+ All Categories
Home > Documents > apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

Date post: 28-Feb-2018
Category:
Upload: chango68
View: 218 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 44

Transcript
  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    1/44

    Psychological

    Bulletin

    1996. Vol.

    1 1 9 ,

    No. 3,488-531

    Copyright

    1996bv theAmerican Psychological Association, Inc.

    0033-2909/96/$3.00

    TheRelationship Between Social SupportandPhysiological Processes:

    AReview With EmphasisonUnderlying Mechanisms

    and

    Implications

    for

    Health

    BertN.Uchino

    University

    ofU tah

    JohnT.

    Cacioppo

    andJaniceK.Kiecolt-Glaser

    Ohio S tateU niversity

    Inthis

    review,

    theauthors examinetheevidence linking social support tophysiological processes

    andcharacterizethepotential mechanisms responsiblefo rthese covariations. Areviewof

    8 1

    studies

    revealedthat social support

    w as

    reliably related

    tobeneficial

    effects

    on

    aspects

    of the

    cardiovascular,

    endocrine,

    an d

    immune systems.

    A n

    analysis

    of

    potential mechanisms underlying these associations

    revealed that(a) potential health-related behaviorsdo notappearto beresponsibleforthese associ-

    ations;

    (b)

    stress-buffering effects operate

    in

    some studies;

    (c)

    familial sources

    of

    support

    may be

    important;and( d )emotional support appearsto be atleast1important dimensionofsocial support.

    Recommendations

    an d

    directions

    fo r future

    research include

    t he

    importance

    of

    conceptualizing

    social supportas amultidimensional construct, examinationofpotential mechanismsacrosslevels

    of

    analyses,andattention to thephysiological processo finterest.

    Social relationshipsare aubiquitous part oflife,serving impor-

    tant social, psychological, andbehavioral functions across the life-

    span. Moreimportant, both

    the

    quantity

    and

    quality

    of

    social

    re-

    lationships havebeen reliably related

    to

    morbidity

    and

    mortality

    (see

    reviewsbyBlazer,

    1982;Broadheadetal., 1983;Cassell,

    1976;

    Cobb,

    1976;S.Cohen&Syme, 1985;andHouse,

    Landis,

    & Um-

    berson, 1988) .For

    instance,

    Houseet

    al.

    reviewed evidencefrom6

    largeprospective studies indicating that mortalityishigher among

    more socially isolated individuals. These associations hold even

    afterinclusionofstandard control variables suchas age andinitial

    healthstatus. Indeed, House

    et al.

    summarized evidence showing

    that

    theassociation

    between social relationships

    and

    health

    is

    com-

    parable with standard risk factors, including smoking, blood

    pres-

    sure,an dphysical activity.

    An important issue concerns the potential mechanismsre-

    sponsible

    for the

    epidemiological links between social relation-

    ships

    and

    such long-term health consequences

    (S .

    Cohen,

    1988;

    S .Cohen &Wills, 1985;

    Kiecolt-Glaser

    &Glaser, 19 89) . In the

    present review,

    we first

    examine

    the

    evidence linking

    th e

    posi-

    Bert N .Uchino, Department of

    Psychology

    an d Health Psychology

    Program, University

    of

    Utah; John

    T.

    Cacioppo, Department

    of

    Psy-

    chology and

    Brain, Behavior,

    I mmu n i ty ,

    an d

    Health Program, Ohio

    State University; Janice K. Kiecolt-Glaser, Department ofPsychiatry

    an d

    Brain,

    Behavior, Immunity, an d

    Health Program, Ohio State

    University.

    W e

    thank Timothy Smith for hiscommentso n adraft ofthis article.

    This study

    w as

    partially supported

    by

    Grants

    T 3 2 - M H 1 8 8 3 1 ,

    MH44660,

    an d

    MH42096

    from th e

    National Institute

    of

    Mental

    Health,

    asupplementto MH42 096from th eOffice ofWomen's Health,

    Grant D B S 9 2 1 1 4 8 3 from th eNational Science Foundation, and the

    John

    D. and

    Catherine

    T .

    MacArthur Foundation.

    Correspondence concerning this article should

    b e

    addressed

    to

    Bert

    N .Uchino, Department ofPsychology,5 02Social-Behavioral Sciences

    Building,

    U niversity

    ofUtah, Salt Lake

    City,

    Utah 8 41 1 2 .Electronic

    mail may be

    sent

    v ia

    Internet

    to

    [email protected].

    tiveaspectsofsocial relationships (i.e., social support)tophys-

    iologicalprocesses.

    We

    characterize these associations

    by

    exam-

    ining

    the

    influence

    of social

    support

    on

    aspects

    ofthe cardiovas-

    cular, endocrine, andimmune systems.The literature search

    w asconducted using

    the

    ancestry approach

    and

    with

    PsycLIT

    (1974-1995) an d Medline (1983-1995) by crossing thekey-

    words

    socialsupport, social networks or social integrationwith

    cardiovascular, blood

    pressure endocrine

    or

    immune. Only

    studies whose researchers directly examinedtheassociationbe-

    tween

    social support an dphysiological function were included

    in

    this review. Based onthis research, w eexamined potential

    mechanisms responsible for the

    associations

    between social

    support an dphysiologicalfunction (S .Cohen, 198 8 ) .

    W esummarizetheresearch examining social supportandphys-

    iological processes by using both qualitative and meta-analytic

    procedures. Major details regarding studies (e.g., typeof support

    assessment andmainfindings)werefirstcharacterized andana-

    lyzed

    in

    tabular

    form.

    Based

    on

    this qualitative analysis, meta-

    analytic procedures were used primarily when (a) thepattern of

    results were equivocal

    and (b)

    there were

    a

    sufficient number

    of

    relativelyhomogeneous studies(e.g.,similar paradigms) toreli-

    ablycharacterize

    th e

    effects

    of

    interest.

    In

    addition, meta-analytic

    procedures were used

    to

    testspecifichypothesesfrom

    ou r

    qualita-

    tive

    analyses.

    T he

    meta-analysis

    w as

    performed using

    a

    commer-

    cially available software package(Mullin, 1989)thatprovidedde-

    tailed results regarding combined testsof

    significance

    levels,effect

    sizes, tests

    of

    variability regarding significance levels

    an d

    effect

    sizes, and a

    fail-safe number.

    1

    Results of theunweighted meta-

    analysis

    a re reported, butanalyses weightedbysample size were

    also performedand produced comparable results. Toreduce the

    1

    Th e fail-safe

    number represents

    the

    number

    of

    unpublished

    null

    studiesthat would

    be

    needed

    to

    overturn

    th e

    conclusions

    found in the

    meta-analysis. Although there is no standard fail-safe number, Rosen-

    thai ( 1 9 8 4 )suggests that

    5k +

    10,where

    k

    represents the number of

    retrieved

    studies, representsareasonable tolerance

    level.

    48 8

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    2/44

    SOC IAL SUPPORT AND PHYSIOLOGY

    489

    problem

    of

    nonindependence

    for

    studies with multiple assess-

    ments of social support, results were first transformed within a

    studyto z scores, averaged, and then entered into themeta-analy-

    sis. Therefore,

    as

    recommended

    by

    Rosenthal

    (1984),

    only

    one

    statistic

    was

    included fromeach study. Finally, when resultswere

    reported

    as

    nonsignificant,

    a

    conservative significance

    level

    of .50

    was

    used

    (Mullin,

    1989).

    Oneimportant sourceofheterogeneityin theliteratureon so-

    cial support and health revolves around the conceptualization and

    measurement ofsupport (Barrera, 1986;S .Cohen&Wills, 1985;

    Heitzmann

    &

    Kaplan, 1988;Orth-Gomer&Unden, 1987;Tardy,

    1985;

    W inemiller,Mitchell,Sutliff,

    &

    Cline, 19 93).

    In the

    present

    review,w e

    include diverse studies with both structural (e.g., social

    network)

    andfunctional(e.g., emotional support) measuresofso-

    cial

    support.Structural measures of support assess the existence

    and interconnection between various social relationships (e.g.,

    number of

    siblings), whereas functional measures

    of

    support

    as-

    sess

    the

    particular

    functions

    that social relationships

    may

    serve

    (e.g.,providing emotional

    or

    informationalsupport).

    The un-

    derlying

    theme

    of

    these assessments

    is

    that they conceptually mea-

    sure the potentially positive aspects of social relationships. This

    diversity

    reflects,

    inpart,the interestthatsocial support has gener-

    ated in different

    areas

    ofinquiry(e.g.,sociology, psychology,and

    health). Whenthenumbero fstudies permitted it, weperformed

    focused

    comparisons between structural

    and

    functional measures

    of

    support

    to

    examine

    if

    they

    are

    associated withquantitatively

    different effects on

    physiological

    function (S .

    Cohen

    &

    Wills,

    1985).Inaddition, whenappropriatew ediscusstheimplications

    ofboth measures

    in

    research regarding social support, physiologi-

    cal

    processes,and health.

    Social Support and Physiological Processes

    More than

    18

    years have passed sincetheseminal reviewsby

    Cassell

    ( 1 9 7 6 )and

    Cobb

    ( 1 9 7 6 )on the

    importance

    of

    social

    relationships

    for

    health.These

    2

    reviews

    in

    particular have been

    responsible

    for

    generating interest

    in

    social support

    and its

    rela-

    tionshipto psychological and physical well-being.Cobbfocused

    primarily on the stress-buffering effects ofsocial support an d

    emphasized the informational value of social support processes

    (e.g.,thatone iscaredfor and loved)in fosteringcopingand

    adaptation. Similarly, Cassell viewed social relationships

    as po-

    tentiallybuffering

    th e

    individual

    from life

    stressors

    bu t

    further

    emphasized theimportanceofphysiologicalprocesses in medi-

    atingtheeffectsofsocial relationships:

    The

    psychosocial processes thus

    can be envisaged as

    enhancingsus-

    ceptibility todisease.T he

    clinical

    manifestations ofthis enhanced

    susceptibility

    will not be a function of the

    particular psychosocial

    stressor,

    but of the physicochem ical or microbiologic

    disease agents

    harbored by the organism or to

    which

    the organism isexposed.

    (Cassell,

    1976,p.109)

    As

    suggested

    by

    Cassell,

    the

    associations between social support

    and

    physical health have been found

    on

    such diverse heath out-

    comes (e.g., coronary heart disease, cancer, and infectious

    illnesses)thatthere are probably multiple physiological path-

    ways bywhich social support m ay

    influence

    diseasestates. In

    thisreview,wefocuson thecardiovascular, endocrine, and im-

    m u n e

    systemsaspotential physiological pathwaysbywhichso-

    cial

    support

    influences

    physical health.

    CorrelationalStudies Examiningth e

    Asso ciation

    Between

    Social

    Support and Cardiovascular

    Function

    O fthe

    81studies whose researchers examined social support

    and

    physiological processes,

    57

    focused

    on

    aspects

    of

    cardiovas-

    cular

    function. This emphasis

    is

    understandable considering

    thatcardiovasculardisorders

    are

    still

    the

    leading cause

    of

    death

    inthe United

    States

    and that social support has been linked to

    lowercoronary heart disease

    (CHD)

    rates (House etal.,1988).

    Conceptually,

    an

    examination

    of the

    relationship between

    so-

    cialsupport and the cardiovascular system is important because

    of its

    implications

    for

    both

    th e

    development

    and

    maintenance

    of

    CHD. For instance, the prognostic value of tonic arterial blood

    pressure in

    predicting cardiovascular disorders

    is

    widely

    ac-

    cepted

    (J. J.

    Smith

    & Kampine,

    1990). Additionally,

    the

    reac-

    tivityhypothesis suggests that increased cardiovascular reactiv-

    ity

    to

    stress

    may be an

    important factor

    in the

    development

    of

    cardiovascular

    disorders (see Krantz & Manuck,

    1984;

    Ma-

    nuck, 1994;and Matthewsetal.,

    1986).

    Because

    of the

    relatively large number

    of

    studies examining

    cardiovascular parameters,w e now

    briefly

    review basic princi-

    plesofcardiovascular physiology.Thecardiovascular systemis

    involvedin thetransportofoxygenand theremovalofcarbon

    dioxide,

    acritical

    function

    foreverycellandorganin thebody

    (see

    Larsen, Schneiderman,

    &

    Pasin, 1986;

    and J. J.

    Smith

    &

    Kampine, 1990,for

    detailed

    reviews).T he

    heart muscle gener-

    ates

    the

    necessaryforce

    for the

    circulatory process.

    The

    vascu-

    lature (i.e., arteries, veins, and capillaries) serves as the vehicle

    for thepumpingof theheart.

    The

    most commonly used cardiovascular measures

    in

    this

    re-

    viewinclude heart rate, systolic blood pressure

    ( S B P ) ,and

    dia-

    stolic

    blood pressure (DBP). Heart rate, a measure of cardiac

    chronotropy, is

    usually

    expressed in beats per minute. It is

    jointly

    determined

    by the

    sympathetic

    and

    parasympathetic

    nervoussystems: Sympathetic activation increases heart rate,

    whereas

    parasympathetic activation decreases heartrate.

    S BP and DBP are

    measures

    of the

    force

    of

    blood against

    the

    arterial walls

    and are a

    function

    of

    both cardiac output

    and the

    relativestate

    of the

    vasculature. Because

    of the

    importance

    of

    blood pressure in the transport of blood, it is normally a regu-

    lated endpoint.

    SBP is

    associated with ventricular contraction

    (i.e., systole)and therefore corresponds to the peak arterial

    pressure.

    DBP is

    associated with ventricular relaxation (i.e.,

    diastole)

    and corresponds to the lowest arterial pressure.

    Fo rpurposes

    of

    this review,

    it is

    important

    to

    distinguish

    be-

    tweentonic

    and

    phasic components

    of

    cardiovascular activity

    (Cacioppo, Berntson,&Andersen,

    1 9 9 1 ) .

    Tonic orbasal levels

    of

    cardiovascular activity provide information

    on the

    tonic

    physiologicstate

    of an

    individual.

    The

    correlational studies

    ex-

    amining

    the association between social support and cardiovas-

    cular

    function

    have

    focused primarily

    on

    tonic measures.

    The

    phasicor

    reactivity components

    o f

    cardiovascular activity refer

    to momentary fluctuations

    from

    tonic levels. Recent laboratory

    studies, reviewed later, have

    focusedon the

    possibility that

    so-

    cial

    support may reduce cardiovascular reactivity to acute psy-

    chosocial stressors.

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    3/44

    49 0

    U C H I N O ,

    CACIOPPO, A N DKIECOLT-GLASER

    Animportant issue to consider is the psychometric properties

    of

    the physiological assessmentsbecause they bear on the po-

    tential mechanisms linking social support to long-term physical

    health. In this regard, it is conceptually important to distinguish

    between measurement reliability

    and

    temporal stability.Mea-

    surement

    reliabilityrefers

    to the

    accurate assessment

    of the

    physiologicalstateat onepointin time. In comparison,tempo-

    ra l

    stabilityrefers to a

    dispositional

    characterization of physio-

    logical function (i.e., stability

    of the

    physiological assessment

    across different situations and occasions). Adequate measure-

    ment reliability

    is

    necessary

    but not

    sufficient

    for

    temporal sta-

    bility.

    The distinction between measurement reliability and

    temporal stability is important because if social support is to

    have

    effects

    ondisease processes withalong-term etiology,the

    physiological assessments should be characterized by temporal

    stability. The assessment context (e.g.,specifictasks), popula-

    tion (e.g., phobics), and techniques (e.g., specificityof tracers

    inradioimmunoassay) may all influence anindividual differ-

    ence assessment

    of

    physiological function.

    As an

    example,

    a

    needle stick

    isoftenassociated

    with relatively short-term eleva-

    tionsincatecholamines. Becauseof themeasurement reliability

    of

    current techniques

    ( B a u m

    &Grunberg,1 9 9 5 ),th ecatechol-

    amine changes due to venipuncture would be accurately as-

    sessed at that point in time. However, this may be a poor index

    of

    an individual's cathecholamine response across time and

    situations.

    Past researchers haveexaminedthetemporal stabilityofheart

    rate, SBP,andDBF reactivity.A sreviewedby

    Manuck,

    Kaspro-

    wicz,

    Monroe,

    Larkin,

    and Kaplan

    ( 1 9 8 9 ) ,

    measures of heart rate

    reactivity evidence the strongest

    test-retest

    correlation, typically

    rangingfrom.67 to

    .91.

    S BPreactivity tendstoevidence adequate

    temporal stability that isslightly lower thanth estability seenfor

    heart rate, whereas

    DBF

    tends to

    show

    relatively low

    test-retest

    stability. Although manyof thestudies reviewedbyManucket

    al .

    did not reportdataon the

    test-retest

    stability of tonic measures,

    the patterns of stabilityacrossheart rate, SBP, and

    DBF

    appear

    similartothat forreactivity assessments. Itshouldbenoted, how-

    ever,that researchershavedemonstrated thatthestabilityofthese

    cardiovascular assessments, including

    DBF,

    are enhanced consid-

    erably when assessments

    are

    aggregated across multiple time

    pointsandmultipletasks (Kamarck, 1992;KamarcketaL,1992;

    Manuck, 1994).

    There are several methodological issues related to an exami-

    nation of the relationship between social support and cardio-

    vascular function.

    In

    particular,

    the use of

    appropriate statisti-

    cal

    controls

    is

    important

    as

    many

    of the

    studies reviewed

    in

    this

    section are correlational studies in which potential associations

    withconfounding variables

    may

    occur.

    For

    instance, social sup-

    port may be correlated with socioeconomic status,medication

    use, age,

    and

    other factors that

    mayhave

    direct influences

    on

    physiological function. We should note that there is some dis-

    crepancy

    in the

    literature

    on

    whether such variables

    are

    poten-

    tial confounding variables or mechanisms by which social sup-

    port has an association with health (S. Cohen,

    1988;

    House et

    al.,

    1988).

    In our

    tabular analyses

    of

    each study,

    we

    explicitly

    note when such statistical controls were used.

    In

    addition,

    we

    discuss

    the

    attention

    (or

    lack thereof) paid

    to

    appropriate sta-

    tistical controls

    and its

    implications

    for the

    mechanisms

    un-

    derlyingthe relationships between social support and cardiovas-

    cular function.

    Many of the studies on social support and cardiovascular

    function

    have used a correlational design with normotensive in-

    dividuals.Table 1summarizes28correlational studies, most

    ofwhich used middle-aged and older adult samples

    from

    the

    community.Twentystudies examined both

    men and

    women,

    5

    examined only men,2examined only women, and 1study did

    not report the gender composition of the sample. Researchers

    of 14 of

    these studies explicitly assessed some aspect

    of familial

    support. In addition, researchers of 7 studies assessed structural

    measures

    of

    support,

    of

    15studies assessed functional measures

    ofsupport,

    and of 6

    studies assessed both structural

    and func-

    tional measures of support.

    In general, the results of the correlational studies are consis-

    tent with the notion that higher social support isassociatedwith

    better cardiovascular regulation (e.g., lower blood pressure). In

    1

    of the first

    studies investigating

    the

    relationship between social

    support

    and

    cardiovascularfunction,Kasl

    and

    Cobb(1980)

    ex-

    amined the

    influence

    of social support on blood pressure

    changes

    in response to job termination; they reported that per-

    ceptions of social support were negatively related to blood pres-

    surechanges

    in

    response

    to job

    loss.

    To

    summarize Table

    1 ,

    researchers of 23 studies reported some evidence that social

    supportwasassociated with better cardiovascular function,of

    4 studies reported no relationship (see Ely &Mostardi,

    1986;

    Houben, Diedriks, Kant,

    &

    Notermans, 1990; Kaufmann

    &

    Beehr,1986;

    an d

    Lercher, Hortnagl,

    &

    Kofler,

    19 93), and of 1

    reported opposite

    effects

    (Hansell,

    1985) . Ameta-analysis of

    21 correlational studies whose researchers reported data

    on the

    association between social support and blood pressure revealed

    a significantcombined test( z =4.22,p =.00001,fail-safe n =

    117.38) .

    2

    Th e

    mean effect size

    (r) w as

    .08, suggesting

    a

    small

    but

    reliable

    effect

    across studies. None

    of the

    tests

    ofvariability

    w as

    significant (p >.45).Thus,theevidencefor anassociation

    between

    social support and lower blood pressure levels appears

    reliable.

    W e

    coded eachof thestudies includedin themeta-analysisas

    measuringstructuralorfunctional measuresofsupport.Of the

    6studies that assessed both types of support, we were able to

    separate the

    effects

    in 4 ofthese studies. Therefore, data from 9

    studies were identifiedasstructural, an ddata from 14studies

    2

    The meta-analysisconsistedof

    21

    studies that directly examinedthe

    association between social support an dtonic blood pressure levels.In 2

    cases,w e

    averaged

    the

    results reported across

    2

    differentpublished stud-

    ies(i.e., Dressier, 1980, 1983;Janes, 1990; Janes &Pawson, 1986)b e-

    cause data were apparently reported on the same sample. In addition,

    the 5studies examining job-related social support were excluded

    from

    thisanalysis because it had been identified a priori as a

    feature

    associ-

    atedwithinconsistent

    effects. In the

    text,

    w e

    examine

    in

    detail potential

    reasons

    w hyjob-related

    support

    m ay be

    associatedw ith weak

    effects on

    blood

    pressure.

    In o urinitial search,w eexcluded2studies examiningtherelationship

    between

    social support and blood pressure for methodological reasons

    (James,

    LaCroix, Kleinbaum,

    &

    Strogatz, 1984;

    Orth-Gomer, Rosen-

    gren, &Wilhelmsen, 1993). More specifically,these studies included

    participantso ncardiovascular medicationbut did notaccount fo rthis

    factor in reporting the association between social support and blood

    pressure.

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    4/44

    SOCIAL

    SUPPORTAN DPHYSIOLOGY

    491

    were

    identified

    as

    functional measures

    of

    support. Focused

    comparisons

    betweenthestructuralandfunctional

    measures

    of

    support

    revealed

    n o

    differences

    in significance level(p =

    .29)

    or

    effectsize(p =

    .47).

    Although appropriate caution

    is

    warranted

    because of the

    small

    numberof

    studies contrasted,

    these

    data

    are consistent with the larger literature, suggesting that both

    structural

    and

    functional measures

    ofsupport

    predict

    benefi-

    cial effects on

    physical health. However,

    the

    specificpsychologi-

    cal

    and behavioral mechanisms that contribute to these

    effects

    may

    differ

    for structural and functional

    measures(S.Cohen,

    1988).We

    return

    to a

    discussion

    of

    such issues later

    in the

    review.

    Researchers in 3 of the 4

    studies that

    did not find anyrela-

    tionship between indices

    of

    social support

    and

    blood pressure

    regulation measuredjob-relatedsocial support (Houben

    et

    al.,

    1990;

    Kaufmann &

    Beehr, 1986; Lercher

    et

    al.,1993). How-

    ever, Winnubst, Marcelissen, & Kleber (1982) and Unden,

    Orth-Gomer,&Elofssen(1991)also examined work-related

    social support and reported some effects oncardiovascular

    function.

    One

    potential

    reason for this discrepancy may be re-

    latedto thepsychometric propertiesof themeasuresof social

    support.For example, the Lercher et al. measure of social sup-

    port was two dichotomous questions (alsosee Houben etal.,

    1990,

    which also contains two questions), whereas Winnubst et

    al.'s measure containedfivequestionsandUndenetal.'smea-

    sure

    containedsixquestions. AlthoughKaufmann andBeehr

    did

    report highinternal consistenciesfortheir job-related social

    support measures(.59 90 mm/Hg, or

    current

    use of hypertensive

    medication), particularly

    in low

    income Black

    participants. These dataareconsistentwiththenotion thatspecific

    support components may be moreeffectivewhen they meet the

    demands of related situations.

    One concern in these correlational studies was the rarity in

    whichpsychometricdataregarding the measurement ofsocial

    support werereported. Only 11studies made referenceto the

    psychometric

    properties

    oftheir scale

    (e.g.,

    factor analysisand

    internal consistency).Giventheheterogeneityinwhichstudies

    summarized

    in

    Table

    1 have

    conceptualized

    and

    measured

    so-

    cial support, thescales'

    psychometric

    properties are important

    to examine, especially for the less validated measures of

    support.

    Relatedly, onlyresearchersof 4studies inTable 1reported

    any

    data

    on the

    temporal stability

    of the

    cardiovascular assess-

    t xt continues

    on

    page 499)

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    5/44

    49 2

    UCHINO,

    CACIOPPO,

    AND KIECOLT-GLASER

    2

    1

    -J

    K

    2

    1

    u

    s

    e g

    i

    r

    d

    a

    a

    a

    s

    s

    1

    a.

    $

    ^

    0

    to

    tf.

    B

    4 ;

    oi

    c

    c o

    Z

    5

    s

    a **

    *

    o

    1? c

    S a

    .2 * 3 t

    *y K

    gt>

    il

    E-

    8

    S

    S.O.

    >-S-i.6

    Is

    11

    a

    C

    l3

    C M

    *=

    c

    i-f ie

    , 3

    - t a - ir

    g

    c go.

    3

    J

    O. *-*

    - E

    3

    e

    o-

    S

    I c9

    o*

    3

    D

    co Q

    a.

    03

    a

    flu

    03

    CO

    e S

    1-2

    &3-

    03

    -*

    a

    S'?T3

    s

    I

    G"

    u

    c

    .2

    -o

    c

    I

    3

    a T 3

    '

    w

    C

    t

    c

    c *

    o u t 73

    l slrfl

    1sj

    s s 5 g

    fc B ft

    10

    W5

    13

    3

    S

    '>

    '5

    c

    o.

    lc

    Ert

    i

    e

    y

    m

    m

    * ec

    g o

    j

    a

    o

    g

    s

    b

    n

    2

    o

    c

    o

    m

    t

    }

    0 - 5

    1.5

    si

    I

    1

    "

    ,

    T

    o

    2,

    u

    >

    Ci

    o

    g

    c

    2

    '

    u^

    c

    t-

    J5

    E

    e

    e

    o

    n

    i^

    'rt

    Q

    ft

    '

    g

    (U

    S

    &

    8

    co

    3 - s

    85

    '-S trt

    on

    .S

    W5

    tfi

    2

    "~*

    "O _O

    c5 T3

    V 5

    C

    bO

    01

    ^

    a

    S c

    Si

    c

    o

    b

    w

    l

    o

    e

    h

    g

    n

    s

    g*

    S tS

    c

    x:

    ^

    IU

    E

    _ c

    Q,

    IS

    hg

    2 5

    .S

    E

    .g

    uo

    C

    b

    o

    e

    e g

    S 2

    o

    (A

    'S

    8 .

    3

    w c #

    5IS

    z

    CL,

    CQ

    Q

    A."

    S3

    CO

    5

    1

    o

    r

    o

    m

    c

    w

    r

    v

    s

    o

    s

    II

    W5

    .S

    o c a

    c

    1

    o

    o

    IS-

    c

    O "

    E n

    r^ -

    v^

    vo

    "a

    .0

    S

    g

    6

    "3

    o

    |R

    3 OO

    C

    Os

    C-

    ^^"

    .1

    i)

    S 2

    ?

    oa o

    5-8*

    m

    - py

    > k>

    A

    >

    ^

    >

    *"

    9 S-0

    S3

    "o" *

    g'S-g

    5

    O

    ^O J2

    rt

    '31 BJ >

    k.t- -a

    iSS-oS

    >f > 8.S

    i--*

    U

    5

    rj

    o> MIu

    O X

    &2&PS

    303

    g

    cflJ3 w "

    t

    Ipl

    C Q *-

    3

    ^

    *" O

    9

    g

    "

    S"U 9-"

    e

    &

    c

    -5

    J i f f e S

    .2so 3 9 13

    o

    co

    o Q

    0 0 , 0

    co co H

    0-

    03

    Q

    oT

    03

    CO

    15

    3 ^j

    '5

    1

    8

    .S

    - s

    a

    'S .S

    s a s

    O

    '?

    'B

    "si's

    4> O *-i

    |1

    35 a j C

    a c 5

    ft. Z

    VO

    u-> c

    a a

    e

    o

    a*

    .2

    ^1 MOS

    IB2

    ^^s

    T f

    T I

    73

    o

    c 3

    1

    m

    oo

    o>

    t

    u

    "

    Q

    g.

    i

    y

    p

    n

    a

    v

    o

    u

    sis

    a

    2

    s

    E

    0 U

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    6/44

    SOCIAL

    SUPPORT

    AND

    PHYSIOLOGY

    493

    ^

    ^ s

    s

    ^

    ^

    c

    2

    R

    u

    s

    S

    trt

    Z S

    >

    5 c

    C -

    (E|

    c

    0

    ji

    t

    & r

    x 8 S

    i

    5 > 1

    c

    - c - at:

    *

    A

    y

    p

    o

    m

    s

    a

    s

    c

    y

    c

    o

    n

    f

    o

    a

    a

    r

    e

    a

    v

    b

    w

    g

    N

    m

    b

    o

    m

    e

    n

    o

    p

    w

    m

    a

    n

    m

    b

    o

    c

    o

    f

    e

    a

    a

    r

    e

    g

    o

    a

    a

    o

    a

    o

    a

    e

    w

    h

    o

    w

    r

    a

    e

    o

    h

    e

    o

    W

    i

    d

    w

    h

    h

    g

    r

    a

    e

    o

    h

    e

    o

    N

    m

    b

    o

    p

    e

    v

    n

    n

    h

    d

    a

    o

    a

    e

    v

    i

    n

    e

    h

    e

    o

    A

    y

    p

    o

    m

    e

    e

    u

    n

    d

    a

    h

    e

    N

    e

    v

    w

    m

    e

    w

    h

    p

    e

    s

    o

    a

    s

    u

    e

    d

    m

    g

    n

    y

    h

    g

    S

    B

    p

    =

    0

    e

    s

    a

    s

    c

    c

    o

    s

    f

    o

    a

    a

    c

    c

    u

    m

    o

    c

    g

    e

    e

    h

    s

    r

    e

    a

    v

    w

    g

    a

    e

    o

    go

    A t

    >

    .S f Q .K

    I^

    |

    & .

    s -o

    u

    g a g g o jc

    >,t" c a o v p.

    S t/3

    W 5

    t

    *-

    1

    >i

    i

    -i

    tl * ,r 35 - - i oiS.SS

    w ^ O v c a 3 tn^^

    2l2l|

    8 a

    E * -s

    a s*

    w ^ n ^

    5

    d - ^ c v

    1

    till

    * .iv-L

    w|||-8| S f f i l

    S t c n ^ S ' S o J S o s C f c

    3 o - p ' S - a S g > . g , > J 5

    llillllliill

    (2 a

    g

    0

    S

    P

    2 P -SS

    S s

    c

    S

    5

    A

    y

    p

    o

    m

    e

    s

    a

    s

    c

    y

    c

    o

    n

    f

    o

    s

    g

    a

    S

    a

    a

    e

    m

    e

    R

    y

    p

    m

    a

    y

    n

    w

    k

    r

    o

    e

    a

    b

    o

    n

    w

    k

    r

    o

    e

    a

    o

    a

    e

    w

    h

    h

    g

    S

    B

    I

    o

    a

    e

    n

    w

    k

    r

    o

    e

    a

    o

    a

    e

    w

    h

    o

    w

    S

    B

    0-

    ca

    oT

    m

    e /5

    "si

    a

    '5

    _

    %

    "S

    c

    'la-S 1gji

    "O IS

    ^

    'C

    O

    ^"*5

    itlllil

    2

    g

    saa

    rf

    2 E

    8"8-S

    S'S'S

    - u u > " ' 0 ' S S ; g

    g 'S

    o 'g

    g g

    g ' g < E

    ?

    js

    .e o'C'C

    'C

    o

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    7/44

    49 4 UCHINO, CACIOPPO, AND

    KIECOLT-GLASER

    c

    o

    I

    n

    o

    UJ

    Z

    2

    Q

    ft.

    ffi

    00

    C

    c 8

    v c

    .H 3

    il

    a.

    a

    Q

    eu

    n

    C/ 3

    a.

    S

    s

    73.1 .

    1

    1

    &i

    .9

    is

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    8/44

    SOCIAL SUPPORT

    AND

    PHYSIOLOGY

    495

    C e

    a ^ >

    ; . g

    u

    OJ

    >,

    1

    5 5

    It

    .K

    Si

    Q

    Q.

    03

    Q

    o T

    C O

    c/i

    0-

    e a

    y

    ^ ^

    u

    , c

    >

    o o

    t>

    o

    '

    "

    oo-S. "5

    c A < r .

    S

    g- o a

    S

    C O M

    c

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    9/44

    496

    U C H 1 N O ,CACIOPPO,

    A N D

    KIECOLT-GLASER

    T3

    J

    3

    ,

    gfe^

    "3 fc

    u

    M

    C U

    ^

    ^

    /j

    u

    '5

    .

    o

    w

    E

    rt fN*

    0 II

    _

    .2

    c 3

    1

    0

    u

    ^

    2 ;

    ;

    C

    0

    E

    Hliil

    Hi

    ilistifli

    .1

    s i

    fiiii

    ilpl

    il|i

    HI

    H M S

    mil

    il1 1

    II

    S

    I

    HI

    f i

    ifII

    lllii

    5i|

    i

    lll

    I

    5?

    S ffl D">^Kl

    ^ ^

    c*j

    J

    CH 3

    *~

    ffl

    "O

    t

    V- ^*

    "o p a

    "" tS

    E. 4. Q ^ " c s ' O >->>,c cR.^^

    .Sii

    ^"c3o *j13D,'*-*-^-,

    fc-

    gj-g

    ugi^^S ca $'3

    S " ^ =

    ^-^

    Hs^^ 'o S

    2 ^ x >

    S S ^

    gc^c^lx.a^i^sfet. i issoif iSaSQ

    f e *

    pa

    Q,

    1

    i/5

    ggt

    0

    "S >> T3 -0 g

    _

    S

    c| 8.fe2 3

    g

    | , 2 _ _ < 2

    t nil M i l

    i

    in

    i

    in

    iinni it i

    i

    ini

    n

    1

    |

    i]i&

    i- f t

    1

    H

    1H 5 - S 4 J - S 3 s iS^a |

    1|

    .s|||

    3-05.

    Sg

    s

    8 S

    > > h g

    8|ol

    1

    1

    f l i 11

    l? ,

    s

    Ilii

    lllilli IWi

    tilgiii iiilil il-iilinii

    " ( O

    U f t , C L , < o , c / )

    a, wi 1/3

    &a."

    a

    o

    M a, a,

    so

    a"

    n n

    Irffe

    Q Q

    ft" e

    E

    Q

    &'

    Bu * 3 C

    1 ffl n

    W 3 < c/3

    ^

    50 "g

    x,

    C W O

    JS

    Eg-ri| 3?E S

    1

    o g

    M

    -sa

    c

    S

    o w

    u

    5

    ii

    l|

    ill

    I|il3? Ill

    fill?lit

    1J5JJ I -

    15'

    *iSI* fl*

    ls|IIl;| illf Ill

    8s | 1 8.815?

    lit

    Isslsll|li |o3sl111

    fl.

    V U 1

    5 aj o

    4 J

    e

    -S

    .S

    .a ^

    1I f Ll

    aJfisl

    I

    IS iirfis

    g s s . g ^ ll|=ggS|^gs^l-s s

    g|s IBlilgl'llS

    8f

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    12/44

    SOCIAL SUPPORT A N D PHYSIOLOGY

    499

    Z

    E E f a

    M

    rTJ2

    a

    I2III

    &

    K -O "B T5

    j|l

    c

    rt

    > B..S

    pjll

    c a

    c5

    o.

    S

    fe

    Q-

    oa

    Q

    S - a

    "S - KS

    tS - a

    S

    &

    .s l ' sas

    )

    OU(9

    2

    2

    . S E ^

    5

    I

    c S Cav"tZ

    S

    oI

    1

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    15/44

    50 2

    UCHINO, CACIOPPO, AND KIECOLT-GLASER

    leisure activities and, more important, lower SBP andDBF.The

    control condition evidenced

    no

    such changes.

    Only

    researchers

    of 2 of the

    studies conducted

    to

    date

    re-

    portedno

    effect

    of a socialsupportintervention on blood pres-

    sure

    regulation (Arnetz, Theorell, Levi, Kallner,

    &

    Eneroth,

    1983;

    Gill,

    Veigl,

    Shuster,

    &

    Notelovitz,

    198 4 ) .

    However, Gill

    et

    al.

    did not find a

    significant manipulation check

    on

    social sup-

    port, which suggests that

    the

    intervention

    was

    unsuccessful

    in

    affecting

    participants' supportnetworks.

    The

    null

    finding by

    Arnetz et al. is

    more difficult

    to

    explain. However,

    familial

    rela-

    tionshipsmay berelatively important forblood pressure regu-

    lation (seePo tentialMechanisms).

    In

    general, stronger associa-

    tions might

    be

    obtained with interventions that focus

    on

    famil-

    ial

    sources of support.

    Although

    the

    studies summarized

    in

    Tables

    1 and 2

    suggest that

    social support influences cardiovascular function,

    few of

    these

    studies suggested

    its

    effect

    on

    established risk factors.

    To

    this point,

    wesummarize

    8

    prospective

    (primarily intervention)

    studies with

    hypertensivepatients inTable3.Researchersof 5 of the 8studies

    in

    Table

    3

    explicitly noted that they examined both White

    and

    Black

    participants. Researchers

    of 6

    studies examined both

    men

    and women, whereasof 2studies examined only men.S ix of the

    interventions usedfor themost part familialsourcesofsupport.

    However,

    researchers

    of 1

    study simply assessed naturalistic social

    support,

    and of 1

    study used organizational social support.

    Evidence for the role of social support on cardiovascular

    function

    and

    risk factors comes from

    the

    prospective interven-

    tionstudiesonhypertensive individuals summarizedinTable

    3. In an early study, Levine et al.

    (1979)

    identified 400 hyper-

    tensive

    patients and assigned them to interventions consisting

    ofan

    exit interview,

    family

    support, small group, various com-

    binations of these groups (e.g., exit interview and family

    support), or a control condition. In the

    family

    support condi-

    tion, patients were asked

    to

    identify

    a

    target individual with

    whom

    they

    had

    frequent contact (typically

    a spouse). The

    target individuals were then trained to increase understanding,

    support, and reinforcement regarding positive management of

    thepatient's

    hypertensive

    state.

    Results revealed that family

    support alone decreased DBF (i.e., DBF was below the hyper-

    tensive

    limitsfor theparticipant'sparticularag e

    group)

    by 11%

    at an 18-monthfollow-up assessment. Predictably, exposure to

    all

    intervention conditions was associated

    with

    the best blood

    pressure control ( 2 8 % ) .Subsequent follow-upsofthis project

    sample revealed reliable long-term effectsof the social support

    manipulation on blood pressure regulation(M orisky,DeMuth,

    Field-Pass,Green,

    &

    Levine, 1985; Morisky

    et

    al., 1983).

    A

    meta-analysis

    of

    studies whose researchers have used social sup-

    portmanipulations

    to

    control blood pressure

    in

    at-risk popula-

    tions (Earp, Ory,

    &

    Strogatz, 1982;

    Erfurt ,

    Foote,

    &

    Heirich,

    1991;

    Levine

    etal.,

    1979; Morisky

    et

    al., 1983, 1985; Stahl,

    Kelley,

    Neill,

    Grim &Mamlin, 198 4 ) revealed a significant

    combined test(z =3.32,p =.0004, fail-safe n =

    12 .29) .

    3

    The

    mean effect size

    w asr = .15, and no

    test

    of

    variability

    w as

    sig-

    nificant

    (p >.20).These prospective data fromat-risk popula-

    tions provide evidence that social support

    may

    have beneficial

    effects

    on

    established risk factors.

    Although

    the results of the prospective intervention studies

    withnormotensives and hypertensives suggestthatsocial sup-

    port leads to better blood pressure regulation, there are several

    issues raised by these studies. The prospective intervention

    studies with hypertensives were primarily designedtoaffecttan-

    gible aspects of support. However, the social support manipula-

    tions may have

    affected

    other

    aspects

    of social support, includ-

    ing

    appraisal support

    due to the

    increased

    participationand

    knowledge

    of the

    support

    provider. Furthermore, although

    these studies suggest tangible support may have been important

    because

    of

    better medical adherence (e.g., Levine

    et

    al.,1979),

    none of the studies researchers performed statistical analyses to

    directly examine

    the

    importance

    of

    thisfactor.Interestingly,

    the

    prospective intervention studies generally preceded

    the

    correla-

    tional studies summarizedinTable 1that suggest tangible fac-

    tors alone cannot explain

    the

    associations between social sup-

    port

    and

    blood pressure regulation. Therefore,

    the

    prospective

    data are only suggestive of tangible support influences on blood

    pressure regulation because other unmeasured components of

    social support

    may

    have contributed

    to

    these

    effects.

    W eshould also note that aspects

    of

    several interventionswith

    normotensive participants might have affected other health-

    related processes (e.g., Andersson, 1985; Sallis, Trevorrow,

    Johnson, Hovell, & Kaplan,

    198 7 ) .

    For instance, the Sallis et al.

    manipulation also informed participants

    of the

    h a rm f u l effects

    of

    stress

    (alsosee

    Clifford, Tan,

    &Gorsuch, 1 9 9 1 ) .

    Therefore,

    lifestyle

    or

    behavioral changes related

    to

    stress,

    but not

    directly

    involving

    socialsupport,may have also contributed to the re-

    sults

    of these studies. Nevertheless, the prospective design of the

    studies

    in

    Tables

    2 and 3,

    along with

    the

    importance

    of

    blood

    pressure regulation in hypertensive individuals, provides rela-

    tivelystrongevidence linking

    social support

    torisk factors.

    PotentialMechanisms Linking SocialSupport to

    Cardiovascular Function

    Because of the consistency of the associations between social

    support

    and

    cardiovascular parameters presented

    in

    Tables

    1 to

    3,

    we now

    turn

    to specifyingthe

    potential mechanisms respon-

    sibleforthese covariations.In areviewofpotential mechanisms

    linking

    social support to health, S. Cohen

    (1988)

    suggests that

    social support m ayhave beneficialeffectsthrough social (e.g.,

    stress

    buffering),

    psychological (e.g.,

    affectivestates),and be-

    havioral(e.g., health-promoting) mechanisms. Consistent with

    S .Cohen( 1 9 8 8 ) ,

    we

    examined

    the

    mechanisms linking social

    support

    to

    physiological processes

    at

    differentlevels

    of

    analysis

    (also

    see

    Cacioppo

    &

    Berntson,

    1992).

    Atasocial psychologicallevelofanalysis,itappears thatfa-

    milial sources of social support may be associated with reliable

    effects

    on

    blood pressure regulation.

    A

    meta-analysis

    of

    12

    cor-

    relational studies whose researchers explicitly noted that they

    3

    The meta-analysis consisted of 4 studies that used social support

    manipulations

    tocontrol blood pressureinat-risk populations.Onetest

    statistic is entered forthe 3 Levine et al. and Morisky et al. studies above

    to

    reduce potential problems with

    the

    nonindependent samples.

    The

    Pinto, Sirota,

    and

    Brown ( 1 9 8 5 )study

    was not

    included because

    no

    statistics were presented for their case study. For all prospective studies,

    the

    statistic entered

    was the

    difference

    in

    blood pressure level

    or

    control

    betweenthe social support manipulation and a control condition during

    the finalassessment, thereby providing evidenceon thelong-term effects

    ofthese interventions.

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    16/44

    SOCIAL SUPPORT AN D PHYSIOLOGY

    503

    CO

    Z

    I

    a.

    a a

    Q

    a.

    C O

    Q

    I

    fc,

    J3

    5

    "

    .2?

    c

    jj

    .a

    S

    jjr-

    S C

    a %

    S

    t.

    o

    \ B

    r C

    , MO

    imn

    t

    "> m

    C - s

    "

    ~ - o & a 9

    zz?

    a -s e -g

    ||rio"IS i

    lifll

    P aoi

    1

    to

    CD

    IS

    ;

    c

    '

    s

    :

    &

    [3

    S

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    17/44

    504

    UCH1NO, CACIOPPO,

    AN D

    K1ECOLT-GLASER

    /rm

    ^

    *

    S

    8

    T

    3

    c a

    H

    R

    u

    s

    -

    o

    Z

    s

    '5

    S g

    c '2 J; ' g c S u i J S ' a e g C

    Cr r1

    2,.lfi- -S3C

    C

    ^

    SS

    C 2

    n

    S K ^ g 3 w 5 " O 3

    >

    ^

    5 > >;

    O.-B m "S

    SJ

    S -oJ <

    O OO *~*

    5

    ""

    > ' '

    tt

    = * >

    * o

    - g

    E ,

    s i " 2

    ' Sa

    "S

    r n

    ^ - . ' S 3 - S

    - o - a ^ e S _ . .

    c S

    ' ' S ' " '

    t

    7 j ' T ? - 5 -

    'S inUj gMg 'So ' S 'cSC9 ^9CMC' K

    *2S

    '

    * *O ^*P ^ 33T33*i5

    ^^O ^

    S

    u

    c ^ T3 "O

    ^

    TD*Q*

    PfllH

    Pi li'stilltill

    ( < e o

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    18/44

    SOCIAL SUPPORT

    AND PHYSIOLOGY

    505

    assessed social support related

    to family

    members revealed

    a

    reliable

    combined test

    ofsignificance (z =

    4.17,p =.00001,

    fail-safe n=64.97).T he

    associatedeffect size

    for

    this analysis

    wa sr .12,

    and no

    test

    of

    variability

    w as

    significant(p >.09).

    In

    1

    study w hose researchers directly exa m ined blood pressu re

    in

    the

    presence

    of

    different

    social relationships,

    Spitzer, Llabre,

    Ironson, G ellman,

    an d

    Schneiderman( 1 9 9 2 )found that being

    around afamily member wasassociatedwith lower am bulatory

    SBP and DBF compared with being around a friend or a

    stranger. The prospective interventions with hypertensive pa-

    tients that directly used

    family

    members

    as

    sources

    of

    support

    provide convergent evidence on the importance of familial

    sources of support on blood pressure regulation.

    Researchers

    of 8 of the

    correlational studies have directly

    tested the potential

    stress-buffering

    effects of social support on

    cardiovascularfunction (Dressier, 1980,

    1991;

    Dressier, M atar,

    etal., 1986; Janes, 1990; Kasl & Cobb, 1980;

    Kaufmann

    &

    Beehr, 1986; Strogatz

    &

    James, 1986;W innubst

    et

    al.,

    1982).

    As

    argued by S. Cohen and W ills {1 98 5), one methodological

    requirementfor atestof the

    buffering

    model consistsofdemon-

    strating

    a

    significant main

    effect for thestress

    assessm ent

    to en-

    sure that the measure was characterized by an adequate range

    of

    scores

    an d

    m easurem ent reliability. However, only research-

    ers of 4 of these studies reported data indicating that their m ea-

    sure ofstresswasassociated withblood p ressure. These4stud-

    ieswere

    associated

    withasignificant

    combinedtest (

    z=3.39,p

    =.0003,

    fail

    safen

    =

    12.99)and aneffect sizeofr = . 18. Notest

    of

    variabilityw as significant (p > .34). In 1illustrative study,

    Dressier

    (1980 ) reported

    an interaction between structural

    measures

    of

    suppo rt (i.e.,

    no. of

    siblings)

    and

    levels

    of

    lifestress

    for

    SBP and

    DBF: Individuals high

    in

    number

    of

    siblings

    and

    low in life

    stress

    werecharacterized by the lowestblood pres-

    sure. Although Cohen

    and

    Wills suggest that

    buffering

    effects

    are

    more likely

    to be

    found when there

    is a

    reasonable match

    between the

    stressor

    typeandsupport function, theyalsore-

    ported that

    buffering effects

    were sometimes

    found

    when

    re-

    searchers assessed close interpersonal relationships. Consistent

    with Cohen and W ills, all5studies whoseresearchersexamined

    familial

    relationships (e.g., spouse and siblings) reported asig-

    nificant

    buffering effect

    oncardiovascular regulation (z = 3.43,

    p

    =

    .0003,

    fail-safe n =16.74), with

    an

    effect size

    ofr =

    .14.

    N o

    testofvariability wassignificant

    (p >.55)*

    These studies fur -

    therunderscorethepotential importance ofexaminingfamilial

    sources

    of

    social suppo rt

    in

    studies

    of

    cardiovascular regulation.

    Th e studies summarized in Table 2 suggest that structured

    interactions with others m ayalso produce

    beneficial

    effects on

    cardiovascular func tion. However, these resultsm ay notsimply

    be a functionof theintervention discussion because such struc-

    tured interactions appear to generalizeto othersinone's net-

    work

    (e.g.,A ndersson,1985). Therefore,

    the

    studies

    in

    Table

    2

    m ay producepart of their effects by increasing social compe-

    tence

    or theperceived im portanceofsocial interactionsin one's

    social network (Sallis etal., 1987).

    At

    am ore behavioral

    level

    o fanalysis, partof the association

    between

    social support

    and

    cardiovascular function

    may be a

    result of health-related lifestyle factors

    (Umberson,

    1987). For

    example,social support may be associated with better cardio-

    vascular regulation because ind ividu als high in social support

    engage

    in better health practices (e.g., better diet and more

    physical activity). C ontrarytothisposition,theassociationsbe-

    tweenaspectso fsocial supportand cardiovascular

    function

    re-

    mained significant even afterstatistically controlling for

    a

    num-

    ber ofhealth-related variables, includ ing weightor body mass

    (e.g., Blandet

    al.,

    1991 ; Janes &Pawson, 1 986; Stavig,Igra, &

    Leonard,

    1984).

    How ever,

    it

    should

    be

    noted

    that

    many

    of

    these

    researchers havenot

    assessed

    specific health-related behaviors

    (e.g., substance abuse). In addition, of those researchers that

    did assess specific health-related behaviors, data on the reliabil-

    ity or valid ity of their assessments we re typically notreported

    (see

    Umberson, 1987).

    Atapsychological levelofanalysis, perceptions ofstress, feel-

    ings

    ofcontrollability, intrusiveorrum inative thinking, feelings

    of loneliness, depression, and other emotional processes (e.g.,

    anxiety)are potential psychological mechanisms for theassoci-

    ations between social support

    and

    cardiovascular function

    (Collins, Dunkel-Schetter, Lobel, & Scrimshaw, 1993;Pierce,

    Sarason,

    &

    Sarason, 1991; Quittner, Glueckauf,

    &

    Jackson,

    1990;

    Russell &Cu trona, 1991;Stokes, 1985; Solomon, Miku-

    lincer,

    &Hobfoll, 19 86).

    U nfortunately,

    empirical data are un-

    available

    concerningthepsychological mechanisms responsible

    for the

    associations

    between

    social

    support and cardiovascular

    function reported in Tables 1-3. Future research is clearly

    needed in this area ofinquiry.W e return to this imp ortant point

    later in the review.

    LaboratoryStudiesExamining th eEffects ofSocial

    Support

    on

    Cardiovascular Function

    Whereas

    the

    prior studies have focused primarily

    on

    tonic

    measures of cardiac function, many of the recent studies have

    been experimental, laboratory

    studies

    conducted underthe ru-

    bricof thereactivity hypothesis. Briefly, thereactivity hypothe-

    sis

    suggests that exaggerated cardiovascular reactivityto stres-

    sorsm ay be a pathogenic mechanism influencingthe develop-

    ment

    of cardiovascular disorders (see Krantz & Manuck,

    1984;

    Manuck,

    1994;

    and

    Matthews

    et al.,

    1986). These

    15

    studies

    aresummarized inTable4.Thirteenofthese studies tested rel-

    ativelyyoungparticipants und er

    the age of 30. O ne

    study used

    a middle-aged sample, and 1 study used an older adultsample.

    Researchers of 8 of these

    studies

    exclusively exam ined wom en,

    of3 examined m en, and of 4 examined both m en and women.

    In 1 1 studies, researchers examined social support throughex-

    perimental manipulation. Researchers

    of the

    remaining

    4

    stud-

    iesassessed

    n atura listic levels of socialsupport.Of these 4 stud-

    ies, 3

    studies' researchers exam ined fun ctional measures

    of

    sup-

    port, and 1

    examined

    a

    combined index

    of

    structural

    an d

    functional

    support.

    The

    laboratory studies

    in

    Table

    4

    collectively suggestthat

    so-

    cial supp ort m ay reduce cardiovascular

    ( or

    autonomic nervous

    system)reactivity

    to

    acute psychologicalstress.

    O ne

    salient fea-

    (textcontinuesonpage 510)

    4

    W e should note th at 4 of the 5 studies' researchers wh o assessed

    familialsources

    ofsupport

    also

    reported

    significant

    effectsoftheir stress

    measures. Therefore, the influenceof

    close

    interpersonal relationships

    and the methodological requirement suggested by S. Cohen and Wills

    (1985) are

    potentially

    confounded

    in

    these meta-analyses

    of

    buffering

    effects.

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    19/44

    506

    UCHINO, CACIOPPO,

    AND

    KIECOLT-GLASER

    c

    y

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    20/44

    SOC IAL SUPPORT A N D PHYSIOLOGY

    507

    1

    m i.

    a E S

    la

    S - C

    S

    '$

    g

    O P

    a.9

    U BOuU

    I I Iif

    0

    tfS

    B

    1

    1

    ' 3

    s

    o

    d

    d

    n

    r

    o

    o

    o

    m

    e

    a

    k

    n

    d

    n

    d

    a.

    t

    o

    e

    c

    &

    I s

    4>

    1

    a j

    s

    u

    c

    o

    m

    o

    r

    e

    p

    o

    d

    s

    o

    w

    I

    Q

    v

    n

    s

    u

    S

    i

    o

    o

    B

    T3

    3

    a

    e

    y

    a

    a

    w

    o

    r

    e

    u

    n

    c

    o

    a

    D

    s-

    BQ

    W 5

    t

    X

    i

    w

    n

    e

    u

    c

    o

    a

    o

    a

    e

    w

    h

    o

    o n

    s

    u

    -C

    If?

    C to

    i

    S . B

    Is

    & 2

    a

    e

    w

    h

    n

    s

    u

    t

    a

    k

    n

    d

    s

    w

    h

    e

    p

    e

    h

    s

    a

    p

    e

    &

    i

    1

    (2

    II

    |Sfffl la

    IHS

    w a s #

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    21/44

    508

    U C H I N O , CAC1OPPO, AN D KIECOLT-GLASER

    a

    s

    c

    a

    a ^

    '3.e

    'g -

    S

    x

    s

    1)

    S

    c-

    ffl ft,

    V5 gg

    a.

    03

    Q

    a."

    n

    on

    IIIa

    fi

    .S 3 .5

    a.

    s o

    s s n i i t i

    "ill

    si

    II

    CL.

    e a

    CO

    .

    .^

    O R W O CS

    c o

    _ '3

    g;

    12o 3c,

    D

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    22/44

    SOCIAL SUPPORT

    AND

    PHYSIOLOGY

    509

    u

    1

    4=

    S

    C Q

    - Q Q

    g

    i>

    C

    o

    ,6.o g

    "S v i

    O

    r:

    4J 43 1>

    < = :

    I*I

    &O

    I

    i

    o o

    if

    K o3

    U T3 C :s

    r-

    nS c

    JD

    S

    O3

    i

    8

    fS s,c

    le

    li

    'aD

    +

    rfS

    |l

    Cu o o

    a

    .49)

    .*

    6

    A

    literature search

    onPsycLIT(

    1974- 1994)

    a ndMedline(1983-

    1994)

    crossing

    the

    keywords

    immuneo r

    immunology withpsychomet-

    ricsorreliabilityrevealed no additional researchers who had exam ined

    the

    temporal stability

    of the

    im m un e assessments discussed

    in

    this

    review.

    7

    O ur

    initial search produced

    2 4

    studies whose researchers exam ined

    the

    relationship between social support

    and

    i m m u n e

    function. W e ex-

    cluded analyses of 6 studies in part or in w hole for several reasons. Data

    on

    the relationship between social support and

    white

    blood cell or total

    lymphocyte

    counts

    were

    not included in the present

    review

    (Arnetz et

    al.,

    1983;

    R. S.

    Baron, Cutrona,

    Hicklin,

    Russell,

    &

    Lubaroff,

    1990;

    Mclntosh, Kaplan, Kubena, & Landmann, 1993; Thomas, Goodwin,

    & Goodwin, 1985) because

    of

    difficulties

    in

    interpreting

    the

    signifi-

    cance

    of

    these m easures.

    In

    addition,

    w e

    excluded

    1

    study that exam -

    ined salivary

    IgA

    (Jemm ott

    &

    Magloire, 1 98 8) because

    of

    m ethodolog-

    ical issues regarding the reliability of the salivary IgA assessment that

    was

    used

    in

    this study (Herbert

    &

    Cohen, 1993a; Stone, Cox,

    Valdimarsdottir, &Neale, 1987) .O nestudywasalso excludedfrom the

    review

    because

    the

    small number

    of

    participants

    (3 ) in

    their

    lo w

    stress

    and no social support

    cell

    precluded

    definitive

    an alyses (Herrera, Alva-

    rado, & M artinez, 1988 ).

    8

    The meta-analysis consisted of 9 studies whose researchers directly

    examined the association between social support and functional mea-

    sures

    of

    i m m u n e

    function. To

    reduce problems

    associated

    w ith nonin-

    dependence, the results of

    Snyder,

    Roghmann, and

    Sigal

    (1990, 1993)

    were

    averaged

    to

    produce

    one

    test statistic because data were reported

    from

    the

    same sample.

    S ix

    studies were excludedfrom

    the

    m eta-analysis

    because it was determined a priori that intervention studies and popu-

    lations

    w ith individuals

    having HI V

    w ere associated w ith inconsistent

    effects.These studies are discussed in detail later in the text.

    In

    the meta-analysis, we examinedfunctional imm une measures be-

    cause there were

    only4

    rem ainin g studies whose researchers examined

    the association between social support and quan titative measures of im-

    m u n e function.

    The interested reader is referred to Table 5 for a sum -

    mary of the

    relationship between social support

    an d

    quantitative

    im -

    m u n e

    measures.

    In

    addition, there

    were

    only

    2

    remaining studies that

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    33/44

    52 0

    UCHINO, CA CIOPPO,

    A N D

    KIECOLT-GLASER

    Tw o of the studies that did not find an association between

    social support and immunological data were interventions de-

    signed to facilitate social interactions (Arnetzetal., 198 7; Kie-

    colt-Glaseretal.,1985). The Kiecolt-Glaser et al.(19 85)inter-

    vention documented increased social contact and interactions.

    However, keep

    in

    m ind that

    the

    sam ple sizes

    in 2 of

    these stud ies

    are relatively small

    (n

    < 45.00). In addition, we reviewed evi-

    dence earlier suggestingthat familial sources of support may

    be important. It is possible that interventions aimed directly at

    increasing

    familial

    contact and support may yield even larger

    effects sizesonimm une function.

    Tw o

    of the studies

    that

    did not find a

    significant

    association

    between social support and aspects of imm une fu nction exam-

    ined men who

    wereHIV+ (Goodkin

    et

    al., 199 2; Perry, Fish-

    m an, Jacobsberg, & Frances, 1992) .There are important m eth-

    odological reasons that might explain a lackof an association

    between social support and immune function in individuals

    H IV + . For instance, stage of disease, age, gender, dru g abuse,

    and health behaviors are potentially important confounding

    variables (Ironson et al., 1994).

    However, 2 recent studies have reported an association be-

    tween social support and CD4+ counts (a marker of HIV

    progression) in men with HIV (Persson, Gullberg, Hanson,

    Moestrup,&O stergren, 19 94; Theorelletal., 19 95). In 1pro-

    spective study with

    data

    across a 5-year period, Theorell et al.

    found

    that

    th eavailability ofsociala nd emotional support pre-

    dicted subsequent changes inCD4+counts in a representative

    Swedish sample

    of men

    with HIV. Results revealed that high

    and low social support groups did not differ in CD4+ counts

    duringthe early years of the study. How ever, the prediction of

    CD4+ counts as a

    function

    of social support w as evident du ring

    Years

    4 and 5 of the study. For instance, during Year 5 of the

    study,individu als highinsocial su pport showeda 3 7 % change

    in CD4+ counts, whereas individuals

    low in

    social support

    showeda -64% changeinCD4+counts (Theorellet

    al.,

    1995) .

    Note that Perry et al.

    ( 1 9 9 2 )

    reported nullfindings on the rela-

    tionship between social support and CD4+ counts only up to

    Year 1 of their study. These preliminary prospective data sug-

    gest that social support may

    influence

    the progression of HIV

    infection

    and

    provide evidence

    on the

    utility

    of

    su ch long-term

    prospective designs.

    An

    important implication

    of

    these data

    is

    that if a researcher was to only examine the relationship be-

    tween

    social support and C D4+ counts later in the stage of dis-

    ease, information on the longer length of time that individuals

    highin social support took to get tothatstage wou ld be lost.

    9

    A

    population of particular interest in this review is older

    adults because social support may be especially important for

    these individuals (House etal., 1988) . Alterations in im m une

    function m ayhave significant consequences in this population

    as

    aging

    is

    associated w ith

    a

    dow n regulation

    o f

    i m m u n efunc-

    tion

    (Goidl, 19 87; Goodw in, Searles,&Tung, 19 82; Roberts-

    Thomson, W hittingham, Youngchaiyud, &

    Mackay,

    1974;

    Schleifer,Keller, Bond, Cohen, &Stein, 1 9 8 9 ) , andinfectious

    illnesses

    are the

    fourth leading cause

    of

    death

    in the

    elderly

    included

    a

    structural assessment

    of

    support. Therefore, focused com-

    parisons

    between structural

    an d

    functional measures

    of

    support were

    not performed.

    (Effros &

    Walford,

    1987) .

    Excluding

    th e

    intervention studies

    discussed earlier,it isimportant tonotethatthe association be-

    tween social support and functional measures of immunity is

    consistent

    in

    older adults.

    A

    meta-analysis

    of 7studiesin

    mid-

    dle-agedtoolder adult populationsconfirmed this hypothesis (z

    =

    4.27,

    p =.000009, fail-safe n=

    40.30), with

    a neffect

    size

    of

    r= .23.Notestof

    variabilityw as

    significant (p> .35) .

    Although

    9 studies' researchers examined both men and

    women, 8 studies' researchersdid not report analyses aimedat

    examining

    potential genderdifferences. In the only study with

    data on potential gender differences, Thomas, Goodwin, and

    Goodwin

    ( 1 9 8 5 )foundthat

    the availability of a confidan t was

    associated with

    a

    stronger proliferative response

    to PHA for

    women but not men.

    How ever,

    th e

    correlations were

    in the

    same

    direction, and no statistical test was performed to directlytest

    the difference between men and wom en. In addition, Thomas et

    al. provided a conservative test of the effects of social support

    on im m un efunction,

    as

    they statistically controlled

    for

    psycho-

    logical distress as

    well

    as potential health-related variables (e.g.,

    alcohol consumption).

    Sim ilar conceptual issues exist in the research exa m ining so-

    cial

    support

    and

    im m une function

    as in the

    research reviewed

    earlier.O n ly2 of thestudies summarized inTable5conceptu-

    alized social support as a m ultidimensional construct and re-

    ported analyses regarding a relatively specific dimension of so-

    cial

    support

    (R. S.

    Baron

    et al.,

    1990; Persson

    etal.,

    1994).

    Although

    3

    additional studies used multidimensional social

    support m easures, results wereonly reported on the total scale

    (e.g., Glaser, Kiecolt-Glaser, Bonneau, Malarkey, & Hughes,

    1992;

    G oodkin

    etal., 1992; Perry

    etal.,

    19 92). An examination

    of relatively distinct dimensions of social support may have

    revealed

    greater specificity (Glaser et al., 19 92) and stronger

    associations between social support

    an d

    i m m u n e function

    (Goodkin

    et

    al., 199 2; Perry

    et

    al., 1 9 9 2 )

    due to a

    better m atch

    between

    thesample needsand thesupport resource.

    PotentialMechan isms Linking S ocialSupport to

    Immune Function

    The studies summarized in Table 5 suggest that social sup-

    port

    is associated with better immune function. These results

    and

    a

    recent meta-analysis conducted

    by

    Herbert

    and

    Cohen

    ( 1 9 9 3 a ) provide converging evidence for the effects of social

    support on physiological

    function.

    However, Herbert and Co-

    hen

    only exam ined aspects

    of

    im m une function

    and

    focused

    on

    social stressorsinvolving

    the

    loss

    or

    disruption

    of

    interpersonal

    resources (e.g., bereavement and marital conflict).

    As in the review of social su pport and cardiovascular fun ction

    (see Tables 1-3 ), the studies sum m arized in Table 5 suggest

    that close relationships, such as fam ilial ties, m ay be a particu-

    larlyim portant source of social support. Researchers of 2 stud-

    ies

    assessed social support specific to close relationships

    (Levy

    9

    Weshouldnote that atypical strategy in such designs mightbe to

    statistically

    control for the length of

    time

    since illness. However, if the

    interaction between thiscovariatean dsocialsupportwere significant,it

    would invalidatethe use ofthis

    statistical

    control procedure(J. Cohen

    &

    Cohen, 19 83)

    bu t

    accurately reflect

    the findings of

    Theorell

    et al.

    ( 1 9 9 5 ) .

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    34/44

    SOC IAL SUPPORT AND PHYSIOLOGY

    52 1

    etal., 1990; Thomas et

    al.,

    1985), and both found social sup-

    port

    to be

    related

    to

    aspects

    of

    immune function, including

    a

    stronger proliferativeresponse to PHA and greater NK cell lysis.

    Researchersof 4 of thestudiesinTable5directlytested the

    bufferingmodel of social support (Goodkin etal., 1992;

    Kie-

    colt-Glaser,

    Dura,

    Speicher,

    Trask,

    &

    Glaser,

    1 9 9 1 ;

    Snyder,

    Roghmann,

    &

    Sigal, 1990, 1993).

    Of

    these studies, onlyKie-

    colt-Glaseret al. ( 1 9 9 1 )reported a reliable effect of stress on

    immune

    function.Asnoted earlier,S.CohenandW ills( 1 9 8 5 )

    argued that this is a methodological requirement for an ade-

    quate test

    of

    the

    buffering

    model. More important,

    Kiecolt-Gla-

    ser et al. found

    evidence

    for a

    buffering

    effect of

    social support

    on

    immunefunction.However, becauseof thesmall numberof

    studies, moredataare needed to adequately test the buffering

    model on immune function.

    Tw oof thestudies summarizedinTable5conceptualizedso-

    cial

    support

    as a

    multidimensional construct

    and

    reported anal-

    yses regarding relatively

    specific

    dimension

    of

    social support

    (R. S. Baron etal.,1990; Levy et al., 1990). Levy et al. exam-

    inedthedimensionofemotional

    support

    from

    aspouse (orin-

    timate other) and emotional support

    from

    one's doctor and

    foundboth to beassociatedwith greater NK celllysisin cancer

    patients. In a study of spouses of cancer patients, Baron et al.

    used the social provisions scale and foundthat higher levels on

    all

    support

    dimensions(i.e.,guidance, reliable alliances, reas-

    surances of worth, social integration, attachment, and opportu-

    nity fornurturance) were equallya nd significantly associated

    witha stronger proliferative response to PHA and greater NK

    cell lysis. A snoted byBaron et al., caring for a spouse with

    cancermay result in a mobilization ofone'ssupport network,

    suchthatthere was little

    differentiation

    among support compo-

    nents. Consistent with this possibility, Baron et al. reported high

    intercorrelations among the components of support.

    Levy

    et al. (1990)

    suggest that emotional support

    may be one

    dimension

    of

    social support that

    is

    associated with immune

    function.Researchers of 4 additional studies also assessed, in

    part,emotionalsupport(also see R. S. Baron et al., 1 9 9 0 ).Kie-

    colt-Glaseret al.

    ( 1 9 9 1 )

    and Esterling, Kiecolt-Glaser, Bodnar,

    andGlaser ( 1 9 9 4 )used acompositeindex of emotional and

    tangible support. Snyder

    et al.

    (1990, 1 9 9 3 )used

    a

    composite

    index of

    emotional

    and

    informational support.

    A

    meta-analysis

    ofthese studies revealedasignificant combined testof signifi-

    cance

    (z =

    4.02,p

    =

    .00003,fail-safen

    =

    24.90).

    The

    effectsize

    associatedwith this test wasr =.26, and no test of variability

    w as significant (p > .44).Thesedata suggestthatemotional

    supportmay be atleastone important aspect of social support

    in

    predicting immune function. Additional research

    is

    needed,

    however,

    that directly compares the predictive utility of specific

    dimensions of socialsupport.

    At

    a

    behavioral level

    of

    analysis, part

    of the

    association

    be-

    tweensocialsupport

    and

    immunefunction

    may be due to

    their

    effects onpotential health-related variables

    (Kiecolt-Glaser

    &

    Glaser, 1988b). Several researchers assessed the effectsof po-

    tential health-related behaviors andfoundthat the associations

    between social support and immune function were significant

    evenwhen statistically controllingforhealth practices (Thomas

    etal.,1985; also see Theorell, Orth-Gomer, & Eneroth, 1990).

    Thesedataareconsistentwith results reviewed earlier on social

    supportand blood pressure, suggesting that such behaviors do

    not appear to be necessary for an association between social

    support

    and

    immune

    function.

    However, these

    findings

    should

    be

    taken

    as

    preliminary, given

    the

    restricted number

    of

    health-

    related

    practicesassessed

    and the

    lack

    of reported

    data

    on the

    validityand reliabilityofsuch assessments in manyofthese

    studies.

    Psychological

    factors such

    as

    levels

    of

    stress

    and

    depression

    havereliableeffects

    on

    immune function (Herbert

    &

    Cohen,

    1993a,

    1993b). Therefore, part of the association between so-

    cial

    support

    and

    immunefunction

    may be

    mediated

    by

    these

    factors.Researchers of 3 studies in Table 5 reporteddatarelat-

    ing

    to

    potential psychological mechanisms responsible

    for the

    associations between social support

    and

    immune function

    (R. S.

    Baron

    et

    al., 1990; Glaser

    etal.,

    1992; Kiecolt-Glaser

    et

    al., 1 9 9 1 ) .Baron et al.(1990) foundthat the associations be-

    tween

    social support and immune functionwere not mediated

    by lifeevents.Inaddition, Baronet al.(1990)an dKiecolt-Gla-

    ser et al. (1 9 9 1 )

    foundthat depression

    was not

    mediating

    the

    associations between social support and immunity. Finally, Gla-

    ser

    et al.

    ( 1 9 9 2 )

    reported

    evidence indicating that anxiety levels

    were

    not

    responsible

    for the

    associations between social support

    and

    immunefunction.Therefore, although health-related

    be-

    haviors, depression, and life stress have reliable effects on as-

    pects of immunefunction, these factors do not appearto be

    majorpathways

    explainingthe

    associations between social sup-

    port and immunefunction.

    Discussion

    Social support has been linked to lower rates of morbidity

    and

    mortality

    from

    diverse disease processes and endpoints.

    Therefore,

    the

    majoraims

    of

    this review were

    to

    examine

    the

    evidencelinkingsocial support

    to

    multiple aspects

    of

    physio-

    logical

    function

    and tocharacterizethepotential mechanisms

    responsibleforthese covariations.To thebestof ourknowledge,

    this

    is the first

    comprehensivereview

    on

    this topic.

    The

    present

    review

    indicates that there

    is

    relatively strong evidence linking

    social support to aspects of the cardiovascular, endocrine, and

    immunesystems. These

    dataare

    consistent with research sug-

    gestingthattheformationanddisruptionofsocial relationships

    haveimportant immunological

    and

    endocrinological sequalae

    in n o n h umanprimatesand humans(Coe, 1993;Gunnar,1992;

    Herbert & Cohen, 1993a). More important, the physiological

    systems

    reviewedmay

    playimportant roles

    in the

    leading causes

    of

    death

    in the

    United States, including cardiovascular disor-

    ders, cancer, and respiratory illnesses.

    10

    Conceptual and meth-

    10

    C. E.

    Smith, Fernengel, Holcroft, Gerald,

    an d

    Marien

    (1994)

    con-

    ducted

    a

    m eta-analysis

    on the

    effects

    of

    social support

    on

    various health

    measures,

    including

    physicalandstress-related outcomes. They opera-

    tionalized

    stress outcomesasreportsof negative lifeevents,conflict or

    distress, andlaboratory measures,suchascatecholamine levels. Physi-

    calhealth status

    w as

    operationalized

    assubjective

    states, such

    as

    symp-

    t om s

    a nd

    signs,

    and as

    objectivedata, such

    as

    weightloss, activities

    of

    daily

    living,

    blood pressure, blood glucose,andreportsof sexualactivity

    posthysterectomy.

    Th eresultsof the

    meta-analyses

    revealed

    effect size

    estimatesranging

    from

    .01 to.22.S m ithet al.concluded thatth erela-

    tively smalleffect sizessuggest thattherelationship between social sup-

    port an d healthm ay not be

    significant

    orgeneralizable.

    Thereare

    several issues that warrant discussion regarding

    the C. E.

    S m ithet al.( 1 9 9 4 )meta-analysis.First, Smithet al. did notpresentany

  • 7/25/2019 apoyo social y enfermedad review.pdf

    35/44

    522

    UCHINO,

    CACIOPPO,

    AND

    KIECOLT-GLASER

    odologicalissues were also raised regarding the associations be-

    tweensocial support and physiological processes. We now turn

    tothese issues.

    One basic andrecurring issuein thesocial support

    literature

    relatesto the measurement of socialsupport(Barrera, 1986; S.

    Cohen

    &

    Wills, 1985; Heitzmann

    &

    Kaplan, 1988;

    Orth-

    Gomer&Unden, 1987;

    Tardy, 1985; Winemiller

    etal.,

    1993).

    Questions remainaboutthe factorstructure of social support

    and the temporal stability and psychometric properties of such

    assessments.

    In the

    present review,

    few

    studies' researchers

    re-

    porteddata pertainingto thepsychometric propertiesoftheir

    socialsupportmeasures. Given the heterogeneity in the mea-

    sures of socialsupportcovered in this

    review,

    psychometricdata

    m ayhelpclarifyreliable relationships.

    An additional measurement issue concerns the specific

    sources of socialsupport.The present reviewindicates that fa-

    milialtiesappearto be an important source of social support

    to consider in studies of physiological function.Socialsupport

    researchers might gain greaterspecificity

    and

    prediction

    by ex-

    amining specific types of

    social

    relationships. The studies sum-

    marized in this

    review

    whose researchers examined cross-cul-

    tural

    andgender

    effects

    ofsocial supportareexamplesofsuch

    applications. In addition, behavioral data obtained during lab-

    oratory studiesmay amplify the relationships found between

    self-report data and physiological processes (e.g., Kiecolt-

    Glaser et al., 1993; Malarkey,Kiecolt-Glaser,Pearl, & Glaser,

    1994 ) .

    Most of the studies reviewed in this article

    have

    conceptual-

    ized

    social support as a unidimensional construct. As noted ear-

    lier,multidimensional assessments m ayallowfor anexamina-

    tion

    of

    more specific associations

    and

    mechanisms

    (Uchino,

    Cacioppo,Malarkey, Glaser, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1995).For in-

    stance, Seeman et al.( 1 9 9 4 )foundthat emotional support was

    a more consistent predictor of neuroendocrine

    function

    than

    informational

    support.More important, such

    specificity

    would

    have

    been lost

    if an

    aggregate measure

    of

    social support

    was

    used.

    The

    relative importance

    of

    specific dimensions

    of

    social

    data

    on

    combined tests

    of

    significance

    for the

    relationships that they

    examined.Therefore,someof therelationships between social support

    m ay havebeen statisticallysignificant,albeit characterized

    by

    small

    to

    moderate

    effect

    sizes. Second,

    th e

    aggregation

    of

    suchdiversemeasures

    asindicesofphysical healthandstress outcomes(seeabove)may ob-

    scurereliable relationships

    within

    particular measures (e.g., blood pres-

    sure and

    catecholamines). Finally,

    and

    perhaps most important,

    the

    selection

    o f

    studies

    in the

    meta-analysis

    w as

    limited. None

    of the

    major

    prospective studies

    on

    social relationship

    and

    mortality were included

    in themeta-analysis (see Housee tal., 1988,for areview).Inaddition,

    thereappearsto belittle overlapin thestudies examinedinSmithet al.

    and the

    present

    review. Th e

    reasons

    for

    this discrepancy

    is

    unclear

    be -


Recommended