+ All Categories
Home > Documents > By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of...

By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of...

Date post: 12-Jun-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 1 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
51
AN EXAMINATION OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING IN A PEDIATRIC SAMPLE WITH ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 2017
Transcript
Page 1: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

AN EXAMINATION OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING IN A PEDIATRIC SAMPLE WITH ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER

By

DANIELLE LAINE COOKE

A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF

FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE

OF MASTER OF SCIENCE

UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

2017

Page 2: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

© 2017 Danielle Laine Cooke

Page 3: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

To my patients, who inspire me, and to my family, who believe in me

Page 4: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

4

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This thesis would not have been possible without the encouragement, support

and guidance of Dr. Gary R. Geffken, Dr. Joseph P.H. McNamara, Mr. Andrew Giles

Guzick, Ms. Andrea Guastello, Ms. Lacie Lazaroe, and Dr. Brian Olsen. I would

especially like to thank my incredible research assistants, particularly Mr. Luc Loyola,

Ms. Haley Ehrlich, Mr. Jesse Blogg, Ms. Kassidy Schaeffer, Ms. Darby Monagle, and

Ms. Pooja Surkanti. I would also like to thank Dr. W. Keith Berg, Dr. Carol Mathews, Dr.

Michael Marsiske and Dr. Shelley Heaton who all took time out of their busy schedules

to guide this project.

Page 5: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

5

TABLE OF CONTENTS

page

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ............................................................................................... 4

LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................ 7

LIST OF FIGURES .......................................................................................................... 8

LIST OF ABREVIATIONS ............................................................................................... 9

ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................... 10

CHAPTER

1 INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................ 12

ADHD ......................................................................................................................... 14

ADHD And Anxiety ..................................................................................................... 15

Executive Measures Of ADHD ................................................................................... 17

Study Aims ................................................................................................................. 18

2 METHODS ................................................................................................................. 20

Participants ................................................................................................................ 20

Procedure .................................................................................................................. 20

Measures ................................................................................................................... 21

Demographics ....................................................................................................... 21

Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System .............................................................. 21

The Wechsler Intelligence Scale For Children, Fourth Edition ................................ 22

Behavior Rating Inventory Of Executive Functioning ............................................. 22

Statistical Analyses .................................................................................................... 24

Aim 1. Examination Of Common Factors Between Tests ....................................... 24

Hypothesis 1: Subsections of the BRIEF, D-KEFS and WISC-IV will load onto

processing speed, working memory, and inhibitory control. ............................... 24

Aim 2: Predicting Comorbid Anxiety Diagnosis In Children With ADHD ................. 25

Hypothesis 2a: On parental measures, children with an anxiety diagnosis will

demonstrate poorer behavioral regulation. ......................................................... 25

Hypothesis 2b: On performance measures, children with an additional anxiety

diagnosis will have better working memory than those without an anxiety

diagnosis. ........................................................................................................... 25

Page 6: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

6

3 RESULTS ................................................................................................................... 27

Preliminary Correlational Analyses ............................................................................ 27

Aim 1. Examination of Common Factors Between Tests ........................................... 27

Aim 2: Predicting Comorbid Anxiety Diagnosis In Children With ADHD..................... 29

Hypothesis 2a: On Parental Measures, Children With An Anxiety Diagnosis Will

Demonstrate Poorer Behavioral Regulation. ...................................................... 29

Hypothesis 2b: On Performance Measures, Children With An Additional Anxiety

Diagnosis Will Have Better Working Memory Than Those Without An Anxiety

Diagnosis. ........................................................................................................... 30

4 DISCUSSION ............................................................................................................. 41

REFERENCES .............................................................................................................. 46

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH ............................................................................................ 51

Page 7: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

7

LIST OF TABLES

Table page

2-1 Sample demographics ........................................................................................ 26

3-1 Correlations between performance-measures of executive functioning and subsections of the BRIEF ................................................................................... 32

3-2 Communalities ................................................................................................... 34

3-3 Total variance explained .................................................................................... 35

3-4 Pattern matrix ..................................................................................................... 37

3-5 Structure matrix .................................................................................................. 38

3-6 Factor correlation matrix .................................................................................... 38

3-7 Hierarchical binary logistic regression: step 1 .................................................... 39

3-8 Hierarchical binary logistic regression: step 2 .................................................... 39

3-9 Hierarchical binary logistic regression: step 3 .................................................... 40

3-10 Classification table .............................................................................................. 40

Page 8: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

8

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure page

3-1 Scree plot ........................................................................................................... 36

Page 9: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

9

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS

ADHD Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

ADHD + AD Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with comorbid anxiety

disorders

BRIEF Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function

D-KEFS Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System

DSM-IV Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition

DSM-V Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition

EF Executive functioning

IC Inhibitory control

TD Typically developing

WM Working memory

Page 10: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

10

Abstract of Thesis Presented to the Graduate School of the University of Florida in

Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science

AN EXAMINATION OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING IN A PEDIATRIC SAMPLE WITH ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER

By

Danielle Laine Cooke

May 2017

Chair: David M. Janicke Major: Psychology

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder

that is associated with impairments in important areas of executive functioning (EF),

including processing speed, shifting, inhibition, and working memory. Children with

anxiety are hypothesized to show similar deficits. This study attempted to evaluate the

utility of using EF measures to distinguish children with ADHD from children with ADHD

and comorbid anxiety (ADHD+AD). Archival data was analyzed from a clinical sample of

209 youth (aged 7 to 18) with an ADHD diagnosis referred for a psychoeducational

assessment; of this sample, 90 youth had an additional anxiety diagnosis. Parent-report

of EF was assessed using the BRIEF, while subsections of the WISC-IV and D-KEFS

were used to assess performance-based executive functioning. A factor analysis

revealed that the BRIEF subtests cross load along the Metacognition and Behavioral

Regulation Indices, while the D-KEFS and WISC-IV subscores loaded onto a distinct

factor. A binary logistic regression revealed that performance-based EF tasks, but not

parent-report of EF, predicted comorbid anxiety diagnosis in children with ADHD

Page 11: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

11

significantly better than chance. Better performance on a working memory task and

worse performance on a processing speed and switching task predicts a comorbid

anxiety diagnosis in children with ADHD. These preliminary findings suggest that

performance-based measures of EF may be useful in helping to differentiate between

children with ADHD and children with ADHD+AD. Limitations of this study include

examination of a limited testing battery and high rates of comorbidity in the sample.

Page 12: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

12

CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION

Executive functioning is a broad term for higher order cognitive functions

(Friedman & Miyake, 2016). It impacts how human behavior is expressed (Jurado &

Roselli, 2007; Banich, 2009) and is important for almost all areas of daily living

(Diamond, 2013). Many studies have examined and conceptualized executive

functioning, leading to a variety of different, and at times conflicting, definitions of what

comprises and constitutes “executive functioning” (Banich, 2009; Chung, Weyandt &

Swentosky, p. 4-6; Juardo & Rosselli, 2007).

Recent research has demonstrated that executive functioning tasks require the

use of a complex frontoparietal control network (Banich, 2009; Chung et al., 2014;

Niendam et al., 2012), with the prefrontal, dorsal anterior cingulate, and parietal cortices

being consistently implicated across domains of executive functioning (Niendam et al.,

2012). Given the complexity of this system, it comes as little surprise that researchers

have debated whether executive functioning can be viewed as a single construct or as a

compilation of several distinct functions. Currently there is cautious consensus that

executive functioning is composed of at least a few distinct, general areas. However, a

number of studies have had difficulty in separating these areas, and attempts to specify

their contributions to specific executive tasks had only moderate success (Diamond,

2013, Jurado & Rosselli, 2007), resulting in a variety of heterogeneous tasks that utilize

a variety of executive domains (Miyake, 2000). Evidence has shown that broad areas of

executive functioning areas are clearly distinguishable, and yet, at the same time, are

not completely independent of one another (Miyake et al., 2000). This has resulted in

the general agreement that executive functioning is comprised of broad areas that work

Page 13: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

13

independently and in conjunction: working memory, inhibitory control, shifting (Diamond,

2013; Friedman & Miyake, 2016), and processing speed (Anderson, 2002).

Working memory (WM) can be defined as the capacity to temporarily retain and

manipulate relevant information (Chung et al., 2014; Jurado & Rosselli, 2007; McCabe,

Roediger, McDaniel, Balota, & Hambrick, 2010; Niendam et al., 2012; Olivers, Peters,

Houtkamp & Roelfsema, 2011). WM tasks require maintenance, manipulation, and

updating of information (Friedman & Miyake, 2017). While the “updating” portion of WM

has previously been described as a distinct process that is “closely related” to WM

(Miyake, Friedman, Rettinger, Shah, & Hegarty, 2001), many other authors have used

WM (Jurado & Rosselli, 2007; McCabe et al., 2010; Niendam et al., 2012; Olivers et al.,

2011) to describe Miyake’s “updating” process, and both terms have been used

interchangeably (Salthouse et al., 2003). Therefore, this study defines the processes of

WM as the capacity not only to store, but to actively manipulate task-relevant

information, and thus, for the purpose of this study, the terms WM and updating will be

considered interchangeable.

Other areas of executive functioning include inhibitory control (IC), or the deliberate,

intentional decision to override dominant responses (Friedman & Miyake, 2017), and

shifting (also referred to as switching, set shifting, or cognitive flexibility) which refers to

the ability to switch flexibly back and forth between tasks or mental sets (Diamond,

2013; Friedman & Miyake, 2017). Though not traditionally defined as an aspect of

executive functioning, processing speed will also be considered throughout this study.

Processing speed, often referred to as perceptual speed, can be defined as a measure

of how quickly cognitive functions take place.

Page 14: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

14

ADHD

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder

that is characterized by a persistent pattern of inattention and/or hyperactivity and

impulsivity that occurs across multiple settings and interferes with functioning or

development (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). It is associated with several

impairments in important areas of executive functioning, including shifting, IC, and WM

(Snyder, Miyake, & Hankin, 2015b). Recent meta-analyses estimate prevalence rates of

ADHD to range from 7.2 to about 9.5% (Polanczyk, Willcutt, Salum, Kieling & Rohde,

2014; Thomas, Sanders, Doust, Beller & Glasziou, 2015).

ADHD has been shown to cause impairments in several areas of life, including

work performance (Brook, Brook, Zhang, Seltzer, & Finch, 2013), employment (Erskine

et al., 2016), school performance (Taanila et al., 2014), self-esteem, social functioning

(Harpin, Mazzone, Raynaud, Kahle, & Hodgkins, 2016; Tarver, Daley, & Sayal, 2014)

and both parent and child rated quality of life (Lee et al., 2016). Additionally, ADHD

carries a high societal burden; with the annual incremental costs of ADHD ranging from

$143 to $266 billion in the United States alone (Doshi et al., 2012). Of this estimate,

children and adolescents are estimated to only account for $38 to $72 billion while

adults account for $105 to $194 billion. The majority of the adult cost can be found in

productivity and income losses (Doshi et al., 2012), suggesting that early diagnosis of

ADHD is vital.

Executive functioning is crucial to the effective treatment of ADHD. Unfortunately,

many studies examining executive dysfunction within ADHD have been inconclusive,

inconsistent, or reported only small to medium effect sizes (Eycke & Dewey, 2016,

Page 15: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

15

Snyder et al., 2015b; Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, & Pennington, 2005). The

strongest and most consistent effects can be found on measures of IC and WM; even

after the effects of comorbid disorders, intelligences and academic achievement are

controlled (Willcutt et al., 2005). Children with ADHD have been shown to perform more

poorly than controls on almost all neuropsychological functions examined, including

inhibition, working memory and shifting, when compared to their typically developing

(TD) peers (Sjöwall, Roth, Lindqvist, & Thorell, 2013). At least part of this deficit

appears to be biological in nature with children with ADHD demonstrating reduced WM

task specific brain activation in comparison to TD peers (Fassbender et al., 2011).

Furthermore, a recent meta-analysis suggested that adults and adolescents with ADHD

demonstrated reduced IC task specific brain activation when compared to controls

(Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols, & Rubia, 2013). Processing speed impairments have

been associated with ADHD, and it has been suggested that observed impairments in

WM may be better accounted for by lower processing speed (Weigard & Huang-Pollock,

2016).

ADHD And Anxiety

ADHD and anxiety disorders are highly comorbid disorders, with up to 25% of

children with ADHD meeting criteria for a comorbid anxiety disorder (Adler, Spencer &

Willins, 2015; Efron, Bryson, Lycett, & Sciberras, 2016; Schatz & Rostain, 2006). Given

this, the impact of a comorbid anxiety diagnosis on executive functioning is of significant

clinical interest. Limited research has examined executive impairment in anxiety

disorders, and what has been done has yielded contradictory results. Similar to

research with ADHD, anxiety disorders (panic disorder, social anxiety disorders and

Page 16: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

16

generalized anxiety disorders) have been associated with impairments in shifting, IC,

and WM though these results have not been reliably replicated (Snyder et al. 2015b).

Although obsessive-compulsive disorder is not currently classified as an anxiety

disorder in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (DSM-

V; American Psychological Association, 2013) research has consistently demonstrated

executive deficits in this population. Namely, individuals with this diagnosis have been

shown to have impaired performance for shifting, inhibition and WM when compared to

controls (Snyder, Kaiser, Warren & Heller, 2015a). Given that obsessive-compulsive

disorder displays impairments similar to those posited in anxiety spectrum disorders,

and that our subjects were diagnosed using Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV) criteria when obsessive-compulsive disorder was

classified as an anxiety disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder will be considered a

“comorbid anxiety disorder” for the purpose of this study.

With nearly one fourth of those with ADHD meeting criteria for at least one

anxiety disorder (Adler et al., 2015; Efron et al., 2016; Schatz & Rostain, 2006), the

impact of a comorbid anxiety diagnosis on executive impairment in ADHD is important

to examine. As with anxiety disorders alone, research examining ADHD with a comorbid

anxiety disorder (ADHD + AD) has been somewhat limited and contradictory. Some

studies have suggested that children with ADHD + AD have better performance on

inhibitory tasks and greater impairments in WM (Jarrett, Wolff, Davis, Cowart &

Ollendick, 2016). On the BRIEF, children with ADHD + AD were rated significantly

higher on the inhibition scale than children with “pure” ADHD or anxiety (Sørensen,

Plessen, Nicholas, & Lundervold, 2011).

Page 17: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

17

Executive Measures Of ADHD

Both the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) and Behavior Rating

Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) are commonly administered clinical measures

posited to examine executive functioning. The D-KEFS is a traditional neurocognitive

exam that could be posited to measure optimal executive functioning while the BRIEF, a

parental report-based measure, could be considered to capture “everyday” executive

functioning. The BRIEF has been shown to distinguish between children with ADHD and

TD peers better than performance-based measures of executive functioning (Davidson,

Cherry & Corkum, 2016). Individuals with ADHD have variable performance on

neurocognitive batteries. The current recommendation is to provide both rating scales

and performance tasks (Davidson et al., 2016); rating scales are hypothesized to

capture behavioral aspects of executive functioning, while performance tasks are

hypothesized to capture cognitive aspects of executive functioning (Davidson et al.,

2016; McAuley et al., 2010). The reason for this discrepancy between performance

measures and rating scales is unclear; while it is possible that this variable performance

could be due to our current diagnostic criteria not adequately capturing differences in

cognitive impairment ranging across ADHD subtypes (Roberts, Martel, & Nigg, 2017), it

is also possible that this discrepancy is a result of variations in operational definitions of

executive constructs (Eycke & Dewey, 2016), and the issue of task impurity.

Given that executive functioning is composed of broad subcomponents that work

together and independently (Friedman & Miyake, 2017), various tasks differ in their

dependence on certain subcomponents of executive functioning thereby contributing to

unexpected variations in performance (Miyake et al., 2000). Additionally, as executive

Page 18: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

18

tasks increase in complexity, they tend to utilize other cognitive functions (Miyake et al.,

2000). This issue of “task impurity,” or the idea that executive tasks will tap into more

than one area of executive functioning, is a common impediment to executive

functioning research. It has, in part, contributed to the vast collection of contradictory

executive functioning research, including low intercorrelations between different

executive tasks and low internal and test-retest reliability in complex executive tasks

(Miyake et al., 2000). Thus, this limitation is important to recognize, and it was

necessary to attempt to select for tasks that tapped predominantly into each of the

primary components of executive functioning examined in this study.

Given the question of task impurity, the Processing Speed section of the fourth

edition of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WISC-IV) was utilized in order to examine

processing speed; the WM section of the WISC-IV was utilized in order to examine WM;

the D-KEFS Trail-Making, Condition 4, Letter-Number Switching Scaled Score and D-

KEFS Color-Word, Condition 3, Inhibition-Switching Scaled Score were utilized in in

order to examine IC; and the D-KEFS Trail-Making, Condition 4, Letter-Number

Switching Scaled Score and D-KEFS Color-Word, Condition 3, Inhibition-Switching

Scaled Score were utilized.

Study Aims

Aim 1. To investigate the common factors between test subsections on the

BRIEF, D-KEFS and WISC-IV.

Hypothesis 1: Subsections of the BRIEF, D-KEFS and WISC-IV will load onto

processing speed, working memory, and inhibitory control.

Aim 2. To investigate if parental and performance measures (test subsections

from Aim 1) predict a comorbid anxiety diagnosis in children with ADHD

Page 19: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

19

Hypothesis 2a: On parental measures, children with an additional anxiety

diagnosis will demonstrate poorer behavioral regulation.

Hypothesis 2b: On performance measures, children with an additional anxiety

diagnosis will have better working memory than those without.

Page 20: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

20

CHAPTER 2 METHODS

Participants

Participants in the study consisted of 209 children and adolescents referred for

psychoeducational testing and evaluation selected from a clinical database. Participants

ranged in age from 7 to 19, and had a diagnosis of ADHD as determined by a licensed

psychologist or psychiatrist. All participants in this sample completed the D-K EFS and

had a parent or guardian complete the BRIEF. The population was predominately male

(n=162, 71.7%) and ranged in age from 94 to 225 months (mean = 146.50 months; SD

= 31.28 months). Of this sample, nearly 40% also met criteria for a DSM-IV anxiety

diagnosis (n=90, 39.8%). Anxiety disorders included in the analysis were separation

anxiety disorder, social phobia, generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, obsessive-

compulsive disorder, anxiety not otherwise specified, and unspecified anxiety disorder.

Sample demographics are presented in Table 2-1.

Procedure

A retrospective chart review was performed in order extract relevant data.

Existing medical records and psychoeducational reports from UF Health Shands

Springhill and Vista were examined. Only individuals with a diagnosis of ADHD, who

were aged 7 to 19, who had completed both the D-KEFS and the BRIEF were

considered for inclusion. The study was approved by the University of Florida

institutional review board.

Page 21: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

21

Measures

Demographics

Demographic data, including participant age, sex and diagnoses were collected

from a clinical database and a retrospective chart review.

Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS)

The D-KEFS is a standardized parent-rated neurocognitive exam for individuals

aged 8 to 89 years of age that has been validated in a number of clinical populations as

well as a representative national sample (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Holdnack, 2004;

Homack, Lee, & Riccio, 2005). From the D-KEFS, three tests were examined: the Trail

Making Test (composed of Condition 1: Visual Scanning, Condition 2: Number

Sequencing, Condition 3: Letter Sequencing, Condition 4: Letter-Number Sequencing

and Condition 5: Motor Speed), the Verbal Fluency Test (composed of Condition 1:

Letter Fluency, Condition 2: Category Fluency, and Condition 3: Category Switching),

and the Color-Word Interference Test (composed of Condition 1: Color Naming,

Condition 2: Word Reading, Condition 3: Inhibition, and Condition 4:

Inhibition/Switching).

The Trail Making Test is intended isolate set-shifting from letter sequencing and

visual scanning by including four baseline conditions (Condition 1: Visual Scanning,

Condition 2: Number Sequencing, Condition 3: Letter Sequencing, and Condition 5:

Motor Speed). The primary executive task in this test is Letter-Number Sequencing,

which measures the speed in which a participant can physically sequence a series of

numbers and letters.

Page 22: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

22

The Verbal Fluency Test is composed of three tasks in which participants are

given 60 seconds to generate as many items as possible per trial. In the letter fluency

condition the examinee is asked to generate as many words as they can think of that

begin with the letters “f,” “a,” and “s.” In the category fluency condition, the examinee is

asked to generate as many different words as possible from specific categories;

“animals” and “boy’s names.” In the category switching condition, the final and primary

executive task, the examinee is asked to alternate between naming a type of fruit and a

piece of furniture.

The Color-Word Interference test required recalling and stating words of visual

stimuli presented in multiple rows. The first task, Color Naming, measures the speed of

verbalization when asked to identify blocks of colors and the second, Word Reading,

measures the speed in which a child can read the names of different colors printed in

black ink. The primary executive tasks are the third and fourth conditions. The third task,

Inhibition, is a Stroop-style task that measures the speed of verbalization in which a

child states the ink colors of the words “red,” “green” and “blue” that are printed in

incongruous ink (ignoring what the words presented said or meant and attending only to

the ink colors of these words). The fourth and final task, Inhibition/Switching, is identical

to the previous task, however half the words are outlined by a box. The participant is

required to read the word if it appears in a box, and state the ink color of the word if it

does not. The third task is styled after a well-known measure of IC, while the fourth task

requires the child to apply new instructions to the task and is intended to be a measure

of a child’s ability to switch attention between visual stimuli.

Page 23: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

23

The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV)

The WISC-IV is a standardized intelligence exam that is composed of four first

order factor index scores. This study utilized two of these scores: the WM Index,

composed of the Letter-Number Sequencing and Digit Span subsections, and the

Processing Speed Index, composed of the Coding, and Symbol Search subsections.

Behavior Rating Inventory Of Executive Function (BRIEF)

The BRIEF is a psychometrically sound, parent-rated questionnaire for children

aged 5 to 18 years of age (Gioia et al., 2000). It is one of the first attempts to measure

executive functioning via self and informant reports of every day functioning (Gioia et

al., 2000). It provides two indices: the Behavioral Regulation Index (composed of three

scaled scores: inhibit, shift and emotional control) and the Metacognition Index

(composed of five scaled scores: initiate, working memory, plan/organize, organization

of materials and monitor). Additionally, it provides an overall score, the General

Executive Composite (GEC) that incorporates all of the BRIEF clinical scales.

Within the Behavioral Regulation Index, the inhibit scale is intended to assess IC,

the shift scale is intended to assess a child’s ability to move freely between tasks and

problems as needed, and the emotional control scale is intended to assess the impact

of an executive deficit on a child’s ability to control his or her emotional response. Within

the Metacognition Index, the initiate scale is intended to reflect a child’s ability to begin

a task or activity independently , the working memory scale is intended to assess a

child’s WM, the plan/organize scale is intended to reflect a child’s ability to manage

current and future-oriented task demands, the organization of materials is intended to

Page 24: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

24

reflect a child’s ability to organize spaces of work and play, and the monitor scale is

intended to reflect a child’s ability to assess his or her task performance.

Statistical Analyses

To address the primary goal of determining the relationship between the

submeasures within the BRIEF and subsections of the D-KEFS and WISC-IV, a

correlation matrix was generated.

Aim 1. Examination Of Common Factors Between Tests Hypothesis 1: Subsections of the BRIEF, D-KEFS and WISC-IV will load onto processing speed, working memory, and inhibitory control

In order to assess whether the parental-ratings (BRIEF) of executive dysfunction

in and ADHD population reflected the performance-based deficits observed in this

population, an exploratory factor analysis was performed. Factor analyses allow for the

examination of the internal structure of a set of variables or indicators by generating a

theoretical set of factors (Henson & Roberts, 2006). To assess whether the individual

subsections of the BRIEF, D-KEFS and WISC-IV loaded onto distinct factors

representing processing speed, WM, and IC, an exploratory factor analysis was

performed in order to investigate the common factors between test subsections. Given

the independent and dependent nature of executive functioning, a promax rotation was

used to allow for general factor variation. Eight factors were examined from the BRIEF:

Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Control, Initiate, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Organization

of Materials and Monitor. Only the primary executive tasks from the D-KEFS were

included: from the Verbal Fluency test, Category Switching was included, from the Trail

Making test, Letter-Number Sequencing was included and from the Color-Word

Interference test, Inhibition and Inhibition-Switching were included. Finally, from the

Page 25: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

25

WISC-IV, the Processing Speed and Working Memory composite scores were included.

A total of fourteen variables were entered into the final model.

Aim 2. Predicting Comorbid Anxiety Diagnosis In Children With ADHD Hypothesis 2a: On parental measures, children with an anxiety diagnosis will demonstrate poorer behavioral regulation

In order to assess whether the parental-ratings (BRIEF) of executive dysfunction

was able to distinguish between ADHD and ADHD + AD, a hierarchical binary logistic

regression was performed using the BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index and the BRIEF

Metacognition Index to predict diagnosis. In the first step, sex was entered in order to

control for the skewed sample size. In the second step, the BRIEF Behavioral

Regulation Index and the BRIEF Metacognition Index scores were entered.

Hypothesis 2b: On performance measures, children with an additional anxiety diagnosis will have better working memory than those without an anxiety diagnosis

In order to assess whether performance measures of executive functioning were

able to distinguish between ADHD and ADHD + AD, another hierarchical binary logistic

regression was performed. As in the previous analysis, sex was entered in the first step.

In the second step, a number of measures representing a variety of executive functions

were entered. From the assessment battery, the WISC-IV Processing Speed Composite

was used to assess processing speed, the WISC-IV Working Memory Composite was

used to assess working memory, the D-KEFS Letter-Number Switching score was used

to assess motor switching, the D-KEFS Category Switching score was used to assess

verbal switching, and the D-KEFS Inhibition and Inhibition Switching scores were used

to assess IC.

Page 26: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

26

Table 2-1. Sample demographics (n=226)

Age in years M (SD) 12.2 (2.6) Sex N (%)

Male Female

162 (71.7) 64 (28.3)

Anxiety N (%) Separation Anxiety Disorder Social Phobia Generalized Anxiety Disorder Panic Disorder Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder Anxiety NOS Unspecified Anxiety Disorder

90 (39.8)

Page 27: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

27

CHAPTER 3 RESULTS

Preliminary Correlational Analyses

An initial evaluation of the correlation matrix suggested that some measures of

the BRIEF were correlated with some performance measures (Table 3-1). Specifically,

the WISC-IV processing speed index was related to the emotional control, initiate,

working memory and monitor scales on the BRIEF. On the D-KEFS, the color-word

inhibition scale was related to the BRIEF working memory and monitor scales and the

color-word inhibition-switching scale was related to the BRIEF initiate, working memory,

and monitor scales. All performance measures showed a correlation with one another.

Aim 1. Examination Of Common Factors Between Tests

All scales examined have variances from ranging from 0.136-0.726 (Table 3-2).

An exploratory factor analysis with a promax rotation to allow for general factor variation

was performed. An examination of the factor’s eigenvalue indicates that there are three

factors that are above the criteria of <1 (Table 3-3), with a fourth factor approaching

significance. An examination of the cumulative variance suggests that the three factors

explain about 50% of the total variance. These result should be interpreted with caution,

however, as promax rotation and oblique rotations will miss shared variance.

The screeplot (Figure 3-1) is fairly clear, suggesting that there are three distinct

factors. As seen in the eigenvalue, there is a fourth factor approaching significance, but

there is still a clear inflection point. In sum, the scree plot does not support the four

factors, and although it is slightly unclear, there does appear to be a distinct inflection

point after three factors.

Page 28: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

28

An examination of the Pattern Matrix (Table 3-4), for factor 1, shows that there

are high loadings for four parent-report scales (BRIEF Plan/Organize, BRIEF Working

Memory, BRIEF Initiate, and BRIEF Organization of Materials). All scales load strongly

onto the first factor and do not appear to cross load. The next six scales are the

performance-measures (Category Switching, Letter-Number Sequencing, Inhibition,

Inhibition-Switching, Processing Speed and Working Memory) and load primarily on the

second factor. As with the first factor, the scales appear to load strongly onto this factor

and do not appear to cross load between either of the other factors. The final factor

contains the remaining parent-report scales (BRIEF Emotional Control, BRIEF Shift,

BRIEF Inhibit, and BRIEF Monitor). The final scale, BRIEF monitor, is almost perfectly

cross loads between factor 1 and factor 3 with loadings of .458 and .465, respectively.

However, all other scales load strongly onto factor three without cross loading.

The Structure Matrix does not give any further evidence that is not provided by

the pattern matrix, with the exception of BRIEF Monitor loading onto factor 1 instead of

factor 3. As before, however, it appears to be almost perfectly cross load between

factors (Table 3-5).

An examination of the scales and their loadings suggests that the first factor is

best referred to as “Metacognition Index,” as all scales that comprise the Metacognition

Index load onto the first factor, excluding BRIEF Monitor. Likewise, the third factor could

be referred to as “Behavioral Regulation Index” as all scales that comprise the

Behavioral Regulation Index load onto this factor, excluding BRIEF Monitor. The third

factor, as it is comprised exclusively of the executive functioning performance

measures, could be referred to as “Executive Performance.”

Page 29: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

29

The Behavioral Regulation Index is positively related to the Metacognition Index,

with the factor correlation matrix (Table 3-6) reflecting moderate magnitudes (0.554).

Executive Performance does not appear to be related to either the Metacognition Index

or the Behavioral Regulation Index, with a negative relationship of small magnitude

reflected in the correlation matrix.

Aim 2. Predicting Comorbid Anxiety Diagnosis In Children With ADHD

In the first step, sex was entered in order to control for its impact on diagnosis.

The omnibus test for the model was significant, χ2(1, N=209) = 4.775, p <0.029,

representing a significant improvement over the null model (Table 3-7). Sex was

significant (p=0.029), suggesting that the female sex was associated with 1.9% higher

odds of an anxiety diagnosis. Although the omnibus model tests revealed significance,

Cox and Snell pseudo r-squared was 0.023 and Nagelkerke pseudo r-squared was

0.031; thus this suggests that the model accounted for very little of the individual

differences in diagnosis.

Hypothesis 2a: On Parental Measures, Children With An Anxiety Diagnosis Will Demonstrate Poorer Behavioral Regulation

In the second step, the BRIEF Metacognition Index and the BRIEF Behavioral

Regulation Index were entered. The omnibus test for the model was significant, χ 2(3,

N=209) = 8.048, p <0.045, representing a significant improvement over the null model.

However, the omnibus test for the block was nonsignificant, χ 2(2, N=209) = 3.273, p

=.195 which suggests that there was no significant improvement over the previous

model. Therefore, the significance of this model can likely be attributed to the effects of

sex (Table 3-8), which remained significant (p<0.015). Neither the BRIEF Metacognition

Index nor the BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index was significant. As would be

Page 30: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

30

expected, Cox and Snell pseudo r-squared and Nagelkerke pseudo r-squared increased

only slightly (0.038 and 0.051, respectively) thus this suggests that this model accounts

for very little of the individual differences in diagnosis.

Hypothesis 2b: On Performance Measures, Children With An Additional Anxiety Diagnosis Will Have Better Working Memory Than Those Without An Anxiety Diagnosis

In the third step, performance measures were entered. Specifically, the WISC-IV

Processing Speed Composite score, the WISC-IV Working Memory Composite score,

the D-KEFS Letter-Number Switching scaled score, the D-KEFS Category Switching

scaled score, the D-KEFS Inhibition scaled score and the D-KEFS Inhibition Switching

scaled score were included in the model. The omnibus test for the model was

significant, χ 2(9, N=209) = 30.832, p <.001, representing a significant improvement over

the null model (Table 3-9). The omnibus test for the block was significant, χ 2(6, N=209)

= 22.784, p =.001 representing a significant improvement over the previous model.

Although the omnibus model tests revealed significance, Cox and Snell pseudo r-

squared was 0.137 and Nagelkerke pseudo r-squared was 0.186; thus this suggests

that the model accounted for relatively little of the individual differences in diagnosis.

In this model, sex remained significant (p<0.001), and of the entered

performance measures, the WISC-IV Working Memory Composite (p=0.007), WISC-IV

Processing Speed Composite (p=0.002), and the D-KEFS Verbal Fluency, Category

Switching (p=.034) are all significant. Each unit increase in WISC-IV Working Memory

Composite score was associated with 3.6% higher odds of having a comorbid anxiety

diagnosis, each unit decrease in WISC-IV Processing Speed Composite score was

associated with 4.7% higher odds of having a comorbid anxiety diagnosis, and each unit

Page 31: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

31

decrease in D-KEFS Verbal Fluency, Category Switching scaled score was associated

with 10% higher odds of having a comorbid anxiety diagnosis.

Overall, 61.2% of patients were correctly classified (Table 3-10). 77.8% of those

who do not have an anxiety diagnosis were correctly predicted, while 36.1% of those

who did have a comorbid anxiety diagnosis were correctly identified, giving this model a

sensitivity of 36.1% and a specificity of 77.8%. Chance classification would be 0.53 and

25% better than chance would be 0.657. Actual correct classification was 67.9%,

suggesting that this model predicts diagnosis better than chance.

An examination of the model suggests that the C-statistic is 0.718 and

significantly greater than 0.5 (p < .001), and thus meets the 0.7 minimum criteria

suggested by Hosmer and Lemeshow (1982). Agresti’s point-biserial is 0.136 and

suggests a weak association between model-estimated individual probabilities of

correctly predicting a comorbid anxiety disorder.

Page 32: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

32

Table 3-1. Correlation Matrix

D-KEFS: Number-

Letter Switching

D-KEFS: Category Switching

D-KEFS: Inhibition

D-KEFS: Inhibition-Switching

WISC-IV: WM

WISC-IV: Processing

Speed

BRIEF: Inhibit

BRIEF: Shift

BRIEF: Emotional

Control

BRIEF: Initiate

BRIEF: Memory

BRIEF: Plan/

Organize

BRIEF: Organize Materials

D-KEFS: Category Switching

.233** 1

D-KEFS: Inhibition

.392** .277** 1

D-KEFS: Inhibition-Switching

.315** .251** .699** 1

WISC-IV: Working Memory

Composite

.423** .141* .286** .140* 1

WISC-IV: Processing

Speed Composite

.413** .275** .474** .367** .368** 1

BRIEF: Inhibit

0.023 0.049 -0.049 0.014 -0.047 -0.043 1

BRIEF: Shift

0.005 -0.032 -0.049 -0.016 -0.012 -0.061 .511** 1

Page 33: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

33

Table 3-1. Continued

D-KEFS: Number-

Letter Switching

D-KEFS: Category Switching

D-KEFS: Inhibition

D-KEFS: Inhibition-Switching

WISC-IV: WM

WISC-IV: Processing

Speed

BRIEF: Inhibit

BRIEF: Shift

BRIEF: Emotional

Control

BRIEF: Initiate

BRIEF: Memory

BRIEF: Plan/

Organize

BRIEF: Organize Materials

BRIEF: Emotional

Control -.048 .019 -.024 .012 -.015 -.156* .582** .663** 1

BRIEF: Initiate

-.037 -.045 -.106 -.147* -.012 -.198** .346** .469** .438** 1

BRIEF: Memory

-.057 -.022 -.131* -.161* .012 -.173** .422** .347** .259** .617** 1

BRIEF: Plan/

Organize -.002 .012 -.061 -.120 .034 -.105 .343** .384** .256** .638** .681** 1

BRIEF: Organize Materials

.089 .043 -.023 -.023 .033 -.096 .388** .222** .277** .434** .465** .491** 1

BRIEF: Monitor

-.076 .020 -.148* -.128* -.056 -.179** .613** .583** .503** .588** .573** .617** .438**

**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). *. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

Page 34: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

34

Table 3-2. Communalities

Initial Extraction

BRIEF - Inhibit 0.514 0.494

BRIEF - Shift 0.583 0.623

BRIEF - Emotional Control 0.592 0.726

BRIEF - Initiate 0.570 0.590

BRIEF - Memory 0.588 0.672

BRIEF - Plan/Organize 0.614 0.718

BRIEF - Organization of Materials 0.340 0.348

BRIEF - Monitor 0.636 0.685

D-KEFS - Trail Making: Number-Letter Switching 0.339 0.382

D-KEFS - Verbal Fluency: Category Switching 0.586 0.622

D-KEFS - Color-Word: Inhibition 0.537 0.471

D-KEFS - Color-Word: Inhibition-Switching 0.129 0.136

WISC-IV - Working Memory Composite 0.282 0.219

WISC-IV - Processing Speed Composite 0.377 0.431

Extraction method: principal axis factoring.

Page 35: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

35

Table 3-3. Total Variance Explained

Factor

Initial Eigenvalues Extraction Sums of Squared

Loadings

Rotation Sums of Squared

Loadingsa

Total % of

Variance Cumulative

% Total % of

Variance Cumulative

% Total

1 4.414 31.526 31.526 4.033 28.810 28.810 3.520

2 2.697 19.262 50.788 2.135 15.250 44.060 2.269

3 1.341 9.581 60.369 .950 6.786 50.846 3.113

4 .990 7.072 67.441

5 .850 6.074 73.515

6 .698 4.986 78.501

7 .568 4.056 82.557

8 .549 3.919 86.476

9 .464 3.317 89.793

10 .359 2.562 92.355

11 .325 2.320 94.676

12 .270 1.929 96.605

13 .257 1.836 98.441

14 .218 1.559 100.000

Extraction method: principal axis factoring. a. When factors are correlated, sums of squared loadings cannot be added to obtain a total variance.

Page 36: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

36

Figure 3-1. Scree plot

Page 37: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

37

Table 3-4. Pattern Matrix

Factor

1 2 3

BRIEF - Plan/Organize .895 .062 -.074

BRIEF - Working Memory .845 .005 -.046

BRIEF - Initiate .667 -.037 .151

BRIEF - Organization of Materials .579 .124 .034

D-KEFS - Color-Word: Inhibition -.059 .781 .065

WISC-IV - Processing Speed Composite .003 .643 -.099

D-KEFS - Color-Word: Inhibition-Switching -.204 .638 .215

D-KEFS - Trail Making: Number/Letter Switching

.154 .626 -.084

WISC-IV - Working Memory Composite .166 .469 -.106

D-KEFS - Verbal Fluency: Category Switching .083 .374 .007

BRIEF - Emotional Control -.124 -.040 .911

BRIEF - Shift .088 .010 .738

BRIEF - Inhibit .124 .042 .629

BRIEF - Monitor .458 -.078 .465

Extraction method: principal axis factoring, 3 factors extracted, 14 iterations required.

Page 38: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

38

Table 3-5. Structure Matrix

Factor

1 2 3

BRIEF - Plan/Organize 0.843 -0.087 0.417

BRIEF – Working Memory 0.819 -0.138 0.421

BRIEF - Initiate 0.757 -0.163 0.523

BRIEF - Monitor 0.729 -0.190 0.724

BRIEF - Organization of Materials 0.577 0.021 0.346

D-KEFS - Color-Word: Inhibition -0.158 0.787 -0.023

D-KEFS - Color-Word: Inhibition-Switching -0.195 0.658 0.057

WISC-IV Processing Speed Composite -0.163 0.650 -0.142

D-KEFS - Trail Making: Number-Letter Switching -0.001 0.605 -0.043

WISC-IV Working Memory Composite 0.026 0.448 -0.048

D-KEFS - Verbal Fluency: Category Switching 0.023 0.359 0.027

BRIEF - Emotional Control 0.388 -0.083 0.846

BRIEF - Shift 0.495 -0.057 0.786

BRIEF - Inhibit 0.465 -0.024 0.695

Extraction method: principal axis factoring, 3 factors extracted, 14 iterations required.

Table 3-6. Factor correlation matrix

Factor 1 2 3

1 1.000 -.173 .554

2 -.173 1.000 -.070

3 .554 -.070 1.000

Extraction method: principal axis factoring. Rotation method: promax with Kaiser

normalization.

Page 39: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

39

Table 3-7. Hierarchical binary logistic regression, step 1

Block 1 B S.E. Exp(B) 95% CI for Exp(B)

Predictor Lower Upper

Sex .684* .313 1.981 1. 072 3.661

Note. * p <.05, ** p < .01

Table 3-8. Hierarchical binary logistic regression, step 2

Block 2 B S.E. Exp(B) 95% CI for Exp(B)

Predictor Lower Upper

Sex .799** .327 2.222 1. 170 4.220

BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index

-.009 .013 .991 .967 1.016

BRIEF Metacognition Index -.019 .020 .981 .944 1.019

Note. * p <.05, ** p < .01

Page 40: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

40

Table 3-9. Hierarchical binary logistic regression, step 3

Block 3 B S.E. Exp(B) 95% CI for

Exp(B)

Predictor Lower Upper

Sex 1.345** .381 3.836 1.817 8.103

BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index -.009 .014 .991 .964 1.018

BRIEF Metacognition Index -.034 .022 .967 .926 1.009

WISC-IV Working Memory Composite .035** .013 1.036 1.010 1.063

WISC-IV Processing Speed Composite -.048** .016 .953 .925 .983

D-KEFS Color-Word – Inhibition .070 .069 1.073 .936 1.229

D-KEFS Trail-Making – Letter-Number Switching

.060 1.381 1.061 .961 1.172

D-KEFS Verbal Fluency – Category Switching

-.106* 4.516 .900 .816 .992

D-KEFS Color-Word – Inhibition-Switching

.062 .736 1.064 .923 1.228

Note. * p <.05, ** p < .01

Table 3-10. Classification table

Observed

Predicted

Anxiety Diagnosis Status

Percentage

Correct ADHD alone

ADHD and

anxiety

Anxiety

Diagnosis

Status

ADHD alone 104 22 82.5

ADHD and anxiety 45 38 45.8

Overall Percentage: 67.9

Page 41: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

41

CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION

Although preliminary results from the correlational analysis suggested that

parental-ratings from the BRIEF and performance measures may have some

relationship, results from this study reveal that performance measures and parental-

report based measures do not capture similar constructs. Indeed, contrary to the

hypothesis, performance measure and parental-report measures appear to capture

entirely different constructs. This contributes to the growing body of research, which

suggests that parent-report and performance based measures of executive functioning

measure different constructs (Davidson et al., 2016; Eycke & Dewey, 2016). Indeed, in

this study there appeared to be almost no correlation between the BRIEF subscales and

the performance subscales selected from the D-KEFS and WISC-IV.

The factors generated by the exploratory factor analysis appear to reflect three

distinct measures. The first, referred to as the “Behavioral Regulation Index,” is almost

perfectly composed of scales composing the Behavioral Regulation Index on the

BRIEF. The second factor, referred to as the “Metacognition Index,” likewise reflects the

BRIEF’s Metacognition Index. The scale “monitor” was almost perfectly cross loads on

these two factors, suggesting that it captures a construct within this specific population

that is similar to both the Metacognition Index and the Behavioral Regulation Index. The

final factor, referred to as “Executive Performance,” contained all performance

measures. This is likely reflective of the timed nature of these tasks, and is thus

reflective of the common nature of these measurements, rather than being suggestive

of their ability to measure a common construct. Taken together, this factor analysis

suggests that the BRIEF’s Metacognition Index and Behavioral Regulation Index each

Page 42: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

42

measure unique constructs within a pediatric ADHD population. Furthermore, the BRIEF

does not reflect a common construct with any performance measures examined.

The results of the hierarchical binary logistic regression suggest that the BRIEF

cannot distinguish between children with ADHD and children with ADHD + AD.

However, the performance measures examined were able to predict a comorbid anxiety

diagnosis in the ADHD population 25% better than chance, such that better

performance on the WISC-IV Working Memory Composite was associated with higher

odds of having a comorbid anxiety diagnosis and worse performance on WISC-IV

Processing Speed Composite and D-KEFS Verbal Fluency, Category Switching was

associated higher odds of having a comorbid anxiety diagnosis. While limited previous

research has been done on the executive functioning differences between these

populations, previous research has implicated greater impairment in processing speed

in an ADHD + AD population, as compared to an ADHD population (Bloemsma et al.,

2013). However, the improved performance in WM runs contrary to previous research,

which has suggested that an additional anxiety diagnosis is associated with poorer

performance on WM tasks (Jarrett et al., 2016). There are a variety of possible

explanations for this; one explanation is that it is reflective of methodological

differences, specifically differences in task selection for WM, or the high rates of

additional comorbidity within our sample. Another explanation that should be considered

is the potential for misdiagnosis. Anxiety disorders are associated with poor executive

attention (Mogg et al., 2015), and many complaints associated with anxiety bare

remarkable resemblance to the diagnostic criteria for ADHD (such as poor attention and

Page 43: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

43

concentration). It is, therefore, possible that some children within this sample have

symptoms better accounted for by only an anxiety diagnosis, rather than ADHD + AD.

Few conclusions can be drawn from this. While the BRIEF failed to predict

diagnosis classification in this sample, it would be premature to state that parental-

measures do not distinguish between ADHD + AD and ADHD. While performance

measures did predict comorbidity, the weak sensitivity of the model examined suggests

that it is a less than desirable method of differentiating between these two populations.

However, these results are suggestive of some objectively measurable, cognitive

difference between these populations. In short, while parental measures may not predict

ADHD + AD, performance measures may. This is of particular interest given that this

sample contained children with a number of comorbid diagnoses that were not taken

into account in this analysis, offering some advantage to clinical application. While it

may be argued that diagnostically “pure” samples will best illuminate cognitive

differences with some degree of certainty, this holds little practical application given the

rampant rate of comorbidity in ADHD (Cuffe et al., 2015) and across the DSM (Cramer,

Waldorp, van der Maas, & Borsboom, 2010).

Though findings from this analysis support recent literature reaching similar

conclusions regarding the relationship between parental-ratings and performance

measures (Davidson et al., 2016; Eycke & Dewey, 2016), these results are preliminary

in nature and are limited by relatively few children diagnosed with ADHD + AD

(n=83/209). These findings were reflective of population rates (Barkely, 2007), with our

sample containing more males than females at a ratio approaching 3:1. However, given

the heterogeneous nature of ADHD (Cuffee et al., 2015), and the differences in

Page 44: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

44

executive functioning on both parental-ratings and performance measures among males

and females with ADHD (Skogli, Teicher, Andersen, Hovik, & Øie, 2013), sex

differences may of be great interest. Furthermore, as previously mentioned, this sample

contained children with a number of comorbid diagnoses that were not considered in

these analyses. While this may offer some advantage in terms of realistic application of

results to a clinical sample, it does little to elucidate the true cognitive differences

between children with ADHD and ADHD + AD. These findings are also limited by a

narrow test battery. The only parental rating considered was the BRIEF, and the only

performance-measures examined were the D-KEFS and the composite scores of the

WISC-IV Processing Speed Index and Working Memory Index. As such, these results

should be interpreted with caution.

Future studies should examine this with a larger sample size, and a more

comprehensive test battery. While the results of these analyses suggest that the BRIEF

does not distinguish between children with ADHD and children with ADHD + AD,

another parental measure might. Furthermore, a larger test battery may be able to

distinguish between children with ADHD and children with ADHD + AD with greater

sensitivity and specificity than the current model. Future research should also consider

examining a “pure” sample, in order to illuminate cognitive differences between children

with ADHD, ADHD + AD, and children with only anxiety disorders. Such findings may be

better able to guide the development of a neurocognitive battery to identify and

distinguish between these populations.

Finally, rating measures should continue to be examined in order to determine

their relationship to performance-based measures. Previous research has suggested

Page 45: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

45

that teacher rating scales may more accurately reflect cognitive impairment, due to

greater familiarity with behavioral responses to increased cognitive demand (Eycke &

Dewey, 2016). While rating scales may currently capture the behavioral expression of

executive dysfunction, the development or identification of a rating measure that

accurately reflects cognitive impairment holds incredible clinical and diagnostic

relevance. Psychological assessment is often expensive, and should reflect

improvements in treatment or prevention efforts in order to justify the monetary cost

(Yates & Taub, 2003). Rating scales that accurately reflect cognitive impairment may

decrease the monetary cost while simultaneously providing improvements in treatment

and prevention efforts, especially to families with limited access to treatment.

ADHD is associated with neurocognitive impairments in addition to behavioral

dysfunction. Anxiety disorders are highly comorbid with ADHD, and many anxiety

disorders are associated with difficulty with attention and concentration (American

Psychological Association, 2013). Some evidence suggests children with ADHD + AD

experience poorer outcomes in treatment when compared to counterparts without

ADHD (Halldorsdottir & Ollendick, 2013). Results from this study suggest that children

with ADHD + AD and children with ADHD evidence some differences in cognitive

impairment. Therefore, accurately distinguishing between ADHD + AD and ADHD alone

could help children and families access the treatment best suited to their needs.

Page 46: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

46

REFERENCES

Adler, L. A., Spencer, T. J., & Wilens, T. E. (Eds.). (2015). Attention-deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Adults and Children. Cambridge University Press.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during

childhood. Child neuropsychology, 8(2), 71-82. Banich, M. T. (2009). Executive function: The search for an integrated account. Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 89-94. Bloemsma, J. M., Boer, F., Arnold, R., Banaschewski, T., Faraone, S. V., Buitelaar, J.

K., ... & Oosterlaan, J. (2013). Comorbid anxiety and neurocognitive dysfunctions in children with ADHD. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 22(4), 225-234.

Brook, J. S., Brook, D. W., Zhang, C., Seltzer, N., & Finch, S. J. (2013). Adolescent

ADHD and adult physical and mental health, work performance, and financial stress. Pediatrics, 131(1), 5-13.

Chung, H. J., Weyandt, L. L., & Swentosky, A. (2014). The physiology of executive

functioning. In S. Goldstein & J. A. Naglieri (Eds.), Handbook of Executive Functioning (pp. 13-27). New York, NY: Springer.

Cuffe, S. P., Visser, S. N., Holbrook, J. R., Danielson, M. L., Geryk, L. L., Wolraich, M.

L., & McKeown, R. E. (2015). ADHD and Psychiatric Comorbidity Functional Outcomes in a School-Based Sample of Children. Journal of Attention Disorders, 1-10.

Cramer, A. O., Waldorp, L. J., van der Maas, H. L., & Borsboom, D. (2010). Complex

realities require complex theories: Refining and extending the network approach to mental disorders. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2-3), 178-193.

Davidson, F., Cherry, K., & Corkum, P. (2016). Validating the behavior rating inventory

of executive functioning for children with ADHD and their typically developing peers. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 5(2), 127-137.

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual review of psychology, 64, 135-168. Doshi, J. A., Hodgkins, P., Kahle, J., Sikirica, V., Cangelosi, M. J., Setyawan, J., ... &

Neumann, P. J. (2012). Economic impact of childhood and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the United States. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(10), 990-1002.

Page 47: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

47

Efron, D., Bryson, H., Lycett, K., & Sciberras, E. (2016). Children referred for evaluation for ADHD: comorbidity profiles and characteristics associated with a positive diagnosis. Child: Care, Health and Development, 42(5), 718-724.

Erskine, H. E., Norman, R. E., Ferrari, A. J., Chan, G. C., Copeland, W. E., Whiteford,

H. A., & Scott, J. G. (2016). Long-term outcomes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(10), 841-850.

Eycke, K. D., & Dewey, D. (2016). Parent-report and performance-based measures of

executive function assess different constructs. Child Neuropsychology, 22(8), 889-906.

Fassbender, C., Schweitzer, J. B., Cortes, C. R., Tagamets, M. A., Windsor, T. A.,

Reeves, G. M., & Gullapalli, R. (2011). Working memory in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a lack of specialization of brain function. PLoS One, 6(11), e27240- e27240.

Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions:

Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex. 86, 186–204. Goldstein, S., Naglieri, J. A., Princiotta, D., & Otero, T. M. (2014). Introduction: a history

of executive functioning as a theoretical and clinical construct. In S. Goldstein & J. A. Naglieri (Eds.), Handbook of Executive Functioning (pp. 3-12). New York, NY: Springer.

Hart, H., Radua, J., Nakao, T., Mataix-Cols, D., & Rubia, K. (2013). Meta-analysis of

functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: exploring task-specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(2), 185-198.

Harpin, V., Mazzone, L., Raynaud, J. P., Kahle, J., & Hodgkins, P. (2016). Long-term

outcomes of ADHD: a systematic review of self-esteem and social function. Journal of Attention Disorders, 20(4), 295-305.

Henson, R. K., & Roberts, J. K. (2006). Use of exploratory factor analysis in published

research: Common errors and some comment on improved practice. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66(3), 393-416.

Homack, S., Lee, D., & Riccio, C. A. (2005). Test review: Delis-Kaplan executive

function system. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(5), 599-609.

Jarrett, M. A., Wolff, J. C., Davis III, T. E., Cowart, M. J., & Ollendick, T. H. (2016).

Characteristics of children with ADHD and comorbid anxiety. Journal of Attention Disorders, 20(7), 636-644.

Page 48: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

48

Jurado, M. B., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17(3), 213-233.

Lee, Y. C., Yang, H. J., Chen, V. C. H., Lee, W. T., Teng, M. J., Lin, C. H., & Gossop,

M. (2016). Meta-analysis of quality of life in children and adolescents with ADHD: By both parent proxy-report and child self-report using PedsQL™. Research in developmental disabilities, 51, 160-172.

Lemeshow, S., & Hosmer, D. W. (1982). A review of goodness of fit statistics for use in

the development of logistic regression models. American Journal of Epidemiology, 115(1), 92-106.

McAuley, T., Chen, S., Goos, L., Schachar, R., & Crosbie, J. (2010). Is the behavior

rating inventory of executive function more strongly associated with measures of impairment or executive function? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(3), 495–505.

McCabe, D., Roediger, H., McDaniel, M., Balota, D., & Hambrick, D. (2010). The

Relationship Between Working Memory Capacity and Executive Functioning: Evidence for a Common Executive Attention Construct. Neuropsychology, 24(2), 222-243.

Miyake, A., Friedman, N.P., Emerson, M.J., Witzki, A.H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T.D.

(2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100.

Miyake, A., Friedman, N., Rettinger, D., Shah, P., & Hegarty, M. (2001). How are

visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,130(4), 621-640.

Mogg, K., Salum, G. A., Bradley, B. P., Gadelha, A., Pan, P., Alvarenga, P., ... &

Manfro, G. G. (2015). Attention network functioning in children with anxiety disorders, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and non-clinical anxiety. Psychological Medicine, 45(12), 2633-2646.

Niendam, T. A., Laird, A. R., Ray, K. L., Dean, Y. M., Glahn, D. C., & Carter, C. S.

(2012). Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12(2), 241-268.

Olivers, C. N., Peters, J., Houtkamp, R., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2011). Different states in

visual working memory: When it guides attention and when it does not. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(7), 327-334.

Page 49: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

49

Roberts, B. A., Martel, M. M., & Nigg, J. T. (2017). Are there executive dysfunction subtypes within ADHD?. Journal of Attention Disorders, 21(4) 284-293.

Polanczyk, G. V., Willcutt, E. G., Salum, G. A., Kieling, C., & Rohde, L. A. (2014). ADHD

prevalence estimates across three decades: an updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(2), 434-442.

Salthouse, T.A., Atkinson, T., & Berish, D. (2003). Executive Functioning as a Potential

Mediator of Age-Related Cognitive Decline in Normal Adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(4), 566-594.

Schatz, D. B., & Rostain, A. L. (2006). ADHD with comorbid anxiety a review of the

current literature. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10(2), 141-149. Skogli, E. W., Teicher, M. H., Andersen, P. N., Hovik, K. T., & Øie, M. (2013). ADHD in

girls and boys–sex differences in co-existing symptoms and executive function measures. BMC Psychiatry, 13(1), 298.

Sjöwall, D., Roth, L., Lindqvist, S., & Thorell, L. B. (2013). Multiple deficits in ADHD:

executive dysfunction, delay aversion, reaction time variability, and emotional deficits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(6), 619-627.

Snyder, H. R., Kaiser, R. H., Warren, S. L., and Heller, W. (2015a). Obsessive-

compulsive disorder is associated with broad impairments in executive function: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(2), 301–330.

Snyder, H. R., Miyake, A., & Hankin, B. L. (2015b). Advancing understanding of

executive function impairments and psychopathology: bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1-24.

Sørensen, L., Plessen, K. J., Nicholas, J., & Lundervold, A. J. (2011). Is behavioral

regulation in children with ADHD aggravated by comorbid anxiety disorder?. Journal of Attention Disorders, 15(1), 56-66.

Taanila, A., Ebeling, H., Tiihala, M., Kaakinen, M., Moilanen, I., Hurtig, T., & Yliherva, A.

(2014). Association between childhood specific learning difficulties and school performance in adolescents with and without ADHD symptoms: a 16-year follow-up. Journal of Attention Disorders, 18(1), 61-72.

Tarver, J., Daley, D., & Sayal, K. (2014). Attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): an updated review of the essential facts. Child: Care, Health and Development, 40(6), 762-774.

Thomas, R., Sanders, S., Doust, J., Beller, E., & Glasziou, P. (2015). Prevalence of

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 135(4), e994-e1001.

Page 50: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

50

Weigard, A., & Huang-Pollock, C. (2016). The role of speed in ADHD-related working memory deficits: a time-based resource-sharing and diffusion model account. Clinical Psychological Science, 1-17.

Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005).

Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1336-1346.

Yates, B. T., & Taub, J. (2003). Assessing the costs, benefits, cost-effectiveness, and

cost-benefit of psychological assessment: we should, we can, and here's how. Psychological assessment, 15(4), 478.

Page 51: By DANIELLE LAINE COOKE - University of Floridaufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/05/12/30/00001/COOKE_D.pdf · 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the

51

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH

Danielle Laine Cooke is a second-year graduate student in the Department of

Clinical and Health Psychology at the University of Florida. She obtained her bachelor’s

degrees in psychology and English from the University of Florida in 2015 and has been

working with the UF OCD Clinic since 2013. Since that time she has had the opportunity

to work with a wide variety of adults and children with anxiety-spectrum disorders and

eating disorders. Danielle was offered the opportunity to begin graduate studies in

clinical psychology at the University of Florida in the fall of 2015 and has continued

working in the Division of Medical Psychology as a graduate assistant. She identifies

strongly with the scientist-practitioner model and is primarily interested in treatment

outcome research, and the impact of transdiagnostic factors, including executive

functioning and sleep.


Recommended