+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon,...

Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon,...

Date post: 31-Mar-2015
Category:
Upload: dontae-pipes
View: 214 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
Popular Tags:
54
Vision after complete blindness: Mike May al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing T
Transcript
Page 1: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Vision after complete blindness: Mike May

Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Page 2: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

But understanding normal perception is the toughestand most important challenge for psychology

We tend to take normal perception for granted, while findingbizarre experiences of exceptional individuals intriguing…

Occasionally, though, an exceptional individual’s experience can advance our understanding of normal perception…

For instance, the case of Mike May helps us not to take perception for granted

Page 3: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

To what extent does visual processing rely on visual

experience?

Page 4: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Ione Fine (UCSD USC)

Page 5: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Ione Fine (UCSD USC)

Geoff Boynton (UCSD/Salk): fMRI+

Page 6: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Ione Fine (UCSD USC)

Geoff Boynton (UCSD/Salk): fMRI+

Brian Wandell (Stanford)and

Alex Wade, Alissa Brewer (Stanford)

Page 7: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Ione Fine (UCSD USC)

Geoff Boynton (UCSD/Salk): fMRI+

Brian Wandell (Stanford)

Alex Wade, Alissa Brewer (Stanford)

Stuart Anstis (UCSD)

Page 8: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Subject Mike May

• Blinded by a chemical accident at age 3. • Light sensitive (no form vision) between ages of 3-43

Page 9: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Subject Mike May

Sight restored by a new procedure - Corneal epithelial stem cell replacement

Page 10: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’
Page 11: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

• Resolution• 2 & 3d Form• Motion• Object/Face Recognition

Page 12: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Campbell & Robson (1968)

Spatial frequency (cycles/degree)

Sensitivity

Contrast-Sensitivity Function (CSF)

Resolution limit: 50cpd

Page 13: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Resolution limit < 2 cpd, despite good optics

0 1 2 30

0.5

1

1.5

2

Spatial frequency (c/deg)

Log

sen

siti

vit

y

MM (Post-operatively)Normal

Page 14: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

0 1 2 30

0.5

1

1.5

2

Spatial frequency (c/deg)

Log

sen

siti

vit

y MM + 5 monthsMM +11 monthsMM +17 monthsMM +21 monthsCONTROL

No improvement over time

Page 15: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

MM could identify simple shapes2D FORM

Page 16: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

…but not shapes defined by illusory contours. 2D FORM

Page 17: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

2D FORM

Mike can identify simple 2d forms (100% correct)

But “constructive” 2d perception is harder

MM = 80%; controls=100%, 90%, 95%)

MM = 73%; controls = 80%, 85%, 100%

guessing

Letters recognizable

Page 18: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

3D FORM

Sensitive (100% correct)to occlusion …

Page 19: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Shading gave no automatic impression of depth:

3D FORM

The circle was seen as a flat disc, with non-uniform surface lightness

Page 20: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

“A square with lines attached”

3D FORM

Fails with:

Shape from Shading:

Perspective:

Page 21: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Could NOT recognize a stationary cube from any angle - “square with lines”

MOTION

Page 22: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Couldn’t identify STATIC cube…but with a ROTATING one, “It’s a cube! …going in …going out”

Page 23: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Could NOT recognize a stationary cube from any angle - “square with lines”

MOTION

Page 24: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Could NOT recognize a stationary cube from any angle - “square with lines”

YET … Can exploit motion to construct 3D structure- “it’s a cube! …moving in, moving out”

MOTION

Page 25: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

MOTIONCould make sense of…

Point-light walker

Rotational Glass patterns

Structure from motion(100% correct)

QuickTime™ and aGIF decompressor

are needed to see this picture.

MM = 90%; controls=95%, 80%, 85%).

Page 26: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’
Page 27: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Sophisticated processing of MOTION:

• Can see form from motion (KDE cube)

• Saw depth in face masks by rocking his head

• Could see Johansson’s walking man

• Can play catch

• Skiing: vision now helps!

Page 28: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Motion SB (Ackroyd et al)“His only signs of appreciation were to moving objects, particularly the pigeons in Trafalgar square… He clearly enjoyed … watching … the movement of other cars on the road …He spotted a speeder coming up very fast behind us”

Virgil (Sacks)“when [the gorilla] finally came into the open he thought that, though it moved differently, it looked just like a large man”

QuickTime™ and aCinepak decompressor

are needed to see this picture.

Page 29: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

GenderMM 70% correctcontrol 100%

Expression (happy/sad/neutral) MM 61% correctcontrol 100%

MM 25% correctcontrol 100%

Poor object & face identification

Page 30: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Clinton&GoreBy Sinha &Poggio

Page 31: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’
Page 32: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

These dissociations between form and motion tasks were consistent with the size and

activation of visual areas measured using fMRI

V1 and (especially) extrastriate areas in the temporal stream, thought to be responsible for form processing, were small and showed low activity levels.

The Medial Temporal complex, thought to be responsible for motion processing, was normal in both size and activation

Page 33: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Cortical area

Su

rfa

ce a

rea

(cm

2)

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

35

V1(L)

V1(R)

MT+(L)

MT+(R)

control observersMM

Size of V1 and MT+

Page 34: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

“The eye of the artist”Matches projective shapes, not real shapes, e.g. NOT susceptible to Shepard’s illusion:

Tables have the SAME projective shape, and to MM they look the same

Page 35: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

“flat world”Matches projective shapes, not real shapes, e.g. NOT susceptible to Shepard’s illusion:

Tables have the SAME projective shape, and to MM they look the same

Page 36: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

“flat world”Matches projective shapes, not real shapes, e.g. NOT susceptible to Shepard’s illusion:

Tables have the SAME projective shape, and to MM they look the same

Page 37: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

“flat world”Matches projective shapes, not real shapes, e.g. NOT susceptible to Shepard’s illusion:

Tables have the SAME projective shape, and to MM they look the same

Page 38: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

“flat world”Matches projective shapes, not real shapes, e.g. NOT susceptible to Shepard’s illusion:

Tables have the SAME projective shape, and to MM they look the same

Page 39: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

“flat world”Matches projective shapes, not real shapes, e.g. NOT susceptible to Shepard’s illusion:

Tables have the SAME projective shape, and to MM they look the same

Page 40: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

“flat world”Matches projective shapes, not real shapes, e.g. NOT susceptible to Shepard’s illusion:

Tables have the SAME projective shape, and to MM they look the same

Page 41: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

“flat world”Matches projective shapes, not real shapes, e.g. NOT susceptible to Shepard’s illusion:

Tables have the SAME projective shape, and to MM they look the same

Page 42: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

“flat world”Matches projective shapes, not real shapes, e.g. NOT susceptible to Shepard’s illusion:

Tables have the SAME projective shape, and to MM they look the same

Page 43: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Images from Ted Adelson

Mike correctly sees thediamonds as similar in lightness, and responds photometrically to illumination and shadow in pictures, seeing shadows as dark things.

http://psylux.psych.tu-dresden.de/i1/kaw/diverses%20Material/www.illusionworks.com/html/shadow.html

Page 44: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

• Normally sighted subjects cannot retrieve any aspect of experience that is a function of retinal illuminance or projected size. But MM has these (and nothing else) available to undirected introspection. In this sense he is free (unfortunately), of the good ‘illusions’ on which normal vision is founded.

• One example of resulting difficulties: Shadows at the edges of sidewalks appeared to him as black ridges that could present a potential hazard in walking

Phenomenal Regression to the Real Object

Page 45: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

• Why can’t we see and judge what’s present at the sensory input, as MM can?

• William James wrote: “Pure sensations can only be realised in the earliest days of life. They are all but impossible to adults with memories and stores of association acquired."

• For MM (though NOT necessarily for a newborn: Granrud), James may be right, perhaps because the irrepressible interpretative processes of the normally sighted brain are not involved.

• For the normally sighted, interpretation is not an integument that can be peeled away to reveal sensory bedrock: it penetrates all our consciousness, presumably thanks to the continuously bidirectional flow of information through the visual system.

• So we have no ‘pure sensations’…but those ares all that MM has.

Phenomenal Regression to the Real Object

Page 46: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

V1

LGN

Parietal (action)

temporal (perception)

The visual process as a causal chain

Page 47: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

V1

LGN

Parietal (action)

temporal (perception)

In the normal visual system, each neural representation depends on the later ones.

The visual process as a feedback system

Page 48: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

MM was not sensitive to perspective cues

DEPTH

Yet he WAS susceptible to the Muller-Lyer and related illusions

Page 49: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Richard Gregory

Page 50: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

“Dumbbell” variant of the Muller-Lyer illusion

Page 51: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Aesthetics

• Color– Variety and vividness were new and impressive

• Bodies– Innate sign stimuli vs. interest based on association

• Dust, waves, fireworks– Meaning confers no aesthetic advantage

Page 52: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

Key points about Mike’s perception

• Mike has a severe neural resolution loss,improving slowly if at all.

• He doesn’t see 2D subjective contours.• 2D Geometrical illusions are present.• Both perspective and shape from shading are

ineffective for depth perception.• But motion cues are effective.• Recognizing faces and common objects is a challenge.• We think of Mike as having “the eye of the artist”, inhabiting

a world of abstract 2-dimensional shapes and colors.• This may be why he now uses his vision, as he puts it, “mainly for entertainment”

Page 53: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’
Page 54: Vision after complete blindness: Mike May Fine et al., Nature Neuroscience 2003; Robert Kurzon, ‘Crashing Through’

The End


Recommended