+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Effects of Methylphenidate on Attention Deficits After Traumatic Brain Injury

Effects of Methylphenidate on Attention Deficits After Traumatic Brain Injury

Date post: 12-Nov-2023
Category:
Upload: independent
View: 0 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
21
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/8539310 Effects of Methylphenidate on Attention Deficits After Traumatic Brain Injury ARTICLE in AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL MEDICINE & REHABILITATION · JUNE 2004 Impact Factor: 2.2 · DOI: 10.1097/01.PHM.0000128789.75375.D3 · Source: PubMed CITATIONS 129 READS 67 7 AUTHORS, INCLUDING: Patricia Neff 4 PUBLICATIONS 237 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Marcia Polansky Drexel University 142 PUBLICATIONS 5,955 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE H. Branch Coslett University of Pennsylvania 226 PUBLICATIONS 8,449 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Available from: H. Branch Coslett Retrieved on: 03 February 2016
Transcript

Seediscussions,stats,andauthorprofilesforthispublicationat:https://www.researchgate.net/publication/8539310

EffectsofMethylphenidateonAttentionDeficitsAfterTraumaticBrainInjury

ARTICLEinAMERICANJOURNALOFPHYSICALMEDICINE&REHABILITATION·JUNE2004

ImpactFactor:2.2·DOI:10.1097/01.PHM.0000128789.75375.D3·Source:PubMed

CITATIONS

129

READS

67

7AUTHORS,INCLUDING:

PatriciaNeff

4PUBLICATIONS237CITATIONS

SEEPROFILE

MarciaPolansky

DrexelUniversity

142PUBLICATIONS5,955CITATIONS

SEEPROFILE

H.BranchCoslett

UniversityofPennsylvania

226PUBLICATIONS8,449CITATIONS

SEEPROFILE

Availablefrom:H.BranchCoslett

Retrievedon:03February2016

Authors:

John Whyte, MD, PhDTessa Hart, PhDMonica Vaccaro, MSPatricia Grieb-Neff, MAAnthony Risser, PhDMarcia Polansky, ScDH. Branch Coslett, MD

Affiliations:

From the Moss RehabilitationResearch Institute, Albert EinsteinHealthcare Network, Philadelphia,Pennsylvania (JW, TH, MV, PGN, AR,HBC); the Department ofRehabilitation Medicine, ThomasJefferson University, Philadelphia,Pennsylvania (JW, TH); theDepartment of Symptom Research,University of Texas, M. D. AndersonCancer Center, Houston, Texas (AR);the Department of Biostatistics,Drexel University, Philadelphia,Pennsylvania (MP); and theDepartment of Neurology, Universityof Pennsylvania School of Medicine,Philadelphia, Pennsylvania (HBC).

Disclosures:

Supported, in part, by grantR01NS39163 from the NationalInstitute on Neurological Diseasesand Stroke, National Institutes ofHealth, and grant R24HD39621 fromthe National Center for MedicalRehabilitation Research, NationalInstitute on Child Health and HumanDevelopment, National Institutes ofHealth.

Correspondence:

All correspondence and requests forreprints should be addressed to JohnWhyte, MD, PhD, Moss RehabilitationResearch Institute, 1200 West TaborRoad, Suite 213, Philadelphia, PA19141.

0894-9115/04/8306-0401/0American Journal of PhysicalMedicine & RehabilitationCopyright © 2004 by LippincottWilliams & Wilkins

DOI: 10.1097/01.PHM.0000128789.75375.D3

Effects of Methylphenidate onAttention Deficits After TraumaticBrain InjuryA Multidimensional, Randomized,Controlled TrialABSTRACTWhyte J, Hart T, Vaccaro M, Grieb-Neff P, Risser A, Polansky M, Coslett HB: Effects of methyl-phenidate on attention deficits after traumatic brain injury: A multidimensional, randomized, con-trolled trial. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2004;83:401–420.

Objective: To evaluate the effects of methylphenidate on a variety of aspects of attention,ranging from laboratory-based impairment measures to caregiver ratings and work productivity, inindividuals after traumatic brain injury.

Design: A total of 34 adults with moderate to severe traumatic brain injury and attention com-plaints in the postacute phase of recovery were enrolled in a 6-wk, double-blind, placebo-con-trolled, repeated crossover study of methylphenidate, administered in a dose of 0.3 mg/kg/dose,twice a day. A wide range of attentional measures was gathered weekly, including computerizedand paper-and-pencil tests of attention, videotaped records of individual work in a distractingenvironment, real-time observational scoring of attentiveness in a classroom environment, andcaregiver and clinician rating scales of attentiveness. Participants also attempted to guess theirdrug condition each week. Data from the first ten participants were used for pilot purposes, todevelop attentional factors for composite scoring, and to identify attentional dimensions sugges-tive of a treatment effect for independent replication. The remaining 24 participants’ results wereused to confirm potential treatment effects seen in the pilot sample, using Wilcoxon’s signed-rankstest on composite factor scores and individual variables.

Results: A total of 54 dependent variables were reduced to 13 composite factors and 13remaining individual variables. Of the 13 attentional factors, five showed suggestive treatmenteffects in the pilot sample. Of these, three showed statistically significant treatment effects in thereplication sample: speed of information processing (effect sizes, �0.06 to 0.48; P � 0.001),attentiveness during individual work tasks (effect sizes, 0.15–0.62; P � 0.01), and caregiverratings of attention (effect sizes, 0.44–0.50; P � 0.01). Of the individual variables, four showedsuggestive treatment effects in the pilot sample, but only one showed significant treatment effectsin the replication sample: reaction time before errors in the Sustained Attention to Response Task(effect size, 0.20; P � 0.03). No treatment-related improvement was seen in divided attention,sustained attention, or susceptibility to distraction. None of the variables showed suggestive ordefinite negative treatment effects. Effect sizes for those performance measures positively af-fected by methylphenidate were in the small to medium range and included both impairment andactivity level measures. Improvements in processing speed did not seem to come at the expenseof accuracy.

Conclusions: Methylphenidate, at 0.3 mg/kg/dose, given twice a day to individuals with atten-tional complaints after traumatic brain injury, seems to have clinically significant positive effects onspeed of processing, caregiver ratings of attention, and some aspects of on-task behavior innaturalistic tasks. Further research is needed to identify the optimal dose and to extend thesefindings to less carefully selected individuals.

Key Words: Traumatic Brain Injury, Methylphenidate, Attention Deficits

June 2004 Effects of Methylphenidate After TBI 401

Research Article

Brain Injury

Attentional impairments are amongthe most common cognitive deficitsobserved after traumatic brain injury(TBI)1. Like attention itself, these im-pairments are multifaceted in nature;they can include lowered vigilance, in-creased distractibility, slowed process-ing time, and impaired ability to director allocate attention across differentfacets of the environment2. Attentionalimpairments may also contribute tomemory deficits and impairments ofexecutive functions, the two other pre-dominant neuropsychological disor-ders observed in moderate-to-severeTBI3. The importance of attention ineveryday life, its diversity in normaland defective expression, and the typi-cal mechanisms of injury sustainedduring the closed head injuries thatresult in moderate-to-severe TBI allcreate the need for comprehensive ex-aminations to specify the types of at-tentional problems that are present atany given time for an individual.4

Although moderate-to-severe TBIaccounts for only roughly 20% of alldocumented TBIs,5 survivors requiresignificant acute medical and rehabili-tation inpatient care and postacute re-habilitation services. These survivorsoften present with changes in indepen-dent daily functioning, psychosocial re-lationships, and academic and voca-tional status.6 Of course, attentionalimpairments are not alone in deter-mining functional outcome, but ad-dressing them is essential to successfulrehabilitation efforts. Attempts to im-prove acquired attentional problemshave included both cognitive-behav-ioral remediation efforts and psycho-pharmacologic intervention, but nei-ther has been conclusively shown to beof therapeutic benefit. Thus, there is acritical need for carefully controlledevaluations of treatment efficacy tosupport evidence-based approaches tointervention,7 for both cognitive-reme-diation8 and drug-based9 clinical-trialresearch.

The lack of evidence-based treat-ment recommendations stems from a

number of methodologic challenges.Although clinicians, caregivers, andsometimes TBI survivors themselvescite attention deficits as key cognitiveproblems, it has been difficult for re-searchers to reach consensus on thenature of these problems.10 Althoughit is widely agreed that TBI results inslowed information processing,11 it isnot clear that this, alone, can accountfor the range of the attention com-plaints reported.12 Other problems,including susceptibility to distrac-tion,13 inefficient attentional selec-tion,14 impaired sustained atten-tion,12 impaired divided attention,15

and increased rates of off-task behav-ior in naturalistic tasks,16 have beenreported in some but not all studies.This difficulty is not confined to theclinical study of attention after TBI.Indeed, controversy remains in cog-nitive psychology about how to defineattention, how to subdivide it, andhow best to measure its constitu-ents17. When considering attentiondeficits from the perspective of reha-bilitation, it also becomes importantto consider the level of conceptualanalysis of the attention measures ofinterest.18 That is, some researchtasks focus primarily at the impair-ment level, others assess manifesta-tions of attention in real-world tasks(activity level), and still others mayaddress the effect of attention deficitson an individual’s ability to resumepremorbid roles (participation level).Related to this point, pharmacologictreatments target the impairmentlevel, whereas approaches to cogni-tive remediation may target impair-ment, activity levels, or a combina-tion. Thus, measuring the effect ofsuch treatments may involve atradeoff between measurement sensi-tivity (which may be maximal whenusing impairment-level outcomemeasures) and clinical relevance(which is more related to activity andparticipation measures).18 Finally,shortcomings in experimental designhave limited the interpretation of theresults of a number of studies. Pre–

post designs without an untreatedcontrol group are rarely interpretablebecause of spontaneous recovery orpractice effects.19 Small parallelgroup studies run the risk of bothtype II error, due to lack of statisticalpower, and type I error, due to be-tween-group differences in patientcharacteristics, incompletely con-trolled by randomization.

Methylphenidate (MP) and otherpsychostimulants have been used totreat attention deficit hyperactivitydisorder (ADHD) at least since 1938,when it was discovered that benze-drine led to increased interest andeffort in school for affected chil-dren20. A recent review and meta-analysis covering 40 yrs of researchon MP treatment in ADHD concludedthat despite great variability acrossmethodology and quality of studies,MP consistently affects the inatten-tion, excessive behavior, and impul-sivity that constitute the core clinicalfeatures of the disorder21. Stimulantssuch as MP decrease hyperactivityand increase on-task behavior inADHD22. Compared with the amphet-amines to which it is chemically re-lated, MP may affect mental functionsmore prominently than motor activ-ity23. Recent studies have sought toisolate the cognitive functions mostaffected by MP in children with atten-tion disorders. For example, on theStroop Color Word Task, MP does notseem to improve interference control(e.g., ability to suppress prepotentword naming responses in the inter-ference condition)24,25, but the drugdoes affect speed of “effortful” pro-cessing (e.g., color-naming alone) ina highly positive way.25

MP has also been used to treatADHD in adults, although the re-search on this use is far less exten-sive. Variations in treatment responseamong adults may relate to differ-ences in diagnostic criteria used,presence of co-morbidities, anddoses. However, a rigorously de-signed crossover study showed highly

402 Whyte et al. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. ● Vol. 83, No. 6

significant treatment effects amongadults with ADHD.26

The success of MP in childrenwith attention and behavior disordershas prompted researchers and clini-cians to use the drug for similar dis-orders seen in TBI and other acquiredbrain injuries. Other types of post-traumatic problems also have beenaddressed with MP. For example, thedrug has been reported to be effectivefor the treatment of posttraumaticnarcolepsy27 and difficulties with an-ger control28. Less conclusive effectshave been suggested for overall ad-justment29 and behavior30. However,the majority of published studieshave tested the effects of MP on cog-nition. Recently, we published a de-tailed review and critique of studieson MP and other psychostimulants inTBI,9 including a previous random-ized, controlled trial from our labo-ratory.31 As discussed in more detailin that review, studies are difficult tocompare because they vary widely indegree of experimental rigor, statisti-cal power, acuity and severity of TBIin the sample, age of the sample,number and type of dependent mea-sures, and other key factors that af-fect the interpretation of results. Theweight of the evidence thus far, how-ever, seems to indicate that MP doesnot have a very strong effect on as-pects of attention such as maintain-ing vigilance over time (sustained at-tention)29,31,32 or resistance todistraction.29,31 MP does seem tohave a beneficial effect on processingspeed, as measured in tasks of reac-tion time and speeded decision-mak-ing.31–34 In our previous controlledinvestigation,31 we demonstratedthat this effect of MP was due to aspecific effect on cognitive speedrather than speed of motorperformance.

The purpose of this study was toextend previous research findings onthe effect of MP on attention deficitsresulting from TBI. Because of themultifaceted nature of attention, asdiscussed above, we chose to study

the effects of MP on a wide range ofattention measures, including thoseat impairment and activity levels, as-sessed over a 6-wk period of intensivedata collection. Recognizing that theneed to conduct statistical testing ofthe drug’s effect in each of many ar-eas increases the risk of type I error,we sought to develop compositescores reflecting several differentvariables. We also designed the studyto include a pilot phase, followed byan independent replication in a largersample, of any drug effects thatseemed promising. This complex de-sign was undertaken to achieve twobroad research objectives. One was toexamine the clinical efficacy of MPfor attention-related deficits associ-ated with TBI in a more systematicand controlled fashion than has beenundertaken in previous research. Theother, equally important, objectivewas to clarify which attentional do-mains are responsive to MP treat-ment in this population so that fu-ture clinical trials can focus on asmaller number of empirically vali-dated primary outcomes.

METHODS

Participants

Participants were recruited froma variety of sources, with most refer-rals coming from the outpatient clin-ical programs at the Drucker BrainInjury Center at MossRehab Hospitaland the research registry maintainedat MossRehab, of inpatients fromMossRehab Hospital, Magee Rehabil-itation Hospital, and Bryn Mawr Re-habilitation Hospital, all located inthe Philadelphia area. We also adver-tised the study through the local pub-lic transportation system, a freenewspaper distributed in a variety ofplaces in the area, and with the braininjury associations of Pennsylvaniaand New Jersey.

Participants who were interestedin the study were interviewed andtheir medical records reviewed. To be

included, participants had to be be-tween the ages of 16 and 60 yrs and tohave a history of nonpenetrating TBIof at least moderate severity at least 3mos before enrollment. Severity levelwas defined by significant and welldocumented loss or alteration of con-sciousness after injury (i.e., lowestGlasgow Coma Scale score of �12[Although many of the GlasgowComa Scale scores used to determineeligibility were postresuscitationscores, in some instances, the initialGlasgow Coma Scale score was ob-tained from later secondary recordsin the absence of the acute care flowsheets, such that the exact timing ofthe score was unclear.] or prospec-tively documented posttraumatic am-nesia of �1 hr) or focal abnormalityon a neuroimaging study that wasattributable to traumatic injury. Par-ticipants needed to be able to performtasks for 10–15 mins semi-indepen-dently and be available to attend theproject five full days per week for 6wks. A subjective complaint of atten-tion difficulties by the participant,treating clinician, or caregiver wasalso required.

Potential participants were ex-cluded if they were currently hospi-talized; if they were pregnant or likelyto become pregnant; if they had ahistory of premorbid neurologic dis-ease, psychosis, major affective disor-der, mental retardation, or ADHD; ifthey were taking psychotropic medi-cations other than anticonvulsants; ifthey were currently abusing alcoholor recreational drugs, or if their his-tory of using these substances wassignificant enough to place them atrisk of long-term neurologic effects.Individuals were also excluded for be-havioral problems or for impairmentsin vision, hearing, or motor functionthat were severe enough to precludeparticipation in the research tasks.Participants or their involved care-givers (depending on the partici-pant’s cognitive capacity) providedinformed consent.

During a period of several years,

June 2004 Effects of Methylphenidate After TBI 403

we reviewed �1,500 individuals forpossible participation in this study, asshown in Figure 1. Possible partici-pants who were involved in clinicalprograms could often be excludedbased on the available records. Thosewho were encountered as a result ofpublic advertising were generally in-terviewed first to obtain releases fortheir medical records if they seemedeligible through interview. However,ascertainment of some of the eligibil-ity criteria (e.g., history of ADHD orsubstance abuse, availability for 6wks) generally required an interview,and this, in turn, required explainingthe basics of the study as an intro-duction. Thus, there were a numberof potential participants whose finaleligibility could not be ascertainedeither because they did not returntelephone calls or because they de-clined after hearing about the studybut before answering the interviewquestions. A total of 39 participantsconsented to the study and were ran-domized to a drug order (see below).Two subsequently withdrew due tosubjective adverse effects (one whilereceiving placebo), and one wasdropped because of possible exacerba-tion of baseline hypertension. Thesethree participants had insufficientdata for analysis. Two additional par-ticipants were dropped because ofsuspected substance abuse duringdata collection. Although one of thesewas dropped near the end of data col-lection, the data were not included inthe analysis because of the potentialconfounding effects of the substanceabuse. Thus, the sample of 34 indi-viduals who completed the study wasclearly a highly selected subgroup ofthose who were theoretically eligible.

Participants who met all eligibilitycriteria and provided informed consentunderwent additional data collection tocharacterize the sample and establishbaseline levels of function. Duration ofposttraumatic amnesia, as an addi-tional indicator of injury severity, wasassessed retrospectively using themethod developed by McMillan et al.35

In brief, this involved comparing par-ticipants’ memory for events aftertrauma against temporal milestonesabstracted from the medical record. Sixparticipants were unable to provide rel-evant information, either because theirinjury was so long before the assess-ment that they could not adequatelyrecall relevant events or because theyhad communication impairments thatprecluded participating effectively inthe interview. For all participants, theDisability Rating Scale36 was scored byproject staff based on participant andfamily interview at the time of enroll-ment as a measure of the level of cur-rent disability. Premorbid intellectual

status was estimated using demo-graphic and occupational history da-ta.37 Before starting the trial, partici-pants also underwent a battery ofneuropsychological tests focused pri-marily on attention and frontal/execu-tive function; these results will be pre-sented separately.

Table 1 summarizes the demo-graphic, injury and disability charac-teristics of the 34 participants. As istypical of research on moderate to se-vere TBI, the majority were male. Ex-cept for the exclusion of elderly partic-ipants, the distributions of age,education level, and ethnicity were typ-ical of TBI samples in metropolitan ar-

Figure 1: Flowchart detailing the 1,500 individuals reviewed for possibleparticipation in the study. MP, methylphenidate; P, placebo.

404 Whyte et al. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. ● Vol. 83, No. 6

eas. In keeping with the exclusion forpremorbid cognitive limitations, esti-mated premorbid intellectual statusesall fell within the average to high-aver-age range. It may be seen in Table 1that the typical participant was severalyears postinjury, exhibited a moderatelevel of functional disability, and re-ported a significant degree of posttrau-matic amnesia. Table 1 also shows theproportion of participants for whomobjective records verified at least mod-erate neurologic severity by significantalteration of consciousness or neuro-imaging abnormalities. For two partic-ipants, medical records were not avail-

able for documentation of severity.However, their histories, levels of dis-ability, and motor impairment (onehad spastic quadriparesis, one a densehemiparesis) left no doubt as to theseverity of injury.

Procedure

The data collection for this studywas integrated into a day activitiesprogram infrastructure, provided freeof charge to participants. A rehabili-tation clinician ran a “research class-room” for three to four participantsat a time, from approximately 9:30a.m. to 3:30 p.m., Monday through

Friday. Figure 2 shows a schematicdiagram of the weekly schedule, in-cluding daily/weekly data collectionelements described below. This class-room environment served severalpurposes. It provided a positive groupenvironment to help maintain partic-ipation for the required 6 wks, whichwas an incentive for both participantslacking other productive activity andfor their caregivers, who were fre-quently seeking structured activityfor their family members. It also pro-vided a standardized environment forbehavioral ratings of attentiveness inindividual work and group activities.Finally, it provided a milieu for par-ticipants when they were not beingtested individually in the laboratoryand allowed assessment of vital signs,side effects, and behavioral self-rat-ings. The classroom was a single,large room with a small office for theclassroom clinician attached. Theroom was filled with large work ta-bles, a whiteboard, and materials foreducational and recreational activi-ties. The clinician designed activitiesthat could be tailored to the func-tional levels of individual participantsand that required attention for theirperformance. These activities werenot designed to meet individual clin-ical goals but were intended to beengaging and functionally relevant.The clinician was trained not to in-tervene to increase attentivenessthrough cueing or reinforcement be-cause the intent of this study wasspecifically to assess drug effects.

Participants were involved in thestudy for six consecutive weeks. Theyreceived a participant fee of $50/wkplus transportation expenses. Duringhalf of this time, they received MP,the study medication; during theother half, they took an identical-ap-pearing placebo, with the two condi-tions alternating weekly. Half of theparticipants started with MP followedby placebo and the other started withplacebo followed by MP (i.e., MP, pla-cebo, MP, placebo, MP, placebo orplacebo, MP, placebo, MP, placebo,

TABLE 1Characteristics of participants (n � 34)Demographic characteristics

Age, yrsMean 37Range 20–55

Sex, n (%)Male 29 (85)Female 5 (15)

Ethnicity, n (%)White 22 (65)African American 10 (29)Hispanic 2 (6)

Education, yrsMean 12.7Median 12Range 9–18

Estimated premorbid intelligencequotient

Mean 101Range 85–119

Injury and disability characteristicsParticipants with medical recorddocumentation of, n (%):

Loss/alteration of consciousness 22 (65)Abnormality on neuroimaging 23 (68)

Estimated duration of posttraumaticamnesia (n � 28), n (%)

�2 wks 1 (4)2 wks to �1 mo 4 (14)1 mo to �3 mos 7 (25)3 mos to �6 mos 10 (36)�6 mos 6 (21)

Disability Rating ScaleMean 4 (moderate)Range 1 (mild) to 8

(moderate to severe)Time postinjury

Median 3.2 yrsRange 4 mos to 34.2 yrs

June 2004 Effects of Methylphenidate After TBI 405

MP). A pharmacist who did not haveday-to-day involvement in the studyprepared the medications weekly andrandomly determined the order of ad-ministration so that the conditionswere balanced and concealed fromthe research staff, participants, andcaregivers. Randomization occurredin blocks of four participants (two toeach treatment order). The dose ofMP was 0.3 mg/kg/dose, rounded tothe nearest 2.5 mg, administeredtwice a day at approximately 8:30a.m. and 12 noon, Monday throughSaturday. The average dose of MP(given twice a day) was 24.4 mg(range, 15–40). Sunday was a wash-out day before the crossover to theopposite condition (Fig. 2).

Participants were screened forchanges in vital signs for the first 2wks of the study and for the occur-rence of adverse events throughoutthe study. The screening procedureand the effects of the study drug onvital signs and adverse events havebeen reported separately.38 Once aweek, each participant also guessedwhat drug he or she was receivingand provided a certainty rating for

this judgment to help ascertain thesuccess of the double-blindingprocedure.

Before receiving the medication,participants were interviewed abouttheir use of tobacco, caffeine, alcohol,prescription and over-the-counterdrugs, and recreational drugs. Be-cause of the length of the project, itwas not realistic to expect partici-pants to refrain from caffeine, to-bacco, or alcohol, so they were in-structed to maintain a consistentpattern of use of these substances.While in the study, participants filledout daily forms asking about theiruse to ensure regularity. They werealso asked about sleep, pain, illness,medications taken, and any stressfulevents they might have experiencedthat day. The intent was to determinethat the participants’ state was con-sistent enough from day to day sothat variations in performance weremost likely caused by the medicationand not some other event or state.

Research staff called participantsin the early morning, at home, toremind them to take their medica-tion. At arrival, they were asked

whether and when they had taken it.If a participant missed a dose, thetasks for that morning were skippedand made up at a later date under thecorrect medication condition. Middaydoses were self-administered by par-ticipants under the observation of re-search staff.

As depicted in Figure 2, partici-pants attended four 1-hr sessions inthe classroom each day, during whichtime observational data on on-taskbehavior were collected in both indi-vidual and group tasks. A 90-min pe-riod of unconstrained time plus alunch break occurred each day, dur-ing which no classroom data werecollected. Unconstrained time al-lowed the participants to work on ac-tivities of personal interest as a sup-plement to scheduled activities.Participants were taken from theclassroom for an hour each day to doa variety of individual attention as-sessment tasks. During each of the 6wks, the same tasks and assessmentswere performed, providing threesamples of performance on each mea-sure in the MP and placebo condi-tions and allowing a direct compari-

Figure 2: Weekly schedule for research classroom. CFQ/RSAB, Cognitive Failures Questionnaire/Rating Scale ofAttentional Behavior.

406 Whyte et al. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. ● Vol. 83, No. 6

son of the participants’ performancein the two conditions. Data were col-lected using laboratory measures ofinformation processing, controlledsimulations of attentional demandsof everyday tasks, and ratings of at-tentiveness in everyday life. The orderof tasks was the same each week, andtasks were performed at the sametime each day for a given participant.Morning sessions began 90 mins afterthe morning dose, and afternoon ses-sions began 90 mins after the noondose. When participants were unableto attend on a given day, the individ-ual testing session for that day wasskipped and made up at the end of the6-wk period. The pharmacist suppliedreplacement doses of the appropriatemedication for that date. Missingclassroom data were not made up.

Attention Tasks

Several of the following tasks,marked with an asterisk (*), werepresented on an Apple PowerMac G3.All but one of these were pro-grammed in PsyScope. For thesetasks, participants were seated ap-proximately 30 inches from thescreen. Responses were recorded andtimed using a PsyScope button box.For those tasks with two asterisks(**), the duration of stimulus presen-tation was individualized before studyonset to minimize ceiling and flooreffects. Other tasks described belowused paper-and-pencil responding,rating scales, or observational data.

**Sustained Arousal and AttentionTask 50/50. The Sustained Arousaland Attention Task 50/5012 is a sim-ple go/no-go visual reaction time tasklasting approximately 20 mins. Par-ticipants were presented with 161stimuli with an average interstimulusinterval of 6 secs and instructed torespond as quickly and accurately aspossible to the targets and to ignorethe foils. The target rate for this taskwas 50%. The data derived from thistask were divided into five blocks. Theinitial block of performance defined

the vigilance level, and robust regres-sion slopes were calculated across allof the blocks to determine the vigi-lance decrements in reaction time,response bias, and accuracy.

**Sustained Arousal and AttentionTask 20/80. This modification of theabove-described task used a 20% tar-get rate, based on the hypothesis thatindividuals’ attentional lapses mightbe more frequent if fewer responseswere required. Scoring on this taskwas identical to the original sus-tained arousal and attention task.

*Speed/Accuracy Tradeoff Task. Thistask differed from the two describedabove in that the presented stimuliwere visually degraded so that it wasmore difficult to distinguish betweentargets and foils, but they remainedexposed for much longer (2000msecs), allowing participants to pri-oritize speed or accuracy of respond-ing. Specifically, we were interestedin learning whether drug-inducedimprovements in speed came at theexpense of accuracy or whether bothspeed and accuracy were positivelyaffected by treatment. This task hadthe same number of trials and inter-stimulus interval as the two listedabove and was scored in the samefashion.

Unfortunately, a technical prob-lem was identified after data collec-tion was complete. This task was suf-ficiently more difficult than thesustained attention tasks describedabove, so response times were muchslower for most subjects, but re-sponse times of �2000 msecs werenot recorded by the computer. Thus,some subjects seemed to have verylow response rates, and their re-sponse time distributions were trun-cated at 2000 msecs. Consequently,this task was not incorporated intothe overall analyses described below.However, those subjects with useabledata were selected to specifically as-sess whether any effects of the drug

on speed occurred at the expense ofaccuracy.

**Distraction Task. This task was avariation on one previously pub-lished.13 The same stimuli were pre-sented with a 50% target rate withone addition: brightly colored shapesmoved rapidly up and down eitherabove or below the stimuli. These dis-tracters occurred at various intervalsjust before or after stimulus presen-tation. There were seven intervalsranging from 200 msecs before thepresentation of the stimulus to 200msecs after stimulus presentation. Inaddition, a no-distracter conditionwas included, which allowed directcomparison of distracted and undis-tracted performance within the sametask. There were 192 trials in thistask, with an average interstimulusinterval of 6 secs, organized into 12blocks of 16 trials each (one targetand one foil at each distracter inter-val, in random order). Variables fromthis task were derived by subtractingthe scores in the no-distracter condi-tion from the participant’s meanscores achieved at five of the dis-tracter intervals at which a perfor-mance decrement was noted. Thisyielded distracter effect scores for ac-curacy, response bias, and reactiontime.

*Choice Reaction Time Task. Theparticipant was required to press anumber key on the numeric keypadof the computer keyboard in responseto presentation of a digit on thescreen. There were three blocks inthis experiment, in which there weretwo, four, or six digits displayed. Re-sponse keys not being used during agiven block were covered to makeclear to the participants that thenumber of choices varied. The num-ber stimulus remained on the screenuntil the participant responded. Aftereach response, there was a 1000-msecblank interval, followed by an 800-msec presentation of a central fixa-tion cross. This was followed by the

June 2004 Effects of Methylphenidate After TBI 407

presentation of the potential digit setin a random array for 1000 msecs.Finally, an auditory tone signaled thesimultaneous brightening of one ofthe digits to identify it as the target. Aregression slope of mean reactiontime for correct responses vs. the nat-ural logarithm of the number ofchoices available served as the indexof processing speed, based on evi-dence that the steepness of this slopeis an index of speed of mental pro-cessing (i.e., steeper � slower).39

*Dual Task. This program, written inC by Daniel Kimberg,15 was designedto assess the ability to divide atten-tion across two concurrent tasks. Theparticipant was first asked to respondas quickly as possible, by pressing thespacebar on the computer keyboard,to dots that appeared in pseudo-ran-dom locations on the computer mon-itor. The mean response time forthese trials defined the participant’sbaseline speed. The participant wasthen asked to listen to and repeatstrings of digits read to them by theresearch assistant while respondingto the dots as before. Digit stringlength was determined for each par-ticipant by initial digit span testing.The dual task decrement score is thedifference between mean responsetime in the single vs. dual taskversions.

*Sustained Attention to ResponseTask. In this task,40 225 digits (num-bers 1–9) are presented randomly,one at a time, in the center of thecomputer monitor, during a 4.5-mininterval. The participant must press aresponse key as quickly as possiblefor all but one of the digits (3) andmust withhold a response for thatdigit. This is also a go/no-go task, butit differs from those previously de-scribed in that targets are commonand foils are rare (1:9). Measures inthis task are the number of omissionerrors, the number of commission er-rors, and the reaction time of re-sponses before errors.

Test of Everyday Attention. Thistask,41 not administered by com-puter, includes eight subtests avail-able in three equivalent forms (givenas forms A, B, C, A, B, C on successivetesting weeks). The subtests are de-signed to assess selective attention,sustained attention, and attentionalswitching by simulating naturalistictasks, such as locating items on amap, looking for phone numbers in adirectory, determining which floor toget off an elevator, and checking win-ning lottery numbers.

Inattentive Behavior Task. In thistask,42 the participant was asked toperform three tasks at a worktable,including making a collage, sortingitems into their correct bowls, andworking on a complex jigsaw puzzle,while being videotaped. During thesession, a research assistant carriedout a series of naturalistic distrac-tions (e.g., making a phone call, play-ing a noisy computer game, droppinga book) on cue from taped messagesdelivered through a concealed ear-phone. The videotapes were coded ata later time on a coding workstation,which consisted of a Dell, Windows-compatible personal computer with aPentium II processor and a Panasonicvideo cassette recorder. Observa-tional Coding Software, Version 3.2,developed by Triangle Research Col-laborative, Inc. (Research TrianglePark, NC), was installed on the per-sonal computer. This software al-lowed the beginning, end, and dura-tion of each off-task event, externaldistraction, and period of the taskduring which the research assistantgave directions to be identified. Off-task events were defined with respectto direction of eye gaze, as describedpreviously.16,42 Rates of off-task be-havior were calculated separately foreach task as events per minute of tasktime. The average duration of off-taskevents in each session was also calcu-lated by dividing the total off-tasktime by the number of off-taskevents. The number of items used in

the collage and number of items cor-rectly sorted were also recorded.

Interrater reliability was assessedusing Cohen’s Kappa for the begin-nings and endings of off-task events,external distractions, and time-outevents (e.g., direction giving). Tworesearch assistants coded videotapedrecords of inattentive behavior. Ini-tially, all videotaped records weredouble coded, until adequate agree-ment, as measured by an averageKappa of �0.8, was achieved. Thisprocess was maintained for 68records, after which records sub-jected to interrater agreement assess-ment was reduced to 33% of allrecords. At this point, two researchassistants coded one randomly se-lected record for each drug conditionfor each participant, without knowl-edge of drug condition. In all, 111 ofall videotaped records of inattentivebehavior (54%) were double coded.Whenever the agreement betweencoders on a given record fell belowthe standard (Kappa of 0.8) on any ofthe six variables included in the anal-ysis, the coders met to reconcile thedifferences. The reconciled data wereused for the final analysis of drugeffects. To minimize the potential forbias (because reconciliation typicallyincreases, rather than decreases, thenumber of events noted), whenever arecord was subjected to reconcilia-tion, another session in the oppositedrug condition was also reconciled,regardless of the level of agreementbetween the coders on that secondrecord. Mean Kappas for videotapedrecords of inattentive behavior dur-ing the posttraining phase were: 0.83,0.82, 1.0, 98, 0.91, and 0.89 for off-task begin, off-task end, distracter be-gin, distracter end, time-out begin,and time-out end, respectively.

Classroom Attentiveness. In additionto the laboratory testing, participantswere observed as they participated inclassroom activities. Data collectionoccurred during four 1-hr sessionseach day: a group activity and an in-

408 Whyte et al. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. ● Vol. 83, No. 6

dividual activity in both the morningand the afternoon. During individualsessions, the participant was asked tocarry out independent work (e.g.,reading a book, working on cross-word puzzles, or individual craftprojects) in the same room withother participants. During group ses-sions, activities such as playing boardgames, discussing current events,and participating in lectures, whichrequired the interaction and involve-ment of all participants, were con-ducted. For both types of session, aset of rules that defined the appropri-ate targets of attention was laid outby the classroom therapist before theactivity began. For example, in agroup activity with structured turn-taking, responding during someoneelse’s turn or failing to respond dur-ing one’s own turn were both codedas off-task behavior. During each ses-sion, a research assistant sat in theclassroom to observe and code off-task behavior. The research assistantwore a vibrating watch that provideda silent cue once per minute. A ran-dom sequence of participant identifi-cation codes was preprinted on a datacollection sheet. Each minute, whenthe watch cued, the research assis-tant looked at the appropriate partic-ipant and coded whether, at that mo-ment, he or she was on task or offtask according to operationalizedwritten behavioral criteria. Data werecollected on off-task behavior as mea-sured by eye gaze, speaking, and be-ing out of seat. However, only theeye-gaze data are reported here be-cause scores in the other two do-mains were frequently at ceiling. On-task eye gaze was defined based onwhether the research participant waslooking at the appropriate task mate-rials (for individual tasks) or the task-relevant speaker or materials (forgroup tasks).

Two research assistants indepen-dently observed 27 classroom ses-sions (approximately 2% of all datacollection classroom sessions) usingthe same vibrating watch cue-driven

time sampling and the same randomorder of participant observation. In-terrater agreement assessment, usingboth Cohen’s Kappa and percentageof agreement, was conducted on thepresence or absence of off-task eventsas indicated by direction of eye gaze.Agreement between pairs of raterscoding attentiveness in classroom ac-tivities was high. Perfect agreementwas achieved in 23 of 27 sessions.Considering percentage of agreementacross all double-coded sessions, av-erage agreement was 99%. Kappaswere calculated for those sessions inwhich both on-task and off-taskevents were observed (n � 15) andranged from 0.65 to 1.0, with a meanof 0.95 and median of 1.0. There were12 sessions during which both codersagreed that no off-task events oc-curred, and therefore, Kappa couldnot be calculated.

Attention Ratings. Two different rat-ings of the participants’ attentivenessin everyday life were gathered on aweekly basis. Different versions of theCognitive Failures Questionnaire43

were completed weekly by the partic-ipants themselves, their chosen care-giver, and the research staff (by con-sensus). This measure assesses thefrequency of everyday lapses in atten-tion using a 5-point Likert-type scale,ranging from “very often” to “never.”The participants completed a 25-itemversion, which includes more subjec-tive questions about perceived atten-tional difficulties, whereas the care-givers and research staff filled out aneight-item version. The Rating Scaleof Attentional Behavior11 is a 14-item, 5-point Likert-type scale de-signed to assess observable attention-related behaviors, such as rest-lessness and distractibility. This wascompleted by the participant’s care-giver and the research staff.

Data Analysis

Initial Data Reduction. Performanceon most of the computerized infor-

mation processing tasks was assessedin several dimensions: D' (a signaldetection measure of accuracy, orability to discriminate the target vs.the foil), yes rate (a measure of re-sponse bias, or tendency to press theresponse key, irrespective of accu-racy), and median response time (ameasure of speed of processing andresponding, less sensitive to outliersthan the mean, and calculated onlyon correct key presses). For the tasksassessing sustained attention, theabove three scores were calculated onthe first block (first 20% of trials) todetermine initial performance, andthe slope of a robust regressionacross the scores of all five blocks wasused to represent the ability to sus-tain performance over the duration ofthe task. The relevant scores fromeach task were averaged across thethree (or in a few cases, due to miss-ing data, two) sessions from eachdrug condition, yielding a pair ofscores representing the drug condi-tions for each participant.

Assessment of the Effects of MP. Asdiscussed above, it was thought im-portant to assess the effect of MP onnumerous aspects of attentionalfunction to avoid missing clinicallyimportant effects on unmeasured at-tentional dimensions, to evaluate thedrug’s effect at impairment and activ-ity levels of analysis, and to be able tocompare the drug’s profile of actionwith those of other drugs. This pro-duced a total of 54 separate scores foranalysis (Table 2 contains descrip-tions of the scores used in the finalanalysis). These variables assessedinitial performance in a variety oftasks, as assessed by accuracy, speed,and overall response rate; deteriora-tion in performance over time, basedon these same three performance in-dices; impulsive responding; distrac-tion by competing stimuli; the abilityto divide attention; attentiveness asjudged by ratings scales; and atten-tiveness as judged by direct observa-tion of off-task behavior.

June 2004 Effects of Methylphenidate After TBI 409

TAB

LE2

Sign

ifica

nce

test

ing

ofm

ethy

lphe

nida

teef

fect

son

perf

orm

ance

fact

ors

and

indi

vidu

alva

riab

les

Task

Scor

e

PVa

lue

Effe

cta

Size

Pilo

tSa

mpl

eR

eplic

atio

nSa

mpl

e

Com

posi

teFa

ctor

sIn

itia

lac

cura

cyPe

rcep

tual

lysi

mpl

evi

sual

go/n

o-go

(50%

targ

ets)

D',

first

bloc

k0.

070.

29Pe

rcep

tual

lysi

mpl

evi

sual

go/n

o-go

(20%

targ

ets)

D',

first

bloc

kAc

cura

cyde

clin

ePe

rcep

tual

lysi

mpl

evi

sual

go/n

o-go

(50%

targ

ets)

Rob

ust

regr

essi

onsl

ope,

D'v

s.ta

skbl

ock

1.00

—Pe

rcep

tual

lysi

mpl

evi

sual

go/n

o-go

(20%

targ

ets)

Rob

ust

regr

essi

onsl

ope,

D'v

s.ta

skbl

ock

Init

ial

spee

dPe

rcep

tual

lysi

mpl

evi

sual

go/n

o-go

(50%

targ

ets)

Med

ian

RT,

first

32tr

ials

0.02

�0.

001

�0.

06Pe

rcep

tual

lysi

mpl

evi

sual

go/n

o-go

(20%

targ

ets)

Med

ian

RT,

first

32tr

ials

0.07

Visu

algo

/no-

gota

sk(5

0%ta

rget

s)w

ith

salie

ntm

ovin

gdi

stra

cter

sM

edia

nR

T,no

ndis

trac

tion

tria

ls0.

48C

hoic

eR

TR

obus

tre

gres

sion

slop

e:m

ean

RT

vs.

log

(no.

ofch

oice

s)0.

32D

ual

task

Mea

nR

T,ba

selin

eco

ndit

ion

0.12

Test

ofE

very

day

Atte

ntio

nm

apse

arch

No.

ofsy

mbo

lsci

rcle

d(2

min

s)0.

18Te

stof

Eve

ryda

yAt

tent

ion

tele

phon

ese

arch

Tim

epe

rco

rrec

tly

circ

led

targ

et0.

40In

atte

ntiv

ebe

havi

orta

sk:

wor

kpr

oduc

tivi

tyN

o.of

item

sso

rted

0.48

Spee

dde

clin

ePe

rcep

tual

lysi

mpl

evi

sual

go/n

o-go

(50%

targ

ets)

Rob

ust

regr

essi

onsl

ope,

med

ian

RT

vs.

task

bloc

k0.

35—

Perc

eptu

ally

sim

ple

visu

algo

/no-

go(2

0%ta

rget

s)R

obus

tre

gres

sion

slop

e,m

edia

nR

Tvs

.ta

skbl

ock

Init

ial

resp

onse

bias

Perc

eptu

ally

sim

ple

visu

algo

/no-

go(5

0%ta

rget

s)R

espo

nse

rate

,fir

st32

tria

ls0.

040.

88Pe

rcep

tual

lysi

mpl

evi

sual

go/n

o-go

(20%

targ

ets)

Res

pons

era

te,

first

32tr

ials

Visu

algo

/no-

gota

sk(5

0%ta

rget

s)w

ith

salie

ntm

ovin

gdi

stra

cter

sR

espo

nse

rate

,no

ndis

trac

tion

tria

lsD

eclin

ein

resp

ondi

ngPe

rcep

tual

lysi

mpl

evi

sual

go/n

o-go

(50%

targ

ets)

Rob

ust

regr

essi

onsl

ope,

resp

onse

rate

vs.

task

bloc

k0.

85—

Perc

eptu

ally

sim

ple

visu

algo

/no-

go(2

0%ta

rget

s)R

obus

tre

gres

sion

slop

e,re

spon

sera

tevs

.ta

skbl

ock

Div

ided

atte

ntio

nD

ual

task

RT

decr

emen

t(m

ean

RT

wit

hdi

git

span

–mea

nR

Tdu

ring

base

line)

0.88

Test

ofE

very

day

Atte

ntio

n,el

evat

orco

unti

ngw

ith

dist

ract

ion

No.

corr

ect

Test

ofE

very

day

Atte

ntio

n,te

leph

one

sear

chw

hile

coun

ting

Tim

e/ta

rget

Test

ofE

very

day

Atte

ntio

n,vi

sual

elev

ator

Tim

e/sw

itch

Test

ofE

very

day

Atte

ntio

n,el

evat

orw

ith

reve

rsal

No.

corr

ect

Sust

aine

dAt

tent

ion

toR

espo

nse

Task

Err

ors

ofco

mm

issi

onN

o.ke

ypr

esse

sto

“3”

0.91

—E

rror

sof

omis

sion

No.

ofm

isse

sto

non-

“3”

No.

key

pres

ses

wit

hR

TIm

puls

ive

erro

rsR

T�

150

mse

cs

410 Whyte et al. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. ● Vol. 83, No. 6

TAB

LE2

Con

tinu

ed

Task

Scor

e

PVa

lue

Effe

cta

Size

Pilo

tSa

mpl

eR

eplic

atio

nSa

mpl

e

Fam

ilyra

ting

sC

ogni

tive

Failu

res

Que

stio

nnai

reTo

tal

scor

e0.

040.

010.

44R

atin

gSc

ale

ofAt

tent

iona

lB

ehav

ior

Tota

lsc

ore

0.50

Staf

fra

ting

sC

ogni

tive

Failu

res

Que

stio

nnai

reTo

tal

scor

e0.

85—

Rat

ing

Scal

eof

Atte

ntio

nal

Beh

avio

rTo

tal

scor

eIn

atte

ntio

n—in

divi

dual

Mor

ning

clas

sroo

m—

indi

vidu

alac

tivi

ties

%O

n-ta

skey

ega

zeco

des

0.06

0.01

0.28

Afte

rnoo

ncl

assr

oom

—in

divi

dual

acti

viti

es%

On-

task

eye

gaze

code

s0.

62In

atte

ntiv

ebe

havi

or—

task

1Av

erag

edu

rati

onof

off-

task

epis

odes

0.22

Inat

tent

ive

beha

vior

—ta

sk2

Aver

age

dura

tion

ofof

f-ta

skep

isod

es0.

15In

atte

ntiv

ebe

havi

or—

task

3Av

erag

edu

rati

onof

off-

task

epis

odes

0.47

Inat

tent

ion—

indi

vidu

alra

teIn

atte

ntiv

ebe

havi

or—

task

1R

ate

ofof

f-ta

skep

isod

es0.

26—

Inat

tent

ive

beha

vior

—ta

sk2

Rat

eof

off-

task

epis

odes

Inat

tent

ive

beha

vior

—ta

sk3

Rat

eof

off-

task

epis

odes

Inat

tent

ion—

grou

pM

orni

ngcl

assr

oom

—gr

oup

acti

viti

es%

On-

task

eye

gaze

code

s0.

92—

Afte

rnoo

ncl

assr

oom

—gr

oup

acti

viti

es%

On-

task

eye

gaze

code

sIn

divi

dual

scor

esVi

sual

go/n

o-go

task

(50%

targ

ets)

wit

hsa

lient

mov

ing

dist

ract

ers

D�

nond

istr

acti

ontr

ials

0.70

—Vi

sual

go/n

o-go

task

(50%

targ

ets)

wit

hsa

lient

mov

ing

dist

ract

ers

D'w

ith

dist

ract

ion

�D

'wit

hout

dist

ract

ion

0.62

—Vi

sual

go/n

o-go

task

(50%

targ

ets)

wit

hsa

lient

mov

ing

dist

ract

ers

Res

pons

era

tew

ith

dist

ract

ion

�re

spon

sera

tew

itho

utdi

stra

ctio

n0.

92—

Visu

algo

/no-

gota

sk(5

0%ta

rget

s)w

ith

salie

ntm

ovin

gdi

stra

cter

sM

edia

nR

Tw

ith

dist

ract

ion

�m

edia

nR

Tw

itho

utdi

stra

ctio

n0.

49—

Dua

lta

skM

easu

red

digi

tsp

an0.

190.

70D

ual

task

Dig

itst

ring

sad

min

iste

red

0.56

—D

ual

task

Dig

itst

ring

sco

rrec

t,%

0.63

—C

ogni

tive

Failu

res

Que

stio

nnai

re—

self

rati

ngs

Tota

lsc

ore

0.18

0.66

Sust

aine

dat

tent

ion

tore

spon

seta

skM

ean

RT

befo

reer

ror

ofco

mm

issi

on0.

110.

030.

20Te

stof

Eve

ryda

yAt

tent

ion—

visu

alel

evat

orac

cura

cyN

o.co

rrec

t0.

120.

33Te

stof

Eve

ryda

yAt

tent

ion—

lott

ery

No.

corr

ect

0.56

—In

atte

ntiv

ebe

havi

orC

olla

gepa

per

used

0.65

RT,

resp

onse

tim

e.aE

ffect

size

asm

easu

red

byC

ohen

’sD

.

June 2004 Effects of Methylphenidate After TBI 411

As mentioned previously, to re-duce the risk of type I error, a two-stage analytic strategy was employed.Data gathered from the first ten par-ticipants were used only for pilot pur-poses, not in the final analysis, whichserved as an independent replicationon 24 additional participants. Datafrom the pilot sample were used intwo ways. First, the three sessions ofplacebo data from these ten partici-pants were used to develop methodsfor collapsing scores from multipleempirically or conceptually relatedtasks into fewer composite scores.Second, data from the three placeboand three MP scores for these tenparticipants were analyzed for possi-ble drug effects, using a relaxed P-value cutoff of 0.20. Only those vari-ables that seemed promising, basedon meeting this cutoff, were sub-jected to confirmatory analysis, usingdata from the next 24 participants(the replication sample). Thus, to bedefined as responsive to MP, a partic-ular variable had to show a drug ef-fect in the same direction in the pilotsample (with a P value of �0.20) andin the replication sample (with a Pvalue of �0.05). Each of these analy-ses is described in greater detailbelow.

Development of Composite Scores.The small sample size precluded for-mal factor analysis as a method toarrive at composite scores. Therefore,a combination of a priori reasoningand Spearman’s correlation was used.Scores from different tasks weregrouped together in a preliminaryfashion, based on similarities in taskdemands and scoring systems. Forexample, a number of different tasksemphasized speeded performance,suggesting that processing speedmight be a single factor to whichscores from multiple tasks could con-tribute. Spearman’s correlation ma-trices were then constructed amongthe potential members of a given fac-tor (based on the average of threeplacebo scores for each of the ten

pilot participants). The two scoresshowing the highest pair-wise corre-lation were then assigned to the fac-tor. Another score that had high cor-relations with each of the twoprevious scores was then identifiedand added to the factor if its averagecorrelation with each of the previousscores was at least 0.50. This processcontinued until no other score satis-fied this criterion. Individual scoresthat failed to “join” a factor were re-tained for individual analysis.

Once scores were assembled intofactors, composite factor scores weredeveloped. Again, a pair of scores foreach variable was computed for eachparticipant by averaging the threesessions for each drug condition.Then, the MP and placebo scoreswere ranked together for each vari-able in the factor. Because there wereoccasions in which data were missingfor a particular task for an individualparticipant, the raw ranks were di-vided by the maximal possible rankfor each variable (i.e., in most in-stances, the raw rank was divided by20 [2 drugs � 10 participants] for thepilot analysis, and 48 [2 drugs � 24participants] for the replication anal-ysis) so that the modified ranksranged from 0 to 1.0 in all instances.The composite score was then com-puted by averaging all the placeboranks for each factor variable for eachparticipant and averaging all the MPranks for each factor variable for eachparticipant, resulting in a pair ofscores (MP, placebo) for each partic-ipant for a factor. A total of 13 suchfactors were created and assessed fordrug effects.

Because average ranks were usedin the final analyses of drug effects,the raw scoring system used in anygiven task had no effect, and this dataanalytic approach implicitly weightedeach contributing score equally inthe determination of the overall rank.Participants who were missing scoresfor a given task were always missingscores for both drug conditions. Thecorrection mentioned above, dividing

by the ranks by the number of par-ticipants, ensured that tasks withfewer participants’ data would nothave excessive weighting (by assum-ing higher ranks simply because offewer possible ranks).

Preliminary Assessment of Drug Ef-fects from the Pilot Sample. Wil-coxon signed rank test was performedseparately on the pairs of compositefactor scores and on the remainingindividual scores from the initial tenpilot participants. These analyseswere done in their exact form (asopposed to their asymptotic approxi-mations) because of the small samplesize. Those factor and individualscores in which the calculated Pvalue was �0.20 were used to derivea priori hypotheses for the larger rep-lication sample.

Final Analysis of Drug Effects Usingthe Replication Sample. Compositefactor scores were computed as de-scribed above on the remaining 24participants. Drug effects were thenassessed as above, using Wilcoxonsigned rank test, limited to those fac-tors and individual scores that metthe P-value cutoff from the pilot sam-ple. Asymptotic approximations wereused for the replication analysis be-cause of the larger sample size. Ininstances in which the results werestatistically significant at an alphalevel of 0.05, the data from all 34participants were combined to calcu-late descriptive statistics and effectsizes to provide the most precise es-timates of the magnitude of the drugeffect. This combining was done onlyafter the effect in the pilot samplewas confirmed independently in thereplication sample. Note that theseeffect sizes were computed paramet-rically (Cohen’s D for within-partici-pant treatment effects � mean ofindividual differences between treat-ment conditions/standard deviationof individual differences betweentreatment conditions),44 despite ourreliance on nonparametric inferential

412 Whyte et al. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. ● Vol. 83, No. 6

statistical testing. Thus, the effectsizes may not correspond precisely tothe P values, despite identical samplesizes, because data outliers have dif-ferent effects on the two calculations.

Effect of Missing Data. Missing datafrom the individual laboratory ses-sions were always because of im-pairments that prevented a partici-pant from performing a particulartask. For example, one participantwas unable to learn to perform go/no-go tasks, despite performingmany other study tasks, includingthe choice reaction time task, ade-quately. These impairments led tothe participant’s omission fromboth drug conditions. Missing datafrom classroom observation oc-curred more frequently becausemost participants performed theirindividual laboratory sessions dur-ing one of the four daily classroomdata collection sessions. Again,these participants’ data were miss-ing from both drug conditions for agiven observation time period.Some participants missed addi-tional scattered days of classroomobservation because of conflictingappointments, illness, or other con-flicts, but all participants had datafrom each study week. These ab-sences seemed to have been randomin nature. Thus, although missingdata reduced the effective samplesize and, hence, statistical power,for certain variables, it should nothave biased the results. Wheneverthe sample for analysis was incom-plete, we assessed qualitativelywhether the obtained P value wasclose to the designated thresholdand, thus, whether we might havecommitted a type II error for thatvariable. This was not a significantissue.

RESULTS

Assessment of the Effects of MP. Theprocedure described above resultedin the creation of 13 factor scores and

left 13 individual scores that were notpart of any of the calculated factors.One of the individual scores, the ele-vator counting task from the Test ofEveryday Attention, was deleted be-cause of ceiling effects in both drugconditions. The scores that were partof each factor, the results of the pilotdata analysis, and where appropriate,the results of the replication analysisand calculated effect sizes are shownin Table 2. Descriptive statistics forthose variables that showed signifi-cant drug effects are shown in Table3.

As shown in Table 2, a numberof factors showed no apparent drugeffect in the pilot analysis and werenot evaluated further. In addition,two factors that seemed promisingfrom the pilot analysis failed to rep-licate. These included the factor re-flecting initial accuracy on two ofthe computerized tasks and the fac-tor reflecting initial response rateson three of the computerized tasks.Three factors did, however, showstatistically significant drug effectsin both the pilot analysis and thereplication.

An initial speed factor, composedof the speed scores from eight differ-ent tasks, showed a statistically sig-nificant positive effect of MP in boththe pilot and replication analyses, andthis was the most robust drug effectdetected. The variables contributingto this factor included not only com-puterized reaction time tasks, butalso speeded paper-and-pencil tasks, ameasure of work productivity, and ameasure of mental processing speed,suggesting that the effects on speedare seen across a wide range of tasksand range from impairment to activ-ity levels. The effect sizes rangedfrom negligible to medium, depend-ing on the task. Only one of the eighttasks showed an effect size in thenegative direction (slower perfor-mance while receiving MP), and thiseffect size was very close to zero.

Caregiver ratings were also sig-nificantly improved by MP, despite

the fact that caregivers’ only oppor-tunity to observe participants duringpeak drug effects occurred on Satur-days. Whereas the Rating Scale of At-tentional Behavior items emphasizespeed, the items on the CognitiveFailures Questionnaire include a va-riety of types of attentional lapses,and the drug effect sizes were quitecomparable for both measures. Of in-terest, differences in the ratings onthese same measures by the projectstaff did not achieve statisticalsignificance.

The third factor that was posi-tively affected by MP was on-task be-havior, in both the classroom envi-ronment’s individual work sessions,and the inattentive behavior task’svideotape coding. Interestingly, thisfactor combines the frequency oftime-sampled classroom ratings thatwere coded as off task with the aver-age duration of individual off-task ep-isodes recorded on videotape. The ef-fect sizes again were in the small tomedium range. Note that MP seemedto have a larger effect on off-task be-havior in the afternoon than in themorning. This result did not seem tobe caused by ceiling effects in themorning on placebo because the af-ternoon effect was larger even in asubset of patients whose morningand afternoon off-task behavior onplacebo was matched (data notshown). The frequency of off-task ep-isodes from the videotaped recordscontributed to a separate factor thatwas not significantly affected by thestudy drug. Off-task behavior in theclassroom during group activities,similarly, did not show a significantdrug effect.

Of the 12 individual scores thatdid not contribute to the above fac-tors, four met the screening cutoff forthe pilot sample. However, only thereaction time before errors of com-mission from the Sustained Attentionto Response Task showed a signifi-cant drug effect in the replicationsample. On the one hand, this is con-sistent with the general effect of MP

June 2004 Effects of Methylphenidate After TBI 413

on speed of performance. However, ingeneral, accelerating response timesduring the Sustained Attention to Re-

sponse Task are interpreted as indi-cating an automatic, or inattentive,mode of responding.

Does MP Improve Speed at the Ex-pense of Quality? Because of thetechnical problem with the speed/ac-

TABLE 3Descriptive statistics for performance variables showing significant drug effects

Factor Name Task na

Minimum/Mean/Median/Maximum

Methylphenidate Placebo

Initial speed Perceptually simple visual go/no-go (50% targets), responsetime in msecs

33 416 390

804 797695 713

1922 1660

Perceptually simple visual go/no-go (20% targets), responsetime in msecs

33 355 421

775 789696 778

1760 1517

Visual go/no-go task (50% targets) with salient movingdistracters, response time in msecs no distractor condition

33 438 455

745 791663 697

1695 1739

Choice, response time (slope coefficient) 34 33 76387 510230 321

2769 2012

Dual task, baseline response time in msecs 33 265 287449 457391 396

1111 1129

Test of Everyday Attention map search, no. of symbolscircled in 2 mins

33 12.0 10.7

45.8 44.849.3 42.377.7 77.0

Test of Everyday Attention, telephone search, time per targetin secs

33 1.87 1.94

5.73 6.314.82 4.75

15.25 19.84

Inattentive behavior task, no. of items sorted 34 122 104484 458478 433916 856

Family ratings Cognitive Failures Questionnaire 32 0.00 0.0010.89 11.9912.33 12.8323.00 22.00

Rating Scale of Attentional Behavior 32 0.00 0.0013.48 16.1313.67 17.532.33 31.67

aSample sizes vary from the full sample of 34 because of individual participants’ inability to perform certain tasks and thescheduling of laboratory tasks for some participants during classroom time.

414 Whyte et al. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. ● Vol. 83, No. 6

curacy tradeoff task, described above,a subset of ten participants was iden-tified whose raw response time distri-butions had no more than 15% oftrials slower than 1750 msecs in ei-ther drug condition. This criterionwas based on preliminary visual in-spection of response time distribu-tions that seemed to be complete(i.e., not abruptly truncated at 2000msecs). Thus, these tended to be thefaster participants overall. For theseindividuals, the drug-effect sizes onspeed and accuracy were both small(0.26 and 0.21, respectively) but wereboth in the direction of improvementwith MP. Considering the flawed dataon the whole sample, the effect onspeed was small (0.21) and on accu-

racy was medium (0.52), but again,both effects are in the direction ofimprovement with MP. Thus, there isno evidence from this task that thefaster processing associated with MPis associated with lowered accuracyor quality. Similarly, in the SustainedAttention to Response Task, individ-uals tend to adopt an automatic orinattentive mode of processing. Asthey do so, their response times ontargets tend to become faster andfaster until they ultimately make anerror of commission to a nontarget.40

Control participants tend to slowdown on their next trial, as they rec-ognize their error. Our participantswith TBI did have faster responsetimes before their errors of commis-

sion with MP than placebo, but theydid not make more errors of commis-sion, suggesting that this more rapidresponding was not evidence of less-ened attentional monitoring.

Success of Blinding. Participants’ av-erage ratings of their drug conditionassessments during placebo and MPweeks were compared via Wilcoxonsigned rank test. The median ratingsdid differ between drug conditions(MP: median � 2.5, range � 1–4;placebo: median � 3.0, range �1–4.67; P � 0.03, where 1 � cer-tainty they were taking the activestudy drug and 5 � certainty theywere taking the placebo). Althoughthe difference between the mean MP

TABLE 3continued

Factor Name Task na

Minimum/Mean/Median/Maximum

Methylphenidate Placebo

Inattention–individual

Morning classroom–individual activities, % ontask

26 65.3 73.5

93.6 92.097.0 96.499.5 100.0

Afternoon classroom–individual activities, % ontask

24 79.1 62.6

96.5 90.898.3 94.6

100.0 100.0

Inattentive behavior–task 1, average duration ofoff-task episodes in secs

34 0.42 0.47

2.72 3.691.49 1.83

14.01 26.63

Inattentive behavior–task 2, average duration ofoff-task episodes in secs

32 0.26 0.41

1.25 1.471.16 1.193.73 8.59

Inattentive behavior–task 3, average duration ofoff-task episodes in secs

30 0.25 0.72

1.52 3.481.06 1.904.74 17.14

Individualscore

Sustained Attention to Response Task, responsetime before commission error in msecs

34 259 259

400 410380 390680 713

June 2004 Effects of Methylphenidate After TBI 415

and placebo ratings is statisticallysignificant, the actual differences arequite small, and in both conditions,the average ratings were in the direc-tion of thinking that they were takingthe active study drug. Inspection ofindividual data indicated that therewas only one participant who wasconsistently accurate in his drugself-assessments.

DISCUSSION

The results of this study arelargely consistent with previous re-search from our laboratory31 andfrom research in other popula-tions,25,45–47 demonstrating that MPhas reliable positive effects on perfor-mance speed across a range of tasksin a postacute adult TBI population.This effect is probably exerted pri-marily on cognitive speed, as judgedby the improvement on the choicereaction time task, for which the mo-tor response is identical across blockswith different numbers of choices.The magnitude of this effect variedfrom task to task, with small to me-dium effect sizes. In a naturalisticwork task (i.e., object sorting), ap-proximately 6% more work was ac-complished with MP in a 15-min in-terval. Although there were technicalproblems with one of the tasks de-signed to assess speed/accuracytradeoffs, it did not seem, from thistask or from the Sustained Attentionto Response Task, that the improve-ment in speed occurred at the ex-pense of accuracy. Given that slowedprocessing is one of the most widelyreported and disabling impairmentsresulting from TBI, a positive effect ofMP in this domain may have consid-erable clinical significance. The clin-ical relevance of this aspect of MP isfurther supported by the fact thatperformance was improved on a widerange of tasks that shared demandson cognitive speed, from computer-ized laboratory tasks to simulatedwork activities. Improved speed ofprocessing with MP also may have

contributed to the higher attentive-ness ratings of caregivers.

In children with ADHD, MP isreported to improve on-task behav-ior. No such effect was identified inour previous TBI pilot study,31 andthis study provided somewhat mixedresults. To interpret these effects, onemust first consider the breakdown ofthe factors that pertain to attentive-ness in the classroom and inattentivebehavior task. As mentioned previ-ously, variables that pertain to theduration of off-task episodes in thevideotaped records did not correlatewith the frequency of such events inthe same records, but they did corre-late with the proportion of off-taskobservations in the classroom. Thevideotape coding attempted to cap-ture each off-task event that oc-curred, regardless of its duration,whereas the time-sampling methodused in the classroom merely pro-vided “spot checks” of whether par-ticipants were on task or off task atrandom moments. Thus, it is possiblethat longer off-task events occurringin the classroom were more likely tobe captured by this sampling method.If so, this factor might more accu-rately be thought of as assessing theduration of these off-task episodesrather than their frequency. Underthis account, it is quite possible thatMP’s effects are primarily on howrapidly participants return to task,once off, rather than on the numberof such events, per se. Thus, the factthat no drug effect was seen on off-task event frequency on the video-taped records would not be surpris-ing. Although MP did seem todecrease the counts (perhaps by de-creasing their duration) of off-taskbehavior in individual classroom ses-sions, no such effect was seen ingroup sessions. It is possible that theduration of off-task behaviors duringindividual work tasks is determinedprimarily by the participant’s internalgoal state, whereas those same behav-iors during group sessions may bemore related to the nature and sa-

lience of distracting behaviors pro-duced by other group members. If so,it would not be surprising for MP tohave more robust effects on theformer than the latter. It is also ofinterest that MP affected on-taskclassroom behaviors more strongly inthe afternoon than in the morningsessions. In children with ADHD, MPseems to improve cognitive speedparticularly on effortful tasks.25 Pos-sibly, maintaining one’s attentionduring a mild diurnal “slump” is par-ticularly enhanced by MP because itis more effortful than paying atten-tion to the same activity in themorning.

Of particular importance, MPproduced substantial improvementsin caregiver ratings of attentivenesson two different measures, an aspectof the drug not assessed in our pre-vious research. This is somewhat sur-prising because one would expect theeffects of MP to be waning by thetime participants returned home onweekdays, and caregivers only spent 1day/wk (Saturdays) with the partici-pants when they were at peak druglevels. It is difficult to know preciselywhat changes the caregivers observedto compose these ratings. The resultsof the laboratory studies, however,may suggest that improved process-ing speed contributes to improve-ments in a variety of domains. Alter-natively, drug effects not measured inthis study, such as improved mood ordecreased irritability, may have con-tributed to caregivers’ perceptions ofpositive drug benefits, although theassessment questions did not pertainto these dimensions. Alternatively, ifthe study was not successfullyblinded, and participants and caregiv-ers communicated about the per-ceived drug condition, this couldhave contributed to bias in the care-giver ratings. However, most partici-pants were close to chance in guess-ing the correct drug condition,making this explanation less likely.The lack of drug effects on self-rat-ings is likely attributable to some

416 Whyte et al. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. ● Vol. 83, No. 6

combination of impairment in partic-ipants’ memory for their perfor-mance over days and weeks and totheir reduced self-awareness. Regard-less of its source, however, it doessuggest that participants remainedreasonably well blinded to their drugcondition.

The fact that staff ratings did notshow effects of MP but that caregiverratings did may relate to the moreintimate effect on caregivers of theparticipants’ usual behaviors and,hence, their greater sensitivity to be-havioral changes. Differences in theperception of deficits and their effectby professional staff and caregiversare often noted in the literature. Thecurrent data suggest that caregiverratings may be particularly importantin assessing real-world treatmenteffectiveness.

Our previous study31 also foundthat the decline in responding seenduring a sustained attention task wasblunted by MP, an effect that was notreplicated in this study. This differ-ence may simply have occurred bychance, given the number of perfor-mance measures assessed in the pre-vious study, or may be attributable tobroader impairment across a range ofaspects of attention at a more acutestage of recovery. The previous studyalso found a reduction of the effect ofexternal distracters on measures ofaccuracy. However, it seemed thatthat result relied on faster processingof the target, before the onset of thedistracter (i.e., another manifestationof faster processing with MP). In thecurrent study, modifications in dis-tracter timing may have eliminatedthat effect.

Although MP’s effects were sta-tistically and clinically significant ina number of performance domains,the effect sizes ranged from small tomedium, with absolute improve-ments ranging from 5% to 25%. Thisis in contrast to a study of adults withADHD by Spencer et al.26 in whichrating scale improvements rangedfrom 17% to 55%. There are a num-

ber of differences between studiesthat might account for the differ-ences in magnitude of treatment ef-fects. It is certainly possible thatadults with ADHD respond more dra-matically to MP than do those withTBI, particularly because improve-ments in hyperactivity (55%) and im-pulsivity (54%), symptom clustersthat are not prominent in TBI, weregreater in the ADHD study than wereimprovements in inattentiveness(17%). It is also possible that the out-come measures used in the Spenceret al.26 study are more sensitive todrug effects than those used in thisresearch. However, because of thedifferent presentation of ADHD vs.TBI-related inattentiveness, thescales used by Spencer et al.26 wouldnot be appropriate in this population.Finally, the difference may be attrib-utable to differences in dosage. Spen-cer et al. used 1.0 mg/kg/day, dividedinto three doses, whereas we used 0.6mg/kg/day (0.3 mg/kg/dose), dividedinto two doses. Although the dailydose administered in the Spencer etal.26 study was dramatically largerthan ours, individual doses were sim-ilar (0.3 mg/kg/dose vs. 0.33 mg/kg/dose). Given the short duration ofaction of MP, one would expect thesedifferent dosing regimens to affectduration of the effect more than mag-nitude, particularly for those short-duration tasks that were assessednear the time of peak drug levels.Nevertheless, it is possible that largereffects would have been seen in therating scale data with three-times-per-day dosing. Given the absence ofserious adverse events, it would be ofinterest to assess whether larger ef-fect sizes could be obtained in pa-tients with TBI by escalating thedoses of MP used.

A number of limitations in thisstudy must be kept in mind in inter-preting its findings. The study samplemight be most accurately termed an“inconvenience sample” in the sensethat a very large number of potentialparticipants had to be screened to iden-

tify the 34 participants who met theinclusion and exclusion criteria andwho were willing to commit 6 wks oftheir time. Thus, individuals who hadreturned to employment or were tooimpaired to travel, or those with pre-morbid neurologic deficits or currentpsychoactive drug treatment, were un-derrepresented in the sample. More-over, because of the time elapsed be-tween injury and enrollment for manyparticipants, the details of their initialneuropathology were often unavail-able, making it difficult to assess vari-ations in hypoxic/ischemic injury andother factors that might have affectedtreatment response. On the otherhand, in routine clinical practice, oneis often called on to treat patients inthe chronic stage whose initial histo-ries are similarly unobtainable, requir-ing the physician to tailor treatment tothe current functional status ratherthan the original injury. Although gen-eralization to a broader population ofindividuals with TBI is limited by thisselection and data collection process,we believe that the types of drug effectsseen here can be plausibly expected inother individuals with TBI. In addition,the goal of this study was less to makedefinitive recommendations regardingclinical use of MP than to help under-stand the mechanisms by which thisdrug might modify cognitive functionafter TBI. It seems likely that the fun-damental effects of MP on attentionalprocesses are likely to be fairly general.Moreover, by identifying a smaller sub-set of domains in which MP effects arelikely to be seen, this study should fa-cilitate the conduct of a more stream-lined clinical trial in a broader sample,with data collection limited to a fewvariables likely to be responsive. Alarger clinical trial of this type will ad-dress more definitively the issue ofgenerality of treatment response andwhether individual patient characteris-tics substantially modify this response.

Another limitation is the use of asingle weight-adjusted MP dose forall participants rather than conduct-ing individualized dosage titration to

June 2004 Effects of Methylphenidate After TBI 417

the optimal dose for each participantor testing each participant fully onseveral different doses. This decisionwas made largely for practical rea-sons. In our experience, individualdosage titration is very difficult be-cause of the substantial variability inperformance at any particular doseand the presence of confoundingpractice effects, not to mention theuncertainty regarding the measureagainst which to optimize the dose.Testing each participant at two orthree different doses would havelengthened the testing protocol to aninfeasible degree. Thus, our goal herewas to establish a restricted set ofvariables that can be used to assessMP response. These variables canthen be used more efficiently to studythe response to different drug doses.This approach cannot, however, com-pletely rule out the possibility thatother behavioral domains thatshowed no treatment effect at thisdose might show such an effect at adifferent dose.

Finally, the complex statisticalmethod used may have resulted ineither type I or type II error. Cer-tainly, the fact that a given score orfactor does not meet a probabilitylevel cutoff of 0.20 in a sample of tenparticipants does not mean that itcould not show a drug effect in alarger sample. Thus, we may havesacrificed the opportunity to detectsome treatment effects (particularlysmall ones) by failing to further as-sess those variables that did not meetthe pilot screening cutoff. In addi-tion, that different factors containeddifferent numbers of raw scores mayhave caused variations in sensitivityto detect drug effects. To the extentthat the data are variable or “noisy,”collapsing more scores into a singlefactor tends to improve the signal-to-noise ratio. Thus, one might expectthis method to be slightly less sensi-tive in detecting drug effects on thosefactors that have fewer score “mem-bers.” Whereas it is true that thespeed factor had the largest number

of contributing variables and themost robust treatment effect, thecaregiver rating factor had only twomembers, yet it proved highly signif-icant. With respect to type I error,this two-stage method of analysismakes is more difficult to determineprecisely the odds that a variablecould reach statistical significance bychance. However, it is worth notingthat the direction of effect for all ofthe variables that met the screeningcutoff was in the direction of MP ben-efit (with the exception of one speedfactor that had essentially a zero ef-fect size), an occurrence that wouldbe extremely unlikely by chance.

With these limitations in mind,the current findings do support thenotion that MP has a beneficial effecton cognitive processing speed aftermoderate to severe TBI. This effect,and possibly other specific benefits ofMP, translates not only into fasterperformance on laboratory measuresof attention, but also the ability toattend to naturalistic tasks or returnmore quickly to them after lapses ofattention. Although MP did not affectseveral facets of attention measuredin the current study, there was ahighly significant effect on everydayattentional behavior as assessed byrelatives very familiar with the day-to-day effect of attention deficits onour participants with chronic TBI.

CONCLUSION

MP, in a dose of 0.3 mg/kg twicea day, seems to have clear and con-sistent positive effects on speed ofprocessing and caregiver ratings ofattentiveness in a highly selectedsample of individuals with moderateto severe TBI. The effects on inatten-tiveness are more complex and maybe traceable to shorter duration ofoff-task behaviors without a reduc-tion in their frequency. Effects onsustained attention were not evident,in contrast to our previous pilotstudy, nor did MP improve dual taskperformance, in contrast to a previ-

ous study of bromocriptine.48 Posi-tive drug effects covered a range fromimpairment to activity and participa-tion measures. Further clinical trialson larger and more representativesamples of individuals with TBI arewarranted and should benefit fromthese results in selecting their pri-mary outcomes. Similarly, studiescomparing different doses of MPcould help shed light on the optimaldose for specific target goals.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank the many individualswho assisted in the research: JosephAlban, Natosha Bailey, ChristopherGantz, Monica Hopson, and VonettaDrakes managed participant recruit-ment, testing, and behavioral observa-tion; Adelyn Brecher, Anne Hammond,Karen Hawkey, Andrea Laborde, WalterLewis, Nathaniel Mayer, Jeanne Pelen-sky, and Rosadele Plumari all helped toidentify and refer eligible participants;Kelly Card and Vivian Ly facilitated theresearch classroom; Thouron Smithprovided volunteer assistance in thelaboratory and classroom; StephenHinshaw suggested the model for theresearch classroom and providedmethodological consultation; GuylaineGagnon, Mary Kennedy, and RonaldCharno managed the blinded random-ization code and the preparation ofstudy medications; Antonella Paveseand Tracy Veramonti programmed thecomputerized laboratory tasks; Fei Shaand Gemma Baldon programmed theresearch database; and Mary Czerniakassisted in the preparation of themanuscript; and most importantly, wethank the participants and their fami-lies for investing their time and energyin this study.

REFERENCES

1. Capruso DX, Levin HS: Neurobehav-ioral sequelae of head injury, in CooperPR, Golfinos JG (eds): Head Injury. NewYork, McGraw-Hill, 2000, pp 525–53

2. van Zomeren AH, Brouwer WH: Clin-

418 Whyte et al. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. ● Vol. 83, No. 6

ical Neuropsychology of Attention. NewYork, Oxford University Press, 1994

3. Mateer CM, Mapou RL: Understanding,evaluating, and managing attention dis-orders after traumatic brain injury.J Head Trauma Rehabil 1996;11:1–16

4. Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assess-ment, ed 3. New York, Oxford UniversityPress, 1995

5. Kraus JF, Sorenson SB: Epidemiology,in Silver JM, Yudofsky SC, Hales RE (eds):Neuropsychiatry of Traumatic Brain In-jury. Washington, DC, American Psychi-atric Press, 1994

6. Prigatano GP: Principles of Neuropsy-chological Rehabilitation. New York, Ox-ford University Press, 1999

7. Rehabilitation of persons with trau-matic brain injury. NIH Consensus State-ments 1998;16:1–41

8. Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Kalmar K,et al: Evidence-based cognitive rehabili-tation: Recommendations for clinicalpractice. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2000;81:1596–1615

9. Whyte J, Vaccaro M, Grieb-Neff P, etal: Psychostimulant use in the rehabilita-tion of individuals with traumatic braininjury. J Head Trauma Rehabil 2002;17:284–99

10. Whyte J, Hart T, Laborde A, et al:Rehabilitation of the patient with trau-matic brain injury, in DeLisa J (ed): Re-habilitation Medicine: Principles andPractice. Philadelphia, Lippincott, 1998,pp 1191–1239

11. Ponsford J, Kinsella G: The use of arating scale of attentional behavior. Neu-ropsychol Rehabil 1991;1:241–57

12. Whyte J, Polansky M, Fleming M, etal: Sustained arousal and attention aftertraumatic brain injury. Neuropsycholo-gia 1995;33:797–813

13. Whyte J, Fleming M, Polansky M, etal: The effects of visual distraction follow-ing traumatic brain injury. J Int Neuro-psychol Soc 1998;4:126–36

14. Stuss DT, Stethem LL, HugenholtzH, et al: Reaction time after head injury:Fatigue, divided and focused attention,and consistency of performance. J NeurolNeurosurg Psychiatry 1989;52:742–8

15. McDowell SK, Whyte J, D’Esposito M:Working memory impairments in trau-matic brain injury: Evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Neuropsychologia 1997;35:1341–53

16. Whyte J, Schuster K, Polansky M, etal: Frequency and duration of inattentive

behavior after traumatic brain injury: Ef-fects of distraction, task and practice.J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2000;6:1–11

17. Mirsky AF, Anthony BJ, Duncan CC,et al: Analysis of the elements ofattention: A neuropsychological ap-proach. Neuropsychol Rev 1991;2:109–45

18. Whyte J: Distinctive methodologicchallenges, in Fuhrer M (ed): AssessingMedical Rehabilitation Practices: ThePromise of Outcomes Research. Balti-more, Paul H. Brookes, 1997, pp 43–59

19. Park NW, Ingles JL: Effectiveness ofattention rehabilitation after an acquiredbrain injury: A meta-analysis. Neuropsy-chology 2001;15:199–210

20. Pliszka S: Child and adolescentpsychopharmacology: The use of psycho-stimulants in the pediatric patient. Pedi-atric Clin North Am 1998;45:1085–99

21. Conners CK: Forty years of methyl-phenidate treatment in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Atten Disord2002;6(Suppl 1):S17–30

22. Birmaher B, Greenhill LL, CooperMA, et al: Sustained release methyl-phenidate: Pharmacokinetic studies inADDH males. J Am Acad Child AdolescPsychiatry 1989;28:768–72

23. Hoffman BB: Catecholamines, sym-pathomimetic drugs, and adrenergic re-ceptor antagonists, in Hardman JG, Lim-bird LE (eds): The Pharmacological Basisof Therapeutics. New York, McGraw-Hill,2001

24. Scheres A, Oosterlaan J, Swanson J,et al: The effect of methylphenidate onthree forms of response inhibition in boyswith AD/HD. J Abnorm Child Psychol2003;31:105–20

25. Bedard AC, Ickowicz A, Tannock R:Methylphenidate improves Stroop nam-ing speed, but not responsive interferencein children with attention deficit hyper-activity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psycho-pharmacol 2002;12:301–9

26. Spencer T, Wilens T, Biederman J, etal: A double-blind crossover comparisonof methylphenidate and placebo in adultswith childhood-onset attention-deficithyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychi-atry 1995;52:434–43

27. Francisco G, Ivanhoe C: Successfultreatment of post-traumatic narcolepsywith methylphenidate: A case report.Am J Phys Med Rehabil 1996;75:63–5

28. Mooney GF, Haas LJ: Effect of meth-ylphenidate on brain injury-related an-

ger. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1993;74:153–60

29. Speech TJ, Rao SM, Osmon DC, et al:A double-blind controlled study of meth-ylphenidate treatment in closed head in-jury. Brain Inj 1993;7:333–8

30. Williams SE, Ris MD, Ayyanger R, etal: Recovery in pediatric brain injury: Ispsychostimulant medication beneficial?J Head Trauma Rehabil 1998;13:73–81

31. Whyte J, Hart T, Schuster K, et al:The effects of methylphenidate on atten-tional function after traumatic braininjury: A randomized, placebo-controlledtrial. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 1997;76:440–50

32. Gualtieri C, Evans R: Stimulanttreatment for the neurobehavioural se-quelae of traumatic brain injury. BrainInj 1988;2:273–90

33. Mahalick DM, Carmel PW, GreenbergJP, et al: Psychopharmacologic treatmentof acquired attention disorders in chil-dren with brain injury. Pediatr Neuro-surg 1998;29:121–6

34. Evans RW, Gualtieri CT, Patterson D:Treatment of chronic closed head injurywith psychostimulant drugs: A controlledcase study and an appropriate evaluationprocedure. J Nerv Ment Dis 1987;175:106–9

35. McMillan TM, Jongen ELMM, Green-wood RJ: Assessment of post-traumaticamnesia after severe closed head injury:Retrospective or prospective? J NeurolNeurosurg Psychiatry 1996;60:422–7

36. Rappaport M, Hall KM, Hopkins K, etal: Disability rating scale for severe headtrauma: Coma to community. Arch PhysMed Rehabil 1982;63:118–23

37. Barona A, Reynolds CR, Chastain R: Ademographically based index of pre-mor-bid intelligence for the WAIS-R. J ConsultClin Psychol 1984;52:885–7

38. Alban JP, Hopson MM, Ly V, et al:Effect of methylphenidate on vital signsand adverse effects in adults with trau-matic brain injury. Am J Phys Med Reha-bil 2004;83:131–7

39. Posner MI: Chronometric Explora-tions of Mind. New York, Oxford Univer-sity Press, 1986

40. Robertson IH, Manly T, Andrade J, etal: “Oops!”: Performance correlates of ev-eryday attentional failures in traumaticbrain injured and normal subjects. Neu-ropsychologia 1997;35:747–58

41. Robertson IH, Ward T, Ridgeway V, etal: The structure of normal human

June 2004 Effects of Methylphenidate After TBI 419

attention: The test of everyday attention.J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1996;2:525–34

42. Whyte J, Polansky M, Cavallucci C, etal: Inattentive behavior after traumaticbrain injury. J Int Neuropsychol Soc1996;2:274–81

43. Broadbent DE, Cooper PF, FitzGeraldP, et al: The cognitive failures question-naire (CFQ) and its correlates. Br J ClinPsychol 1982;21:1–16

44. Portney LG, Watkins MP: Foundationsof clinical research, in: Applications to

Practice: Appendix C. Power Analysis andDetermination of Sample Size. New York,Appleton and Lange, 1993, pp 651–7

45. Carlson CL, Pelham WE, SwansonJM, et al: A divided attention analysis ofthe effects of methylphenidate on thearithmetic performance of children withattention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1991;32:463–71

46. Reid MK, Borkowski JG: Effects ofmethylphenidate (Ritalin) on information

processing in hyperactive children. Ab-norm Child Psychol 1984;12:169–85

47. Coons HW, Peloquin LJ, Klorman R,et al: Effect of methylphenidate on youngadult’s vigilance and event-related poten-tials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neuro-physiol 1981;51:373–87

48. McDowell S, Whyte J, D’Esposito M:Differential effect of a dopaminergic ago-nist on prefrontal function in traumaticbrain injury patients. Brain 1998;121:1155–64

Letters to the Editor

The American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation welcomes letters to the Editor that objectivelycritique information appearing in the Journal. Letters should be limited to a maximum of 500 words (approximatelytwo double-spaced pages). No unsigned letters will be accepted for publication. If a letter is accepted for publication,a copy will be sent to the principal author of the material being discussed, who will have an opportunity to submita response for publication. All letters are subject to editing by the Editor.

420 Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. ● Vol. 83, No. 6


Recommended