+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Body image drawings dissociate ethnic differences and ...1 Nutrition Unit, Department of Diabetes,...

Body image drawings dissociate ethnic differences and ...1 Nutrition Unit, Department of Diabetes,...

Date post: 02-Aug-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 0 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
6
Goldzak‑Kunik and Leshem Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health (2017) 11:13 DOI 10.1186/s13034‑017‑0150‑y RESEARCH ARTICLE Body image drawings dissociate ethnic differences and anorexia in adolescent girls Galit Goldzak‑Kunik 1,2* and Micah Leshem 2 Abstract Objectives: To distinguish between ethnic differences among segregated schoolgirls and restrictive anorexia ner‑ vosa using a simple culture‑fair test of body image (BI) figure drawings. Methods: Several responses to BI figure drawings by 178 adolescent schoolgirls from three ethnically distinct and segregated schools and communities in Israel, Jewish secular (JS), Jewish Haredi (H), and Christian Arab (C), and a group of 14 severely restricting anorexic girls (AN). BI evaluations were analyzed by MANCOVA, followed by paired or Student‑t tests for comparisons between responses and groups respectively. Pearson r served for correlations and the Fisher Z for differences between slopes. Results: Despite the total ethnic segregation among the schoolgirls, there are commonalities; all prefer a thinner ideal BI, and are similarly dissatisfied with their BI. However, ethnic differences also emerge: C underestimate their BI and how others view them, and H true and Ideal BI evaluations correlate, unlike the other groups. Despite this variabil‑ ity, and in stark contrast, the anorexic girls show a gross misperception of their BI, even in comparison to girls equated for BMI. Discussion: The findings show that figure drawings evaluation of BI is a simple and robust instrument dissociating clinical and ethnic responses. Clinicians may consider body figure drawings as a simple, supportive, diagnostic for first‑line recognition for risk of AN in adolescent girls. Keywords: Adolescent girls, Anorexia nervosa, Body image dissatisfaction, Body image figure drawings, Ethnic differences © The Author(s) 2017. This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/ publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated. Background Eating disorders (ED) are among the most common psy- chiatric disorders in young women. “A registered dieti- cian may be the first to recognize an individual’s ED symptoms or be the first health care professional con- sulted by a patient for this condition” [1] and “early iden- tification is crucial because shorter duration of illness is associated with improved outcome in ED” [2]. “the pres- entation of eating disorders is often cryptic—for exam- ple, via physical symptoms in primary care. e ability to diagnose the condition varies and can be inadequate, and existing questionnaires for detection are lengthy and may require specialist interpretation” [3]. e 5-question SCOFF is intended for non-specialist use but its utility for the general population and specifically for adolescents is uncertain [4, 5]. Hence, simple diagnostic aids are con- tinually sought. In multi-ethnic communities it is often helpful to dis- tinguish between cultural norms and potential diagnostic features, particularly because cultural factors influence body weight norms, weight control, obesity, and body image (BI), body image dissatisfaction, and consequently may both obscure or increase vulnerability for ED [618]. At the same time in anorexia nervosa (AN), BI dissatis- faction is an essential diagnostic and together with the drive for thinness is extreme and underlies and perpetu- ates the psychopathology [8, 15, 19, 20]. Women, especially adolescent, are more likely to expe- rience BI concerns [2, 7, 9, 18, 21, 22]. Social norms of feminine beauty, particularly western norms of shape and Open Access Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health *Correspondence: [email protected] 1 Nutrition Unit, Department of Diabetes, Haifa and Western Galilee, Clalit Health Services, Lin Medical Center, Rothschild Street 37, Haifa, Israel Full list of author information is available at the end of the article
Transcript
Page 1: Body image drawings dissociate ethnic differences and ...1 Nutrition Unit, Department of Diabetes, Haifa and Western Galilee, Clalit Health Services, Lin Medical Center, Rothschild

Goldzak‑Kunik and Leshem Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health (2017) 11:13 DOI 10.1186/s13034‑017‑0150‑y

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Body image drawings dissociate ethnic differences and anorexia in adolescent girlsGalit Goldzak‑Kunik1,2* and Micah Leshem2

Abstract

Objectives: To distinguish between ethnic differences among segregated schoolgirls and restrictive anorexia ner‑vosa using a simple culture‑fair test of body image (BI) figure drawings.

Methods: Several responses to BI figure drawings by 178 adolescent schoolgirls from three ethnically distinct and segregated schools and communities in Israel, Jewish secular (JS), Jewish Haredi (H), and Christian Arab (C), and a group of 14 severely restricting anorexic girls (AN). BI evaluations were analyzed by MANCOVA, followed by paired or Student‑t tests for comparisons between responses and groups respectively. Pearson r served for correlations and the Fisher Z for differences between slopes.

Results: Despite the total ethnic segregation among the schoolgirls, there are commonalities; all prefer a thinner ideal BI, and are similarly dissatisfied with their BI. However, ethnic differences also emerge: C underestimate their BI and how others view them, and H true and Ideal BI evaluations correlate, unlike the other groups. Despite this variabil‑ity, and in stark contrast, the anorexic girls show a gross misperception of their BI, even in comparison to girls equated for BMI.

Discussion: The findings show that figure drawings evaluation of BI is a simple and robust instrument dissociating clinical and ethnic responses. Clinicians may consider body figure drawings as a simple, supportive, diagnostic for first‑line recognition for risk of AN in adolescent girls.

Keywords: Adolescent girls, Anorexia nervosa, Body image dissatisfaction, Body image figure drawings, Ethnic differences

© The Author(s) 2017. This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

BackgroundEating disorders (ED) are among the most common psy-chiatric disorders in young women. “A registered dieti-cian may be the first to recognize an individual’s ED symptoms or be the first health care professional con-sulted by a patient for this condition” [1] and “early iden-tification is crucial because shorter duration of illness is associated with improved outcome in ED” [2]. “the pres-entation of eating disorders is often cryptic—for exam-ple, via physical symptoms in primary care. The ability to diagnose the condition varies and can be inadequate, and existing questionnaires for detection are lengthy and may require specialist interpretation” [3]. The 5-question

SCOFF is intended for non-specialist use but its utility for the general population and specifically for adolescents is uncertain [4, 5]. Hence, simple diagnostic aids are con-tinually sought.

In multi-ethnic communities it is often helpful to dis-tinguish between cultural norms and potential diagnostic features, particularly because cultural factors influence body weight norms, weight control, obesity, and body image (BI), body image dissatisfaction, and consequently may both obscure or increase vulnerability for ED [6–18]. At the same time in anorexia nervosa (AN), BI dissatis-faction is an essential diagnostic and together with the drive for thinness is extreme and underlies and perpetu-ates the psychopathology [8, 15, 19, 20].

Women, especially adolescent, are more likely to expe-rience BI concerns [2, 7, 9, 18, 21, 22]. Social norms of feminine beauty, particularly western norms of shape and

Open Access

Child and Adolescent Psychiatryand Mental Health

*Correspondence: [email protected] 1 Nutrition Unit, Department of Diabetes, Haifa and Western Galilee, Clalit Health Services, Lin Medical Center, Rothschild Street 37, Haifa, IsraelFull list of author information is available at the end of the article

Page 2: Body image drawings dissociate ethnic differences and ...1 Nutrition Unit, Department of Diabetes, Haifa and Western Galilee, Clalit Health Services, Lin Medical Center, Rothschild

Page 2 of 6Goldzak‑Kunik and Leshem Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health (2017) 11:13

body weight are consistently correlated with increased weight consciousness and risk of ED [14–16, 21, 23, 24]. However, most studies of ethnic differences in risk fac-tors for ED have examined largely intermingled ethnici-ties, often in the same institution, exposed to similar media, and with little segregation. In addition, question-naires used across cultures, often in translation, may not be understood as intended [12].

In Israel, distinct ethnic communities have been stud-ied in relation to propensity for ED. Indigenous Moslem, Druze, Circassian, Christian and Bedouin schoolgirls, as a group in the north of Israel, score higher on ED ques-tionnaires than their Jewish peers, while within the group Circassians score lower and Bedouin higher. Among the Jews, Kibbutz girls scored higher in one study, but not another, and religious girls lower. Such differences are often attributed to socio-cultural traits, ‘modernity’. and exposure to western media [6–8, 13–16, 19, 24].

What makes Israel multi-ethnicity different, and some-what unique, is the complete segregation of different cultural groups often in adjacent habitation. Ethnic and religious identities may be profoundly segregated by resi-dential area, education (schools and colleges separated by ethnic and religious makeup and teaching programs), language, degree of traditionality and religiosity, expo-sure to different mass media or none at all, basic values and attitudes toward femininity, sex roles, marriage and divorce, family relations, child-rearing, dietary restric-tions, and more [7, 12–17]. Such profound demarcations may place an even greater onus on ED tests to reliably distinguish between the cultural and clinical. Thus, in 10 distinct groups of Israeli schoolgirls, EAT-26 subscales in at least 3 of the groups overlapped with AN scores [15].

Hence it is of interest to enquire whether segregated ethnicities differ in BI perception, whether such differ-ences can be evaluated without recourse to language and translation, and whether such differences might over-lap and compromise clinical evaluation of BI distortion [8]. Thus, tests that can screen for risk of ED, are robust across ethnicities and culture-fair, are of interest, espe-cially if simple and brief [5, 8, 13].

Here we examined the robustness of a simple instru-ment (body image figure drawings [23]) to distinguish between cultural and clinical differences in evaluating BI. We compared three groups of ethnically diverse and seg-regated schoolgirls, and a group of severely restricting adolescent AN girls. Christian Arab girls (C) and Jewish secular girls (JS) differ in cultural, traditional, and reli-gious content and norms, access distinct media differing in language (Arabic and Hebrew), with the Arabs tending to the more traditional. Jewish Haredi girls (H) also differ in language, using both Yiddish and Hebrew, are deeply religious and traditional, and are totally isolated from

media (radio, TV, theatre, film, internet, non-sectarian magazines and newspapers, and books (other than scrip-tures)], and State educational programs [10, 12, 13].1

The three groups are entirely separated in and out of school and have negligible or no knowledge of each other.

Among Arab girls, Christians score lower on ED symptoms, but score higher than Jewish girls [7, 12, 17]. Among Jewish girls, the Orthodox score lower, and their self-esteem relates positively to religious fervor and nega-tively to ED scores, the authors suggesting that religiosity provides protection from ED [13, 25]. Haredis are con-siderably more religious, traditional, and segregated than the Orthodox Jewish stream, possibly predicting even lower ED scores for the H girls [9, 10].

Such differences are often attributed to socio-cultural traits and exposure to western media [7, 11–18, 24], although there have been few empirical studies relating specific cultural traits and ED [18, 24].

Our aim was to examine whether a simple test would distinguish the marked ethnic differences between these three groups and to what extent they might overlap those of AN.

MethodsParticipantsEthics approval was obtained from the Ministry of Edu-cation and the School Head for the schoolgirls. Girls and parents signed informed consent forms sent from the school. No incentives were offered. The researchers explained the selection criteria to the School Head: grade, sex (girls—Haredi schools are not mixed), no ED prior or current, and no weight change greater than 5  kg in the previous 6  months. The Head scheduled the classes for the study, and in effect, all the girls in the selected classes volunteered. Testing was carried out by two research assistants in Hebrew or Arabic. The schoolgirls com-pleted the questionnaire with the figure drawings at their desks in the classroom. Subsequently, in a separate room, individually, they were weighed and height measured. To reduce the possibility of ED, schoolgirls reporting weight changes greater than 5  kg in the previous 6  months, or who were currently counselled by a clinical dietitian, were excluded (6 girls).

For AN, ethics approval was obtained from their hos-pital and the university. Parents and daughters gave

1 Jews (including Haredis) are ~80% of the population, Christian Arabs ~0.016%, and Haredis ~9.9%.Statistical abstract of Israel 2010. Demographic characteristics. Population, by population group, religion, sex and age http://www.cbs.gov.il/reader/shnaton/templ_shnaton_e.html?num_tab=st02_19&CYear=2010. Accessed 02 April 2016.Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia. Demographics of Israel. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Demographics_of_Israel#Ethnic_and_religious_groups. Accessed 02 April 2016.

Page 3: Body image drawings dissociate ethnic differences and ...1 Nutrition Unit, Department of Diabetes, Haifa and Western Galilee, Clalit Health Services, Lin Medical Center, Rothschild

Page 3 of 6Goldzak‑Kunik and Leshem Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health (2017) 11:13

informed consent and permission for access to relevant medical information which included BMI, because we did not weigh or measure height to avoid stress. In addi-tion, to circumvent the possible conflictual nature of the AN-therapist relation, we emphasized to the participants that our researchers were not involved in their treatment, and were from an unrelated university (in Haifa, a differ-ent town). To minimize variability in diagnosis, treatment regimen, and environment, we enlisted participants from the same closed ward, in their first episode, and similarly diagnosed with restrictive anorexia with no comorbidi-ties. Girls were tested within 2.9 ± 0.1 day of hospitaliza-tion to minimize entrenchment of AN behavior patterns often acquired from veteran AN patients, and prior to any recovery. Drug therapy commenced at intake and included olanzapine, amisulpride, or risperidone. Four-teen girls meeting the criteria were enrolled, 3 refused. They completed the BI questionnaire with the figure drawings individually in a separate room [20]. No incen-tive was offered but after the tests an unanticipated token gift of a necklace (value ~$4) was offered in appreciation.

Participant group size, age and BMI are presented in Table 1.

Body image evaluationUsing 7 different schematic figure drawings of girls [23], participants marked a continuous linear scale below the images in response to each of 3 questions: (1) the image

“most like you”, (2) their “ideal body shape”, and (3) “which figure would others consider most like you” (in Hebrew, or verbally in Arabic).

Statistical analysisFor analysis, scores were converted to age-corrected BMI in the 3rd–97th percentiles (BMI%) (26). BI Dissatisfac-tion was calculated as the difference between questions 1 and 2. BI evaluations were analyzed by MANCOVA with BMI% and age as covariates, followed by paired or Student-t test for comparisons between responses and groups respectively (SPSS 19). Dissatisfaction was sepa-rately evaluated by univariate analysis because it is not independent of the questions.

Means are presented with standard deviation, and effect size is the partial eta squared (PES). Pearson r served for correlations and the Fisher Z for differences between slopes.

ResultsMANCOVA for the 4 groups and 3 questions with BMI% and age covariates shows a group effect, F (9558)=4.3, p < 0.001, PES = 0.065, due to question 1, “most like you”, F (3186) = 10.8, p < 0.001, PES = 0.149, and 3, “others consider most like you”, F (3186) =  8.8, p < 0.001, PES = 0.124. The girls do not differ in their thinner Ideal (Table  2). Univariate analysis with BMI% and age covariates for dissatisfaction shows a group effect, F (3187)  =  2.9, p  <  0.05, PES  =  0.045, due to AN more dissatisfied than schoolgirls, whereas it does not differ among schoolgirls. However, 1-sample tests show that all are dissatisfied with their BI (p’s  <  0.001. Table 2).

For a detailed analysis of trends between the groups we compared slopes of BI scores. Self-estimation of body size correlates with BMI% in groups JS, C and H but not in AN. Ideal preference correlates with BMI% only in H, and dissatisfaction correlates with BMI% in schoolgirl

Table 1 Participant data (±SD)

Different from other groups, * p’s < 0.05–0.001

Age (years) BMI n

Restrictive anorexic (AN) 16.0 ± 1.1* 17.3 ± 1.8* 14

Jewish Secular (JS) 14.2 ± 1.3 19.4 ± 3.9 81

Christian Arab (C) 14.9 ± 0.4 22.1 ± 2.9* 37

Haredi (H) 14.5 ± 1.3 20.1 ± 3.3 60

Table 2 BI self-evaluation expressed as BMI% compared to true BMI%

Responses to a series of schematic body image drawings thin to obese

AN anorexics, JS Jewish secular, C Christian Arab, H Haredi, schoolgirls

Differ from true BMI%: * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001

Thin schoolgirls matched to AN for BMI%. AN differ from Thin: + p < 0.05,++ p < 0.005,+++ p < 0.001. Thin girl evaluations also diverge significantly from true BMI% (not indicated)

True BMI% Question 1. Most like you

Question 2. Ideal shape

Question 3. Others consider most like you

Dissatisfaction (questions 1–2)

AN 15.3 ± 15.6 58.8 ± 17.1*** 30.4 ± 12.0* 55.4 ± 20.0*** 28.4 ± 28.5

JS 52.1 ± 26.2 50.5 ± 15.3 41.9 ± 11.7*** 47.1 ± 20.9 8.7 ± 17.1

C 66.7 ± 26.9 50.6 ± 17.0*** 39.5 ± 12.6*** 48.4 ± 18.0*** 11.1 ± 16.5

H 48.4 ± 29.8 51.0 ± 17.0 39.1 ± 12.2* 48.3 ± 20.0 11.8 ± 16.2

Thin 10.13 ± 6.3 34.1 ± 15.3++ 38.0 ± 13.6+ 31.4 ± 15.9++ −3.9 ± 14.7+++

Page 4: Body image drawings dissociate ethnic differences and ...1 Nutrition Unit, Department of Diabetes, Haifa and Western Galilee, Clalit Health Services, Lin Medical Center, Rothschild

Page 4 of 6Goldzak‑Kunik and Leshem Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health (2017) 11:13

groups (p’s < 0.001) but not in AN, who are more dissatis-fied (Fig. 1; Table 2).

Left panel: body size estimates correlate for the school-girls (JS, 0.61, C, 0.70, H, 0.67, p’s < 0.001) but not for AN. AN differ from the other slopes (Fisher Z, JS = −2.06, C-2.33, H = −2.33, p’s  <  0.05). Center panel: ideal esti-mates correlate significantly only for H (0.33, p = 0.01). Slopes do not differ significantly. Right panel, dissatisfac-tion with own BI (difference between questions 1 and 2). Dissatisfaction correlates for the schoolgirls, (JS, 0.45, C, 0.58, H, 0.46, p’s < 0.001), but not for AN (0.08). The slopes do not differ. Horizontal reference line denotes ‘no Dissatisfaction’ and below it—‘Satisfaction’.

Finally, to isolate any effect of low BMI we directly compared AN responses with 29 schoolgirls selected for BMI% <25 (‘Thin’). MANCOVA for the 2 groups and 3 questions with BMI% and age as covariates shows a group effect F (3,37)  =  4.6, p  <  0.01, PES  =  0.273, due to group effects on question 1, “most like you”, F (1,39) =  9.1, p  <  0.005, PES =  0.189, question 2, “Ideal shape”, F (1,39) = 4.3, p < 0.05, PES = 0.099, and ques-tions 3, “others consider most like you”, F (1,39) = 10.7, p  <  0.005, PES  =  0.215 (Table  2). Univariate ANOVA confirmed that Dissatisfaction differs between AN and Thin, F (1,39) =  13.5, p  <  0.001, PES =  0.257, because Thin girls are not dissatisfied with their BI (Table 2). Like the other schoolgirls, Thin girl evaluations diverge signif-icantly from true BMI% (data not shown).

DiscussionIn comparing 3 groups of schoolgirls we find that despite substantive ethnic differences, BI evaluations are similar: Ideal shape is thinner than true BMI%, and dissatisfac-tion significant.

Ethnic differences in BI evaluations also emerged. Although C slightly underestimate their BI, show a greater disparity between true BMI%, and their thinner Ideal, and believe that others see them as thinner than

they are, this might be an artefact of their greater BMI. Traditional Arab society viewed plumpness as desirable [12, 15], but we doubt that these girls were still influ-enced because their Ideal is similar to that of their peers, and they are equally dissatisfied, suggesting that their greater disparity between true BMI%, and their thinner Ideal reflects an adaptive BI. Our finding that C do not differ in body dissatisfaction from JS and other ethnic groups is supported by assessments using the EDI-2 [7, 12], although there were higher scores among the Arab girls in other ED-2 subscales that were suggested to be due to Arab women’s stressors and difficulties in a patri-archal community rather than specific eating or thinness ideals [17].

The correlation of BMI% and Ideal preference in H is not found in the other groups. Although like the other girls their ideal was thinner, and dissatisfaction similar, the correlation suggests that the thinner ideal was deter-mined by their own BI, i.e. each girl would like to be thin-ner in relation to her own BI rather than to an externally determined ideal (as by media exposure, to which H are not exposed). Conversely, because the ideal for the other schoolgirls is unrelated to their BI, it may derive from an external, consensual ideal, consistent with the belief that media exposure contributes to the desire for thinness by its incessant portrayal as beauty [6, 7, 9, 11, 12, 18, 21, 22, 24].

Haredi women conform to strict dress codes, almost uniform clothing concealing arms and legs in public, and wigs to conceal hair or their shaved head. The reli-gious rationale is to minimize attraction to men, and may therefore also diminish the importance of BI [10]. Along with media isolation, religion, and culture, this may account for less ED in this and other traditional commu-nities [7, 10, 13, 16, 25].

In stark contrast, AN show a gross misperception of their BI. Unlike the healthy schoolgirls, there is no rela-tionship between dissatisfaction and BMI percentile

Fig. 1 BMI% estimates by the girls for each of the 3 questions against ‘true’ BMI%

Page 5: Body image drawings dissociate ethnic differences and ...1 Nutrition Unit, Department of Diabetes, Haifa and Western Galilee, Clalit Health Services, Lin Medical Center, Rothschild

Page 5 of 6Goldzak‑Kunik and Leshem Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health (2017) 11:13

among AN (r  =  0.08, Fig.  1 right panel) [19–21] who are thus dissatisfied with their BI irrespective of its true dimensions. We now show that, again, unlike the school-girls, this misperception extends to believing that others also see them as much bigger than they are. As repeatedly reported and we confirm here, they overestimate their size, are clearly dissatisfied with their BI, and their Ideal is thinner [8, 19, 20, 22].

Finally, a direct comparison with equally thin, but healthy, schoolgirls confirms the clear difference in BI evaluation. A thin healthy girl has positive body satisfac-tion, and her ideal is only slightly heavier than how she sees herself. This is important because it suggests that body figure drawings might be a simple instrument to help first-line clinicians recognize adolescent girl patients presenting with, eg, weight loss and high BI dissatisfac-tion, at risk for ED.

Limitations of the studyThe AN group was small because of the difficulty of finding AN homogenous by age, diagnosis (severely restricted) and recency of hospitalization, as our AN par-ticipants were. Ethnically they were Jewish secular, how-ever, in all measures they clearly differed from the healthy JS, C and H schoolgirls including those matched for BMI. AN were also older but the differences survived statistical adjustment for age.

ConclusionsWe show that BI evaluation with figure drawings, while sensitive to ethnic differences, is robust across adoles-cent girls of segregated ethnicities and media exposure. On the other hand, the test clearly distinguishes between ethnic variation and restrictive anorexia nervosa.

Clinicians may consider body figure drawings for sim-ple first-line recognition and referral for risk of AN in adolescent girls.

Although these preliminary results are suggestive, validation by extensive prospective studies would be of interest.

AbbreviationsAN: anorexia nervosa; BI: body image; C: Christian; ED: eating disorder; JS: Jew‑ish secular; H: Haredi; BMI%: BMI percentile by age; PES: partial eta squared.

Authors’ contributionsBoth authors contributed equally to planning the research, writing the paper, statistical analysis and interpretation of the data. Both authors approved the submitted manuscript. GGK carried out the study. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Author details1 Nutrition Unit, Department of Diabetes, Haifa and Western Galilee, Clalit Health Services, Lin Medical Center, Rothschild Street 37, Haifa, Israel. 2 Depart‑ment of Psychology, University of Haifa, 31905 Haifa, Israel.

AcknowledgementsWe thank Noga David, Raneen Hashoul, Zimratya Duchan, Riki Shotten, Shani Wilman, Limor Ganor; Ruthi Leyder and Navit Greydi for their invaluable help in the study.

Competing interestsThe authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Availability of data and materialsThe datasets used and/or analysed during the current study is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Consent to publishWe obtained consent to publish from the participant and from her legal par‑ent or guardian to report individual patient data anonymously.

Ethics approval and consent to participateEthics approval was obtained from the Ministry of Education for the school‑girls and for AN from their hospital and the university. Girls and parents signed informed consent forms.

Received: 13 May 2016 Accepted: 21 February 2017

References 1. Ozier AD, Henry BW. American Dietetic Association. Position of the Ameri‑

can Dietetic Association: nutrition intervention in the treatment of eating disorders. J Am Diet Assoc. 2011;111:1236–41.

2. Golden NH, Katzman DK, Sawyer SM, Ornstein RM, Rome ES, Garber AK, Kohn M, Kreipe RE. Position Paper of the Society for Adolescent Health and Medicine: medical management of restrictive eating disorders in adolescents and young adults. J Adolesc Health. 2015;56:121–5.

3. Morgan JF, Fiona R, Lacey JH. The SCOFF questionnaire a new screening tool for eating disorders. West J Med. 2000;172:164–5.

4. Hansson E, Daukantaité D, Johnsson P. SCOFF in a general Swedish ado‑lescent population. J Eat Disord. 2015;3:48.

5. Solmi F, Hatch SL, Hotopf M, Treasure J, Micali N. Validation of the SCOFF questionnaire for eating disorders in a multiethnic general population sample. Int J Eat Disord. 2015;48(3):312–6.

6. Keel PK, Klump KL. Are eating disorders culture bound syndromes? Impli‑cations for conceptualizing their etiology. Psychol Bull. 2003;129:747–69.

7. Latzer Y, Witztum E. Stein. Eating disorders and disordered eating in Israel: an updated review. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2008;16:361–74.

8. White MA, Grilo CM. Ethnic differences in the prediction of eating and body image disturbances among female adolescent psychiatric inpatients. Int J Eat Disord. 2005;38:78–84.

9. Gluck ME, Geliebter A. Racial/ethnic differences in body image and eating behaviors. Eat Behav. 2002;3:143–51.

10. Gluck ME, Geliebter A. Body image and eating behaviors in Orthodox and Secular Jewish women. J Gend Specif Med. 2002;5:19–24.

11. Evans P, McConnell A. Do racial minorities respond in the same way to mainstream beauty standards? Social comparison processes in Asian, Black, and White women. Self Identity. 2003;2:153–67.

12. Latzer Y, Tzischinsky O, Azaiza F. Disordered eating related behaviors among Arab schoolgirls in Israel: an epidemiological study. Int J Eat Disord. 2007;40:263–70.

13. Latzer Y, Orna T, Gefen S. Level of religiosity and disordered eating psy‑chopathology among modern‑orthodox Jewish adolescent girls in Israel. Int J Adolesc Med Health. 2007;19:511–21.

14. Safir MP, Flaisher‑Kellner S, Rosenmann A. When gender differences surpass cultural differences in personal satisfaction with body shape in Israeli College Students. Sex Roles. 2005;52:369–78.

15. Apter A, Abu Shah M, Iancu I, Abramovitch H, Weizman A, Tyano S. Cul‑tural effects on eating attitudes in Israeli subpopulations and hospitalized anorectics. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr. 1994;120:83–99.

16. Latzer Y, Azaiza F, Tzischinsky O. Eating attitudes and dieting behavior among religious subgroups of Israeli‑Arab adolescent females. J Relig Health. 2009;48:189–99.

Page 6: Body image drawings dissociate ethnic differences and ...1 Nutrition Unit, Department of Diabetes, Haifa and Western Galilee, Clalit Health Services, Lin Medical Center, Rothschild

Page 6 of 6Goldzak‑Kunik and Leshem Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health (2017) 11:13

• We accept pre-submission inquiries

• Our selector tool helps you to find the most relevant journal

• We provide round the clock customer support

• Convenient online submission

• Thorough peer review

• Inclusion in PubMed and all major indexing services

• Maximum visibility for your research

Submit your manuscript atwww.biomedcentral.com/submit

Submit your next manuscript to BioMed Central and we will help you at every step:

17. Latzer Y, Tzischinsky O, Geraisy N. Comparative study of eating‑related attitudes and psychological traits between Israeli‑Arab and‑Jewish schoolgirls. J Adolesc. 2007;30(4):627–37.

18. Rieger E, Touyz SW, Swain T, Beumont PJ. Cross‑cultural research on anorexia nervosa: assumptions regarding the role of body weight. Int J Eat Disord. 2001;29:205–15.

19. Skrzypek S, Wehmeier PM, Remschmidt H. Body image assessment using body size estimation in recent studies on anorexia nervosa. a brief review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;10:215–21.

20. Goldzak‑Kunik G, Friedman R, Spitz M, Sandler L, Leshem M. Intact sen‑sory function in anorexia nervosa. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;95:272–82.

21. Cortese S, Falissard B, Pigaiani Y, et al. The relationship between body mass index and body size dissatisfaction in young adolescents: spline function analysis. J Am Diet Assoc. 2010;110:1098–102.

22. Ahern AL, Bennett KM, Kelly M, Hetherington MM. A qualitative explora‑tion of young women’s attitudes towards the thin ideal. J Health Psychol. 2011;16:70–9.

23. Collins EM. Body figure perceptions and preferences among preadoles‑cent children. Int J Eat Disord. 1990;10:199–208.

24. Te’eni‑Harari T, Eyal K. Liking them thin: adolescents’ favorite televi‑sion characters and body image. J Health Commun. 2015;20:607–15.

25. Lazter Y, Weinberger‑Litman S, Gerson B, Rosch A, Mischel R, Hinden T, et al. Negative religious coping predicts disordered eating pathology among orthodox Jewish adolescent girls. J Relig Health. 2015;53:1760–71.


Recommended