+ All Categories
Home > Documents > The anxiolytic buspirone shifts coping strategy in novel environmental context of mice with...

The anxiolytic buspirone shifts coping strategy in novel environmental context of mice with...

Date post: 12-Nov-2023
Category:
Upload: u-szeged
View: 0 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
7
Behavioural Brain Research 250 (2013) 32–38 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research j ourna l h o mepa ge: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report The anxiolytic buspirone shifts coping strategy in novel environmental context of mice with different anxious phenotype János Horváth a , Titanilla Szögi a , Géza Müller a , Viktor Szegedi b,a Department of Medical Chemistry, University of Szeged, Szeged, Hungary b Biological Research Center, Institute of Biochemistry, Szeged, Temesvári krt. 32, Hungary h i g h l i g h t s Anxious (AX) and nonanxious (nAX) mice were studied in elevated plus maze for 8 days. Both strains showed decreased open-arm exploration over time. Exposing mice to a novel environmental context changed behavioral profile. AX mice were passive, while nAX mice were active. This behavioral profile was reversed with chronic 2.5 mg/kg buspirone treatment. a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 18 January 2013 Received in revised form 10 April 2013 Accepted 13 April 2013 Available online xxx Keywords: Anxiety Elevated plus maze Context change Inbred mice AX nAX a b s t r a c t Patients suffering from anxiety disorders show increased fear when encounter a novel environment. Rodents, placed in new environmental context may respond either with increased novelty seeking (active), or enhanced anxiety (passive coping style), which may depend on the trait anxiety of the animal. Here, the connection between the initial level of anxiety and the behavioral responses in a novel environ- ment was investigated. Two inbred mouse strains having either high- or low-anxiety related behavior (AX and nAX) were exposed to elevated plus maze (EPM), a standard test for assessing anxiety level, for 8 consecutive days. The initial anxiety level was modulated by chronic treatment with buspirone (bus) treatment, a clinically effective anxiolytic, using 2.5 mg/kg and 5.0 mg/kg doses. Both strains showed a gradual decrease of open-arm exploration, which was not prevented by bus treatment. Another cohort of animals was exposed to EPM for 2 days, and then we changed to blue light illumination and used a different cleaning substance with citrus odor (context change, CC). It was found that upon CC AX mice exhibited increased, while nAX mice showed decreased anxiety. Bus in 2.5 mg/kg changed the coping strategy from passive to active exploration after CC in the AX mice; however, the same treatment ren- dered nAX mice passive upon CC. Bus in 5.0 mg/kg failed to alter the overall coping style in the novel environment of both strains. These results suggest that these mouse lines use different coping strategy in novel context, which can be changed with bus treatment. © 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Anxiety disorders, the most common forms of mental illness [16], are often accompanied by increased fear in novel contexts and patients were reported to have difficulties in habituating to a new environment. In preclinical research, elevated plus maze (EPM) is a widely used model to assess anxiety-related behav- ior of mice (for review see [8,21]). The time spent in the aversive open arm is an inverse measure of fear, because anxious animals Corresponding author at: Biological Research Center Biochemistry, Temesvári krt. 32, Szeged H-6726, Hungary. Tel.: +36 70 2418260. E-mail address: [email protected] (V. Szegedi). prefer the safe closed arm. An intriguing feature of EPM is that mice show profound behavioral changes when re-exposed to the plus- maze. Typically, undrugged animals exhibit a significant decrease of open arm exploration upon re-exposure to the EPM, a finding often referred to as one-trial tolerance (OTT) (e.g. [3,6,18,25]). There are conflicting data about the physiological background of OTT: it may represent an increased anxiety response (phobic-like) to the open arms during the first trial [3,5], or alternatively, mice could lose the motivation for exploring the aversive open arms [26]. In support of this hypothesis, when an aversive stimulus is introduced in the closed arm during re-exposure, animals did not show OTT [22]. Although the readout would be the same (decreased time spent in the open-arms), these possibilities are different in terms of anxiety expression and coping style. 0166-4328/$ see front matter © 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.014
Transcript

R

Te

Ja

b

h

•••••

ARRAA

KAECIAn

1

[aa(io

k

0h

Behavioural Brain Research 250 (2013) 32– 38

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Behavioural Brain Research

j ourna l h o mepa ge: www.elsev ier .com/ locate /bbr

esearch report

he anxiolytic buspirone shifts coping strategy in novelnvironmental context of mice with different anxious phenotype

ános Horvátha, Titanilla Szögia, Géza Müllera, Viktor Szegedib,∗

Department of Medical Chemistry, University of Szeged, Szeged, HungaryBiological Research Center, Institute of Biochemistry, Szeged, Temesvári krt. 32, Hungary

i g h l i g h t s

Anxious (AX) and nonanxious (nAX) mice were studied in elevated plus maze for 8 days.Both strains showed decreased open-arm exploration over time.Exposing mice to a novel environmental context changed behavioral profile.AX mice were passive, while nAX mice were active.This behavioral profile was reversed with chronic 2.5 mg/kg buspirone treatment.

a r t i c l e i n f o

rticle history:eceived 18 January 2013eceived in revised form 10 April 2013ccepted 13 April 2013vailable online xxx

eywords:nxietylevated plus mazeontext change

nbred mice

a b s t r a c t

Patients suffering from anxiety disorders show increased fear when encounter a novel environment.Rodents, placed in new environmental context may respond either with increased novelty seeking(active), or enhanced anxiety (passive coping style), which may depend on the trait anxiety of the animal.Here, the connection between the initial level of anxiety and the behavioral responses in a novel environ-ment was investigated. Two inbred mouse strains having either high- or low-anxiety related behavior(AX and nAX) were exposed to elevated plus maze (EPM), a standard test for assessing anxiety level, for8 consecutive days. The initial anxiety level was modulated by chronic treatment with buspirone (bus)treatment, a clinically effective anxiolytic, using 2.5 mg/kg and 5.0 mg/kg doses. Both strains showed agradual decrease of open-arm exploration, which was not prevented by bus treatment. Another cohortof animals was exposed to EPM for 2 days, and then we changed to blue light illumination and used a

XAX

different cleaning substance with citrus odor (context change, CC). It was found that upon CC AX miceexhibited increased, while nAX mice showed decreased anxiety. Bus in 2.5 mg/kg changed the copingstrategy from passive to active exploration after CC in the AX mice; however, the same treatment ren-dered nAX mice passive upon CC. Bus in 5.0 mg/kg failed to alter the overall coping style in the novelenvironment of both strains. These results suggest that these mouse lines use different coping strategyin novel context, which can be changed with bus treatment.

. Introduction

Anxiety disorders, the most common forms of mental illness16], are often accompanied by increased fear in novel contextsnd patients were reported to have difficulties in habituating to

new environment. In preclinical research, elevated plus maze

EPM) is a widely used model to assess anxiety-related behav-or of mice (for review see [8,21]). The time spent in the aversivepen arm is an inverse measure of fear, because anxious animals

∗ Corresponding author at: Biological Research Center – Biochemistry, Temesvárirt. 32, Szeged H-6726, Hungary. Tel.: +36 70 2418260.

E-mail address: [email protected] (V. Szegedi).

166-4328/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.ttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.014

© 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

prefer the safe closed arm. An intriguing feature of EPM is that miceshow profound behavioral changes when re-exposed to the plus-maze. Typically, undrugged animals exhibit a significant decreaseof open arm exploration upon re-exposure to the EPM, a findingoften referred to as one-trial tolerance (OTT) (e.g. [3,6,18,25]). Thereare conflicting data about the physiological background of OTT: itmay represent an increased anxiety response (phobic-like) to theopen arms during the first trial [3,5], or alternatively, mice couldlose the motivation for exploring the aversive open arms [26]. Insupport of this hypothesis, when an aversive stimulus is introduced

in the closed arm during re-exposure, animals did not show OTT[22]. Although the readout would be the same (decreased timespent in the open-arms), these possibilities are different in termsof anxiety expression and coping style.

l Brain

ttewcPhaThwoailnlhiwecF(

2

2

wuaHobX

2

tcas

2

o1ta1hswNcAao2wwac

2

pc

J. Horváth et al. / Behavioura

Inbred rodent models selected for extremes in anxious pheno-ypes are valuable models of anxiety research. Differences in therait anxiety level may be accompanied by different coping strat-gy or resilience to stress. Indeed, rats bred for extremes in anxietyere shown to use active (less anxious) or passive (more anxious)

oping strategy in the forced swim [14,15] and hole-board test [20].reviously, we have developed two mouse strains having eitherigh- or low-anxiety related behavior (AX and nAX, respectively)s was shown in the EPM, open-field, and light/dark test [28,31].he initial level of anxiety may determine the coping style or theabituation of the animals in new environment, thus those miceere exposed to EPM for eight consecutive days. Furthermore, in

rder to enhance the motivation for re-exploring the open arms, change in the environmental context of the EPM apparatus wasntroduced after 2 days of EPM exposure, and the behavior was fol-owed for 6 days. As the animals have a choice between safe andovel, but aversive compartments, it was expected that mice with

ow trait anxiety would exhibit a preference for novelty, whereasigh trait anxiety subjects would prefer safety. Rather introduc-

ng a negative stimulus, like hot air stream in one closed arm [22],e aimed to enhance the exploratory motivation by changing the

nvironmental context. We asked whether changing some externalues would modify the coping strategy of the animals in the EPM.urthermore, the effect of a clinically effective anxiolytic, buspironebus) was tested in two doses on the behavioral profiles.

. Materials and methods

.1. Animals

Inbred mouse strains having either high- or low anxiety level (AX and nAX)ere bred in our animal facility. Male mice of 2.5–3 month were housed individuallynder a light/dark 12 h cycle (lights on at 08:00) at 24 ± 1 ◦C and given ad libitum foodnd water. Mice were handled for 7 days prior to testing, the procedure described byurst and West [13]. For the experiments, 45th–48th inbred generations were usedf both strains. The study conformed to EU directive 2010/63/EU and was approvedy the regional Station for Animal Health and Food Control under Project LicenseXXI/2012.

.2. Treatment

Mice were injected chronically (daily injection for 4 weeks before and duringhe tests) with buspirone (Sigma–Aldrich, Budapest) at 2.5 mg/kg and 5.0 mg/kgoncentrations. Drug was dissolved in phosphate buffer saline (PBS) pH 7.2 and wasdministered intraperitoneally (i.p.) with 0.1 ml. The control group was treated withaline of the same volume.

.3. Behavioral testing

The EPM was made of stainless steel (painted matt black) consisting of twopposite open arms (35 cm × 7 cm) and two opposite closed arms surrounded by5 cm high walls of the same dimensions. The middle section that allows the animalo transit from arm to arm consisted of a square with dimension of 7 cm × 7 cm. Thepparatus was elevated 50 cm from the floor and the open arms were equipped with.5 cm × 1.5 cm ledges to ensure, that no animals would fall of the maze. The animalouse and the testing location were at the same room. Inside the room the maze waseparated with black curtains. The behavior of the mice was recorded and analyzedith an EthoVision software package (EthoVision XT 8.5 Noldus Technology, Theetherlands). Animals were exposed to EPM for eight consecutive days. Trials werearried out under low white light condition (open arms 45 lux, closed arms 15 lux).ll tests were conducted during the light phase of light/dark period between 13:00nd 17:00 h. The mice were placed individually in the center of the maze facing thepen arm and allowed 5 min to explore the apparatus. The maze was cleaned with0% ethyl alcohol before each trial. The white illumination/ethanol cleaning scenarioas defined as context A. In certain experiments, blue-light illumination combinedith cleaning the apparatus with a substance having light citrus-fragrance was used,

nd this was defined as context B. The strength of illuminance did not differ inontext B (open arms 40 lux, closed arms 14 lux).

.4. Statistical analysis

The data were compared by Repeated Measures of ANOVA (RM ANOVA) withost hoc Bonferroni, unless otherwise stated. Results were considered to be signifi-antly different at a probability level of p < 0.05. Data are presented as means ± SEM.

Research 250 (2013) 32– 38 33

3. Results

The first cohort of mice (AX n = 9, nAX n = 10) was exposedto EPM daily for 8 days. The two strains showed clearly distinctphenotype at the first day (open arms: 120.4 ± 15.1 s for AX vs.193.1 ± 5 s for nAX; p < 0.001; closed arms: 130.2 ± 9.8 s for AX vs.57.2 ± 2.8 s for nAX; p ≤ 0.001, independent samples t-test). Bothstrains showed a continuously decreasing open-arm exploration,however this decline was stronger in the nAX mice (p = 0.020 forAX, and p < 0.001 for nAX; paired samples t-test; Fig. 1). Exemplartracks are shown at Fig. 1 and Supplementary Table 1 contains amore detailed dataset of other behavioral parameters.

Next, using another cohort of mice (AX n = 8, nAX n = 10), theeffect of introducing a change in the surrounding context on thebehavioral profile was investigated. Mice were exposed to the EPMusing normal illumination and ethanol to clear the apparatus beforeeach test. At day 3, the environmental context was changed to blue-light illumination and a citrus-fragrance cleaning substance, whichwe refer to as context B. It was found that the time spent with open-arms exploration increased in context B of the nAX, but significantlydecreased of the AX mice (RM ANOVA, normal condition vs. CC: AXopen arms: F(1,17) = 8.036, p = 0.011; closed arms: F(1,17) = 10.507,p = 0.005, nAX open arms: F(1,18) = 19.947, p < 0.001; closed arms:F(1,18) = 6.974, p = 0.017; Fig. 2). These behavioral patterns suggestthat AX mice were more anxious in the novel environment, andavoided the stressful open-arms. NAX mice, however, acted differ-ently, because the time spent in open-arms did not decrease overtime upon context B introduction in such a rate, as in the normalcondition). It might reflect a proactive behavioral pattern of thenAX mice, e.g. they tried to explore the environment in context B.The actual numbers can be found in Supplementary Table 2.

The same behavioral profile was found in mice which wereexposed first to context B, then to context A (AX n = 7 and nAX n = 6;Fig. 3. and Supplementary Table 3). Using this schema, context A isthe novel environment, where AX mice showed increased anxi-ety (RM ANOVA, open arms: F(1,14) = 1.711, p = 0.212; closed arms:F(1,14) = 7.373, p = 0.017), whereas nAX mice showed increasedexploratory behavior (RM ANOVA, open arms: F(1,14) = 6.674,p = 0.022; closed arms: F(1,14) = 1.712, p = 0.212), suggesting that itwas not the blue-light illumination per se that elicited changes inthe behavioral pattern, but instead the change in the surroundingcontext of the EPM apparatus.

3.1. The effect of buspirone

Buspirone, a clinically effective anxiolytic was injected i.p. intwo different doses for 4 weeks daily before, and during the exper-iments, and the behavior of mice were investigated in the EPM.The anxiolytic effect of bus was evident at the first day, whentreated AX mice (n = 8 for 2.5 mg/kg and n = 14 for 5.0 mg/kg)showed reduced anxiety, and this was dose-dependent (closedarms: 130.3 ± 9.8 s for control AX, 118.4 ± 10.4 s for AX 2.5 mg/kgbus, p = 0.947 vs. control and 90.7 ± 3.9 s for AX 5.0 mg/kg bustreated mice, p = 0.002 vs. control; open arms: 120.4 ± 15.1 s forcontrol AX, 109.5 ± 11.2 s for AX 2.5 mg/kg bus, p = 1.000 vs. control,138.9 ± 9.4 s for AX 5.0 mg/kg bus, p = 0.003 vs. control, one-wayANOVA post hoc Bonferroni). Interestingly, bus increased anxiety-like behavior of nAX mice (n = 10 for 2.5 mg/kg and n = 18 for5.0 mg/kg; closed arms: 57.2 ± 2.8 s for control nAX, 69.3 ± 6.1for nAX 2.5 mg/kg bus, p = 0.170 vs. control, 69.9 ± 2.8 for nAX5.0 mg/kg bus, p = 0.076 vs. control; one-way ANOVA post hoc Bon-ferroni; open arms: 193.2 ± 5.1 s for control nAX mice, 169.9 ± 8.4 s

for nAX 2.5 mg/kg bus, p = 0.705 vs. control, 172.6 ± 4.3 s for nAX5.0 mg/kg bus mice, p < 0.001 vs. control; one-way ANOVA post hocBonferroni Fig. 4. and Supplementary Table 4). Investigating thechange of kinetics of open-arms exploration over the 8-day long

34 J. Horváth et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 250 (2013) 32– 38

Fig. 1. Bar charts showing the total time spent in the open (A) and closed arms (B) of AX and nAX mice during the 8 day long trial. Representative tracks of the days indicatedas red rectangles are shown at panel C and D. The closed arms of the EPM are denoted with green borders. * P ≤ 0.05, RM ANOVA. (For interpretation of the references tocolour please see to the web version of article.)

Fig. 2. Mice responded differently to context change (indicated as a blue background between day 3 and 8 at panel A and B). Representative tracks from day 1 and day 3 (firstday at the novel context), as indicated as a red rectangle, are shown below (C and D). The closed arms of the EPM are denoted with green borders. * P ≤ 0.05, RM ANOVA. (Forinterpretation of the references to colour please see to the web version of article.)

J. Horváth et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 250 (2013) 32– 38 35

F nel B)2 o the w

ttFnnFi8

e

Fa

ig. 3. Bar charts showing the total time spent in the open (Panel A) and closed (Pa. * P ≤ 0.05, RM ANOVA. (For interpretation of the references to colour please see t

est session using RM ANOVA showed no difference between con-rol vs. 2.5 mg/kg bus group neither in AX or nAX mice (open arms:(1,16) = 0.653, p = 0.432 for AX mice and F(1,17) = 0.007, p = 0.937 forAX mice; Fig. 4.). However the time spent in the closed arms for theAX 2.5 mg/kg bus group was significantly different (RM ANOVA:(1,17) = 4.447, p = 0.049 vs. control; Fig. 4.). Similarly, bus treatmentn 5.0 mg/kg altered the time spent in closed-arms throughout the

days of AX mice (RM ANOVA: F(1,21) = 4.511, p = 0.046 vs. control).Mice treated with 2.5 mg/kg bus (AX n = 11, nAX n = 10), how-

ver, showed dramatic change in the novel context. AX mice spent

ig. 4. The effect of chronic buspirone administration (2.5 and 5.0 mg/kg) on the behaviorrms). Significance is indicated in the main text.

arms. Blue backgrounds indicate context B (see main text for details) at day 1 andeb version of article.)

much more time in the open arm compared to control group, indi-cating that low dose of bus changed the coping style of AX mice(RM ANOVA: F(1,20) = 19.170, p < 0.001 vs. control; Fig. 5. and Sup-plementary Table 5.). In contrast, treated nAX mice spent muchless time exploring the open arm than control counterparts, sug-gesting that anxiety has developed in the novel context due to bustreatment (RM ANOVA: F = 5.395, p = 0.032 vs. control).

(1,19)

AX mice treated with the higher dose of bus (n = 9) showed con-tinuously decreasing open-arm exploration upon context change,similarly to control mice (Fig. 5.). Although the difference was

of AX (A for open and B for closed arms) and nAX mice (C for open and D for closed

36 J. Horváth et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 250 (2013) 32– 38

F rofile

m ANOVa

sf5tt

(oFAeo

4

tisAsetbtc

mceb

ig. 5. The effect of chronic administration (2.5 and 5.0 mg/kg) on the behavioral pice upon context change. Blue background indicate novel context. * P ≤ 0.05, RM

rticle.)

ignificant (RM ANOVA: F(1,18) = 4.831, p = 0.042), there was no dif-erence between the time spent in the closed-arms of control vs. bus.0 mg/kg AX mice (RM ANOVA: F(1,18) = 2.428, p = 0.138), suppor-ing the notion that this dose of bus does not modify dramaticallyhe behavioral pattern of AX mice in a novel environment.

Similar results were found for nAX treated with 5.0 mg/kg busn = 9; Fig. 5.). Although there was significant difference in thepen-arms time between 5.0 mg/kg bus vs. control (RM ANOVA:(1,18) = 5.755, p = 0.028) the closed-arm time did not differ (RMNOVA: F(1,18) = 0.871, p = 0.364). This also supports the hypoth-sis, that 5.0 mg/kg bus does not alter profoundly the coping stylef nAX mice upon context change.

. Discussion

The conflicting hypotheses about the reasons of OTT assumehat different coping strategy is being used by the animals. Theres a strong correlation between trait anxiety and passive copingtyle with stressful situations, like a novel environmental context.lso, animal studies have found that inadequate coping strategy istrongly correlated with anxious phenotype. However, trait anxi-ty, by definition, does not change rapidly, thus we hypothesizedhat the reasons behind the decreasing open arm exploration maye different in the two strains. Thus one cohort of mice was exposedo the EPM for 8 consecutive days and an environmental contexthange was also introduced in another cohort of mice.

It was found that besides showing OTT on the second day, AX

ice showed no further decrease of exploratory motivation. In

ontrast, nAX mice exhibited a continuous decrease of open-armxploration, which is, as we suggest, not due to increased anxiety,ut to habituation. The lack of similar habituation in the AX strain

of AX (A for open and B for closed arms) and nAX (C for open and for closed arms)A. (For interpretation of the references to colour please see to the web version of

may be the consequence of elevated anxiety. This is supported byfindings, that in anxious rats, the level of plasma corticosteroidsdoes not habituate between trials [4]. These differences implydifferent coping styles of the two strains, thus we investigatedthe responses upon environmental context change. The observedeffects were not due to the blue illumination per se, because whenthe context was switched from blue light to normal white illumi-nation, the behavioral responses of AX and nAX were the same,suggesting that the change of familiar environmental to a novelone caused these behavioral patterns.

The readout for anxiety in the EPM is based on the conflictbetween the drive to explore (open arm) and the drive to avoid(closed arm), thus highly anxious mice are expected to prefer thesafe, and familiar closed arm in the novel context. Indeed, AXmice responded with a significantly reduced exploratory behav-ior (passive coping style) to the novel context. On the contrary,the response of nAX mice was manifested in increased open armexploration (active coping style). These results suggest that strainshaving different trait anxiety utilize different coping strategy in anexploration/aversive conflict. This proposal is supported by reportsshowing that the highly anxious DBA2 mouse shows decreasedopen arm exploration upon retest in changed environment (mazeorientation or laboratory environment) [24]. The available data isvery limited in the literature about the reactions of non-reactivestrains in a second EPM exposure combined with environmentalchanges. However, we expect that a less anxious strain, such asBALB/c [30] would behave similarly as our nAX mice. The only avail-

able data we are aware of shows that wild mice, who exhibit amarked decreased closed-arm activity compared to standard labo-ratory mice, do not show signs of OTT as is measured by open/closedarm occupancy upon the second EPM exposure [12]. Likewise, nAX

l Brain

mfiwdrw

4

arfe(EiltOtSspeftmmmeatisnastlrmirci

pprn

A

84Afaf

A

t

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

J. Horváth et al. / Behavioura

ice exhibited a reduced OTT at day 2, compared to AX mice. Thendings of Thiel et al. [29] also supportive: they report that ratsith high- or low-rearing behavior in a novel open-field test showifferent behavior upon re-exposure to the test apparatus. High-earing rats (active) respond with more time spent in the center,hile low-rearing rats (passive) show no change in this respect.

.1. The effect of buspirone

Buspirone, a 5-HT1A partial agonist is widely used in the clinics anxiolytic. Similarly to most antidepressants, the onset of actionequires several weeks of medication in human patients. There-ore we have used chronic, 4 weeks of bus treatment in the presentxperiments. It was found that chronic bus treatment in low dose2.5 mg/kg) did not alter the behavioral profiles of AX in the 8-dayPM session, but the larger dose (5.0 mg/kg) induced a graduallyncreasing closed-arm stay. Interestingly, mice treated with theower dose showed increased open-arm activity in the novel con-ext, but here the larger dose did not alter the profile of behavior.n the contrary, bus treatment in any of the doses did not dras-

ically modify open/closed arm profile of nAX in the 8-day EPM.trikingly, low dose bus treated mice exhibited a shift in the copingtrategy in the novel context, namely nAX mice showed a greaterreference for the closed arm, while AX mice exhibited increasedxploratory drive upon context change. The reason behind the dif-erence between the responses for the two applied doses is not easyo explain. First, it is known that activating the 5-HT1A receptor

ay induce anxiogenic and anxiolytic responses in laboratory ani-als. Bouwknecht et al. reported that a 5-HT1A agonist increasedeasures of anxiety in open-field activity test and the light-dark

xploration test of C57BL/6J (B6) mice, a strain regarded to be lessnxious. In contrast, the same treatment had minimal effect onhe highly anxious 129S6/SvEvTac (S6) strain [2]. Second, 5-HT1As localized pre- and postsynaptically, having opposite effects onerotonin neurosignalling [9,23]. The intricate mechanism gover-ing serotonin release is further complicated by the fact, that 5-HTutoreceptors, which provide a negative feedback loop inhibitingerotonin release [10], tend to desensitize, but even at high synap-ic serotonin concentration, they are capable of responding to largeeaps in serotonin level, e.g. at stressful situations [11]. This may beeflected in the dose-dependent effect of 5-HT1A agonist on anxietyeasured in unconditioned anxiety tests: low doses of were anx-

olytic, while high doses were anxiogenic [1,7,19]. Low doses mayesult in different 5-HT release pattern and extracellular level andonsequently different activation of 5-HT receptor subsets whichs manifested in decreased anxiety [17,27].

Based on these results we hypothesize that 5-HT1A stimulationattern plays a role in the development of coping strategy of micelaced in a new environmental context. These findings may haveelevance in treating patients suffering from pathological fears ofovel environments.

cknowledgements

This study was supported by the following grants: OTKA PD3581 from the Hungarian National Scientific Fund and TÁMOP-.2.2.A-11/1/KONV-2012-0052 from the National Developmentgency (NFÜ) grant. The authors wish to thank to Botond Penke

or his support and to Emoke Borbély for her excellent technicalssistance. The authors are also grateful to Kseniya Toropova forruitful discussions.

ppendix A. Supplementary data

Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, inhe online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.014.

[

Research 250 (2013) 32– 38 37

References

[1] Borsini F, Brambilla A, Grippa N, Pitsikas N. Behavioral effects of flibanserin(BIMT 17). Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 1999;64(1):137–46.

[2] Bouwknecht JA, van der Gugten J, Groenink L, Olivier B, Paylor RE. Behav-ioral and physiological mouse models for anxiety: effects of flesinoxanin 129S6/SvEvTac and C57BL/6J mice. European Journal of Pharmacology2004;494(1):45–53.

[3] File SE, Zangrossi Jr H. “One-trial tolerance” to the anxiolytic actions of benzo-diazepines in the elevated plus-maze, or the development of a phobic state?Psychopharmacology 1993;110(1–2):240–4.

[4] File SE, Zangrossi Jr H, Sanders FL, Mabbutt PS. Raised corticosterone inthe rat after exposure to the elevated plus-maze. Psychopharmacology1994;113(3–4):543–6.

[5] File SE, Zangrossi Jr H, Viana M, Graeff FG. Trial 2 in the elevated plus-maze: adifferent form of fear? Psychopharmacology 1993;111(4):491–4.

[6] Frussa-Filho R, Ribeiro Rde A. One-trial tolerance to the effects of chlor-diazepoxide in the elevated plus-maze is not due to acquisition of aphobic avoidance of open arms during initial exposure. Life Sciences2002;71(5):519–25.

[7] Griebel G, Misslin R, Pawlowski M, Guardiola Lemaitre B, Guillaumet G, Bizot-Espiard J. Anxiolytic-like effects of a selective 5-HT1A agonist, S20244, and itsenantiomers in mice. Neuroreport 1992;3(1):84–6.

[8] Haller J, Alicki M. Current animal models of anxiety, anxiety disorders, andanxiolytic drugs. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 2012;25(1):59–64.

[9] Hillegaart V, Estival A, Ahlenius S. Evidence for specific involvement of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A/C receptors in the expression of patterns of spontaneousmotor activity of the rat. European Journal of Pharmacology 1996;295(2–3):155–61.

10] Hjorth S. Serotonin 5-HT1A autoreceptor blockade potentiates the ability ofthe 5-HT reuptake inhibitor citalopram to increase nerve terminal output of5-HT in vivo: a microdialysis study. Journal of Neurochemistry 1993;60(2):776–9.

11] Hjorth S, Bengtsson HJ, Kullberg A, Carlzon D, Peilot H, Auerbach SB. Serotoninautoreceptor function and antidepressant drug action. Journal of Psychophar-macology (Oxford, England) 2000;14(2):177–85.

12] Holmes A, Parmigiani S, Ferrari PF, Palanza P, Rodgers RJ. Behavioral profile ofwild mice in the elevated plus-maze test for anxiety. Physiology & Behavior2000;71(5):509–16.

13] Hurst JL, West RS. Taming anxiety in laboratory mice. Nature Methods2010;7(10):825–6.

14] Keck ME, Welt T, Post A, Muller MB, Toschi N, Wigger A, et al. Neuroendocrineand behavioral effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in a psy-chopathological animal model are suggestive of antidepressant-like effects.Neuropsychopharmacology 2001;24(4):337–49.

15] Keck ME, Wigger A, Welt T, Muller MB, Gesing A, Reul JM, et al. Vaso-pressin mediates the response of the combined dexamethasone/CRH test inhyper-anxious rats: implications for pathogenesis of affective disorders. Neu-ropsychopharmacology 2002;26(1):94–105.

16] Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Prevalence,severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the NationalComorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry 2005;62(6):617–27.

17] Kostowski W, Plaznik A, Stefanski R. Intra-hippocampal buspirone in animalmodels of anxiety. European Journal of Pharmacology 1989;168(3):393–6.

18] Lee C, Rodgers RJ. Antinociceptive effects of elevated plus-maze expo-sure: influence of opiate receptor manipulations. Psychopharmacology1990;102(4):507–13.

19] Misslin R, Griebel G, Saffroy-Spittler M, Vogel E. Anxiolytic and sedativeeffects of 5-HT1A ligands, 8-OH-DPAT and MDL 73005EF, in mice. Neuroreport1990;1(3–4):267–70.

20] Ohl F, Toschi N, Wigger A, Henniger MS, Landgraf R. Dimensions of emotionalityin a rat model of innate anxiety. Behavioral Neuroscience 2001;115(2):429–36.

21] Pawlak CR, Ho YJ, Schwarting RK. Animal models of human psychopathologybased on individual differences in novelty-seeking and anxiety. Neuroscienceand Biobehavioral Reviews 2008;32(8):1544–68.

22] Pereira JK, Vieira RJ, Konishi CT, Ribeiro RA, Frussa-Filho R. The phenomenon of“one-trial tolerance” to the anxiolytic effect of chlordiazepoxide in the elevatedplus-maze is abolished by the introduction of a motivational conflict situation.Life Sciences 1999;65(10):PL101–7.

23] Pineyro G, Blier P. Autoregulation of serotonin neurons: role in antidepressantdrug action. Pharmacological Reviews 1999;51(3):533–91.

24] Rodgers RJ, Johnson NJ, Carr J, Hodgson TP. Resistance of experientially-induced changes in murine plus-maze behaviour to altered retest conditions.Behavioural Brain Research 1997;86(1):71–7.

25] Rodgers RJ, Lee C, Shepherd JK. Effects of diazepam on behavioural andantinociceptive responses to the elevated plus-maze in male mice dependupon treatment regimen and prior maze experience. Psychopharmacology1992;106(1):102–10.

26] Rodgers RJ, Shepherd JK. Influence of prior maze experience on behaviour andresponse to diazepam in the elevated plus-maze and light/dark tests of anxiety

in mice. Psychopharmacology 1993;113(2):237–42.

27] Stefanski R, Palejko W, Bidzinski A, Kostowski W, Plaznik A. Serotoner-gic innervation of the hippocampus and nucleus accumbens septi and theanxiolytic-like action of midazolam and 5-HT1A receptor agonists. Neuro-pharmacology 1993;32(10):977–85.

3 l Brain

[

[

[30] Trullas R, Skolnick P. Differences in fear motivated behaviors among inbred

8 J. Horváth et al. / Behavioura

28] Szego EM, Janaky T, Szabo Z, Csorba A, Kompagne H, Muller G, et al. A mouse

model of anxiety molecularly characterized by altered protein networks in thebrain proteome. European Neuropsychopharmacology 2010;20(2):96–111.

29] Thiel CM, Muller CP, Huston JP, Schwarting RK. High versus low reactivity to anovel environment: behavioural, pharmacological and neurochemical assess-ments. Neuroscience 1999;93(1):243–51.

[

Research 250 (2013) 32– 38

mouse strains. Psychopharmacology 1993;111(3):323–31.31] Virok DP, Kis Z, Szegedi V, Juhasz G, Zvara Jr A, Muller G, et al. Functional changes

in transcriptomes of the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in a mouse modelof anxiety. Pharmacological Report 2011;63(2):348–61.


Recommended