+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Well l Being

Well l Being

Date post: 04-Apr-2018
Category:
Upload: fkkfox
View: 223 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 91

Transcript
  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    1/91

    Stress and well-being 1

    Stress and Well-Being at Work

    Mark A. Griffin

    University of Sheffield

    Sharon Clarke

    University of Manchester

    To appear in S. Zedeck (Ed.). Handbook of Industrial/Organizational Psychology (Volume 3) .

    Washington DC: American Psychological Association

    August 2009

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    2/91

    Stress and well-being 2

    STRESS AND W ELL -BEING AT W ORK

    INTRODUCTION

    Stress is the single most common reason given for absence from work in the UK, and one in

    six Americans report they are extremely stressed (HSE, 2008). However, the general notion of

    stress at work can be contentious, and the possibility that work causes illness has been received with

    varying degrees of skepticism, indifference, and alarm across sectors of modern society. Regulators,

    employers, unions, insurers, and health professionals continue to struggle with the meaning and

    management of stress in the workplace.

    Although there is much diversity across concepts of work stress, a considerable body of

    stress-related theory and practice has developed over the past 40 years. Researchers from

    psychology, economics, sociology, public health, engineering and medicine represent just some of

    the major disciplines that have addressed the nature and consequences of work stress (Ganster &

    Schaubroeck, 1991). Within the field of psychology, Beehr and Franz (1987) identified medical,

    clinical psychology, engineering psychology, and organizational psychology approaches. This

    chapter focuses on research from the field of organizational psychology but draws from multiple

    areas where appropriate.

    Work stress is not a single event or a specific psychological state. Rather, work stress

    describes a general process in which individuals respond to and manage demands to meet multiple

    goals over time. A basic distinction between stressors (e.g., excessive workloads) as the primary

    drivers of this process, and strains (e.g., anxiety and depression) as its primary outcomes, has

    proved useful building a more complete picture of the stress process (e.g., Caplan, Cobb, French,

    Harrison, & Pinneau, 1980; Kahn, Wolfe, Quinn, Snoek, & Rosenthal, 1964; Lazarus, 1966;

    McGrath, 1976). Stressors describe the demands experienced by individuals and strains capture a

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    3/91

    Stress and well-being 3

    range of negative outcomes for health and well-being. Nevertheless, most popular writing and many

    research studies refer to stress as if it was a single outcome, typically as a shorthand for the strain

    experience of feeling stressed.

    All theories of stress must grapple with the common observation that although some events

    are intrinsically stressful (e.g., experiencing a violent event at work), individuals respond to

    stressful events in different ways. At the extreme, the same work environment can be debilitating

    and negative for one individual while at the same time it is exciting and challenging for another.

    The distinction between stressors and strains has proved useful for addressing this core problem

    (Kahn & Byosiere, 1992). A stressor is the perceived demand from the environment and therefore

    comprises both external stimuli and the perceptual processes of the individual. Strains are the

    psychological, behavioral, and physiological outcomes of this process and include such diverse

    responses as anxiety, absenteeism, and illness. Overall, the stressor-strain distinction captures the

    elements of a negative transaction between the individual and the environment.

    The stressor-strain distinction has proved less useful for articulating how positive and

    negative experiences interact in the stress process or for elaborating the way the stress process

    unfolds over time. Recent approaches to work stress draw on more fully developed theories of

    affect (e.g., Watson, Wiese, Vaidya, & Tellegen, 1999) and give greater attention to systems of

    positive psychology (e.g., Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000) to build better elaborated models of

    well-being and the stress process (Folkman, 2008). Time spans have always been important

    considerations in models of stress, but most empirical studies have used cross-sectional designs

    (Zapf, Dormann, & Frese, 1996). Recently more attention has been paid to research designs that

    capture the unfolding transaction between individuals and their work context. For example, models

    of recovery incorporate a cyclical process of resource depletion and repair that unfolds from day to

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    4/91

    Stress and well-being 4

    day and week to week (e.g., Sonnentag & Zijlstra, 2006). Importantly, just as work can have

    negative consequences for the health and well-being of individuals, so too can being out of work.

    Whilst unemployment may be viewed as a challenge by some people, depending on their personal

    agency, it can also be experienced as an extremely stressful experience for others, especially in the

    long-term (Fryer, 1986). The stress process associated with unemployment will not be considered

    further in this chapter, which focuses on stress related to work (for a review see Fryer, 1998).

    The remainder of this chapter is divided into four sections. First, we review seven different

    approaches to the stress process, highlighting influential research and theory over the past forty

    years. Second, we introduce an integrative framework as a basis for discussing similarities and

    differences across the major approaches. The integrated framework also provides a basis for

    reviewing the role of time in the stress process and considering implications for different

    methodological approaches and research paradigms. Third, we review organizational interventions

    to reduce stress and fourth, we conclude with a summary and review some future directions for the

    study of the stress process. Excluded from this review are specific sources of work-related stress,

    such as international assignments and home-work balance, which are covered elsewhere in this

    Handbook (see also Vol. 3, Chapter 23, by Leung and Peterson, on "Managing a Globally

    Distributed Workforce: Social and Interpersonal Issues" and Vol. 3, Chapter 14, by Hammer and

    Zimmerman, on "Quality of Work Life.")

    M AJOR APPROACHES TO THE STRESS PROCESS

    Below we review seven different theoretical approaches that have been influential in

    shaping understanding of the stress process at work. This coverage is not exhaustive but the

    approaches provide an overview of the different issues addressed in the study of stress and illustrate

    the different elements of the stress process at work that have been articulated to date.

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    5/91

    Stress and well-being 5

    The socio-cognitive model

    The socio-cognitive or transactional model of stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) emphasizes

    the ongoing interaction between the person and the environment. As such, stress is not located in

    the person or the environment, but in the relationship between the environment, individuals

    appraisals of the environment, and ongoing attempts to cope with issues that arise (Cooper, Dewe,

    & O'Driscoll, 2001). The model describes two stages of cognitive appraisal. First, primary

    appraisal involves appraisal of potential stressors as threatening and posing some kind of threat to

    the individual. Then, secondary appraisal involves the evaluation of coping resources and

    alternative responses. If an individual perceives that a situation is threatening, but that he or she has

    the ability to cope with it, then strain is not experienced. Indeed, the situation may be perceived as

    challenging. Coping has been defined as the thoughts and behaviors used to manage the external

    and internal demands of situations that are appraised as stressful and also shapes emotional

    responses (Folkman & Moskowitz, 2004, p. 745). A distinction is commonly made between

    problem focused coping, which seeks to solve the demands of a stressor, and emotion focused

    coping, which helps the individual to feel better about the stressor (Cooper et al., 2001). Strain

    arises when an individual appraises the demands of a particular situation as about to exceed

    available resources and, therefore, to threaten their well-being, necessitating a change in individual

    functioning to restore the imbalance (Lazarus, 1966). The model suggests that the relationship

    between the environment and person is ongoing and reciprocal, as it is the interactions between the

    two that determine strain. Interactive effects have been researched extensively and these are

    discussed in more detail below in the section titled Longer-term dynamics of the stress process.

    Stable individual differences, such as personality, and within-person fluctuations in mood

    can affect both the appraisal of stressors as threatening and the appraisal of the individuals ability

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    6/91

    Stress and well-being 6

    to cope. For example, moods have been shown to mediate the link between daily stressors and

    same-day job performance (Stewart & Barling, 1996), where subjective perceptions of stress

    influence mood which in turn impacts on job performance. This study found that the indirect

    influence of subjective stress on job performance (mediated by negative mood, B = -.38) was

    considerably stronger than the direct effect of subjective stress on job performance ( B = -.21).

    Relatively stable personal factors, such as self-esteem, can also influence individuals appraisals.

    Individuals with high self-esteem will be less likely to view a potential stressor as threatening and

    more likely to view themselves as able to cope (Rector & Roger, 1996).

    The demands-control model

    The demands-control (DC) model (Karasek, 1979, 1989; Karasek & Theorell, 1990)

    proposes that job demands and job control play the key role in the stressor-strain relationship. Job

    demands refer to aspects of the job which require additional or sustained physical, psychological, or

    emotional effort (de Jonge & Dormann, 2003); job control, or decision latitude, refers to the degree

    of control over decisions concerning the job (Karasek & Theorell, 1990). The model predicts that

    excessive job demands increase strain, while high job control mitigates these adverse effects.

    Together, these effects constitute the strain hypothesis. Those in active jobs with high demands

    and high control should be able to minimize strain by actively managing job demands. The iso-

    strain model (Johnson, Hall, & Theorell, 1989) added the role of support, proposing that social

    support also buffers the effects of job demands on strain. Iso-strain occurs in jobs characterized by

    high demands, low control and low support. Job control and social support are viewed as resources

    because both are aspects of the job which can lead to buffering job demands and related efforts

    (Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004). Both the strain and the iso-strain hypotheses have received substantial

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    7/91

    Stress and well-being 7

    support in the literature (van der Doef & Maes, 1999; de Lange, Taris, Kompier, Houtman, &

    Bongers, 2003).

    The DC model assumes that those in active jobs will take advantage of the high level of

    control to actively manage high demands. However, there is research evidence to suggest that there

    are individual differences in the way that people react in these situations, such that personal

    characteristics moderate the demands-control relationship. Salanova, Peir, and Schaufeli (2002)

    found that for those with high self-efficacy, job control buffered the effect of job demands on strain,

    but for low self-efficacy individuals, job control acted as an additional stressor, leading to increased

    strain ( R2 = .02) Thus, for some individuals high job control can exacerbate, rather than buffer, job

    stress in demanding jobs. Parker and Sprigg (1999) found that for those with a highly proactive

    personality in a high-control job, as demands increased, job strain decreased ( R2 = .02). However,

    for passive employees, there was no demands-control interaction, indicating that strain increased

    with demands, regardless of the level of control. Meier, Semmer, Elfering, and Jacobshagen (2008)

    found for those with an internal locus of control, the predictions of the DC model were supported

    ( R2 = .07). However, for those with an external locus of control, high demands had a more negative

    effect on strain under conditions of high control (rather than low control). Given the difficulty of

    finding a substantial three-way interaction in field studies, an additional contribution of up 3% can

    be considered of practical significance. Thus, relatively stable personality characteristics have a

    significant influence on the impact of job demands and job control on strain, particularly locus of

    control.

    The role stress

    Roles describe the behavioral patterns and expectations of individuals in complex systems

    (Ilgen & Hollenbeck, 1991), and the process through which work roles create the experience of

    model

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    8/91

    Stress and well-being 8

    stress was one of the earliest and most fruitful approaches to work stress (Kahn & Byosiere, 1992).

    Kahn et al. (1964) reported the first of a series of studies based at the Institute of Social Research

    (ISR) and exploring the nature of role conflict and role ambiguity in the stress process. Role conflict

    describes two or more sets of incompatible work demands, whereas role ambiguity describes a lack

    of specificity or predictability in role functions and responsibilities (Beehr & Newman, 1978; Kahn

    et al., 1964; Katz & Kahn, 1978). Role overload is the third most common form of role stress and is

    sometimes viewed as a particular form of role conflict. Role overload is a function of too much

    work, time pressures, and a lack of resources to meet commitments and responsibilities (Beehr &

    Glazer, 2005). Role conflict and role ambiguity are probably the two most frequently studied

    stressors in organizational life (Kahn & Byosiere, 1992). There is substantial support for the role

    stress model, with large effect sizes (.43 < r < .48) reported for the effect of role stressors on

    measures of affective strain (Jackson & Schuler, 1985; Ortqvist & Wincent, 2006; Sauter, Murphy,

    & Hurrell, 1990).

    The cybernetic model

    Cybernetic models define goals and feedback loops as the central feature of the stress

    process (Cummings & Cooper, 1979). Through a feedback cycle, individuals monitor the

    discrepancy between a preferred or reference state and the actual work conditions as they are

    perceived. The perceived discrepancy is a source of strain for the individual and motivates action to

    reduce the discrepancy by changing or adapting to the environment in some way. For example, a

    discrepancy between perceived and preferred levels of workload leads to an attempt to reduce the

    level of workload or to adapt to this level of load. This coping action is then followed by a new

    comparison of the discrepancy between preferred and actual states (Edwards, 1992).

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    9/91

    Stress and well-being 9

    The feedback cycle assumes that individual behavior is directed toward a steady state of

    balance between the individual and the environment. This drive towards homeostasis creates strain

    when homeostasis is disrupted, and it motivates the need for engaging in coping activities (Miller,

    1965). Edwards (1992) noted that feedback cycles are part of most theories of stress including

    transactional and role stress models. However, he argued that the underlying cybernetic principles

    had been given insufficient attention in these models of the stress process. The proposed benefits of

    the cybernetic approach include a more systematic definition of the components of the feedback

    cycle that can be used to identify and explain a wide range of stressors, strains, and coping

    responses.

    The cybernetic feedback cycle is hierarchically organized so short-term cycles (e.g., a

    disruption to work load) are embedded within longer-term cycles (e.g., achieving life goals). For

    each cycle, perceived and actual standards provide a comparison point for determining action

    (Carver & Scheier, 1990). The source of preferred states or reference criterion derives from

    individual factors such as values and personality (Cummings & Cooper, 1998). Both positive and

    negative affect can be elicited by the feedback cycle. The cybernetic model often reflects a highly

    dynamic situation as individuals are faced with allocating limited resources in terms of time and

    attention to managing multiple goals over time. Schmidt and DeShon (2007) found that resource

    allocation is affected by both progress towards goals and the consequences of failing to reach goals,

    with avoidance goals (avoiding losses) attracting greater resources in the short term. However, in

    the longer term, this may be damaging to health and well-being. Carver and Scheier (1990)

    conceptualized a second feedback system that played a meta-monitoring function in the evaluation

    of goal progress. Affect is proposed to arise as a function of the speed of progress toward goals.

    From this perspective, homeostasis is maintained by the hierarchically nested negative feedback

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    10/91

    Stress and well-being 10

    loop while affect arises from the meta-monitoring of the speed of progress through feedback loops

    toward goal attainment.

    Although not as well-researched as the previous stress models, the major tenets of the

    cybernetic model have received support. The primary prediction of the cybernetic model is that the

    discrepancy between a preferred or desired state and actual work conditions will affect strain; as the

    discrepancy increases, so too will the adverse effects on well-being. Elsass and Veiga (1997)

    operationalized this process by examining the effect of desired goals (in this case, desired control)

    over and above the effect of job characteristics (in this case, actual control); they found that desired

    control accounted for significant additional variance in job strain, controlling for both job autonomy

    ( R2 = .04) and desired participation ( R2 = .03). Stronger support was demonstrated where desired

    states were operationalized as personal goals (ter Doest, Maes, Gebhardt, & Koelewijn, 2006); with

    personal goal facilitation accounting for significant additional variance in four measures of job

    strain (job satisfaction, personal accomplishment, emotional exhaustion and psychological

    symptoms), controlling for demographics and job characteristics (.05 < R2

    < .07).

    The challenge-hindrance model

    LePine, Podsakoff, and LePine (2005) proposed the challenge-hindrance framework, based

    on Lazarus socio-cognitive model, to account for the inconsistent evidence of the relationship

    between stressors and performance. Within this framework, stressors may be appraised as either

    challenges or hindrances. When stressors are appraised as challenging, positive emotions are

    evoked and active coping strategies, such as problem-solving, are engaged. Challenge stressors

    include job and role demands, pressure, time urgency, and workload. Drawing on expectancy

    theory, LePine et al. (2005) suggest that challenge stressors are associated with high motivation

    and, therefore, lead to better performance because individuals are likely to believe that there is a

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    11/91

    Stress and well-being 11

    positive relationship between effort and expectancy. When stressors are appraised as threatening,

    negative emotions ensue and a passive or emotion-focused style of coping is used. These are

    described as hindrance stressors and include constraints, hassles, role ambiguity, role and

    interpersonal conflict, role overload, supervisor-related stress, and organizational politics. These

    stressors are not motivating because effort expended to cope with them is unlikely to be successful

    or bring valued rewards.

    The model predicts that hindrance and challenge stressors are both subject to the same

    psychological process (primary appraisal) and both will result in strain. Indeed, LePine et al.

    (2005) found that there is some degree of overlap between these types of stressor ( r = .33), perhaps

    due to the similarity in some of the stressors, e.g., role overload (hindrance) and role demands

    (challenge). However, whilst both types of stressor are significantly related to strain (hindrance ,

    = .50; challenge, = .23) , other outcomes will differ, as challenge stressors are associated with

    positive emotions and attitudes (LePine et al, 2005). Research has supported the distinction between

    these two types of stressors, and their differential relationship with job satisfaction (Cavanaugh,

    Boswell, Roehling, & Boudreau, 2000). Podsakoff, LePine, and LePine (2007) showed strong

    support for the differential relationships between hindrance stressors and job satisfaction ( = -.57)

    compared to challenge stressors and job satisfaction ( = -.02). A similar differential relationship

    has also been demonstrated with role-based performance (Wallace, Edwards, Arnold, Frazier, &

    Finch, 2009). This framework de-emphasizes the need for individual difference explanations as it

    assumes a degree of consistency in the perception of stressors, i.e., some will be consistently

    appraised as challenging, whilst others will be consistently appraised as hindrances. However,

    despite some supporting evidence (LePine et al, 2005; Podsakoff et al, 2007; Wallace et al, 2009),

    this model is inconsistent with a substantial body of research that has emphasized the moderating

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    12/91

    Stress and well-being 12

    effect of personality, and other individual difference variables, on how individuals appraise

    stressors (see section titled Individual differences below).

    The conservation of resources model

    The Conservation of Resources (COR) approach to stress proposes that people

    fundamentally seek to obtain, retain, protect, and restore resources. Resources describe a wide range

    of objects (e.g., shelter), personal characteristics (e.g., self-esteem), conditions (e.g., status), or

    energies (e.g., knowledge) that are important for adaptive functioning (Hobfoll, 1989). Resources

    are valued in their own right or because they lead to other valued resources. Strain results from the

    threat of resource loss, the actual loss of resources, or failure to gain sufficient resources.

    Individuals also strive to develop surplus resources, which are the source of higher levels of well-

    being. Stressors are generally perceived to be negative because they deplete resources. This was

    supported by Lee & Ashforth's (1996) meta-analysis which found that five of eight work demands

    were strongly related to affective strain ( r >.50).

    COR proposes that the main motivational process in stress is preventing the loss of

    resources. Losing resources creates strains, and coping responses are enacted with the goal of

    protecting and restoring resources. COR has most frequently been studied in relation to burnout (see

    next section), and Lee and Ashforth (1996) found support for the basic propositions of COR theory

    in their meta-analysis of burnout correlates.

    The burnout model

    The term burnout has received substantial attention as a distinct dysfunctional outcome of

    the stress process for individuals. Burnout research focuses on strain outcomes of the stress process,

    in particular the more extreme forms of strain with long term negative consequences for individuals.

    Maslach and Jackson (1981) defined three dimensions of burnout: emotional exhaustion,

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    13/91

    Stress and well-being 13

    depersonalization, and diminished personal accomplishment. Emotional exhaustion describes

    feelings of being emotionally overextended, depersonalization refers to cynical and detached

    responses to others, and reduced personal accomplishment refers to a decline in efficacy and

    feelings of competence and productivity (Maslach, 1998).

    Burnout research initially focused on employees working in caregiving and people oriented

    roles where high demands arose from working with clients although the approach is widely adopted

    in other settings (Cooper et al., 2001). Lee and Ashforth (1996) argued that service providers suffer

    strain when their emotional resources are depleted to such an extent that they are no longer able to

    meet the demands of interpersonal stressors. Their results supported the link between burnout and a

    wide range of individual, job, and organizational stressors.

    The three dimensions of burnout are part of a theoretical process whereby exhaustion leads

    to actions that distance the individuals from elements of their work that are stressful, resulting in

    cynicism and depersonalization. Over time, these actions and withdrawal result in decreased

    experiences of efficacy. The causal sequence among dimensions has been controversial and

    alternative sequences have been proposed (Golembiewski, Munzenrider, & Carter, 1983), with

    some suggesting that personal accomplishment develops somewhat independently of the other two

    dimensions (Leiter, 1993). This is supported by meta-analytic findings (Lee & Ashforth, 1996) that

    exhaustion and depersonalization are more closely related ( = .64) than personal accomplishment

    with exhaustion ( = -.33) or with depersonalization ( = -.36). Exhaustion is the most widely

    reported and studied dimension of burnout (Maslach & Leiter, 2008). Lee and Ashforth's (1996)

    meta-analysis supports the proposition that exhaustion (mean = .44) and depersonalization (mean

    = .33 ) are more strongly related to job stressors than diminished personal accomplishment (mean

    = .10) . However, Halbesleben (2006) did not find that social support was a stronger predictor of

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    14/91

    Stress and well-being 14

    exhaustion as proposed in burnout models, although it acted as a moderator. Evidence linking

    burnout to individual difference variables is inconsistent, with the exception of an association

    between neuroticism and burnout. Personality traits were found to account for significant

    additional variance over and above job stressors ( R2 = .12), but only neuroticism was a significant

    predictor of burnout ( = .32) with those who are high in neuroticism more likely to develop the

    symptoms of burnout in response to stressors (Zellars, Perrewe, & Hochwarter, 2000).

    INTEGRATING FRAMEWORK

    There is much overlap in the issues and constructs addressed by different approaches to

    stress, and all of the models presented above have some features in common. Overall, there are few

    examples where different theoretical approaches result in different predictions (although see

    Edwards, 1992 for examples). Rather, the complexity of the stress process means that different

    approaches tend to emphasize some features of the process and place less emphasis on others. For

    example, role theories of stress emphasize the demands imposed by the environment, transactional

    theories emphasize the appraisal of demands, and burnout theories emphasize the responses and

    consequences of demands. Although there is substantial support for components of these stress

    models, there is also evidence that focusing on a limited number of stressors (such as job demands

    and job control) fails to adequately reflect the complexity of the stress-strain relationship (van

    Veldhoven, Taris, de Jonge, & Broersen, 2005).

    To provide a comprehensive comparison and review of stress models we present an

    integrating framework in Figure 1 that summarizes the ongoing transaction between the person and

    the situation. The goal of the overall framework is to provide a broad picture of the relationships

    between more complex sub-components or facets. For example, overall models such as Beehr

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    15/91

    Stress and well-being 15

    (2000) and Cox and MacKay (1981) build on specific facet models of stress (Beehr & Newman,

    1978; Newman & Beehr, 1979).

    Insert Figure 1 about here

    The framework highlights two key processes of work stress: a transactional process linking

    the person and environment, and a dynamic process that unfolds over time. First, transactional

    approaches describe how the individual evaluates and responds to environmental conditions. This

    transaction is a feature of many stress models particularly the socio-cognitive model (Lazarus &

    Folkman, 1984) and the challenge-hindrance model (LePine et al., 2005) that specify how

    environmental features are appraised by individuals; and cybernetic approaches (Edwards &

    Cooper, 1990) where the discrepancy between the individuals preferred state and the perceived

    environment is the main motivational process. Other approaches emphasize the role of the

    environment more directly. Role theory (Kahn & Byosiere, 1992) describes how specific task

    conditions can have an impact on strain outcomes, the demands-control model (Karasek, 1989)

    focuses on interaction of specific work characteristics, and the conservation of resources approach

    (Hobfoll, 1989) emphasizes the way the environment helps build or deplete resources. Despite the

    large role played by the environment in models of stress, Daniels (2006) notes that external features

    such as job characteristics are typically assessed by individual self-reports that make it difficult to

    disentangle environmental conditions from psychological processes such as bias, mood, and social

    interaction.

    Second, the stress process is dynamic because the transaction between the person and

    environment unfolds over time through a process of mutual influence. The figure incorporates this

    dynamic by specifying a short term time cycle of ongoing appraisal, goals, and action within a

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    16/91

    Stress and well-being 16

    longer term cycle of interaction between the individual and the environment. This nesting of

    processes recognizes the hierarchical structure of feedback loops in the stress process (Carver &

    Scheier, 1990). Stress theories typically incorporate propositions about the temporal ordering of

    events, and each major theory described above identifies a primary causal sequence through which

    stressors are translated into strains. There is also some attention to stressors that might have long or

    short term consequences (Beehr & Franz, 1987). However, the dynamics of mutual influence over

    time have proved difficult to specify (Beehr, 2000).

    In the following section, we provide a more detailed review of elements that constitute the

    longer-term and the shorter-term dynamics of the stress process. We then explore the role of time

    for linking short-term and long-term dynamics and conclude this section with a discussion of

    methodological implications for studying the stress process.

    Longer-term dynamics of the stress

    The interaction between the individual and the environment

    process

    We begin reviewing the specific content of Figure 1 by identifying the relatively longer-

    term elements of the person and the environment interaction. We first review studies that inform

    main effects of the environment and individual on strain outcomes, then review issues associated

    with understanding person-environment interactions.

    The environment

    The environment creates demands and provides supports in the stress process. The potential

    range of environmental factors is vast: almost any event or situation that a person encounters could

    be viewed as a source of stress. Most stress models propose that relatively stable job characteristics

    such as job demands and control have an impact on workers health and well-being. Generic role

    stressors and demands have been found to differ significantly across occupations (Sparks & Cooper,

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    17/91

    Stress and well-being 17

    1999). In addition, job characteristics specific to particular occupations can be important predictors

    of stress outcomes. For example, Cooper, Clarke, and Rowbottom (1999) identified occupational

    characteristics within medical specialties that were specifically relevant for anaesthetists.

    Alfredsson and Theorell (1983) developed an assessment of the risks posed by objective job

    characteristics for myocardial infarction in 118 occupational groups.

    In a review of 63 studies, van der Doef and Maes (1999) reported that two-thirds supported

    a relationship between job characteristics and psychological well-being and this conclusion has

    been supported in reviews of longitudinal studies (de Lange et al., 2003). Overall, the design of

    jobs can have a significant impact on a wide range of positive and negative health outcomes

    associated with the stress process (Parker, Turner, & Griffin, 2003).

    In addition to job characteristics, organization design factors, such as the structure and

    climate of the organization, management style, communication, level of consultation and politics,

    influence individuals health and well-being. Indeed, a number of studies have found a more

    negative effect associated with organizational stressors than stressors more intrinsically related to

    the job in occupations including police (Hart, Wearing, & Headey, 1995; Thompson, Kirk-Brown,

    & Brown, 2001), ambulance staff (Glendon & Coles, 2001), and teachers (Hart, 1994). For

    example, Hart et al (1995) reported that police officers reported higher levels of psychological

    distress in relation to organizational hassles (such as administration and paperwork) compared to

    operational hassles (such as dealing with victims of crime or facing physical danger).

    Sparks, Faragher, and Cooper (2001) identified four sources of stress that have become

    increasingly important as employment conditions change: job security; long work hours; control at

    work; and, managerial style. In particular, a lack of job security has been found to have detrimental

    effects on health and well-being in European (Borg, Kristensen, & Burr, 2000; Domenighetti,

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    18/91

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    19/91

    Stress and well-being 19

    The home-work interface is a distinct topic in the stress literature and a chapter in this

    Handbook reviews home-work issues in more detail (see also Vol. 3, Chapter 14, by Hammer and

    Zimmerman, on "Quality of Work Life"). Meta-analyses have explored the antecedents and

    consequences of work-family conflict, generally supporting the proposition that work stressors have

    an impact on the extent to which work interferes with family (e.g., Byron, 2005). Stressors at work

    can also have an impact on the health of the employees family members (Bakker, Demerouti, &

    Dollard, 2008).

    Physical demands (such as exposure to heat, noise, and toxic substances) have received less

    attention than psychological stressors. These demands are sometimes used as a control variable to

    estimate the effects of psychological stressors or excluded from consideration as psychological

    stressors (e.g., Ganster, 2008). The impact of shift work and work schedules, in contrast, has

    received a great deal of attention. Totterdell (2005) provides an extensive review of the impact of

    scheduling on factors such as fatigue, disease, and absence. There is widespread agreement that

    shift work and long work hours significantly increase the risk of sleep disturbance and fatigue and

    also increase the likelihood of some cancers, particularly breast cancer, gastrointestinal disorders

    and cardiovascular disease. The increased risk is quite substantial for both breast cancer (50-60%)

    and cardiovascular disease (40%) (see Totterdell, 2005, for further discussion).

    The role of individual differences

    Stable dispositional factors, such as personality, influence the stress-strain process in a

    number of different ways, including: exposure to stressors; appraisal and reactivity to stressful

    events; consistent use of coping strategies and development of coping style; and, susceptibility to

    stress outcomes (Bolger & Zuckerman, 1995; Grant & Langan-Fox, 2006). For example, exposure

    to stressors appears to occur more frequently for individuals high in neuroticism because they are

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    20/91

    Stress and well-being 20

    more likely to view a situation as threatening and to use maladaptive coping techniques

    (Hemenover, 2003; McCrae & Costa, 1986). In contrast, those high in extraversion and

    conscientiousness are more likely to view potential stressors as a challenge and to adopt problem-

    solving coping strategies (Watson & Hubbard, 1996). Research on the interactive effects of

    personality traits (Grant & Langan-Fox, 2006) highlighted the role of combinations of neuroticism,

    extraversion and conscientiousness specifically that combinations of high neuroticism-high

    conscientiousness predicted higher stressor exposure and low neuroticism-high extraversion and/or

    high conscientiousness predicted lower stressor exposure. In relation to coping, high extraversion-

    high conscientiousness generally predicted higher problem-focused coping and high neuroticism-

    low conscientiousness generally predicted lower problem-focused coping. Overall, personality type

    explained approximately 8% of the variance in stressor exposure and 11% of the variance in coping.

    Low strain was consistently associated with low neuroticism-high extraversion-high

    conscientiousness; however, high strain was differentially related to personality type, depending on

    the outcome variable (Grant & Langan-Fox, 2006). Physical ill-health was most associated with

    impulsive personality types (high extraversion-high neuroticism-low conscientiousness), whereas

    job dissatisfaction was most associated with individuals low on all three personality dimensions.

    Low agreeableness in combination with high neuroticism was related to job dissatisfaction, but not

    physical ill-health; this finding suggests an affective, rather than a physiological, response is typical

    for such individuals.

    Kahn and Byosiere (1992) argued that locus of control influences how individuals cope with

    stress; those with an internal locus of control will engage more actively to cope with stress, and so

    display better health outcomes. An internal locus of control is associated with the belief that

    outcomes can be controlled and so individuals are more likely to adopt a proactive approach to

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    21/91

    Stress and well-being 21

    dealing with a negative work environment. Individuals with an internal locus of control are more

    likely to prevent the development of stressful conditions and to actively seek out ways of managing

    negative situations when they occur. Overall, internals are more likely to perceive work stressors as

    manageable and less threatening. For example, Daniels and Guppy (1994) found that job control

    moderated the stress-strain relationship, but only for individuals with internal locus of control. Ng,

    Sorensen, and Eby (2006) found that internal locus of control was strongly associated with well-

    being, including mental well- being ( = .36), physical health ( = .31) and burnout ( = -.27),

    predominately through cognitive processes, self-evaluation, motivation, and coping strategies.

    Extending locus of control, the concept of core self-evaluation (CSE) reflects an individuals

    overall perception of self-worth and is comprised of: locus of control, plus generalized self-efficacy,

    and neuroticism (Judge, Erez, Bono, & Thoresen, 2002; Judge, Locke, & Durham, 1997). CSE can

    influence the type of jobs that individuals select, the work environments that they experience and

    their perceptions of the environment (Dormann, Fay, Zapf, & Frese, 2006). Feeling able to control

    ones job situation is a central part of CSE and an important aspect of well-being; feeling in control

    will lead to satisfaction regardless of whether that control is actually exercised. Therefore, an

    internal locus of control can be more important for job satisfaction than available control. In some

    situations, an internal locus of control might lead to less engagement with the external environment

    to protect mental resources that otherwise would be depleted (Schnpflug, 1983). There are

    circumstances, however, where an internal locus of control may have adverse effects, for example,

    an internal locus of control might lead one to expend effort to manage a situation that cannot be

    controlled, thus depleting resources (Muraven & Baumeister, 2000).

    A wide variety of additional factors have been studied as individual antecedents in the stress

    process, or as moderators of the stress process, including negative affectivity, hardiness, Type A

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    22/91

    Stress and well-being 22

    personality, and optimism (Cooper et al., 2001). We consider individual factors as moderators in

    more detail in the next section.

    Interactions between the individual and the environment

    A defining feature of the stress process is the way individual characteristics interact with

    features of the environment. In this section we review various approaches to describing and

    analyzing this interaction from the perspective of more stable person and environment features. The

    socio-cognitive approach (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) and the demands-control model of stress

    (Karasek & Theorell, 1990) include person-environment interactions as a central part of each

    theory. Other approaches incorporate this interaction as an implicit link between core constructs

    such as person-environment fit (Edwards & Cooper, 1990) and homeostasis (Hobfoll, 1989).

    One complexity associated with assessing an interaction between individuals and

    environments is the sheer number of interactions that can be considered. All environmental

    demands and resources can potentially interact with all individual differences. A further

    methodological complexity is the form of the interaction. In its simplest form, an interaction can be

    represented by a multiplicative relationship between two variables so that the effect of one variable

    depends on the level of the second variable. Extensive research has assessed this interaction using

    concepts of moderation, contingency, and buffering. We reviewed some of these issues in the

    discussion of social support above. The DC model incorporates interaction as part of its central

    propositions and we review this model next in more detail together with other features of the

    person-environment interaction.

    Many different forms of interaction between demands and control have been tested

    empirically (Van Vegchel, De Jonge, & Landsbergis, 2005). Karasek (1989) proposed that the

    interaction could simply reflect the joint effect of demands and control on strain via an additive

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    23/91

    Stress and well-being 23

    effect, given that multiplicative terms are difficult to detect in practical situations. Van Vegchel et

    al. (2005) found that both multiplicative and ratio interaction terms were significant predictors of

    strain; both supported the combination of high demands, low control as high strain, but differed on

    other conditions. The results were consistent with Warr's (1994) suggestion that relationships

    between environmental characteristics, such as job demands, and well-being are non-linear.

    The existence of non-linear relationships is problematic for testing interactional effects.

    Inconsistencies in the reporting of interaction effects may also be partially explained by the

    matching hypothesis (de Jonge & Dormann, 2006), whereby interaction effects are more likely to

    be demonstrated when there is a match between the type of stressor, resource and strain. de Jonge

    and Dormann (2006) found that interactions were more likely to be supported when there was a

    triple match between the stressor, resource and strain. For example, physical resources and physical

    stressors were more likely to interact in the prediction of physical outcomes compared to other

    outcomes.

    A review conducted by de Lange et al. (2003) included only longitudinal studies that met a

    high quality standard. They found that of 19 studies reviewed, 8 reported support for the joint

    effects of job demands, job control or social support on health outcomes over time.

    The basic interaction between demands and control has been extended in a variety of ways.

    Three-way interactions have been supported for self-efficacy (Salanova et al., 2002; Schaubroeck &

    Merritt, 1997), active coping (de Rijk, Le Blanc, Schaufeli, & De Jonge, 1998), proactive

    personality (Cunningham & De La Rosa, 2008; Parker & Sprigg, 1999), information (Jimmieson &

    Terry, 1999), and locus of control (Meier et al., 2008). Although the theoretical basis for individual

    moderators is elaborated in these studies, an overall framework for selecting substantively different

    types of moderators has not been established. It is possible failures to demonstrate interactions

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    24/91

    Stress and well-being 24

    between demands and control result from a lack of methodological and theoretical sophistication

    (Parker et al., 2003). For example, significant interactions have been found when the level of

    analysis at which constructs are theorized is matched to the measurement strategies and analyses

    (Bliese & Castro, 2000; Morrison, Payne, & Wall, 2003).

    In addition to the DC model, other approaches to the stress process either test an interaction

    between the person and the environment directly or describe some form of mutual and

    interdependent relationship. The concepts of fit and discrepancy are particularly important for

    conveying this interdependence in conservation of resources, P-E fit and cybernetic models of

    stress.

    Cummings and Cooper (1979) provide a comprehensive portrayal of the person-

    environment interaction in terms of time, information, and feedback. An underlying feature of

    interaction arguments is a drive toward homeostasis, where the individual and environment achieve

    a state of equilibrium (Hobfoll, 1989). From this perspective, a stressor is a force disrupting a

    system beyond its range of stability, and the stress process involves the actions that achieve a new

    state of equilibrium.

    It is difficult to assess the unique importance of interactions in the stress process

    independent of main effects. Interactions are often reported in relation to multiple main effects, null

    effects are often observed, and the proportion of variance explained by interactions is frequently

    small. Design, measurement, and statistical artifacts can also bias the size of interaction effects

    downwards (Aguinis, Beaty, Boik, & Pierce, 2005). Overall, effect sizes for interactions explaining

    between 2% and 5% of strain outcomes appear to have a significant and meaningful impact in the

    stress process.

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    25/91

    Stress and well-being 25

    Longer-term outcomes of the stress

    We now look at the final element of the longer-term time cycle in the Figure, the outcomes

    of the stress process. Individuals can vary greatly in their response to stressors. For example, a

    heavy workload might cause psychological effects in one person, making them anxious, whilst

    another might experience physiological symptoms, such as headaches. There are also differences of

    the impact of some stressors on health for men and women. For example, long work hours have a

    negative effect on health (Sparks et al., 2001) but the risk is greater for women (Starrin, Larsson,

    Brenner, Levi, & Petterson, 1990).

    process

    Various categorizations of longer-term stress outcomes have been proposed but are

    generally grouped as psychological, physiological, or behavioral outcomes (Kahn & Byosiere,

    1992). Most research has focused on the psychological outcomes of the stress process and the

    strongest empirical effects of stressors have been observed in relation to psychological outcomes.

    This stronger effect might be inflated because of common method bias resulting from similar

    measurement for stressors and strains (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003). It is

    possible that experience of one strain can increase vulnerability to other types of strain. For

    example, the long-term effects of job dissatisfaction associated with role ambiguity might be

    physical ill-health. This causal process would lead to a weaker relationship between stressors and

    physical outcomes compared to psychological outcomes.

    We now explore the negative outcomes of the stress process and integrate some more

    positive features of health and well-being. We review the different categories of outcome in terms

    of psychological strain and well-being, physical health, and behavioral outcomes.

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    26/91

    Stress and well-being 26

    Psychological strain and well-being

    Kahn and Byosiere (1992) identified over 40 different measures of psychological strain,

    although they noted considerable conceptual overlap among categories. Job dissatisfaction was the

    most commonly used measure of strain, and other common measures were anxiety, depression, and

    generalized strain. Most research reviews indicate that psychological strains, such as anxiety and

    depression, are strong correlates of work-related stressors (Jackson & Schuler, 1985; Jex & Beehr,

    1991; Kahn & Byosiere, 1992). Viswesvaran et al (1999) estimated a correlation of .43 between the

    general construct of work stressors and the general construct of strain.

    In the DC model, the benefits of control and support in buffering against the negative health

    effects of high demands, via coping, accumulate over time. Control and support have their buffering

    effect by facilitating problem-solving. Over time, consistent use of coping mechanisms leads to a

    coping style, a cross-situational consistency in coping. Daniels, Beesley, Cheyne, and Wimalasiri

    (2008) found that consistent use of problem-solving coping, enacted through support and control,

    led to fewer risky decisions, but could have detrimental effects in the short term. These findings

    would support the adjustment model, where strain increases proportionately with increases in the

    intensity of the stressor until the individual adjusts to the stressor (Frese and Zapf, 1988).

    Although it is clear that the stress process is associated with a wide range of psychological

    strains, there has often been limited theoretical justification for the specific strains included in a

    particular study (Cooper et al., 2001). The study of burnout is an exception, with the three

    dimensions of exhaustion, depersonalization, and lower efficacy derived from theoretical

    propositions about the aetiology of symptoms.

    Further progress toward a more theoretical description of stress outcomes has been made by

    integrating the broader construct of well-being. The typical conceptualization of stress covers only a

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    27/91

    Stress and well-being 27

    part of the broader domain of well-being (Warr, 2005). Mental health should not be considered

    merely as the absence of stress symptoms, but in terms of the presence of active mental health

    (Parker et al., 2003). Such a state would have positive indicators, such as competence, mastery,

    aspiration and desire for autonomy (Warr, 1994). These indicators encompass the motivational and

    behavioral dimensions of well-being as well as its affective nature.

    Ryff & Keyes, (1995) developed a six-factor model of wellness: autonomy, environmental

    mastery, personal growth, positive relations with others, purpose in life, and self-acceptance. These

    aspects of well-being are defined as: a sense of self-determination and the ability to resist social

    pressures to think and act in certain ways (autonomy); the capacity to effectively manage ones life

    and the surrounding world (environmental mastery); the sense of continued growth and

    development as a person as well as openness to new experiences (personal growth); positive regard

    for other people, such as a genuine concern about the welfare of others (positive relations with

    others); the belief that ones life is purposeful and meaningful and that one has something to live for

    (purpose in life); and a positive evaluation of oneself and ones past life (self-acceptance). Warr

    (1994) emphasizes the importance not only of these separate dimensions, but also of integrated

    functioning that represents the person as a whole. The importance of achieving such a balance is

    reflected in the definition of happiness as the balance between positive and negative affect

    (Bradburn, 1969).

    Models of burnout have also been extended to include engagement dimensions that

    represent positive poles of the burnout dimension (Maslach & Leiter, 2008). Positive mental well-

    being has been related to work engagement, where engaged workers are defined as those who

    have a sense of energetic and effective connection with their work activities and [they] see

    themselves as able to deal well with the demands of their job (Schaufeli, Taris, & van Rhenen,

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    28/91

    Stress and well-being 28

    2008, p. 176). It comprises three aspects: high levels of energy and mental resilience (vigor); a

    sense of significance, enthusiasm, inspiration, pride and challenge (dedication); and, being fully

    engrossed in work (absorption). The latter element of absorption has also been associated with

    workaholism; however, workaholism has been linked with poor well-being (Schaufeli et al., 2008;

    Taris, Schaufeli, & Verhoeven, 2005). There is moderate support for a positive relationship

    between work engagement and good mental health: Schaufeli & Bakker (2004) found that

    emotional exhaustion was negatively correlated with vigor ( r = -.40) and dedication ( r = -.28), but

    had little relationship with absorption ( r = -.07). The association with well-being may be explained

    by engaged individuals response to highly demanding, but highly resourced jobs. Engaged

    workers become engrossed in their work, but perceive work as fun. In contrast, workaholics tend

    to work excessively hard in poorly resourced jobs, i.e., those that are highly demanding, but have

    poor support (Demerouti, Bakker, de Jonge, Janssen, & Schaufeli, 2001). Thus, motivational

    aspects differ: workaholics are driven to work hard, whilst engaged workers are more intrinsically

    motivated. However, managing highly demanding jobs over time, even for engaged workers, may

    eventually lead to burnout (Schaufeli et al., 2008).

    Physical health

    Physiological outcomes are studied less often than other outcomes in stress research.

    However, there is a substantial body of evidence that the stress process, over time, can have a

    significant negative effect on workers physical health and immunity (van der Doef & Maes, 1999;

    Segerstrom & Miller, 2004). Whilst acute and time-limited stressors lead to an adaptive response,

    chronic stressors result in decreased potential adaptiveness of the immune system over time

    (Segerstrom & Miller, 2004). Chandola et al. (2006) assessed data from over 4000 individuals on

    four occasions to link work stressors to health outcomes, including a metabolic syndrome

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    29/91

    Stress and well-being 29

    associated with increased risk of heart disease and Type 2 diabetes. The study found that chronic

    work stress substantially increased the likelihood of developing the metabolic syndrome: men were

    nearly twice as likely and women over five times more likely to have the metabolic syndrome,

    compared to those with no exposure to work stress. Heart rate, blood pressure and catecholamines

    have been assessed most often as indicators of physiological functioning. Jones et al. (2007) argue

    that one mechanism linking job strain to health is through a range of health behaviors. Those most

    studied are alcohol and smoking (e.g., Kouvonen, Kivimaki, Cox, Cox, & Vahtera, 2005). Other

    studies have shown that job strain is associated with decreased healthy eating or increased body

    mass index (Hellerstedt & Jeffery, 1997; Kouvonen et al., 2005; Lallukka et al., 2004; Tsutsumi et

    al., 2003).

    Studies investigating health outcomes have not reported consistent findings, although Zapf

    et al. (1996) argue that the effect size of the stressor-strain relationship is limited, due to the large

    number of potential causative factors influencing health and well-being (including environment,

    leisure and family stressors, social class, personality, and health behaviors). Segerstrom (2007)

    noted that the most psychologically healthy individuals sometimes have the least robust immune

    system, further complicating a straightforward link between long-term stressors and health; for

    example, unnecessary energy allocated to the immune system makes less energy available for other

    systems. When resources are threatened, it could be adaptive for organisms to direct energy away

    from the immune system and toward protecting or restoring resources (Segerstrom, 2007).

    Work stress has been associated with a range of musculoskeletal diseases and complaints

    (Bongers, de Winter, Kompier, & Hildebrandt, 1993; Carayon, Smith, & Haims, 1999).

    Biomechanical processes explain some of the reasons that stressors such as workload might lead to

    musculoskeletal complaints. However, psychological processes also play a part in shaping the

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    30/91

    Stress and well-being 30

    experience of these factors. Authors have proposed that psychological affective strains might

    mediate the link between stressors and musculoskeletal complaints (e.g., Kjellberg & Wadman,

    2007). The prevalence of office based work and increasing computer use mean that these problems

    are likely to increase. Sprigg, Stride, Wall, Holman, and Smith (2007) found that the impact of

    workload on musculoskeletal disorders for call centre employees was partially mediated by the

    psychological experiences of anxiety and depression.

    Behavioral outcomes

    Indirectly, a broad domain of behavioral responses has been linked to the stress process. Jex

    and Beehr (1991) divide behaviors into those with significance for the organization, including job

    performance, turnover and absenteeism; and those with significance to the individual, including use

    of alcohol, smoking, substance use and destructive behaviors. Behaviors can be associated with

    stressors via a direct relationship (e.g., work overload causes absence from work) or mediated by an

    affective state (e.g., work overload causes anxiety, which in turn causes absenteeism).

    The experience of transient positive moods has also been linked to enhanced outcomes (Erez

    & Isen, 2002; Totterdell, 2000). Tsai, Chen, and Liu (2007) found that positive mood states

    predicted task performance. Furthermore, the study showed that positive moods have a lasting

    effect on task performance measured three weeks later. This effect may be facilitated through

    interpersonal processes, such as helping coworkers whereby an employees positive mood results

    in greater engagement in helping coworkers motivated by desire to maintain his or her positive

    mood. Positive moods can be contagious as individuals helping behavior encourages positive

    moods in coworkers. Walter and Bruch (2008) argue that although most research has considered

    positive moods and emotion at the individual level, they may be viewed as collective constructs

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    31/91

    Stress and well-being 31

    operating at a group level. George (1990) showed that positive group affect is related to

    absenteeism and individual well-being.

    Proactive work behavior has also been linked to enhanced well-being (Crant, 2000); this

    behavior is defined as taking initiative in improving current circumstances; it involves challenging

    the status quo rather than passively adapting present conditions (p.436). Examples of proactive

    behavior include generating and implementing new ideas and taking the initiative in problem-

    solving. Such behavior involves a conscious decision-making process and is affected by the

    individuals level of self-efficacy (need to feel in control of a situation) and felt responsibility for

    change (motivated to approach initiative and willing to accept responsibility). Parker, Williams,

    and Turner (2006) showed that self-efficacy played a significant role in proactive behavior,

    highlighting the importance of building employees perceptions of their own capability as a means

    of enhancing employee proactivity, and subsequently their well-being.

    Response to stressors can include a range of work withdrawal behaviors (R. L. Kahn &

    Byosiere, 1992). Darr and Johns (2008) reviewed 153 studies and found positive but small

    connections among work strain, illness, and absenteeism, challenging the popular workplace

    estimates that introduced this chapter. They estimated that work strain accounts for between six and

    29% of the variance in absenteeism. In comparing whether absence was primarily a response to a

    noxious workplace or a function of illness, they concluded with tentative support for a partial illness

    explanation.

    Workplace safety is another potentially important outcome of the stress process. Stress is

    considered to be responsible for 60-80% of all workplace accidents (Cooper, Liukkonen, &

    Cartwright, 1996), yet there is a lack of research examining accidents as an outcome of

    occupational stress. Occupational stress is related to increased accident risk in a variety of working

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    32/91

    Stress and well-being 32

    environments. For example, job stress was found to be a small but significant predictor of work

    accidents ( = .13, p

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    33/91

    Stress and well-being 33

    Appraisal Processes

    First, the appraisal process involves the perception of environmental stimuli. Different

    approaches to stress propose categorization processes that describe how features of the environment

    are encoded by individuals. In the appraisal process, the environment becomes salient due to events

    that occur in relation to histories and propensities of the individual. Many approaches to stress

    emphasize appraisal although the conceptualization and measurement of ongoing appraisals is

    problematic (Troup & Dewe, 2002).

    The socio-cognitive approach places particular emphasis on appraisal of the environment

    (e.g., Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Primary appraisal involves the recognition of a potential stressor

    and initial evaluation of whether the stressor presents a loss, threat, or challenge. Although a wide

    range of factors can be appraised as stressors, the categorization of a situation as threatening and

    uncertain is particularly important.

    The socio-cognitive approach to appraisal has been further refined through hindrance-

    challenge models of stress (Podsakoff, LePine, & LePine, 2007). The central appraisal process in

    this approach is whether a potential stressor is categorized as a challenge or a hindrance. The nature

    of the stressor is an important determinant of the categorization. As noted above, many individual

    and situational characteristics combine to determine how the environment is categorized, although

    the hindrance-challenge approach allows for intrinsic features of environmental demand.

    The DC model involves initial appraisal of demands and resources to meet the demands and

    implies strain related outcomes are derived when demands exceed resources. Cybernetic models

    incorporate appraisal directly with perceptions of discrepancy. Individuals appraise the environment

    with reference to internal standards and it is these appraisals that motivate subsequent responses.

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    34/91

    Stress and well-being 34

    Surprisingly little research has elaborated the appraisal process by integrating development

    in social cognition and cognitive psychology. An exception is provided by Daniels, Harris, and

    Briner (2004) who identify automatic and controlled information processing pathways through

    which the perception of an event is encoded. Recent biological research investigates the relationship

    between the appraisal process and cortical activity. For example, appraisal of a stressor with low

    controllability is associated with higher cerebral blood flow in the orbitofrontal and prefrontral

    cortices, which in turn were related to peripheral autonomic and immune activities (Ohira et al.,

    2008).

    In summary, most major approaches to stress incorporate some form of cognitive appraisal

    involving an evaluation and categorization of environmental features. Central to this evaluation is

    the categorization and comparison of resources and demands.

    Goal processes

    Next, we consider how appraisal of the environment engages the individuals goal systems.

    Goals are cognitive representations of desired or undesired states (Austin & Vancouver, 1996).

    Goals are distinct from the evaluation of referents and discrepancies described in the appraisal

    process because they guide the selection and persistence of further action. Goal processes are

    generally considered controlled and intentional, though once established they can be activated

    quickly and automatically (Bargh & Ferguson, 2000).

    Cybernetic models of stress include goals as a central mechanism (Cummings & Cooper,

    1979; Edwards, 1992; Miller, 1965). The goal system comprises a negative feedback loop which

    acts to minimize discrepancies between environmental characteristics and relevant reference criteria

    (Cummings & Cooper, 1979; Edwards, 1992). The cybernetic approach has been criticized for

    failing to capture important features of human goal systems such as forethought and the deliberate

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    35/91

    Stress and well-being 35

    creation of discrepancies to motivate action (Locke, 1991, 1994). In response, Edwards (1998)

    argued that the hierarchical arrangement of feedback loops explains how effort to resolve

    discrepancies at lower levels also helps to resolve discrepancies at higher levels. This system

    explains the link between short term discrepancies and the striving to longer term value-based

    goals. In effect the discrepancy perceived in the appraisal processes, is incorporated into the goals

    system of the hierarchical set of discrepancies. Setting standards might also be considered here, as

    this activity involves forethought (Edwards, 1998).

    Both cybernetic and socio-cognitive theories of stress describe how perceptions of threat

    and discrepancy engage goal systems that motivate further action. The socio-cognitive theory of

    stress incorporates aspects of this goal process in terms of secondary appraisal. The evaluation of

    response options and resources for coping occurs as part of this appraisal. Daniels et al. (2004)

    described a cognitive process through which a mental model of work events is built dynamically to

    identify and evaluate progress toward goals.

    A further elaboration of the goal process is contained in the distinction between approach

    and avoidance goals (Elliot, 2006). Based on the dichotomy between pleasure-pain, the approach

    and avoidance distinction has been part of human philosophy for hundreds of years, and theories of

    stress incorporate the distinction in various ways. For example, primary appraisal in the socio-

    cognitive model includes categorization of an event as harmful, beneficial, or neutral (Lazarus &

    Folkman, 1984). In recent years, a more fine-grained development of approach and avoidance

    motivations has been developed and integrated with the study of affect, cognition, and behavior

    (Carver, 2006; Elliot & Thrash, 2002; Higgins, 1997). Approach motivation is the direction of

    behavior toward positive stimuli whereas avoidance motivation is behavior directed away from

    negative stimuli (Elliot, 2006). Goals provide cognitive representation of an outcome that guides the

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    36/91

    Stress and well-being 36

    individual toward or away from that outcome (Elliot & Fryer, 2007). This work is important for

    extending the study of goals in the stress process in a number of ways.

    First, stress research through its focus on negative outcomes is intrinsically concerned with

    avoidance goals. However, most stress models incorporate negative feedback or discrepancy

    reducing loops (Edwards, 1998). In contrast, the motivational process underlying avoidance

    involves discrepancy enlarging loops. Discrepancy enlarging loops differ from discrepancy

    reducing loops because the distancing process has no specific direction. That is, a discrepancy

    reducing loop helps individuals to home in on a specific goal whereas a discrepancy enlarging loop

    simply leads away from the anti-goal. The discrepancy enlarging goal is therefore less directive

    (Carver, 2006). In most systems, the enlarging process eventually leads to a positive incentive that

    then engages approach motivation. For example, actively avoiding participation in a group with a

    negative work climate might bring an individual in contact with a group where the work climate is

    positive. Approach goals for the second work group might then guide behavior according to a

    discrepancy reduction loop.

    Stable individual differences can influence whether approach or avoidance goals are

    adopted. For example, Elliot and Thrash (2002, p. 806) described stable temperaments to adopt

    either approach or avoidance goals based on networks of biological sensitivities that are

    responsible for immediate affective, cognitive, and behavioral propensities in response to

    encountered or imagined stimuli. Carver and White (1994) identified differential sensitivity among

    individuals to the experience of punishment and rewards. Using Gray's (1994) distinction between

    behavioral activation system (BAS) and behavioral inhibition systems (BIS) they found that some

    individuals were more sensitive.

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    37/91

    Stress and well-being 37

    In general, the BIS system serves to protect the individuals by inhibiting behavior that could

    result in aversive consequences (Watson et al., 1999). As yet, few studies have applied this

    distinction to stress and the work environment. One example is provided by van der Linden, Taris,

    Beckers, & Kindt (2007) who found that punishment sensitivity interacted with job characteristics

    to predict fatigue at work, where high BIS-individuals with low job control experienced higher

    levels of fatigue ( = -.13, p

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    38/91

    Stress and well-being 38

    example, physiological studies of stress often view increased heart rate as a response to threat with

    long term consequences for health, but not as part of the ongoing cycle of appraisal and goals.

    Reactive responses have a short life span and should be distinguished from outcomes which

    develop over a longer period of time. However, reactive responses may accumulate over time to

    lead to longer-term health outcomes. Second, we view more active responses such as coping

    behavior that involves directly changing the elements of the dynamic process in either a positive or

    negative way.

    Reactive response

    Affective and physiological outcomes are the most commonly studied reactive responses in

    the stress process. Negative affect is a fundamental response to discrepancy (Edwards, 1998),

    mismatch (French, Caplan, & Harrison, 1982), and imbalance (Siegrist, 1996). Although this

    negative affect provides the broad motivational basis for addressing this lack of fit, its role is non-

    specific. The quality of affect is determined not only by the size of the gap between actual and

    desired states, but by the rate of progress towards goals (Carver & Scheier, 1990).

    Different affective responses have been proposed for the pursuit of approach or avoidance

    goals (Carver, 2006). Carver and White (1994) found differences in individual sensitivity to

    activation or inhibition systems were related differentially to the experience of emotion in situations

    of threat or reward. For example, people higher in BIS sensitivity showed higher levels of anxiety

    in response to threat.

    Short term physiological responses within the stress process encompass changes in the

    endocrine, immune, cardiovascular, and musculoskeletal system. In the work context, most interest

    in physiological systems has focused on how that activation or suppression of these systems leads to

    long term negative outcomes such as disease or injury (see next section). Sonnentag and Fritz

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    39/91

    Stress and well-being 39

    (2006) conducted an extensive review of research linking acute and chronic stressors to

    catecholamines (adrenaline and noradrenaline) and cortisol. Acute stressors were most commonly

    studied in laboratory settings and were clearly related to an increase in adrenaline levels. Tasks that

    posed social-evaluative threat and low controllability elicited strongest cortisol response (Dickerson

    & Kemeny, 2004). Noradrenaline was more strongly associated with aversive experiences and

    demanding tasks. In field studies, workload and control were the most common factors investigated

    but results across studies were more variable. In general, more evidence was found for the impact of

    acute versus chronic stressors on endocrine responses (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2006).

    Physiological responses to work have most often been studied as a means to understand the

    negative consequences of stressors. However, Heaphy and Dutton (2008) recently called for the

    positive physiological consequences of work to receive more attention and be better integrated

    within this stream of research.

    Active response

    Active responses are viewed as actions that change the unfolding pattern of the dynamic

    stress process. Here, the concept of coping with stressors is most relevant. Coping is one of the

    most common and important responses studied in the stress process (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).

    Coping serves four major functions: approach, avoidance, emotional regulation and reappraisal

    (Ferguson & Cox, 1997), although a widely accepted definition of the content and function of

    coping responses has proved elusive. However, a distinction between problem focused coping that

    seeks to solve the demands of a stressor, emotion focused coping that helps the individual to feel

    better about the stressor, and appraisal focused coping that attempts to redefine the stressor, has

    been used widely to capture different goals of coping (Cooper et al., 2001).

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    40/91

    Stress and well-being 40

    Although many studies treat coping as a dispositional response, the concept also informs the

    more dynamic aspects of the link between the individual and the environment (Folkman & Lazarus,

    1988). From a cybernetic perspective, coping behavior is a process of feedback control (Carver &

    Scheier, 1990) and discrepancies increase the intensity of coping efforts (Edwards, 1998). Coping is

    complex because it is conceived as both a cause and a consequence of the stress process (Kinicki &

    Latack, 1990).

    Coping behaviors are initiated as a result of primary and secondary appraisal (Lazarus &

    Folkman, 1984). Behavioral responses are important because they involve actions that can directly

    influence and change the environment. For example, seeking feedback aids problem solving. On the

    other hand, emotion focused responses, such as emotional discharge, have generally been

    considered escapist strategies that are not beneficial for the individual in the long term. Folkman

    and Moskowitz (2004) identified three types of coping strategy that evoked positive emotions:

    positive reappraisal; problem-focused coping; and imbuing ordinary events with positive meaning.

    Coping strategies that evoke positive emotions, such as humor, may be particularly effective in

    coping with stressful situations that generate negative emotions, such as anxiety and depression.

    Those individuals who are more aware of their own emotions may be better able to use such coping

    mechanisms.

    Affective response can actively influence the dynamic stress process by directly influencing

    the categorization process. Daniels et al.'s (2004) model of affect and well-being proposed that

    affect influences the cognitive processes by which events are categorized and the mental models

    that are constructed to create and pursue goals. Broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 1998)

    accounts for the effects of positive emotions in broadening peoples momentary thought-action

    repertoires (in contrast to negative emotions which tend to have the opposite effect). The theory

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    41/91

    Stress and well-being 41

    states that discrete positive emotionsshare the ability to broaden peoples momentary thought-

    action repertoires and build their enduring personal resources (Fredrickson, 2001, p.219). The

    theory is based on the adaptive role of emotions as triggers for a specific set of behaviors; for

    example, experiencing fear leads individuals to make their escape. Most specific actions, however,

    are linked to negative emotions; whereas positive emotions lead not to specific actions, but

    encourage a broader range of approach behaviors. It is argued that the experience of positive

    emotions broadens the scope of attention, cognition and action and leads to the development of

    physical, intellectual and social resources over time (Fredrickson, 2001). The building of personal

    resources helps individuals to develop psychological resilience when faced with potential stressors

    in the future. Furthermore, the enhancement of psychological resilience can lead to improved well-

    being over time creating the possibility for an ongoing upwards spiral.

    The role of time in the stress

    To understand the dynamic features of psychological processes it is necessary to identify

    stability and change in constructs, allow for multiple patterns of change over time, and distinguish

    recurrent versus ongoing phenomena (Roe, 2008). Neufeld (1999) differentiated five types of time-

    dependent systems that researchers have used to describe the stress process: transactional, dynamic,

    process like, adaptational, and recursive. Transactional systems are represented by a network of

    variables that are closely connected and interdependent, for example reciprocal determinism

    (Bandura, 1978); dynamic systems are characterized by variables that interact with each other over

    time (e.g., McGrath & Beehr, 1990; Roe, 2008); process like systems are those where the evolution

    of variables over time depends on prior continuous change and so remains in a state of flux (e.g.,

    Edwards, 1992); adaptational systems are those in which variables respond and adapt to changes in

    each other (e.g., Lazarus & Folkman, 1984); finally, recursive systems reflect a dynamic state of

    process

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    42/91

    Stress and well-being 42

    interaction between variables that form a predictable pattern, for example, limit cycles which

    reflect a stable oscillation, within a given period and amplitude (Neufeld, 1999). Although time has

    always played a role in models of stress, few approaches provide such a detailed distinction

    between time frames (Beehr, 2000).

    Our framework incorporates time by differentiating a longer-time cycle within which is

    nested a shorter-term cycle. The longer-term processes involve changes in fairly stable individual

    differences (e.g., personality) and environmental factors (e.g., task structures). The short-term cycle

    denotes more variable fluctuations such as the effect of daily work patterns on mood changes

    (DeLongis, Folkman, & Lazarus, 1988). In reality, the time frame for the stress process ranges on a

    continuum from moment-to-moment appraisals of the environment and coping responses (e.g.,

    Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007) to the long term effect of chronic stressors such as heart disease which

    might be expressed over periods of years (e.g., Chandola, Brunner, & Marmot, 2006). A review of

    45 longitudinal studies identified time lags ranging between 28 days and 12 years with

    approximately two thirds providing a theoretical rationale for the particular time lag (de Lange et

    al., 2003).

    Our framework also indicates a cyclical process whereby strain outcomes have an influence

    on stressors over both the short term and longer term. For example, an anxiety response in the work

    place can lead to a subsequent experience of stressors by contributing to a more fearful

    environment. This form of reciprocal determinism is fundamental to social cognitive processes

    (Bandura, 1978) but difficult to establish empirically (Lance, Lautenschlager, Sloan, & Varca,

    1989). Kohn and Schooler (1978) conducted a 10-year longitudinal study of the reciprocal effects of

    work complexity and intellectual flexibility and demonstrated a pattern of reciprocal relationship

    between work and personal characteristics. Frese, Garst, and Fay (2007) identified a reciprocal

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    43/91

    Stress and well-being 43

    relationship between personal initiative and work characteristics as a positive feedback loop

    occurring over a four-year time lag. It seems likely that this reciprocal influence is weaker than the

    alternative path from stressors to strains (de Lange et al., 2003). There is fairly strong evidence

    from longitudinal studies that work characteristics can influence stress-related outcomes, and some

    high quality longitudinal studies investigating reciprocal relationships have failed to identify reverse

    causation (Carayon, 1992, 1993).

    The process through which short-term responses shape long term outcomes raises both

    theoretical and methodological hurdles. Stress theories are generally better suited to explaining how

    short-term dynamics are shaped and constrained by the more stable context of person and situation

    characteristics (Griffin, 1997). The mechanism through which short-term dynamics shape long-term

    dynamics is likely to involve not only the translation of short-term responses into long term health

    outcomes, but also result in change for the person and the situation. Frese and Zapf (1988)

    described five types of stress exposure models through which short-term dynamics might be

    expressed in terms of long term strain outcomes. First, a stress reaction model describes the case

    where strain increases proportionately with increases in the intensity of the stressors. This case

    describes the implicit, though largely untested assumption, of most stress models that higher

    appraisals of stress over time lead to greater strain outcomes.

    Second, an accumulation model proposes that accumulation of stressors might only appear

    as strains after a certain breaking point and might not disappear when stressors are removed.

    Jones, Conner, McMillan, and Ferguson (2007) found that daily fluctuations in job demands and

    mood were associated with health-related behaviors such as smoking and caffeine intake which

    might result in the later onset of illness.

  • 7/30/2019 Well l Being

    44/91

    Stress and well-being 44

    Third, a dynamic accumulation model where a person is weakened by exposure to stressors

    so that an inner dynamic continues to increase strain even after the stressors are removed.

    Brosschot, Pieper, and Thayer (2005) describe a process of perseverative cognition where negative

    thoughts and emotions initiated by environmental conditions but prolonged by short-term dynamics

    such as worry, rumination, and anticipatory stress.

    Fourth an adjustment model which begins as with the stress reaction model but only until

    the person adjusts to the


Recommended