+ All Categories
Home > Documents > USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout...

USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout...

Date post: 13-Feb-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 2 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
44
USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinking Working Paper No. 15-08 Doing it Twice, Getting it Right? The Effects of Grade Retention and Course Repetition in Higher Education Darjusch Tafreschi and Petra Thiemann February 2, 2015
Transcript
Page 1: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2561048

USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinking

Working Paper No. 15-08

Doing it Twice, Getting it Right?

The Effects of Grade Retention and Course

Repetition in Higher Education

Darjusch Tafreschi and Petra Thiemann

February 2, 2015

Page 2: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2561048

Doing it Twice, Getting it Right?

The Effects of Grade Retention and Course

Repetition in Higher Education∗

Darjusch Tafreschi†and Petra Thiemann‡

February 2, 2015

Abstract

This paper is the first to study the effects of grade retention and course repetition onstudent performance in higher education. We apply a sharp regression discontinuitydesign to administrative data from a Swiss university. First-year undergraduates whofall short of a pre-defined performance requirement have to repeat all first-year coursesbefore they can proceed to the second year. We find that grade retention increasesdropout probabilities after the first year by about 10 percentage points. Repetition ofa full year persistently boosts grade point averages by about 0.5 standard deviations,but does not affect study pace and major choices.JEL codes: I21, I23, J24Keywords: grade retention, course repetition, higher education, dropout, academicachievement, regression discontinuity

∗A special thanks goes to Sharon Pfister who appeared as a coauthor on an earlier version of thispaper. Her insights were very valuable during the initial stages of this project. We thank Michael Lechner,Hans Fricke, Martin Huber, Sacha Kapoor, Charles Manski, Giovanni Mellace, Marco Manacorda, Hansvan Kippersluis, Conny Wunsch, and Ludger Wössmann, as well as conference participants at the EALEconference in Bonn, the SMYE in Mannheim, the SSES conference in Zurich, the YSEM in Berne, and theRGS conference in Duisburg-Essen for helpful comments and discussions. The usual disclaimer applies.†Erasmus University Rotterdam, Department of Applied Economics, PO Box 1738, 3000DR Rotterdam,

The Netherlands, phone: +31 (0)10 40 81383, [email protected].‡University of Southern California, USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinking and Department

of Economics, 3620 S Vermont Ave, Los Angeles, CA 90089, USA, phone: +1 (213) 821-2035, [email protected].

Page 3: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2561048

1 Introduction

At many universities and colleges around the world, students who fall short of certain

performance standards are forced or encouraged to repeat failed courses, sometimes a

full semester, or even a full year.1 These (grade) retention policies are commonly designed

to improve the match between students’ skill levels and the level of teaching, and thus to

help initially low-performing students succeed in college.

Grade retention, however, is a controversial policy, as little is known about its effec-

tiveness to persistently boost students’ performance in higher education.2 Does retention

improve students’ attainment levels by confronting them again with the course material?

Or does retention rather damage students’ performances, for example through negative

side-effects on soft factors like confidence and social interactions? Does retention lead

to higher student dropout rates? Retention does not only potentially harm the retained

students, but it also imposes high monetary costs on institutions: Public post-secondary

institutions in OECD countries spend on average 9,000 USD annually per student on core

educational services like instruction (OECD, 2014).

The main obstacle to the study of retention is (self-)selection into retention and course

repetition. Students often self-select into retention or are retained by their institutions.

Retained students may thus differ systematically from their fellow students, for example,

in terms of prior performance. Hence, uncorrected differences in educational outcomes

between retained and non-retained students are likely to reflect differences in student

characteristics rather than the effects of retention.

This paper is the first to study grade retention in higher education. We exploit a grade1These policies are widely implemented. For example: Course repetition exists at California Commu-

nity Colleges, the University of California at Berkeley, the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, HarvardUniversity, Northwestern University, New York University. Repetition of a full semester exists at PrincetonUniversity. Retention of a full year occurs at the Federal Institute of Technology in Zurich, the University ofSt. Gallen (both Switzerland), and Queen Mary University of London. Institutions have idiosyncratic rulesfor retention, and for how repeated courses count towards grade point averages. Unfortunately, we are notable to provide a comprehensive overview over retention policies around the world, because of the lack ofdata.

2An alternative policy is “remedial education” at the college level, which refers to the repetition of high-school-level courses. Bettinger and Long (2009) find a positive impact of remedial education.

2

Page 4: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

retention policy at the University of St. Gallen (Switzerland) in a sharp regression discon-

tinuity design. First-year students who do not achieve a strict performance requirement

after the first year have to repeat all first-year courses before they can proceed to the

second year. We exploit local variation in the retention status around the performance

threshold (henceforth also “cutoff”) in a sharp regression discontinuity design and esti-

mate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all

first-year courses on outcomes that occur after repeating: grade point averages, credits

obtained per semester, and major choice. Both performance and choice outcomes are

important for later labour market success (c.f. Altonji et al. (2012), Arcidiacono (2004)).

This paper contributes to both the empirical literature and the policy debate on retention

policies at different schooling levels. The effects of grade retention on individual student

outcomes are difficult to predict theoretically (c.f. Manacorda (2012)), which motivated a

number of empirical studies (see below for a review). While positive effects may arise be-

cause of learning gains and a better match between students’ knowledge and the level of

teaching, negative effects can occur because of stigmatisation by teachers or classmates,

lower self-confidence, and slow adjustment to a new classroom environment.

We are not aware of any empirical study on the effects of grade retention in the context

of higher education. By contrast, researchers have so far focused on retention in primary

and secondary education. Recent studies find positive effects on grades, especially for

primary school children.3 Yet, grade retention increases dropout rates of children during

high school.4 Overall, the effects of grade retention appear to be age-dependent, with

rather positive results for primary school, and rather negative results in high school.

The effects of grade retention and course repetition in higher education may differ

from results in primary and secondary education for at least three reasons. First, mature3An unequivocally positive effect on test scores seems to exist for retained 3rd graders in the US. Three

independent studies find a positive effect for Chicago (Jacob and Lefgren, 2004), Texas (Lorence andDworkin, 2006), and Florida (Greene and Winters, 2007). This result, however, does not hold for 6th graders(Jacob and Lefgren (2004)). Roderick and Nagaoka (2005) find even negative effects on test scores of 6thgraders in Chicago. All outcomes examined in these studies are short-term outcomes, that is, measured1–3 years after grade retention.

4This result has been confirmed by Jacob and Lefgren (2009) for 6th graders in Chicago, by Ou (2010)for 9th graders in New Jersey, and by Manacorda (2012) for 7th to 9th graders in Uruguay.

3

Page 5: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

students may cope better with negative events, and thus, the negative effects may in

general be less pronounced for older students. Moreover, stigmatisation by fellow students

and instructors may be mitigated because university students are less exposed to small-

classroom settings. Bonding with initial cohort members may be stronger or weaker in a

university environment, too. Second, the effects of grade retention on dropout probabilities

may be higher for university students. In contrast to high school education, university

education is voluntary. Furthermore, university dropouts have better outside options than

high school dropouts. Third, university students may benefit especially from repeating the

first year. Some students need additional time to develop new study habits, such as self-

guided learning. For these students, grade repetition may provide a valuable chance of

adjusting.

We derive the following conclusions in a higher education setting: Retention increases

dropout probabilities significantly. Students who have to repeat a full year are 10 percent-

age points more likely to drop out after the first year than students who are immediately

promoted. We find large and persistent effects on subsequent academic performance due

to the repetition of first-year courses. By the time of on-time graduation, repeaters outper-

form non-repeaters by about 0.5 standard deviations in grade point averages. The result

is robust across specifications. In contrast to grades, credits obtained per semester as

well as major choice after the first year remain unaffected.

2 Institutional setup

The University of St. Gallen offers three-year undergraduate degree courses in Business

Administration, Economics, International Affairs, and Legal Studies. It plays an important

role for the education of Managers and Economists in Switzerland: In the past years,

around 30% of all Swiss graduates in Economics and Business Administration received

their degree from St. Gallen (Table A.1). According to federal law, the university must

4

Page 6: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

admit all students with either a Swiss high school degree (“matura”) or a Swiss nationality.5

Following the general trend, the number of students has increased strongly over the last

two decades: Between 1990 and 2013, the number of first-year undergraduates more

than doubled (from 582 students to 1328 students) (Table A.2).

To maintain control of the number of enrolled students, the university introduced a

probation period, or “assessment year”, in 2001: Students have to fulfil a strict perfor-

mance requirement in order to continue their studies after the first year. This requirement

is based on course performance across the bundle of all compulsory first-year classes

(see details below). Students who either do not meet the performance requirement or do

not complete all compulsory courses are allowed to repeat the first year once. Over the

years 2001-2008, approximately 70% of all entering undergraduate students passed the

first year at their first attempt, and approximately 80% passed in total (first and second

attempts combined, see Table A.5).

Students in the same specialisation, or track, of the first year take the same set of com-

pulsory courses, and have identical exam schedules (Table A.3). In the following, we focus

on the Business/Economics track, which accounts for about 90% of all students (see Ta-

ble A.4).6 The core curriculum of this track consists of courses in Business Administration,

Economics, Legal Studies, and Mathematics, which are tested during central examination

periods of the first and second semesters. Furthermore, students submit a term paper in

one of the core subjects, which they hand in before they register for the second central

examination period. In addition to the core subjects, students take courses in leadership

skills and critical thinking. Moreover, they have to prove sufficient skills in one foreign

language.

Students accumulate “minus credits” for every course they fail. The total amount of

minus credits determines whether they pass or fail the first year. The grading scheme5Foreign students are admitted based on an entrance test. The admission quota for foreign students

varies by year, but is usually around 25%.6There are two subgroups of students for which the first year differs. First, students who intend to spe-

cialise in legal studies follow a different curriculum during the first year. Second, students of non-Germanmother tongue can chose to complete the first-year courses within two years instead of one (“extendedtrack”). Because of their special status, both groups are excluded from all analyses in this paper (seeTable A.4).

5

Page 7: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

and the computation of minus credits work as follows: Each course is graded. Grades

range from 1 to 6 in steps of 0.25, with 1 being the lowest grade, 4 being the minimum

passing grade, and 6 being the highest grade.7 Credit points are granted for each course,

no matter if the student fails or passes the course.8 Minus credits for each failed course

are defined as the difference between the obtained grade and 4, multiplied by the number

of credits for this course. If, for example, a student receives a grade of 3.5 in a course

worth 4 credits, he obtains 2 minus credits for this course. The total sum of minus credits

(MC) is then calculated as

MC =S∑

s=1

(4−Gs) ∗ Cs ∗ 1(Gs < 4), (1)

where S is the number of compulsory subjects in the first year, Gs is the grade obtained

in course s, and Cs is the number of credit points associated with this course, 1(.) is the

indicator function.

Students can proceed to the second year if they complete all compulsory courses and

accumulate at most 12 minus credits during the first year; students who do not fulfil these

criteria can repeat the entire first year once (grade retention treatment). Enforcement of

this rule is strict, and students cannot compensate minus credits by grades greater than

4 in other subjects. If a student repeats and successfully passes the second attempt, he

also proceeds to the second year (see Figure A.1). In case of repeated failure, the student

is coercively ex-matriculated and cannot repeat again.

After the first year, students have to choose one out of four majors (“Business”, “Eco-

nomics”, “International Affairs”, or “Law and Economics”). In contrast to the curriculum

of the first year, the curriculum of the following years is major-specific. We therefore de-

note the phase of undergraduate studies which starts after the first year as “major-specific

level.” At the major-specific level, students can decide how many courses and credit points

they complete per semester. Students graduate on time if they complete the major-specific7Unauthorised absence from an exam results in the lowest possible grade, i.e. a grade equal to 1.8The curriculum of the first year consists of 60 credit points in total (55 in 2001).

6

Page 8: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

level within four semesters. Only a minority of students (34%) completes their undergrad-

uate studies on time. The majority of students (84%) completes the major-specific level

within six semesters (Table 2).

The grade point average (GPA) that a student obtains at the end of his Bachelor’s

Degree is a weighted average of all exam grades obtained at the major-specific level

(weighted by the number of credits). Grades that a student obtains during the first year

neither count toward the final GPA, nor appear on the final grade sheet. GPA is one

of our main outcomes. It does not only reflect performance at the major-specific level–

not intermingled with first-year GPA–, but also serves as the main signal of students to

employers.

3 Data

3.1 Data sources, variables, and samples

We use anonymised administrative student records from the University of St. Gallen, that

is, enrolment, course performance, and graduation information for all students who start

their undergraduate degree between 2001 and 2008. In addition to individual background

characteristics, the data contain information on dropout decisions, major choice, and per-

formance (i.e., the number of credits and grades) for all completed courses up to the fall

semester 2013. Thus, we can track all students for at least 8 semesters, that is, we ob-

serve both repeaters and non-repeaters up to the point of on-time graduation. Because of

censoring in 2013, we exclude students who enter after 2008.

We further restrict the sample in two ways. First, we include only first-year students

with a German mother tongue who start the Business/Economics track. These students

have to complete identical courses. Different course requirements or exam procedures

apply to students with non-German mother tongue or to students in other tracks (in total

about 15%, see Table A.4). Second, we include only students who completed all first-year

courses. This criterion leads to a further 20% reduction in the sample size. We provide

7

Page 9: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

details on this sample selection criterion and the characteristics of excluded students in

Section 3.2.

We construct all outcome measures from the course and enrolment data. In partic-

ular, we investigate the following outcomes: whether a student drops out after the first

year; whether a student enrols into the major-specific level; major choice after the first

year; the number of credits accumulated by the end of each major-specific semester; and

grade point averages accumulated by the end of each semester at the major-specific level.

Thus, we cover all important measures to track a students’ choices and performance. We

standardise grades at the level of the entry year into the major-specific level to account

for changes in grade distributions over time. To assess the effect of repetition, we com-

pare repeaters and non-repeaters always at the same stage of their studies. For example,

to investigate differences in grades at the major-specific level, we compare repeaters and

non-repeaters who are both in their first major-specific semester. Thus, at the point of time

of comparison, repeaters have completed already one additional year at the university, and

are on average one year older. This is the most frequently used method in grade reten-

tion studies (same-grade-comparison), especially in the absence of standardised testing

procedures.

To determine treatment status, we apply Equation 1 to the course data. The perfor-

mance cutoff is sharp, that is, students are retained if and only if they fail. We verify the

sharpness in our data (not shown, as the graph would be trivial). We define a student as

a repeater if he enrols again in the first year for at least one semester after being retained.

The data contain the following background characteristics: age at entry; gender; na-

tionality; mother tongue; country of origin as well as region of origin for students from

Switzerland; and country, region and date of a student’s high school graduation. From

these variables, we can derive whether the student had to take an entrance test to be ad-

mitted to the university.9 We use the background characteristics to assess the identifying

assumptions of the regression discontinuity design.9These are students with neither a Swiss high school degree nor a Swiss nationality. See Section 2 for

details.

8

Page 10: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

3.2 What happens during the first year?

Our main estimation sample contains all freshmen entering, between 2001-2008, who

complete all compulsory first-year exams. These students are likely to differ from their

peers who do not complete all courses in their performance as well as background char-

acteristics. Thus, they present a selected sample. To understand the characteristics of

our selected sample versus the sample of excluded students, we define and describe the

different types of excluded and included students based on their performance during the

first year.

Table 1: Description of student types

Types Dropout decision Exams taken Minus credits Obs. % of1st sem. 2nd sem. 1st sem. sample

1 Drops out during 1st sem. Not all None - 392 7.32 Not admitted to 2nd sem. All None > 12 437 8.13 Drops out after 1st or during 2nd sem. All Not all ≤ 12 181 3.44 Completes full 1st year All All ≤ 12 4,382 81.3

Total 5,392 100

The table characterises different student types according to their behaviour and performance during thefirst year. Based on all first-year students with German mother tongue who enrolled into theBusiness/Economics track between 2001 and 2008 (5,392 students). Type 4 constitutes the estimationsample.

In particular, we exclude the following types of students: (i) students who have not

completed all compulsory first-semester courses (type 1, N=392); (ii) students who have

completed all compulsory first-semester courses, but have exceeded the threshold of 12

minus credits already during the first semester and are therefore not admitted to the sec-

ond semester (type 2, N=437); and (iii) students who have passed the first semester but

have dropped out voluntarily after the first or during the second semester (type 3, N=181).

Students who have completed all required first-year courses and who did not accumulate

more than 12 minus credits during the first semester form our estimation sample (type 4,

N=4,382). These students account for 81% of all students with a German mother tongue

who enter the Business/Economics track.

9

Page 11: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Figure 1: First-year grade distributions by student type

1 2 3 4 5 6Grade

Term paperLegal Studies 2

Business 2Economics 2

Mathematics 2Legal Studies 1

Business 1Economics 1

Mathematics 1

Drops out during 1st semesterType 1

1 2 3 4 5 6Grade

Term paperLegal Studies 2

Business 2Economics 2

Mathematics 2Legal Studies 1

Business 1Economics 1

Mathematics 1

Not admitted to 2nd semesterType 2

1 2 3 4 5 6Grade

Term paperLegal Studies 2

Business 2Economics 2

Mathematics 2Legal Studies 1

Business 1Economics 1

Mathematics 1

Drops out after 1st/during 2nd semesterType 3

1 2 3 4 5 6Grade

Term paperLegal Studies 2

Business 2Economics 2

Mathematics 2Legal Studies 1

Business 1Economics 1

Mathematics 1

Completes full first yearType 4

Note: The figure shows course performance distributions (absolute course grades, with 1 being the lowestand 6 being the highest grade) for different student types, based on first-semester core courses. The boxplots display the median values (thick black lines), 25th percentiles (p25, left margin of each box), 75thpercentiles (p75, right margin of each box) and lower/upper adjacent values (left margin of thewhiskers/right margin of the whiskers). The lower and upper adjacent values are defined asp25− 1.5 ∗ (p75− p25) and p75 + 1.5 ∗ (p75− p25), respectively. Based on the sample of all first-yearstudents with German mother tongue who enter the Business/Economics track (5,392 students). Detailedtype definitions are presented in Section 3.2.

10

Page 12: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Students who complete the full first year outperform students who drop out before

they complete all first-year courses. Figure 1 compares course performance distributions

across types, based on completed courses during the first year. Median comparisons re-

veal little differences between types 1 and 2, that is, between voluntary and involuntary

dropouts during the first semester. By contrast, median performance for type-3 students

lies above median performance for type-2 students in all four first-semester core courses.

In more than half of the first-year courses (five out of nine), however, the median perfor-

mance of type-3 students still remains below the passing grade of 4. Students who com-

plete all first-year courses (type-4 students) fare best: They outperform type-3-students in

terms of median performance in eight out of nine courses.

Table A.6 shows differences in background characteristics across student types. Stu-

dents who complete all first-year courses are more often male and on average slightly

younger than students who drop out prematurely. Moreover, non-Swiss students and stu-

dents who have taken an entrance test are over-represented among students who com-

plete the full first year. These are students with higher costs of starting their degree in

St. Gallen (e.g., they have to study for the entrance exam, they have to move to a for-

eign country, they have to pay higher student fees). Thus, the data are in line with the

hypothesis that students who face higher initial costs are initially more motivated or able

(c.f. Manski (1989)).

3.3 Descriptive statistics

Table 2 provides descriptive statistics for the estimation sample (type 4), and compares

those who passed and those who failed their first attempt. As the composition of the

student pool as well as grading schemes remain stable over the years (see Table A.7), we

focus on the pooled sample for all eight cohorts.

The large majority of students is male (73%), and the average age of students is just

above 20. Non-Swiss students account for only 24% of the sample, as separate admission

rules apply to non-Swiss students: The fraction of students with neither a Swiss high

11

Page 13: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Table 2: Estimation sample: Descriptive statistics (cohorts 2001-2008)

Full sample Pass Fail Difference (p-value) # Obs.

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)# Obs. 4,382 3,817 565Background characteristics

Age 20.26 20.22 20.50 -0.28 (0.000) 4,382Male 73% 74% 72% 2% (0.318) 4,382Non-Swiss nationality 24% 25% 16% 9% (0.000) 4,382Entrance test 20% 22% 8% 14% (0.000) 4,382High school St. Gallen 15% 14% 18% -4% (0.014) 4,382

Minus creditsFraction: MC > 0 57% 50% 100% -50% (0.000) 4,382# MC in first year 4.67 2.46 19.64 -17.19 (0.000) 4,382

Course performance 1st yearGrade Business 1 4.22 4.30 3.68 0.62 (0.000) 4,382Grade Economics 1 4.58 4.69 3.90 0.79 (0.000) 4,382Grade Mathematics 1 4.53 4.66 3.59 1.07 (0.000) 4,382Grade Legal Studies 1 4.47 4.58 3.74 0.84 (0.000) 4,382Grade Business 2 4.26 4.38 3.46 0.92 (0.000) 4,382Grade Economics 2 4.45 4.60 3.50 1.10 (0.000) 4,382Grade Mathematics 2 4.52 4.68 3.47 1.20 (0.000) 4,382Grade Legal Studies 2 4.33 4.47 3.41 1.05 (0.000) 4,382Grade Term paper 4.98 5.03 4.59 0.45 (0.000) 4,382

Outcomes: DropoutDropout after 1st year 2% 1% 14% -13% (0.000) 4,382Repeater 11% 0 86% -86% - 4,382

Outcomes: Major-specific levelMajor-specific level started 96% 99% 71% 28% (0.000) 4,382Major Business 69% 69% 73% -4% (0.084) 4,189Major Economics 16% 17% 9% 7% (0.000) 4,189Major International Affairs 15% 15% 13% 3% (0.166) 4,189Major Law and Economics 6% 6% 4% 3% (0.026) 4,189

Outcomes: Credits at the major-specific levelCredits by 1st major-specific semester 26.5 26.7 24.9 1.8 (0.000) 4,189Credits by 2nd major-specific semester 54.0 54.3 51.0 3.3 (0.000) 4,189Credits by 3rd major-specific semester 77.5 77.8 74.1 3.7 (0.000) 4,189Credits by 4th major-specific semester 103.2 103.7 98.0 5.7 (0.000) 4,189

Outcomes: Grade point averages (GPA)GPA (std) by 1st major-specific semester 0.08 0.15 -0.55 0.70 (0.000) 4,079GPA (std) by 2nd major-specific semester 0.10 0.18 -0.65 0.82 (0.000) 4,079GPA (std) by 3rd major-specific semester 0.11 0.19 -0.68 0.87 (0.000) 4,079GPA (std) by 4th major-specific semester 0.12 0.20 -0.67 0.87 (0.000) 4,079

Bachelor’s Degree completedWithin 4 major-specific semesters (on time)(1) 34% 36% 20% 15% (0.000) 3,522Within 5 major-specific semesters(1) 59% 61% 45% 16% (0.000) 3,522Within 6 major-specific semesters(1) 84% 86% 70% 16% (0.000) 3,522

The table shows descriptive statistics for the estimation sample, that is, for all students who havecompleted all first-year courses. Column (1) shows means for the full sample, and columns (2) and (3)show the means for the sub-samples of students who passed and who failed the first year. Column (4)displays the difference in mean characteristics, and column (5) shows the p-value of the t-test for the H0 ofno difference in mean characteristics. Variables at the major-specific level are missing for individuals whonever enrol into the second year. Variables for major choice do not sum to 100% due to individuals whocomplete double majors. Grade point averages are missing for individuals who do not complete any creditsduring the first major-specific semester in which they are enroled. Information on completion of theBachelor’s Degree is missing for the cohort of 2008 due to censoring.(1)Values not available for the cohort of 2008. 12

Page 14: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

school diploma nor Swiss citizenship is restricted to at most 25% of the student body.

Students who pass are different from students who fail, both in terms of background

characteristics and in terms of their performance at all levels (Table 2). Students who pass

are younger, less likely to come from St. Gallen, and more likely to come from outside

Switzerland than students who fail. They also perform significantly better in all exams,

which translates into a low dropout rate of only 1% after the first year. By contrast, 14%

of students who fail decide not to repeat the first year, that is, they drop out. The superior

performance of students who pass carries over to the major-specific level. Students who

pass the first year in their first attempt collect on average more credits and better grades

than students who do not pass the first attempt. This translates into earlier graduation

dates, counted from the beginning of the major-specific level, compared to students who

repeat and pass the first year in their second attempt.

The raw comparison between repeaters and non-repeaters would thus lead to the

biased conclusion that repetition has negative effects. Repeaters perform significantly

worse than non-repeaters at the major-specific level; but repeaters are low-performing

students in the first place. For instance, 50% of students who pass never obtain any

minus credits.

4 Empirical strategy

4.1 Identification

A comparison between the outcomes of all retained with all non-retained students would

lead to biased estimates of the effects of interest. The bias occurs as retained students are

lower performing students on average. To solve this identification problem, we implement

a regression discontinuity design, that is, we rely on a comparison of first-year students

who tightly pass with first-year students who tightly fail.

The identification of the treatment effect in a regression continuity design requires

the assumption of “local continuity” at the performance cutoff (c.f. Imbens and Lemieux

13

Page 15: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

(2008)). This assumption states that the distribution of observed and unobserved charac-

teristics in the student pool must change smoothly at the threshold. For example, the aver-

age ability level of students must not abruptly deteriorate at the threshold. The assumption

implies that students who tightly fail would achieve on average the same outcome as stu-

dents who tightly pass, if they were exposed to the same policy (e.g., if everyone was

directly promoted to the second year).

Local continuity is likely to hold in our setting as students have–if anything–only impre-

cise control over the number of minus credits they accumulate. Students may set their

effort level before or during the semester to target a specific number of minus credits.

But grades are disclosed exclusively at the end of each semester, and all course grades

are jointly disclosed on the same day by mail. Therefore, students cannot adjust their

effort level in one subject in response to an unexpectedly high number of minus credits in

another subject.

Thus, whether a student accumulates, for example, 13 instead of 12 minus credits may

result from bad luck on just one of the core exams (e.g., equivalent to failing a four-credit

exam by the minimal grade margin, which is 0.25). This may be unrelated to how well

the student prepared for the whole exam period. If this holds for all students, the pool of

students with 12 minus credits resembles the pool of students with 13 minus credits in

terms of their behaviour and characteristics (e.g., learning behaviour, motivation, gender).

We can continue this thought experiment for other values in the neighbourhood around

the threshold (for example, 12 versus 12.25, 12 versus 14, or 11 versus 13 credits). As

we move farther away from the threshold, we expect the groups on opposite sides of the

threshold to become less comparable.

We test for potential violations of the local continuity assumption in various ways. First,

the assumption is violated if some students who are in danger of failing study just enough

to ensure that they do not have to repeat. Then, we would observe significantly fewer

students who just fail than students who just pass. Students, however, cannot adjust their

effort in such a precise way, as the university reveals grades and final credits only at the

14

Page 16: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

end of each central exam period. Consequently, Figure 2 shows no visual indication of

sorting around the threshold. Likewise, the McCrary test (McCrary, 2008) does not reject

the null-hypothesis of continuity of minus credits at the cutoff (log difference in height =

0.21, standard error = 0.14).

Figure 2: Histogram of the assignment variable

010

020

030

040

050

0F

requ

ency

−10 0 10 20Number of minus credits (recentred)

Note: The figure shows a histogram of minus credits for all students in the estimation sample who accu-mulated at least one minus credit during the first year (2,486 students). The assignment variable (minuscredits) is the re-centred sum of minus credits accumulated during the first year (i.e. MC - 12.25, where MCis computed according to equation 1).

Second, the assumption is violated if students can manipulate grades of single exams.

For example, students may try to influence their graders to adjust pivotal grades. Students,

however, have no control over the grading process, and cannot otherwise manipulate

single grades. Thus, average grades in all core courses change smoothly at the cutoff

(Figure 3).

Third, we investigate the local continuity of background characteristics around the

threshold. We compare the means of student characteristics (age, gender, nationality,

whether students completed an entrance test, whether students completed their high

school in the canton of St. Gallen) in a neighbourhood of 0.5 and 1 minus credits to

both sides of the cutoff. In addition, we fit parametric linear and higher polynomial models,

as well as nonparametric models to both sides of the cutoff to detect non-continuities (see

Section 4.2 for details). Table 3 presents the coefficient estimates and standard errors

from these estimations. For gender, nationality, and entrance-test participation, we find no

15

Page 17: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Figure 3: Smoothness of first-year course grades around the threshold (first attempt)

34

5G

rade

Bus

ines

s 1

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .06 (.04)

(1) Business 1

34

5G

rade

Eco

nom

ics

1

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .07 (.06)

(2) Economics 1

34

5G

rade

Mat

h 1

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): −.04 (.07)

(3) Mathematics 1

34

5G

rade

Mat

h 1

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .05 (.05)

(4) Legal Studies 1

34

5G

rade

Bus

ines

s 2

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): −.11 (.05)

(5) Business 2

34

5G

rade

Eco

nom

ics

2−10 −5 0 5 10

Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): −.03 (.06)

(6) Economics 2

34

5G

rade

Mat

h 2

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .1 (.07)

(7) Mathematics 2

34

5G

rade

Mat

h 2

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .05 (.05)

(8) Legal Studies 2

34

5G

rade

r T

erm

pap

er

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .05 (.06)

(9) Term paper

Note: The figure illustrates the smoothness of first-year course grades. The black dots indicate means ofcourse grades in bins of a width of 1 minus credit. Grades range from 1 to 6 with 1 being the lowest grade,6 being the highest grade, and 4 being the passing grade. The solid lines present a linear fit to both sidesof the threshold. The number of minus credits is re-centred around the cutoff of 12.25 minus credits. Thedashed lines indicate 95% confidence intervals. The discontinuity coefficient indicates the distance of thetwo regression lines exactly at the cutoff. Standard errors are in parentheses. The sample consists of allindividuals in the estimation sample within a range of 10 minus credits to either side of the cutoff (n = 1967).

16

Page 18: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

evidence of sorting around the threshold. For age and local high school degree, we find

at most weak evidence of sorting. As none of the differences at the threshold are robust

across specifications, we conclude that sorting patterns around the threshold are due to

chance variations. Figure A.2 visualises the non-systematic patterns on both sides.

Table 3: RDD estimates: Student background characteristics

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)Male -0,04 -0.01 -0.09 -0.06 0.04 -0.03 -0.08 -0.01 -0.08

(0.12) (0.08) (0.18) (0.07) (0.11) (0.16) (0.11) (0.09) (0.19)Age -0.43 -0.10 -1.00* -0.37 -0.60 -0.33 -0.70* -0.75* -0.55

(0.38) (0.22) (0.51) (0.31) (0.49) (0.69) (0.40) (0.34) (0.22)Non-Swiss nationality -0.09 -0.04 -0.08 -0.02 0.00 -0.11 0.01 0.01 -0.07

(0.09) (0.06) (0.13) (0.05) (0.06) (0.14) (0.06) (0.06) (0.05)Entrance Test -0.12 -0.07 -0.10 -0.02 -0.04 -0.17 0.01 -0.02 -0.09

(0.08) (0.05) (0.11) (0.03) (0.05) (0.14) (0.05) (0.05) (0.06)High school St. Gallen -0.17 -0.10 -0.26 -0.04 -0.18* -0.26 -0.15 -0.08 -0.14

(0.11) (0.07) (0.16) (0.06) (0.11) (0.18) (0.10) (0.08) (0.15)

Observations 65 140 140 790 790 790 1967 2378 2378Estimation [-0.5;0.5] [-1;1] [-1;1] [-5;5] [-5;5] [-5;5] [-10;10] [-12;12] [-12;12]windowPolynomial 0 0 1 1 2 3 3 3 NPorder

Note: RDD estimates (average marginal effects for all outcomes except age) using student backgroundcharacteristics as outcome variables. The different columns use different estimation windows and varyingpolynomial orders of the underlying running variable (minus credits). The respective estimation window foreach specification is reported as the minus credit range on each side of the threshold. All parametricestimates are based on logistic regression models for binary outcomes (male, non-Swiss nationality,entrance test, high school St. Gallen) and OLS models for continuous outcomes (age). Following Imbensand Kalyanaraman (2012), the bandwidth for the local-linear nonparametric specification (NP) isdetermined by cross-validation. The standard errors for the NP estimates are computed using 5000bootstrap replications. In all regressions, we control for a student’s cohort.* Significant at 10%-level, ** Significant at 5%- level, *** Significant at 1%-level. Standard errors are inparentheses.

Missing outcomes further complicate the identification of the effects of repeating. In

particular, outcomes at the major-specific level (credits, grades per semester) are miss-

ing for students who drop out after being retained (i.e., they do not repeat, they fail the

second attempt, or they drop out before or during the first major-specific semester). If

dropout probabilities are correlated with observed or unobserved student characteristics,

the estimated effect of repetition on educational achievement may be biased. For exam-

ple, students who are highly motivated may be less likely to drop out, even if they have to

repeat. Thus, the pool of repeaters may contain more highly motivated students than the

17

Page 19: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

pool of non-repeaters. As a result, effect estimates of repeating may reflect differences in

the level of motivation between the two groups rather than the true effect of repeating.

In the presence of these potential biases, we cannot identify point estimates of the ef-

fects of repetition without additional assumptions; yet, we can compute interval estimates,

or “bounds”, for the effect. We follow Lee (2009), who presents identification results for

bounds under the assumption of “weak monotonicity”. Weak monotonicity implies that

grade retention affects dropout probabilities only in one direction. In other words, no stu-

dent drops out if and only if he is not retained. Data as well as economic reasoning

support this assumption. First, only 1% of non-retained students drop out, whereas 14%

of retained students drop out (Table 2). Second, as repetition is costly, both in terms of

effort and in terms of opportunity costs, no student should continue only because he is

retained. The bounds presented by Lee (2009) are sharp, that is, one cannot tighten the

bounds without additional assumptions. The bounds, however, are not valid for the whole

student population, but only for a sub-population. This sub-population contains only stu-

dents who would continue to study under any circumstances, that is, regardless of their

retention status (“never drop-outs”).

We provide descriptive evidence to support the importance of accounting for miss-

ing outcomes. To test whether dropout probabilities among retained students depend on

observable characteristics, we regress an indicator for non-missing GPA at the major-

specific level on first-year course performances as well as on student characteristics, and

control for minus credits during the first year (Table A.8). Retained students with better

performance in the first-year, second-semester courses “Business 2”, “Economics 2”, and

“Legal Studies 2” are significantly less likely to drop out than retained students with lower

performance in these subjects. Moreover, retained students who are older are less likely

to drop out than retained students who are younger. By contrast, we do not find equivalent

patterns among non-retained students, partly as dropout probabilities among non-retained

students are close to zero (Table A.9). These findings emphasise the importance of ac-

counting for potential biases which may arise from missing outcomes.

18

Page 20: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

4.2 Estimation

For continuous outcomes (e.g., GPA), we estimate models of the following type:

Y =K∑k=0

αk ∗MCk +K∑k=0

βk ∗MCk ∗ 1(MC ≥ 0) +X ′γ + ε, (2)

where Y represents the educational outcome , MC corresponds to the re-centred number

of minus credits collected at the end of the first year (re-centred around the cutoff of 12 mi-

nus credits), k is the polynomial order (between 0 and 3 in our application), X is a vector of

control variables (e.g., cohort dummies), and ε is an error term. β0 denotes the estimated

causal effect of grade retention and repetition, respectively. We use varying windows of

data around the threshold. Using higher order polynomials and interaction terms, we allow

for a non-linear relationship as well as different slopes on both sides of the cut-off and bet-

ter fits for larger estimation windows (Imbens and Lemieux, 2008). For binary outcomes

(e.g., whether a student drops out), we use logistic regression models, but the results are

robust to alternative specifications (linear probability models, probit models; results not

shown). In all parametric specifications we control for a student’s cohort to account for

time-varying conditions such as course contents and grading schemes.

We also provide non-parametric point estimates (Imbens and Lemieux, 2008), which

rely on local linear regressions to either side of the threshold. The optimal bandwidth is

derived by cross-validation, and standard errors are computed using the bootstrap method

as suggested by Imbens and Kalyanaraman (2012).10

The non-parametric estimation of bounds for the population of never drop-outs, that is,

students that continue regardless of their retention status, follows Lee (2009). We focus

on mean comparisons within small windows around the cutoff, that is, windows of 0.5, 1,

and 1.5 minus credits to either side of the cutoff.

The estimation procedure for the bounds is based on a trimming method. We observe

that more retained students than non-retained students drop out. The difference p in

dropout probabilities can be estimated from the data and signifies the fraction of students10We implement the analysis using the -rd- command in STATA (Nichols, 2011).

19

Page 21: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

who drop out if and only if they are retained (“compliers”, c.f. Lee (2009)) among the

joint population of compliers and never drop-outs. To compute the first bound, we trim the

upper p percent of the outcome distribution of non-retained students. We then compute the

first bound as the difference between the trimmed mean outcome for the sample of non-

retained students and the (untrimmed) mean outcome for the sample of retained students.

To compute the second bound, we proceed as before, but now start by trimming the lower

p percent of the outcome distribution of non-retained students. Whether the first bound

is the upper or lower bound depends on the direction of the treatment effect. Because

of small sample sizes, we do not include further control variables. We report analytic

standard errors based on Lee (2009).11

5 Results

5.1 The effect of grade retention on dropout probabilities

Retained students are potentially more likely to drop out for two reasons. First, students

who failed the first year and choose to repeat may fail for a second time and thus never en-

ter the major-specific level. Second, conditional on passing their second attempt, retained

students will enter the labour market one year later. As a result, the utility from continuing

to study may be lower for retained students, which may lead to higher dropout rates (c.f.

Manski (1989)). Stigmatisation by fellow students or instructors, and the costs related to

re-adjustments to new peers are likely to influence the drop-out decision, too.

Dropout probabilities immediately after the first year are indeed higher among retained

students than among non-retained students. Figure 4 shows a jump in the dropout proba-

bility directly at the cutoff, and a further increase in dropout probabilities with the number

of minus credits. The dropout probability lies below 2% among non-retained students, but

increases to more than 10% at the cutoff. The regression discontinuity estimates confirm

this pattern (Table 4). Because of the further increase after the cutoff, our preferred spec-11We implement the analysis using the -leebounds- command in STATA (Tauchmann, 2012).

20

Page 22: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Figure 4: RDD estimates: Dropout probability after the first year and probability of entering themajor-specific level

0.2

.4.6

.81

Pro

babi

lity

of d

ropo

ut a

fter

the

first

yea

r

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .083 (.027)

(1) Dropout probability after the first year

0.2

.4.6

.81

Pro

babi

lity

of e

nter

ing

the

maj

or−

spec

ific

leve

l

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): −.194 (.034)

(2) Probability of entering the major−specific level

Note: The panels above provide a graphical illustration of the effects of retention on dropout probabilitiesafter the first year and probabilities of entering the major-specific level. The dots represent proportions,computed within bins of 2 minus credits. The black lines display quadratic fits to either side of the cutoff,together with 95% confidence intervals (dashed lines). The discontinuity coefficient indicates the distanceof the two regression lines exactly at the cutoff. Standard errors are in parentheses. The sample consistsof all individuals in the estimation sample within a range of 10 minus credits to either side of the cutoff (n =1967).

ifications are either small windows of at most one minus credit to both sides around the

cutoff (columns 1 and 2), wider windows with at least a quadratic term (columns 7 and

8), or even non-parametric estimates (column 9). The estimates from these specifications

vary between three and ten percentage points, and are not always significant, due to the

large variance in dropout probabilities for retained students.

The negative effect of retention on the probability of entering the major-specific level

is larger than the effect on dropout probabilities immediately after the first year: It ranges

from -9 to -20 percentage points in our preferred specifications and is mostly statistically

significant (Table 4). At least three reasons account for the increase in dropout rates

between the end of the first year and the beginning of the major-specific level. First,

some of the retained students may enrol for their second attempt while looking for outside

opportunities. Second, retained students may update the costs of repeating only after

having started their second attempt. Third, students may fail the first year for a second

21

Page 23: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Table 4: RDD estimates: Dropout probabilities (average marginal effects)

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)Dropout probability 0.03 0.10** -0.04 0.10** 0.00 -0.03 0.07 0.06 0.04after the first year (0.06) (0.04) (0.10) (0.04) (0.12) (0.19) (0.07) (0.08) (0.08)Probability of entering -0.09 -0.20*** -0.05 -0.19*** -0.10 -0.05 -0.16* -0.16* -0.15the major-specific level (0.07) (0.05) (0.13) (0.05) (0.13) (0.20) (0.09) (0.09) (0.14)

Observations 65 140 140 790 790 790 1967 2378 2378Estimation [-0.5;0.5] [-1;1] [-1;1] [-5;5] [-5;5] [-5;5] [-10;10] [-12;12] [-12;12]windowPolynomial 0 0 1 1 2 3 3 3 NPorder

Note: The table shows RDD estimates (average marginal effects) for the effect of grade retention onstudent dropout probabilities. Students with zero minus credits are excluded from the estimation sample.All parametric estimates are based on logit models and control for cohort effects. Following Imbens andKalyanaraman (2012), the bandwidth for the local-linear non-parametric specification (NP) is determined bycross-validation. Standard errors for NP estimates are computed using 5000 bootstrap replications.* Significant at 10%-level, ** Significant at 5%-level, *** Significant at 1%-level. Standard errors are inparentheses.

time and thus be forced to drop out. The chance of passing the second attempt, however,

is high: approximately 80% of all students who complete the second attempt pass the first

year (Table A.5).

5.2 The effect of repeating on major choice

Students decide upon their major at the end of the first year. Repetition of the first year may

thus induce students to update their preferences for certain subjects and majors. Major

choice is a relevant outcome for two reasons. First, major choice determines which skills

a student acquires, and thus influences future earnings (c.f. Altonji et al. (2012), Arcidia-

cono (2004)). Second, students’ performance (grades, credits) might differ across majors.

Thus, if we detect differences in major choice between repeaters and non-repeaters, we

should account for these differences in the further analysis.

Overall, repetition does not affect major choice at the threshold. Table 5 investigates

major choice for students who enter the major-specific level. Repeaters are less likely to

choose “Business”, “Economics”, and “Law and Economics” as majors, and are slightly

more likely to choose International Affairs. The coefficients do not sum to one as some

22

Page 24: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Table 5: RDD estimates: Major choice probabilities (average marginal effects)

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)Major: Business -0.04 -0.06 -0.11 -0.07 -0.04 -0.02 -0.12 -0.10 -0.09

(0.12) (0.08) (0.19) (0.07) (0.12) (0.17) (0.11) (0.10) (0.20)Major: Economics -0.13 -0.02 -0.16 0.01 -0.02 -0.22 0.04 0.03 -0.08

(0.09) (0.05) (0.11) (0.04) (0.07) (0.17) (0.06) (0.05) (0.16)Major: International 0.10 0.08 0.12 0.05 0.10 0.09 0.07 0.07 0.14***Affairs (0.07) (0.06) (0.15) (0.05) (0.09) (0.10) (0.07) (0.07) (0.02)Major: Law and -0.06 -0.05 0.00 -0.06 -0.05 -0.04 -0.06 -0.05 -0.06Economics (0.06) (0.04) (0.10) (0.04) (0.06) (0.06) (0.06) (0.06) (0.08)

Observations 58 122 122 737 737 737 1857 2256 2256Estimation [-0.5;0.5] [-1;1] [-1;1] [-5;5] [-5;5] [-5;5] [-10;10] [-12;12] [-12;12]windowPolynomial 0 0 1 1 2 3 3 3 NPorder

Note: The table shows RDD estimates (average marginal effects) for the effect of retention on major choiceprobabilities. Students with zero minus credits are excluded from the estimation sample. The coefficientsdo not sum to one as some students choose double majors. All parametric estimates are based on logitmodels and control for cohort effects. Following Imbens and Kalyanaraman (2012), the bandwidth for thelocal-linear nonparametric specification (NP) is determined by cross-validation. Standard errors for NPestimates are computed using 5000 bootstrap replications.* Significant at 10%-level, ** Significant at 5%-level, *** Significant at 1%-level. Standard errors are inparentheses.

students take double majors. Yet, none of the regression coefficients is significant, with the

exception of the non-parametric specification for the major in International Affairs (column

9). Thus, performance effects at the major-specific level are unlikely to be biased because

of major choices.

5.3 The effect of repeating on study pace and grade point averages

Repeating may affect both study pace and grades throughout the remaining studies. At the

major-specific level students can choose the number of subjects they take per semester.

Repeaters may attend more courses per semester to make up for the time lost because

of retention. Students, however, also face a trade-off between the number of courses per

semester and the effort that they spend on each course.

Repetition barely affects study speed. In general, the number of credits per semester

at the major-specific level is uncorrelated with the performance of students during their

first year. As Figure 5 shows, the number of credits is almost constant at around 25 cred-

23

Page 25: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Table 6: RDD estimates: Credit points by the end of each major-specific semester

Credits by major- 1.58 3.03** 3.70 2.42* 3.03 5.22* 2.96 2.00 2.36**specific semester 1 (2.07) (1.43) (3.37) (1.33) (2.08) (2.89) (1.81) (1.61) (1.30)Credits by major- 0.85 4.88* 2.76 5.20** 3.50 6.01 4.58 4.39 4.47specific semester 2 (3.77) (2.63) (6.19) (2.28) (3.57) (4.95) (3.09) (2.75) (6.50)Credits by major- -1.66 2.56 -0.87 3.87 0.61 2.45 2.07 1.54 2.16***specific semester 3 (5.06) (3.51) (8.25) (2.85) (4.46) (6.19) (3.84) (3.42) (0.52)Credits by major- -1.52 1.25 -3.24 1.21 -1.10 0.17 -0.67 -1.17 0.05specific semester 4 (6.48) (4.51) (10.59) (3.62) (5.66) (7.85) (4.87) (4.32) (7.57)

Observations 58 122 122 737 737 737 1,857 2,256 2,256Estimation [-0.5;0.5] [-1;1] [-1;1] [-5;5] [-5;5] [-5;5] [-10;10] [-12;12] [-12;12]windowPolynomial 0 0 1 1 2 3 3 3 NPorder

Note: The table shows RDD estimates for the effect of retention on credits per semester. Students withzero minus credits are excluded from the estimation sample. All parametric estimates are based on linearmodels and control for cohort effects. Following Imbens and Kalyanaraman (2012) the bandwidth for thelocal-linear nonparametric specification (NP) is determined by cross-validation. Standard errors for NPestimates are computed using 5000 bootstrap replications.* Significant at 10%-level, ** Significant at 5%-level, *** Significant at 1%-level. Standard errors are inparentheses.

its per semester on average, regardless of the number of minus credits, and regardless of

the repeater status. Retained students close to the cutoff accumulate slightly more credit

points per semester. Table 6 confirms this pattern. The effect of course repetition on cred-

its per semester is significant only in the first two semesters. Moreover, the effect amounts

to at most 5 credit points by the end of the second major-specific semester. As the en-

tire major-specific curriculum consists of 120 credit points, the effects on study speed are

negligible. Furthermore, the differences in credits per semester between repeaters and

non-repeaters fade out by the fourth major-specific semester.

By contrast, course repetition has strong and significant effects on grade point aver-

ages at the major-specific level. Overall, repeaters perform consistently about 0.5 stan-

dard deviations better than non-repeaters (Table 7). The effect is robust across speci-

fications and ranges from between 0.42 standard deviations to 0.7 standard deviations.

Moreover, the gains persist throughout all semesters. Figure 6 provides visual evidence.

The performance of students at the major-specific level decreases overall in the number of

minus credits during the first year. Repetition leads to an upward jump in grade point aver-

24

Page 26: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Figure 5: RDD estimates: Credits accumulated by the end of each major-specific semester

2040

6080

100

120

Cre

dits

by

maj

or−

spec

ific

sem

este

r 1

−5 0 5Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): 2.2 (1.37)

(1) Credits by major−specific semester 1

2040

6080

100

120

Cre

dits

by

maj

or−

spec

ific

sem

este

r 2

−5 0 5Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): 4.74 (2.34)

(2) Credits by major−specific semester 2

2040

6080

100

120

Cre

dits

by

maj

or−

spec

ific

sem

este

r 3

−5 0 5Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): 3.34 (2.89)

(3) Credits by major−specific semester 3

2040

6080

100

120

Cre

dits

by

maj

or−

spec

ific

sem

este

r 4

−5 0 5Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .68 (3.6)

(4) Credits by major−specific semester 4

Note: The panels above provide a graphical illustration of the effects of retention on the number of creditsaccumulated for the Bachelor’s Degree by the end of each of the first four major-specific semesters. Thedots represent the mean outcomes in bins of 1 minus credits. The solid lines display a linear fit to eitherside of the cutoff (dashed lines: 95% confidence intervals). The discontinuity coefficient indicates thedistance of the two regression lines exactly at the cutoff. Standard errors are in parentheses. The sampleconsists of all individuals in the estimation sample within a range of 5 minus credits to either side of thecutoff who have non-missing information on credits (n = 737).

25

Page 27: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Figure 6: RDD estimates: Grade point averages (GPA) by the end of each major-specific semester(standardised)

−1

−.8

−.6

−.4

−.2

0G

PA

(st

d) b

y m

ajor

−sp

ecifi

c se

mes

ter

1

−5 0 5Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .53 (.14)

(1) GPA (std) by major−specific semester 1

−1

−.8

−.6

−.4

−.2

0G

PA

(st

d) b

y m

ajor

−sp

ecifi

c se

mes

ter

2−5 0 5

Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .52 (.14)

(2) GPA (std) by major−specific semester 2

−1

−.8

−.6

−.4

−.2

0G

PA

(st

d) b

y m

ajor

−sp

ecifi

c se

mes

ter

3

−5 0 5Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .46 (.12)

(3) GPA (std) by major−specific semester 3

−1

−.8

−.6

−.4

−.2

0G

PA

(st

d) b

y m

ajor

−sp

ecifi

c se

mes

ter

4

−5 0 5Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .48 (.12)

(4) GPA (std) by major−specific semester 4

Note: The panels above provide a graphical illustration of the effects of retention on grade point averagesby the end of each of the first four major-specific semesters. The dots represent the mean outcomes inbins of 1 minus credits. The solid lines display a linear fit to either side of the cutoff (dashed lines: 95%confidence intervals). The discontinuity coefficient indicates the distance of the two regression lines exactlyat the cutoff. Standard errors are in parentheses. The sample consists of all individuals in the estimationsample within a range of 5 minus credits to either side of the cutoff who have non-missing information ongrades (n = 700).

26

Page 28: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Table 7: RDD estimates: Grade point averages (GPA) by the end of each major-specific semester

GPA (std) by major- 0.44** 0.38*** 0.61* 0.55*** 0.54** 0.57* 0.61*** 0.70*** 0.55***specific semester 1 (0.22) (0.13) (0.31) (0.15) (0.23) (0.32) (0.20) (0.18) (0.06)GPA (std) by major- 0.61*** 0.42*** 0.61* 0.55*** 0.48** 0.59* 0.58*** 0.64*** 0.51***specific semester 2 (0.20) (0.13) (0.31) (0.14) (0.22) (0.30) (0.18) (0.16) (0.19)GPA (std) by major- 0.61*** 0.43*** 0.58** 0.49*** 0.50*** 0.62** 0.56*** 0.63*** 0.53***specific semester 3 (0.18) (0.11) (0.27) (0.12) (0.19) (0.26) (0.17) (0.15) (0.13)GPA (std) by major- 0.55*** 0.46*** 0.43 0.53*** 0.51*** 0.53** 0.60*** 0.63*** 0.49specific semester 4 (0.19) (0.11) (0.27) (0.12) (0.18) (0.25) (0.17) (0.15) (0.46)

Observations 56 118 118 700 700 700 1,788 2,177 2,177Estimation [-0.5;0.5] [-1;1] [-1;1] [-5;5] [-5;5] [-5;5] [-10;10] [-12;12] [-12;12]windowPolynomial 0 0 1 1 2 3 3 3 NPorder

Note: The table shows RDD estimates for the effect of retention on grade point averages by the end ofeach major-specific semester. All parametric estimates are based on linear models and control for cohorteffects. Following Imbens and Kalyanaraman (2012), the bandwidth for the local-linear nonparametricspecification (NP) is determined by cross-validation. Standard errors for NP estimates are computed using5000 bootstrap replications.* Significant at 10%-level, ** Significant at 5%-level, *** Significant at 1%-level. Standard errors are inparentheses.

ages at the cutoff. The performance of retained students, however, remains relatively low

compared to the performance of all students. At the cutoff, the performance of repeaters

lies still more than 0.4 standard deviations below the median by their fourth major-specific

semester.

5.4 Accounting for missing outcomes

The analysis of bounds on the effect of repetition confirms the positive effects of grade re-

tention on grade point averages, at least for the third and fourth major-specific semesters.

We investigate estimation windows of 0.5, 1, and 1.5 minus credits to each side of the

cutoff (Table 8). For the first two major-specific semesters, the bounds are only signifi-

cantly positive for the smallest estimation window. For the third and fourth major-specific

semester, the bounds are significant for all windows. For the smallest window, the effect

ranges between 0.42 and 0.69 standard deviations by the fourth major-specific semester.

The effect of repetition on academic achievement is thus substantial, even if we only take

27

Page 29: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

the lower bound into account.

Table 8: Bounds on the effects of repetition on grade point averages

Outcome Estimation Mean Bounds 95% CI # obs. Trimmingwindow comparison of bounds proportion

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)GPA (std) by major- [-0.5;0.5] 0.54 [0.42; 0.66]** [0.02; 1.07] 65 0.10specific semester 1 [-1;1] 0.39 [0.15; 0.62] [-0.08; 0.86] 140 0.19

[-1.5;1.5] 0.38 [0.18; 0.54] [-0.02; 0.74] 216 0.14

GPA (std) by major- [-0.5;0.5] 0.59 [0.48; 0.70]** [0.13; 1.04] 65 0.10specific semester 2 [-1;1] 0.40 [0.14; 0.60] [-0.09; 0.83] 140 0.19

[-1.5;1.5] 0.35 [0.15; 0.50] [-0.03; 0.69] 216 0.14

GPA (std) by major- [-0.5;0.5] 0.61 [0.52; 0.75]** [0.22; 1.04] 65 0.10specific semester 3 [-1;1] 0.43 [0.23; 0.61]** [0.03; 0.81] 140 0.19

[-1.5;1.5] 0.32 [0.16; 0.44]** [0.00; 0.61] 216 0.14

GPA (std) by major- [-0.5;0.5] 0.53 [0.42; 0.69]** [0.10; 1.02] 65 0.10specific semester 4 [-1;1] 0.45 [0.27; 0.62]** [0.06; 0.83] 140 0.19

[-1.5;1.5] 0.34 [0.18; 0.48]** [0.01; 0.65] 216 0.14

Note: The table shows estimates for the bounds on the effects of repetition of a full year on grade pointaverages by the end of each of the first four major-specific semesters, following Lee (2009). The estimatesare computed for three different samples, that is, for individuals within a range of 0.5, 1, and 1.5 minuscredits to each side of the threshold, respectively. Column (2) shows a raw comparison of means betweenrepeaters and non-repeaters, column (3) shows the bounds, and column (4) displays the 95% confidenceinterval around the bounds. Column (6) indicates the fraction of individuals who were trimmed from thesample of non-repeaters. See Section 4.2 for details on the estimation.* Significant at 10%-level, ** Significant at 5%-level, *** Significant at 1%-level. Standard errors are inparentheses.

5.5 Are gains from repeating consistent with learning gains during

the first year?

Grade repetition can improve student performance for at least two reasons. First, stu-

dents may gain knowledge from repeating the course material. We denote this channel as

“learning gains”. Second, students may persistently change their behaviour in response

to grade retention, for example by exerting more effort, by becoming more efficient in pro-

cessing the study material, or by changing their peer groups. In practice, it is likely that a

combination of these mechanisms leads to the grade improvements we find.

The data does not allow us to disentangle the myriad channels through which grade

28

Page 30: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Table 9: Performance improvements in minus credits and first-year course grades between the firstand the second attempt of the first year

(1) Estimation window: student at most 1 MC away from threshold

Mean Mean Difference 95% CI t-stat # Obs.1st attempt 2nd attempt of difference

Minus credits (MC) 12.73 6.05 -6.68 [-10.57; -2.79] -3.37 64Grade (std) in Business 1 -0.17 0.25 0.42 [0.28; 0.55] 6.05 63Grade (std) in Economics 1 -0.19 0.35 0.54 [0.35; 0.72] 5.58 64Grade (std) in Mathematics 1 -0.41 0.20 0.61 [0.41; 0.81] 6.05 60Grade (std) in Legal Studies 1 -0.17 0.35 0.52 [0.33; 0.71] 5.31 64Grade (std) in Business 2 -0.54 -0.01 0.52 [0.29; 0.76] 4.31 61Grade (std) in Economics 2 -0.61 -0.03 0.58 [0.33; 0.84] 4.47 60Grade (std) in Mathematics 2 -0.69 -0.12 0.56 [0.34; 0.79] 5.03 57Grade (std) in Legal Studies 2 -0.49 0.03 0.52 [0.29; 0.75] 4.46 61Grade (std) in Term paper -0.09 -0.04 0.05 [-0.29; 0.40] 0.31 61

(2) Estimation window: student at most 5 MC away from threshold

Mean Mean Difference 95% CI t-stat # Obs.1st attempt 2nd attempt of difference

Minus credits (MC) 14.54 5.57 -8.97 [-10.33; -7.61] -12.95 234Grade (std) in Business 1 -0.14 0.19 0.33 [0.23; 0.42] 6.81 232Grade (std) in Economics 1 -0.21 0.26 0.47 [0.36; 0.57] 8.70 234Grade (std) in Mathematics 1 -0.41 0.09 0.51 [0.41; 0.60] 10.51 224Grade (std) in Legal Studies 1 -0.26 0.28 0.54 [0.44; 0.64] 10.62 234Grade (std) in Business 2 -0.59 -0.04 0.55 [0.44; 0.65] 10.47 225Grade (std) in Economics 2 -0.70 -0.11 0.58 [0.47; 0.70] 9.68 224Grade (std) in Mathematics 2 -0.70 -0.16 0.54 [0.43; 0.65] 9.38 214Grade (std) in Legal Studies 2 -0.66 -0.05 0.61 [0.51; 0.70] 12.18 224Grade (std) in Term paper -0.33 -0.11 0.22 [0.06; 0.38] 2.72 225

(3) Estimation window: student at most 10 MC away from threshold

Mean Mean Difference 95% CI t-stat # Obs.1st attempt 2nd attempt of difference

Minus credits (MC) 16.28 5.97 -10.31 [-11.39; -9.24] -18.79 364Grade (std) in Business 1 -0.20 0.18 0.38 [0.31; 0.46] 10.05 361Grade (std) in Economics 1 -0.21 0.21 0.42 [0.34; 0.50] 10.56 364Grade (std) in Mathematics 1 -0.44 0.11 0.56 [0.48; 0.63] 14.42 350Grade (std) in Legal Studies 1 -0.32 0.22 0.55 [0.47; 0.62] 13.79 364Grade (std) in Business 2 -0.66 -0.10 0.56 [0.47; 0.64] 12.80 351Grade (std) in Economics 2 -0.77 -0.16 0.61 [0.51; 0.70] 12.74 350Grade (std) in Mathematics 2 -0.79 -0.20 0.59 [0.50; 0.68] 12.83 336Grade (std) in Legal Studies 2 -0.78 -0.14 0.64 [0.56; 0.72] 15.50 349Grade (std) in Term paper -0.38 -0.09 0.29 [0.17; 0.41] 4.79 351

The table shows performance differences between the first and the second attempt of the first year (minuscredits, standardised grades in core courses). In panel 1 (panel 2, panel 3), the sample is restricted torepeaters who are at most 1 (5, 10) minus credit(s) away from the cutoff after their first attempt of the firstyear. Minus credits are reported in absolute, non-recentred, values. Course grades are reported instandard deviations. Column “t-stat” refers to the value of the t-statistics for a test for significantimprovements between the two attempts (H0: no difference in means between the two attempts).

29

Page 31: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

retention affects educational outcomes. However, we can assess whether the effects are

consistent with learning gains through repetition. On that account, we compare exam

grades and minus credits from the first and second attempt for all repeaters who complete

at least one course during their second attempt.

On average, repeaters who were close to the cutoff in their first attempt exceed the

goal of passing by a large margin in their second attempt. Table 9 shows substantial

improvements in minus credits, both for repeaters close to the cutoff (at most one minus

credit away, panel 1) and for repeaters further away from the cutoff (5 or 10 minus credits

away, panel 2 and 3). Repeating students finish their second attempt with only six minus

credits on average. This implies that the average repeater would pass his second attempt

even if his minus credits doubled. The average gain in minus credits is largest for repeaters

who performed worst in their first attempt (panel 3). The improvements in minus credits

are statistically significant across all samples.

Lower amounts of minus credits correspond to higher grades (Table 9). Repeaters

who fail their first attempt by at most one minus credit point show significant grade im-

provements of between 0.42 and 0.61 standard deviations on average, with the exception

of the “Term paper” (panel 1). Similar patterns are observed for students covered by pan-

els 2 and 3. The findings suggest that repeaters master the first-year course material far

better after repetition. Grade improvements beyond the first year are thus consistent with

learning gains during the first year, both in direction and in size.

6 Conclusion

This paper investigates the effect of grade retention and course repetition in higher educa-

tion on dropout decisions, academic performance, and major choice. Using administrative

data for 8 cohorts of first-year undergraduate students at the University of St. Gallen

(2001-2008), this paper studies a system where students have to repeat their first year

when they fall short of a predefined performance requirement. We account for the endo-

30

Page 32: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

geneity of the retention status in a regression discontinuity framework. We thus obtain

local estimates for the causal effects of grade retention and course repetition on dropout

probabilities, major choice, credits per semester, and grade point averages up to four

semesters after repeating. Retention increases dropout probabilities immediately after

the first year, and decreases the probability of enrolment into the second year. Course

repetition persistently increases students’ GPAs by about 0.5 standard deviations, while

study pace and major choice remain unaffected. Hence, the findings suggest significant

and persistent improvements in student GPA as a result of repeating.

The findings contribute to the research on the determinants of student success. First,

the study emphasises that course repetition is a policy that is highly effective in improving

the grades of initially low-performing students. This finding is important as repetition poli-

cies in higher education are both widespread and costly. Second, the empirical evidence

contributes to the grade retention literature, which shows that the effects of grade retention

are strongly age-dependent. While retention can have detrimental effects for high school

students, our results suggest that retention can be an effective policy in higher education.

This study leaves several questions unanswered that can be addressed in future re-

search. First, our findings are based on data from one university and, because of the

research design, provide local estimates. It is therefore important to assess the general-

isability of the results by looking at other countries and settings. Second, this study only

investigates the partial effects of the retention system on individual students. It would

be worthwhile to study retention versus other counterfactual policies in a general equilib-

rium setting, also taking into account how the retention policy affects the overall quality of

the student pool. For example, a university that implements a retention policy may deter

students who do not want to expose themselves to such a policy. Third, the pathways

through which retention and course repetition affect dropout decisions and subsequent

student performance are under-researched. Fourth, to assess the scope of the policy, it

will be important to study transitions from school to work and in particular how retention

policies affect individual labour market success.

31

Page 33: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

References

ALTONJI, J. G., E. BLOM, AND C. MEGHIR (2012): “Heterogeneity in Human Capital

Investments: High School Curriculum, College Major, and Careers,” Annual Review of

Economics, 4, 185–223.

ARCIDIACONO, P. (2004): “Ability sorting and the returns to college major,” Journal of

Econometrics, 121, 343–375.

BETTINGER, E. P. AND B. T. LONG (2009): “Addressing the Needs of Underprepared

Students in Higher Education: Does College Remediation Work?” Journal of Human

Resources, 44, 736–771.

GREENE, J. AND M. WINTERS (2007): “Revisiting grade retention: An evaluation of

Florida’s test-based promotion policy,” Education Finance and Policy, 2, 319–340.

IMBENS, G. AND K. KALYANARAMAN (2012): “Optimal Bandwidth Choice for the Regres-

sion Discontinuity Estimator,” The Review of Economic Studies, 79, 933–959.

IMBENS, G. AND T. LEMIEUX (2008): “Regression discontinuity designs: A guide to prac-

tice,” Journal of Econometrics, 142, 615–635.

JACOB, B. AND L. LEFGREN (2004): “Remedial education and student achievement: A

regression-discontinuity analysis,” Review of Economics and Statistics, 86, 226–244.

JACOB, B. A. AND L. LEFGREN (2009): “The Effect of Grade Retention on High School

Completion,” American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 1, 33–58.

LEE, D. S. (2009): “Training, Wages, and Sample Selection: Estimating Sharp Bounds on

Treatment Effects,” The Review of Economic Studies, 76, 1071–1102.

LORENCE, J. AND A. DWORKIN (2006): “Elementary grade retention in Texas and reading

achievement among racial groups: 1994–2002,” Review of Policy Research, 23, 999–

1033.

32

Page 34: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

MANACORDA, M. (2012): “The Cost of Grade Retention,” Review of Economics and Statis-

tics, 94, 596–606.

MANSKI, C. F. (1989): “Schooling as experimentation: a reappraisal of the postsecondary

dropout phenomenon,” Economics of Education Review, 8, 305–312.

MCCRARY, J. (2008): “Manipulation of the running variable in the regression discontinuity

design: A density test,” Journal of Econometrics, 142, 698–714.

NICHOLS, A. (2011): “rd 2.0: Revised Stata module for regression discontinuity estima-

tion,” Statistical Software Components, Boston College Department of Economics.

OECD (2014): Education at a Glance 2014: OECD Indicators, Paris: OECD Publishing.

OU, D. (2010): “To leave or not to leave? A regression discontinuity analysis of the impact

of failing the high school exit exam,” Economics of Education Review, 29, 171–186.

RODERICK, M. AND J. NAGAOKA (2005): “Retention under Chicagos high-stakes testing

program: Helpful, harmful, or harmless?” Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis,

27, 309.

TAUCHMANN, H. (2012): “LEEBOUNDS: Stata module for estimating Lee (2009) treat-

ment effect bounds,” Statistical Software Components, Boston College Department of

Economics.

33

Page 35: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Appendix

Figures

Figure A.1: Time line: Institutional setup

First year

(first attempt)

First year

(second attempt)

pass fail

On-time graduation

dropoutdropout

semester 4

semester 3

semester 10

semester 9

semester 8

semester 7

semester 6

semester 5

semester 2

semester 1

Major-specific

level

exit

pass fail

On-time graduation

Major-specific

level

Note: The figure displays the time line of undergraduate education at the University of St. Gallen. Studentscan decide to drop out at any time, but dropout rates are particularly high during and after the first year.On-time graduation takes place after 6 semesters for non-repeaters and after 8 semesters for repeaters.The majority of students does not graduate on time. Students who fail the second attempt of the first yearare exmatriculated and not allowed to repeat again.

34

Page 36: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Figure A.2: RDD estimates: Student background characteristics

19.5

2020

.521

Age

(ye

ars)

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .02 (.19)

(1) Age (years)

.65

.7.7

5.8

.85

Fra

ctio

n: M

ale

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): 0 (.05)

(2) Fraction: Male

.05

.1.1

5.2

.25

.3F

ract

ion:

Non

−S

wis

s na

tiona

lity

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): −.04 (.04)

(3) Fraction: Non−Swiss nationality

0.0

5.1

.15

.2F

ract

ion:

Ent

ranc

e te

st

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): −.04 (.04)

(4) Fraction: Entrance test

.1.1

5.2

.25

.3F

ract

ion:

Hig

h sc

hool

St.

Gal

len

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): −.03 (.04)

(5) Fraction: High school St. Gallen

Note: The figure investigates the smoothness of background characteristics at the cutoff. The black dotsindicate means of characteristics in bins of a width of 2 minus credits. The solid lines present a linear fit toboth sides of the threshold. The number of minus credits is re-centred around the cutoff of 12.25 minuscredits. The dashed lines indicate 95% confidence intervals. The discontinuity coefficient indicates thedistance of the two regression lines exactly at the cutoff. Standard errors are in parentheses. The sampleconsists of all individuals in the estimation sample within a range of 10 minus credits to either side of thecutoff (n = 1,967).

35

Page 37: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Figure A.3: RDD estimates: Major choice

0.2

.4.6

.8M

ajor

cho

ice:

Bus

ines

s

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): −.039 (.078)

(1) Business

0.2

.4.6

.8M

ajor

cho

ice:

Eco

nom

ics

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): −.007 (.053)

(2) Economics

0.2

.4.6

.8M

ajor

cho

ice:

Inte

rnat

iona

l Affa

irs

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): .077 (.063)

(3) International Affairs

0.2

.4.6

.8M

ajor

cho

ice:

Law

and

Eco

nom

ics

−10 −5 0 5 10Number of minus credits (adjusted)

discontinuity coeff. (se): −.067 (.037)

(4) Law and Economics

Note: The panels above provide a graphical illustration of effects of retention on major choice after the firstyear. The dots represent the mean outcomes in bins of 2 minus credits. The solid lines display a linear fit toeither side of the cutoff (dashed lines: 95% confidence intervals). The discontinuity coefficient indicates thedistance of the two regression lines exactly at the cutoff. Standard errors are in parentheses. The sampleconsists of all individuals in the estimation sample within a range of 10 minus credits to either side of thecutoff (n = 1,857).

36

Page 38: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Tables

Table A.1: Graduation statistics for Switzerland

Graduation Average 2007 2008 2009 2010Total (N=82233) 20558 17797 20205 21230 23001Business Administration or Economics (N=12258) 3065 2904 2963 3009 3382in % of Total 15.0 16.3 14.7 14.2 14.7at University of St. Gallen (N=3640) 910 903 903 881 953in % of Total 4.5 5.1 4.5 4.1 4.1in % of Business Administration or Economics 29.8 31.1 30.5 29.3 28.2

Note: Graduation consists of Licentiate, Bachelor or Master in Switzerland. All Percentages arerounded to one decimal place. Source: Federal Statistical Office of Switzerland (2014) and owncalculations. The data can be retrievedfrom http://www.bfs.admin.ch/bfs/portal/de/index/themen/15/06/data/blank/01.html.

Table A.2: Time trend in student numbers at the University of St. Gallen, 1990-2013.

Academic year Total number of Number ofstudents first-year

undergraduates(1) (2) (3)

1990 3908 582... ... ...

2000 4701 8432001 4938 9712002 4917 9532003 4852 9002004 4569 7892005 4508 9542006 4915 10222007 5367 10602008 5928 13152009 6418 13142010 6726 11912011 7126 13322012 7325 12932013 7666 1328

Note: The table shows the time trend for student numbers at the University of St. Gallen. Column(2) shows the total number of enrolled students at the beginning of the academic year, includingall degrees. Column (3) shows the corresponding number of enrolled first-year undergraduates.Source: University of St. Gallen, HSG Annual Reports and own calculations. The annual reportscan be retrieved fromhttp://www.unisg.ch/en/hsgservices/hsgmediacorner/publikationen/hsgjahresbericht.

37

Page 39: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Tabl

eA

.3:

Typi

calc

urric

ulum

ofa

stud

ent

Cal

enda

rWee

k38

45-4

646

5051

3-7

101s

tSem

este

rS

tart

regi

ster

toex

ams

Ass

ignm

ents

End

1stc

entra

lexa

min

atio

npe

riod

getg

rade

sE

xam

LSC

TE

con

BA

Law

Mat

hC

redi

ts3

25.

55

5.5

3.5

Cal

enda

rWee

k8

15-1

650

-15

1616

-21

2125

-29

352n

dS

emes

ter

Sta

rtre

gist

erto

exam

sA

ssig

nmen

tsE

nd2n

dce

ntra

lexa

min

atio

npe

riod

getg

rade

sE

xam

Ess

ayB

A*

LSE

con

BA

Law

Mat

hC

TFL

Cre

dits

52

35.

55

5.5

3.5

24

The

tabl

esh

ows

aty

pica

lcur

ricul

umof

ast

uden

tin

the

first

year

.A

bbre

viat

ions

ofco

urse

s:LS

–Le

ader

ship

skill

s;C

T–

Crit

ical

Thin

king

;BA

–B

usin

ess

Adm

inis

tratio

n:B

A*

–B

usin

ess

Adm

inis

tratio

n:C

ase

stud

y;FL

–Fo

reig

nLa

ngua

ge;A

ssig

nmen

ts–

Ess

ays

oror

alas

sign

men

tsdu

ring

the

sem

este

r;E

xam

s–

Writ

ten

exam

sdu

ring

the

exam

perio

d.S

ourc

e:C

ours

ecu

rric

ula

ofth

eU

nive

rsity

ofS

t.G

alle

n,m

ade

avai

labl

eby

the

univ

ersi

tyad

min

istra

tion.

38

Page 40: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Table A.4: Sample construction, cohorts 2001-2008

% dropped

Entering first-year students 6,706Dropped: Non-German mother tongue 397 5.9%Dropped: Not Business/Economics track 582 8.7%Dropped: Inconsistent or missing data 85 1.3%

Final sample 5,392

The table documents the exclusion of subgroups from the analysis. We exclude students with non-Germanmother tongue who follow different exam procedures. We furthermore exclude students who do not enterthe Business/Economics track of the first year. A minor fraction of students is included due to missing dataentries for some of the variables used in the analysis.

Table A.5: Statistics on retention and repetition, cohorts 2001-2008

Sample of first-year students 5,392Passed 1st attempt 3,817 % of sample 71%Failed 1st attempt 1,575 % of sample 29%Repeater 942 % of sample 17%

% of failed 60%Repeater: Completes all courses (2nd attempt) 741 % of repeaters 79%Passed 2nd attempt 584 % of repeaters 62%

% of repeaters – all courses 79%Passed: 1st and 2nd attempt combined 4,401 % of sample 82%

The table shows statistics on retention, repetition, and passing rates for cohorts 2001-2008. The initialsample is based on all students with non-German mothertongue who enter the Business/Economics track(n = 5,392, see Table A.4).

39

Page 41: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Table A.6: Student background characteristics and outcomes, by type

Type 1: Type 2: Type 3: Type 4:Drops out Not admitted Drops out after Completes

during to 1st/during 2nd full1st semester 2nd semester semester 1st year

Background characteristicsAge 21.0 20.4 20.6 20.3Male 73% 65% 67% 73%Non-Swiss nationality 19% 16% 22% 24%Entrance test 9% 6% 14% 20%High school St. Gallen 17% 19% 13% 15%

Minus credits# MC in 1st semester - 19.4 7.2 1.8# MC in first year - - - 4.7Failed - - - 13%

Course performance 1st semesterBusiness 1 taken 56% 100% 100% 100%Grade Business 1 3.0 3.1 3.7 4.2Economics 1 taken 51% 100% 100% 100%Grade Economics 1 3.1 3.2 4.0 4.6Mathematics 1 taken 44% 100% 100% 100%Grade Mathematics 1 2.8 2.8 3.7 4.5Legal Studies 1 taken 58% 100% 100% 100%Grade Legal Studies 1 3.2 3.1 3.8 4.5

Course performance 2nd semesterBusiness 2 taken - - 40% 100%Grade Business 2 3.6 4.3Economics 2 taken - - 39% 100%Grade Economics 2 3.8 4.5Mathematics 2 taken - - 34% 100%Grade Mathematics 2 3.8 4.5Legal Studies 2 taken - - 40% 100%Grade Legal Studies 2 3.7 4.3Term paper taken - - 67% 100%Grade Term paper 4.6 5.0

Major-specific levelMajor-specific level started 13% 25% 43% 96%

# obs 392 437 181 4,382

The table displays differences in mean background characteristics and outcomes across different types ofstudents. Students are characterised according to their behaviour and performance during the first attemptof the first year. See Section 3.2 for details on the type definition. Course grades are reported as absolutegrades, with 1 being the lowest and 6 being the highest grade. The sample is based on all students withGerman mother tongue who enter the Business/Economics track (5,392 students).

40

Page 42: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Tabl

eA

.7:

Stu

dent

back

grou

ndch

arac

teris

tics

and

perfo

rman

ce,b

yco

hort

Full

sam

ple

2001

2002

2003

2004

2005

2006

2007

2008

#O

bs.

4,38

261

852

445

538

552

256

561

070

3B

ackg

roun

dch

arac

teris

tics

Age

20.2

620

.47

20.5

120

.16

20.1

420

.24

20.2

720

.03

20.2

2M

ale

73%

74%

73%

75%

75%

74%

72%

73%

73%

Non

-Sw

iss

natio

nalit

y24

%19

%25

%26

%27

%24

%21

%26

%26

%E

ntra

nce

test

20%

16%

20%

23%

22%

19%

18%

21%

23%

Hig

hsc

hool

St.

Gal

len

15%

13%

14%

17%

18%

14%

16%

13%

16%

Min

uscr

edits

Frac

tion:

MC>

057

%64

%61

%56

%51

%56

%51

%53

%60

%#

MC

infir

stye

ar4.

674.

425.

594.

244.

225.

403.

844.

515.

02Fa

iled

13%

13%

15%

10%

11%

16%

10%

13%

14%

Cou

rse

perfo

rman

ce1s

tyea

rG

rade

Bus

ines

s1

4.22

4.23

4.22

4.19

4.22

4.25

4.28

4.21

4.15

Gra

deE

cono

mic

s1

4.58

4.58

4.47

4.56

4.80

4.61

4.55

4.63

4.53

Gra

deM

athe

mat

ics

14.

534.

424.

584.

484.

634.

534.

554.

654.

43G

rade

Lega

lStu

dies

14.

474.

494.

464.

544.

644.

474.

594.

344.

33G

rade

Bus

ines

s2

4.26

4.30

4.10

4.46

4.77

4.02

4.42

4.22

4.04

Gra

deE

cono

mic

s2

4.45

4.39

4.26

4.26

4.42

4.52

4.76

4.62

4.36

Gra

deM

athe

mat

ics

24.

524.

514.

434.

604.

854.

544.

384.

474.

51G

rade

Lega

lStu

dies

24.

334.

054.

194.

494.

294.

424.

384.

384.

45G

rade

Term

pape

r4.

984.

904.

984.

924.

984.

995.

025.

015.

00O

utco

mes

:D

ropo

utD

ropo

utaf

ter1

stye

ar2%

3%3%

2%2%

3%2%

2%3%

Rep

eate

r11

%11

%14

%9%

9%14

%8%

11%

12%

Out

com

es:

Maj

or-s

peci

ficle

vel

Maj

or-s

peci

ficle

vels

tart

ed96

%94

%94

%97

%98

%95

%96

%96

%95

%B

ache

lor’s

Deg

ree

com

plet

edW

ithin

4m

ajor

-spe

cific

sem

este

rs(o

ntim

e)(1

)45

%47

%49

%40

%33

%26

%23

%23

%-

With

in5

maj

or-s

peci

ficse

mes

ters

(1)

66%

69%

71%

64%

60%

55%

50%

50%

-W

ithin

6m

ajor

-spe

cific

sem

este

rs(1

)87

%87

%88

%88

%85

%80

%80

%83

%-

The

tabl

esh

ows

desc

riptiv

est

atis

tics

fort

hees

timat

ion

sam

ple,

that

is,f

oral

lstu

dent

sw

hoha

veco

mpl

eted

allfi

rst-y

earc

ours

es,s

epar

atel

yfo

reac

hco

hort

(4,3

82st

uden

ts).

Eac

hco

lum

ndi

spla

ysei

ther

mea

nsor

perc

enta

ges.

Info

rmat

ion

onco

mpl

etio

nof

the

Bac

helo

r’sde

gree

ism

issi

ngfo

rthe

coho

rtof

2008

due

toce

nsor

ing.

(1)Va

lues

nota

vaila

ble

forc

ohor

t200

8.

41

Page 43: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Table A.8: Test for randomness of missing outcomes: Retained students

Dependent variable: Non-missing grades at the major-specific level (binary)

(1) (2) (3) (4)Estimation window 5 MC 5 MC 5 MC 10 MC(max. distance from cutoff)

coef (se) coef (se) coef (se) coef (se)Grade (std) in Business 1 -0.006 -0.014 -0.011

(0.057) (0.057) (0.049)Grade (std) in Economics 1 -0.022 -0.013 -0.005

(0.054) (0.057) (0.048)Grade (std) in Mathematics 1 0.054 0.042 0.032

(0.046) (0.047) (0.039)Grade (std) in Legal Studies 1 0.045 0.060 0.065

(0.062) (0.062) (0.052)Grade (std) in Business 2 0.102* 0.117** 0.118***

(0.054) (0.055) (0.042)Grade (std) in Economics 2 0.076 0.090* 0.083**

(0.048) (0.049) (0.041)Grade (std) in Mathematics 2 -0.029 -0.029 0.020

(0.046) (0.046) (0.038)Grade (std) in Legal Studies 2 0.084 0.070 0.094**

(0.051) (0.052) (0.045)Grade (std) in Term paper 0.027 0.022 0.027

(0.033) (0.034) (0.028)Age (years) -0.018* -0.022** -0.024***

(0.009) (0.010) (0.009)Male -0.042 -0.043 -0.036

(0.057) (0.061) (0.051)Non-Swiss nationality 0.141 0.158 0.093

(0.116) (0.115) (0.077)Entrance test -0.187 -0.186 -0.060

(0.143) (0.141) (0.106)High school St. Gallen 0.017 0.031 -0.005

(0.072) (0.071) (0.056)

Minus credits (third order polynomial) Yes Yes YesCohort dummies Yes Yes Yes

Pseudo R-squared 0.052 0.092 0.078Number of observations 267 267 267 428

The table shows average marginal effects from a probit regression of a binary indicator – whether a studenthas non-missing grades at the major-specific level – on standardised course grades during the first yearand individual student characteristics. The sample includes only students who completed the first year, butfailed. The different specifications allow for different distances from the threshold, i.e., a distance of up to 5minus credits from the threshold in specifications (1)-(3), and a distance of up to 10 minus credits from thethreshold in specification (4). All specifications include cohort dummies and control for the exact number ofminus credits.* Significant at 10%-level, ** Significant at 5%-level, *** Significant at 1%-level. Standard errors are inparentheses.

42

Page 44: USC Dornsife Institute for New Economic Thinkingmate the effects of (1) grade retention on dropout probabilities and (2) repetition of all ... The grading scheme 5Foreign students

Table A.9: Test for randomness of missing outcomes: Non-retained students

Dependent variable: Non-missing grades at the major-specific level (binary)

(1) (2) (3) (4)Estimation window 5 MC 5 MC 5 MC 10 MC(max. distance from cutoff)

coef (se) coef (se) coef (se) coef (se)Grade (std) in Business 1 -0.007 -0.006 0.015

(0.025) (0.025) (0.012)Grade (std) in Economics 1 0.000 0.015 -0.010

(0.022) (0.023) (0.011)Grade (std) in Mathematics 1 0.009 0.005 -0.011

(0.021) (0.022) (0.011)Grade (std) in Legal Studies 1 0.057** 0.053** 0.019

(0.025) (0.025) (0.012)Grade (std) in Business 2 0.018 0.015 -0.004

(0.019) (0.019) (0.009)Grade (std) in Economics 2 -0.011 -0.006 -0.005

(0.017) (0.017) (0.009)Grade (std) in Mathematics 2 0.023 0.021 0.018*

(0.020) (0.020) (0.010)Grade (std) in Legal Studies 2 -0.019 -0.022 0.002

(0.018) (0.018) (0.010)Grade (std) in Term paper 0.024* 0.019 0.011*

(0.013) (0.013) (0.006)Age (years) 0.017* 0.017* -0.000

(0.009) (0.009) (0.003)Male -0.080*** -0.079** -0.004

(0.030) (0.031) (0.013)Non-Swiss nationality -0.040 -0.012 -0.003

(0.038) (0.040) (0.023)Entrance test 0.070 0.038 0.042

(0.049) (0.050) (0.029)High school St. Gallen 0.023 0.018 0.011

(0.031) (0.031) (0.016)

Minus credits (third order polynomial) Yes Yes Yes YesCohort dummies Yes Yes Yes Yes

Pseudo R-squared 0.125 0.130 0.169 0.084Number of observations 523 523 523 1,539

The table shows average marginal effects from a probit regression of a binary indicator – whether a studenthas non-missing grades at the major-specific level – on standardised course grades during the first yearand individual student characteristics. The sample includes only students who passed their first year intheir first attempt. The different specifications allow for different distances from the threshold, i.e., adistance of up to 5 minus credits from the threshold in specifications (1)-(3), and a distance of up to 10minus credits from the threshold in specification (4). All specifications include cohort dummies and controlfor the exact number of minus credits.* Significant at 10%-level, ** Significant at 5%-level, *** Significant at 1%-level. Standard errors are inparentheses.

43


Recommended