    High School. The data we gathered were part of a larger study exploring youths

    attitudes. Four questionnaires were developed to obtain a wide range of information

    from students (and to assess how variations in asking questions might influence

    student responses). The items we now report appeared on one of the four formsthat were distributed.

    Permission to conduct the study was obtained from both the principal of the high

    school and the superintendent of the school district. Parents were informed about

    the questionnaire study in writing and given the opportunity to refuse their childs

    participation, and less than 1% did so. Each student was asked to fill out only one

    form, and procedures were set so that forms were assigned to students randomly.

    Students were also given Scantron answer sheets to be used for indicating their

    answers. All questionnaires were handed out on a Thursday morning during second

    period, because this was reported to be one of the peak attendance times during

    the week. Upon administration, students were asked to provide honest, thoughtful

    responses to all questions.Junior High School. In contrast to the high school study, one questionnaire was

    used that explored students attitudes on a variety of topics including cheating,

    homework, and peer pressure. Permission was obtained from the principal and the

    superintendent of the school district. Parents were informed about the questionnaire

    study in writing and given the opportunity to refuse their childs participation, and

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    13/40

    UNDERESTIMATING YOUTH 13

    less than 1% did so. Teachers were given the questionnaire and Scantron answer

    sheets with a four-day time period to administer it to their students.

    PARTICIPANTS

    High School. In all, 347 students out of approximately 1,750 students in the school

    provided data for this study. Student participants were enrolled in Grades 9 through

    12. Fifty-two percent were female, and 48% were male. Approximately 4% were

    African-American, 6% were American-Indian, 3% were Asian-American, 23%

    were Hispanic-American, 50% were Caucasian-American, 6% were Inter-racially

    mixed, and about 8% of the students reported their ethnicity as other. Students

    socioeconomic status was estimated by how they paid for lunch: 46% reported they

    paid full price, 6% paid reduced price, 15% received a free lunch, 6% brought their

    lunch, and 25% reported other.

    Junior High School. Participants included 358 students from the junior high

    school which was the feeder school for the high school also used in this study.

    Students ages ranged from 12 to 15 years. Fifty-two percent were male, while

    48% were female. Four percent indicated their ethnicity as Black, 25% Hispanic,

    2% Native American, 63% White, and 6% reported other as their ethnicity. We

    again used how students paid for their lunch as a general indicator of SES, and

    43% of the students reported that they paid full price, 10% paid reduced price, 25%

    received a free lunch, 8% brought their own lunch, and 14% indicated other.

    INSTRUMENT

    Although the questionnaires given to high school and junior high school samples

    differed in other respects, items analyzed for purposes of this study were the samein both surveys. Items included in the analyses pertained to homework, cheat-

    ing, and peer pressure perceptions. Specifically, there were seven homework items

    which probed students for information regarding various ways they might define

    homework as well as their behavior patterns pertaining to (including the value

    they placed on) homework see Appendix B (e.g., How many hours per day

    [on average] do you spend on homework?; It is still considered homework when

    I finish assignments during school time; If I dont finish homework, it is usually

    because I dont have time). Similarly, students were asked to indicate how they

    defined cheating as well as their behavior regarding cheating based on six items

    see Appendix C (e.g., It is cheating if I copy someone elses test; It is cheating

    if I copy someone elses homework; I usually cheat on my homework assign-ments). Lastly, students were asked to indicate their level of agreement based on

    six items relating to peer pressure see Appendix D (e.g., I am not doing as well

    as I could because my friends would make fun of me; If I spend a lot of time

    doing homework, my friends will make fun of me; In terms of academics, I feel

    pressure to perform in school like my friends). As indicated in the Appendices,

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    14/40

    14 THOMAS L. GOOD ET AL.

    most questions were to be answered by choosing a response from a four-category,

    Likert-type scale.

    Identity of the school in which respondents were enrolled was generated by

    study-design information. Information about gender was provided by students in

    response to a questionnaire. Junior high school students were asked to indicate their

    average prior grade in school (GPA) and were given the five choices of A, B, C,

    D, or Below D. High school students were asked to indicate their prior cumulative

    GPA, and these were transcribed into the following ranges: 3.6 to 4.0 = A; 3.1 to

    3.5 = B; 2.6 to 3.0 = C; 1.5 to 2.5 = D; and Below 1.5 = Below D.

    ANALYSIS

    Central Tendencies and Variations. To examine central tendencies and variations

    in responses, we computed mean scores for each relevant category of respondents.

    These are displayed in the tables to be found below. The degree of variation in

    responses may be difficult to judge from these tables, however, so we also pre-

    pared figures for three crucial variables which display that variation. These are

    also provided below.

    Main Effects. We examined the data for interactions but did not find any beyond

    chance levels; therefore, only main effects are discussed here. For both schools,

    independent sample t tests were conducted to compare female and male responses

    to items regarding homework, cheating, and peer pressure. One-way analysis of

    variance tests were conducted to compare students who differed by prior GPA

    across all dependent variables.

    Effect Size. We used two different measures of effect size to represent the mag-nitude of our findings for variation. The first, Cohens d, was used to assess the sizes

    of standardized mean difference between the two categories of gender. The second

    was one of two roughly equivalent measures of covariation, the point-biserial cor-

    relation, r, and eta. When assessing the independent variable of gender, this second

    measure was the point-biserial correlation (r) between gender and the dependent

    variable. When assessing the independent variable of academic achievement, more

    than two response categories were involved, so we used eta to represent effect size.

    Presenting Cohens dand rtogether in Tables 1, 3, and 5 (concerned with gender)

    enables the reader to interpret the effect sizes for Tables 2, 4, and 6 (concerned

    with prior GPA) when Cohens d is not appropriate. Although eta and r are not

    strictly comparable because they involve differing formulas (see Murray & Dosser,1987), the interpretations of these two statistics are close enough to Cohens cri-

    teria. Cohen (1988) suggested that one may use the same standards for interpreting

    the size of eta and r, namely: small = .1, medium = .243, and large = .371. These

    conventions correspond to the values of .2, .5, and .8 respectively for the dstatistic

    that was also developed by Cohen.

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    15/40

    UNDERESTIMATING YOUTH 15

    strongly agree

    agree

    disagree

    strongly disagree

    Percent

    50

    40

    30

    20

    10

    0

    sex: % response

    M: 52.2%

    F: 47.8%

    Figure 1. Response Distribution of Homework by Gender in JHS: If I dont finish my

    homework, its because I dont care.

    Results

    HOMEWORK

    Table 1 reports means and standard deviations for males and females in both the

    high school and junior high school for the seven homework items, and Table 2

    reports the same data broken down by categories of prior GPA. These tables reveal

    two major effects: average responses to questions about homework were a lot less

    negative in tone than those claimed by Steinberg; and those responses varied a

    good deal as a function of school, gender, and prior GPA. It may be difficult to seethese effects in Tables 1 and 2, however, so we have also drawn up four figures that

    display response distributions for one of the homework variables, If I dont finish

    homework, its because I dont care. (see Figures 14)

    In addition, Tables 1 and 2 display statistics that access the directions and sizes

    of differences among mean responses we found for types of students. To illustrate,

    female students reported that they spent significantly more time doing homework

    than did males in both schools. Of the remaining six items, males and females

    differed significantly on two items in junior high and four in high school. Within

    the junior high, males indicated a higher level of agreement than did females for

    item H4 (If I dont finish my homework, it is usually because I dont care), while

    females indicated a higher level of agreement on item H5 (Homework helps melearn). Within the high school sample, females indicated a higher level of agree-

    ment on items H3 (If I dont finish my homework, it is usually because I dont

    have time) and H5 (Homework helps me learn). In contrast, males reported a

    higher level of agreement on item H4 (If I dont finish my homework, its because

    I dont care). Finally, in the high school sample, although both males and females

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    16/40

    Table I. Homework Responses: Means, Standard Deviations, Cohens d, and Point Biser

    Junior high

    Female Male Female

    (N = 160) (N = 179) (N = 175)

    M SD M SD d r M SD

    H1: How many hours per day do you

    spend on homework 2.26 1.19 1.99 1.05 .246 .177 2.17 1.14

    H2: It is considered homework when

    finished during school time 2.82 .79 2.88 .95 .061 .031 2.83 .80

    H3: If I dont finish homework, its

    because I dont have time 2.90 .93 2.76 1.01 .144 .072 2.98 .86

    H4: If I dont finish homework, itsbecause I dont care 1.78 .89 2.15 1.02 .385 .189 1.97 .84

    H5: Homework helps me learn 2.90 .87 2.67 1.07 .247 .122 2.86 .84

    H6: Ill get good grade if I do my

    homework 3.31 .76 3.21 .84 .119 .060 3.20 .71

    H7: If I was assigned better homework

    I would learn more 2.58 .93 2.45 1.12 .124 .062 2.39 .83

    Note: Item H1 is based on the scale 1 = less than 1 hr, 2 = about 1 hr, 3 = about 2 hrs, 4 = about 3 hrs, 5 = over

    strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = agree, 4 = strongly agree. Independent samples t tests (two-tailed) were con

    means within each school where p < .05; p < .01; p < .001. Cohens d = [XF XM] / [SDpooled].

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    17/40

    Table II. Homework Responses by Reported Academic Grades: Means, Standard Deviation

    Junior high school

    Grade A B C

    (N = 94) (N = 123) (N = 91) (N

    Means and standard deviations M SD M SD M SD M

    H1: How many hours per day do you spend on homework 2.41 1.26 2.13 1.05 1.99 1.06 1.5

    H2: It is considered homework when finished during school time 2.90 .87 2.94 .78 2.71 .94 2.9

    H3: If I dont finish my homework, its because I dont have time 2.92 .92 2.86 .95 2.69 .97 2.7

    H4: If I dont finish my homework, its because I dont care 1.65 .86 1.92 .91 2.18 .91 2.5

    H5: Homework helps me learn the material 3.08 .87 2.79 .92 2.66 .99 2.

    H6: I will get a good grade if I do my homework 3.35 .83 3.29 .69 3.31 .74 2.9

    H7: If I was assigned better homework I would learn more 2.57 .97 2.48 1.01 2.48 1.06 2.

    High school

    Grade A B C

    (N = 63) (N = 116) (N = 81) (N

    Means and standard deviations M SD M SD M SD M

    H1: How many hours per day do you spend on homework 2.40 1.17 2.08 1.14 1.84 1.03 1.8

    H2: It is considered homework when finished during school time 2.98 .77 2.87 .98 2.87 .93 3.0

    H3: If I dont finish my homework, its because I dont have time 3.00 .98 2.87 .94 2.65 .94 2.7

    H4: If I dont finish my homework, its because I dont care 1.92 .89 1.98 .87 2.23 .97 2.4

    H5: Homework helps me learn the material 2.78 .82 2.76 .87 2.57 .92 2.6

    H6: I will get a good grade if I do my homework 3.11 .73 3.13 .72 3.02 .75 3.

    H7: If I was assigned better homework I would learn more 2.41 .83 2.32 .85 2.35 .94 2.6

    Note: Item H1 is based on the scale 1 = less than 1 hr, 2 = about 1 hr, 3 = about 2 hrs, 4 = about 3 hrs, on the scale 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = agree, 4 = strongly agree. One-way analysis of va

    GPA where p < .05; p < .01; p < .001.

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    18/40

    18 THOMAS L. GOOD ET AL.

    strongly agree

    agree

    disagree

    strongly disagree

    Percent

    50

    40

    30

    20

    10

    0

    sex: % response

    M: 48.7%

    F: 51.3%

    Figure 2. Response Distribution of Homework by Gender in HS: If I dont finish my

    homework, its because I dont care.

    strongly agree

    agree

    disagree

    strongly disagree

    Percent

    70

    60

    50

    40

    30

    20

    10

    0

    Cum GPA: % response

    < D: 2.0%

    D: 6.3%

    C: 27.3%

    B: 36.1%

    A: 28.4%

    Figure 3. Response Distribution of Homework by GPA in JHS: If I dont finish my homework,

    its because I dont care.

    generally agreed with item H6 (Ill get a good grade if I do my homework),

    respectively, females agreed more strongly than males.Within the junior high school, students differed with respect to reported grades

    on items H1, H4, H5, and H6. Students with higher GPAs indicated that they (a)

    spent more hours per day on their homework, (b) more strongly disagreed that if

    they did not finish their homework, its because they did not care, (c) more strongly

    agreed that homework helps them learn the material, and (d) more strongly agreed

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    19/40

    UNDERESTIMATING YOUTH 19

    strongly agree

    agree

    disagree

    strongly disagree

    Percent

    50

    40

    30

    20

    10

    0

    Cum GPA: % response

    < D: 4.3%

    D: 18.6%

    C: 24.1%

    B: 34.2%

    A: 18.8%

    Figure 4. Response Distribution of Homework by GPA in HS: If I dont finish my homework,

    its because I dont care.

    that they will get a good grade if they do their homework. Within the high school,

    students differed significantly by prior GPA category on items H1, H3, H4, H5, and

    H7. In comparison to students with lower GPAs, students with higher GPAs said

    they (a) spent more time on homework, (b) more strongly disagreed that if they did

    not finish their homework, it is because they did not care, (c) more strongly agreed

    that homework helps them learn the material, and (d) more strongly disagreed

    that if they were assigned better homework, they would learn more. Furthermore,

    students who indicated their average grade was an A or Below D more strongly

    agreed that if they did not finish their homework, it was because they did not havetime.

    CHEATING

    Means and standard deviations calculated for six cheating items are reported for

    gender in Table 3 and prior GPA in Table 4. In addition, Figures 5 through 8

    display response distributions for one of the cheating variables, I usually cheat

    on exams. As can be seen again in these tables and figures, the cheating responses

    we found were far less negative in tone and far more variable than findings claimed

    by Steinberg.

    In addition, within both schools, students differed according to gender on itemsC2 and C3 (see Table 3). In both cases, female means were higher than male means

    indicating a stronger level of agreement by females that it is cheating if they copy

    someones elses test (C2) and if they let someone else copy their test (C3).

    Students in junior high school with higher prior GPAs also agreed more frequently

    with items C1, C2 and C3 (It is cheating if I copy someone elses homework;

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    20/40

    Table III. Cheating Responses: Means, Standard Deviations, Cohens d, and Point Biser

    Junior high High

    Female Male Female

    (N = 164) (N = 175) (N = 176

    M SD M SD d r M SD

    C1: Cheating if I copy someone elses homework 3.07 .80 2.91 .91 .181 .090 2.90 .81

    C2: Cheating if I copy someone elses test 3.52 .70 3.35 .87 .213 .106 3.62 .65

    C3: Cheating if I let someone else copy my test 3.25 .85 2.96 .97 .305

    .151 3.29 .80C4: Cheating if I let someone copy my homework 2.86 .88 2.76 .97 .106 .053 2.75 .88

    C5: I usually cheat on tests 1.60 .77 1.73 .87 .156 .078 1.59 .78

    C6: I usually cheat on my homework assignments 1.81 .81 1.97 .95 .177 .088 1.94 .82

    Note: Items C1-C6 are based on the scale 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = agree, 4 = strongly

    t tests (two-tailed) were conducted to compare female and male means within each school where p

    .001. Cohens d = [XF XM] / [SDpooled].

  • 7/30/2019 Underestimating Youths Commitment to Schools and Society: Toward a More Differentiated View

    21/40

    Table IV. Cheating Responses by Reported Average Grades: Means, Standard Deviations

    Junior high schoolGrade A B C

    (N = 95) (N = 123) (N = 88) (N

    Means and standard deviations M SD M SD M SD M

    C1: It is considered cheating if I copy someone elses homework 3.11 .76 3.00 .81 2.99 .87 2.7

    C2: It is considered cheating if I copy someone elses test 3.59 .68 3.48 .69 3.38 .82 3.1

    C3: It is considered cheating if I let someone else copy my test 3.40 .75 3.00 .90 3.02 .95 2.9

    C4: It is cheating if I let someone else copy my homework 3.01 .81 2.66 .96 2.81 .92 2.8

    C5: I usually cheat on exams 1.40 .61 1.69 .79 1.70 .85 2.1

    C6: I usually cheat on homework assignments 1.68 .75 1.90 .91 2.00 .89 2.3

    High school

    Grade A B C

    (N = 64) (N = 117) (N = 82) (N

    Means and standard deviations M SD M SD M SD M

    C1: It is considered cheating if I copy someone elses homework 2.96 .84 2.88 .84 2.83 .85 2.6

    C2: It is considered cheating if I copy someone elses test 3.83 .42 3.53 .75 3.36 .88 3.1

    C3: It is considered cheating if I let someone else copy my test 3.35 .76 3.19 .89 3.00 .97 3.0

    C4: It is cheating if I let someone else copy my homework 2.69 .88 2.73 .88 2.65 .93 2.6

    C5: I usually cheat on exams 1.33 .59 1.52 .77 1.84 .88 1.8

    C6: I usually cheat on homework assignments 1.77 .81 1.82 .73 2.16 .79 2.1

    Note: Items C1-C6 are based on the scale 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = agree, 4 = strongl

    variance calculated by reported GPA where

    p


Recommended