+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Foot-thumping as an alarm signal in macropodoid marsupials: prevalence and hypotheses of function

Foot-thumping as an alarm signal in macropodoid marsupials: prevalence and hypotheses of function

Date post: 10-May-2023
Category:
Upload: independent
View: 0 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
18
Mammal Rev . 2006, Volume 36, No. 4, 281–298. Printed in Singapore. © 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298 Blackwell Publishing LtdOxford, UKMAMMammal Review0305-1838Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 2006 ? 2006364281298Review ArticleFoot-thumping in macropodoidsT. A. Rose et al. Correspondence: T. A. Rose. E-mail: [email protected] Foot-thumping as an alarm signal in macropodoid marsupials: prevalence and hypotheses of function TANIA A. ROSE*, ADAM J. MUNN†, DANIEL RAMP* and PETER B. BANKS* *School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2052 and Institute of Wildlife Research, School of Biological Sciences, A08, University of Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia ABSTRACT 1. Alarm signalling as a means to reduce predation risk is an important component of the behavioural repertoire of many species. It has previously been noted that many of the macropodoid marsupials (kangaroos, wallabies and rat-kangaroos) produce a foot-thump, an audible signal created by striking the ground with one or both feet, that is most likely an alarm signal. 2. The prevalence of foot-thumping within the macropodoids and hypotheses of its function as an alarm signal have been poorly documented. To address this issue, we investigate the prevalence of foot-thumping in macropodoids and interpret possible function according to current alarm signalling theory. Evidence for foot-thumping was found in almost all macropodoids. In light of this, the behaviour appears to be a conservative trait that may have arisen alongside or followed the evolution of bipedal locomotion, and suggests that this trait carries significant benefits that transcend ecological and predation differences among species. 3. Nine alarm signal hypotheses were explored in order to determine the function of foot- thumping in macropodoid marsupials. However, the existing evidence for consistent function remains inconclusive. Therefore, a series of predictions were developed to provide the foun- dation for future research to investigate more thoroughly the function of foot-thumping in macropodoid marsupials. Keywords: altruism, kangaroo, Macropodidae, Macropodoidea, Potoroidae, predator–prey, wallaby, warning INTRODUCTION Many prey animals have developed anti-predator strategies to mitigate the risk of predation; some reduce visibility using cryptic colouration or reduced activity, whereas others rely on the dilution of risk through grouping or on the early detection of predators through behav- iours such as vigilance. In contrast, alarm signalling is a particularly conspicuous anti- predator strategy. Unlike concealment strategies, alarm signalling direct to predators or to neighbours may remove any chance of concealment. Alarm signals may be olfactory, visual or acoustic, and are found in a great variety of species (e.g. Hamilton, 1963; Maynard-Smith, 1965; Estes & Goddard, 1967; Trivers, 1971; Hasson, 1991; Caro et al., 2004). Macropodoid marsupials (order: Diprotodontia; superfamily: Macropodoidea) are a radiation of mammals that includes kangaroos, wallabies (family: Macropodidae) and
Transcript

Mammal Rev

. 2006, Volume 36, No. 4, 281–298.

Printed in Singapore

.

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society,

Mammal Review

,

36,

281–298

Blackwell Publishing LtdOxford, UKMAMMammal Review0305-1838Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 2006

? 2006

36

4281298

Review Article

Foot-thumping in macropodoidsT. A. Rose

et al.

Correspondence: T. A. Rose. E-mail: [email protected]

Foot-thumping as an alarm signal in macropodoid marsupials: prevalence and hypotheses of function

TANIA A. ROSE*, ADAM J. MUNN†, DANIEL RAMP* and PETER B. BANKS*

*

School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2052 and

Institute of Wildlife Research, School of Biological Sciences, A08,

University of Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia

ABSTRACT1.

Alarm signalling as a means to reduce predation risk is an important component of thebehavioural repertoire of many species. It has previously been noted that many of themacropodoid marsupials (kangaroos, wallabies and rat-kangaroos) produce a foot-thump,an audible signal created by striking the ground with one or both feet, that is most likely analarm signal.

2.

The prevalence of foot-thumping within the macropodoids and hypotheses of its functionas an alarm signal have been poorly documented. To address this issue, we investigate theprevalence of foot-thumping in macropodoids and interpret possible function according tocurrent alarm signalling theory. Evidence for foot-thumping was found in almost allmacropodoids. In light of this, the behaviour appears to be a conservative trait that may havearisen alongside or followed the evolution of bipedal locomotion, and suggests that this traitcarries significant benefits that transcend ecological and predation differences among species.

3.

Nine alarm signal hypotheses were explored in order to determine the function of foot-thumping in macropodoid marsupials. However, the existing evidence for consistent functionremains inconclusive. Therefore, a series of predictions were developed to provide the foun-dation for future research to investigate more thoroughly the function of foot-thumping inmacropodoid marsupials.

Keywords

: altruism, kangaroo, Macropodidae, Macropodoidea, Potoroidae, predator–prey,wallaby, warning

INTRODUCTION

Many prey animals have developed anti-predator strategies to mitigate the risk of predation;some reduce visibility using cryptic colouration or reduced activity, whereas others rely onthe dilution of risk through grouping or on the early detection of predators through behav-iours such as vigilance. In contrast, alarm signalling is a particularly conspicuous anti-predator strategy. Unlike concealment strategies, alarm signalling direct to predators or toneighbours may remove any chance of concealment. Alarm signals may be olfactory, visualor acoustic, and are found in a great variety of species (e.g. Hamilton, 1963; Maynard-Smith,1965; Estes & Goddard, 1967; Trivers, 1971; Hasson, 1991; Caro

et al

., 2004).Macropodoid marsupials (order: Diprotodontia; superfamily: Macropodoidea) are a

radiation of mammals that includes kangaroos, wallabies (family: Macropodidae) and

282

T. A. Rose

et al.

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society,

Mammal Review

,

36,

281–298

rat-kangaroos (family: Potoroidae). All extant macropodoids are exposed to the risk ofpredation at some stage of their lives and demonstrate various strategies of crypsis, conceal-ment, vigilance and grouping in order to reduce their risk of capture (Jarman & Coulson,1989). However, many macropodoid species also strike the ground with one or both hindfeet, producing an audible thump (Coulson, 1989), in response to a disturbance or approach-ing predator. This ‘foot-thumping’ by macropodoids is considered an acoustic form ofcommunication because it is visually inconspicuous, and it is considered by many to be adeliberate alarm signal (e.g. Croft, 1981; Blumstein

et al

., 2000, 2002a; Bender, 2005; reviewsby Coulson, 1989, 1996). Although a form of macropodoid foot-thumping may occur in othercontexts (e.g. in sexual contexts, see Coulson, 1989), most reports of the behaviour indicatethat it is more common in alarm contexts, and as such our specific interest in this review isto consider the behaviour within the framework of predator–prey interactions. Within thiscontext, it is unclear whether all species perform the behaviour and for whom the signal isintended: the predator, conspecifics, or both. Few studies have specifically investigated thefoot-thumping in macropodoids within a predator–prey context (but see Shepherd, 1981;Coulson, 1996; Blumstein

et al

., 2000, 2002a) and hence, it is not clear how macropodoidfoot-thumping fits with current alarm signal theories.

In this review, we investigate the occurrence of foot-thumping in the superfamilyMacropodoidea; exploring possible function in the context of other behavioural and ecolog-ical traits such as sociality, habitat and diets as they relate to predictions of the nine currenthypotheses on alarm signal function developed for other taxa. Each hypothesis is assessed inlight of documented studies of foot-thumping in macropodoid species, their varying ecologiesand relationships with key predators. Subsequently, a series of predictions are developed toenable the function of macropodoid foot-thumping to be explored experimentally, buildinga foundation for future research.

PREVALENCE AND ECOLOGY OF FOOT-THUMPING IN MACROPODOID MARSUPIALS

The superfamily Macropodoidea contains 50 Australian and 13 Papua New Guinean species(Strahan, 1995; Flannery, 1996). The macropodoids are distinguished from other marsupialsby their powerful hind limbs with elongated feet, weaker forelimbs and strong tail. Thesefeatures enable rapid economical locomotion via a distinctive hopping gait (Dawson, 1977),used by all but one monospecific genus, the musky rat-kangaroo

Hypsiprymnodon moschatus

(Burk, Westerman & Springer, 1998). There is considerable ecological diversity in the group,as their habitats vary from tropical rainforests to alpine tussock grasslands and arid inlandplains (Grigg, Jarman & Hume, 1989). Body sizes range from less than 500 g (musky rat-kangaroo) to over 85 kg (red kangaroo

Macropus rufus

) (Strahan, 1995). Diets also vary,from omnivorous to both browsing and grazing herbivores (Strahan, 1995). Somemacropodoids are strictly solitary, whereas others form tight social groups (Strahan, 1995).In other taxa, these factors (habitat, body size, diet, sociality) typically influence the functionof an alarm signal (e.g. see Taylor, Balph & Balph, 1990; Blumstein & Armitage, 1998).

Determining the prevalence of foot-thumping among the macropodoids is logisticallydifficult, largely because there are numerous species for which no documented behaviouralrepertoires exist, although Coulson (1989) reported foot-thumping in 18 species. Therefore,to assess its prevalence, 27 species specialists (research scientists and keepers of captivemacropodoids) were contacted to report on instances of foot-thumping in a given speciesbased upon our operational definition (see Tables 1–3). From these combined sources, foot-thumping was reported to occur in 46 macropodoid species and subspecies out of the 48 for

Foot-thumping in macropodoids

283

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society,

Mammal Review

,

36,

281–298

Tab

le 1

.

Spec

ies

of M

acro

podo

idea

kno

wn

to f

oot-

thum

p

Gen

usSp

ecie

sC

omm

on n

ame

Foo

t-th

umpi

ng

sour

ceB

ody

wei

ght*

†So

cial

ity*

Die

t*H

abit

at*

Act

ivit

y ti

me*

Aep

ypry

mnu

sru

fesc

ens

Ruf

ous

bett

ong

Cou

lson

(19

89)‡

To 3

.5 k

g (

);to

3.0

kg

(

)Sm

all,

loos

e po

lygy

nous

as

soci

atio

ns

Fru

it, i

nsec

ts,

fung

iR

ainf

ores

tD

iurn

al

Bet

tong

iaga

imar

di

Tas

man

ian

bett

ong

Cou

lson

(19

89)‡

1.7

kgSo

litar

ySe

eds,

roo

ts,

bulb

s,un

derg

roun

d fu

ngi

Ope

n fo

rest

Noc

turn

al

trop

ica

Nor

ther

n be

tton

gP.

Joh

nson

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

1.2

kgSo

litar

yF

ungi

, roo

tsO

pen

fore

stN

octu

rnal

peni

cilla

ta

Bru

sh-t

aile

d be

tton

g, W

oylie

B. S

mit

h (p

ers.

co

mm

.)1.

3 kg

Solit

ary

Fun

gi, b

ulbs

, tu

bers

,se

eds,

inse

cts,

re

sin

Ope

n fo

rest

, w

oodl

and

Noc

turn

al

Den

drol

agus

inus

tus

Gri

zzle

d tr

ee-

kang

aroo

U. G

ansl

osse

r (p

ers.

com

m.)

B. S

mit

h (p

ers.

co

mm

.)

9.0

kg (

);17

.9 k

g (

) (F

lann

ery,

199

6)

Unk

now

n(F

lann

ery,

199

6)L

eave

sR

ainf

ores

t(F

lann

ery,

199

6)C

repu

scul

ar(F

lann

ery,

199

6)

lum

holt

zi

Lum

holt

z’s

tree

-ka

ngar

ooB

. Sm

ith

(per

s.

com

m.)

6.0

kg (

);7.

5 kg

(

)So

litar

yL

eave

s, f

ruit

Rai

nfor

est

Noc

turn

al

mat

schi

ei

Huo

n tr

ee-k

anga

roo

J. B

less

ingt

on (

pers

. co

mm

.)9.

2 kg

(

);8.

4 kg

(

)(F

lann

ery,

199

6)

Solit

ary

(Fla

nner

y, 1

996)

Lea

ves

Rai

nfor

est

(Fla

nner

y, 1

996)

Unk

now

n (F

lann

ery,

199

6)

Dor

cops

islu

ctuo

sa

Gre

y do

rcop

sis

Men

zies

(19

89)

3.6

kg (

);11

.6 k

g (

)(B

ourk

e, 1

989)

Gre

gari

ous

(Bou

rke,

198

9)B

row

seF

ores

ts(M

enzi

es, 1

989)

Cre

pusc

ular

/no

ctur

nal

(Men

zies

, 198

9)

Lag

orch

este

sco

nspi

cilla

tus

Spec

tacl

ed

hare

-wal

laby

P. J

ohns

on (

pers

. co

mm

.)1.

6–4.

5 kg

Solit

ary,

but

up

to 3

in f

eedi

ng

aggr

egat

ions

Bro

wse

Tro

pica

l gra

ssla

nd,

open

for

est,

ope

nw

oodl

and,

tal

lsh

rubl

and

Noc

turn

al

hirs

utus

Ruf

ous

hare

-wal

laby

Cou

lson

(19

89)

1.7

kg (

);1.

6 kg

(

)(S

trah

an, 1

983)

Solit

ary

For

bs &

gra

sses

Ari

d &

sem

i-ar

id

plai

ns, g

rass

land

,sh

rubl

and

Noc

turn

al

Mac

ropu

sag

ilis

Agi

le w

alla

bySt

raha

n (1

995)

11.0

kg

(

);19

.0 k

g (

)G

rega

riou

sG

rass

es, g

rass

ro

ots

Ope

n fo

rest

and

ad

jace

nt g

rass

land

s,co

asta

l san

d du

nes

Noc

turn

al

(Sti

rrat

& F

ulle

r, 19

97)

*Obt

aine

d fr

om S

trah

an (

1995

), u

nles

s ot

herw

ise

cite

d. †

Med

ian

body

wei

ght,

unl

ess

othe

rwis

e st

ated

. ‡R

efer

ence

s w

ithi

n a

revi

ew o

f m

acro

podo

id b

ehav

iour

al r

eper

toir

es c

ompi

led

by C

ouls

on (

1989

).

284

T. A. Rose

et al.

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society,

Mammal Review

,

36,

281–298

anti

lopi

nus

Ant

ilopi

ne w

alla

roo

Cou

lson

(19

89)‡

17.5

kg

(

);37

.0 k

g (

)G

rega

riou

sG

rass

esT

ropi

cal w

oodl

ands

Pri

mar

ily n

octu

rnal

bern

adus

Bla

ck w

alla

roo

D. C

roft

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

13.0

kg

(

);21

.0 k

g (

)So

litar

yG

rass

esR

ocky

esc

arpm

ents

inw

oodl

and,

gra

ssla

ndan

d sh

rubl

and

orra

info

rest

Noc

turn

al

dors

alis

Bla

ck-s

trip

ed

wal

laby

P. J

arm

an (

pers

. co

mm

.)C

. De

Alw

is (

pers

. co

mm

.)

6.5

kg (

);16

.0 k

g (

)G

rega

riou

sG

rass

esF

ores

tN

octu

rnal

fulig

inos

us

Wes

tern

gre

y ka

ngar

ooC

ouls

on (

1989

)‡27

.5 k

g (

);53

.5 k

g (

)G

rega

riou

s(J

arm

an &

C

ouls

on, 1

989)

Gra

sses

& f

orbs

Sem

i-ar

id s

crub

land

to w

oodl

and

and

fore

st

Noc

turn

al

giga

ntus

Eas

tern

gre

y ka

ngar

ooC

ouls

on (

1989

)‡To

32.

0 kg

(

);to

66.

0 kg

(

)G

rega

riou

s(J

arm

an &

C

ouls

on, 1

989)

Gra

sses

& f

orbs

Sem

i-ar

id s

crub

land

to

woo

dlan

d an

d fo

rest

Noc

turn

al

frae

nata

Bri

dled

nai

ltai

l w

alla

byD

. Sig

g (p

ers.

co

mm

.)P.

Joh

nson

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

4.0–

5.0

kg (

);5.

0–8.

0 kg

(

)So

litar

y, b

ut

fem

ales

may

form

fee

ding

ag

greg

atio

nsof

4–5

For

bs, g

rass

& b

row

seO

pen

fore

st, g

rass

yw

oodl

and,

tal

lsh

rubl

and

& a

djac

ent

vege

tati

on

Noc

turn

al

parm

a

Par

ma

wal

laby

Cou

lson

(19

89)‡

3.2–

4.8

kg (

);4.

1–5.

9 kg

(

)P

redo

min

antly

so

litar

yG

rass

es &

her

bsF

ores

t, r

ainf

ores

tN

octu

rnal

parr

yi

Whi

ptai

l wal

laby

Cou

lson

(19

89)‡

11.0

kg

(

);16

.0 k

g (

)G

rega

riou

sG

rass

es &

her

bsO

pen

fore

st w

ith

agr

ass

unde

rsto

rey

Diu

rnal

robu

stus

Com

mon

wal

laro

o,

Eur

oC

ouls

on (

1989

)‡To

25.

0 kg

(

);to

76.

5 kg

(

)So

litar

yG

rass

es &

shr

ubs

Roc

k es

carp

men

tsN

octu

rnal

rufo

gris

eus

Red

-nec

ked

wal

laby

Cou

lson

(19

89)

14.0

kg

(

);19

.7 k

g (

)So

litar

y, b

ut m

ayag

greg

ate

to f

eed

Gra

sses

& h

erbs

For

est

wit

h op

enar

eas

near

by, t

all

coas

tal h

eath

Noc

turn

al

rufu

s

Red

kan

garo

oC

ouls

on (

1989

)‡26

.5 k

g (

);66

.0 k

g (

)G

rega

riou

s(S

trah

an, 1

983)

Gra

sses

& h

erbs

(Str

ahan

, 198

3)O

pen

plai

ns,

shru

blan

ds(S

trah

an, 1

983)

Noc

turn

al

(Str

ahan

, 198

3)

Gen

usSp

ecie

sC

omm

on n

ame

Foo

t-th

umpi

ng

sour

ceB

ody

wei

ght*

†So

cial

ity*

Die

t*H

abit

at*

Act

ivit

y ti

me*

*Obt

aine

d fr

om S

trah

an (

1995

), u

nles

s ot

herw

ise

cite

d. †

Med

ian

body

wei

ght,

unl

ess

othe

rwis

e st

ated

. ‡R

efer

ence

s w

ithi

n a

revi

ew o

f m

acro

podo

id b

ehav

iour

al r

eper

toir

es c

ompi

led

by C

ouls

on (

1989

).

Tab

le 1

.

(

Con

tinu

ed

)

Foot-thumping in macropodoids

285

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society,

Mammal Review

,

36,

281–298

Ony

chog

alea

conc

inna

Lit

tle

rock

-wal

laby

, N

arba

rlek

Cou

lson

(19

89)‡

1.3–

1.4

kgU

nkno

wn

Gra

sses

Roc

k ou

tcro

ps, t

all

gras

s, lo

w s

crub

Noc

turn

al, d

iurn

al

duri

ng w

et s

easo

n

ungu

ifera

Nor

ther

n na

iltai

l w

alla

byP.

Joh

nson

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

5.8

kg (

);7.

5 kg

(

)So

litar

y, b

ut m

ay

form

fee

ding

ag

greg

atio

nsof

up

to 4

Her

bs, s

uccu

lent

s,fr

uits

and

gre

engr

ass

shoo

ts

Ope

n w

oodl

ands

,ta

ll sh

rubl

and

&gr

assl

ands

Noc

turn

al

Pet

roga

leac

ssim

ilis

Alli

ed r

ock-

wal

laby

Bar

ker

(199

0)P.

Joh

nson

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

4.3

kg (

);4.

7 kg

(

)G

rega

riou

sF

orbs

, bro

wse

and

gras

sU

nkno

wn

Noc

turn

al

brac

hyot

is

Shor

t-ea

red

rock

-w

alla

byP.

Joh

nson

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

3.7

kg (

);4.

4 kg

(

)G

rega

riou

sB

row

se a

nd s

eeds

Sava

nna

gras

slan

dsN

octu

rnal

coen

ensi

s

Cap

e Y

ork

rock

-w

alla

byP.

Joh

nson

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

4.0–

4.2

kg (

);4.

3–5.

0 kg

(

)G

rega

riou

sG

rass

es, h

erbs

, so

me

leav

es a

nd

frui

t

Roc

k ou

tcro

ps, t

all

gras

s, lo

w s

crub

Noc

turn

al

godm

ani

God

man

’s ro

ck-

wal

laby

M. E

ldri

dge

(per

s. co

mm

.)P.

Joh

nson

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

4.3

kg (

);5.

2 kg

(

)G

rega

riou

sG

rass

es, h

erbs

, so

me

leav

es a

nd

frui

t

Roc

k ou

tcro

ps, t

all

gras

s, lo

w s

crub

Noc

turn

al

herb

erti

Her

bert

’s ro

ck-

wal

laby

M. E

ldri

dge

(per

s. co

mm

.)P.

Joh

nson

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

4.3

kg (

);6.

0 kg

(

)G

rega

riou

sG

rass

es, h

erbs

, so

me

leav

es a

nd

frui

t

Roc

k ou

tcro

ps, t

all

gras

s, lo

w s

crub

Noc

turn

al

inor

nata

Una

dorn

ed r

ock-

wal

laby

M. E

ldri

dge

(per

s. co

mm

.)P.

Joh

nson

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

4.2

kg (

);5.

0 kg

(

)G

rega

riou

sG

rass

es, h

erbs

, so

me

leav

es a

nd

frui

t

Roc

k ou

tcro

ps, t

all

gras

s, lo

w s

crub

Noc

turn

al

late

ralis

late

ralis

Bla

ck-f

oote

d ro

ck-

wal

laby

M. E

ldri

dge

(per

s. co

mm

.)P.

Jar

man

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

C. D

e A

lwis

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

3.5

kg (

);4.

5 kg

(

)G

rega

riou

sG

rass

esR

ock

outc

rops

, ope

ngr

assl

ands

Noc

turn

al

late

ralis

purp

urei

colli

s

Pur

ple-

neck

ed r

ock-

wal

laby

P. J

ohns

on (

pers

. co

mm

.)5.

0 kg

(

);6.

5 kg

(

)G

rega

riou

sG

rass

esR

ock

outc

rops

, ope

ngr

assl

ands

Noc

turn

al

Mar

eeba

Mar

eeba

roc

k-w

alla

byM

. Eld

ridg

e (p

ers.

com

m.)

P. J

arm

an (

pers

. co

mm

.)P.

Joh

nson

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

3.8

kg (

);4.

5 kg

(

)G

rega

riou

sG

rass

es, h

erbs

, so

me

leav

es a

nd

frui

t

Roc

k ou

tcro

ps, t

all

gras

s, lo

w s

crub

Noc

turn

al

Gen

usSp

ecie

sC

omm

on n

ame

Foo

t-th

umpi

ng

sour

ceB

ody

wei

ght*

†So

cial

ity*

Die

t*H

abit

at*

Act

ivit

y ti

me*

*Obt

aine

d fr

om S

trah

an (

1995

), u

nles

s ot

herw

ise

cite

d. †

Med

ian

body

wei

ght,

unl

ess

othe

rwis

e st

ated

. ‡R

efer

ence

s w

ithi

n a

revi

ew o

f m

acro

podo

id b

ehav

iour

al r

eper

toir

es c

ompi

led

by C

ouls

on (

1989

).

286 T. A. Rose et al.

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

peni

cilla

taB

rush

-tai

led

rock

-w

alla

byC

ouls

on (

1989

)‡P.

Joh

nson

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

6.3

kg (

�);

7.9

kg (

�)

Gre

gari

ous

Gra

sses

, for

bs,

brow

se, s

eeds

, fr

uit

and

flow

ers

Rai

nfor

est,

wet

and

dr

y fo

rest

, ope

n w

oodl

and

& s

emi-

arid

cou

ntry

Noc

turn

al

pers

epho

neP

rose

rpin

e ro

ck-

wal

laby

D. S

igg

(per

s.

com

m.)

5.2

kg (

�);

7.2

kg (

�)

Gre

gari

ous

Gra

sses

Roc

k ou

tcro

ps in

clos

ed c

anop

y fo

rest

s,gr

ass

unde

rsto

rey

inop

en w

oodl

ands

Noc

turn

al

roth

schi

ldi

Rot

hsch

ild’s

rock

-w

alla

byR

. Tea

le (

pers

. co

mm

.)3.

4 kg

Gre

gari

ous

Gra

sses

, her

bs,

som

e le

aves

and

fr

uit

Roc

k ou

tcro

ps, g

rass

-st

eppe

and

shr

ubve

geta

tion

Noc

turn

al

shar

man

iSh

arm

an’s

rock

-w

alla

byM

. Eld

ridg

e (p

ers.

com

m.)

P. J

ohns

on (

pers

. co

mm

.)

4.1

kg (

�);

4.4

kg (

�)

Gre

gari

ous

Gra

sses

, her

bs,

som

e le

aves

and

fr

uit

Roc

k ou

tcro

ps, t

all

gras

s, lo

w s

crub

Noc

turn

al

trid

acty

lus

Lon

g-no

sed

poto

roo

J. N

edve

d (p

ers.

co

mm

.)1.

0 kg

(�

);1.

2 kg

(�

)G

rega

riou

sR

oots

, tub

ers,

fu

ngi a

nd

inve

rteb

rate

s

Coa

stal

hea

th, d

ryan

d w

et f

ores

tsN

octu

rnal

xant

hopu

sce

leri

esY

ello

w-f

oote

dro

ck-w

alla

by (

Qld

)P.

Joh

nson

(pe

rs.

com

m.)

6.0–

11.0

kg

Gre

gari

ous

Gra

sses

, for

bsan

d br

owse

Sem

i-ar

id c

ount

ryN

octu

rnal

xant

hopu

sxa

ntho

pus

Yel

low

-foo

ted

rock

-wal

laby

Cou

lson

(19

89)‡

6.0–

12.0

kg

Gre

gari

ous

Gra

sses

, for

bsan

d br

owse

Sem

i-ar

id c

ount

ryN

octu

rnal

Set

onix

brac

hyur

usQ

uokk

aC

. De

Alw

is (

pers

. co

mm

.)C

ouls

on (

1989

)‡

2.9

kg (

�);

3.6

kg (

�)

Gre

gari

ous

Coa

rse

brow

seF

ores

t, w

oodl

and

and

heat

hN

octu

rnal

Thy

loga

lebi

llard

ieri

iT

asm

ania

n pa

dem

elon

Cou

lson

(19

89)‡

3.9

kg (

�);

7.0

kg (

�)

Solit

ary,

but

so

met

imes

fe

edin

g ag

greg

atio

ns

Gra

sses

, her

bsF

ores

ts a

djac

ent

toop

en g

rass

land

Noc

turn

al

stig

mat

ica

Red

-leg

ged

pade

mel

onC

ouls

on (

1989

)‡4.

1 kg

(�

);5.

1 kg

(�

)So

litar

y, b

ut

som

etim

es

feed

ing

aggr

egat

ions

Lea

ves,

fru

itan

d gr

asse

sR

ainf

ores

t, f

ores

ted

geN

octu

rnal

/cr

epus

cula

r

thet

isR

ed-n

ecke

d pa

dem

elon

Cou

lson

(19

89)‡

3.8

kg (

�);

7.0

kg (

�)

Solit

ary,

but

so

met

imes

fe

edin

g ag

greg

atio

ns

Gra

sses

and

sh

rubs

Rai

nfor

est,

for

est

Noc

turn

al

Wal

labi

abi

colo

urSw

amp

wal

laby

Cou

lson

(19

89)‡

13.0

kg

(�);

17.0

kg

(�)

Solit

ary,

but

so

met

imes

fe

edin

g ag

greg

atio

ns

Coa

rse

brow

seF

ores

t, w

oodl

and,

heat

hN

octu

rnal

Gen

usSp

ecie

sC

omm

on n

ame

Foo

t-th

umpi

ng

sour

ceB

ody

wei

ght*

†So

cial

ity*

Die

t*H

abit

at*

Act

ivit

y ti

me*

*Obt

aine

d fr

om S

trah

an (

1995

), u

nles

s ot

herw

ise

cite

d. †

Med

ian

body

wei

ght,

unl

ess

othe

rwis

e st

ated

. ‡R

efer

ence

s w

ithi

n a

revi

ew o

f m

acro

podo

id b

ehav

iour

al r

eper

toir

es c

ompi

led

by C

ouls

on (

1989

).

Tab

le 1

.(C

onti

nued

)

Foot-thumping in macropodoids 287

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

Tab

le 2

.Sp

ecie

s of

Mac

ropo

doid

ea c

onsi

dere

d no

t to

foo

t-th

ump

Gen

usSp

ecie

sC

omm

on n

ame

Com

men

tsB

ody

wei

ght*

†So

cial

ity*

Die

t*H

abit

at*

Act

ivit

y ti

me*

Den

drol

agus

good

fello

wi

Goo

dfel

low

’str

ee-k

anga

roo

No

foot

-thu

mpi

ng

obse

rved

in 1

4 ye

ars

wit

h ca

ptiv

e po

pula

tion

s (C

. A

ndru

s, p

erso

nal

com

mun

icat

ion)

9.5

kg(F

lann

ery,

19

96)

Unk

now

n(F

lann

ery,

19

96)

Rai

nfor

est

(Fla

nner

y, 1

996)

Unk

now

n (F

lann

ery,

19

96)

Var

ies

from

diu

rnal

to n

octu

rnal

(Fla

nner

y, 1

996)

Hyp

sipr

ymno

don

mos

chat

usM

usky

ra

t-ka

ngar

ooN

o fo

ot-t

hum

ping

ob

serv

ed in

13

year

s w

ith

capt

ive

popu

lati

on

(P. J

ohns

on, p

erso

nal

com

mun

icat

ion)

0.5

kg (

� &

)So

litar

y, b

ut u

p to

8 in

fee

ding

ag

greg

atio

ns

Fru

its,

inse

cts,

fu

ngi

For

est

Diu

rnal

*Obt

aine

d fr

om S

trah

an (

1995

), u

nles

s ot

herw

ise

cite

d. †

Med

ian

body

wei

ght,

unl

ess

othe

rwis

e st

ated

.

288 T. A. Rose et al.

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

Tab

le 3

.Sp

ecie

s of

Mac

ropo

doid

ea f

or w

hich

foo

t-th

umpi

ng is

unk

now

n

Gen

usSp

ecie

sC

omm

on n

ame

Com

men

tsB

ody

wei

ght*

†So

cial

ity*

Die

t*H

abit

at*

Act

ivit

y ti

me*

Bet

tong

iale

sueu

rB

urro

win

g be

tton

gO

nly

on f

our

isla

nds

off

Wes

tern

Aus

tral

iac.

1.5

kg

Gre

gari

ous

Tub

ers,

bul

bs, s

eeds

,nu

ts, f

ungi

, ins

ects

Var

ied

habi

tats

wit

hde

ep s

oil,

rock

you

tcro

ps

Noc

turn

al

Den

drol

agus

benn

etti

anus

Ben

nett

’s tr

ee-

kang

aroo

No

capt

ive

popu

lati

ons

11.5

–13.

7 kg

(�

);8.

0–10

.6 k

g (�

)So

litar

yL

eave

s, f

ruit

Rai

nfor

est

Noc

turn

al

dori

anus

Dor

ia’s

tree

-ka

ngar

ooA

necd

otal

evi

denc

e of

foot

-thu

mpi

ng10

.1 k

g (�

);13

.3 k

g (�

)(F

lann

ery,

199

6)

Gre

gari

ous

(Fla

nner

y, 1

996)

Unk

now

n(F

lann

ery,

199

6)R

ainf

ores

t(F

lann

ery,

199

6)C

repu

scul

ar

(Fla

nner

y, 1

996)

scot

tae

Ten

kile

Lit

tle

know

n of

the

gene

ral b

iolo

gy9.

3 kg

(�

);11

.5 k

g (�

)(F

lann

ery,

199

6)

Solit

ary

(Fla

nner

y, 1

996)

Lea

ves

(Fla

nner

y, 1

996)

Rai

nfor

est

(Fla

nner

y, 1

996)

Diu

rnal

/cre

pusc

ular

(F

lann

ery,

199

6)

Urs

inus

Vog

elko

p tr

ee-

kang

aroo

Poor

ly k

now

n, n

ativ

e to

In

done

sia

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Lea

ves,

fru

it(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Rai

nfor

est

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Unk

now

n (A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Dor

cops

isat

rata

Bla

ck d

orco

psis

Not

hing

is k

now

nof

the

life

his

tory

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

3.9

kg (

�);

4.8

kg (

�)

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Rai

nfor

est

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Unk

now

n (A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

hage

niW

hite

-str

iped

do

rcop

sis

Not

hing

is k

now

n of

the

life

hist

ory

(Arc

her

&

Fla

nner

y, 1

985)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Rai

nfor

est

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Par

tly

diur

nal

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

mac

leay

iM

acle

ay’s

dorc

opsi

sR

are,

onl

y kn

own

from

a fe

w s

peci

men

s(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Rai

nfor

est

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

*Obt

aine

d fr

om S

trah

an (

1995

), u

nles

s ot

herw

ise

cite

d. †

Med

ian

body

wei

ght,

unl

ess

othe

rwis

e st

ated

.

Foot-thumping in macropodoids 289

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

vanh

eurn

iL

ittl

e do

rcop

sis

The

hab

its

and

life

hist

ory

are

larg

ely

unkn

own

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Lea

ves,

fal

len

frui

t(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Rai

nfor

est

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Noc

turn

al

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

vete

rum

Bro

wn

dorc

opsi

sA

lmos

t no

thin

g kn

own

abou

tit

s ha

bita

t or

eco

logy

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Rai

nfor

est

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Lag

orch

este

sfa

scia

tus

Ban

ded

hare

-w

alla

byO

nly

know

n on

3A

ustr

alia

n is

land

s1.

3–3.

0 kg

Shel

ters

in s

mal

l gro

ups

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Gra

sses

, spi

nife

x &

sh

rubs

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Tal

l scr

ub(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Noc

turn

al

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Mac

ropu

sir

ma

Wes

tern

bru

sh

wal

laby

Lit

tle

know

n of

gen

eral

biol

ogy

(Str

ahan

, 199

5)8.

0 kg

Unk

now

nG

rass

esO

pen

fore

st a

nd

woo

dlan

dC

repu

scul

ar

Pet

roga

lebu

rbid

gei

Mon

jon,

War

abi

Lit

tle

know

n of

gen

eral

biol

ogy

(Str

ahan

, 199

5)1.

3 kg

Gre

gari

ous

Unk

now

nO

pen

woo

dlan

dN

octu

rnal

Pot

orou

slo

ngip

esL

ong-

foot

ed

poto

roo

End

ange

red

1.7

kg (

�);

2.1

kg (

�)

Poss

ibly

mon

ogam

ous

Und

ergr

ound

fun

gi,

vasc

ular

pla

nt

mat

eria

l, in

vert

ebra

tes

Rai

nfor

est,

for

ests

Noc

turn

al

Thy

loga

lebr

unii

Dus

ky

pade

mel

onL

ittl

e kn

own

abou

tth

e ge

nera

l bio

logy

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Unk

now

n(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Gra

sses

and

her

bs(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Rai

nfor

est-

gras

slan

d ec

oton

e(A

rche

r &

Fla

nner

y,

1985

)

Noc

turn

al

(Arc

her

& F

lann

ery,

19

85)

Gen

usSp

ecie

sC

omm

on n

ame

Com

men

tsB

ody

wei

ght*

†So

cial

ity*

Die

t*H

abit

at*

Act

ivit

y ti

me*

*Obt

aine

d fr

om S

trah

an (

1995

), u

nles

s ot

herw

ise

cite

d. †

Med

ian

body

wei

ght,

unl

ess

othe

rwis

e st

ated

.

290 T. A. Rose et al.

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

which information was available (Table 1). No information was available for the remaining15 species (Table 3), which predominantly comprise species occurring in remote areas ofPapua New Guinea, extremely rare species or those that have not been well studied. Althoughno observations of foot-thumping in free-living tree-kangaroos were reported, this may bedue to the difficulty of observing the behaviour of these animals in dense rainforest habitats(Flannery, 1996). In captivity, foot-thumping has been observed in three species of tree-kangaroo: the grizzled tree-kangaroo Dendrolagus inustus (U. Ganslosser, personal commu-nication; B. Smith, personal communication), Lumholtz tree-kangaroo D. lumholtzi (B.Smith, personal communication); and the Huon tree-kangaroo D. matschiei (J. Blessington,personal communication).

Of the macropodoid species for which enough information is available, only two specieshad not been observed foot-thumping, Goodfellow’s tree-kangaroo D. goodfellowi (C.Andrus, personal communication) and the musky rat-kangaroo (P. Johnson, personal com-munication) (Table 2). This is despite the respective species specialists having spent consider-able time with captive populations (13–14 years, Table 2). Notably, the musky rat-kangaroois the most primitive of the extant macropodoids (Flannery, 1984; Burk et al., 1998). Thisspecies has a quadrupedal, bounding gait, unlike the bipedal hopping of other macropodoids(Ganslosser, 1992; Strahan, 1995; Burk et al., 1998) and lacks several tarsal specialisationsof the hind foot that are adaptations for bipedal hopping (e.g. the calcaneofibular facet isabsent; see Burk et al., 1998). Burk et al. (1998) argued that bipedal hopping evolved onlyonce in the macropodoid radiations subsequent to Hypsiprymnodon. As macropodoidsusually foot-thump just prior to or during bipedal locomotion (Coulson, 1989), the presenceof foot-thumping in every macropodoid genus except Hypsiprymnodon suggests that it is ahighly conservative trait, and that foot-thumping has arisen alongside or following theevolution of bipedal locomotion in macropodoids, carrying fitness benefits irrespective ofspecies’ individual characteristics. Such behavioural consistency across so many species withdifferences in social structure (solitary and gregarious), habitats (open and dense) and life-history strategies is relatively unusual.

If foot-thumps are an alarm-call response to predation threat, predation must have influ-enced the evolution of macropodoid behaviour. Despite suggestions that predation has beengenerally insignificant in the evolution of marsupial behaviour (Flannery, 1984; Lee & Cock-burn, 1985; Burk et al., 1998), it does appear that over evolutionary time, macropodoids ofall sizes have been subject to predation at some stage of their life cycles (Jarman & Coulson,1989). There have been at least three genera of the now extinct leopard-sized marsupial lions(Thylacoleonidae) that, from their bone morphology, appear to have been cursorial huntersof macropodoids of all sizes (Jarman & Coulson, 1989). More recently, extinct thylacines(Thylacinidae) were a pursuit hunter of macropodoids, as were some large Dasyurid species(family: Dasyuridae, e.g. Sarcophilus), and ambush reptiles like the giant Maglania monitors(family: Varanidae), and giant crocodilians Quinkana spp. Extant predators of macropodoidsinclude the quolls Dasyurus spp. and an array of large ambush-hunting raptors (order:Falconiformes), both preying on small to medium-sized macropodoids. Larger raptors, suchas wedge-tailed eagles Aquila audax and white-bellied sea eagles Haliaeetus leucogaster, arealso known to attack larger macropodoids (see Jarman & Coulson, 1989). For the past4000 years, dingoes Canis familiaris dingo have preyed on all body sizes of macropodoids,hunting both in groups and alone (Shepherd, 1981; Jarman & Coulson, 1989), alteringmacropodoid population dynamics (Robertshaw & Harden, 1989). There has also beensubstantial hunting pressure on macropodoids by humans (Jarman & Coulson, 1989), andpredation pressure has increased via introduced predators such as the red fox Vulpes vulpes,

Foot-thumping in macropodoids 291

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

a hunting predator whose diet includes small to medium-sized macropodoids; and the feralcat Felis catus, an ambush predator whose diet includes small macropodoids (see Mitchell &Banks, 2005). Thus, good evidence exists that there has been a sustained suite of predators,notably with cosmopolitan distributions, diverse modes of attack and preying upon a widerange of body sizes of macropodoids (Jarman & Coulson, 1989; Wroe, Argot & Dickman,2004).

FOOT-THUMPING AS AN ALARM SIGNALFoot-thumping in alarm contexts is common in many mammalian taxa, although in no othermajor taxonomic unit is it as prevalent as in the superfamily Macropodoidea. As is the casewith many macropodoids (Coulson, 1989), some taxa that use foot-thumping strike theground with one or both feet to produce a single or double thump. This ‘foot-thumping’ isdistinguished here from ‘foot-drumming’, where the hind feed are drummed repeatedlyin rapid succession, such as the foot-drumming of kangaroo rats (Randall, 2001). Foot-drumming occurs in many small mammals, particularly rodents (see Randall, 2001). Foot-thumping, as opposed to foot-drumming, is more common in larger mammals, and, inaddition to the Macropodoidea, has been observed in some ungulates (Perissodactyla andArtiodactyla) and some rabbits (Lagomorpha) (Ewer, 1968; Randall, 2001).

Communication inherently involves a sender and a recipient. As an acoustic alarm signal,foot-thumps can be used when the sender and the recipient are not in visual contact such asby nocturnal species, grazers or animals living in dense cover. Similarly, as a vibrational alarmsignal, the acoustic thump may operate at low frequencies, causing ground vibrations iden-tifiable over large distances. The recipients of alarm signals are variously argued to beconspecifics, predators or both (e.g. Maynard-Smith, 1965; Woodland, Jaafar & Knight,1980). Generally, the type of alarm signal that an animal uses depends on which of the costsof signalling are outweighed by the benefits (Caro, 1986a; Taylor et al., 1990). Costs varyaccording to the type of signal and its function, and may include a metabolic expense, a timecost involved with execution of the signal, the foregoing of alternate activities, and anincreased predation risk from attracting the predator’s attention (Krebs & Davies, 1981; Caro,1986a; Johnstone, 1997; Randall, 2001). In macropodoids, the metabolic expense of foot-thumping is likely to be trivial when apportioned over the time and energy expense of bipedalflight. The time cost of executing a foot-thump while hopping may depend upon the animal’sflight speed: at high speeds, creating an audible thump with the hind feet rather than pushingoff from the ground may reduce stride length and so increase the time it takes to escape,whereas when the animal is stationary or moving at low speeds, the time cost of foot-thumping may be negligible. The risks associated with attracting a predator’s attention willvary according to the function of the signal and the attack mode of the predator. The benefitsare primarily a reduction in predation risk for the signaller or, if the signal acts as an altruisticwarning of danger, for the signaller’s conspecifics.

The numerous theories that describe the benefits of alarm signals fall broadly into two (notmutually exclusive) categories: signals directed towards conspecifics, and signals directedtowards predators. In an effort to discern the function of stotting in Thomson’s gazelles, Caro(1986a) developed an array of hypotheses. These hypotheses form the basis of the alarm signalhypotheses reviewed here; however, not all of Caro’s hypotheses were applicable to foot-thumping in macropodoids (e.g. attracting the mother’s attention and play). Furthermore,some alarm signalling hypotheses that were not considered by Caro (1986a) have beenincluded (warning conspecifics to decrease future risk and signalling to create havoc) becauseof their potential relevance to foot-thumping behaviour in macropodoids.

292 T. A. Rose et al.

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

Signalling to conspecificsSignals directed to conspecifics may serve several functions. They may warn conspecifics ofpotential danger, improve social cohesion to reduce an individual’s immediate risk, or maycreate havoc to improve an individual’s chances of escape. In warning conspecifics, there isthe risk of attracting the attention of a predator (Maynard-Smith, 1965; Estes & Goddard,1967; Trivers, 1971; Sherman, 1977). Yet warning signals are argued not to function as purealtruism but to provide direct or indirect benefits to the signaller (Maynard-Smith, 1965;Trivers, 1971; Sherman, 1977). The signaller may benefit directly via decreased future preda-tion risk because an unsuccessful predation event may deter predators from that patch(Sherman, 1977) or indirectly from warning close relatives (kin-selected altruism: Maynard-Smith, 1965; Estes & Goddard, 1967) or by the potential of future reciprocation by others(reciprocal altruism: Trivers, 1971).

If macropodoids are signalling to conspecifics, then signalling will only occur when indi-viduals are in close proximity (groups); only one animal (usually the first animal to see apredator) should signal; and, if the signal functions as kin-selected altruism, signalling willoccur more frequently in groups of closely related individuals. Clear tests providinganswers to these predictions are lacking. Blumstein et al. (2000) suggested that foot-thumpswere conspecific signals because tammar wallabies M. eugenii responded to audio play-backs of foot-thumps by decreasing time spent foraging and increasing vigilance, althoughplaybacks did not affect locomotion. Similarly, playbacks of foot-thumps to red-neckedpademelons Thylogale thetis (Blumstein et al., 2002a) yielded a decrease in foraging and anincrease in vigilance behaviours, but so did a range of other sounds (including calls ofpredators and magpies). Blumstein et al. (2002a) proposed these interspecific differenceswere functionally different anti-predator strategies: both species aggregate when feeding,but red-necked pademelons assess predation risk independently of the presence of conspe-cifics. Notably, Strahan (1995) describes both species as essentially solitary, aggregatingonly occasionally and in small groups when feeding in clearings or along forest edges.Hence, tests of the ‘warning to conspecifics’ hypotheses in essentially solitary species maynot give the clearest evidence for alarm function, and a response to playback may merelyrepresent an eavesdropping phenomenon.

Observations of free-living social kangaroos similarly offer only limited insights into thewarnings hypotheses. Shepherd (1981) noted that red kangaroos showed little response to thefoot-thumps of conspecifics in response to dingoes. And foot-thumping in social western greykangaroos M. fuliginosus was observed when individuals were alone, in small groups(2.5 ± 0.3 individuals), and sometimes when they were more than 300 m from their nearestneighbour (Coulson, 1996). Gregarious eastern grey kangaroos M. gigantus are also knownto foot-thump when alone or when in groups (Bender, 2004). Furthermore, the prevalence offoot-thumping in both solitary and social macropodoids (Table 1) suggests that, althoughfoot-thumping may sometimes act as a communication to conspecifics, this may not be itsonly function.

Evidence for signals to kin is also weak as few species form social groups, mainlywith relatives; instead, grouping is often highly labile (Croft, 1989) and yet foot-thumping occurs in both types of social aggregations. Although some macropodoids candiscriminate between kin and non-kin (e.g. tammar wallabies: Blumstein et al., 2002b),thumping by adult males is common in western grey kangaroos (37% of observations)(Coulson, 1989) and eastern grey kangaroos (50% of observations) (Bender, 2004), eventhough males are the dispersing sex and are therefore unlikely to be related to signalrecipients.

Foot-thumping in macropodoids 293

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

That macropodoid foot-thumps signal conspecifics to create a social cohesion or grouphavoc response has some support because the signal often causes many individuals in a groupto flee. Social cohesion is thought to dilute the risk to any one individual (Malcolm & VanLawick, 1975; Treisman, 1975), as avoiding the periphery of a group lowers an animal’s riskof predation (Hamilton, 1971). But it is often difficult to assess whether social cohesion iscaused by the alarm signal or by a predator’s presence. Similarly, havoc is defined as disruptivechaos (Rooney, 1999) and may eventuate from a proportion of the group, such as 50% (Caro,1986b), simultaneously reacting to the signal by scattering or moving about erratically (Char-nov & Krebs, 1975). Havoc may confuse the predator and lessen its chance of a successfulattack (Hoogland & Sherman, 1976; Wittenberger, 1981). The presence of foot-thumping insolitary individuals and solitary species (Table 1) suggests that it is unlikely that the solefunction of foot-thumping is to act as a catalyst for either social cohesion or the generationof havoc, and that this hypothesis may not fully explain the function of foot-thumping inmacropodoids. Observations that a simultaneous chaotic reaction to foot-thumping occursin a substantial proportion of the group [Caro (1986b) suggests 50%] will more fully evaluatewhether foot-thumping does create havoc in some species.

Signalling to predatorsAlarm signals directed to predators may also function in several ways. They may act as apursuit-deterrent by communicating early detection of the predator or by advertising thesignaller’s body condition; they may invite the predator to prematurely initiate chase; orthey may confuse the predator and thus improve the chances of escape (see Caro, 1986a).The pursuit-deterrent hypothesis suggests that signals carry an implicit message that a chasewould be unsuccessful, causing the predator to abandon pursuit (Woodland et al., 1980;Hasson, 1991; Caro, 1995). Pursuit-deterrent signals may not always honestly communicatethe ability of the prey to escape. Prey may misjudge their ability to escape (juveniles, naiveanimals), or animals may ‘cheat’ and provide fake information (Bildstein, 1983; Caro, 1986a).Although this may be a successful strategy for some animals, it is likely that cheaters withinthe population will ensure that the honesty of the signal is frequently tested. If the numberof cheaters in a population becomes too high, predators may soon become aware that thesignal is no longer a reliable indicator of capture success, and the signal would eventuallylose its effectiveness. Therefore, for a condition advertisement signal to be maintained, cheat-ing would only be favoured in a small proportion of the population.

If foot-thumping in macropodoids acts as a pursuit-deterrent, signalling would occurregardless of whether the animals are alone or in groups, which is supported by observations(Table 1). To further support the pursuit-deterrent theory, experimental research is needed toprovide evidence that predators pursue individuals that foot-thump less often than individ-uals that do not foot-thump, that macropodoids do not foot thump when predators are closeenough that a successful attack is likely, and/or that foot-thumping does not occur once thepredator has initiated chase. Furthermore, if foot-thumping advertises that the signaller is ingood condition, it should occur less frequently in sick, injured, young or very old individuals(see FitzGibbon & Fanshawe, 1988).

In contrast, the pursuit-invitation hypothesis proposes that the alarm signal induces adetected predator to attack prematurely, before it is sufficiently close to capture the prey. Thismay cause it to depart after a failed attempt, and enable both the predator and the preyto resume activities that are more profitable than high-level vigilance or stalking a watchfulprey (Smythe, 1970, 1977). Macropodoid foot-thumping is likely to act as a pursuit-invitationif it occurs only when the thumper is far enough from the predator that escape is likely, but

294 T. A. Rose et al.

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

should not occur once the predator has initiated chase, regardless of whether the animals arealone or in groups. Predators should respond to pursuit-invitation signals by preferentiallychasing individuals that foot-thump over individuals that do not, but there is little researchconducted on macropodoid–predator interactions, and no studies suggest that predatorspreferentially chase individuals that foot-thump. The distance from predators at which anindividual initiates flight varies according to the individual’s age and condition (Jarman &Wright, 1993), and although Coulson (1996) found that foot-thumping occurred at safedistances in western grey kangaroos, knowledge of the flight-initiation distances of individ-uals (or age classes) was lacking. A study involving the measurement of individuals’ flight-initiation distances would better determine whether thumps always occur at safe distancesfrom predators.

The predator-confusion hypothesis is an extension of the havoc hypothesis except thatmany individuals simultaneously signal to the predator, hindering its chances of capturingany individual prey (Humphries & Driver, 1967; Walther, 1969). This hypothesis was firstraised to explain erratic behaviour, such as stotting and zigzagging in Thomson’s gazelleswhile attempting to escape predators (Humphries & Driver, 1967). The predator-confusionhypothesis predicts that signalling will only occur in grouped animals, that more than oneanimal will signal, and that predators will become visibly confused after multiple animalshave signalled. The only evidence that suggests that the sole function of macropodoid foot-thumping may not be to confuse predators, is that foot-thumping occurs in both solitary andsocial animals (Table 1). Clearly, studies involving the measurement of the number of indi-viduals foot-thumping in response to a predator stimulus, along with observations of predatorresponses to multiple foot-thumps, would provide better support for this hypothesis.

Testing the alarm signal hypotheses against the predictions outlined above will exploremore thoroughly the function of foot-thumping in macropodoid marsupials. To test thevarious hypotheses critically, experiments could measure macropodoid reactions to con-trolled predator approaches and playbacks of foot-thump sounds. A summary of potentialobservations/manipulations that would test the predictions is provided in Tables 4 and 5. Thetiming (e.g. time after hearing a thump or proximity to predator stimuli), extent (e.g. howmany individual respond) and nature of responses (e.g. flight or foot-thumping) can be usedto distinguish among alarm-calling hypotheses. But because the same response can be pre-dicted under several different hypotheses, manipulation of group size, age class and related-ness will be needed to untangle the adaptive mechanism(s) behind foot-thump behaviour.

CONCLUSIONThe presence of foot-thumping in every extant macropodoid genus, with the notable excep-tion of the quadrupedal Hypsiprymnodon, suggests that foot-thumping is a conservative trait,having evolved alongside or following bipedal locomotion. The function of foot-thumpingmay vary between macropodoids; however, the prevalence of foot-thumping over a wide rangeof ecological conditions, and differences in historical and current predation pressure amongspecies, suggests that there may be some commonality to the foot-thump’s function.

Of the alarm signalling hypotheses proposed, some support is found for foot-thumping inmacropodoids acting to warn conspecifics, but the use of foot-thumping by solitary animalssuggests that this may not be its only function. It remains possible that the foot-thump actsas a communication to predators, and members of social species merely opportunisticallyreact to the sound of a foot-thump by conspecifics. Nonetheless, current knowledge ofmacropodoid foot-thumping does not provide conclusive support for any of the alarm sig-nalling hypotheses examined, and further research is needed to clarify the function(s) of this

Foot-thumping in macropodoids 295

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

Table 4. Predictions of macropodoid responses to predator approach and relationship to alarm signalling hypotheses

Manipulation: GroupingPredator approach to solitary and grouped macropods (gregarious species)Response Possible function of foot-thumpFoot-thumping will not occur when animals are solitary Warning conspecifics

Confusing predatorCreating havocSocial cohesion

Foot-thumping will occur when solitary or grouped Pursuit-invitationPursuit-deterrent (perception advertisement)Pursuit-deterrent (condition advertisement)

Manipulation: Response of conspecificsPlayback of foot-thump sound to grouped macropodoids (gregarious species)Response Possible function of foot-thumpMacropodoids react to the foot-thump sound by fleeing or

significantly increasing vigilance relative to a control soundWarning conspecifics

Macropodoids react to the foot-thump sound bydecreasing their space from one another

Social cohesion

Macropodoids react to the foot-thump sound by fleeing Warning conspecificsCreating havoc

Macropodoids react to the foot-thump sound by foot-thumping

Confusing predator

Macropodoids show no reaction to the foot-thump sound relative to a control sound

Pursuit-invitationPursuit-deterrent (perception advertisement)Pursuit-deterrent (condition advertisement)

Manipulation: Condition-dependencePredator stimulus approaches solitary animals, varying age classes(NB assumes age class is an indicator of condition)Response Possible function of foot-thumpFoot-thumping occurs in animals of all age groups Pursuit-deterrent (perception advertisement)

Pursuit-invitationFoot-thumping does not occur in very young or old animals Pursuit-deterrent (condition advertisement)Foot-thumping does not occur in any animals Warning conspecifics

Social cohesionConfusing predator

Manipulation: Distance from threatControlled predator approach towards solitary animals, measuring distances at which foot-thumping occursResponse Possible function of foot-thumpFoot-thumping only occurs far from the threat Pursuit-invitationFoot-thumping occurs as soon as the animal perceives the

threatPursuit-deterrent (perception advertisement)

Strong (adult) animals foot-thump closer to the threat thanjuvenile or elderly animals

Pursuit-deterrent (condition advertisement)

Foot-thumping does not occur Warning conspecificsSocial cohesionConfusing predator

Manipulation: RelatednessTowards groups of related or unrelated animals (gregarious species)Response Possible function of foot-thumpFoot-thumping does not occur in unrelated group Warning conspecifics (kin selection)Foot-thumping occurs equally often in both related

and unrelated groupsWarning conspecifics (reciprocal altruism)Social cohesionConfusing predatorPursuit-invitationPursuit-deterrentCreating havoc

296 T. A. Rose et al.

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

behaviour. Manipulations using various species of captive macropodoids, combined withobservations of interactions between macropodoids and predators in the wild, are necessaryto test the predictions generated by the alarm signalling hypotheses described here.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTSThanks to the following keepers of captive macropodoids for providing information onfoot-thumping: M. Hawkins from Taronga Zoo, B. Smith from Currumbin Sanctuary, J.Blessington from Kansas City Zoo, J. Steenberg (co-author of the Tree-Kangaroo HusbandryManual), C. Andrus from San Diego Zoo, and C.De Alwis from Royal Melbourne Zoo.Thanks also to the research scientists who passed on their experiences with foot-thumping:D. Croft, T. Dawson, H. Bender, U. Ganslosser, L. Kazmeier, G. Newell, P. Johnson, P.Jarman, M. E., P. Christensen, D. Pearson, S. Ingleby, T. Flannery, A. Burbidge, A. Kabat,R. Rose, N. Noakes, D. Sigg, R. Teale, T. Pople and J. Nedved. Special thanks are given toC. Barclay, B. Mitchell and B. Russell for useful discussions.

Manipulation: Number of signallersPredator approach towards group (gregarious species)Response Possible function of foot-thumpMore than one individual will foot-thump, irrespective of age Confusing predatorMore than one individual will foot-thump, but not

very young or old individualsPursuit-deterrent (condition advertisement)

Only one individual will foot-thump Warning conspecificsCreating havocPursuit-deterrent (perception advertisement)Social cohesion

Observations of predators approaching macropodoidsResponse Possible function of foot-thumpFoot-thumping does not occur once the predator has initiated

chasePursuit-invitationPursuit-deterrent (perception advertisement)

Foot-thumping will occur at any time (from predator detection to capture)

Warning conspecificsSocial cohesionCreating havocPursuit-deterrent (condition advertisement)

Table 5. Predictions of predator responses to macropodoid foot-thumping and relationship to alarm signalling hypotheses

Observations of predators approaching macropodoids

Response Possible function of foot-thump

Predators chase individuals that foot-thump less often than Pursuit-deterrent (perception advertisement)individuals that do not foot-thump Pursuit-deterrent (condition advertisement)

Predators chase individuals that foot-thump more oftenthan individuals that do not foot-thump

Pursuit-invitation

Predators show no preference for chasing individuals that foot-thump or individuals that do not foot-thump

Warning conspecificsSocial cohesionCreating havoc

Predators should become visibly confused when attacking agroup of foot-thumpers

Confusing predator

Table 4. (Continued)

Foot-thumping in macropodoids 297

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

REFERENCESArcher, M. & Flannery, T.F. (1985) The Kangaroo. Weldons Pty Ltd, McMahons Point, New South Wales.Barker, S.C. (1990) Behaviour and social organisation of the allied rock-wallaby Petrogale assimilis. Australian

Wildlife Research, 17, 301–312.Bender, H. (2004) Auditory stimuli as a method to deter kangaroos in agricultural and road environments.

PhD Thesis. University of Melbourne.Bender, H. (2005) Effectiveness of the eastern grey kangaroo foot thump for deterring conspecifics. Wildlife

Research, 32, 649–655.Bildstein, K.L. (1983) Why white-tailed deer flag their tails. The American Naturalist, 121, 709–715.Blumstein, D.T. & Armitage, K.B. (1998) Why do yellow-bellied marmots call? Animal Behaviour, 56, 1053–

1055.Blumstein, D.T., Daniel, J.C., Griffin, A.S. & Evans, C.S. (2000) Insular tammar wallabies (Macropus eugenii)

respond to visual but not acoustic cues from predators. Behavioral Ecology, 11, 528–535.Blumstein, D.T., Daniel, J.C., Schnell, M.R., Ardron, J.G. & Evans, C.S. (2002a) Antipredator behaviour of

red-necked pademelons: a factor contributing to species survival? Animal Conservation, 5, 325–331.Blumstein, D.T., Ardron, J.G. & Evans, C.S. (2002b) Kin discrimination in a macropod marsupial. Ethology,

108, 815–823.Bourke, D.W. (1989) Observations on the behaviour of the Grey Dorcoposos Wallaby, Dorcopsis luctuosa

(Marsupialia: Macropodidae), in captivity. In: Kangaroos, Wallabies and Rat-Kangaroos (Ed. by G.J.P.Grigg, P. Jarman & I. Hume), pp. 457–473. Surrey Beatty and Sons Pty Ltd, Chipping Norton, New SouthWales.

Burk, A., Westerman, M. & Springer, M. (1998) The phylogenetic position of the musky rat-kangaroo andthe evolution of bipedal hopping in kangaroos (Macropodidae: Diprotodontia). Systematic Biology, 47,457–474.

Caro, T.M. (1986a) The functions of stotting: a review of the hypotheses. Animal Behaviour, 34, 649–662.Caro, T.M. (1986b) The functions of stotting in Thomson’s gazelles: some tests of the predictions. Animal

Behaviour, 34, 663–684.Caro, T.M. (1995) Pursuit-deterrence revisted. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 10, 500–503.Caro, T.M., Graham, C.M., Stoner, C.J. & Vargas, J.K. (2004) Adaptive significance of antipredator behaviour

in artiodactyls. Animal Behaviour, 67, 205–228.Charnov, E.L. & Krebs, J.R. (1975) The evolution of alarm calls: altruism or manipulation? The American

Naturalist, 109, 107–112.Coulson, G. (1989) Repertoires of social behaviour in the Macropodoidea. In: Kangaroos, Wallabies and Rat-

Kangaroos (Ed. by G.J. P. Grigg, P. Jarman & I. Hume), pp. 457–473. Surrey Beatty and Sons Pty Ltd,Chipping Norton, New South Wales.

Coulson, G. (1996) Anti-predator behaviour in marsupials. In: Comparison of Marsupial and PlacentalBehaviour (Ed. by D.B. Croft & U. Ganslosser), pp. 158–186. Filander Verlag GmbH, Furth.

Croft, D. (1981) Social behaviour of the Euro, Macropus robustus, in the Australian arid zone. AustralianWildlife Research, 8, 13–49.

Croft, D. (1989) Social organisation of the Macropodoidea. In: Kangaroos, Wallabies and Rat-Kangaroos (Ed.by G.J.P. Grigg, P. Jarman & I. Hume), pp. 457–473. Surrey Beatty and Sons Pty Ltd, Chipping Norton,New South Wales.

Dawson, T.J. (1977) Kangaroos. Scientific American, 237, 78–89.Estes, R.D. & Goddard, J. (1967) Prey selection and hunting behaviour of the African wild dog. Journal of

Wildlife Management, 31, 52–70.Ewer, R.F. (1968) Ethology of Mammals. Logos Press, London.Fitzgibbon, C.D. & Fanshawe, J.H. (1988) Stotting in Thomson’s gazelles: an honest signal of condition.

Behavioural Ecology & Sociobiology, 33, 69–74.Flannery, T.F. (1984) 15 million years of Australian bounders. In: Vertebrate Zoogeography and Evolution in

Australia (Ed. by M. Archer & G. Clayton), pp. 817–835. Hesperian Press, Carlisle, Western Australia.Flannery, T.F. (1996) Tree-Kangaroos: A Curious Natural History. Reed Books Australia, Port Melbourne,

Victoria.Ganslosser, U. (1992) Behavioural data support the currently proposed phylogeny of the Macropodoidea

(Marsupialia). Australian Mammalogy, 15, 89–104.Grigg, G., Jarman, P. & Hume, I. (1989) Introduction and acknowledgements. In: Kangaroos, Wallabies and

Rat-Kangaroos (Ed. by G.J.P. Grigg, P. Jarman & I. Hume), pp. 457–473. Surrey Beatty and Sons Pty Ltd,Chipping Norton, New South Wales.

Hamilton, W.D. (1963) The evolution of altruistic behaviour. The American Naturalist, 97, 354–356.Hamilton, W.D. (1971) Geometry for the selfish herd. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 31, 295–311.

298 T. A. Rose et al.

© 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2006 Mammal Society, Mammal Review, 36, 281–298

Hasson, O. (1991) Pursuit-deterrent signals – communication between prey and predator. Trends in Ecologyand Evolution, 6, 325–329.

Hoogland, J.L. & Sherman, P.W. (1976) Advantages and disadvantages of bank swallow (Riparia riparia)coloniality. Ecological Monographs, 46, 33–58.

Humphries, D.A. & Driver, P.M. (1967) Erratic display as a device against predators. Science, 156, 1767–1768.Jarman, P.J. & Coulson, G. (1989) Dynamics and adaptiveness of grouping in macropods. In: Kangaroos,

Wallabies and Rat-Kangaroos (Ed. by G.J.P. Grigg, P. Jarman & I. Hume), pp. 457–473. Surrey Beattyand Sons Pty Ltd, Chipping Norton, New South Wales.

Jarman, P.J. & Wright, S.M. (1993) Macropod studies at wallaby creek. IX. Exposure and responses of easterngrey kangaroos to dingoes. Wildlife Research, 20, 833–843.

Johnstone, R.A. (1997) The evolution of animal signals. In: Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach(Ed. by J.R. Krebs & N.B. Davies), pp. 155–178. Blackwell Science, Oxford.

Krebs, J.R. & Davies, N.B. (1981) An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Blackwell Science, Oxford.Lee, A.K. & Cockburn, A. (1985) Evolutionary Ecology of Marsupials. Cambridge University Press,

Cambridge.Malcolm, J.R. & Van Lawick, H. (1975) Notes on wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) Hunting Zebras. Mammalia, 39,

231–240.Maynard-Smith, J. (1965) The evolution of alarm calls. The American Naturalist, XCIX, 59–63.Menzies, J.I. (1989) Observations on a captive forest wallaby (Dorcopsis luctuosa) colony. In: Kangaroos,

Wallabies and Rat-Kangaroos (Ed. by G.J.P. Grigg, P. Jarman & I. Hume), pp. 457–473. Surrey Beattyand Sons Pty Ltd, Chipping Norton, New South Wales.

Mitchell, B.D. & Banks, P.B. (2005) Do wild dogs exclude foxes? Evidence for competition from dietary andspatial overlaps. Austral Ecology, 30, 581–591.

Randall, J.A. (2001) Evolution and function of drumming as communication in mammals. American Zoologist,41, 1143–1156.

Robertshaw, J.D. & Harden, R.H. (1989) Predation on Macropodoidea: a review. In: Kangaroos, Wallabiesand Rat-Kangaroos (Ed. by G.J.P. Grigg, P. Jarman & I. Hume), pp. 457–473. Surrey Beatty and Sons PtyLtd, Chipping Norton, New South Wales.

Rooney, K., ed. (1999) Encarta World English Dictionary. Bloomsbury Publishing Plc, London.Shepherd, N.C. (1981) Predation of red kangaroos by the dingo in NW NSW. Australian Wildlife Research,

8, 255–262.Sherman, P.W. (1977) Nepotism and the evolution of alarm calls. Science, 197, 1246–1253.Smythe, N. (1970) On the existence of ‘pursuit invitation’ signals in mammals. The American Naturalist, 104,

491–494.Smythe, N. (1977) The function of mammalian alarm advertising: social signals or pursuit invitation. The

American Naturalist, 111, 191–194.Stirrat, S.C. & Fuller, M. (1997) The repertoire of social behaviours of agile wallabies, Macropus agilis.

Australian Mammalogy, 20, 71–78.Strahan, R. (1983) The Complete Book of Australian Mammals. Angus & Robertson Publishers, Sydney, New

South Wales.Strahan, R. (1995) The Mammals of Australia. Reed New Holland, Sydney, New South Wales.Taylor, R.J., Balph, D.F. & Balph, M.H. (1990) The evolution of alarm calling: a cost-benefit analysis. Animal

Behaviour, 39, 860–868.Treisman, M. (1975) Predation and the evolution of gregariousness. I. Models for concealment and evasion.

Animal Behaviour, 23, 779–800.Trivers, R.L. (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.Walther, F.R. (1969) Flight behaviour and avoidance of predators in Thomson’s gazella (Gazella thomsoni).

Behaviour, 34, 184–221.Wittenberger, J.F. (1981) Animal Social Behavior. Duxbury Press, Boston.Woodland, D.J., Jaafar, Z. & Knight, M. (1980) The ‘pursuit deterrent’ function of alarm signals. The

American Naturalist, 115, 748–753.Wroe, S., Argot, C. & Dickman, C. (2004) On the rarity of big fierce carnivores and primacy of isolation and

area: tracking large mammalian carnivore diversity on two isolated continents. Proceedings of the RoyalSociety of London Series B – Biological Sciences, 271, 1203–1211.

Submitted 5 July 2005; returned for revision 5 September 2005; revision accepted 22 December 2005Editor: RM


Recommended